ArticlePDF Available

The Punishment Gap: School Suspension and Racial Disparities in Achievement

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

While scholars have studied the racial “achievement gap” for several decades, the mechanisms that produce this gap remain unclear. In this article, we propose that school discipline is a crucial, but under-examined, factor in achievement differences by race. Using a large hierarchical and longitudinal data set comprised of student and school records, we examine the impact of student suspension rates on racial differences in reading and math achievement. This analysis—the first of its kind—reveals that school suspensions account for approximately one-fifth of black-white differences in school performance. The findings suggest that exclusionary school punishment hinders academic growth and contributes to racial disparities in achievement. We conclude by discussing the implications for racial inequality in education. Mientras que los eruditos han estudiado la "brecha racial educativa" desde hace varias décadas, los mecanismos que producen este vacío no están claros. En este trabajo, proponemos que la disciplina escolar es muy importante, pero poco examinada en el factor en las diferencias de rendimiento según la raza. Utilizando un conjunto de datos de jerárquica y datos longitudinales compuestos por registros escolares de estudiantes, examinamos el impacto de los tipos de suspensión de los estudiantes y las diferencias raciales en la lectura y el rendimiento en matemáticas. Este análisis es primero de su tipo y revela que las suspensiones escolares representan aproximadamente una quinta parte de las diferencias entre el rendimiento escolar entre negros-blancos. Los hallazgos sugieren que el castigo de la escuela de exclusión dificulta el crecimiento académico y contribuye a las disparidades raciales en el rendimiento académico. Concluimos con una discusión de las implicaciones para la desigualdad racial en la educación.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Punishment Gap: School Suspension
and Racial Disparities in Achievement
Edward W. Morris
1
and Brea L. Perry
2
1
University of Kentucky and
2
Indiana University
ABSTRACT
While scholars have studied the racial “achievement gap” for several decades, the mecha-
nisms that produce this gap remain unclear. In this article, we propose that school discipline
is a crucial, but under-examined, factor in achievement differences by race. Using a large hi-
erarchical and longitudinal data set comprised of student and school records, we examine
the impact of student suspension rates on racial differences in reading and math achieve-
ment. This analysis—the first of its kind—reveals that school suspensions account for ap-
proximately one-fifth of black-white differences in school performance. The findings suggest
that exclusionary school punishment hinders academic growth and contributes to racial dis-
parities in achievement. We conclude by discussing the implications for racial inequa lity in
education.
KEYWORDS: achievement gap; school discipline; racial disparity; punishment; at-risk
students.
Racial disparities in educational achievement are one of the most important sources of American in-
equality. Racial inequalities in adulthood in areas as diverse as employment, incarceration, and health
can be traced to unequal academic outcomes in childhood and adolescence (Belfield and Levin
2007). While the racial “achievement gap” has been consistently documented over several decades,
scholars are still working to understand the mechanisms that produce this gap (Jencks and Phillips
1998; Magnuson and Waldfogel 2008). In this article, we propose that school discipline is a crucial,
but under-examined, factor in achievement differences by race. Though large racial disparities in disci-
pline exist, this pattern has never been empirically examined as an explanation of racial gaps in school
performance. This article presents evidence that exclusionary school punishment may hinder aca-
demic growth and contribute to racial inequalities in achievement.
Using detailed data from school records and controlling for a host of school and non-school fac-
tors, we confirm that minority students are more likely to be suspended from school. Moreover, using
variance decomposition methods that isolate within-student trajectories, we show that suspension is
associated with significantly lower achievement growth across time. Finally, we conduct the first com-
prehensive analysis of suspension as an explanation for the racial gap in achievement. This analysis re-
veals that school suspensions account for approximately one-fifth of black-white differences in school
This research was supported by a grant from the Spencer Foundation. The authors wish to thank Rebecca DiLoretto and the
Children’s Law Center for their contributions to this project and for their commitment to equity and justice for all children in public
education. Direct correspondence to: Edward W. Morris, 1515 Patterson Office Tower, Department of Sociology, University of
Kentucky, 40506. E-mail: edward.morris@uky.edu.
V
C
The Author 2016. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society for the Study of Social Problems. All rights reserved.
For permissions, please e-mai l: journals.permissions@oup.com
68
Social Problems, 2016, 63, 68–86
doi: 10.1093/socpro/spv026
Advance Access Publication Date: 8 January 2016
Article
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
performance, demonstrating that exclusionary discipline may be a key driver of the racial achievement
gap. We suggest that the escalation of exclusionary discipline in schools can result in severe academic
consequences for minority students.
THE ACHIEVEMENT GAP
Racial differences in achievement between white and African American children have long been a
concern for researchers and policy makers. Today, this issue continues to present a complex and vex-
ing social problem. Data from the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) reveals that
although gaps in reading and mathematics achievement between black students and white students
have narrowed in the past 40 years, they remain significantly different (Hedges and Nowell 1999;
Jencks and Phillips 1998; Magnuson and Waldfogel 2008). In 2013, for example, African American
students, on average, scored 31 points below white students in eighth-grade math and 26 points be-
low in eighth-grade reading (NCES 2014).
1
Historically, black students made steady gains in closing
the gap after school desegregation in the 1960s; however, this progress leveled off in 1990. The gap
has fluctuated slightly since then, but has ultimately changed little over the past two decades. For
twelfth grade students, in fact, the gap in NAEP reading is wider now than it was in 1992 (NCES
2014).
Scholars have offered an array of explanations for these differences in academic performance.
Racial gaps in school readiness exist when children enter school, which suggests that inequalities out-
side of schools play an important role (Downey, von Hippel, and Broh 2004). Studies in this vein fo-
cus on family and neighborhood effects ranging from economic inequality (Berends, Lucas, and
Penaloza 2008; Magnuson and Waldfogel 2008) to non-cognitive skills (Grissmer and Eiseman
2008) to parental incarceration (Wildeman 2009). Such effects may be compounded when concen-
trated in specific schools and neighborhoods (Condron et al. 2012; see also Coleman et al. 1966).
Another proposed outside-of-school factor is student resistance to schooling. The widely debated op-
positional culture model asserts that minority students perceive schools as white dominated and this
prompts ambivalence toward achievement and disengagement from school.
2
Other explanations focus within education itself. Dennis Condron (2009) argued that outside-of-
school factors explain learning gaps by socioeconomic status, but not by race per se. Condron (2009)
and others point to de facto school segregation, which decreased through the 1980s before reversing
course and increasing beginning in the 1990s (Condron et al. 2012; Vigdor and Ludwig 2008).
Related research asserts that certain characteristics of predominately minority schools depress student
achievement, such as per pupil funding (Condron and Roscigno 2003), teaching experience
(Corcoran and Evans 2008), and school-level poverty (Rumberger and Palardy 2005). In addition to
across-school differences, research has examined processes within schools, especially ability grouping
or tracking (Berends et al. 2008; Tyson 2011). This work suggests that the learning opportunities of
minority students are restricted by instructional differentiation, which increases learning gaps over
time.
Certainly, these explanations are not mutually exclusive, and racial inequality in achievement arises
from a complex interplay of school and non-school factors. However, we argue that this literature has
not adequately considered one indispensable piece of the puzzle: school punishment. Anne Gregory,
Russell Skiba, and Pedro Noguera (2010) have proposed that school discipline could be related to
achievement differences, but no empirical work has tested this claim. Yet, school punishment is a log-
ical explanation for achievement differences for several reasons. First, punishment varies widely by
race, meaning that it is potentially related to racial variation in achievement. Second, exclusionary
1. The data for fourth-grade and twelfth-grade students show similar patterns. The scale of the NAEP tests ranges from 0 to 500
points. The gap is equivalent to nearly one standard deviation, on average (Condron et al. 2012; NCES 2014).
2. It is impossible to summarize the extensive debate on oppositional culture in the limited space here. For key works, see
Ainsworth-Darnell and Downey (1998), Fordham and Ogbu (1986), and Harris (2011).
The Punishment Gap 69
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
forms of school punishment, such as suspension, extract students from the learning environment,
which can threaten academic progress. Third, school suspensions increased markedly beginning in
the 1990s at the same time that progress on narrowing the achievement gap waned. This indicates
that overuse of exclusionary discipline may pose barriers in efforts to reduce racial inequalities in edu-
cation. The consideration of school punishment adds an important dimension to the argument that
school-level processes help reproduce the racial achievement gap.
RACIALIZED DISCIPLINE AND THE INCREASED USE OF SUSPENSION
Beginning in the 1990s, school discipline approaches became increasingly authoritarian and intrusive.
Several scholars have proposed that contemporary regimes of school discipline “criminalize” student
misbehavior in ways that mirror the criminal justice system (Hirschfield 2008; Kupchik 2010;
Kupchik and Monahan 2006; Wacquant 2001; Welch and Payne 2010). School resource officers
(uniformed police officers stationed in schools), security cameras, random searches, and “zero toler-
ance” policies requiring automatic suspension or expulsion for specified offenses all exemplify this
strict, encompassing approach. This shift in disciplinary mentality has resulted in a sharp increase in
school suspensions. Suspension rates in U.S. public schools have doubled since the 1970s, and in
2010, almost three million students were suspended from school (Losen and Gillespie 2012).
Zero tolerance policies in particular have markedly impacted school suspensions. Disciplinary re-
formers modeled these policies after “tough on crime” approaches to policing and sentencing that
grew in popularity in the late 1980s (Garland 2001; Simon 2007). According to the logic underpin-
ning these approaches, loose social control will allow deviance to flourish. Thus, even small transgres-
sions left unpunished can evolve into larger transgressions and eventually create a deviant normative
context. This logic dictated that early, “tough” punishments were critical to maintaining social order.
Hence, criminal sentencing guidelines such as “three strikes” laws emerged in the late 1980s. Zero tol-
erance policies in schools, which mandated automatic suspension or expulsion for serious or repeated
offenses, soon followed suit. Despite evidence that zero tolerance does not actually enhance school
climate or safety (see American Psychological Association 2008; Skiba and Peterson 1999), schools
across the country continue to be enamored with strict disciplinary policies (Hoffman 2014). Under
such policies, exclusionary school punishments such as suspension and expulsion have become wide-
spread, replacing milder repercussions such as detention or loss of privileges.
Although these new punitive policies intend to mete out discipline fairly, they disproportionately
impact minority students, especially African Americans. Since the publication of the Children’s
Defense Fund’s, School Suspensions: Are They Helping Children (1975), research has consistently re-
vealed that African American students are punished at higher rates, including classroom reprimands
(Ferguson 2000; Morris 2005), office referrals (Rocque 2010; Skiba et al. 2002), suspensions (Losen
2011; McCarthy and Hoge 1987; Wallace et al. 2008), and expulsions (Kewal Ramani et al. 2007 ;
Wallace et al. 2008). Black students are also more likely to experience severe punishment, such as
court action or notification of the police (Welch and Payne 2010). Research suggests that African
American students are approximately three times as likely as white students to be suspended
(Gregory et al. 2010; Wallace et al. 2008). A recent report found that nationwide, one out of six black
students has been suspended at least once (Losen and Gillespie 2012).
3
In addition, predominately
minority schools are most likely to rely on punitive forms of discipline such as out-of-school suspen-
sion or expulsion (Irwin, Davidson, and Hall-Sanchez 2013; Rocque and Paternoster 2011; Welch
and Payne 2010). While the discipline of minority students has long occurred at higher rates
3. For Latinos, the picture is more complex. Some research finds that the punishment of Latino students tends to be less extreme,
but still occurs at higher rates than whites (Losen and Gillespie 2012; Peguero and Shekarkhar 2011). Other research (including
our own analyses) finds that Latinos are not punished at higher rates after controlling for background factors such as free and re-
duced lunch eligibility. We focus on black-white gaps in this article, but future research should explore school discipline and
Latinos in more depth.
70 Morris and Perry
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
compared to white students, this gap has widened as the prevalence of suspension has increased over-
all (Verdugo 2002). Using a natural experiment, Stephen Hoffman (2014) found that strict, punitive
discipline polices increase the racial gap in suspension and expulsion.
These alarming racial disparities in school discipline have prompted a response from the federal
government. In January 2014, the U.S. Department of Education issued a set of guiding principles
concerning discipline in public schools. Although the federal government cannot dictate local disci-
plinary policies, this document encourages schools to rely less on exclusionary forms of discipline and
reminds schools that they cannot discriminate in administering discipline (U.S. Department of
Education 2014). The missive cautions that punishments such as suspension, which remove students
from the learning environment, have been linked to ongoing educational problems. However, despite
the growing realization of negative consequences, there is surprisingly little research able to specify
the direct impact of suspension on outcomes such as academic achievement.
IMPACTS OF EXCLUSIONARY PUNISHMENT
Suspension appears to have few behavioral or academic benefits for suspended students. Virginia
Contenbader and Samia Markson (1998) found that suspension does little to improve subsequent
student behavior, and may even exacerbate students’ anger or apathy. Exclusionary discipline can
weaken school bonds, which may actually increase the likelihood of further deviant behavior (Hirschi
1969; Jenkins 1997). Academically, school suspension has been correlated with low academic
performance (Davis and Jordan 1994) and higher risk of dropout (Ekstrom et al. 1986). A quasi-
experimental study by Emily Arcia (2006) followed two groups of similar students over time, the
only major difference between the groups was that one had been suspended and the other had not.
After two years, the suspended group was nearly five grade levels behind the non-suspended group,
which suggests that suspension greatly impedes academic progress. More recently, Brea Perry and
Morris (2014) found that high rates of suspension at the school level tend to depress student achieve-
ment, even for students who were not personally suspended.
However, research that traces the effects of suspension on achievement longitudinally for a large
and diverse group of students remains thin. While prior educational research has connected exclu-
sionary discipline to lower achievement, it is still unclear whether or to what extent suspension redu-
ces achievement. In addition, no empirical research to our knowledge has been able to link
suspension disparities by race to achievement disparities by race. In this article, we use detailed longi-
tudinal data from school district records and conservative, unbiased fixed-effects modeling to more
accurately specify the impact of suspension on achievement over time. Moreover, we extend this anal-
ysis directly to the racial achievement gap to determine the extent to which school discipline dispar-
ities explain this gap.
Our unique data and analysis provide the first comprehensive study of the impact of suspen-
sion on racial differences in achievement. Using advanced multilevel methods that capitalize on
the rich explanatory power of longitudinal and hierarchical data, we focus on the following ques-
tions: (1) Are racial and ethnic minorities at disproportionate risk for school suspension? (2)
Are racial-ethnic background and school suspension associated with academic achievement in
reading and math, controlling for other individual characteristics and all school-level heterogene-
ity? (3) Do racial differences in the likelihood of suspension explain a significant proportion of
the racial achievement gap?
METHODS
This research uses data from the Kentucky School Discipline Study (KSDS) (Perry and Morris
2014). Data are comprised of existing, deidentified school records and supplementary data collected
routinely from parents in a large, urban public school district. All data on school discipline and test
scores come directly from school records, eliminating any selection bias and social desirability effects
that occur when students or parents report on their own behavior. For each student offense resulting
The Punishment Gap 71
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
in any disciplinary action (office referral, detention, suspension, expulsion, etc.), school personnel are
required to complete an electronic form containing information about the offense, all students in-
volved, and any response by school officials. This information is stored for the purposes of monitor-
ing school safety and reporting discipline statistics to the state, and is well regulated. Only
information on family structure (i.e., single parent family, number of people living in the home) is
drawn from the parent survey.
Our sample includes students in grades 6 through 10 (middle and high school) who were enrolled
in a district public school over a three-year period beginning in August 2008 and ending in June
2011. The full sample includes 24,347 students. However, 8,089 students (33 percent of the full sam-
ple) are dropped due to missing data on end-of-year (spring) Measure of Academic Progress (MAP)
test scores. MAP testing by the school district was inconsistent prior to 2009 during the pilot phase.
By the 2009-2010 school year, full implementation of the testing was in place. Because the piloting
process was random, missing data are unlikely to lead to biases. An additional ten cases were dropped
due to missing data on other variables.
The analysis sample includes 16,248 students nested in 17 schools, providing a total of 25,221 ob-
servations over three years of data. At baseline, about 65 percent of students are in grades 6 to 8
(ages 11 to 13), and 35 percent in grades 9 to 10 (ages 14 to 16). Approximately 49 percent of stu-
dents in the sample are girls and 51 percent are boys. The majority of these students are white (59
percent) or black (25 percent). However, 10 percent are Latino, 4 percent are Asian, and 3 percent
classify themselves as some other race. Also, 48 percent of students qualify for free or reduced-price
meals. These data, which are drawn from one school system, are not nationally representative of all
public school children. Most notably, a smaller percentage of the U.S. student population is non-
Hispanic black (17 percent) compared to our sample, and a greater percentage is Latino (21 percent;
NCES 2014). However, black populations tend to be concentrated in the Southeast where this school
district is located. Consequently, these data may be reasonably representative of the Southeastern
United States.
With respect to patterns of exclusionary discipline, our sample is on par with national trends
(Aud, Fox, and KewalRamani 2010). Specifically, rates of out-of-school suspension in the KSDS and
nationally representative National Household Education Surveys (NHES 2007) (U.S. Department of
Education 2007) are the same (22 percent had ever been suspended). There are also similar patterns
of racial disparities in suspension, which is critical for this analysis in particular. In the KSDS, about
42 percent of black students had ever been suspended, compared to 43 percent in the NHES sample
(a non-significant difference). Among Latinos, 26 percent in the KSDS district had ever been sus-
pended compared to 22 percent nationally (p < .001). Also, Asians in both data sets were less likely
to be suspended, though this difference is larger in Kentucky (4 percent and 11 percent, respectively;
p < .001). Finally, 18 percent of girls and 26 percent of boys in the KSDS had been suspended com-
pared to 15 percent of girls and 28 percent of boys nationally. This indicates that boys in the general
population are slightly more likely to have been suspended than students in the Kentucky district.
Overall, these patterns are remarkably similar in magnitude and always in the same direction. These
results suggest that exclusionary discipline patterns in the data used for this analysis are representative
of national trends, supporting the use of cautious inference to students in other districts.
Measures
Several static characteristics of individual students are examined as independent variables in multivariate
models. Gender is coded as a binary variable (1 ¼ female; 0 ¼ male). Race is measured in five catego-
ries and coded as binary indicators: white, African American, Latino, Asian, and other. Family structure
is measured by a binary variable indicating whether two parents or guardians were listed on each stu-
dent’s parent information form (1 ¼ two parents; 0 ¼ one parent). Because this measure is available
only in the final wave of the study, missing values on 14 percent of observations are replaced via a
72 Morris and Perry
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
logistic regression multiple imputation method. Ten imputations are computed, and Stata’s -mi- com-
mands are used for imputation and estimation of models that include the family structure variable.
Time is coded using academic year beginning with 0 at baseline in 2008-2009 and ending with 2
in 2010-2011. Time-squared and time-cubed are also calculated to assess the non-linearity of the
growth or decline in school suspensions and academic achievement over time. All other time-varying
measures are divided into their between-person and within-person information to differentiate the
degree to which outcomes are due to average differences between students across waves or differ-
ences over time in the characteristics of a student compared to him or herself at other waves
(Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). Between-person variance is reflected in the average score for the three
waves of the study, and is held constant across observations nested within the same individual.
Within-person variance is the average score subtracted from the score for the current wave of the
study, and measures how different a person is in a given wave from their own average. For binary var-
iables, the between-person measure is equivalent to the proportion of waves in which each student
had the characteristic in question. The within-person score is the difference between the binary indi-
cator for a given wave and the between-person proportion. It ranges from -.67 (having the character-
istic in every wave except the current wave) to .67 (having the characteristic only in the current
wave), with zero indicating no change across waves.
Socioeconomic status is measured using participation in the free or reduced meal program. For
this variable, between-person variance is the average of free/reduced lunch status (coded 1 ¼ yes;
0 ¼ no) across three waves of the study. This is also equal to the proportion of waves in which each
student participated in the free/reduced lunch program. The within-person measure is the difference
between the binary variable for the current wave and the proportion of waves in which the student
participated in the free/reduced lunch program. Receipt of special education services is also measured
using binary coding, and is decomposed into between- and within-person variation.
Out-of-school student suspension is measured as a dichotomous variable and is the dependent var-
iable in the first set of regressions. Information on student suspensions is drawn from official school
records. Though a small minority of students experienced multiple out-of-school suspensions in a
given school year, there are insufficient cases to employ a count variable. In subsequent regressions
predicting academic achievement, suspension is an independent variable and is split into between-
and within-person variation. The between-person measure of suspension is the proportion of waves
in which a student is suspended, while the within-person measure is suspension in the current wave
minus the proportion of years with suspensions.
Performance on tests in math and reading are used to assess achievement, and are also drawn
from official school records. Between 2008 and 2011 in the targeted school district, academic achieve-
ment was measured using MAP testing across the state. This is a computerized adaptive test that is
designed to help schools monitor academic growth in reading and math and make informed decisions
about placement and needed services. Scores are numeric and normally distributed. The tests are not
timed, and are administered multiple times per year. To reduce concerns about reverse causation
(i.e., low academic performance leading to suspension), scores from the end-of-year MAP testing are
used in this analysis, making it unlikely that any suspensions occurred following testing. In cases
where data from the end-of-year academic achievement tests are missing, the average scores from
MAP testing occurring earlier in the same school year are imputed. Currently, similar racial differ-
ences in the NAEP appear for both math and reading components of the test (NCES 2014).
Therefore, it is appropriate for us to use both math and reading outcomes here. MAP scores for read-
ing and math are examined separately to provide a strong overall assessment of achievement.
Analyses
Analyses focus on identifying the association between race and ethnicity, suspension, and academic
achievement. Multivariate effects are modeled with multi-level mixed logistic and linear regression
The Punishment Gap 73
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
models using Stata 13 (StataCorp 2013). These adjust for the hierarchical data structure and the
interdependence among observations resulting from having multiple observations over time for each
student and multiple students in schools. The models have a three-level structure where level-one ob-
servations (time points) are nested in level-two individual students, which are nested in level-three
schools.
Because these models focus on predicting an individual-level outcome using both time-invariant
and time-variant characteristics, the models include a random intercept at level two. To control for
unmeasured time- and student-invariant characteristics of schools, these models include level-three
fixed effects using dichotomous school indicators (estimates not shown in tables). This means that
mechanisms of suspension and achievement for students in a particular school are estimated relative
to other students in the same school. Variables such as the neighborhood in which the school is lo-
cated and other potential confounding school-level effects that are time invariant or which can rea-
sonably be expected to change very little over a three-year period are controlled since all
comparisons are between students within the same school. This strategy also eliminates the small n
problem at level three (i.e., 17 schools) because school-level variation is controlled in the fixed-effects
model rather than being used for prediction.
The basic mixed-effects model with three levels predicting test scores using two independent vari-
ables, for example, takes the following form:
y
ijk
¼ b
0
þ b
1
x
1ijk
þ b
2
x
2ijk
þ f
j
þ a
k
þ e
ijk
In this model, i corresponds to time (level one), j to student (level two), and k to school (level
three). The symbol f
j
represents the random intercept at the student level and a
k
is a fixed parameter
representing all differences between schools that are stable over time. The fixed parameter at the
school level is accomplished through binary school indicators, as noted above. Finally, e
ijk
is the level
one residual. Together, f
j
and e
ijk
represent the random parts of the model, while the other compo-
nents are fixed.
The first set of models examines the effects of race and ethnicity on the log odds of suspension. A
baseline model (1a) includes race and ethnicity as well as time, but does not include dichotomous
school indicators. This is the only model estimated without school-level fixed effects, and this is to
demonstrate that part of the increased susceptibility of minorities to exclusionary discipline is ex-
plained by racial and ethnic segregation into different schools. In addition, a supplemental regression
of school-level characteristics is computed to confirm that the partial confounding effect of dichoto-
mous school indicators is due to black students attending schools with higher suspension rates, con-
trolling for school size and socioeconomic status composition (results not shown). The second
model (2a) predicting student suspension includes race and ethnicity, time, and dichotomous school
indicators. The third model (3a) adds potential confounding factors, including sociodemographics
and special education placement. The fourth model (4a) adds a family structure variable and is esti-
mated using multiple imputation procedures due to missing data on that variable.
In the second set of analyses, quadratic growth curve models are estimated to determine how read-
ing and math achievement scores change over time in this school system. Baseline models include
time and race and ethnicity (1b and 1c). The second set of models (2b and 2c) add between- and
within-person measures of suspension to assess the degree to which group differences in exclusionary
discipline experiences explain the racial and ethnic academic achievement gap. Mediation of the rela-
tionship between race and ethnicity and academic achievement by suspension is formally tested using
the -sgmediation- command in Stata. The purpose of this analysis, following Michael E. Sobel (1986)
and Reuben M. Baron and David A. Kenny (1986), is to test whether a mediator carries the influence
of an independent variable (IV) to a dependent variable (DV). The -sgmediation- command tests all
four relationships required to meet criteria for mediation: (1) the IV significantly affects the mediator,
74 Morris and Perry
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
(2) the IV significantly affects the DV in the absence of the mediator, (3) the mediator has a signifi-
cant unique effect on the DV, and (4) the effect of the IV on the DV is reduced when the mediator is
added to the model. The indirect effect of race on achievement through suspension is tested using a
conservative bootstrapped estimation procedure with case resampling (MacKinnon and Dwyer
1993). This method for testing the statistical significance of an indirect effect (i.e., mediation) has
been shown to produce less biased estimates than the Baron and Kenny (1986) and Sobel (1986)
methods in simulation studies (MacKinnon, Warsi, and Dwyer 1995).
The third set of models (3b and 3c) predicting test scores add student sociodemographic charac-
teristics that may confound the relationship between race, suspension, and academic achievement
(e.g., socioeconomic status). In these models, time invariant characteristics (i.e., gender and race and
ethnicity) are measured at level two, while time variant characteristics (i.e., suspension, socioeco-
nomic status, and special education status) are measured at level one. All level one variables are sepa-
rated into between-student effects (e.g., Why are students different from each other, on average?)
and within-student effects (e.g., Why are students different from themselves this year compared to
other years?). In addition, the family structure variable is added to the final models (4b and 4c),
which are estimated using multiple imputation procedures.
To demonstrate the long-term effects of suspension on academic achievement, we use the above
models to generate a graph of predicted values for test scores over time. These depict trajectories of aca-
demic achievement based on early and repeated suspensions in the academic career. Between-student
effects of suspended and never-suspended students are reflected in intercept differences between
groups, while within-student effects of suspension are depicted by changes in the angles of the lines
over time. This figure is based on a model containing all student- and school-level control variables.
Though between-school (i.e., time invariant) school-level characteristics are controlled by the
fixed-effects approach, we conduct supplemental analyses to assess the sensitivity of the models to
time-variant school-level variables that might be correlated with test scores and/or suspension. These
variables include within-school variation on percent racial/ethnic minority, percent free/reduced
lunch, percent special education, expenditures per student, school size, and total number of offenses
in a school in a given year. Estimates of these effects are for the most part unreliable because there is
little variation over three years in these indicators, with the exception of number of offenses.
However, including time-variant school-level indicators has very little impact on the coefficients for
race or suspension in models predicting test scores, and did not change the substantive conclusions
of this research. Consequently, these models are not included in tables of results.
A number of student- and school-level variables (e.g., race, socioeconomic status, and likelihood of
suspension) are correlated, introducing the possibility of multicollinearity. However, variance infla-
tion factors (VIFs) do not exceed 3.08 for any model. This reduces concerns about the degree to
which multicollinearity might lead to biased estimates.
RESULTS
Descriptive statistics in Table 1 suggest that 12 percent of public school students will receive an out-
of-school suspension in any given year. Academic achievement scores in reading (m ¼ 220.21;
s ¼ 17.49) and math (m ¼ 231.33; s ¼ 19.60) vary substantially across the sample, which includes stu-
dents in grades 6 through 10. However, scores within schools are less variable, ranging from a stan-
dard deviation of 10.80 to 23.28 when accounting for time invariant school-level heterogeneity. Also,
the interclass correlations for MAP reading and math scores are .71 and .81, respectively, suggesting
substantial correlation in academic achievement across time within each student.
The Racial and Ethnic Gap in Exclusionary Discipline
Table 2 contains the results from a mixed-effects logistic regression of suspension on race and ethnic-
ity. Findings in Model 1a do not include school-level fixed effects, permitting the relationship
The Punishment Gap 75
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
between race and ethnicity and suspension to reflect group differences in the kinds of schools that
minority students are likely to attend. These indicate that black students are estimated to be 7.57
times as likely to be suspended as white students (p < .001), and Latinos are over twice as likely as
whites (OR ¼ 2.39; p < .001). Students of other races are predicted to be 2.61 times more likely to
be suspended than whites (p < .001), while Asians are less likely than whites (OR ¼ .20; p < .001).
Findings in Model 2a add school-level fixed effects, controlling for all observed and unobserved time
invariant heterogeneity in characteristics of schools. In other words, all estimates reflect differences
between students in the same school. These findings indicate that black students are still estimated to
be almost six times as likely to be suspended as white students (OR ¼ 5.91; p < .001), while Latinos
are nearly twice as likely (OR ¼ 1.87; p < .001). Students of other races are 2.47 times more likely to
be suspended than whites (p < .001), on average, while Asians are estimated to be suspended at
lower rates than whites (OR ¼ .23; p < .001). In all, racial segregation into different schools explains
about 12 percent of the effect of being black on the odds of suspension, and supplemental analyses
confirm that schools with larger concentrations of black students have significantly higher rates of
out-of-school suspension. Each additional percentage of the student body that is black is estimated to
increase the annual number of school suspensions by about ten, controlling for school size and socio-
economic composition (b ¼ 10.16; p < .01).
As shown in Model 3a of Table 2, the addition of sociodemographic covariates reduces the magnitude
of the impact of race and ethnicity on suspension, and this result is attributable almost entirely to racial
and ethnic differences in socioeconomic status (i.e., free/reduced lunch). Students who qualify for free/re-
duced lunch in all three waves of the study are predicted to be over six times as likely to be suspended as
those who never qualify (OR ¼ 6.36; p < .001). Students who receive special education serves are also es-
timatedtobemorelikelytobesuspended(OR¼ 3.19; p < .001), while girls are less likely to be sus-
pended than boys (OR ¼ .36; p < .001). However, even after controlling for socioeconomic status,
special education services, and gender, black students are predicted to have nearly three times the odds of
suspension compared to whites (OR ¼ 2.80; p < .001), and students of other races are 57 percent more
likely than white students to be suspended (p < .05). In contrast, the elevated risk of suspension associ-
ated with being Latino is entirely explained by this group’s lower levels of socioeconomic status.
Results in Model 4a of Table 2 include a variable measuring family structure, and are estimated
using multiple imputation procedures. Students with two parents are 56 percent less likely to be
suspended
, on average, than those with only one parent or guardian (p < .001). Family structure
Table 1. Descriptive Sample Characteristics
Students Proportion Mean SD Range
Female .49
Race and ethnicity
White .59
Black .25
Latino .10
Asian .04
Other .03
Free/reduced lunch .48
Special education .09
Suspended .12
Two-parent family .63
MAP reading score 220.21 17.49 141.00-280.00
MAP math score 231.33 19.60 143.00-300.00
N ¼ 16,248
76 Morris and Perry
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
explains a small amount of the variation in the effect of being black on suspension, but black students
are still estimated to be nearly two and a half times as likely to be suspended as white students in
this model (OR ¼ 2.46; p < .001). The effect of being some other race or ethnicity becomes non-
significant in this model, suggesting that differences in suspension rates for this group are entirely
explained by socioeconomic status and family structure. Also, the effect of free/reduced lunch qualifi-
cation on odds of suspension is partially explained by family structure, but continues to have a large
significant effect in this full model (OR ¼ 4.81; p < .001).
Effects of Exclusionary Discipline on Academic Achievement
Table 3 displays the effects of race and ethnicity and suspension on academic achievement in reading.
There is evidence of significant curvilinear growth in academic achievement over the study period
such that the test scores grow more substantially early in the study period, but that growth begins to
taper off over time (p < .001). This is consistent with expectations for MAP growth, where gains are
Table 2. Mixed-Effects Logistic Regression of Suspension on Race and Ethnicity over Time
Model 1a
a
Model 2a Model 3a Model 4a
Within-student D
Time (years) 1.05
(.96-1.15)
.99
(.90-1.09)
.96
(.88-1.06)
.97
(.88-1.06)
Free/reduced lunch .90
(.61-1.31)
.90
(.61-1.31)
Special education .95
(.45-2.02)
.96
(.45-2.05)
Between-student
Race and ethnicity
b
Black 7.57
(6.36-9.01)***
5.91
(4.98-7.00)***
2.80
(2.38-3.30)***
2.46
(2.09-2.89)***
Latino 2.39
(1.89-2.03)***
1.87
(1.48-2.36)***
.84
(.67-1.07)
.90
(.71-1.13)
Asian .20
(.11-.35)***
.23
(.13-.40)***
.29
(.17-.50)***
.33
(.19-.57)***
Other 2.61
(1.74-3.91)***
2.47
(1.66-3.68)***
1.57
(1.06-2.33)*
1.39
(.94-2.05)
Female .36
(.31-.42)***
.35
(.30-.41)***
Free/reduced lunch 6.36
(5.30-7.63)***
4.81
(4.01-5.75)***
Special education 3.19
(2.60-3.92)***
2.92
(2.36-3.56)***
Two-parent family .44
(.38-.52)***
N 16,284 16,284 16,284 16,284
Obs 25,221 25,221 25,221 25,221
q .63 .61 .57 .56
Wald X
2
/F 555.87*** 709.11*** 978.35*** 38.99***
Notes: Odds ratios are presented, confidence intervals in parentheses. Models 2 through 4 control for dichotomous school indicators.
a
Model 1 does not control for dichotomous school indictors (i.e., there is no school-level fixed effect).
b
Omitted category is white.
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001 (two-tailed tests)
The Punishment Gap 77
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
more substantial in earlier grades relative to later ones. As shown in Model 1b, students who are black
(b ¼ -10.87; p < .001), Latino (b ¼ -12.95; p < .001), Asian (b ¼ -2.04; p < .01), and some other race
(b ¼ -4.79; p < .001) are all predicted to have significantly lower scores on achievement in reading
compared to white students, controlling for school-level fixed effects.
Table 3. Mixed-Effects Linear Regression of Reading Achievement on Student Suspension
Over Time
Model 1b Model 2b Model 3b Model 4b
Within-student D
Time (years) 3.40
(.54)***
3.43
(.54)***
2.94
(.52)***
2.94
(.52)***
Time-squared 2.37
(.20)***
2.33
(.20)***
2.14
(.19)***
2.14
(.19)***
Suspended 1.01
(.31)***
1.10
(.31)***
1.10
(.31)***
Free/reduced lunch .55
(.44)
.55
(.44)
Special education 1.73
(1.07)
1.71
(1.07)
Between-student
Race and ethnicity
a
Black 10.87
(.30)***
8.72
(.30)***
4.60
(.29)***
4.39
(.29)***
Latino 12.95
(.44)***
12.36
(.41)***
7.97
(.41)***
8.04
(.41)***
Asian 2.04
(.65)**
2.82
(.63)***
4.15
(.56)***
4.28
(.56)***
Other 4.79
(.78)***
4.02
(.76)***
1.62
(.68)*
1.42
(.68)*
Suspended 15.05
(.45)***
8.61
(.42)***
8.37
(.42)***
Female 1.49
(.22)***
1.55
(.22)***
Free/reduced lunch 9.21
(.27)***
8.78
(.28)***
Special education 20.19
(.40)***
20.07
(.40)***
Two-parent family 1.39
(.28)***
Constant 215.42
(.42)***
217.68
(.62)***
221.93
(.61)***
220.78
(.65)***
N 16,284 16,284 16,284 16,284
Obs 25,221 25,221 25,221 25,221
q .78 .76 .71 .71
Wald X
2
/F 4,055.50*** 5,371.83*** 10,696.51*** 352.93***
Notes: Unstandardized coefficients, standard errors in parentheses; models control for dichotomous school indicators.
a
Omitted category is white.
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
(two-tailed tests)
78 Morris and Perry
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Model 2b adds between- and within-person variation in suspension over time, and demonstrates
that out-of-school suspension is significantly related to academic achievement. The proportion of
waves in which a student is suspended (i.e., propensity to be suspended) is associated with decreases
in reading such that those who have been suspended each year of the study are predicted to
have a MAP reading score that is over 15 points lower than those who have never been suspended
(b ¼ -15.05; p < .001). This is nearly a one-standard deviation decrease in academic achievement. In
other words, being suspended is a strong predictor of a student’s academic performance relative to
other students in the same school. Also, having a suspension in a given wave is associated with signifi-
cantly lower performance on reading evaluations (b ¼ -1.01; p < .001) at the end of that academic
year relative to other years, comparing each student to him or herself.
As seen in Models 3b and 4b of Table 3 , girls tend to score higher than boys in reading achieve-
ment (b ¼ 1.49; p < .001), on average. Between-person variation in proportion of waves spent in
free/reduced lunch status is associated with significant differences in reading achievement (b ¼ -9.21;
p < .001), as is between-student variation in special education placement (b ¼ -20.19; p < .001).
However, within-person changes in these statuses over time do not significantly affect reading or
math achievement. Model 4b includes a measure of family structure, suggesting that students in
two-parent families perform better in reading than those with one parent (b ¼ 1.39, p < .001). Most
importantly, the addition of these potential confounding factors only partially explains differential
academic achievement by race and ethnicity and by suspension.
Findings in Table 4 reflect the effects of race and ethnicity and suspension on math achievement.
Again, there is evidence of curvilinear growth in math scores over the study period (p < .001), as antici-
pated. As shown in Model 1c, students who are black (b ¼ -13.34; p < .001), Latino (b ¼ -12.57;
p < .001), and some other race (b ¼ -6.97; p < .001) are all predicted to have significantly lower scores
on achievement in math compared to white students, controlling for school-level fixed effects. In contrast,
Asian students are estimated to perform better in math than whites, on average (b ¼ 9.40; p < .001).
The effects of suspension on math achievement are included in Model 2c of Table 4. The propor-
tion of waves in which a student is suspended is associated with decreases in math performance such
that those who have been suspended each year of the study are predicted to have a MAP math score
that is 16.21 points lower than those who have never been suspended (p < .001; nearly a one stan-
dard deviation reduction). Also, having a suspension in a given wave is associated with significantly
lower math performance (
b ¼ -.56; p < .05)
at the end of that academic year relative to other years,
comparing each student to him or herself.
Effects of control variables on math achievement mirror those for reading achievement. Gender
differences are the exception (see Models 3c and 4c of Table 4), as girls score lower than boys, on av-
erage, in math achievement (b ¼ -2.09; p < .001). Also, between-person variation in free/reduced
lunch (b ¼ -11.14; p < .001) and special education status (b ¼ -24.21;p< .001) are associated with
lower math performance. Model 4c shows that students with two parents are estimated to score
higher in math achievement than those with one parent (b ¼ 1.68; p < .001). As with reading
achievement, the addition of these potential confounding factors only partially explains differential
math achievement by race and ethnicity and suspension.
Figure 1 depicts results from Model 3c in Table 4. Differences in math achievement between sus-
pended and never-suspended students (i.e., between-student effects) are reflected in baseline pre-
dicted values of math MAP performance (year 0). Within-student effects of suspension are depicted
by changes in predicted values over time. A student who is never suspended has a linear growth in
math performance that is reflected in a six-point increase across the three measures, as would be ex-
pected for students making normal academic progress. Suspended students have lower baseline
scores than never-suspended students, on average, possibly reflecting other unmeasured mechanisms
of student success that are correlated with suspension. However, suspension does have meaningful
and lasting adverse effects over time independent of early disparities between ever- and never-
suspended students. Though students experiencing one early suspension begin with only a three-
The Punishment Gap 79
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
point deficit relative to those without a suspension, that deficit grows to nine points at the end of the
two-year study period. Students with an early suspension experience no significant growth in math
achievement. Students with two years of suspension do demonstrate modest growth (three points),
but they begin with a much larger eight-point deficit relative to never-suspended students. By the end
Table 4. Mixed-Effects Linear Regression of Math Achievement on Student Suspension
Over Time
Model 1c Model 2c Model 3c Model 4c
Within-student D
Time
(years)
1.91
(.50)***
1.98
(.50)***
1.61
(.49)***
1.61
(.49)***
Time-squared 1.99
(.18)***
1.97
(.18)***
1.82
(.18)***
1.81
(.18)***
Suspended .56
(.28)*
.60
(.27)*
.60
(.27)*
Free/reduced lunch .16
(.39)
.16
(.39)
Special education 1.14
(.98)
1.11
(.98)
Between-student
Race and ethnicity
a
Black 13.34
(.34)***
10.99
(.34)***
5.76
(.32)***
5.51
(.33)***
Latino 12.57
(.50)***
11.90
(.49)***
6.50
(.46)***
6.59
(.46)***
Asian 9.40
(.73)***
8.51
(.71)***
6.89
(.63)***
6.73
(.63)***
Other 6.97
(.89)***
6.13
(.86)***
2.99
(.76)***
2.76
(.76)***
Suspended 16.21
(.51)***
9.40
(.47)***
9.11
(.47)***
Female 2.09
(.25)***
2.02
(.25)***
Free/reduced lunch 11.14
(.30)***
10.62
(.31)***
Special education 24.21
(.45)***
24.06
(.45)***
Two-parent family 1.68
(.31)***
Constant 228.01
(.62)***
230.21
(.62)***
237.05
(.61)***
235.65
(.66)***
N 16,284 16,284 16,284 16,284
obs 25,221 25,221 25,221 25,221
q .86 .85 .81 .81
Wald X
2
4,460.13*** 5,640.90*** 11,539.23*** 379.40***
Notes: Unstandardized coefficients, standard errors in parentheses; models control for dichotomous school indicators.
a
Omitted category is white.
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
(two-tailed tests)
80 Morris and Perry
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
of the study period, that deficit has grown to 11 points. Importantly, this figure suggests that when stu-
dents who were initially at risk for low performance are suspended, this event places them at further risk of
academic decline.
Reproduction of Racial Inequality through Exclusionary Discipline
The first set of analyses demonstrates that racial and ethnic minorities are disproportionately suscep-
tible to suspension. This effect is particularly pronounced for black students, and this effect is only
partially explained by socioeconomic status, family structure, and other variables. The suspension dis-
parity operates at both the school and individual levels such that black students are more likely than
white students to attend schools that employ higher levels of exclusionary discipline, and black stu-
dents are also more likely to be suspended than their white peers within the same schools. In turn, ra-
cial and ethnic minorities underperform on reading and math achievement tests relative to white
students in this school system. As shown in Model 2 of Tables 3 and 4, adding between- and within-
person measures of suspension to the regression of academic achievement on race and ethnicity
reduces the effect of minority status. To assess the extent to which group differences in exclusionary
discipline experiences explain the racial and ethnic academic achievement gap, mediation analyses
with a bootstrapped estimation of the indirect effect are conducted. These findings suggest that 20
percent of the effect of being black on reading achievement (b ¼ -2.07; p < .001) and 17 percent on
math achievement (b ¼ -2.24; p < .001) works indirectly through inequalities in exclusionary disci-
pline experiences. In other words, the racial achievement gap for black students is reproduced in part
through disproportionate exposure to exclusionary discipline in public schools.
DISCUSSION
Our analysis provides evidence that school suspension contributes to racial inequalities in achieve-
ment. According to our results, African Americans and Latinos are disproportionately susceptible to
suspension. Because there are fixed-effects parameters at the school level, this result cannot be
Figure 1. Predicted Values of MAP Math Scores Over Time as a Function of Suspensions
Note: Based on Model 3c in Table 4.
The Punishment Gap
81
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
explained by racial and ethnic segregation into different kinds of schools. In other words, African
Americans and Latinos are more likely to be suspended than whites and Asians within the same
school. For African Americans, this finding persists even after controlling for socioeconomic status
and other relevant individual-level variables.
Results indicate that suspension has important linkages to student academic achievement.
Students who have been suspended score substantially lower on end-of-year academic progress tests
than those who have not, and even students with a propensity to be suspended perform worse in
years where they are suspended relative to years when they are not. We find that the effects of sus-
pension are long lasting, setting into motion a trajectory of poor performance that continues in subse-
quent years, even if a student is not suspended again. Indeed, our results show that academic growth
drops precipitously after one early suspension (see Figure 1). In all, our analysis provides strong evi-
dence that suspension is harmful to academic achievement.
As hypothesized, the most striking finding from this research is the important association between
suspension and patterns of achievement disparity. Our study is the first to our knowledge to directly
examine the implications of racial differences in punishment for racial differences in achievement.
The results support the proposition that school discipline is a major source of the racial achievement
gap and educational reproduction of inequality (Gregory et al. 2010). Particularly for African
American students in our data, the unequal suspension rate is one of the most important factors hin-
dering academic progress and maintaining the racial gap in achievement. Consistent with previous re-
search, we find that family economic background and family structure explain much, but certainly not
all, of the achievement gap (Hedges and Nowell 1999) and the discipline gap (Skiba et al. 2002).
Our findings add a critical new dimension to the long-standing discussion of academic disparities
by race. Recent perspectives on the achievement gap emphasize a complex interplay of between-
school, within-school, and non-school factors, instead of an either-or view ( Berends et al. 2008;
Condron et al. 2012). We agree with this multifaceted approach, and our findings on school disci-
pline align with each set of factors. The discipline disparities we observe emanate at least partially
from the types of schools black students attend (Condron 2009). In addition, home-based inequal-
ities are undoubtedly an important part of why suspension reduces achievement (Downey et al.
2004), as schools send suspended students home, often with little academic guidance or oversight.
However, our findings on school punishment most directly add to the notion that practices within
schools contribute to the achievement gap.
Because we find that school discipline is related to racial differences in achievement, we cast our
findings as a possible example of hidden inequality embedded within routine educational practices.
Scholars of race assert that subtle, covert forms of discrimination are major drivers of racial inequality
in the post-civil rights “color-blind” era (Bonilla-Silva 2006; Pager and Shepherd 2008; Quillian
2006). Such inequality occurs indirectly, through the routine enactment of everyday institutional poli-
cies and procedures (Pager and Shepherd 2008). Similarly, education scholars from Pierre Bourdieu
and Jean-Claude Passeron (1977) to Karolyn Tyson (2011) have argued that seemingly neutral pro-
cesses in schools conceal certain biases and reproduce inequalities. Indeed, purportedly neutral disci-
pline policies that increase the overall use of suspension in schools (e.g., zero tolerance) have been
shown to exacerbate the racial gap in suspension (Hoffman 2014; Verdugo 2002).
Although we lack the data to test racial bias in discipline directly, we do show an alarming racial
gap in punishment even after controlling for a host of background variables. This racial difference in-
dicates that the enactment of discipline, while likely holding no discriminatory intent, nevertheless
generates de facto racial inequalities. While it is possible that black students simply misbehave more
than white students, previous studies have found racial discrepancies in how punishment is adminis-
tered, even for similar offenses (Ferguson 2000; Morris 2005; Skiba et al. 2002). Thus, our results
align with evidence of racial inequality (even if subtle and unrecognized) in school punishment. Our
analysis advances this research by linking such punishment to disparate academic outcomes. Future
82 Morris and Perry
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
research could complement our study by fleshing out the micro-level processes of discipline and aca-
demic progress in greater detail.
Limitations and Future Directions
While our research reveals a strong relationship between school suspension and achievement, it also
has limitations. The primary limitation is that our data, while longitudinal, cannot prove a causal link
between suspension and achievement. In particular, unmeasured endogenous factors could be driving
the association between exclusionary discipline and achievement.
One of the most likely intervening factors is that black students could demonstrate worse behavior
on average, which would lead to more suspensions for black students. This same behavior could also
interfere with the learning process, resulting in lower achievement. We do not possess the data to di-
rectly examine differences between student behavior and the discipline they receive. However, we can
draw from previous studies, which have noted that minority students are disciplined more harshly
than white students for similar misbehavior. For example, in an analysis that controls for teacher-
reported behavior, Michael Rocque and Raymond Paternoster (2011) found that “(racial) dispropor-
tionality in discipline is not explained by differential behavior” (p. 662). Moreover, qualitative studies
by Ann Arnett Ferguson (2000) and Edward Morris (2005) have shown that black and Latino stu-
dents are more closely monitored and more often punished than white students for similar types of
infractions. According to Ferguson (2000:68), school officials tend to interpret behavior through a
“racialized key” that accentuates transgressions of minority students. Thus, while we cannot assert it
definitively based on our data, we can look to previous research to suggest that disciplinary polices
and interpretations within schools contribute to at least part of the racial disparity in discipline.
However, we think that the association between discipline and behavior is ultimately complex, and
begs further study. Student behavior, discipline, and achievement interact as students progress through
schooling. The challenge for future research is to plumb this relationship further to gain a deeper pic-
ture of the mechanisms producing differences in punishment. It would be especially fruitful for studies
to examine students progress over time, as they transition across various levels of schooling, and to ex-
amine the types of academic resources students have access to after a suspension. It would also be use-
ful to compare the effects of different types of discipline to ascertain whether any act of punishment is
associated with diminished achievement, or whether it is exclusionary discipline per se. Likewise, an im-
portant next step is assessing whether missed instruction is a mechanism of our findings. For instance,
future research should compare the influence of missed instruction due to suspension and other causes
(e.g., illness, truancy, etc.) to determine whether it is punishment per se or lost classroom time in gen-
eral that underlies the link between exclusionary discipline and achievement.
Another significant limitation is that we do not possess data on student perceptions of discipline
and relationships with school officials. Even when strict, if students perceive discipline as fair, this
may foster a positive relationship with school and result in higher achievement (Arum 2003; Kupchik
and Ellis 2008). For minority students in particular, developing supportive bonds with institutional
officials appears critical for academic success (Conchas 2006; Stanton-Salazar 1997). These bonds
may be enhanced by minority teachers, who tend to assess the behavior of minority students more
positively (Downey and Pribesh 2004; McLoughlin and Noltemeyer 2010; Quiocho and Rios 2000;
Rocha and Hawes 2009), but such teacher-student dynamics are complex (McGrady and Reynolds
2013). Future research should examine student perceptions of discipline and relationships with
school officials as potentially important factors in school punishment disparities.
CONCLUSION
This study adds a critical new piece to the puzzle over racial disparities in achievement. In particular,
it demonstrates how exclusionary forms of punishment such as suspension have important, racialized
academic consequences. Our study presents evidence that disparate suspension lowers school
The Punishment Gap 83
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
performance and contributes to racial gaps in achievement. Discipline is a necessary condition for stu-
dent learning. However, unequal exclusionary discipline severely restricts opportunities for students
to learn and grow. For genuine progress to be made in closing the racial achievement gap, we must
also make progress in closing the racial punishment gap.
REFERENCES
Ainsworth-Darnell, James W. and Douglas B. Downey. 1998. “Assessing the Oppositional Culture Explanation for
Racial/Ethnic Differences in School Performance.” American Sociological Review 63:536–53.
American Psychological Association. 2008. “Are Zero Tolerance Policies Effective in the Schools? An Evidentiary
Review and Recommendations.” American Psychologist 63:852–62.
Arcia, E. 2006. “Achievement and Enrollment Status of Suspended Students: Outcomes in a Large, Multicultural
School District.” Education and Urban Society 38:359–69.
Arum, Richard. 2003. Judging School Discipline: The Crisis of Moral Authority. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Aud, Susan, Mary Ann Fox, and Angelina KewalRamani. 2010. Status and Trends in the Education of Racial and Ethnic
Groups (NCES 2010–015). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington,
DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Baron, R. M. and D. A. Kenny. 1986. “Moderator-Mediator Variables Distinction in Social Psychological Research:
Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51:1173–82.
Belfield, Clive R. and Henry M. Levin. 2007. The Price We Pay: Economic and Social Consequences of Inadequate
Education. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.
Berends, Mark, Samuel R. Lucas, and Roberto V. Penaloza. 2008. “How Changes in Families and Schools are Related
to Trends in Black-White Test Scores.” Sociology of Education 81:313–44.
Bonilla-Silva, Eduardo. 2006. Racism without Racists: Color-Blind Racism and the Persistence of Racial Inequality in the
United States. New York: Roman and Littlefield.
Bourdieu, Pierre and Jean-Claude Passeron. 1977. Reproduction in Education, Society, and Culture. London, UK: Sage
Publications.
Children’s Defense Fund. 1975. School Suspensions: Are They Helping Children? Cambridge, MA: Washington Research
Project.
Coleman, James S., Ernest Q. Campbell, Carol J. Hobson, James McPartland, Alexander M. Mood, Frederick D.
Weinfield, and Robert L. York. 1966. Equality of Educational Opportunity. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Conchas, Gilberto Q. 2006. The Color of Success: Race and High Achieving Urban Youth. New York: Teachers College
Press.
Condron, Dennis J. 2009. “Social Class, School and Non-School Environments, and Black/White Inequalities in
Children’s Learning.” American Sociological Review 74:683–708.
Condron, Dennis J., Daniel Tope, Christina R. Steidl, and Kendralin J. Freeman. 2012. “Racial Segregation and the
Black-White Achievement Gap, 1992-2009.” The Sociological Quarterly 54:130–57.
Condron, Dennis J. and Vincent J. Roscigno. 2003. “Disparities Within: Unequal Spending and Achievement in an
Urban School District.” Sociology of Education 76:18–36.
Contenbader, Virginia and Samia Markson. 1998. “School Suspension: A Study with Secondary School Students.”
Journal of School Psychology 36:59–82.
Corcoran, Sean P. and William N. Evans. 2008. “The Role of Inequality in Teacher Quality.” Pp. 212–49 in Steady
Gains and Stalled Progress: Inequality and the Black-White Test Score Gap, edited by K. Magnuson and J. Waldfogel.
New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Davis, J. E. and W. J. Jordan. 1994. “The Effects of School Context, Structure, and Experiences on African American
Males in Middle and High Schools.” Journal of Negro Education 63:570–87.
Downey, Douglas B., Paul T. von Hippel, and Beckett A. Broh. 2004. “Are Schools the Great Equalizer? Cognitive
Inequality during the Summer Months and the School Year.” American Sociological Review 69:613–35.
Downey, Douglas B. and Shana Pribesh. 2004. “When Race Matters: Teachers’ Evaluations of Students’ Classroom
Behaviors.” Sociology of Education 77:267–82.
Ekstrom, R. B., M. E. Goertz, J. M. Pollack, and D. A. Rock. 1986. “Who Drops out of High School and Why? Findings
from a National Study.” Teachers College Record 87:356–73.
Ferguson, Ann Arnett. 2000. Bad Boys: Public Schools in the Making of Black Masculinity. Ann Arbor: University of
Michigan Press.
84
Morris and Perry
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Fordham, Signithia and John U. Ogbu. 1986. “Black Students’ School Success: Coping with the Burden of ‘Acting
White’.” Urban Review 18:176–206.
Garland, David. 2001. The Culture of Control: Crime and Social Order in Contemporary Society. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Gregory, Anne, Russell J. Skiba, and Pedro Noguera. 2010. “The Achievement Gap and the Discipline Gap: Two Sides
of the Same Coin?” Educational Researcher 39:59–68.
Grissmer, David and Elizabeth Eiseman. 2008. “Can Gaps in the Quality of Early Environments and Noncognitive
Skills Help Explain Persisting Black-White Achievement Gaps?” Pp. 139–80 in Steady Gains and Stalled Progress:
Inequality and the Black-White Test Score Gap, edited by K. Magnuson and J. Waldfogel. New York: Russell Sage
Foundation.
Harris, Angel L. 2011. Kids Don’t Want to Fail: Oppositional Culture and the Black-White Achievement Gap. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Hedges, Larry V. and Amy Nowell. 1999. “Changes in the Black-White Gap in Achievement Test Scores.” Sociology of
Education 72:111–35.
Hirschfield, Paul. 2008. “Preparing for Prison? The Criminalization of School Discipline in the USA.” Theoretical
Criminology 12:79–101.
Hirschi, Travis. 1969. The Causes of Delinquency. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Hoffman, Stephen. 2014. “Zero Benefit: Estimating the Effect of Zero Tolerance Discipline Polices on Racial
Disparities in School Discipline.” Educational Policy 28:69–95.
Irwin, Katherine, Janet Davidson, and Amanda Hall-Sanchez. 2013. “The Race to Punish in American Schools: Class
and Race Predictors of Punitive School-Crime Control.” Critical Criminology 21:47–71.
Jencks, Christopher and Meredith Phillips, eds. 1998. The Black-White Test Score Gap. Washington, DC: Brookings
Institution Press.
Jenkins, Patricia H. 1997. “School Delinquency and the School Social Bond.” Journal of Research in Crime and
Delinquency 34:337–67.
Kewal Ramani, Angelina, Lauren Gilbertson, Mary Ann Fox, and Stephen Provasnik. 2007. Status and Trends in the
Education of Racial and Ethnic Minorities. Washington, DC: National Center for Educational Statistics. Retrieved
April 11, 2011 (http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid¼2007039).
Kupchik, Aaron. 2010. Homeroom Security: School Discipline in an Age of Fear. New York: New York University Press.
Kupchik, Aaron and Nicholas Ellis. 2008. “School Discipline and Security: Fair for All Students?” Youth & Society
39:549–74.
Kupchik, Aaron and Torin Monahan. 2006. “The New American School: Preparation for Post-Industrial Discipline.”
British Journal of Sociology of Education 27:617–31.
Losen, Daniel J. 2011. Discipline Policies, Successful Schools, and Racial Justice. Boulder, CO: National Education Policy Center.
Losen, Daniel J. and Jonathan Gillespie. 2012. Opportunities Suspended: The Disparate Impact of Disciplinary Exclusion
from School. Los Angeles, CA: The Civil Rights Project.
MacKinnon, D. P. and J. H. Dwyer. 1993. “Estimating Mediated Effects in Prevention Studies.” Evaluation Review
17:144–58.
MacKinnon, D. P., G. Warsi, and J. H. Dwyer. 1995. “A Simulation Study of Mediated Effect Measures.” Multivariate
Behavioral Research 30:41–62.
Magnuson, Katherine and Jane Waldfogel, eds. 2008. Steady Gains and Stalled Progress: Inequality and the Black-White
Test Score Gap. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
McCarthy, John D. and Dean R. Hoge. 1987. “The Social Construction of School Discipline: Racial Disadvantage out
of Universalistic Process.” Social Forces 65:1101–20.
McGrady, Patrick B. and John R. Reynolds. 2013. “Racial Mismatch in the Classroom: Beyond Black-White
Differences.” Sociology of Education 86:3–17.
Mcloughlin, Cavin S. and Amity L. Noltemeyer. 2010. “Research into Factors Contributing to Discipline Use and
Disproportionality in Major Urban Schools.” Current Issues in Education 13(2). Retrieved December 8, 2014
(http://cie.asu.edu/).
Morris, Edward W. 2005. “‘Tuck in that Shirt!’: Race, Class, Gender, and Discipline in an Urban School.” Sociological
Perspectives 48:25–48.
National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). 2014.
2013 Mathematics and Reading Assessments. Retrieved June 12,
2014 (http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/).
Pager, Devah and Hana Shepherd. 2008. “The Sociology of Discrimination: Racial Discrimination in Employment,
Housing, Credit, and Consumer Markets.” Annual Review of Sociology 34:181–209.
The Punishment Gap 85
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Peguero, Anthony A. and Zahra Shekarkhar. 2011. “Latino/a Student Misbehavior and School Punishment.” Hispanic
Journal of Behavioral Sciences 33:54–70.
Perry, Brea L. and Edward W. Morris. 2014. “Suspending Progress: Collateral Consequences of Exclusionary
Punishment in Public Schools.” American Sociological Review 79:1067–87.
Quillian, Lincoln. 2006. “New Approaches to Understanding Racial Prejudice and Discrimination.” Annual Review of
Sociology 32:299–328.
Quiocho, Alice and Francisco Rios. 2000. “The Power of Their Presence: Minority Group Teachers and Schooling.”
Review of Educational Research 70:485–528.
Raudenbush, Stephan W. and Anthony S. Bryk. 2002. Hierarchical Linear Models. 2d ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Rocha, Rene R. and Daniel P. Hawes. 2009. “Racial Diversity, Representative Bureaucracy, and Equity in Multiracial
School Districts.” Social Science Quarterly 90:326–44.
Rocque, Michael. 2010. “Office Discipline and Student Behavior: Does Race Matter?” American Journal of Education
116:557–81.
Rocque, Michael and Raymond Pasternoster. 2011. “Understanding the Antecedents of the School-to-Jail Link: The
Relationship between Race and School Discipline.” The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 101:633–65.
Rumberger, R. and G. Palardy. 2005. “Does Segregation Still Matter? The Impact of Student Composition on
Academic Achievement in High School.” Teachers College Record 107:1999–2045.
Simon, Jonathan. 2007. Governing through Crime: How the War on Crime Transformed American Democracy and Created
a Culture of Fear. New York: Oxford University Press.
Skiba, Russell J. and Reece L. Peterson. 1999. “The Dark Side of Zero Tolerance: Can Punishment Lead to Safe
Schools?” Phi Delta Kappan 80:372–82.
Skiba, Russell J., Robert S. Michael, Abra Carroll Nardo, and Reece L. Peterson. 2002. “The Color of Discipline:
Sources of Racial and Gender Disproportionality in School Punishment.” The Urban Review 34:317–42.
Sobel, M. E. 1986. “Some New Results on Indirect Effects and their Standard Errors in Covariance Structure Models.”
Sociological Methodology 16:159–86
Stanton-Salazar, Ricardo. 1997. “A Social Capital Framework for Understanding the Socialization of Racial Minority
Children and Youths.” Harvard Educational Review 67:1–41.
StataCorp. 2013. Stata Statistical Software: Release 13. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.
Tyson, Karolyn. 2011. Integration Interrupted: Tracking, Black Students, and Acting White after Brown. New York:
Oxford University Press.
U.S. Department of Education. 2007. Household Education Surveys Program. National Center for Education Statistics.
Retrieved September 2, 2014 (http://nces.ed.gov/nhes/index.asp)
—. 2014. Guiding Principles: A Resource Guide for Improving School Climate and Discipline. Retrieved June 6, 2014
(www2.ed.gov/policy/gen/guid/school-discipline/guiding-principles.pdf.)
Verdugo, Richard R. 2002. “Race-Ethnicity, Social Class, and Zero-Tolerance Policies: The Cultural and Structural
Wars.” Education and Urban Society 35:50–75.
Vigdor, Jacob L. and Jens Ludwig. 2008. “Segregation and the Test Score Gap.” Pp. 181–211 in Steady Gains and
Stalled Progress: Inequality and the Black-White Test Score Gap, edited by K. Magnuson and J. Waldfogel. New York:
Russell Sage Foundation.
Wacquant, Loic. 2001. “Deadly Symbiosis: When Ghetto and Prison Meet and Mesh.” Punishment and Society 3:95–134.
Wallace, John M., Sara Goodkind, Cynthia M. Wallace, and Jerald G. Bachman. 2008. Racial, Ethnic, and Gender
Differences in School Discipline among U.S. High School Students: 1991-2005.” The Negro Educational Review 59:47–62.
Welch, Kelly and Allison Ann Payne. 2010. “Racial Threat and Punitive School Discipline.” Social Problems 57:25–48.
Wildeman, Christopher. 2009. “Parental Imprisonment, the Prison Boom, and the Concentration of Childhood
Disadvantage.” Demography 46:265–80.
86
Morris and Perry
by guest on January 20, 2016http://socpro.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
... The Office for Civil Rights in the U.S. Department of Education defines exclusionary discipline as "the formal or informal removal, whether on a short-term or long-term basis, of a student from a class, school, or other educational program or activity for violating a school rule or code of conduct…includ [ing] detentions, in-school suspensions, out-of-school suspensions, suspensions from riding the school bus, expulsions, disciplinary transfers to alternative schools, referrals to law enforcement, and school related arrests" (2). Since 1990, decades of data and research have documented the negative outcomes linked to the receipt of these types of school discipline (3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)(10). Within the past 15 years, the American Psychological Association (9) and the American Academy of Pediatrics (8) have released reports against the broad and frequent use of exclusionary school discipline, citing the association between these types of discipline and poor academic outcomes, the link between these types of discipline and arrest and incarceration, and the ineffectiveness of these disciplinary policies in creating a safer school environment. ...
... Furthermore, a vast array of evidence demonstrates that these punishments are not equally applied: students with marginalized identities and from racial/ ethnic minoritized groups are disproportionately affected (e.g., Huang, 2020). These inequities account for a significant proportion of the Black-White academic achievement gap and contribute to a "school-to-prison" pipeline in which these students are pushed out of schools and into incarceration systems (Morris & Perry, 2016;Schiff, 2018). Similarly, zero-tolerance punitive approaches in Western legal systems (particularly the United States) have led to large numbers of citizens from minoritized groups ending up in jail or prison (Beckett & Kartman, 2016;Pettit & Gutierrez, 2018). ...
Chapter
Building and maintaining peace across various levels requires engaging with ideas about justice and fairness. Drawing on Johan Galtung’s tripartite conceptualization of peace, the construct of justice/fairness plays a key role in ending cycles of direct violence, as well as undoing structural and cultural violence. These connections are rooted in individual and group psychologies reflecting how justice and fairness are understood and experienced. In turn, these cognitive lenses are given meaning through the ways we learn, are socialized, and actively build understandings as children and youth. In this chapter, we discuss justice and fairness as character strengths linked to peace by exploring the lessons and insights offered by moral development. We also specifically discuss school-based restorative justice as a framework that aligns with this developmental lens. Specifically, it can offer young people opportunities to engage in meaning making: actively building nuanced and prosocial bases for justice and fairness that feed into broader positive peace in communities and societies. The chapter ends with a summary of the intersections between character strengths, peace, justice, fairness, development, and education.
... For instance, greater use of exclusionary discipline has been shown not to deter classroom disruptions or student misbehavior or improve school climate (Perry and Morris 2014;Raffaele Mendez 2003;Skiba 2000). Moreover, removing students from their learning environments through exclusionary discipline has been associated with a range of adverse academic outcomes, such as a reduction in standardized test scores, graduation rates, educational matriculation, and postsecondary enrollment (Balfanz, Byrnes, and Fox 2015;Christle, Jolivette, and Nelson 2007;Fabelo et al. 2011;Kinsler 2013;Morris and Perry 2016;Raffaele Mendez 2003). ...
Article
School suspension has been linked to numerous adverse social outcomes in adolescence and adulthood. While research continues to highlight the deleterious consequences tied to suspension, less is known about how exposure to suspension influences health over the life course. Drawing from panel data, we address this gap by investigating whether school suspension is associated with self-rated health from adolescence to midlife. Compared to youth with no history of suspension, suspended youth were more likely to report poorer health in adolescence. Findings also demonstrate that school suspension plays a significant role in self-rated health patterns over time. Specifically, we find suspended youth experience a persistent health gap in excellent health from adolescence to midlife and a more rapid acceleration of fair and poor health from their late 20s to midlife. Taken together, this study provides new evidence of the role of exclusionary discipline in shaping health disparities from adolescence to midlife.
Article
Full-text available
This study explores the academic experiences and success of Black girls and women, providing a shift in discourse from highlighting perceived weaknesses of Black girls and women as the problem to a more strengths-based approach that identifies and elaborates on the factors contributing to their academic achievements. This research also offers actionable strategies for enhancing support for Black girls and women in educational environments. Grounded in a retrospective constructivist grounded theory, the methodology enables an in-depth exploration of the educational trajectories of Black women who have achieved doctoral degrees while also considering the author's role as an active participant in the research process. Through intensive interviews with 12 Black women across various disciplines, this study uncovers how parental influence and psychosocial and interpersonal factors contribute to academic success. This research enriches the current academic discourse on the topic and offers insights and, ultimately, a conceptual framework that can inform practices and policies to enhance Black girls and women's educational experiences across their academic journeys. This article offers a theoretical framework for studying academic success among Black women in the United States of America. A more holistic representation of factors that lead to academic persistence and success among this group is vital, as current studies mainly emphasize educational barriers that lead to dropout (Spady, 1970; Tinto, 1975). Second, current academic theories fail to apply an intersectional lens to recognize how students' unique, interconnecting social identities inform their social and educational experiences in varied school settings, especially how they encounter and overcome multiple forms of oppression (Ricks, 2014; Evans-Winters, 2010). To address these gaps, this article first includes a brief review of literature that explores the gendered racial academic experiences of Black girls and women. Then, this article will present a more holistic model of academic success rooted in the lived experiences of Black women from early childhood to doctoral education.
Article
Full-text available
To link to this article: https://repository.usfca.edu/be/ The Black Educology Mixtape is an open-access mixtape that moves beyond academic articles to feature various art forms and voices that are typically muted. We feature a collective of Black people working to amplify and empower Black educational voices. Our scope and sequence focus on the past, present, and future of Black education, which has been historically and systemically caught in the underbelly of western education. Our work is grounded in creating mixtapes that are both revolutionary and emancipatory in the name of love, study, struggle, and refusal. This is an Open Access article, which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way. The terms on which this article has been published allow the posting of the Accepted Manuscript in a repository by the author(s) or with their consent.
Article
Joining the chorus of Black/Critical Race Feminist scholarship in education, this article explores how both affect and visuality inform understandings of safety for Black girls in schools. Using participatory visual ethnography, I explore how five young Black women at a Philadelphia magnet school conceptualize safety as an affective achievement of visionary school design. I argue that any attempt to address safety for Black girls in urban schools must include attention to the visual school culture and the affective dimensions of the school environment to fully account for the ways these dimensions counter or enable further physical, emotional, or spiritual harm.
Article
Culturally responsive student–teacher relationships are critical to the academic success and social‐emotional learning (SEL) outcomes of students of color and/or students from low‐income backgrounds. Yet, little is known about how students’ perceptions of their schools’ culturally responsive environment are related to the quality of student–teacher relationships. To address this gap, the current study examined how students’ perceptions of their school's culturally responsive environment contributes to positive student–teacher developmental relationships, and in turn, their academic motivation and SEL. A total of 2,069 public middle and high school students of color from 46 schools across the country were surveyed (51.4% female; M age = 13.4, SD = 1.86; 30.7% Hispanic or Latina/o/x, 30.4% Multiracial or Multiethnic, 17.5% Black or African American, 12.4% Asian or Pacific Islander, 8.7% American Indian or Alaska Native; 0.4% identified as another race or ethnicity). Students’ perceptions of their school as a culturally responsive environment were both directly and indirectly associated with greater academic motivation and SEL via stronger student–teacher developmental relationships. Findings have important implications for school administrators’ and school psychologists’ efforts to create more culturally responsive education environments.
Article
It is well documented that Black children are disproportionately suspended and expelled in schools, but less explored is the importance of place in shaping disciplinary decision-making and student experiences of punishment. Drawing on over a year of ethnographic work at Abundance Junior High School (AJHS), a predominantly Black school of 150 students, I argue that school discipline in the rural northeast Louisiana Delta is shaped by racial, spatial, and social dimensions of place. Specifically, I argue that treatment and punishment of Black children in schools is shaped by (1) cultural expectations that they remain in “a child’s place,” a social position characterized by obedience and deference, and (2) socialization reinforcing respectability and compliance with authority informed by historical and contemporary threats of racial violence and Louisiana’s carceral logics. I conclude by discussing how these disciplinary and socialization practices, while motivated in part by the desire to protect Black children, ultimately reproduce forms of social control and harm students.
Article
This review starts from the premise that racial inequality has become a normalized backdrop within U.S. education, in many ways following the prevailing racial hierarchy in society. While enduring racial stratification produces numerous educational disparities, I argue that these disparities, in turn, contribute to reinforcing the symbolic meanings associated with racial categories. Put simply, racial inequality in education is both a consequence and a cause of race itself. I review these “predictable” patterns of racial inequality in education, distilling some of the nuances that are most frequently misunderstood and summarizing trends through recent data. Next, I examine explanations for racial inequality in education, debunking some persistent misconceptions. Then I synthesize the patterns and explanations with literature highlighting how racial inequality in education re‐constructs durable notions of racial categories and racialization. All three of these areas have produced significant scholarship, but they are seldom fully in conversation with one another. Finally, I discuss the role of race and racism in the latest politicization of schools to argue that it can be understood as a new wave in the colorblind era, with significant implications for the social construction of whiteness and the potential to stall progress toward reducing racial inequality in education.
Book
Across America today gated communities sprawl out from urban centers, employers enforce mandatory drug testing, and schools screen students with metal detectors. Social problems ranging from welfare dependency to educational inequality have been reconceptualized as crimes, with an attendant focus on assigning fault and imposing consequences. Even before the recent terrorist attacks, non-citizen residents had become subject to an increasingly harsh regime of detention and deportation, and prospective employees subjected to background checks. How and when did our everyday world become dominated by fear, every citizen treated as a potential criminal? In this startlingly original work, Jonathan Simon traces this pattern back to the collapse of the New Deal approach to governing during the 1960s when declining confidence in expert-guided government policies sent political leaders searching for new models of governance. The War on Crime offered a ready solution to their problem: politicians set agendas by drawing analogies to crime and redefined the ideal citizen as a crime victim, one whose vulnerabilities opened the door to overweening government intervention. By the 1980s, this transformation of the core powers of government had spilled over into the institutions that govern daily life. Soon our schools, our families, our workplaces, and our residential communities were being governed through crime. This powerful work concludes with a call for passive citizens to become engaged partners in the management of risk and the treatment of social ills. Only by coming together to produce security, can we free ourselves from a logic of domination by others, and from the fear that currently rules our everyday life.
Article
Using the most comprehensive data set on school dropouts that we have to date, the High School and Beyond study, Ruth Ekstrom, Margaret Goertz, Judith Pollack, and Donald Rock provide an analysis of the salient characteristics of the dropout population.
Article
Seriousness of purpose in seeking to avert the tragedy of school violence does not necessarily demand rigid adherence to harsh and extreme measures. There are alternatives to politically facile get-tough strategies, the authors point out.
Article
Disparities in educational outcomes between African Americans and whites declined steadily for most of the twentieth century, but this progress has halted or even reversed in recent years (Neal 2006). Understanding why the black-white test score gap narrowed over time, and why this progress stalled during the 1990s, is critical if we are to design policies capable of further reducing inequality in schooling outcomes in the United States. Given the widely documented association between educational outcomes and earnings, health and crime, successful efforts to further reduce the gap would undoubtedly have far-reaching consequences for society as a whole. This chapter considers the role of school and neighborhood segregation in explaining trends in the black-white schooling gap. As we will discuss, provocative time-series and cross-sectional evidence points to strong associations between segregation and achievement gaps. The break in trend toward narrower test score gaps coincided with another toward greater school integration. Cross-sectionally, states with more segregated schools also tend to have wider gaps. It is not clear, however, whether causal interpretations should be attached to these correlations. Social science has struggled mightily to produce definitive proof that segregation and racial disparities are causally linked, and the available evidence-backed by eminently plausible theoretical arguments and proposed mechanisms-has influenced policy debates for decades. Key to the Supreme Court's transformational decision fifty years ago in Brown v. Board of Education is the assumption that racial segregation within the public schools contributes to black-white inequality in schooling outcomes. Attending a disproportionately minority school might affect both the motivation of students and their perceptions of the larger opportunity structure they face in society and their exposure to high-quality school resources or even the academic climate in the school. It is possible that the race-ethnicity of one's schoolmates might simply be a stand-in for their academic achievement level, which could affect the way or rate at which teachers present material or the productivity of student study groups, or as a stand-in for their socioeconomic status (SES). In fact, the influential report by James Coleman in the mid-1960s argued that a school's SES composition was at least as important in explaining inequality in student achievement as is school racial composition (Coleman et al. 1966). Understanding the distinct influences of school racial versus social class composition is relevant because some policies focused on reducing school racial segregation may not have very large impacts on school socioeconomic composition, and vice versa. Measures of school segregation by either race or social class could matter in largest part because they are proxies for the racial or class composition of the local neighborhood. Because most children attend local public schools, in national data there will be a great deal of cross-sectional correlation between school and neighborhood measures of race or SES segregation. The neighborhood social environment could matter above and beyond the composition of the local public school by shaping the youth social norms that help shape children's behavior, particularly given that so much socializing and exposure to local role models occur outside of school (Wilson 1987). Understanding the distinct influences of school versus neighborhood environments is important for policy because policies such as public or private school choice have the potential to change school but not neighborhood social compositions, and some housing mobility interventions might generate larger changes in neighborhood than school characteristics (see, for example, Sanbonmatsu et al. 2006). Figure 5.1 presents a basic conceptual framework outlining the potential causal mechanisms linking segregation to test score gaps. Neighborhood segregation could affect these outcomes directly or through its influence on school segregation. Direct links between neighborhood and test scores might be through neighborhood-level deviant peer influence, role model effects, or impacts on parental income derived from spatial mismatch-type effects. Direct links between school segregation and test scores could be mediated by differences in school input quality, or by peer influence operating at the school or classroom level. Thinking carefully about the particular mechanism through which social environment affects children is also important in understanding patterns in black-white student outcomes because different measures of segregation have been following different trajectories in recent years. Neighborhood racial and economic segregation actually declined during the 1990s, though these national trends mask important differences by region and so do not necessarily imply that neighborhood segregation is not relevant for understanding the slowdown in narrowing of the blackwhite test score gap (Glaeser and Vigdor 2003; Jargowsky 2003). Controversy continues about whether school segregation has increased or decreased over this same period. Increases reported in some studies (for example, Clotfelter 2004; Orfield and Eaton 1996) confound the increasing diversity of the student body in American public schools with increases in the separation of blacks from students of other races (Logan 2004). Measures more commonly accepted in the sociological and economic literature on segregation indicate very slight decreases in the degree of segregation over the past two decades. Regardless of measure, it is clear that school segregation has declined more slowly than neighborhood segregation over the same time period. The failure of school segregation to track neighborhood segregation reflects a broad decline in governmental efforts to integrate public schools (Orfield and Eaton 1996; Clotfelter, Ladd, and Vigdor 2005). In our judgment, the evidence linking school segregation to student achievement is stronger than evidence supporting a direct causal role for neighborhood segregation. The current evidence implicating school race segregation certainly outweighs the available research on the effects of school segregation by social class, though the specific mechanisms through which racial composition affects student outcomes remain poorly understood. The best available evidence suggests that a 10 percent increase in the black share of a school's student body would reduce achievement test scores for black students by between 0.025 and around 0.08 standard deviations, and reduce test scores for whites by perhaps one-quarter to two-fifths as much. Thus, if school segregation had displayed a decrease in the 1990s commensurate with the observed decrease in neighborhood segregation, our best estimate is that the black-white test score gap would be roughly 0.01 to 0.02 standard deviation narrower.
Article
Alarge body of research in social science has been directed toward addressing the causes of and solutions to continuing inequality of outcomes between black and white Americans (for recent summaries, see Neckerman 2004; Jencks and Phillips 1998). Historically, this research has created an expectation that such gaps would close over generations in a competitive economy if educational and labor market opportunities were equalized. The research, however, has been unable to account for several key aspects of the empirical data-specifically, to explain why progress is so slow, why there can be periods of either rapid or no progress, and why substantial gaps might remain. In this chapter, we examine three factors that might help explain these more complex dynamics in the empirical data: • Recognizing that early childhood environments create much of the achievement gap and may set limits on later achievement, educational attainment and labor force outcomes • Recognizing that the behavior and proximal processes that underlie early cognitive development may be different for racial-ethnic groups, and these differences may not be strongly linked to differences in commonly used surrogate variables (income, parent education), and may not change even as social and economic equality improves • Recognizing that a narrow focus on cognitive achievement measures rather than a broader focus on noncognitive skills such as behavioral, emotional, social and motor skills can leave persisting gaps because long-term educational attainment and labor force outcomes depend not only on cognitive skills, but also on noncognitive skills ■ some of these noncognitive skills may be implicit cognitive skills,1 that is, linked to the development or performance of cognitive skills The identification of these factors is done through reviews of recent literature and empirical estimations with the Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey of Kindergartners (ECLS-K). The major contributions from the literature are related to the interactive genetic and environmental mechanisms driving development from conception to kindergarten entrance, the emerging importance of noncognitive skills in explaining educational and labor market outcomes, and identifying implicit cognitive skills. Historically, researchers assumed that achievement gaps emerge during schooling through inequality of schools and family characteristics, and looked to the equalization of schooling opportunity as a major policy lever. However, research now shows that a substantial share of the gap is present at school entry, and that school equalization may therefore not fully close score gaps (Lee and Burkam 2002; Fryer and Levitt 2004, 2006).2 Research also suggests that closing these gaps may be more difficult at later ages (Heckman and Masterov 2007; Cunha et al. 2006) and that some intense early environments may limit later cognitive development even in the best of later circumstances (O'Connor, Rutter, and Beckett 2000; Beckett et al. 2006). Eliminating gaps before school entrance now appears to be critical to achieving social and economic equality in adulthood. This task may be even more challenging than equalizing schooling, given the numerous, diverse, and complex influences on child development in the early years. The behaviors and proximal processes that underlie the causative mechanisms in early cognitive development may not be adequately captured by the traditional surrogate variables focusing on family and neighborhood characteristics. Research suggests that interactions between genes and these behaviors and processes may account for much variance making their identification and measurement difficult (Rutter 2002; Shonkoff and Phillips 2000). Yet it will likely be in the discovery of these behaviors and processes and the possible differences across groups that will lead to the knowledge required for efficient early interventions to eliminate gaps. Finally, an implicit assumption in achieving social equality has been that cognitive development plays the dominant role in predicting gaps in later, long-term outcomes. However, noncognitive skills such as social, motor, emotional, and behavioral skills are now also known to affect educational attainment and other long-term outcomes and are receiving increasing attention in early development (Bowles, Gintis, and Osborne 2001, 2002; Heckman and Rubinstein 2001; Heckman, Stixrud, and Urzua 2006; Borghaus, Duckworth, and Heckman forthcoming). In fact, such noncognitive skills may directly affect more than longer-term outcomes. Developmental research and the evidence in this chapter suggest that some noncognitive skills may also be implicitly linked to development and cognitive skills at kindergarten entrance (Diamond 2000; Duncan et al. 2007; Raver, Garner, and Smith-Donald 2007; Snow 2007; Blair et al. 2007; Diamond et al. 2007). Historically, educational policy makers and researchers who have studied schooling effects have focused on cognitive skills as measured by achievement scores as the most important and often as their sole measure of interest. Such focus might help explain persisting score gaps if noncognitive skills also influence later, long-term outcomes. Developmental researchers studying early childhood have included both cognitive and noncognitive skills in their research, but the specific links between cognitive and noncognitive skills are not yet well understood, and persisting score gaps are possible until these links become better known.
Article
Taken together, there are few observable teacher characteristics that have consistently shown evidence of large and systematic effects on student achievement on standardized tests. The metric offering the most compelling evidence-a teacher's academic aptitude-is seldom available to researchers, particularly those interested in drawing inferences about changes in teacher quality over time.13 Yet each of the attributes cited has been found in at least one rigorous empirical study to have an important effect-if sometimes small-on student achievement. Several observable qualifications or traits-such as teaching experience, content knowledge, NBPTS certification, and race-more often than not show systematic effects on student outcomes. Other qualifications such as advanced degrees may matter more for the academic achievement of black students than for white. Beyond the independent effects of individual teacher characteristics on student learning, there is only limited evidence on how teacher attributes interact or cumulate in the production of education. It may be that students exposed to teachers lacking along multiple dimensions fall behind more than those whose teachers fall short on only one qualification. Further, repeated exposure to inexperienced or underqualified teachers may have cumulative effects, where differences that appear small in any one year of achievement growth compound into a much larger inequalities in acquired skills over time (Sanders and Rivers 1996). In the following section, we consider how a wide range of teacher attributes varies across schools and classrooms of varying racial compositions, and how the distribution of teachers over students has changed over time.