ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The brain as a highly dynamically organized structure can change and adapt as a result of activities and demands imposed by the environment. Musical activity has proven to be a powerful stimulus for this kind of brain adaptation, or brain plasticity. This chapter suggests that music-induced brain plasticity may produce benefits for wellbeing in general and may influence neurohormonal status as well as cognitive and emotional processes in healthy and diseased individuals, helping to improve various sensory, motor, coordinative, or emotional disabilities. It first reviews mechanisms of musicinduced brain plasticity. It then clarifies the impact of music on emotion and neurohormones. It demonstrates the transfer effects of music exposure and making music to other cognitive and emotional domains, and shows examples of the potential of music to serve as a supportive and facilitative therapy in rehabilitation from motor impairment and aphasia following brain injury.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This chapter was originally published in the book Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 217 published by
Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the
author's institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in
instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who know you, and providing a copy to
your institution’s administrator.
All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or
licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or
repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's
permissions site at:
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial
From Eckart Altenmüller and Gottfried Schlaug, Apollo's gift: new aspects of neurologic music therapy.
In: Eckart Altenmüller, Stanley Finger and François Boller, editors, Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 217,
Amsterdam: Elsevier, 2015, pp. 237-252.
ISBN: 978-0-444-63551-8
© Copyright 2015 Elsevier B.V.
Elsevier
Provided for non
-
commercial research and educational use only.
Not for reproduction, distribution or com
mercial use.
CHAPTER
Apollo’s gift: new aspects
of neurologic music
therapy 12
Eckart Altenm
uller*, Gottfried Schlaug
†,1
*Institute of Music Physiology and Musicians’ Medicine (IMMM), University of Music, Drama and
Media, Hanover, Lower Saxony, Germany
Department of Neurology, Music and Neuroimaging Laboratory, and Neuroimaging, Stroke
Recovery Laboratories, Division of Cerebrovascular Disease, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical
Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
1
Corresponding author: Tel.: +1-617-6328912; Fax: +1-617-6328920,
e-mail address: gschlaug@bidmc.harvard.edu
Abstract
Music listening and music making activities are powerful tools to engage multisensory and
motor networks, induce changes within these networks, and foster links between distant,
but functionally related brain regions with continued and life-long musical practice. These
multimodal effects of music together with music’s ability to tap into the emotion and reward
system in the brain can be used to facilitate and enhance therapeutic approaches geared toward
rehabilitating and restoring neurological dysfunctions and impairments of an acquired or con-
genital brain disorder. In this article, we review plastic changes in functional networks and
structural components of the brain in response to short- and long-term music listening and mu-
sic making activities. The specific influence of music on the developing brain is emphasized
and possible transfer effects on emotional and cognitive processes are discussed. Furthermore,
we present data on the potential of using musical tools and activities to support and facilitate
neurorehabilitation. We will focus on interventions such as melodic intonation therapy and
music-supported motor rehabilitation to showcase the effects of neurologic music therapies
and discuss their underlying neural mechanisms.
Keywords
brain plasticity, melodic intonation therapy, music-supported training, neurologic music
therapy, neurorehabilitation
1MUSIC AS A DRIVER OF BRAIN PLASTICITY
Apollo’s gift, music, is one of the richest human emotional, sensory-motor, and cog-
nitive experiences. It involves listening, watching, feeling, moving and coordinating,
remembering, and expecting musical elements. It is frequently accompanied by strong
Progress in Brain Research, Volume 217, ISSN 0079-6123, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2014.11.029
©2015 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
237
Author's personal copy
emotions resulting in joy, happiness, and bittersweet sadness or even in overwhelming
bodily reactions like tears in the eyes or shivers down the spine. A large number of
cortical and subcortical brain regions are involved in music listening and music
making activities (for reviews see Altenm
uller and McPherson, 2007; Tramo, 2001).
Primary and secondary regions in the cerebral cortex are critical for any conscious
perception of sensory information, be it auditory, visual, or somatosensory. However,
music also influences and changes activity in multisensory and motor integration
regions in frontal, parietal, and temporo-occipital brain regions. The frontal lobe is
involved in the guidance of attention, in planning and motor preparation, in integr ating
auditory and motor information, and in specific human skills such as imitation and
empathy. The two latter play an important role in the acquisition of musical skills
and emotional expressiveness. Multisensory integration regions in the parietal lobe
and temporo-occipital areas integrate different sensory inputs from the auditory,
visual, and somatosensory system into a combined sensory impression; it is this mul-
tisensory brain representation, which constitutes the typical musical experience. The
cerebellum is another important part of the brain that plays a critical role in musical
experience. It is important for motor coordination, but it also plays an important role in
various cognitive tasks especially when they include demands on timing. Typically,
the cerebellum is activated in rhythm processing, or tapping in synchrony with an
external pacemaker such as a metronome. Finally, the emotional network (comprising
the basis and the inner surfaces of the two frontal lobes, the cingulate gyrus and brain
structures in the evolutionarily old parts of the brain such as the amygdala, the hippo-
campus, and the midbrain) is crucial for the emotional perception of music and there-
fore for an individual’s motivation to listen to or to engage in any musical activity.
The brain is a highly dynamically organized structure that changes and adapts as a
result of activities and demands imposed upon it by the environment. Musical activ-
ity has proven to be a powerful stimulus for this kind of brain adaptation, or brain
plasticity (Wan and Schlaug, 2010). Effects of plasticity are not restricted to musical
prodigies, they occur in children learning to play a musical instrument (Hyde et al.,
2009) and in adult amateur musicians (Bangert and Altenm
uller, 2003), albeit to a
lesser extent. Thus, with the main topic of our article in mind, we suggest that brain
plasticity induced through making music may produce manifold benefits. This holds
not only for changing and/or restoring compromised sensorimotor brain networks,
but also for influencing neurohormonal status as well as cognitive and emotional pro-
cesses in healthy and neurologically diseased/disordered individuals. Thus, various
sensory-motor, coordinative, or emotional disabilities can be improved with music-
supported therapy (MST).
This chapter briefly reviews the literature on music-induced brain plasticity, its
underlying mechanisms, and the impact that music has on emotion and neurohor-
mones. Subsequently, we will demonstrate transfer effects of music exposure and
music making to other cognitive and emotional domains and finally show examples
of the potential of music making to support and facilitate neurorehabilitation.
Our intent is not to provide an exhaustive review, but to focus our chapter on
music-supported interventions geared toward improving the rehabilitation of speech-
and limb-motor impairments following brain injury.
238 CHAPTER 12 Apollo’s gift
Author's personal copy
2SOME MECHANISMS OF MUSIC-INDUCED BRAIN
PLASTICITY
During the past decade, brain imaging has provided important insight into the enor-
mous capacity of the human brain to adapt to complex demands. These adaptations
are referred to as brain plasticity and do not only include the quality and extent of
functional connections of brain networks, but also fine structures of nervous tissue
and even the macroscopic gross structure of brain anatomy (Bangert and Schlaug,
2006). Brain plasticity is best observed in complex tasks, including, for example,
temporospatially precise movements with high behavioral relevance. These behav-
iors are usually accompanied by emotional arousal and motivational activation of the
reward system. Furthermore, plastic changes are more pronounced when the specific
activities have started before puberty and require intense training. Obviously, con-
tinued musical activities throughout the life of a musician provide an ideal setup for
brain plasticity to occur. It is therefore not surprising that the most dramatic effects of
brain plasticity have been demonstrated in professional musicians (for a classic re-
view see M
unte et al., 2002; for more recent reviews Wan and Schlaug, 2010; and
Altenm
uller and Schlaug, 2012, 2013).
Our understanding of the molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying these
adaptations is far from complete. Brain plasticity may occur on different time scales.
For example, the efficiency and size of synapses may be modified in a time window
of seconds to minutes, the growth of new synapses and dendrites may require hours
to days. Other changes require up to several weeks. They include an increase in gray
matter density, reflecting either an enlargement of neurons, a change in synaptic den-
sity, more support structures such as capillaries and glial cells or a reduced rate of
physiological cell death (apoptosis). White-matter density also changes as a conse-
quence of musical training. This effect seems to be primarily due to an enlargement
of myelin cells: The myelin cells, wrapped around the nerve fibers (axons) are con-
tributing essentially to the velocity of the electrical impulses traveling along the
nerve fiber tracts. Under conditions requiring rapid information transfer and high
temporal precision, these myelin cells are growing, and, as a consequence, nerve con-
duction velocity will increase. The axons within these myelin sheaths can potentially
sprout and form more and new connections particularly between the nodal cortical
points that are connected by white tracts (for a recent publication on this see Wan
et al., 2014). Finally, brain regions involved in specific tasks may also be enlarged
after long-term training due to the growth of structures supporting the nervous func-
tion, for example, blood vessels/capillaries that are necessary for the oxygen and glu-
cose transportation sustaining nervous function or glial cells as supporters of the
local homeostasis.
Comparison of the brain anatomy of skilled musicians with that of nonmusicians
shows that prolonged instrumental practice leads to an enlargement of the hand area
in the motor cortex (Amunts et al., 1997). Furthermore, Gaser and Schlaug (2003)
could demonstrate enhancement of gray matter density in cortical sensory-motor
2392 Some mechanisms of music-induced brain plasticity
Author's personal copy
regions, auditory regions, the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and in the cerebel-
lum in professional instrumentalists as compared to nonmusicians and amateurs. In-
terestingly, these plastic adaptations depend on critical periods: musicians, who start
early, before the age of seven do not display these structural adaptations of the brain
at least in the sensory-motor cortices and the callosal fibers, however, they seem to
have an “early optimized network,” which allows superior performance of motor
tasks without enlarged anatomical structures (Steele et al., 2013;Vaquero et al.,
2014). In contrast, relatively late starters, after age seven do show the above-
mentioned structural adaptations accounting for the effects observed in many mor-
phological brain imaging studies (e.g., Bangert and Schlaug, 2006; Ga
¨rtner et al.,
2013).
With respect to the larger callosal body in musicians, it seems plausible to assume
that the high demands on coordination between the two hands, and the rapid ex-
change of information may either stimulate the nerve fiber growth—the myelination
of nerve fibers that determines the velocity of nerve conduction—or prevent the
physiological loss of nerve tissue during the typical pruning processes of adolescence
or during aging. These between-group differences in the midsagittal size of the cor-
pus callosum were confirmed in a longitudinal study comparing a group of children
learning to play musical instruments versus a group of children without instrumental
music experience (Hyde et al., 2009).
Halwani et al. (2011) recently showed another impressive adaptation of white-
matter structures. They reported differences in macrostructure and microstructure
of the arcuate fasciculus (AF)—a prominent white-matter tract connecting temporal
and frontal brain regions—between singers, instrumentalists, and nonmusicians.
Both groups of musicians had higher tract volumes in the right dorsal and ventral
tracts compared to nonmusicians, but did not show a significant difference between
each other. Singers had higher tract volume and different microstructures of the tract
on the left side when compared to instrumental musicians and nonmusicians. This
suggests that the right-hemisphere AF might show a more general effect of music
making, while the left-hemisphere AF has a stronger response to the specific aspects
of vocal-motor training and control that singers engage in. Microstructural parame-
ters (i.e., fractional anisotropy) of the left dorsal branch of the arcuate fasciculus
correlated with the number of years of participants’ vocal training, suggesting that
long-term vocal-motor training might not only lead to an increase in volume, but also
to an increase in microstructural complexity of specific white-matter tracts consti-
tuting the so-called aural–oral loop and connecting regions that are fundamental
to sound perception, production, and its feed forward and feedback control. Simi-
larly, Bengtsson et al. (2005) and recently Rueber et al. (2013) have found structural
differences in the corticospinal tract, particularly in the posterior limb of the internal
capsule, between musicians and nonmusicians as well as within musicians groups
(keyboard players compared to string players). Between group differences were re-
lated to measures of training intensity as well as to the specific requirements of the
instruments played. It is worth to mention that instrumental training does not only
affect cortical regions, but subcortical structures also seem to show adaptation.
240 CHAPTER 12 Apollo’s gift
Author's personal copy
For example, professional musicians in comparison to matched nonmusicians, seem
to have a larger cerebellar volume or cerebellar gray matter (Gaser and Schlaug,
2003; Hutchinson et al., 2003). The cerebellum plays a role in the precise timing,
accuracy, and coordination of motor actions, which is an important aspect of instru-
mental music activities.
In summary, instrumental music training, particularly when it starts at a young
age, leads to plastic adaptations of various cortical and subcortical brain structures
and functional networks. These changes can also include enlarged cortical represen-
tations of, for example, specific fingers or specific sounds or timbre of sounds (e.g., a
string timbre vs. a brass timbre) within existing brain structures.
3THE ROLE OF MUSIC-INDUCED EMOTIONS FOR BRAIN
PLASTICITY
An intriguing question is why music is such a powerful driver of beneficial brain
plasticity. This brings us to the specific motivational and emotional role of musical
experience. Emotional responses to music are often cited when people describe why
they value music and why they ascribe certain effects of music on health. Music is
known to have a wide range of physiological effects on the human body including,
for example, changes in heart rate, respiration, blood pressure, skin conductivity,
skin temperature, muscle tension, and biochemical responses (for a review see
Hodges, 2010; Kreutz et al., 2012).
Joyful musical behaviors, for example, learning to play a musical instrument or to
sing is characterized by curiosity, stamina, and the ability to strive for rewarding ex-
periences in future. This results in incentive goal directed activities over prolonged
time periods, which are mainly mediated by the transmitter substance dopamine.
Most nerve cells sensitive to this neurotransmitter are found in a small part of the
brain, which is localized behind the basis of the frontal cortex, the so-called meso-
limbic system, an important part of the “emotional” brain. Dopamine plays a dom-
inant role in the neurobiology of reward, learning, and addiction. Virtually all drugs
of abuse, including heroin, alcohol, cocaine, and nicotine activate dopaminergic sys-
tems. So-called natural rewards such as musical experiences and other positive social
interactions likewise activate dopaminergic neurons and are powerful aids to atten-
tion and learning (Keitz et al., 2003). There is ample evidence that the sensitivity to
dopamine in the mesolimbic brain regions is largely genetically determined resulting
in the enormous variability in reward-dependent behavior. The genetic
“polymorphism” of dopaminergic response explains the different motivational
drives we observe in children with a similar social and educational background. It
is intriguing that there is a strong link of dopaminergic activity to learning and mem-
ory, which in turn promote plastic adaptations in brain areas involved in the tasks to
be learned.
Serotonin is another neurotransmitter important for music-induced brain plastic-
ity. It is commonly associated with feelings of satisfaction from expected outcomes,
2413 The role of music-induced emotions for brain plasticity
Author's personal copy
whereas dopamine is associated with feelings of pleasure based on novelty or new-
ness. In a study of neurochemical responses to pleasant and unpleasant music, sero-
tonin levels were significantly higher when subjects were exposed to music they
found pleasing (Evers and Suhr, 2000). In another study with subjects exposed to
pleasing music, functional and effective connectivity analyses showed that listening
to music strongly modulated activity in a network of mesolimbic structures involved
in reward processing including the dopaminergic nucleus accumbens and the ventral
tegmental area, as well as the hypothalamus and insula. This network is believed to
be involved in regulating autonomic and physiological responses to rewarding and
emotional stimuli (Menon and Levitin, 2005).
Blood and Zatorre (2001) determined changes in regional cerebral blood flow
(rCBF) with PET technology during intense emotional experiences involving sensa-
tions such as goose bumps or shivers down the spine whilst listening to music. Each
participant listened to a piece of their own favorite music to which they usually had a
chill experience. Increasing chill intensity correlated with rCBF decrease in
the amygdala as well as the anterior hippocampal formation. An increase in rCBF
correlating with increasing chill intensity was observed in the ventral striatum,
the midbrain, the anterior insula, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the orbitofrontal
cortex: Again, these latter brain regions are related to reward and positive emotional
valence.
In a newer study by the same group, the neurochemical specificity of [(11)C]
raclo-pride PET scanning was used to assess dopamine release on the basis of the
competition between endogenous dopamine and [11C]raclopride for binding to do-
pamine D2 receptors (Salimpoor et al., 2011). They combined dopamine-release
measurements with psychophysiological measures of autonomic nervous system ac-
tivity during listening to intensely pleasurable music and found endogenous dopa-
mine release in the striatum at peak emotional arousal during music listening. To
examine the time course of dopamine release, the authors used functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) with the same stimuli and listeners, and found a func-
tional dissociation: the caudate was more involved during the anticipation and the
nucleus accumbens was more involved during the experience of peak emotional re-
sponses to music. These results indicate that intense pleasure in response to music
can lead to dopamine release in the striatal system. Notably, the anticipation of an
abstract reward can result in dopamine release in an anatomical pathway distinct
from that associated with the peak pleasure itself. Such results may well help to ex-
plain why music is of such high value across all human societies. As stated above,
dopaminergic activation regulates and heightens arousal, motivation and supports
memory formation in the episodic and the procedural memory (Karabanov et al.,
2010) and thereby will contribute to memorization of auditory stimuli producing
such strong emotional responses. In a very recent study, the authors could even dem-
onstrate that the degree of activation and connectivity in a network comprising the
nucleus accumbens, auditory cortices, amygdala, and ventromedial frontal cortex
predicted the amount of money, subjects were willing to spend in an auction para-
digm (Salimpoor et al., 2013).
242 CHAPTER 12 Apollo’s gift
Author's personal copy
Taken together, these powerful music-induced modulations of neurohormonal
status may not only account for pleasurable experiences but may also play a role
in neurologic music therapy.
4FACILITATING RECOVERY FROM NONFLUENT APHASIA
THROUGH A FORM OF SINGING
The ability to sing in humans is evident from infancy, and does not depend on formal
vocal training but can be enhanced by training. Given the behavioral similarities be-
tween singing and speaking, as well as the shared and distinct neural correlates of
both, researchers have begun to examine whether forms of singing can be used to
treat some of the speech-motor abnormalities associated with various neurological
conditions (Wan et al., 2010).
Aphasia is a common and devastating complication of stroke or other brain in-
juries that results in the loss of ability to produce and/or comprehend language. It
has been estimated that between 24% and 52% of acute stroke patients have some
form of aphasia if tested within 7 days of their stroke; 12% of survivors still have
significant aphasia at 6 months after stroke (Wade et al., 1976). The nature and se-
verity of language dysfunction depends on the location and extent of the brain lesion.
Accordingly, aphasia can be classified broadly into fluent or nonfluent. Fluent apha-
sia often results from a lesion involving the posterior superior temporal lobe known
as Wernicke’s area. Patients who are fluent exhibit articulated speech with relatively
normal utterance length. However, their speech may be completely meaningless to
the listener and littered with jargon. Furthermore, it may contain violations to syn-
tactic and grammatical rules. These patients also have severe speech comprehension
deficits. In contrast, nonfluent aphasia results most commonly from a lesion in the
left frontal lobe, involving the left posterior inferior frontal region known as Broca’s
area. Patients who are nonfluent tend to have relatively intact comprehension for
conversational speech, but have marked impairments in articulation and speech
production.
The general consensus is that there are two routes to recovery from aphasia. In
patients with small lesions in the left hemisphere, there tends to be recruitment of
both left-hemispheric, perilesional cortices with variable involvement of right-
hemispheric homologous regions during the recovery process (Heiss and Thiel,
2006; Heiss et al., 1999; Hillis, 2007; Rosen et al., 2000). In patients with large
left-hemispheric lesions involving language-related regions of the frontotemporal
lobes, the only path to recovery may be through recruitment of homologous language
and speech-motor regions in the right hemisphere (Geschwind, 1971; Schlaug et al.,
2008). It has been suggested that recovery via the right hemisphere may be less ef-
ficient than recovery via the left hemisphere (Hillis, 2007), possibly because patients
with relatively large left-hemispheric lesions that encompass all of the relevant
speech-motor regions of the left hemisphere are generally more impaired and recover
to a lesser degree than patients with smaller left-hemisphere lesions. Nevertheless,
2434 Facilitating recovery from nonfluent aphasia through a form of singing
Author's personal copy
activation of right-hemispheric regions during speech/language fMRI tasks has been
reported in patients with aphasia, irrespective of their lesion size (Rosen et al., 2000).
For patients with large lesions that cover the language-relevant regions on the left,
therapies that specifically engage or stimulate the homologous right-hemispheric re-
gions have the potential to facilitate the language recovery process beyond the lim-
itations of natural recovery (Gerstman, 1964; Keith and Aronson, 1975). Based on
clinical observations of patients with severe nonfluent aphasia and their ability to
sing lyrics better than they can speak the same words (Albert et al., 1973;
Schlaug et al., 2010; Sparks and Holland, 1976), an intonation-based therapy called
Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) that would emphasize melody and contour and
engage a sensorimotor network of articulation on the unaffected hemisphere through
rhythmic tapping was developed (Albert et al., 1973; Schlaug et al., 2010). The two
unique components of MIT are first the intonation of words and simple phrases using
a melodic contour that follows the prosody of speech, and second the rhythmic tap-
ping of the left hand that accompanies the production of each syllable and serves as a
catalyst for fluency.
To date, studies using MIT have produced positive outcomes in patients with
nonfluent aphasia. These outcomes range from improvements on the Boston Diag-
nostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE; Goodglass and Kaplan, 1983), to improve-
ments in articulation and phrase production (Bonakdarpour et al., 2000; Wilson
et al., 2006) after treatment. The effectiveness of this intervention is further demon-
strated in a recent study that examined transfer of language skills to untrained con-
texts. Schlaug et al. (2008) compared the effects of MIT with a control intervention
(speech repetition) on picture naming performance and measures of propositional
speech. After 40 daily sessions, both therapy techniques resulted in significant im-
provement on all outcome measures, but the extent of this improvement was far
greater for the patient who underwent MIT compared to the one who underwent
the control therapy.
The therapeutic effect of MIT is also evident in neuroimaging studies that show
reorganization of brain functions. MIT resulted in increased activation in a right-
hemisphere network involving the premotor, inferior frontal, and temporal lobes
(Schlaug et al., 2008), as well as increased fiber number and volume of the arcuate
fasciculus in the right hemisphere (Schlaug et al., 2009; Wan et al., 2014). These
findings demonstrate that intensive experimental therapies such as MIT—when ap-
plied over a longer period of time in chronic stroke patients—can induce functional
and structural brain changes in a right-hemisphere vocal-motor network, and these
changes are related to speech output improvements (Wan et al., 2014).
The mechanisms that are underlying the recovery-enhancing effects of MIT are
not completely clear. However, it has been argued that we suggest that there are four
possible mechanisms by which MIT’s therapeutic effect is achieved (for details, see
Schlaug et al., 2008): (1) reduction of speed to approximately one syllable/sec. which
may specifically engage right-hemisphere perceptual and perception–action cou-
pling on the right hemisphere, as the right hemisphere has been shown to integrate
sensory information over a larger time window than the left (Abrams et al., 2008;
244 CHAPTER 12 Apollo’s gift
Author's personal copy
Poeppel, 2003); (2) syllable lengthening that isolates/emphasizes individual pho-
nemes even as they remain part of the continuously voiced words/phrases; and (3)
chunking” that not only combines prosodic information with meaningful content
to facilitate production of longer, more fluent phrases, but has also been shown to
lead to more right- than left-hemisphere activation in healthy subjects (Meyer
et al., 2002; Ozdemir et al., 2006; Zatorre and Belin, 2001). Given that, patients with
right-hemisphere lesions have greater difficulty with global processing tasks (e.g.,
melody and contour processing) than those with left-hemisphere lesions (Peretz,
1990; Schuppert et al., 2000), it is possible that the melodic element of MIT does,
indeed engage the right hemisphere, particularly the right temporal lobe, more than
therapies that do not make use of tonal information or melodic contour, and again
intervention that would integrate information over a larger timescale favoring right
over left-hemispheric processing (Poeppel, 2003). The fourth mechanism—Left-
hand tapping (one tap/syllable,one syllable/sec.)—is likely to play an important role
in engaging a right-hemispheric, sensorimotor network capable of providing an im-
pulse for verbal production in much the same way that a metronome has been shown
to serve as a “pacemaker” when rhythmic motor activities prime and/or entrain sen-
sorimotor networks (Thaut and Abiru, 2010; Thaut et al., 1999). In addition, research
suggesting that hand movements and articulatory movements may share neural cor-
relates (Gentilucci et al., 2000; Meister et al., 2003; Tokimura et al., 1996; Uozumi
et al., 2004) further supports the notion that hand tapping is critically important for
facilitating the coupling of sounds to orofacial and articulatory actions (Lahav et al.,
2007). Since concurrent speech and hand use occurs in daily life, and gestures are
frequently used to emphasize/accompany important and/or elusive concepts in
speech, rhythmic hand movements, in synchrony with articulatory movements,
may have similarly beneficial effects on speech production and in particular in
relearning of speech-motor functions after a stroke.
5MUSIC-SUPPORTED MOTOR THERAPY IN STROKE
PATIENTS
MST in the rehabilitation of fine motor hand skills was first systematically investi-
gated by Schneider et al. (2007). Patients were encouraged to play melodies with the
paretic hand on a piano, or to tap with the paretic arm on eight electronic drum pads
that emitted piano tones. It was demonstrated that these patients regained faster their
motor abilities, and improved in timing, precision, and smoothness of fine motor
skills. Along with fine motor recovery, an increase in neuronal connectivity between
sensory-motor and auditory regions was demonstrated by means of EEG-coherence
measures (Altenm
uller et al., 2009; Rojo et al., 2012; Schneider et al., 2010).
Therefore, establishing an audio-sensory-motor co-representation may support the
rehabilitation process (see Fig. 1). This notion is corroborated by findings in a patient
who underwent music-supported training 20 months after suffering a stroke. Along
with clinical improvement, fMRI follow up provided evidence for the establishment
2455 Music-supported motor therapy in stroke patients
Author's personal copy
of an auditory-sensory-motor network due to the training procedure (Rojo et al.,
2012). Recently, in a larger group of 20 stroke patients, changes in motor cortex ex-
citability following a 4 weeks intervention were demonstrated with the transcranial
magnetic stimulation technique. These changes were accompanied with marked im-
provements of fine motor skills (Amengual et al., 2013).
Music-supported training is undoubtedly efficient and seems to be even more
helpful than functional motor training using no auditory feedback, but otherwise sim-
ilar fine motor training. A randomized prospective study comprising all three groups
is presently under the way and will clarify the differential effects of functional motor
training and music-supported training. With respect to the underlying mechanisms, a
number of open questions still remain. First, the role of motivational factors must
be clarified. From the patients’ informal descriptions of their experience with the
music-supported training, it appears that this was highly enjoyable and a highlight
of their rehabilitation process. Thus, motivational and emotional factors might have
contributed to the success of the training program. Furthermore, according to a study
by Sa
¨rka
¨m
o et al. (2008), music listening activates a widespread bilateral network of
brain regions related to attention, semantic processing, memory, motor functions,
F3 Fz F4
FC4
C4
CP4
P4Pz
CPz
Cz
FCz
P3
CP3
C3
FC3
MG CG CGMG
Affected extremit
y
Nonaffected extremit
y
<0.05 <0.15
<0.1 <0.2
FIGURE 1
Topographic task-related coherence maps for the Music group (MG) compared to the control
group (CG) during self-paced arm movements for the drum pad condition in the beta band
(18–22 Hz). Statistically significant increases in task-related coherence during the motor
performance after 3 weeks and 15 sessions of music-supported therapy on sonified drum
pads are displayed.
From Altenm
uller and Schlaug (2013) with permission.
246 CHAPTER 12 Apollo’s gift
Author's personal copy
and emotional processing. Sa
¨rka
¨m
o and colleagues showed that music exposure sig-
nificantly enhances cognitive functioning in the domains of verbal memory and fo-
cused attention in stroke patients. The music group also experienced less depressed
mood than the control groups. These mechanisms may also hold true for the music-
supported training we applied.
Another issue is related to the auditory feedback mechanisms. Up to now, it has
not been clear whether any auditory feedback (e.g., simple beep tones) would have a
similar effect on fine motor rehabilitation or whether explicit musical parameters
such as a sophisticated pitch and time structure are prerequisites for the success
of the training. This has to be addressed in a study comparing the effects of musical
feedback compared to simple acoustic feedback. With respect to the latter, according
to a study by Thaut et al. (2002), simple rhythmic cueing with a metronome signif-
icantly improves the spatiotemporal precision of reaching movements in stroke
patients.
Furthermore, it is not clear, whether timing regularity and predictability is crucial
for the beneficial effect of MST using key-board playing or tapping on drumpads.
Although it has been argued that the effectiveness of this therapy relies on the fact that
the patient’s brain receives a time-locked auditory feedback with each movement, new
results challenge this viewpoint. In a recent study, 15 patients in early stroke rehabil-
itation with no previous musical background learned to play simple finger exercises
and familiar children’s songs on the piano. The participants were assigned to one of
two groups: in the normal group, the keyboard emitted a tone immediately at key-
stroke, in the delay group, the tone was delayed by a random time interval between
100 and 600 ms. To assess recovery, we performed standard clinical tests such as
the nine-hole-pegboard test and index finger tapping speed and regularity. Surpris-
ingly, patients in the delay group improved more in the nine-hole-pegboard test,
whereas patients in the normal group did not. In finger tapping rate and regularity
both groups showed similar marked improvements. The normal group showed re-
duced depression whereas the delay group did not (van Vugt et al., 2014). Here, we
conclude that music therapy on a randomly delayed keyboard can improve motor
recovery after stroke. We hypothesize that the patients in the delayed feedback group
implicitly learn to be independent of the auditory feedback and therefore outperform
those in the normal condition.
Finally, the stability of improvements needs to be assessed in further studies, and
the length and number of training sessions might be manipulated in future research.
Additionally, the effect of training in chronic patients suffering from motor impair-
ments following a stroke for more than a year will be assessed.
6CONCLUSIONS
Emerging research over the past decade has shown that long-term music training and
the associated sensorimotor skill learning can be a strong stimulant for neuroplastic
changes in the developing as well as in the adult brain, affecting both white and gray
2476 Conclusions
Author's personal copy
matter as well as cortical and subcortical structures. Making music including singing
and dancing leads to a strong coupling of perception and action mediated by sensory,
motor, and multimodal brain regions and affects either in a top-down or bottom-up
fashion important relay stations in the brainstem and thalamus. Furthermore, listen-
ing to music and making music provokes motions and emotions, increases between-
subject communications and interactions and—mediated via neurohormons such as
serotonin and dopamine—is experienced as joyous and rewarding through activity
changes in amygdala, ventral striatum, and other components of the limbic system.
Making music makes rehabilitation more enjoyable and can remediate impaired neu-
ral processes or neural connections by engaging and linking brain regions with each
other that might otherwise not be linked together.
As other experimental interventions, music-based experimental interventions
need to be grounded on a neurobiological understanding of how and why particular
brain systems could be affected. The efficacy of these experimental interventions
should be assessed quantitatively and in an unbiased way. A neuroscientific basis
for music-based interventions and data derived from randomized clinical trials are
important steps in establishing neurologically based music therapies that might have
the power to enhance brain recovery processes, ameliorate the effects of develop-
mental brain disorders, and neuroplasticity in general.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
G. S. gratefully acknowledges support from NIH (1RO1 DC008796, 3R01DC008796-02S1,
R01 DC009823-01), the family of Rosalyn and Richard Slifka, and the Matina R. Proctor
Foundation.
Parts of this Review Article are containing an updated version of a previous review, which
appeared 2013 in the Journal “Music and Medicine.”
REFERENCES
Abrams, D.A., Nicol, T., Zecker, S., Kraus, N., 2008. Right-hemisphere auditory cortex is
dominant for coding syllable patterns in speech. J. Neurosci. 28, 3958–3965.
Albert, M.L., Sparks, R.W., Helm, N.A., 1973. Melodic intonation therapy for aphasia. Arch.
Neurol. 29, 130–131.
Altenm
uller, E., McPherson, G., 2007. Motor learning and instrumental training. In: Gruhn, F.R.
(Ed.), Neurosciences in Music Pedagogy. Nova Science Publisher, New York, NY,
pp. 145–155.
Altenm
uller, E., Schlaug, G., 2012. Music, brain, and health: exploring biological foundations
of music’s health effects. In: MacDonald, R., Kreutz, G., Mitchell, L. (Eds.), Music,
Health, and Wellbeing. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 12–24.
Altenm
uller, E., Schlaug, G., 2013. Neurobiological aspects of neurologic music therapy.
Music Med. 5, 210–216.
248 CHAPTER 12 Apollo’s gift
Author's personal copy
Altenm
uller, E., Schneider, S., Marco-Pallares, P.W., M
unte, T.F., 2009. Neural reorganiza-
tion underlies improvement in stroke induced motor dysfunction by music supported ther-
apy. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 395–405.
Amengual, J.L., Rojo, N., Veciana de las Heras, M., Marco-Pallare
´s, J., Grau, J., Schneider, S.,
Vaquero, L., Juncadella, M., Montero, J., Mohammadi, B., Rubio, F., Rueda, N.,
Duarte, E., Grau, E., Altenm
uller, E., M
unte, T.F., Rodrı
´guez-Fornells, A., 2013. Senso-
rimotor plasticity after music-supported therapy in chronic stroke patients revealed by
transcranial magnetic stimulation. PLoS One 8, e61883. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/jour-
nal.pone.0061883.
Amunts, K., Schlaug, G., Ja
¨ncke, L., Steinmetz, H., Schleicher, A., Dabringhaus, A., Zilles, K.,
1997. Motor cortex and hand motor skills: structural compliance in the human brain. Hum.
Brain Mapp. 5, 206–215.
Bangert, M., Altenm
uller, E., 2003. Mapping perception to action in piano practice: a longi-
tudinal DC-EEG-study. BMC Neurosci. 4, 26–36.
Bangert, M., Schlaug, G., 2006. Specialization of the spezialized in features of external brain
morphology. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 1832–1834.
Bengtsson, S.L., Nagy, Z., Skare, S., Forsman, L., Forssberg, H., Ullen, F., 2005. Extensive
piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter development. Nat. Neu-
rosci. 8, 1148–1150.
Blood, A.J., Zatorre, R.J., 2001. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity
in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.
198, 11818–11823.
Bonakdarpour, B., Eftekharzadeh, A., Ashayeri, H., 2000. Preliminary report on the effects of
melodic intonation therapy in the rehabilitation of Persian aphasic patients. Iran. J. Med.
Sci. 25, 156–160.
Evers, S., Suhr, B., 2000. Changes of the neurotransmitter serotonin but not of hormones dur-
ing short time music perception. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 250, 144–147.
Ga
¨rtner, H., Minnerop, M., Pieperhoff, P., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., Altenm
uller, E.,
Amunts, K., 2013. Brain morphometry shows effects of long-term musical practice in
middle-aged keyboard players. Front. Psychol. 4, 636. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2013.00636.
Gaser, C., Schlaug, G., 2003. Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians.
J. Neurosci. 23, 9240–9245.
Gentilucci, M., Benuzzi, F., Bertolani, L., Daprati, E., Gangitano, M., 2000. Language and
motor control. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 468–490.
Gerstman, H.L., 1964. A case of aphasia. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 29, 89–91.
Geschwind, N., 1971. Current concepts: aphasia. N. Engl. J. Med. 284, 654–656.
Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., 1983. Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, second ed. Lea &
Febiger, Philadelphia, PA.
Halwani, G.F., Loui, P., R
uber, T., Schlaug, G., 2011. Effects of practice and experience on the
arcuate fasciculus: comparing singers, instrumentalists, and non-musicians. Front. Psy-
chol. 2, 156. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.001561.
Heiss, W.D., Thiel, A., 2006. A proposed regional hierarchy in recovery of post-stroke apha-
sia. Brain Lang. 98, 118–123.
Heiss, W.D., Kessler, J., Thiel, A., Ghaemi, M., Karbe, H., 1999. Differential capacity of left and
right hemispheric areas for compensation of poststroke aphasia. Ann. Neurol. 45, 430–438.
Hillis, A.E., 2007. Aphasia: progress in the last quarter of a century. Neurology 69, 200–213.
249References
Author's personal copy
Hodges, D.A., 2010. Psychophysiological measures. In: Juslin, P.N., Sloboda, J.A. (Eds.),
Handbook of Music and Emotion. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 279–311.
Hutchinson, S., Lee, L.H.L., Gaab, N., Schlaug, G., 2003. Cerebellar volume: gender and mu-
sicianship effects. Cereb. Cortex 13, 943–949.
Hyde, K.L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A.C., Schlaug, G., 2009.
Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 29, 3019–3025.
Karabanov, A., Cervenka, S., de Manzano, O., Forssberg, H., Farde, L., Ulle
´n, F., 2010. Do-
pamine D2 receptor density in the limbic striatum is related to implicit but not explicit
movement sequence learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 7574–7579.
Keith, R.L., Aronson, A.E., 1975. Singing as therapy for apraxia of speech and aphasia: report
of a case. Brain Lang. 2, 483–488.
Keitz, M., Martin-Soelch, C., Leenders, K.L., 2003. Reward processing in the brain: a prereq-
uisite for movement preparation? Neural Plast. 1, 121–128.
Kreutz, G., Quiroga Murcia, C., Bongard, S., 2012. Psychoneuroendocrine research on music
and health. An overview. In: MacDonald, R., Kreutz, G., Mitchell, L. (Eds.), Music, Health
and Wellbeing. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 457–476.
Lahav, A., Saltzman, E., Schlaug, G., 2007. Action representation of sound: audio-
motor recognition network while listening to newly acquired actions. J. Neurosci.
27, 308–314.
Meister, I.G., Boroojerdi, B., Foltys, H., Sparing, R., Huber, W., Topper, R., 2003. Motor cor-
tex hand area and speech: implications for the development of language.
Neuropsychologia 41, 401–406.
Menon, V., Levitin, D.J., 2005. The rewards of music listening: response and physiological
connectivity of the mesolimbic system. NeuroImage 28, 175–184.
Meyer, M., Alter, K., Friederici, A.D., Lohmann, G., von Cramon, D.Y., 2002. FMRI reveals
brain regions mediating slow prosodic modulations in spoken sentences. Hum. Brain
Mapp. 17, 73–88.
M
unte, T.F., Altenm
uller, E., Ja
¨ncke, L., 2002. The musician’s brain as a model of neuroplas-
ticity. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 473–478.
Ozdemir, E., Norton, A., Schlaug, G., 2006. Shared and distinct neural correlates of singing
and speaking. NeuroImage 33, 628–635.
Peretz, I., 1990. Processing of local and global musical information by unilateral brain-
damaged patients. Brain 113, 1185–1205.
Poeppel, D., 2003. The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: cerebral
lateralization as “asymmetric sampling in time” Speech Comm. 41, 245–255.
Rojo, N., Amengual, J., Juncadella, M., Rubio, F., Camara, E., Marco-Pallares, J.,
Schneider, S., Veciana, M., Montero, J., Mohammadi, B., Altenm
uller, E., Grau, C.,
M
unte, T.F., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., 2012. Music-supported therapy induces plasticity
in the sensorimotor cortex in chronic stroke: a single-case study using multimodal imaging
(fMRI-TMS). Brain Inj. 25, 787–793.
Rosen, H.J., Petersen, S.E., Linenweber, M.R., 2000. Neural correlates of recovery from apha-
sia after damage to left inferior frontal cortex. Neurology 55, 1883–1894.
Rueber, T., Lindenberg, R., Schlaug, G., 2013. Differential adaptation of descending motor
pathways in musicians. Cereb. Cortex http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht331[Epub
ahead of print].
Salimpoor, V.N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., Zatorre, R.J., 2011. Anatomically
distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music.
Nat. Neurosci. 14, 257–262.
250 CHAPTER 12 Apollo’s gift
Author's personal copy
Salimpoor, V.N., van den Bosch, I., Kovacevicm, N., McIntoshm, A.R., Dagher, A.,
Zatorre, R.J., 2013. Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory cortices pre-
dict music reward value. Science 340, 216–219.
Sa
¨rka
¨m
o, T., Tervaniemi, M., Laitinen, S., Forsblom, A., Soinila, S., Mikkonen, M., 2008.
Music listening enhances cognitive recovery and mood after middle Cerebral artery stroke.
Brain 131, 866–876.
Schlaug, G., Marchina, S., Norton, A., 2008. From singing to speaking: why patients with
Broca’s aphasia can sing and how that may lead to recovery of expressive language func-
tions. Music. Percept. 25, 315–323.
Schlaug, G., Marchina, S., Norton, A., 2009. Evidence for plasticity in white-matter tracts of
patients with chronic Broca’s aphasia undergoing intense intonation-based speech therapy.
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 385–394.
Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Marchina, S., Zipse, L., Wan, C.Y., 2010. From singing to speaking:
facilitating recovery from nonfluent aphasia. Future Neurol. 5, 657–665.
Schneider, S., Sch
onle, P.W., Altenm
uller, E., M
unte, T.F., 2007. Using musical instruments
to improve motor skill recovery following a stroke. J. Neurol. 254, 1339–1346.
Schneider, S., M
unte, T.F., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Sailer, M., Altenm
uller, E., 2010. Music
supported training is more efficient than functional motor training for recovery of fine mo-
tor skills in stroke patients. Music. Percept. 27, 271–280.
Schuppert, M., Munte, T.F., Wieringa, B.M., Altenmuller, E., 2000. Receptive amusia: evi-
dence for cross-hemispheric neural networks underlying music processing strategies.
Brain 123, 546–559.
Sparks, R.W., Holland, A.L., 1976. Method: melodic intonation therapy for aphasia. J. Speech
Hear. Disord. 41, 287–297.
Steele, C.J., Bailey, J.A., Zatorre, R.J., Penhune, V.B., 2013. Early musical training and white-
matter plasticity in the corpus callosum: evidence for a sensitive period. J. Neurosci.
33, 1282–1290.
Thaut, M.H., Abiru, M., 2010. Rhythmic auditory stimulation in rehabilitation of movement
disoders: a review of current research. Music. Percept. 27, 263–269.
Thaut, M.H., Kenyon, G.P., Schauer, M.L., McIntosh, G.C., 1999. The connection between
rhythmicity and brain function. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Mag. 18, 101–108.
Thaut, M.H., Kenyon, G.P., Hurt, C.P., McIntosh, G.C., Hoemberg, V., 2002. Kinematic op-
timization of spatiotemporal patterns in paretic arm training with stroke patients.
Neuropsychologia 40, 1073–1081.
Tokimura, H., Tokimura, Y., Oliviero, A., Asakura, T., Rothwell, J.C., 1996. Speech-induced
changes in corticospinal excitability. Ann. Neurol. 40, 628–634.
Tramo, M.J., 2001. Biology and music. Music of the hemispheres. Science 291, 54–56.
Uozumi, T., Tamagawa, A., Hashimotor, T., Tsuji, S., 2004. Motor hand representation in
cortical area 44. Neurology 62, 757–761.
van Vugt, F.T., Kafczyk, T., Kuhn, W., Rollnik, J.D., Tillmann, B., Altenm
uller, E., 2014. The
role of auditory feedback in music-supported stroke rehabilitation: a single-blinded ran-
domised controlled intervention. Brain Inj. (Submitted).
Vaquero, L., Hartmann, K., Ripolle
´s, P., Rojo, N., Sierpowska, J., Ca
´mara, E.,
Mohammadi, B., Samii, A., M
unte, T.F., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Altenm
uller, E.,
2014. The earlier, the smaller: neuroplastic changes in professional pianists depend on
age of onset. NeuroImage (Submitted).
Wade, D.T., Hewer, R.L., David, R.M., Enderby, P.M., 1976. Aphasia after stroke: natural
history and associated deficits. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 49, 11–16.
251References
Author's personal copy
Wan, C.Y., Schlaug, G., 2010. Music making as a tool for promoting brain plasticity across the
life-span. Neuroscientist 16, 566–577.
Wan, C.Y., R
uber, T., Hohmann, A., Schlaug, G., 2010. The therapeutic effects of singing in
neurological disorders. Music. Percept. 27, 287–295.
Wan, C., Zheng, X., Marchina, S., Norton, A., Schlaug, G., 2014. Intensive therapy induces
contralateral white matter changes in chronic stroke patients with Broca’s aphasia. Brain
Lang. 136, 1–7.
Wilson, S.J., Parsons, K., Reutens, D.C., 2006. Preserved singing in aphasia: a case study of
the efficacy of the melodic intonation therapy. Music. Percept. 24, 23–36.
Zatorre, R.J., Belin, P., 2001. Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex.
Cereb. Cortex 11, 946–953.
252 CHAPTER 12 Apollo’s gift
Author's personal copy
... In contrast, models for music often place a strong emphasis on emotions (Altenmüller and Schlaug, 2012;Brattico and Pearce, 2013;Eerola et al., 2018;Juslin and Västfjäll, 2008;Vuoskoski and Eerola, 2012;Zatorre, 2018). Empirical studies in music, such as those conducted by Zatorre and colleagues, and Brattico and colleagues, often highlight the involvement of emotion-processing circuits in the brain during musical experiences (Blood and Zatorre, 2001;Bogert et al., 2016;Brattico et al., 2016;Salimpoor et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Aesthetic experiences are characterized by a conscious, emotionally and hedonically rewarding perceptions of a stimulus's aesthetic qualities and are thought to arise from a unique combination of cognitive and affective processes. To pinpoint neural correlates of aesthetic experiences, in the present study, we performed a series of meta-analyses based on the existing functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) studies of art appreciation in visual art (34 experiments, 692 participants) and music (34 experiments, 718 participants). The Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) analyses showed that the frontal pole (FP), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) were commonly activated in visual-art-induced aesthetic experiences, whilst bilateral superior temporal gyrus (STG) and striatal areas were commonly activated in music appreciation. Additionally, task-independent Resting-state Functional Connectivity (RSFC), task-dependent Meta-analytical Connectivity Modelling (MACM) analyses, as well as Activation Network Modeling (ANM) further showed that visual art and music engaged quite distinct brain networks. Our findings support the domain-specific view of aesthetic appreciation and challenge the notion that there is a general “common neural currency” for aesthetic experiences across domains.
... There is a growing interest in the literature regarding the putative effects of training a musical instrument on cognitive abilities, especially on executive functions (EFs). EFs are mental processes responsible for controlling goal-oriented behaviors (Diamond, 2013;Miyake et al., 2000), which are essential for daily activities and are extensively drawn on in music performances (Altenmüller & Schlaug, 2012;Fauvel et al., 2013). Although EFs may decline over time (Dennis & Cabeza, 2011;Ferguson et al., 2021;Park et al., 2002), recent studies indicate that older adults with a history of musical practice tend to perform better on different tests measuring EF abilities (Amer et al., 2013;Hanna-Pladdy & MacKay, 2011;Moussard et al., 2016;Strong & Midden, 2020) and to have larger gray matter volume in areas associated with them (Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2021;James et al., 2014;Olszewska et al., 2021;Yamashita et al., 2022). ...
Article
Recent studies have reported positive effects of music training on older adults’ executive functions. However, it is not completely known whether these outcomes were due to improvement in music performance skills or due to other components of training, such as social interaction and music listening. Here, we investigated the effect of a 10-week melodica training program on a group of healthy older adults in Japan and the relationship between their early music performance and improvements in executive functions. Participants were divided into an experimental and a passive control group; both completed a battery of executive function measures before and after the intervention. The experimental group also completed a music performance evaluation developed for the current study. We found a significant improvement in an executive function composite index for the intervention group as compared with the control group. Moreover, individual musical performance was the only factor to predict cognitive improvement. Our results suggest that musical instrument training has a positive impact on older adults’ cognition which is not solely attributed to social interaction or music listening. Further research should consider the potential of examining individual differences in music performance skills inside the experimental group to understand the effects of instrument training programs.
... RA has been described in the literature as " attracts attention, regulates or modifies mood, increases productivity, increases arousal, promotes rhythmic movement, and encourages higher performance" (Thakare et al., 2017). It has been proved that RA is capable of eliciting changes in behavior, i.e., a particular change in brain function can encourage persons to increase their exercise obligation and participation in races (Altenmüller & Schlaug, 2012). Additionally, RA is efficacious in decreasing fatigue and enhancing the motor system's efficiency, where music is associated with rhythm (Bigliassi et al., 2017;Patania et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to know the effect of 12-weeks of special exercises using rhythmic auditory to improve spatio-temporal stride parameters (STSP), lower limb muscles activity (LLMA), and 10000m racewalking time (RT). The parameters of STSP are stride length (SL), step cadence (SC), and center-of-mass velocity (CMV). The research sample included twelve racewalkers, and they were divided into two groups (control group and experimental group, six in each). STSP was measured using a Sony camera at a speed of 50 frames/sec for analyzing 25-samples for each racewalker in the 10000m racewalking. LLMA was measured using EMG. In addition, we applied the 10000m test to measure RT. Rhythmic auditory was used to regulate the step rhythm in seven special exercises and the essential part of the training. The steps rhythm was divided into three levels: the 1st slow auditory rhythm, the 2nd comfortable auditory rhythm, and the 3rd fast auditory rhythm. From our findings, there is a significant difference in the post-test for SC, CMV, LLMA, and RT, favoring the experimental group, where P.04<.05, η2=.732 for SC, P.038<.05, η2=.485 for CMV, P.015<.05, η2=.333 for LLMA, and P.044<.05, η2=.483 for RT. While there is no significant difference in the post-test for SL, where P.264>.05. In addition, there is a significant improvement between groups favoring the experimental group as ROC=1.62,5.76,7.48,33.9, and 7.03% for SL, SC, CMV, LLMA, and RT respectively. Comparisons showed that the post-test achieved greater Megahed et al. SPORT TK. Year 2023. Volume 12. Article 7. 2 improvement with a large effect in STSP, LLMA, and RT compared to the pre-test that achieved a small effect. Therefore, the rhythmic auditory in the training has a beneficial role in improving spatio-temporal stride parameters, lower limb muscles activity and racewalkers' records.
Article
Full-text available
Snake bites accounts for a less common yet significant morbidity and mortality among farmers, plantation workers and those outdoor labour workers, especially in those countries with heavy greeneries and forestry – Malaysia is one example (1). Statistically about 5 million people per year are bitten by snakes and approximately 0.02% - approximately 100,000 develops complication or sequelae from the bites (1). Bites from these venomous snakes can cause local and systemic envenomation that can lead to a life-threatening medical crisis (2). These snakes are prevalent in many tropical and subtropical countries. Malaysian data and literature on the snakebites and epidemiology is scarce. Snake bite incidences are not notifiable diseases in Malaysia however a quantitative data is available in the Malaysian Health Informative Centre (2).
Article
Full-text available
Dysplasia Epiphysealis Hemimelica (DEH) is a rare skeletal developmental disorder involving epiphyses of long bones. It primarily emerges during childhood and is characterised by an osteocartilaginous overgrowth involving epiphyses of long bones. Although the exact aetiology of DEH remains elusive, it can be treated conservatively or surgically via excision or corrective osteotomies, depending on the patient's clinical presentation. Radiological imaging must be carried out before surgery to prevent expected intraoperative complications if the patient is planned for surgical intervention. We are reporting a case of an 8-year-old child who presented with left knee pain with imaging demonstrating a DEH. The patient then underwent surgery. The patient had a rapid recovery following several rehabilitation sessions, and no recurrence was reported at the follow-up. The surgical outcome of DEH is unpredictable and largely dependent on several factors. Surgical options should be offered only to those with impacted symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
This paper attempts to delve more into the complex role that music education plays in both traditional and music schools. Beyond teaching skills, music education frames children’s personality and helps them fit in future working environments. Nurturing environments, modelled after music schools, emphasize comprehensive development, integrating cognitive, psychomotor, socioemotional, and artistic aspects. Beyond conventional lines, a holistic approach to education fosters creativity, emotional intelligence, and cross-cultural understanding. Music emphasizes the importance of self-expression and encourages individuals to explore their own unique voice. It also promotes collaboration and teamwork, as musicians often work together to create harmonious melodies. Additionally, music education has been shown to improve cognitive abilities such as problem-solving and critical thinking skills, which can be applied to various areas of life beyond the realm of music. Music schools emphasize immediacy and subjectivity, whereas traditional schools emphasize different feedback mechanisms. Aligning these components have the potential of enhancing learning opportunities and encourage a more comprehensive approach to learning and evaluation in the traditional school setting. Keywords: music education, traditional schools, music schools, learning mechanisms.
Chapter
Full-text available
In den Medien wird immer wieder eine schädliche Wirkung von bestimmten Musikgenres postuliert. Ferner liegen empirische Studien vor, die solche medialen Stereotypen über vermeintlich negative Auswirkungen von spezifischen Musikgenres zu bestätigen scheinen. Vor allem ältere Arbeiten verweisen auf deutliche Zusammenhänge zwischen einzelnen übergeordneten Musikpräferenzen und Variablen der psychischen und körperlichen Gesundheit, insbesondere von Jugendlichen. Hierbei werden vorwiegend Metal, Rock, Alternative, aber in der neueren Zeit auch Rap und Hip-Hop als kritisch bewertet, Verhaltensauffälligkeiten und psychosomatische Probleme bei Heranwachsenden zu verursachen. Der vorliegende Beitrag untersucht die Belastbarkeit dieser Annahme. Eigene Untersuchungen liefern dabei keine Hinweise auf schädigende Wirkungen der infrage gestellten Musikgenres für Jugendliche. Stattdessen zeigen die Ergebnisse, dass neben Persönlichkeitsmerkmalen vor allem die Musikrezeption resp. die Anwendung von Musik im Alltag zur Emotionsmodulation in diesem Kontext sehr bedeutsam wird. Im Rahmen der hier vorgenommenen Untersuchungen wird zudem gezeigt, dass die Befunde bestehender Studien aufgrund möglicher Stichproben- und Generationseffekte kritisch hinterfragt werden müssen.
Research
Full-text available
Research continues to show resilience is key to coping with adversity. The ability to manage adversity underlies emotional well-being. Both relationship with God and music listening have separately been shown to contribute to resilience. The current study combines the two through song prayers (songs which are prayers) to explore the influence on resilience in women veterans who reflectively listen to a prescribed playlist. The four participants of the qualitative study listened to song prayers while offering these to God for at least twenty minutes per day over twenty-eight consecutive days. The outcome for all participants was interaction with God, which strengthened each one's resilience in personal ways beyond what could have been anticipated. The current research begins with an overview of biblical and select historical roots of song prayer usage, followed by a report of the study. Together, these trace consistent patterns of interaction with God through the use of song prayers over the centuries, which aligns with the study participants' experiences.
Article
Full-text available
"Since the second half of the 20th century, there has been an increasing number of studies on the transfer effects of music learning. There is considerable scientific research in both the national and international literature to support the transfer effects of music learning in different areas of life. In Hungary, these researches started at the instigation of Zoltán Kodály, who saw the observable effects of learning music in several areas. He also considered it important to present scientifically supported results, which is why research on the transfer effect marked by the name of Klára Kokas was started in Hungary. While previously the phenomenon could only be described based on cross-sectional or longitudinal surveys, thanks to technological development we could observe the active areas of the brain during various activities, during which we can interpret the causes of the transfer effects affecting different areas supported by MRI images . During my presentation, I will outline the results of these researches, objectively confirming the reasons behind the transfer effects created by learning music. This gives us the opportunity to learn about the results of research that started from Kodály’s foundations and is nowadays showing an increasingly complex picture. Keywords: transfer effects of music learning, neurological background of music learning, music education"
Article
Full-text available
From an early age, musicians learn complex motor and auditory skills (e.g., the translation of visually perceived musical symbols into motor commands with simultaneous auditory monitoring of output), which they practice extensively from childhood throughout their entire careers. Using a voxel-by-voxel morphometric technique, we found gray matter volume differences in motor, auditory, and visual-spatial brain regions when comparing professional musicians (keyboard players) with a matched group of amateur musicians and non-musicians. Although some of these multiregional differences could be attributable to innate predisposition, we believe they may represent structural adaptations in response to long-term skill acquisition and the repetitive rehearsal of those skills. This hypothesis is supported by the strong association we found between structural differences, musician status, and practice intensity, as well as the wealth of supporting animal data showing structural changes in response to long-term motor training. However, only future experiments can determine the relative contribution of predisposition and practice.
Article
Full-text available
This chapter presents accessible and relevant information from research literature concerned with motor development, motor control, sensorimotor interaction, and motor coordination involved in musical performance and practice. It describes the neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of motor systems in general and the neural basis of sensorimotor learning in the different stages of motor development. The brain mechanisms of skill acquisition and the effects of music making on brain networks as well as brain structure are explained. Furthermore, insights into the neurobiological correlates of training strategies such as mental practice demonstrate the relationship between mental (auditory) imagery and acoustic perception. Our chapter also discusses the expertise model of musical achievement in terms of the neurosciences and the neurohormonal reward mechanisms linked to the neurotransmitter dopamine. Based on these principles, a final section provides implications for teaching and learning musical instruments.
Article
Full-text available
Using a pre-post design, eleven chronic stroke patients with large left hemisphere lesions and nonfluent aphasia underwent diffusion tensor imaging and language testing before and after receiving 15weeks of an intensive intonation-based speech therapy. This treated patient group was compared to an untreated patient group (n=9) scanned twice over a similar time period. Our results showed that the treated group, but not the untreated group, had reductions in fractional anisotropy in the white matter underlying the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG, pars opercularis and pars triangularis), the right posterior superior temporal gyrus, and the right posterior cingulum. Furthermore, we found that greater improvements in speech production were associated with greater reductions in FA in the right IFG (pars opercularis). Thus, our findings showed that an intensive rehabilitation program for patients with nonfluent aphasia led to structural changes in the right hemisphere, which correlated with improvements in speech production.
Article
Full-text available
Between-group comparisons of musicians and nonmusicians have revealed structural brain differences and also functional differences in motor performance. In this study, we aimed to examine the relation between white matter microstructure and high-level motor skills by contrasting 2 groups of musicians with different instrument-specific motor requirements. We used diffusion tensor imaging to compare diffusivity measures of different corticospinal motor tracts of 10 keyboard players, 10 string players, and 10 nonmusicians. Additionally, the maximal tapping rates of their left and right index fingers were determined. When compared with nonmusicians, fractional anisotropy (FA) values of right-hemispheric motor tracts were significantly higher in both musician groups, whereas left-hemispheric motor tracts showed significantly higher FA values only in the keyboard players. Voxel-wise FA analysis found a group effect in white matter underlying the right motor cortex. Diffusivity measures of fibers originating in the primary motor cortex correlated with the maximal tapping rate of the contralateral index finger across all groups. The observed between-group diffusivity differences might represent an adaptation to the specific motor demands of the respective musical instrument. This is supported further by finding correlations between diffusivity measures and maximal tapping rates.
Article
Full-text available
PHYSIOLOGICAL RESEARCH HAS SHOWN THAT AUDITORY rhythm has a profound effect on the motor system. Evidence shows that the auditory and motor system have a rich connectivity across a variety of cortical, subcortical, and spinal levels. The auditory system—a fast and precise processor or temporal information—projects into motor structures in the brain, creating entrainment between the rhythmic signal and the motor response. Based on these physiological connections, a large number of clinical studies have researched the effectiveness of rhythm and music to produce functional change in motor therapy for stroke, Parkinson's disease, traumatic brain injury, and other conditions. Results have been strong in favor of rhythmic auditory stimulation (RAS) to significantly improve gait and upper extremity function. Comparative studies also have shown RAS to be more effective than other sensory cues and other techniques in physical rehabilitation.
Article
Full-text available
MOTOR IMPAIRMENTS ARE COMMON AFTER STROKE but efficacious therapies for these dysfunctions are scarce. Extending an earlier study on the effects of music-supported training (MST), behavioral indices of motor function were obtained before and after a series of training sessions to assess whether this new treatment leads to improved motor functions. Furthermore, music-supported training was contrasted to functional motor training according to the principles of constraint-induced therapy (CIT). In addition to conventional physiotherapy, 32 stroke patients with moderately impaired motor function and no previous musical experience received 15 sessions of MST over a period of three weeks, using a manualized, step-bystep approach. A control group consisting of 15 patients received 15 sessions of CIT in addition to conventional physiotherapy. A third group of 30 patients received exclusively conventional physiotherapy and served as a control group for the other three groups. Fine as well as gross motor skills were trained by using either a MIDI-piano or electronic drum pads programmed to emit piano tones. Motor functions were assessed by an extensive test battery. MST yielded significant improvement in fine as well as gross motor skills with respect to speed, precision, and smoothness of movements. These improvements were greater than after CIT or conventional physiotherapy. In conclusion, with equal treatment intensity, MST leads to more pronounced improvements of motor functions after stroke than CIT.
Article
The discovery of audiovisual mirror neurons in monkeys gave rise to the hypothesis that premotor areas are inherently involved not only when observing actions but also when listening to action-related sound. However, the whole-brain functional formation underlying such "action-listening" is not fully understood. In addition, previous studies in humans have focused mostly on relatively simple and overexperienced everyday actions, such as hand clapping or door knocking. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to ask whether the human action-recognition system responds to sounds found in a more complex sequence of newly acquired actions. To address this, we chose a piece of music as a model set of acoustically presentable actions and trained non-musicians to play it by ear. We then monitored brain activity in subjects while they listened to the newly acquired piece. Although subjects listened to the music without performing any movements, activation was found bilaterally in the frontoparietal motor-related network (including Broca's area, the premotor region, the intraparietal sulcus, and the inferior parietal region), consistent with neural circuits that have been associated with action observations, and may constitute the human mirror neuron system. Presentation of the practiced notes in a different order activated the network to a much lesser degree, whereas listening to an equally familiar but motorically unknown music did not activate this network. These findings support the hypothesis of a "hearing-doing" system that is highly dependent on the individual's motor repertoire, gets established rapidly, and consists of Broca's area as its hub.
Chapter
This chapter examines the influences of musical activities such as listening, singing, or dancing on the endocrine system. The underlying assumption is that psychological processes associated with musical experiences lead to changes in the hormonal systems of brain and body. It begins with a brief introduction to general questions of psychoneuroendocrinology as well as to relevant hormonal systems, followed by an overview of empirical studies, which have begun to investigate hormonal responses to musical stimulation and musical activities. The chapter concludes with suggestions for future work that will be derived from initial evidence showing that music can be seen as a psychoactive stimulant inducing physiological effects that are sometime similar to those produced by pharmacological substances.