ArticlePDF Available

The importance of timing for breaking commuters’ car driving habits

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
Alan Warde & Dale Southerton (eds.) 2012
The Habits of Consumption
Studies across Disciplines in the Humanities and Social Sciences 12.
Helsinki: Helsinki Collegium for Advanced Studies. 130–140.
The Importance of Timing for Breaking
Commuters’ Car Driving Habits1
John Thøgersen
Aarhus University
A large sample of Copenhagen car drivers were randomly assigned to either receive
a free month travel card for public transportation or serve as a control group. As
predicted, the free travel card neutralized the negative effect of car driving habits and
made the use of public transportation more consistent with the traveller’s conscious
intentions. However, the behavioural effects of the free travel card appeared only
among individuals who had recently relocated residence or workplace, prior to the
intervention. This suggests that timing is essential when designing interventions to
promote alternatives to car-driving.
Introduction
The growing number of cars contributes to serious problems all over the World,
including congestion, air pollution and noise at the local level and climate changes
at the global level. In their struggle to solve these problems, governments and
NGOs promote alternative means of transportation, including walking, cycling and
using public transport, but mostly with little success. This paper reports from a
study designed to test an innovative way to assist consumers who want to change
commuting mode from car to public transportation.
Mounting evidence suggests that at least part of the reason why it is so difcult
to get drivers out of their cars and use other travel modes is that the choice of travel
mode tends to become habitual (e.g., Gärling, Boe, & Fujii 2001; Thøgersen & Møller
2008; Verplanken, Aarts, Knippenberg, & Knippenberg 1994; Verplanken, Aarts,
Knippenberg, & Moonen 1998). This has profound inuence on the effectiveness of
interventions that might be considered for inuencing travel-mode choice (Assael
1987; Ronis, Yates, & Kirscht 1989; Aarts, Verplanken, & Knippenberg 1997).
1 A previous version of this paper was published as Thøgersen, J. 2009. Seize the opportunity:
The importance of timing for breaking commuters’ car driving habits. In A. Klein & V. W. Thoresen
(eds.) Making a Difference: Putting Consumer Citizenship into Action. Hedmark: Høgskolen i
Hedm a r k , 35 4 7.
Thøgersen
131
Especially, it means that drivers are unlikely to search for – or even contemplate
– new information before choosing a travel mode (Verplanken, Aarts, & Van
Knippenberg 1997). Hence, in order to be noticed at all, persuasive information
needs to be obtrusive and to be perceived as personally relevant (Dahlstrand &
Biel 1997; Hoyer & MacInnis 2006).
According to psychological theory, individuals form a habit when a behaviour is
repeated frequently in a stable context and leads to rewarding outcomes (Ouellette
& Wood 1998), something which is true for most everyday travel mode choices
(Thøgersen 2006). For example, the daily commute is usually performed frequently
and extensively in stable surroundings, and the car-commuter usually reaches the
destination in a timely and comfortable manner.
Following Verplanken and Aarts (1999), a habit is dened as a learned
sequence of acts that has become an automatic response to specic cues and is
functional in obtaining certain goals or end-states. Frequent repetition in a stable
context facilitates the learning of a habit. The instigation of a new behaviour is
usually volitional and intentional, but subsequent repetitions may eventually be
performed in an unintentional, habitual way (Ouellette & Wood 1998; Verplanken &
Aarts 1999). Before a habit is formed (i.e., learned) several repetitions are usually
required, however. With repetition, each step in the execution of the behaviour
can be done with less effort and less conscious awareness (Ouellette & Wood
1998; Thøgersen & Ölander 2006). Eventually, even the initiation of the behaviour
may become automatic, triggered by the stimulus cues that normally precede it
(Bargh & Barndollar 1996). Because of its obvious advantages, most frequently
repeated behaviours are habitual. Hence, when people are in situations they have
encountered and acted in many times before, their actions tend to be automatic
repetitions of their previously repeated behaviours.
It follows from this that people’s stated intentions are good predictors of their
behaviour only under conditions of weak habits while intentions are a bad predictor
of behaviour when habits are strong (Triandis 1977). Hence, habits moderate the
inuence of behavioural intentions on behaviour. For example, in the eld of travel-
mode choice, Verplanken et al. (1994) found that the correlation between the
attitude towards using a specic travel-mode option and travel-mode choice (for
shopping trips to either of two cities located approximately 5 miles away and where
a realistic public transport option existed) was signicantly weaker for strong than
for weak habit individuals.
As strongly emphasized by Bargh and Barndollar (1996), a habit is not a static
behavioural response, however. A habit is a mental system that interacts with
environmental information and which requires input from the environment in order
to operate. Many established behavioural routines in daily life, such as car use,
The Habits of Consumption
132
have both volitional (e.g., planning to go somewhere) and automatic elements (e.g.,
picking the car, driving). Hence, Bargh and Barndollar (1996) consider the learning
of habits an automated strategy for dealing with the environment to affect desired
goals.
A habit that was functional in obtaining some goal(s) at the time when it was
formed may lose its functionality if the goal(s) change at a later point in time,
however (Verplanken & Aarts 1999). In such cases, the habit may become counter-
intentional. Counter-intentional habits are particularly prevalent when the behaviour
is based on short-term, hedonistic motives at the expense of long-term goals
(Verplanken & Faess 1999).
For example, habitual travel mode choices are often found to deviate from the
person’s expressed intention. In practice, the deviation is usually in the direction of
a higher-than-intended use of private cars and a lower use of public transportation,
bicycling and walking (Møller & Thøgersen 2008; Verplanken et al., 1994;
Verplanken, et al. 1998; Aarts, Verplanken, & Knippenberg 1998). This means that
it is possible to achieve a more desirable modal split (from a societal point of view)
by just helping individual travellers to act according to their expressed intentions.
In general, people reserve deliberation and conscious decision-making for novel
situations and for when new problems arise in old situations, such as situational
barriers (e.g., a freeway closure blocking one’s usual commuting route; cf. Fujii,
Gärling, & Kitamura 2001) or major life changes (e.g., residential relocation; cf.
Bamberg 2006; Verplanken, Walker, Davis, & Jurasek 2008). Hence, the key to
changing habitual behaviour is to create conditions that, for some reason or other,
make the automatic execution of the habit impossible or at least unattractive (Ronis
et al. 1989) and which give individuals sufcient motivation and ability to make a
deliberate choice (Fazio 1990). The challenge is to design interventions that are
effective in producing this outcome, yet politically and individually acceptable.
In this connection, change agents may take advantage of people’s habitual
patterns being more vulnerable to inuence attempts when major changes happen
in their lives (Andreasen 1984). Specically with regard to travel mode choice, it
has been suggested that people’s car-driving habits can more easily be inuenced
when they have recently changed residence (Bamberg 2006).
The approach
The approach proposed here to make car-drivers voluntarily change their everyday
travel mode choices is similar to the way newspapers, telecommunication services,
and other goods and services that are bought on a subscription basis are often
Thøgersen
133
marketed to new customers: by means of a temporary promotion offer, typically
including a trial period for free or at a substantially reduced price. The promotion
is intended to create sufcient initial interest to entice new customers to try the
product or service, and it is hoped that the experience creates a positive attitude and
perhaps a new habit that secures repeated purchase after the trial period. In order
to avoid misuse, it is customary to restrict the promotion offer to people who have
not been subscribers to the product or service for some time. To my knowledge,
there have as yet been only sporadic attempts to promote public transportation this
way, in spite of the obvious similarities between the areas (Thøgersen 2007).
It is an important assumption behind this kind of intervention that at least some of
the receivers of the promotion will continue using the service more than before the
promotion period, even though they have to pay full fare again. In the travel-mode
choice case, there are at least two reasons for expecting such a long-term effect.
One reason is that some car-drivers may hold unjustied negative expectations
about public transport. Hence, trial-based experience resulting from the promotion
period would result in more favourable attitudes towards using public transport
(Bamberg & Schmidt, 1999). Another possible reason is that some car-drivers
have a vague (but not necessarily negative) perception about how it would be to
use public transport. Using their car works for them, so they have not bothered
to seriously consider alternatives. Also, even a relatively small cost in terms of
time and effort needed to investigate and possibly test alternatives seems to be
an insurmountable barrier. Hence, any means that could make them try public
transport would increase the quality of their knowledge of this alternative and some
would realize that for them using public transport is actually preferable to using the
car, at least for some purposes.
Method
We tested the price promotion in a eld experiment with car-drivers in the Greater
Copenhagen area fullling certain screening criteria. The basic idea was to make
car-drivers, many of which were assumed to choose travel mode habitually, “an
offer they could not resist,” and thereby motivate them to deliberate about their
travel mode choices, and in fact try public transport.
Participants
Data were collected by means of telephone interviews carried out in October and
November 2002 and April 2003.2 Subjects were a random sample of car-owners
2 The data were collected by TNS Gallup.
The Habits of Consumption
134
in the Greater Copenhagen area fullling the following screening criteria: have a
driver’s license and a car at their disposal, commute to job or study at least once
a week, and not having been a monthly travel card holder for mass transit in the
Greater Copenhagen area for at least a year. Also, traveling salesmen and others
that are dependent on a private car for their job were excluded. If more than one
person in the household fullled the criteria, the “next birthday” method was used
to pick the participant for the study.
Of those meeting the screening criteria, 1071 agreed to participate in the
rst wave, resulting in a response rate of 75 percent of those qualifying. Thirty
individuals were excluded because letters with experimental treatment material
were returned by post due to incorrect addresses, because they claimed that
they had not received the experimental treatment material, or because of errors
in the administration of experimental treatments during the telephone interview.3
The screening criteria did not take into account that some live so close to their
workplace that they have no need of motorized transport for commuting. As a crude
measure it was judged that everyone who in Wave 1 reported having commuted by
foot more than once or by bicycle more than four times out of the last ten times fell
into this category and they were excluded from the study.
The allocation to experiment and control groups followed a somewhat complex
design: First, participants were randomly assigned to either experimental treatment
(70%) or control group (30%). In the experiment group, subjects were then assigned
to one of several treatments.
Those expressing any intention to use mass transit in the near future were
randomly assigned to one of two treatments: (a) a planning exercise alone or (b) a
planning exercise plus a free month travel card. The planning exercise consisted
in asking subjects to plan their next trip by mass transit (when exactly they would
go, from where to where, using which bus or train connection, see Bamberg 2002).
Those expressing no intention to use mass transit in the near future were
randomly assigned to one of three treatments: (a) a customized timetable alone,
(b) a customized timetable plus a free month travel card, or (c) a free month travel
card alone. The customized timetable treatment consisted in sending subjects a
customized timetable for his or her daily commute based on information about
home and work given during the rst interview. Free month travel cards and
customized timetables were sent to participants by ordinary mail immediately after
the rst interview.
3 The excluded subjects did not differ signicantly (the 5 % level) from included subjects on any of
the target variables in the rst wave.
Thøgersen
135
Neither the planning exercise nor the customized timetable produced an increase
in commuting by public transport over and above the control group (Thøgersen &
Møller, 2008). For this reason, and because the focus here is on the effects of
the price promotion, experimental subjects not receiving a free travel card were
excluded from this study. Hence, the nal sample consisted of 597 car-owners
living in the greater Copenhagen area and being in employment or under education,
who (apparently) did not live too close to work to need motorized transportation for
commuting, serving either as experimental subjects (373, receiving a free month
travel card) or as members of the control group (224).
The gender distribution of the participants was 56/44 percent males/females.
The average age was 43 and the age range 18 to 71. Seventy-ve percent were
living with at least one other adult and 43 percent had children under the age
of 18 in the household. Forty-seven percent had a college or university degree.
Forty-nine percent lived in a house, 49 percent in an apartment and 2 percent in
other types of homes. None of these descriptors differed signicantly between the
experiment group and the control group.
Measures
In all three waves, answers were obtained to questions about travel behaviour and
a number of beliefs and psychological constructs regarding traveling. Participants
were also asked questions about major changes in their lives in the last three
months before lling out the rst questionnaire, including whether they had changed
residence and/or workplace. For the analysis presented in this paper, this latter
information was used to classify participants. Besides this, only the frequency of
using public transportation is used. The wording and scale of this measure are
explained in the note to Figure 1.
Previously reported results
Results of the wider study have previously been reported in several published
papers:
In Møller and Thøgersen (2008), the implications of car use habits for drivers’
use of public transportation is analysed. A relatively low percentage of the drivers
in this study (10-20%) considered commuting by public transportation in the near
future. A hierarchical analysis, where use of public transportation was regressed
onto intentions to do so, car use habit, and the interaction between the two,
conrmed the theory-derived hypothesis that car use habits act as a moderator of
the intention-behaviour relationship for public transportation. In other words, car
The Habits of Consumption
136
use habits are an obstacle to the transformation of intentions to commute by public
transportation into action.
Thøgersen and Møller (2008) extended these results by the eld experiment
where a free month travel card was tested as a tool to persuade drivers to skip the
habitual choice of the car and consider using – and to try – public transport instead.
As predicted, the free month travel card had a signicant impact on drivers’ use
of public transport and it also neutralized the impact of car-driving habits on the
intention-behaviour relationship for public transportation. However, according to
the calculations reported in this article, in the longer run (i.e., four months after the
experiment) experimental subjects did not use public transport more than control
subjects.
In Thøgersen (2009), these data were reanalysed. After excluding participants
that had no need of motorized transportation for commuting, based on their
pattern of walking or bicycling to work or study, a signicant long-term effect of the
free month travel card was revealed. Four months after the free travel card had
expired, those that had received it and actually needed motorized transportation
for commuting still used public transportation signicantly and substantially (40%)
more than at baseline.
In sum, we have shown that:
strong car-driving habits are an obstacle for converting intentions to use
public transportation into action,
a free month travel card can remove this obstacle, and
among drivers with a need for motorized transportation, there is still a
signicant and substantial effect on their use of public transportation four
months later.
This study
In this paper, we study whether the effects of the intervention are contingent on
specic context factors. Specically, we study whether people’s habitual travel-
mode choices are more vulnerable to inuence attempts when major changes
happen in their lives (Andreasen 1984). Major life changes that may have
implications for travel mode choice include change of residence and change of
workplace (Bamberg 2006; Verplanken, et al. 2008). Hence, I test the following
hypothesis:
Hypothesis: The effect of receiving a free month travel card (i.e., the experimental
treatment) on the use of public transportation for commuting is stronger for people
Thøgersen
137
who have recently changed residence or workplace than for who people who have
not.
Operationally, “recently” is dened as the last three months before the
intervention. Since relocation is assumed to inuence people’s receptivity to
inuence attempts, the effect should show up in the near term. Hence, the
hypothesis is tested by comparing the pattern of travel mode choice reported at
baseline (i.e., before the intervention) and at the second interview (i.e., during the
intervention).
Results
I use a 2 (free card vs. control group) x 2 (relocation or not) x 2 (Wave 1 vs. Wave
2) mixed between and within subjects design to analyse the impacts of the free
travel card and relocation on participants’ use of public transportation. The means
are reported in Figure 1.
According to the GLM analysis, there was no direct effect of the time of the
interview (p = .28). However, there was a signicant two-way interaction between
experimental condition (free card vs. control) and time (p = .001) and a signicant
three-way interaction between experimental condition (free card vs. control),
relocation and time (p = .02). The two-way interaction was produced by the use of
public transport increasing more in the experiment group (0.5 trips out of 10) than
in the control group (-0.05 trips out of 10), as expected. The three-way interaction
was the outcome of this difference in the increase in the use of public transportation
between the experiment group and the control group being especially pronounced
among those that had changed residence or workplace in the last three months
(difference between experimental groups: 1.34 trips out of 10, p < .001), whereas
the difference in the change over time between experimental groups was small
among those that were staying put (0.27 trips out of 10, n.s.). This is consistent
with the hypothesis.
Figure 1. Use of public transport by experimental treatment and relocation of residence or
workplace at Time 1 (baseline) and Time 2 (intervention period). Means.
Note. Behaviour frequency was measured on a scale from 0 to 10 with the item: How many of
the last 10 times did you use public transport for the trip between home and work/educational
institution?” N = 536.
The Habits of Consumption
138
Discussion
Previous studies have shown that some drivers would actually like to use public
transportation more, but do not, mainly because of old habits. A price promotion
in the form of a free month travel card was offered to a random sample of
Copenhagen car drivers. The results were encouraging. The price promotion led
to a doubling of the use of public transportation in the experiment group and a
signicant effect remained four months after the intervention (Thøgersen 2009). As
reported in Thøgersen and Møller (2008), the success of the price promotion was
partly due to the fact that it neutralized the negative inuence of car-driving habits
on transforming intentions to use public transportation into behaviour.
However, the results reported in this paper show that the effect of the free
travel card was limited to an identiable sub-set of car-drivers: people who had
recently (i.e., within the last three months) changed either residence or workplace.
Among those that had not experienced a change in any of these locations recently,
the behavioural impact of the free travel card was not signicant. This result is
consistent with previous research suggesting that individuals’ habitual behaviours
are more easily inuenced under circumstances where they experience major life
changes.
Commuters who have not recently experienced a relocation of residence
or workplace make their commuting-mode choices in a situation they have
encountered and acted in many times before. Hence, their choices tend to be
automatic repetitions of their previously repeated choices. They lack the motivation
to consider or even pay attention to alternative options. Even a free month travel
card is not sufcient to change that. However, the relocation of residence or
workplace is usually a sufciently big life change to motivate people to deliberate
and consciously consider their commuting options. Hence, car drivers are more
likely to develop an intention to try public transportation for commuting in connection
with changes of residence or workplace than under stable conditions. Hence, it is
under these circumstances that a discrepancy between intentions and behaviour
are most likely to appear. Further, our study shows that drivers who in this situation
get the chance to try public transport for free for a month are more likely to increase
their use of public transport. A nding that we did not predict is that people who do
not receive a free month pass when they change residence or workplace tend to
decrease their already low use of public transport.
Results that were not predicted should be interpreted with care until replicated in
other studies. However, one may speculate that during relocation, people often feel
that the uncertainties of the new situation are uncomfortable. One way of coping
with this uncertainty would be to stick to general habits that are exible enough to
Thøgersen
139
accommodate the new situation. It has been suggested that the habit of using the
car for commuting is one such exible habit (cf., Verplanken, et al. 1994).
The results presented in this paper show that the cost effectiveness of a price
promotion in the form of a free month travel card can be improved by targeting
it to consumer segments whose lives are undergoing changes that make them
more open to reconsider their travel options. Hence, timing is essential when
designing interventions to promote alternatives to car driving. This insight adds
further renements to our knowledge about price promotions as a method to make
habitual car drivers consider and try public transportation.
After a successful intervention that moves drivers from a habitual to a deliberate
mode of decision-making and try, for example, public transportation, a long-term
change in the way they commute may result. However, it is an obvious prerequisite
that the commuter is satised with the tested alternative to the private car. Hence,
interventions to break car-driving habits, such as the one discussed in this paper,
make little sense unless there are indeed acceptable alternative transport options
available.
References
Aarts, H. & B. Verplanken 1999. Habit, attitude, and planned behaviour: Is habit an empty
construct or an interesting case of goal-directed automaticity? European Review of
Social Psychology 10, 101–134.
Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A., van Knippenberg, C & B. Verplanken 1994. Attitude versus
general habit: Antecedents of travel mode choice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology
24, 285–300.
Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A., Moonen, A. & B. Verplanken 1998. Habit versus planned
behavior: A eld experiment. British Journal of Social Psychology 37, 111–128.
Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A. & B. Verplanken 1997. Habit, information acquisition, and the
process of making travel mode choices. European Journal of Social Psychology 27,
539–560.
Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A. & B. Verplanken 1997. Habit and information use in travel mode
choices. Acta Psychologica 96, 1–14.
Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A. & B. Verplanken 1998. Predicting behavior from actions in
the past: Repeated decision making or a matter of habit. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology 28, 1355–1374.
Andreasen, A. R. 1984. Life status changes and changes in consumer preferences and
satisfaction. Journal of Consumer Research 11, 784 –7 9 4.
Assael, H. 1987. Consumer behavior and marketing action. Boston: Kent Publishing Co.
Bamberg, S. 2002. Effects of implementation intentions on the actual performance of new
environmentally friendly behaviours. Results of two eld experiments. Journal of
Environmental Psychology 22, 399 411.
The Habits of Consumption
140
Bamberg, S. 2006. Is a residential relocation a good opportunity to change people’s travel
behavior? Results from a theory-driven intervention study. Environment & Behavior
38(6), 820840.
Bamberg, S., & P. Schmidt 1999. Regulating transpor t: Behavioral changes in the eld. Journal
of Consumer Policy 22, 479–509.
Bargh, J. A., & K. Barndollar 1996. Automaticity in action: The unconscious as repository of
chronic goals and motives. In J. A. Bargh & P. Golwitzer (eds.) Psychology of action.
New York: Guilford, 457–481.
Biel, A. & U. Dahlstrand 1997. Pro-environmental habits: Propensity levels in behavioral
change. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 27, 588–602.
Davis, A., Jurasek, M., Verplanken, B. & I. Walker 2008. Context change and travel mode
choice: Combining the habit discontinuity and self-activation hypotheses. Journal of
Environmental Psychology 28(2), 121–127.
Faess, S. & B. Verplanken 1999. Good intentions, bad habits, and effects of forming
implementation intentions on healthy eating. European Journal of Social Psychology
29, 591–604.
Fazio, R. H. 1990. Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model
as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (ed.) Advances in experimental social
psychology. Vol. 23. New York: Academic, 75–109.
Fujii, S., Gärling, T., & R. Kitamura 2001. Change in drivers’ perceptions and use of public
transport during a freeway closure: Effects of temporary structural change on cooperation
in a real-life social dilemma. Environment and Behavior 33(6), 796 808.
Boe, O., Fujii, S. & T. Gärling 2001. Empirical tests of a model of determinants of script based
driving choice. Transportation Research Part F: Trafc Psychology and Behaviour 4,
89 –102.
Hoyer, W. D., & D. J. MacInnis 2006. Consumer behavior. 4th edition. Boston: Houghton Mifin.
Møller, B. & J. Thøgersen 2008. Breaking car-use habits: The effectiveness of a free month
travel card. Transportation 35, 329–345.
Møller, B. & J. Thøgersen 2008. Car-use habits: An obstacle to the use of public transportation?
In C. Jensen-Butler, B. Madsen, O. A. Nielsen & B. Sloth (eds.) Road pricing, the
economy, and the environment. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Ouellette, J. A., & W. Wood 1998. Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes
by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin 124, 54–74.
Kirscht, J. P., Ronis, D. L. & J. F. Yates 1989. Attitudes, decisions, and habits as determinants
of repeated behavior. In A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler & A. G. Greenwald (eds.) Attitude
structure and function. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 213–239.
Thøgersen, J. 2006. Understanding repetitive travel mode choices in a stable context: A panel
study approach. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 40, 621–638.
Thøgersen, J. 2007. Social marketing of alternative transportation modes. In T. Gärling & L.
Steg (eds.) Threats to the Quality of Urban Life from Car Trafc: Problems, Causes, and
Solutions. Oxford: Elsevier, 367–381.
Thøgersen, J. 2009. Promoting public transport as a subscription service: Effects of a free
month travel card. Transport Policy 16, 335–343.
Thøgersen, J., & F. Ölander 2006. The dynamic interaction of personal norms and environment-
friendly buying behavior: A panel study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 36, 1758 –
1780.
Triandis, H. C. (1977). Interpersonal behavior. Monterey: Books/Cole.
... As the world continues to deplete natural resources as well as add pollution, it is becoming increasingly important for consumers to understand the importance of sustainable consumption [2] and how they, as individuals, can partake in the same. When considering the individual's behavioral changes toward sustainable consumption practices, research indicates that life stages and events such as pregnancy [7][8][9] and relocation [10,11] can have a positive influence towards sustainable consumption. ...
... Verplanken and Wood [25] found that it is hard to change everyday consumption habits that are unsustainable, but other studies have found that when big events happen, like pregnancy [7][8][9], life events [7], and relocation [11,26], individuals are more likely to be open to the idea of sustainable consumption. Research has also found that the relationship between knowledge, attitude, and behavior is weak [26], making it hard for someone to change their consumption habits. ...
... During the interviews, it was observed that when the participants were relocated to metropolitan areas, their willingness to obtain sustainable products increased, due to the ease of access to those products. This supports the literature [10,11] that relocation is a key driver in sustainable consumption. The data interpretation indicated that entering into a new relationship [79] and/or having a child, had a positive influence towards sustainable consumption behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research sought to explore the influences and motives that lead to decision-making of, and behavioral changes toward, sustainable consumption in heterosexual married couples. Understanding how different factors affect couples’ consumption decision-making dynamics can help provide successful methods to promote behavior towards sustainable consumption. For this study, semi-structured interviews were employed to explore couples’ sustainable consumption behaviors. A total of six heterosexual couples (12 individual participants) provided in-depth narratives regarding their sustainable consumption and behavioral patterns. Four major themes that emerged include: (1) the meaning of sustainable consumption for the couples and their lack of confidence in their knowledge, (2) shopping decisions regarding sustainable consumption, (3) life events that influenced the couples’ views and behaviors towards sustainable consumption, and (4) the motivations behind sustainable consumption. In spite of the small sample size, the findings of the study can be used by businesses to develop more effective marketing strategies. The study results can help businesses understand heterosexual married couples’ sustainable consumption decision-making processes and persuade them to make decisions about the company’s sustainable product offerings.
... However, sometimes situational barriers make the automatic execution of an ingrained habit impossible (Fujii, Gärling, & Kitamura, 2001), or major life changes (e.g., moving home or workplace, starting a family) disrupt the automatic execution of habits, necessitating some extent of conscious decision making (Verplanken, Walker, Davis, & Jurasek, 2008). The effectiveness of interventions to change habits can be radically improved if timed with such a life change or major disruption (Thøgersen, 2012). When a proenvironmental habit has been formed, it creates an equally strong barrier that helps shield the person from temptations to defect for personal gain (Thøgersen, Jørgensen, & Sandager, 2012). ...
... However, when this environmental stability is disrupted, established travel habits may be reconsidered. Verplanken and Wood termed this phenomenon "habit discontinuity," where contextual changes create opportunities for new habit formation [21,39,40]. For example, residential relocation increased public transport use [41], while parking shortages during the 2002 Olympics boosted walking and cycling [42]. ...
Article
Full-text available
While extensive research has examined how major life events affect travel habits, less attention has been paid to the impact of minor environmental changes on commuting behavior, particularly regarding shared autonomous vehicles (SAVs). This study investigated how daily disruptions and incremental environmental changes influence commuter behavior patterns and SAV adoption in Shanghai, applying the theory of interpersonal behavior framework. The study surveyed 517 Shanghai residents, examining travel satisfaction, commuting habits, psychological factors (such as habit strength and satisfaction), and attitudes towards SAVs. Structural equation modeling was employed to test hypotheses about psychological factors influencing SAV adoption, while logistic regression analyzed how these factors affected mode choice across different disruption contexts. Analysis revealed that psychological factors, particularly habit and satisfaction, were stronger predictors of SAV adoption than attitude-based factors. Route obstructions and workplace relocations significantly increased SAV consideration. Even minor, recurring disruptions, such as construction zones, showed strong effects on commuting behavior, supporting the habit discontinuity hypothesis and emphasizing the importance of minor disruptions in driving behavioral change. The study extends the theory of interpersonal behavior by integrating habit discontinuity theory to explain how minor disruptions drive SAV adoption. This research provides actionable insights for urban planners and policymakers, recommending that SAV trials and targeted interventions be implemented during infrastructure changes or other commuting disruptions to promote SAV adoption and foster more sustainable transportation systems.
... In addition, research suggests that behavioral interventions might be more effective when applied during MoCs (Verplanken and Whitmarsh, 2021). For example, transport interventions have been found to be more effective when participants have recently moved house and/or job (Thøgersen, 2012;Ralph and Brown, 2019), while Maréchal (2010) found that residents who had recently moved house were more likely to apply for energy subsidies. Considering water consumption and MoCs, one study found the average length of showers decreased after the disruption of the Covid pandemic (Swaffield et al., 2023), while another reported that participants who had moved house more recently were more likely to maintain new water-saving habits-suggesting that contextual change enables habit change (Dean et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Water saving behavior is of substantial importance in climate change mitigation and resilience, including reducing time spent in the shower. However, water use is, for many, a strong habit, and, as such, incorporating new water saving behaviors into one's domestic routines may be unsuccessful. In this study, we consider the extent to which a composite behavior change intervention (of water-saving information, implementation intention formation, and monitoring using a shower timer) is effective in reducing the domestic water consumption of new university students who have recently moved into university accommodation. We focus on aspects of the habit discontinuity hypothesis, namely that a natural moment of change facilitates behavior change by weaking existing habits. The intervention was found to be effective, increasing the frequency of self-reported water-saving behavior over behavior measured in a control group. However, shower times, and water usage (measured at the residential level), were not affected by the intervention, and strength of existing habits, readiness to change water behavior, and recency of starting university were each not significantly associated with the effectiveness of the intervention. However, all participants (irrespective of intervention) increased water-saving behavior and reduced shower time during the study, with residential water usage being less for residences with more participating students. Contrary to expectations, the timing of the intervention did not show a clear effect upon the efficacy of the intervention. We discuss these findings with respect to moments of change and habit discontinuity theory as well as implications for practical behavior change interventions.
... In recent years, research on susceptibility to changes in transport behavior has emerged in a new trend focusing on important events in the life of residents that determine changes in their behavior [34]. They can be: a neighborhood [1], a change of the workplace [50] or a change in the phase of the family lifecycle, e.g. related to the birth of a child [5]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The concept of influencing changes in transport behavior towards sustainable mobility, which is gaining popularity in the 21st century, is free public transport (FFPT). It is estimated that the number of cities in which attempts were made to introduce FFPT exceeds 100. Most of them are located in Europe, especially in France and Poland. FFPT has mostly been restricted to specific city areas or market segments in the hope of increasing demand for public transport services. Because of this, a number of publications on free fare results refer to specific cases in cities. The main aim of this article is to examine the impact of free fares on the behavior and transport preferences of pupils in Gdynia, Poland. On the basis of the study of preferences and transport behavior of the inhabitants of Gdynia, carried out earlier by the team in 2010, 2012, 2015, and 2018, a preliminary description of the behavior and transport preferences of students was prepared. The research of the pupils was conducted twice: before and after the introduction of free travel entitlements. The results of the research carried out, and the data analysis, confirmed that FFPT had no impact on demand for public transport services or the travel behavior of pupils. According to the authors, the lack of positive effects of FFPT on travel behavior in the segment of students, or even more broadly, for achieving the purposes of sustainable mobility, results from the interaction of the following factors: specificity of students' travel behavior determined by the schedule of school activities, pupils' positive attitude to cars as urban transport means, not covering all means of public transport services of FFPT in Gdynia (the city rail is not covered by FFPT), short period of time since FFPT has been introduced. The results of the presented studies could not be verified due to the COVID-19 pandemic. The authors emphasize that before introducing FFPT, politicians should rely on the analysis of anticipated changes in the behavior of residents and the impact of FFPT on the economy of public transport, sustainable mobility goals and political and social results. This article complements the current knowledge on the results related to the introduction of FFPT for a selected group of residents.
... From transportation planners' perspective, the influence of past habitual behaviors could be one of the barriers to the use of public transit or non-motorized travel since people are reluctant to change their past travel habits. Verplanken et al. [23] proposed that a disruption, such as residential relocation [57], which removes the cues automatically triggering behaviors, obliges people to revert to deliberate decision-making. Furthermore, Bamberg [58] suggested that residential relocations can be considered a "window of opportunity" for behavioral change, offering new transport opportunities. ...
Article
Full-text available
Travel behavior researchers have dominantly explored the influence of increase in development densities with mixed pattern of land-uses, and investment in infrastructures related to public transit toward more sustainable-transportation policies. However, little has been known about the long-term interdependencies between people’s decisions on travel behavior and individual biographies relating to residential relocation and habitual behavior over a longer time period. To fill this gap, the present study aims to investigate the long-lasting impact of past travel behavior on current travel behavior after a process of residential relocation. For this purpose, aggregate analysis at a neighborhood level was carried out, focusing on cause-effect relationships between current travel mode share and the size of in-migrants dependent on a certain mode in the past by using Household Travel Surveys (HTS) and Internal Migration Statistics collected during 2006–2015 (10 years). Accordingly, the size of in-migrants who have their pre-determined travel behavior in the past play an important role in explaining the mode share of a neighborhood on the current state. Further, this study attempted to divide the influence of residential relocation from the influence of habitual behavior on travel behavior after residential relocation. The finding suggested that the habitualized travel pattern can affect the travel patterns in the new neighborhood even after separating the effect of self-selection. Specifically, the past dependency on public transportation represented significant influences on the current travel mode share. This study on travel behavior informs consideration of role of habitual qualities during the process of residential relocation.
Chapter
Full-text available
Passenger transport is an important contributor to unsustainable urban systems. To achieve the necessary socio-ecological transition will require overcoming the entrenched system of automobility. Composed of several mutually reinforcing components, this system has conferred psychosocial dimensions to car ownership and use that leads to important institutional, political, and individual resistance to change both car-centric transportation infrastructure and individual travel behaviour. For this reason, a growing consensus suggests that transitioning to a sustainable mobility system requires a more holistic approach that applies a synergistic integration of “hard” supply-side measures and “soft” demand-side solutions. This means increasing non-automobile accessibility and supporting such change with soft travel behaviour change solutions that target social-psychological barriers to change. While such approaches have demonstrated their effectiveness around the world, this second category of interventions remains underutilized, particularly in North America. Drawing from social psychology and a North American case study, this chapter proposes a theory-to-practice guide for practitioners to designing effective voluntary travel behaviour change interventions based on the Stage Model of Self-Regulated Behaviour Change (SSBC). A four-level integration framework for intervention design based on the SSBC is proposed. The framework proposes intervention approaches from using the model as a simple diagnostic tool to a complete integration to deliver a fully individualized and stage-tailored intervention. Stage-specific messages and strategies are described to shift people away from car use towards active, collective, and shared mobility options. The chapter concludes on suggestions for collaborative efforts between researchers and practitioners to design, evaluate, and enhance the effectiveness of these interventions, thus moving beyond infrastructure-only solutions to foster a successful transition to sustainable mobility in Québec.
Thesis
Full-text available
This dissertation delves into the urgent need for a shift towards less resource-intensive consumption patterns in light of the escalating climate crisis. Recognizing the substantial role that household consumption plays in global emissions, particularly resource-intensive practices related to food, mobility, and housing, this dissertation investigates how relations between food, mobility, and housing practices create implications for change towards less resource-intensive consumption in everyday life. Grounded in a longitudinal qualitative study of 20 households made up of 31 young adults, this dissertation draws on theories of practice. The inquiry is unpacked through four research articles and each article addresses a sub-question related to how food, mobility, and housing relate and interrelate in everyday life. Article 1 delves into the dynamic relations between routinization and reflexivity in changing and reproducing consumption practices. Identifying three analytical tools for exploring relations between routinization and reflexivity, the analysis goes on to compare food, mobility, and housing across all three themes to explore tendencies for reproduction and potentials for change towards less resource-intensive consumption. Article 2 examines the flexibility of household practices related to food, mobility, and housing during the 2022 energy crisis. The analysis suggests that social and material conditions influence the flexibility for less resource-intensive practice performances, offering insights into the dynamics of stability and change within everyday practices under circumstances of disruption. Article 3 explores how food, mobility, and housing practices interrelate and how these interrelations create path dependencies towards more resource-intensive consumption. The analysis finds that food, mobility, and housing become interwoven and embedded in interrelations characterized by conveniencisation, creating pathways towards more resource-intensive consumption. Article 4 assesses how connections between practices related to food and mobility change during a transition to new housing and the implications this has for change towards less resource-intensive consumption. In summary, this dissertation advances our understanding of sustainable consumption by highlighting the complexities of changing related and interrelated consumption practices. It challenges conventional approaches to reducing household emissions, offering a comprehensive framework for fostering less resource-intensive practices through the relations between daily practices.
Article
Full-text available
Past behavior guides future responses through 2 processes. Well-practiced behaviors in constant contexts recur because the processing that initiates and controls their performance becomes automatic. Frequency of past behavior then reflects habit strength and has a direct effect on future performance. Alternately, when behaviors are not well learned or when they are performed in unstable or difficult contexts, conscious decision making is likely to be necessary to initiate and carry out the behavior. Under these conditions, past behavior (along with attitudes and subjective norms) may contribute to intentions, and behavior is guided by intentions. These relations between past behavior and future behavior are substantiated in a meta-analytic synthesis of prior research on behavior prediction and in a primary research investigation.
Article
Full-text available
This article presents an experimental, theory-driven evaluation of the effectiveness of an intervention that combines a free public transportation ticket and personal schedule information on the subsequent use of public transportation in an urban area. The time point when participants received this intervention is unusual. It was delivered to them shortly after a residential relocation. It is assumed that such a situation increases people’s responsiveness to the intervention. At their new living place, the intervention group shows a strong increase in public transportation use. The intervention effect on the individual choice process is modeled via Ajzen’s theory of planned behavior. Besides a main effect on intention, results indicate interactions between the intervention and the change intention existing prior to the move and higher objective public transport service quality after the move.
Article
Full-text available
Habit is often treated as a construct of marginal interest in the literature on attitude–behaviour relations. We argue that this is undeserved, particularly given the current interest in principles of automaticity in social psychology. Basic features of habits, such as goal-directed automaticity, their dependency on situational constancy, and functionality, as well as the measurement of habit strength, are discussed. Research is reviewed that contrasted habit with deliberate action, as approached from the theory of planned behaviour, spontaneous vs. deliberate attitude–behaviour processes, implementation intention theory, and decision-making models. Habits thus appear as boundary conditions of the validity of models of planned behaviour and rational decision-making. A habit seems to be accompanied by an enduring cognitive orientation, which we refer to as “habitual mind-set”, that makes an individual less attentive to new information and courses of action, and thus contributes to the maintenance of habitual behaviour. Focusing on habitual mind-sets and automatic cue-response links, rather than on statistical associations between past and future behaviour, makes habit an interesting construct for future research.
Article
Full-text available
A field experiment with 102 undergraduate students demonstrated that forming implementation intentions was effective in changing complex everyday behavior, in this case establishing a healthier diet. Ss were administered a questionnaire that assessed their current eating habits. The Ss in the experimental condition were then asked to form implementation intentions, i.e., they were asked to pick out a day from the 5 days following the completion of the questionnaire during which they would eat healthily. All Ss were then asked to keep a diary for the 5 days following the completion of the questionnaire detailing their eating patterns. Results show that the effect of implementation intentions was additive to the prediction of healthy eating by behavioral intentions to eat healthily. Implementation intentions were pitted against individual differences in counterintentional (unhealthy) habits. The effects of implementation intentions and counterintentional habits were independent, suggesting that implementation intentions did not break the negative influence of unhealthy habits, and yet managed to make those with unhealthy habits eat healthier in habit-unrelated respects. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Researchers and practitioners working with travel demand management (TDM) seem to be increasingly interested in social marketing as a means for promoting non-car modes of transportation. However, as is true for social marketing in general, there is little clarity of the social marketing approach associated with TDM. Hence, it is not surprising that the effectiveness of this means is subject to considerable uncertainty. This chapter outlines the field and the definitions of social marketing as well as reviews the practical experience and the research on social marketing in TDM as a basis for evaluating the usefulness of social marketing in this area.
Article
This paper summarizes research on determinants of repeated behaviors, and the deci- sion processes underlying them. The present research focuses on travel mode choices as an example ofsuch behaviors. It is proposed that when behavior is performed repeatedly and becomes habitual, it is guided by automated cognitive processes, rather than being preceded by elaborate decision processes (i.e,, a decision based on attitudes and inten- tions). First, current attitude-behavior models are discussed, and the role of habit in these models is examined. Second, research is presented on the decision processes pre- ceding travel mode choices. Based on the present theoretical and empirical overview, it is concluded that frequently performed behavior is often a matter of habit, thereby es- tablishing a boundary condition for the applicability of attitude-behavior models. How- ever, more systematic research is required to disentangle the role of habit in attitude-behavior models and to learn more about the cognitive processes underlying habitual behavior.
Article
A model of travel mode choice is tested by means of a survey among 199 inhabitants of a village. Car choice behavior for a particular journey is predicted from the attitude toward choosing the car and the attitude toward choosing an alternative mode (i.e., train), on the one hand, and from general car habit, on the other hand. Unlike traditional measures of habit, a script-based measure was used. General habit was measured by travel mode choices in response to very global descriptions of imaginary journeys. In the model, habit is predicted from the degree of involvement with the decision-making about travel mode choice for the particular journey (decisional involvement) and from the degree of competition in a household with respect to car use. The model proves satisfactory. Moreover, as suggested by Triandis (1977), there is a tradeoff between attitude and habit in the prediction of behavior: When habit is strong the attitude-behavior relation is weak, whereas when habit is weak, the attitude-behavior link is strong.
Article
The chapter presents the two very different basic processes that link attitudes and behavior, along with variants that amount to a mixture of the essentials of each process. Conditions that promote one process or the other also are discussed in the chapter. This discussion of mixed models illustrates the complexity of the role of spontaneous and deliberative processing to understand the manner in which attitudes influence behavior. The basic difference between the two types of models of the attitude-behavior process centers on the extent to which deciding on a particular course of action involves conscious deliberation about a spontaneous reaction to one's perception of the immediate situation. An individual may analyze the costs and benefits of a particular behavior and, in so doing, deliberately reflect on the attitudes relevant to the behavioral decision. These attitudes may serve as one of possibly many dimensions that are considered in arriving at a behavior plan, which may then be enacted.
Article
A deeper understanding of the attitude–norm–behavior relationship in the environmental field can be obtained by analyzing the dynamic interaction over time between relevant attitudinal variables and specific behaviors of interest. This article is based on a panel survey with a random sample of about 1,500 Danes interviewed up to 3 times in 1998 to 2000, regarding their purchase of organic food products. The panel analysis reveals that the stronger are consumers' personal norms about buying organic food products and the less they perceive organic products as expensive, the greater the likelihood that they change their purchase patterns in favor of organic products. Furthermore, one can observe significant cross-lagged paths from past behavior to belief and norm variables.