ArticlePDF Available

The influence of past relationships on subsequent relationships: The role of the self

Authors:

Abstract

The present study sought to determine whether past relationships have a significant influence on the self, and thereby influence subsequent partner choice. Specifically, it was hypothesized that inclusion of a former partner in the self would be positively correlated to desired similarity between the former and next likely partner in terms of physical characteristics, personality traits, and hobbies/interests. Participants were 27 males and 98 females who were not currently in dating relationships and had broken up with the former partner with the last 6 months. Findings supported the hypothesis, and found that that inclusion of the former partner in the self was significantly associated with desired similarity between former partner and next likely partner in terms of physical characteristics, personality traits, and hobbies/ interests. Further, this association was still evident after controlling for variables such as time since break-up, length of former relationship, current frequency of interaction, and gender. These findings support the idea that former relationships may influence subsequent relationships due to the impact of former relationships on the self.
Individual Differences Research,
2005, 3(4)
269
ISSN: 1541-745X
www.idr-journal.com
C2005 IDR Publishing Ltd. Co. All rights reserved.
The Influence of Past Relationships on
Subsequent Relationships:
The Role of the Self
Gary W. Lewandowski Jr.* & Dani Faith Sahner
Monmouth University
ABSTRACT -
The present study sought to determine whether past relationships
have a significant influence on the self, and thereby influence subsequent partner
choice. Specifically, it was hypothesized that inclusion of a former partner in the
self would be positively correlated to desired similarity between the former and
next likely partner in terms of physical characteristics, personality traits, and
hobbies/interests. Participants were 27 males and 98 females who were not
currently in dating relationships and had broken up with the former partner with
the last 6 months. Findings supported the hypothesis, and found that that
inclusion of the former partner in the self was significantly associated with
desired similarity between former partner and next likely partner in terms of
physical characteristics, personality traits, and hobbies/interests. Further, this
association was still evident after controlling for variables such as time since
break-up, length of former relationship, current frequency of interaction, and
gender. These findings support the idea that former relationships may influence
subsequent relationships due to the impact of former relationships on the self.
Do past relationships influence our subsequent choice of a dating partner? If
this does occur, what might be the mechanism of influence? One possibility is
that past relationships change or influence the self in such a way that we alter our
preferences for subsequent dating partners. If this were the case, it may begin to
explain why some people repeatedly become involved in poor relationships. This
paper hopes to provide support for the hypothesis that past relationships have a
significant influence on the self, through inclusion of the former partner in the
self, and thereby influence future relationships.
Relationship research has demonstrated that romantic relationships have a
significant effect on the partners involved (e.g. Frazier, Byer, Fischer, Wright,
& DeBord, 1996; Ruvolo, Fabin, & Ruvolo, 2001). Studies such as this have
laid the groundwork for the notion that relationships elicit personal changes and
*Gary W. Lewandowski Jr. Ph.D.; 400 Cedar Ave.; Monmouth University; Psychology
Department; West Long Branch, NJ 07764; glewando@monmouth.edu (email).
270
Individual Differences Research,
2005,
3(4)
suggest that changes to the self may then be carried over into future
relationships. One prominent theory that focuses on how relationships may
influence the self is attachment style.
Attachment style refers to the process by which both infants and adults form
emotional bonds, categorized as secure, avoidant, or anxious ambivalent, with
an attachment figure (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Attachment styles, though formed
in infancy, tend to persist through adulthood in the form of romantic
relationships. Research surrounding attachment styles has often focused on the
implications that attachment styles have on mate selection (Frazier et al., 1996).
One study looking at how attachment style influences partner choice found that
people are most attracted to others with an attachment style similar to their own.
The findings of this study suggest that a previous relationship, in this case with
a parent, can influence partner choice in future adult romantic relationships.
It is also possible that a romantic relationship can alter one's attachment style
(Ruvolo et al., 2001). In this study, participants answered questions at two time
points to determine if there was any attachment style change related to the
relationship status of the individual (i.e. whether the couple stayed together or
broke-up). Results showed that those who experienced relationship dissolution
felt less secure in their attachment style. Moreover, distressing breakups were
more likely to generate attachment style change than less distressing break-ups.
These results demonstrate that although people are likely to choose a partner that
has a similar attachment style (Frazier et al., 1996), attachment style can change
over the course of several relationships (Ruvolo et al., 2001). Thus, a
relationship induced change in the self may lead a person to seek a subsequent
partner with a different attachment style than their former partner, a view that is
congruent with the idea that past relationships may influence future relationships.
In light of the research on attachment it seems likely that influences on the self
due to previous relationships may influence subsequent partner choice. One
method of determining how much a person's self-concept has been influenced
by a previous relationship would be to determine the degree to which the present
self-concept overlaps with the former partner. This is potentially important
because if substantial overlap exists, a person might be motivated to seek a
subsequent partner that is similar to their previous partner to avoid losing parts
of the self.
Research has shown that the self-concept is somewhat malleable and changes
in response to increased closeness within a relationship (Agnew, Van Lange,
Rusbult, & Langston, 1998). As a couple becomes more intimate with each other
and the relationship strengthens, they begin to perceive themselves more as a
couple than as two separate beings. This process, known as cognitive
interdependence, is associated with a couple using words such as "we" and "us,"
rather than "I" or "me". A major component of this view involves the self-
expansion model.
Individual Differences Research,
2005,
3(4)
271
This model states that people seek out partners who will increase their
knowledge and offer new experiences that would thereby enhance the self (Aron
& Aron, 1996). According to the self-expansion model, people achieve
expansion via the process of inclusion of other in the self. Inclusion of other in
the self, a key component of cognitive interdependence, is the amount of
cognitive overlap between two people in a close relationship such that the closer
two people are, the more they begin to include aspects of the other person in
their own identity. Inclusion of other in the self is measured by a series of seven
pairs of overlapping circles, labeled as self and other, that indicate the degree to
which there is an intersection of one's self and the partner's self (Aron, Aron, &
Smollan, 1992). The amount of overlap indicates how much of one's self the
person feels is intertwined with the self of their current or former relationship
partner.
A study examining the effects of inclusion of other in the self found that
married couples had a harder time attributing characteristics to either themselves
or their spouses when the given trait was distinctly descriptive of only one
member of the couple (Aron, Aron, Tudor, & Nelson, 1991). These findings are
consistent with the idea that if two people are highly included with each other,
they will have a difficult time separating what they know to be true about the self
from what they know to be true about their partner (Aron et al., 1996). In the
context of the present study, it is possible that if a person still includes their
former partner in the self to a high degree, they may be motivated to seek a
subsequent partner with similar attributes in order to avoid losing valued
elements of the self. Thus, the amount of continued overlap with a former
partner may continue to influence the self and therefore influence subsequent
partner choice.
Past research on attachment has alluded to the possibility that past
relationships influence the self and, in turn, subsequent relationships. However,
this possibility has not been studied directly. This paper hopes to fill this void by
testing cognitive closeness to a former partner as a possible influence on
subsequent partner choice. Specifically, this paper tests the following hypotheses
1)
Those that include a former partner in the self to a high degree will provide
a description of a probable subsequent partner that is more similar to the
description of their former partner in terms of physical characteristics,
personality traits, and hobbies/interests, than those who include their former
partner to a lesser degree.
2)
The association between inclusion of other in the self and similarity between
former and next likely partner will remain when controlling for other factors
such as time since break-up, length of former relationship, current frequency
of interaction, and gender.
272
Individual Differences Research,
2005,
3(4)
Method
Participants
The participants in this study included 27 males and 98 females from a
medium sized private university in the Northeast. The participants ranged in age
froml8 to 30
(
M =
18). The mean length of former relationship was 63.65
weeks, mean time since break-up was 19.71 weeks, and means days of
interaction per week was 4.29. A majority of the participants were Caucasian
(95.3 %) and freshman (78.9 %). Participants were recruited using a convenience
sample from the undergraduate subject pool and received credit for participation.
Measures
Inclusion of Former Partner in Self Scale.
This measure is a slightly modified
version of the Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale (IDS; Aron et al., 1992) that
asks participants to respond about their most recent former partner (rather than
their current partner as in the original scale). The measure includes seven pairs
of overlapping circles labeled to represent the self and former partner, ranging
from circles with no overlap representing low levels of inclusion with former
partner (scored as a 1) to circles that are almost completely overlapping
representing high levels of inclusion (scored as a 7). Participants choose the set
of circles that most closely represent their current level of inclusion with the
former partner.
Desired Similarity between Former and Next Likely Partner.
This measure
asked participants to describe their former and next likely partner in terms of
physical characteristics, personality traits, and hobbies/interests. For the physical
characteristics, participants described their partner on quality such as hair and
eye color, body type, age, and style of dress. For the personality traits,
participants rated 47 common personality traits such as adventurous, emotional,
humorous, persuasive, secure, talkative, and understanding on a 7-Point Likert
Scale ranging from "0" or "not at all" to "7" or "extremely." For personal
interests, participants rated 52 common hobbies such as reading, going to the
movies, jogging, dancing, and watching television on a 7-point Likert Scale
ranging from "0" or "not at all" to "7" or "extremely." Degree of similarity was
calculated as the mean absolute difference between the former and next partner
descriptions in each dimension. Larger numbers indicate a desire for greater
similarity.
Demographics.
In addition to typical demographic items, this section asked
participants about the length of the their former, the amount of time that had
elapsed since the break-up, and how many days they come into contact with the
former partner in a typical week.
Results
Inclusion of Former Partner in Self (Hyp 1) —
To test the first hypothesis, we
ran several correlational analyses. As hypothesized, inclusion of former partner
Individual Differences Research,
2005,
3(4)
273
in the self was significantly associated with desiring a next partner who was
similar to the former partner in terms of physical characteristics
(r =
.27,
p
=
.002), personality traits
(r =
.34,
p <
.001), and hobbies/interests
(r =
.28,
p
=
.001). These results suggest that those who include their former partner to a high
degree will desire a subsequent partner who is similar to their former partner on
the above dimensions.
Role of Inclusion of Former Partner in Self Controlling for Other Variables
(Hyp 2) —
To test the second hypothesis, we ran several multiple regression
analyses in which inclusion of former partner in the self was entered
simultaneously with length of former relationship, time since break-up, days of
interaction per week, and gender. In each case, the association between inclusion
of former partner in the self and desired similarity between next likely partner
and former partner remained significant; physical characteristics (beta =
.22,
p
= .02), personality traits (beta =
.37, p
<
.001), and hobbies/interests
(beta =
.33,
p =
.001). Additionally, in each case the partial
r
in the context of the regression
equation (measuring the unique association of inclusion of former partner in the
self) was similar to the zero-order correlation (.22, .37, and .32 respectively).
These results suggest that inclusion of former partner in the self is largely
responsible for desired similarity between next likely partner and former partner.
Discussion
The present research found support for the hypothesis that inclusion of the
former partner in the self would be positively correlated with desired similarity
between former partner and next likely partner in terms of physical
characteristics, personality traits, and hobbies/interests. Further, this association
was still evident after controlling for variables such as time since break-up,
length of former relationship, current frequency of interaction, and gender.
Similar to research on attachment, this study shows that past relationships
may influence future ones (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Specifically, results suggest
that participants who reported high levels on continued inclusion provided a
description of a next likely partner that was similar to their former partner in
terms of physical characteristics, personality traits, and hobbies/interests.
Further, other variables potentially related to inclusion such as length of former
relationship, time since break-up, and frequency of interaction do not diminish
the effect.
One explanation for this is that when the former relationship is lost, elements
of the self that were included are potentially lost as well. As a result, participants
may attempt to reduce distress from breakup by seeking out a partner who can
effectively replace the components of the self that were provided by the former
partner in the past relationship. This in turn would avoid the distress that is
typically associated with losing elements of the self (Page & Stevens, 1996).
It is also possible that the desired similarity between former and next partner
can be more generally attributed to similarity to the self. It is a long standing
274
Individual Differences Research,
2005, 3(4)
finding within social psychology that similarity between self and partner is a
major determinant of attraction (Byrne, 1997). In the present context it is
possible that we are more likely to include similar others in the self and as a
result, would have similar former partner and desire similar subsequent partners.
However, the self-expansion model would argue that since we are motivated to
expand through novelty, it would be counterproductive to include others in the
self who are overly similar.
Although the findings of this study provided significant support for the
hypotheses, there are always some limitations that must be noted. It is important
to note that the population studied in this research was not very diverse and may
li
mit the generalizability of the results. However, the study also had the strength
of a within-subjects design that asked participants to provide detailed
descriptions of their partners.
Aside from replication in other populations, future research in this area should
explore desired similarity to the self compared to inclusion of other in the self
in the context of subsequent partner choice. Also, it would be beneficial to
determine how the similarity of a present partner to a former one may influence
relationship stability.
The present research, however, is an important first step in understanding the
mechanism by which past relationships may influence future ones. Research
such as this will hopefully provide the foundation for future explorations into
why some people establish patterns of involvement with undesirable partners.
References
Agnew, C.R., Van Lange, P.A.M., Rusbult, C.E., & Langston, C.A. (1998).
Cognitive interdependence: Commitment and the mental representation of
close relationships.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
939-
954.
Aron, A. & Aron, E.N. (1996). Self and self-expansion in relationships. In
G.J.O. Fletcher & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close
relationships: A social psychological perspective (pp. 325-244). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Aron, A., Aron, E.N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale
and the structure of interpersonal closeness.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63,
596-612.
Aron, A., Aron, E.N., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as
including other in the self.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
241-153.
Byrne, D. (1997). An overview (and underview) of research and theory within
the attraction paradigm.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14,
417-431.
Frazier, P. A., Byer, A. L., Fischer, A. R., Wright, D. M., & DeBord, K. A.
(1996). Adult attachment style and partner choice: Correlational and
Individual Differences Research,
2005, 3(4)
275
experimental findings. Personal Relationships, 3,
117-136.
Hazan, C. & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment
process.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,
511-524.
Page, J. & Stevens, H. (1996). Relationships between depression, self-esteem,
and self-silencing behavior.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15,
381-396.
Ruvolo, A. P., Fabin, L. A., & Ruvolo, C. M. (2001). Relationship experiences
and change in attachment characteristics of young adults: The role of
relationship breakups and conflict avoidance.
Personal Relationships, 30,
265-281.
... Angażując się w bliski związek uzależniamy swoje działania, decyzje i uczucia od drugiej osoby, przez co możemy utracić poczucie kontroli nad własnym życiem. Może to prowadzić do lęku przed utratą jakiejś części siebie (Lewandowski & Sahner, 2005), oraz wystawia nas na ryzyko emocjonalnego rozczarowania, jeśli związek się rozpadnie (Wojciszke, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Celem przeprowadzonego badania było uzyskanie odpowiedzi na pytania czy istnieje związek między nasileniem Intymności, Namiętności i Zaangażowania w aktualnym związku, a liczbą poprzednich relacji, sposobem ich zakończenia, częstotliwością kontaktu z byłym partnerem oraz zaangażowaniem emocjonalnym w ten kontakt. W badaniu wzięło udział N = 300 osób, jednak analizą objęto N = 250 kobiet będących w narzeczeństwie lub niesformalizowanym związku, które utrzymywały intencjonalny kontakt z poprzednią sympatią. Jako główne narzędzie badawcze posłużył kwestionariusz Pomiar Miłości Bogdana Wojciszke. Spodziewano się, że większa liczba wcześniejszych związków, większe zaangażowanie emocjonalne w nie i częstsze utrzymywanie kontaktu z byłym partnerem będzie obniżało nasilenie Intymności, Namiętności i Zaangażowania. Jednocześnie, że burzliwe zakończenie poprzedniej relacji nasili emocje przeżywane w obecnym związku. Wyniki badań wykazały jedną istotną statystycznie, ujemną korelację pomiędzy zmiennymi Emocjonalne przywiązanie i Intymność. Oznacza to, że wraz ze wzrostem emocjonalnego zaangażowania w relację z byłym partnerem obniża się nasilenie Intymności w aktualnym związku. Pozostałe wyniki okazały się być nieistotne statystycznie i nie wykazały związku pomiędzy wcześniejszymi doświadczeniami a obecną relacją. Słowa kluczowe: Intymność, Namiętność, Zaangażowanie, relacje romantyczne
... By retaining possessions, individuals can more easily recall romantic relationship events or memories, and even ruminate over what was in the past. Keeping strategies may allow for parts of their relationship identity to persist rather than be lost with dissolution of the relationship (Lewandowski & Sahner, 2005). Alternatively, another reason for retention is that individuals might have ended their romantic relationship, yet maintain their relationship on another level, and want to connect that relationship and perception to their shared social or public networks. ...
Article
As relational partners document their romantic relationships in mediated contexts, they must manage virtual relational possessions to determine what should be kept or deleted, particularly following a breakup. Using the Relational Dissolution Model, this study examines keeping and deleting behaviors through the lens of a newly proposed process – relational curation. Collegiate participants (N = 234) who had experienced a breakup were recruited to complete an online survey. Findings showed that when individuals based their possession decisions on their relationship with their former partner, they were more likely to keep possessions; whereas, when individuals based their possession decisions on their relationship with a potential future partner, they were more likely to delete possessions. By further clarifying the role of former and future relationships in keeping and deleting decisions, the results further delineate the dissolution processes of grave-dressing, relational curation, and resurrection.
... For example, romantic partners have a strong impact on a person's self-concept (e.g., Agnew, 2000;Aron, Aron, Tudor, & Nelson, 1991), and individuals in a romantic relationship often find that their selves become intertwined. If that relationship ends, it suggests that individuals may have to adjust their view of themselves as separate from their former partner (Lewandowski & Sahner, 2005). Research has also shown that divorced women tend to report self-concept change in both desirable and undesirable ways after the divorce (Baum, Rahav, & Sharon, 2005), and that after a romantic breakup, individuals change the content of their selves and feel as though their selves are smaller and less clear (Slotter, Gardner, & Finkel, 2010). ...
Article
Romantic relationships are known to be very influential, but less is known about how these relationships, and particularly the breakup of these relationships, may affect individuals’ relational schemas, or their expectations for relationships. Undergraduate students reported on how their views of themselves, romantic partners, and relationships changed after breaking up with a past partner. Results suggest that relational schemas change following relationship dissolution and that there are both positive and negative aspects to this change. There was also some evidence that aspects of the past relationship predicted change and the valence of change, and that change and the valence of change were related to aspects of current relationship quality. These results are an important first step in understanding how past romantic relationships influence people’s expectations about relationships and, by extension, their health and wellbeing.
... Moreover, communication with a former partner may serve the function of maintaining parts of the identity that may be lost with relationship dissolution (Lewandowski & Sahner, 2005). Maintaining social networks and a friendship with a former partner may be particularly adaptive if both partners share a network of friends or colleagues; in such situations, the loss of a romantic relationship may mean the loss of other friendships as well. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research investigates individual and contextual differences underlying postdissolution friendships by considering communication with former romantic partners among individuals in new romantic relationships. Two studies demonstrate the prevalence, determinants, and implications of former partner communication for the current relationship. Study 1 showed that approximately 40% of individuals in relationships communicate with a former partner and highlighted differences between those who communicate and those who do not. Study 2 factor analyzed reasons why people communicate with former partners and examined how the reasons are differentially associated with current relationship functioning. In general, results support the notion that under certain circumstances and for certain individuals, communication with former partners can have deleterious influences on one's current relationship.
... However, as speculated in Study 1, individuals may engage in more self-presentation in more established types of relationships, using self-presentation as a relationship maintenance tool. Well-established interpersonal relationships may have greater importance for the self than less-established types of relationships, as the loss of such a relationship may have severe ramifications for the self (Leary et al., 1994;Lewandowski & Sahner, 2005). Thus, it may be in the interest of the individual to communicate as much as possible about the self to secure the favor of the important other (i.e., romantic partner); indeed, self-disclosure has been associated with increased intimacy and greater satisfaction with the relationship (Sprecher & Hendrick, 2004). ...
Article
Self-presentation represents behaviors used in establishing an identity with others; such behaviors may differ across various interpersonal relationships. The current manuscript presents two studies examining differences in self-presentation to acquaintances, friends, and romantic partners among college students in relationships. Study 1 was an experiment and Study 2 utilized a within-subject design. Results showed that individuals engaged in more self-presentation in more established types of relationships. Additionally, both closeness and trust served as moderators, such that those lower in closeness/trust reported more self-presentation in more established types of relationships than in less established types of relationships. At higher levels of closeness/trust, the results were somewhat inconsistent, with Study 1 finding no differences between relationship types and Study 2 finding more self-presentation to romantic partners than to friends and acquaintances. These results are among the first to suggest that individuals engage in differing levels of self-presentation, depending on the type of relationship and the extent to which they feel close to and trust the person.
... Pilgrim next battles his ex-girlfriend, Envy Adams, and her new lover, Todd Ingram. Lewandowski and Sahner (2005) found that past relationships can influence a current relationship if one's self-concept has been largely influenced by the former partner. Thus this fight symbolizes Pilgrim's need to work through his unresolved feelings for Adams. ...
... Future studies might also examine what happens if individuals retain attributes from a former partner when a new relationship begins. Perhaps the detrimental effects of dissolution on the self-concept explain why individuals who have high self-other integration with a former partner express a desire for their next partner's attributes, personality, and interests to be similar to those of their former partner (Lewandowski & Sahner, 2005). If the next partner shares attributes with the former partner, the self-concept could remain intact by simply transferring the individual's current self-in-relationship representation onto the new partner (e.g., Andersen & Chen, 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
Individuals in ongoing romantic relationships incorporate attributes from their partner into their own self-concepts. However, little research has investigated what happens to these attributes should the relationship end. Across three studies, the present research sought to examine factors that predicted whether individuals retain or reject attributes from their self-concept that they initially gained during a relationship. We predicted that individuals would be more likely to reject attributes from their self post-dissolution if their ex-partner was influential in them adding those attributes to the self in the first place. However, we expected this effect to be moderated such that individuals who exerted greater, versus lesser, effort in maintaining relevant attributes would retain them as part of the self, regardless of whether the attribute originated from the partner. In addition, in two of our three studies, we explored the roles of partner influence, effort, and attribute rejection on individuals' post-dissolution self-concept clarity.
Article
In an effort to illuminate factors delineating when communication with former romantic partners can be beneficial versus detrimental, this work examines how communication affects both the current and former relationships, and whether these associations vary as a function of one’s self-worth being tied to the relationship (i.e., relationship-contingent self-esteem, RCSE). Over three weeks, undergraduates in relationships who regularly communicated with a former partner (N=46) completed nightly measures of former partner communication and satisfaction with current and former relationships. Results indicated that among those higher in RCSE, communication with former partners undermined current relationship satisfaction and bolstered former relationship satisfaction, patterns not evident among those lower in RCSE. For some, communication with former partners can be problematic for the current relationship.
Article
Full-text available
On the basis of an interdependence analysis, it is proposed that commitment to a close relationship is associated with cognitive interdependence—a mental state characterized by a pluralistic, collective representation of the self-in-relationship. A cross-sectional survey study and a 2-wave longitudinal study revealed that strong commitment to a romantic relationship is associated with greater spontaneous plural pronoun usage, greater perceived unity of self and partner, and greater reported relationship centrality. Commitment and cognitive interdependence operate in a cycle of mutual influence, such that earlier commitment predicts change over time in cognitive interdependence, and earlier cognitive interdependence predicts change over time in commitment. Links between commitment and cognitive interdependence were weak or nonsignificant for relationships among best friends, suggesting that this phenomenon may be unique to romantic relationships. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved). (from the journal abstract)
Article
Full-text available
This article explores the possibility that romantic love is an attachment process--a biosocial process by which affectional bonds are formed between adult lovers, just as affectional bonds are formed earlier in life between human infants and their parents. Key components of attachment theory, developed by Bowlby, Ainsworth, and others to explain the development of affectional bonds in infancy, were translated into terms appropriate to adult romantic love. The translation centered on the three major styles of attachment in infancy--secure, avoidant, and anxious/ambivalent--and on the notion that continuity of relationship style is due in part to mental models (Bowlby's "inner working models") of self and social life. These models, and hence a person's attachment style, are seen as determined in part by childhood relationships with parents. Two questionnaire studies indicated that relative prevalence of the three attachment styles is roughly the same in adulthood as in infancy, the three kinds of adults differ predictably in the way they experience romantic love, and attachment style is related in theoretically meaningful ways to mental models of self and social relationships and to relationship experiences with parents. Implications for theories of romantic love are discussed, as are measurement problems and other issues related to future tests of the attachment perspective.
Article
The Silencing the Self Scale (STSS), the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), and the Multidimensional Self-Esteem Inventory (MSEI) were administered to 90 male and 91 female participants in order to determine gender differences on the STSS and to establish the relationship between self-silencing, depression, and self-esteem. Contrary to our hypotheses, men's scores on the STSS were significantly higher than women's scores, and scores on the BDI and the MSEI showed no significant gender differences. Self-silencing was significantly related to depression at low levels of self-esteem but not at average or high levels, suggesting that a high score on the STSS is not necessarily reflective of the self-silencing schema proposed by Dana lack.
Article
The initiation and subsequent development of what I once immodestly labeled `the attraction paradigm' are described. Though an after-the-fact reconstruction of a given program of research and theory may appear to result from planful, rational, insightful, and even prescient actions, the actual process is more often a combination of multiple personal motives, semi-random input from a wide variety of sources, sheer luck, and semi-delusional tenacity. In any event, some highlights and landmarks of over 35 years of attraction research are summarized. The story includes the initial decision to investigate the effect of attitude similarity-dissimilarity on attraction, the gradual development of the linear function that specifies the relationship between seemingly diverse stimulus events and evaluative responses such as attraction, and the construction of a theoretical model that began with a focus on conditioning but was eventually expanded as `the behaviour sequence', incorporating cognitive constructs in order to deal with such interpersonal complexities as love. As a postscript, I describe our current efforts to place the components of adult attachment patterns within this model in an effort to predict more precisely various aspects of interpersonal relationships.
Article
The cognitive significance of being in a close relationship is described in terms of including other in the self (in K. Lewin's [1948] sense of overlapping regions of the life space and in W. James [1890/1948] sense of the self as resources, perspectives, and characteristics). Exp 1 (with 24 college students), adapting W. B. Liebrand's (see record 1985-20117-001) decomposed-game procedures, found less self/other difference in allocations of money to a friend than to a stranger, regardless of whether Ss expected other to know their allocations. Exp 2 (with 20 female undergraduates), adapting C. G. Lord's (see record 1988-00331-001) procedures, found that Ss recalled fewer nouns previously imaged with self or mother than nouns imaged with a nonclose other, suggesting that mother was processed more like self than a stranger. Exp 3 (with 17 married graduate students), adapting self-schema, reaction-time (RT) procedures (e.g., H. Markus; see record 1977-27587-001) found longer latencies when making "me/not me" decisions for traits that were different between self and spouse versus traits that were similar for both, suggesting a self/other confusion with spouse. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
In 2 studies, the Inclusion of Other in the Self (IOS) Scale, a single-item, pictorial measure of closeness, demonstrated alternate-form and test–retest reliability; convergent validity with the Relationship Closeness Inventory (E. Berscheid et al, 1989), the R. J. Sternberg (1988) Intimacy Scale, and other measures; discriminant validity; minimal social desirability correlations; and predictive validity for whether romantic relationships were intact 3 mo later. Also identified and cross-validated were (1) a 2-factor closeness model (Feeling Close and Behaving Close) and (2) longevity–closeness correlations that were small for women vs moderately positive for men. Five supplementary studies showed convergent and construct validity with marital satisfaction and commitment and with a reaction-time (RT)-based cognitive measure of closeness in married couples; and with intimacy and attraction measures in stranger dyads following laboratory closeness-generating tasks. In 3 final studies most Ss interpreted IOS Scale diagrams as depicting interconnectedness. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This study examined whether close relationship experiences predict change in individuals’attachment Characteristics. Partners from 301 dating couples each rated their attachment characteristics and their conflict avoidance. Five months later 184 women and 138 men reported whether their relationships were intact or broken-up, and they rated their attachment characteristics again. Women became less secure after a breakup and more secure after remaining together. There was a similar result for men who initially had insecure attachment characteristics. Additionally, women low in conflict avoidance became more secure over time, and those high in conflict avoidance became less secure. For men whose relationships remained intact, conflict avoidance was not related to change in attachment characteristics. For men whose relationships ended, however, initial conflict avoidance predicted increased attachment security. These results extend previous research on the stability of attachment characteristics by exploring predictors of change.
Article
Three studies were conducted to assess the role of attachment style in partner selection using both correlational and experimental methods. Study 1 (n = 83 couples) assessed correlations between partner ratings on attachment-style dimensions and the relations between own and partner attachment style and relationship satisfaction. In Study 2 (n = 226) and Study 3 (n = 146), participants who varied in terms of attachment style rated the desirability of potential partners who also differed in terms of attachment style. Results of all three studies generally suggested that individuals were most attracted to partners with similar attachment styles. For example, anxious individuals tended to be dating anxious partners in Study 1, and they preferred anxious partners over secure and avoidant partners in Studies 2 and 3 (combined data). Thus, not all individuals preferred secure partners. Second, unlike previous studies that looked primarily at partner correlations, there was no evidence of anxious/avoidant matching. In fact, anxious individuals seemed particularly averse to avoidant partners. Finally, ratings of parental caregiving styles (especially ratings of mothers) were associated with adult attachment dimensions and partner choices. For example, individuals who rated their mothers as more cold and ambivalent were less attracted to secure partners. Clinical and research implications are discussed.
Knowledge structures in close relationships: A social psychological perspective
  • A Aron
  • E N Aron
Aron, A. & Aron, E.N. (1996). Self and self-expansion in relationships. In G.J.O. Fletcher & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close relationships: A social psychological perspective (pp. 325-244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.