ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

We discuss the complexity of the concept of intergroup reconciliation, offer our definition of it, and identify instrumental and socio-emotional processes as distinct processes that facilitate reconciliation. We then present the needs-based model, according to which conflicts threaten victims' sense of agency and perpetrators' moral image, and social exchange interactions that restore victims' and perpetrators' impaired identities promote reconciliation. We review empirical evidence supporting the model and present extensions of it to ( a) contexts of structural inequality, (b) "dual" conflicts, in which both parties transgress against each other, and (c) contexts in which the restoration of positive identities is external to the victim-perpetrator dyad (e.g., third-parties' interventions). Theoretical and practical implications, limitations, and future research directions are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pers20
Download by: [Tel Aviv University] Date: 03 August 2017, At: 04:12
European Review of Social Psychology
ISSN: 1046-3283 (Print) 1479-277X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pers20
Intergroup reconciliation: Instrumental and socio-
emotional processes and the needs-based model
Arie Nadler & Nurit Shnabel
To cite this article: Arie Nadler & Nurit Shnabel (2015) Intergroup reconciliation: Instrumental and
socio-emotional processes and the needs-based model, European Review of Social Psychology,
26:1, 93-125, DOI: 10.1080/10463283.2015.1106712
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2015.1106712
Published online: 13 Nov 2015.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 550
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Citing articles: 11 View citing articles
Intergroup reconciliation: Instrumental and
socio-emotional processes and the needs-based model
Arie Nadler and Nurit Shnabel
School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, 69978, Israel
(Received 5 October 2014; accepted 30 June 2015)
We discuss the complexity of the concept of intergroup reconciliation, offer our
denition of it, and identify instrumental and socio-emotional processes as distinct
processes that facilitate reconciliation. We then present the needs-based model,
according to which conicts threaten victimssense of agency and perpetrators
moral image, and social exchange interactions that restore victimsand perpetra-
torsimpaired identities promote reconciliation. We review empirical evidence
supporting the model and present extensions of it to (a) contexts of structural
inequality, (b) dualconicts, in which both parties transgress against each
other, and (c) contexts in which the restoration of positive identities is external to
the victimperpetrator dyad (e.g., third-partiesinterventions). Theoretical and
practical implications, limitations, and future research directions are discussed.
Keywords: The needs-based model; Intergroup reconciliation; Apology;
Forgiveness; Competitive victimhood; Perpetrators; Victims.
In recent years there has been a growing realisation of the importance of
removing psychological barriers that forestall adversaries on the way to ending
conicts. One manifestation of this realisation is the changing zeitgeist regarding
the way post-conict societies work to smooth the transition from oppression and
conict to peaceful coexistence. Truth and Reconciliation Commissions (TRCs),
in South Africa and elsewhere (e.g., Avruch & Vejerano, 2002), and public
apologies by political leaders to a formerly victimised group (Blatz, Schumann,
& Ross, 2009; Gibney, Howard-Hassmann, Coicaud, & Steiner, 2008) are
examples of contemporary efforts to disarm conict-related emotional barriers
Correspondence should be addressed to Arie Nadler, Professor (Emeritus), Tel Aviv University,
Ramat-Aviv, 69978, Israel. E-mail: arie.nadler@gmail.com
This work was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG) [grant agreement number
LE1260/3-1 to the rst author]; and the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-
2013) [grant agreement number PCIG09-GA-2011-293602, awarded to the second author].
European Review of Social Psychology, 2015
Vol. 26, No. 1, 93125, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2015.1106712
© 2015 European Association of Social Psychology
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
and thereby facilitate improved intergroup relations. Another manifestation of
this realisation is the growth of alternative dispute resolution (ADR) in legal
theory and practice (e.g., Boyes-Watson, 2008). As opposed to the more tradi-
tional emphases on punishing the wrongdoer (Whitman, 2003), ADR seeks to
end conict by healing ruptured relationships between adversaries through dis-
arming each partys negative emotions toward the other (Braithwaite, 2002).
These societal changes have been reected in the social psychological study
of intergroup conict, which has shifted from the earlier realist approach that
views conict as emanating from disagreement on the division of scarce
resources (e.g., land, water; Campbell, 1965; Sherif, 1967) to consider the role
of the psychological barriers (e.g., lack of trust, need for revenge) that maintain
and escalate intergroup conict. Due to this theoretical shift, since the 2000s the
concept of reconciliation has been increasingly popular in scholarly writings on
this topic (Nadler, 2002,2011,2012). The research presented in the present
article is part of this scientic interest in the study of reconciliation.
We begin by conceptualising reconciliation as denoting both an outcome and a
process. We rst offer a denition of the outcome of intergroup reconciliation as
changes on structural, relational, and identity-related aspects of intergroup rela-
tions. Then, in the second and main section of the article, we shed light on the
socialpsychological process of intergroup reconciliation. We distinguish between
instrumental and socio-emotional reconciliation and then present the needs-based
model, the theoretical framework that has guided our research on socio-emotional
reconciliation. We conclude by discussing the theoretical implications, practical
applications, limitations, and future directions of this theory and research.
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION AS AN OUTCOME:
DEFINITIONS AND DISTINCTIONS
Scholars writing about reconciliation as an outcome have dealt with the elusive
nature of the concept in either of two ways. One was to dene reconciliation very
broadly, for example, as a changed psychological orientation toward the other
(Staub, 2006, p. 868) or a change of motivations, goals, beliefs, attitudes and
emotions(Bar-Tal, 2009, p. 365). Such overarching denitions represent what
Meierhenrich (2008) calls conceptual stretching, because they do not distin-
guish reconciliation from other general concepts such as intergroup harmonyor
peaceful relations. The other way of dealing with this concepts complexity
was by providing a specicdenition that centres on the aspect seen by a
particular scholar as the most cardinal feature of the outcome of reconciliation.
An examination of these denitions reveals three different, but related, emphases:
structural, relational, and identity-related (Nadler, 2012).
The structural emphasis, which is especially relevant to contexts in which
the conicting parties share a common society, views the core of reconciliation
as the transformation of power relations between the advantaged and
94 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
disadvantaged groups into an equality-based social structure (Rouhana, 2004).
To illustrate, scholars studying post-apartheid reconciliation processes in South
Africa argue that stable reconciliation between Blacks and Whites depends on
structural changes towards greater racial equality (Du Toit & Doxtader, 2010).
Achieving such structural change involves macro-level legal processes, such as
afrmative action programmes or nationalisation of resources. Although several
scholars have considered the social psychological processes related to such
macro-level structures (e.g., social identity theory, Tajfel, 1981; see also work
on collective action, e.g. Van Zomeren, Postmes, & Spears, 2008), these
processes have been studied primarily in other social sciences (e.g., sociology
or political science).
The relational emphasis views greater intergroup trust and more positive
relations as key elements in a reconciled intergroup reality (Kriesberg, 2007).
People-to- peopleprogrammes in the era following the Oslo Accords in the
Middle East (Nadler & Saguy, 2004) and community building efforts in post-
conict Balkan societies (Corkalo et al., 2004) that aimed at building greater trust
between former adversaries are real-world examples of this emphasis.
Finally, the identity-related emphasis suggests that conicts threaten the
identities of the parties involved and that these identity threats fuel the con-
tinuation of the conict (Kelman, 2008). For example, when the in-group has
been defeated and victimised, its members may feel humiliated and seek
revenge in an attempt to restore their in-groups dignity (Lindner, 2006).
Also, when the in-group had committed severe violent acts against the out-
group, the need to maintain its positive moral identity might cause its members
to disengage from these immoral acts by denying responsibility (e.g., claiming
that the other side had brought it on itself, Bandura, 1999). Such moral
disengagement prevents the conict from ending. The identity-related emphasis
thus views reconciliation as the amelioration of conict-related threats to
adversariespositive identities.
Taken together, this tripartite view suggests a denition of the outcome of
intergroup reconciliation as: Trustworthy positive relations between former
adversaries who enjoy secure social identities and interact in an equality-based
social environment(Nadler, 2012, p. 294). A social outcome that is charac-
terised by changes in all three aspects is likely to represent a more stable
reconciled intergroup reality than one characterised by change in only one or
two. Importantly, however, the distinction between the structural, relational, and
identity-related aspects of reconciliation is made for the sake of conceptual
clarity, as we acknowledge that the different aspects are interdependent. To
illustrate, research conducted in Northern Ireland (Tam et al., 2008) and the
former Yugoslavia (Cehajic, Brown, & Castano, 2008)shows that warm relation-
ships between individuals from adversarial groups (i.e., a relational aspect of
reconciliation) are associated with readiness to forgive the perpetrator group for
past wrongdoings (an identity-related aspect of reconciliation). Thus, the three
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 95
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
aspects of reconciliation operate together to facilitate a general positive orienta-
tion towards the Other that is solid and enduring.
Our measurements of reconciliation across the studies reviewed in this paper
reect the gradual move from a generalised to a more nuanced understanding of
the outcome of reconciliation. In the early studies (i.e., Shnabel & Nadler,
2008; Shnabel, Nadler, Ullrich, Dovidio, & Carmi, 2009)wemeasuredthe
consequences of restored identities for a generalised shift from animosity
towards more positive perceptions, feelings, and perceived future relations
with the adversary. As our thinking progressed, we rened our measures in a
manner consistent with the tripartite denition of the outcome of reconciliation.
These later experiments thus assessed not only the role of restored identities,
but also the role of trust in the Other (Shnabel, Nadler, & Dovidio, 2014)and
readiness to work for greater intergroup equality (Shnabel, Ulrich, Nadler,
Dovidio, & Aydin, 2013) as additional aspects of the generalised positive
orientation towards the Other.
The remainder of this article focuses on socialpsychological processes that
affect the relational and identity dimensions of the outcome of reconciliation.
THE INSTRUMENTAL AND SOCIO-EMOTIONAL ROUTES TO
RECONCILIATION
Our theorising (Nadler & Shnabel, 2008) distinguishes between instrumental
and socio-emotional routes to intergroup reconciliation: The rst consists of
acts of pragmatic cooperation to achieve common instrumental goals (e.g.,
cleaner environment, better health), whereas the second consists of removing
the emotional barriers (e.g., victimshumiliation or perpetratorsshame) that
prevent reconciliation. We suggest that these routes differ in terms of their
temporal focus: Instrumental processes focus on recurring positive cross-group
interactions in the present, whereas socio-emotional processes require coping
with the pains of the past, namely dealing with issues of historical responsi-
bility and culpability.
Another major difference between the two processes has to do with the goal
of reconciliation. Although both instrumental and socio-emotional processes are
important for intergroup reconciliation in all intergroup conicts, the emphasis
on one or the other depends on whether the desired post-conict reality is
separation or integration. When adversaries desire a future of two separate social
entities, they need to build a pragmatic partnership that will allow them to coexist
in a conict-free environment. Instrumental processes are likely to be sufcient
to achieve this goal. This is especially true immediately after the violent conict
has ended. During this period, socio-emotional processes of reconciliation that
centre on blame, guilt, and victimhood are likely to open the wounds of the very
recent conictual past and thus hamper the partiesability to coexist. However,
when the two groups seek integration as a single social unit, the Other serves as
96 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
an immediate and constant source of threat to the in-groups positive identity, and
reconciliation needs to defuse these threats through socio-emotional processes.
Long and Brecke (2003) make a similar observation by noting that instrumental
processeswhich they call the signalling modelof reconciliationare impor-
tant when adversaries are separate nations, whereas socio-emotional processes
which they call the forgiveness process”—are more important in intra-societal
conicts. The fact that TRCs, which constitute a socio-emotional process of
reconciliation, are more common in intra-societal conicts provides further
support for this idea (Hughes, Scabas, & Thakur, 2007).
Admittedly, instrumental and socio-emotional reconciliation represent
mutually interdependent rather than entirely separate processes. For example,
identity restoration processes (e.g., through expressions of apology or forgive-
ness; see below) can also promote the process of trust building (Shnabel et al.,
2014). Nevertheless, for the sake of conceptual clarity it is useful to distinguish
between these two routes to reconciliation, which we discuss in greater detail
below.
Instrumental processes of reconciliation change the quality of intergroup
relations from relations marked by distrust and animosity to relations marked
by mutual trust and cooperation. A major path to achieving such a change is
recurring cooperative interactions designed to achieve a common, superordinate
goal that is instrumentally important to both parties. Through this interaction the
parties learn to trust each other. Sherifs(1958) seminal Robbers Cave experi-
ment demonstrates this process. In this eld experiment, two hostile groups of
boys became friendlier and more cooperative after having repeatedly coordinated
their efforts to achieve goals that were important for both groups and which
could not be reached by either group acting alone. Consistently, a recent review
(Hewstone et al., 2014) of the research conducted within the framework of
Allports(1954) contact hypothesis in various conictual contexts, including
Northern Ireland (Tam et al., 2008), Bosnia and Herzegovina (Cehajic et al.,
2008), and Cyprus (Psaltis, 2011), found that one of the key mediators of the
effect of intergroup contact on reducing prejudice and promoting positive mutual
behavioural tendencies was out-group trust. Peace-building programmes, which
aim to build peaceful coexistence between former enemies through repeated
intergroup contact focused on mutual pursuit of common goals (e.g., agriculture
development), exemplify the instrumental route to reconciliation (e.g., Corkalo
et al., 2004; Lederach, 1997; Nadler & Saguy, 2004).
Socio-emotional processes of reconciliation focus on removal of threats posed
to the conict partiesidentities due to their involvement in the conict. Social
psychological research on the role of emotions such as guilt, shame, hatred,
humiliation, and vengeance in maintaining and escalating conict (e.g.,
Branscombe & Doosje, 2004; Gross, Halperin, & Porat, 2013; Lickel,
Schmader, & Barquissau, 2004), and on the positive effects of defusing these
feelings on ending conicts (Lindner, 2006), indicates that threats to group
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 97
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
memberssense of adequate identity can block or, if removed, facilitate reconci-
liation. Real-world examples of identity restoration are the apologies made by
leaders of perpetrator groups to victim groups. Admittedly, existing empirical
evidence points to the relative ineffectiveness of public apologies in promoting
intergroup forgiveness (Philpot & Hornsey, 2008). Yet many scholars argue that
under appropriate conditions (e.g., acceptance of guilt, setting historical records
straight, and discussing reparations; Wohl, Hornsey, & Philpot, 2011), apologies
may constitute honest acknowledgement of the perpetratorsdebt to the victims.
This acknowledgement validates and gives voice to the victimspainful experi-
ence (Gobodo-Madikizela, 2003), rehabilitates their previously powerless iden-
tity (Branscombe & Cronin, 2010; Brown, Wohl, & Exline, 2008), and
potentially results in greater willingness to forgive and reconcile (Minow, 1998).
In the next section we present the theoretical framework that has guided our
research on socio-emotional processes of reconciliation: namely, the needs-based
model. While the model has been examined in various contexts of interpersonal
conicts, the present article focuses on the intergroup domain. The remainder of
this article is thus devoted to the review of research on intergroup reconciliation
conducted within and extending the needs-based models framework.
THE NEEDS-BASED MODEL OF RECONCILIATION
The needs-based model is grounded in the premise of social identity theory
(Tajfel & Turner, 1986), according to which group members are generally
motivated to maintain their positive social identity and strive to restore it to the
extent that it is threatened. Applying this reasoning to contexts of intergroup
transgressions, the models novel assertion is that the threats posed to victims
and perpetratorsidentities are of an asymmetrical nature. Victims experience
threat to their sense of power (Foster & Rusbult, 1999), honour (Scheff, 1994),
and control (Baumeister, Stillwell, & Heatherton, 1995), whereas perpetrators
experience threat to their identity as morally adequate social actors (Exline &
Baumaister, 2000).
Our conceptualisation of the differential threats posed to victimsand perpetra-
torsidentities is consistent with theorising about the Big Two in social judgment and
behaviour (Abele & Wojciszke, 2013; see also Abele, Cuddy, Judd, & Yzerbyt,
2008). According to this theorising, there are two fundamental content dimensions
along which social targets (such as groups) perceive and judge themselves and
others: the agency dimension, representing traits such as strong,competent,
inuential,andself-determined; and the moralsocial (or communion) dimen-
sion, representing traits such as moral,warm,andtrustworthy(for similar
reasoning see also the stereotype content model; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002).
Thus, in terms of the Big Two theorising, victims may be said to experience threat to
the agency dimension of their identity whereas perpetrators experience threat to the
moralsocial dimension (SimanTov-Nachlieli, Shnabel, & Nadler, 2013).
98 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
The Big Two identity dimensions represent broad content categories, which
include distinct components. For example, the agency dimension includes com-
ponents such as competence on the one hand and dominance on the other, even
though people perceive these two traits as clearly distinct (Rudman & Glick,
2001). Status and power also fall under the same overarching category of agency,
even though they have been shown to have different effects on peoples beha-
viour (Blader & Chen, 2012). Similarly, the moralsocial dimension includes
components such as warmth and sociability on the one hand and morality on the
other, even though they have been shown to constitute different aspects of group
membersidentities (Leach, Ellemers, & Barreto, 2007). Nevertheless, theorising
on the Big Two argues that these different contents can be subsumed under two
overarching categories (see Abele & Wojciszke, 2013). Based on this argument,
we suggest that in general, victims and perpetrators can be said to suffer from
impairments to their agency and moralsocial dimensions, respectively, yet the
specicthreat experienced depends on the particular transgression context. Thus,
in certain contexts (e.g., intergroup inequality) members of the victim group may
be primarily concerned with their stereotypical portrayal as incompetent (see
Fiske et al., 2002), whereas in others (e.g., open war) they may be primarily
concerned with their impaired sense of power (Shnabel et al., 2009). Similarly, in
certain contexts, members of the perpetrator group may be primarily concerned
about the threat posed to their image as warm and likeable, whereas in other
contexts they may be primarily concerned with their impaired image as just and
moral.
The needs-based model further argues that the experience of threat results in
corresponding motivational states. Using Bakans(1966) terminology, victims
wish to satisfy their basic need for agency (i.e., efcacy and control over
outcomes; Choshen-Hillel & Yaniv, 2011), whereas perpetrators wish to satisfy
their basic need for communion (i.e., being accepted and liked by others).
Consequently, victims show heightened power-seeking behaviour (Foster &
Rusbult, 1999) and often wish to get even with their perpetrators (Frijda, 1994)
as a means to reassert their identity as agentic social actors. Perpetrators, by
contrast, experience anxiety over social exclusion(Baumeister, Stillwell, &
Heatherton, 1994, p. 246), because the sanction imposed upon those who violate
moral standards is social rejection (Tavuchis, 1991). Consequently, perpetrators
are motivated to restore their positive moral image and reassure their identity as
morally accepted social actors. While in principle this motivation could be
predicted to encourage apology and efforts to undo the harm, perpetrators often
try to avoid unpleasant emotions such as collective guilt (Wohl, Branscombe, &
Klar, 2006) and instead turn to restore their moral identity through strategies of
moral disengagementminimising their responsibility for the harm-doing, belit-
tling its consequences for the victims, or dehumanising them (Bandura, 1999).
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 99
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
Disturbingly, the behaviours stemming from victimsand perpetratorsefforts to
restore their positive identities are likely to further intensify conict: Revenge
results in cycles of increased violence (Newberg, dAquili, Newberg, &
deMarici, 2000), and moral disengagement sets the stage for recurring victimisa-
tion (Bandura, 1999).
Optimistically, however, the needs-based model posits that an interactive
process of social exchange through which the perpetrator group empowers the
victim group, and the latter accepts the former, can serve as an alternative means
through which both victims and perpetrators may restore their identities. The
apologyforgiveness cycle is a paradigmatic example of such an interactive
process. The act of apology serves as recognition of the debtthat the perpe-
trator owes the victim such that only the victim can determine whether this
debtwill be absolved. Commenting on this dynamic, Tavuchis (1991) writes
that once the symbolic overture has been made, the victim alone holds the keys
of redemption and reconciliation(p. 35), and Minow (1998) writes that for-
giveness is a power held by the victimised(1998, p. 17). Thus, when perpe-
trators apologise for past wrongdoings they put their fate in the hands of their
former powerless victims, who are empowered by virtue of being the only ones
who can grant forgiveness. Correspondingly, an expression of forgiveness by the
victim removes the threat to the perpetrators moral identity and signals to them
that they are now accepted in the moral community from which they were
potentially excluded. Echoing this idea, North (1998) writes that after forgiveness
has been granted, victims and perpetrators . . . are equal in terms of respect,
esteem and consideration due them . . . (p. 34), and Exline and Baumaister
(2000) write that expressions of forgiveness and repentance could symbolically
erase the roles of victim and perpetrator, placing the involved parties on a more
equal footing(p. 138). Figure 1 summarises the process proposed by the needs-
based model.
The next sections rst review the empirical ndings that support the needs-
based models hypotheses that (a) victims and perpetrators experience differ-
ential identity threats and are consequently motivated to restore their agency
and moral image, and (b) empowering and accepting messages from the out-
group can remove the threats to victimsand perpetratorsidentities and
increase their readiness for reconciliation. Next, three extensions of the
model are presented. The rst examines the models applicability to conicts
characterised by structural inequality, rather than direct violence; the second
examines the models applicability to dual conicts, in which both parties
transgress against each other and engage in competition over the victim
status; nally, the third extension examines whether restoring victim and
perpetrator group membersidentities by efforts external to the victimperpe-
trator dyad (e.g., through interventions by third parties) can also facilitate
reconciliation.
100 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE FOR THE NEEDS-BASED MODELS
BASIC HYPOTHESES
The rst empirical test of the needs-based model used a series of studies that
focused on contexts of interpersonal transgressions (see Shnabel & Nadler,
2008). Then, the next step in our research programme was to test the models
basic hypotheses in contexts of intergroup transgressions. According to self-
categorisation theory, when their in-group afliation becomes salient, group
members dene themselves in terms of the prototypical attributes of their in-
group rather than their unique personal attributes (Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher,
& Blackwell, 1987). Under these conditions, group members can feel guilty
(Doosje, Branscombe, Spears, & Manstead, 1998) or victimised (Volkan, 2001)
due to historical events in which their group has been involved. For example,
Germans may feel like perpetrators when reminded of the Holocaust and like
Figure 1. The needs-based model of reconciliation.
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 101
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
victims when reminded of the Dresden bombing, even if they were born years
after these historical events. Based on this theorising, we hypothesised that the
dynamics between victims and perpetrators at the intergroup levelin terms of
experienced identity threats, and consequent needs and responses to conciliatory
messageswould be similar to those found at the interpersonal level (i.e., in
Shnabel & Nadlers, 2008, set of studies).
To test our hypotheses, we conducted two experiments that tested the models
predictions by reminding participants of historical events in which their in-group
either victimised or was victimised by another group (Shnabel et al., 2009).
Participants in the rst experiment were 62 Jewish and 60 Arab citizens of Israel
who were recruited by e-mail through snowballing sampling to complete a web-
based questionnaire. They were told that they would participate in a study that
compared responses to the same news story reported in text, audio, or video and
that they were randomly assigned to the textcondition. Participants subse-
quently read about the 1956 Kafr Qasim massacre, in which 43 unarmed Arab
civilians were killed by an Israeli border patrol for violating curfew regulations
they were not aware of. Hence, in this historical context, Arab participants
identied with the role of victims, whereas Jewish participants identied with
the role of perpetrators. In line with predictions, Arab participants reported that
the massacre impaired their in-groups sense of power more than Jewish partici-
pants, whereas Jewish participants reported that the massacre impaired their in-
groups moral image more than Arab participants.
Next, participants were exposed to two speeches ostensibly made by an out-
group representative on the massacres 50th anniversary. One included a message
of empowerment, which referred to the right of the participantsin-group to
determine its own fate and live in respect and hold its head up; the other speech
included a message of acceptance, which referred to the participantsin-group as
our brothersand expressed empathy towards its distress following the mas-
sacre. Arabswillingness to reconcile with Jewsthat is, their positive emotional
orientation towards Jews, their optimistic view of future intergroup relations, and
their readiness to make efforts to improve the atmosphere between Arabs and
Jewswas greater in response to the empowering than to the accepting message.
By contrast, Jewsreadiness to reconcile with Arabs was higher in response to
the accepting than to the empowering message. These ndings support the needs-
based hypotheses that the exchange of reciprocal messages that restore victims
and perpetratorsimpaired dimensions of identity should result in both parties
greater readiness to reconcile.
Despite their consistency with the model, an alternative explanation to these
ndings could be that they simply reect preexisting cultural differences between
the groups rather than the construal of the in-group as a victim or perpetrator. The
second experiment (Shnabel et al., 2009, Study 2) was designed to rule out this
possibility by testing the same hypotheses in a different intergroup context.
Participants in this experiment were 56 Germans and 65 (Israeli) Jews who were
102 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
recruited through snowball sampling. The design and procedures were similar to
those of Study 1. Participants rst learned about a public conference that focused
on past and present GermanJewish relationsin light of the Second World War.
Consistent with the rst experiment, Jews, who were the victims in this context,
reported having a lower sense of power than Germans; correspondingly, Germans,
the perpetrators, reported a lower moral image than Jews.
Next, participants were exposed to two speeches ostensibly made by out-
group representatives in this conference. The acceptance speech included state-
ments like we should accept [the in-group] and remember that we are all human
beings . . . the [in-group] had suffered great pain under the Nazi-regime. The
empowerment message included statements like we should cherish the [in-
groups] contribution to Western culture and humanity ...itisthe[in-groups]
right to be strong, proud and determine their own fate. Consistent with the
ndings of the rst experiment, Jewsreadiness to reconcile with Germans was
greater following a message of empowerment than a message of acceptance,
whereas Germanswillingness to reconcile with Jews was greater following a
message of acceptance than a message of empowerment.
Comparing the responses of Jewish participants across both experiments
allowed us to rule out the possibility that group membersresponses to the
different types of messages stemmed from preexisting cultural preferences.
Specically, Jewish participants did not show a constant preference for a specic
type of message. Rather, their preference for a particular message type was
determined by their in-groups role within the given context: In a context in
which their in-group served as perpetrators, Jewish participants responded more
positively to an accepting compared to an empowering message from their out-
group, whereas in a context of victimhood, they responded more positively to an
empowering compared to an accepting message. Figure 2 illustrates the results of
the two studies.
Study 1: the 1956 Kafr Qasim massacre Study 2: the Holocaust
Figure 2. Willingness to reconcile as a function of the in-groups social role and type of message from
the out-group. Reprinted from Shnabel et al. (2009). © 2009 Sage Publications. Reproduced by
permission of Sage Publications. Permission to reuse must be obtained from the rightsholder.
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 103
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
EXTENSIONS OF THE MODEL TO CONTEXTS OF
STRUCTURAL INEQUALITY, DUAL CONFLICTS, AND
IDENTITY-RESTORATION OUTSIDE THE VICTIM
PERPETRATOR DYAD
We now turn to review three lines of research that extended the models original
formulation.
Applying the needs-based model to conicts characterised by
structural inequality
In his seminal analysis of peace-making, Galtung distinguishes between conicts
that involve direct violenceand those that involve structural violence
(Galtung, 1969). Paradigmatic examples of direct violence are wars where parties
kill, maim, and destroy property, whereas structural violence (i.e., group inequal-
ity) characterises relations of discrimination and unequal distribution of concrete
and symbolic resources between advantaged and disadvantaged groups (e.g.,
racial relations in the United States). Whereas in its original formulation our
model focused on conicts of direct violence (e.g., the Holocaust; Shnabel et al.,
2009), we later applied it to contexts characterised by structural inequality
(Nadler & Shnabel, 2011).
The models logic suggests that like victimised groups, disadvantaged groups,
often stereotypically viewed as incompetent (Fiske et al., 2002), would experi-
ence a threat to their identity as agentic and desire more power to restore it.
Advantaged groups, who are often stereotypically viewed as cold and untrust-
worthy (Fiske et al., 2002), would experience a threat to their identity as moral
and just and will seek to restore it by securing othersacceptance. Consistent
with this possibility are the ndings that in interracial interactions with White
Americans, African Americans and Latin Americans are primarily concerned
with challenging their stereotypical portrayal as unintelligent and incompetent,
whereas White Americans are primarily concerned with challenging their stereo-
typical portrayal as racist and bigoted (Bergsieker, Shelton, & Richeson, 2010).
Based on social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1986), however, these differ-
ential threats and consequent needs among disadvantaged and advantaged group
members should be predicted to exist when inequality and discrimination are
viewed as illegitimate, but not when they are widely perceived to be legitimate
(e.g., relations between the genders in past centuries).
This prediction was tested in two experiments by Siem, von Oetingen,
Mummendey, and Nadler (2013). The rst experiment manipulated status differ-
ences and their legitimacy with minimal groups created in lab, and the second
used natural groups. In Study 1, in which 133 students of a German university
participated for course credit, the participantsin-group was said to be either high
or low in its status on important scholastic abilities. Further, half the participants,
104 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
in the low-legitimacy condition, were under the impression that this status
difference had been achieved by illegitimate means (i.e., the high-status group
could use hand calculators when solving maths problems, whereas the low-status
group could not), while the other half, in the high-legitimacy condition, were
under the impression that it had been achieved legitimately. Study 2 used status
differences between real-life groups. One hundred and sixty-nine students of
clinical psychology from various German universities participated in a web-
based experiment in which they compared themselves to either social workers
or psychiatrists, representing lower and higher status groups, respectively.
Further, half learned that because clinical psychologists and the out-group mem-
bers were performing similar work, this status difference was illegitimate,
whereas the other half learned that because of different specialisation require-
ments this difference was legitimate.
Subsequently, in both experiments, participantsneeds for morality (i.e., their
wish to be perceived as fair) and agency (i.e., their wish to be inuential) in their
future interaction with out-group members were measured. As predicted, in the
legitimate status condition of both experiments there were no differences
between high- and low-status group members in terms of their needs for morality
and agency. By contrast, in the low legitimacy status condition, high-status group
membersneed for morality was higher than that of the low-status group
members, whereas low-status group membersneed for agency was higher than
that of high-status group members. Figure 3 summarises the pattern of results
obtained (Siem et al., 2013, Study 1).
The next set of studies to apply the models logic to contexts of structural
inequality examined whether an exchange of messages that restore disadvantaged
Need for Acceptance Need for Empowerment
High Status High Status
Low Status
High Legitimacy
Low Status
Low Legitimacy
Level of Need for Acceptance/Empowerment
Figure 3. Advantaged and disadvantaged group membersneeds for acceptance and empowerment
when group inequality is presented as either legitimate or illegitimate (Siem et al., 2013, Study 1).
Reprinted from Siem et al. (2013). © 2013 John Wiley and Sons Inc. Reproduced by permission of
Wiley and Sons Inc. Permission to reuse must be obtained from the rightsholder.
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 105
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
and advantaged groupsimpaired dimensions of identities can increase their
readiness to reconcile with each other. Importantly, in line with the emphasis
on the structural component of reconciliation (e.g., Rouhana, 2004), we argue
that in contexts of structural inequality, willingness to reconcile means not only
having more positive attitudes towards the out-group, but also showing increased
support for and readiness to act towards intergroup equality. Whereas for mem-
bers of disadvantaged groups such support means increased collective action to
promote their in-groups cause, for members of the advantaged group it means
readiness to give up their privileges.
Two experiments examined these hypotheses (Shnabel, Ulrich, et al., 2013).
Study 1 used natural groups of unequal statusthat is, 199 students of Israeli
universities of relatively higher and lower status, who volunteered to participate in
a survey about universitiesadmission policy(to make the cover story reliable,
they did not receive credit or payment for participation). Study 2 used an experi-
mental manipulation that randomly assigned participants (70 German undergradu-
ate students who completed an academic surveyin exchange for course credit)
into groups of high or low status. Students in the disadvantaged and advantaged
groups were led to believe that their university was being discriminated against or
treated favourably, respectively, in a scholastically important context (access to
scarce places on a Masters programme). Subsequent to this information, which
established unjust discrimination of one group, participants were exposed to a
message from an out-group representative, which reassured either their agency
(e.g., students in your group are highly motivated and competent) or their moral
social dimension (e.g., students in your group are kind and fair people). The
manipulation of agency and morality through messages of competence and warmth
reassurance is consistent with the theorising that low-status groups are often
stereotypically portrayed as incompetent, whereas high-status groups are often
stereotypically portrayed as cold and immoral (Fiske et al., 2002).
Consistent with the needs-based models logic, members of the disadvantaged
group who received a message that reassured their competence evidenced more
positive attitudes towards the advantaged group than did those who received a
message that reassured their warmth. In fact, despite its positive content, the
warmth-reassuring message did not differ from the control condition.
Correspondingly, members of the advantaged group held more positive attitudes
towards the disadvantaged group when the message reassured their warmth than
when it was a competence-reassuring or neutral message. Importantly, in terms of
collective action tendencies, members of the advantaged group whose warmth was
reassured showed greater readiness to change the discriminatory status quo (e.g.,
by signing a petition to change admission regulations) than participants whose
competence was reassured. Correspondingly, a message reassuring disadvantaged
group memberscompetence increased their readiness to act to change the status
quo more than did a message reassuring them of their warmth. These ndings
illustrate the interdependence between the different aspects of reconciliation
106 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
discussed earlier, demonstrating that identity-related changes (i.e., restoration of
groupspositive identities) facilitated the prospects of structural changes (i.e.,
group membersincreased motivation to act for intergroup equality).
In addition, the ndings regarding group memberscollective action tenden-
cies are of particular interest for understanding the critical role of identity
restoration processes in promoting equality (i.e., the structural component of
reconciliation). With regard to members of disadvantaged groups, collective
action research shows that although they tend to perceive inequality as unfair
and wish to amend the situation, they often fail to act collectively to challenge
the status quo because they feel a lack of collective efcacy (i.e., the belief that
their in-group can resolve the injustice inicted upon it through unied effort;
Mummendey, Kessler, Klink, & Mielke, 1999). Optimistically, however, our
ndings suggest that the reassurance of their competence by the advantaged
group restored disadvantaged group membersbelief that they were competent
and worthy of equal treatment, as well as capable of achieving it through
collective action.
Members of advantaged groups were also more willing to make an effort to
increase intergroup equality once the threat to their positive moral image had
been removed by the disadvantaged groups message of reassurance of warmth
and acceptance. In light of the prevailing assumption that a key element prevent-
ing social change is the privileged groupsmotivation to maintain their relative
advantage (e.g., Sidanius & Pratto, 1999; Tajfel & Turner, 1986), this nding is
of special theoretical and applied interest. Theoretically, it is consistent with
Leach et al.s(2007; see also Iyer & Leach, 2010) argument that the traditional
view within social psychological theorising fails to recognise the importance of
morality in intergroup relations. This failure has led to a limited conceptualisa-
tion of social change as driven mainly by the action of disadvantaged groups,
because advantaged groups are assumed to be primarily motivated to maintain
the status quo from which they benet. However, along with a growing body of
evidence that advantaged group members may also exhibit solidarity-based
collective action(Becker, 2012), our ndings optimistically suggest that advan-
taged group members may be willing to give up power and privilege once their
positive identity is restored. This nding is practically important because advan-
taged groups have more resources and inuence, and therefore their support and
cooperation is often critical to achieving change.
Applying the needs-based model to contexts of dual social
roles
The original formulation of the needs-based model adopted a dichotomous view
of two mutually exclusive roles in conicts: victims and perpetrators. This view
is consistent with peoplesintuitions (Gray & Wegner, 2009), and in some
conicts (such as the Holocaust) with consensual historical narratives.
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 107
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
Nevertheless, in many, if not most, conicts the distinction between the two roles
is blurred, and each of the parties serves as victim in certain episodes within the
conict and as perpetrator in others. This is particularly so in protracted, see-
mingly intractable (Bar-Tal, 2013) conicts characterised by reciprocal cycles of
violence. The present section presents research that extended our model to take
account of such contexts.
An experiment conducted in Liberia, which had experienced two consecutive
civil wars between 1989 and 2003, made the rst step towards addressing the
ambiguity of the victimperpetrator distinction (Mazziotta, Feuchte, Gausel, &
Nadler, 2014). Participants were 146 Liberians who were approached by three
trained Liberian research assistants and were asked to take part in a study on war
and reconciliation in Liberia. Participants were asked to write about an episode in
which either their in-group had victimised people from an adversarial group or
another group had victimised their own. In line with previous ndings of group
membersbiased historical memory (Sahdra & Ross, 2007), participants indi-
cated that they found it easier to recall episodes of in-group victimhood than in-
group perpetration. Moreover, half of those asked to describe an event in which
their group had perpetrated violence against an out-group also described how
their group had been victimised by this out-group. None of those asked to
describe an event of victimisation described how the in-group had perpetrated
violence against the out-group. Also, the descriptions in the victim condition
were longer and more detailed than those in the perpetrator condition. These
ndings are consistent with Baumeisters(1996) suggestion that the experience
of victimisation is psychologically more pronounced than the experience of
perpetration.
In terms of psychological needs, in line with our models predictions, parti-
cipants in the perpetrator condition reported a heightened need for acceptance by
the other group and greater readiness to engage in cross-group contact than did
participants in the victim condition. However, as opposed to the models predic-
tions, victims and perpetrators reported similar, relatively high, levels of need for
power. We theorised that the reason for this absence of difference between the
experimental conditions may be that while participants in the victimhood condi-
tion experienced themselves as purevictims, those in the perpetrator condition
did not experience themselves as pureperpetrators. Rather they experienced
themselves as duals”—that is, as victim and perpetrators simultaneously
because even though they were instructed to write solely about incidents in
which their in-group had been the perpetrator, they wrote about episodes invol-
ving both perpetration and victimisation (e.g., violence against a certain out-
group in retaliation against this out-groups prior violence against the in-group).
Consequently, participants assigned to the perpetrator condition probably experi-
enced themselves in both roles at the same time.
To systematically explore this psychological experience of duality,we
conducted two experiments in which we experimentally manipulated pure
108 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
victimisation, pureperpetration, or duality, and examined participantsconse-
quent needs and behaviours (SimanTov-Nachlieli & Shnabel, 2014). In the rst
experiment, which focused on the interpersonal level, 86 undergraduates who
participated in exchange for credit points worked in dyads and had to allocate
valuable resources (i.e., extra credit points) between themselves and their partner.
The feedback on their allocation decisions constituted the experimental manip-
ulation: Participants in the victim role were told that their own allocation
decisions had been fair whereas their partners allocation decisions were unfair;
participants in the perpetrator role were told the opposite; participants assigned to
the dual role were told that both their own and their partners allocations were
unfair; nally, participants assigned to the control condition were told that both
their own and their partners allocations were fair. Next, participants reported
their needs for agency (e.g., wish to exert more control over the experiments
results) and positive moral image (e.g., wish that their partner would understand
that they tried to be fair) and had a chance to retaliate against or compensate their
partner by denying or donating credit points to him or her.
Consistent with previous ndings, compared to the control condition victims
showed enhanced need for agency, which translated into greater antisocial,
vengeful behaviour, whereas perpetrators showed enhanced need for positive
moral image, which translated into greater prosocial behaviour. Most importantly,
duals showed enhanced needs for both agency and positive moral image. In
terms of behaviour, however, duals resembled victims: Like victims, their heigh-
tened need for agency translated into vengeful behaviour; unlike perpetrators,
their heightened need for positive moral image failed to translate into prosocial
behaviour.
Conceptually similar results were obtained in the second experiment, which
focused on an intergroup context. Participants in this experiment were 96 Israeli
Jews recruited by a commercial research rm for research on IsraeliPalestinian
relations. In the perpetrator condition, participants were instructed to recall two
events in which their in-group had harmed Palestinians and, in the victim
condition, two events in which Palestinians had harmed their in-group. In the
third, dual condition, participants were asked to recall one event of each kind.
Participantsresponses were compared to the midpoint of the scale, which
represented a neutral, no changecontrol (for additional methodological details,
see SimanTov-Nachlieli & Shnabel, 2014).
Consistent with the ndings of the rst experiment, Israeli Jews in the victim
condition reported increased need for agency as well as more antisocial beha-
vioural tendencies against Palestinians (e.g., support for use of unrestricted force
in response for any act of terrorism), whereas Israeli Jews in the perpetrator
condition reported increased need to restore moral identity as well as more
prosocial behavioural tendencies towards Palestinians (e.g., support for providing
humanitarian aid to Gaza). Most importantly, Israeli Jews in the dual condition
showed increased needs for both agency and positive moral image. Yet, the
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 109
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
behavioural responses of dualsresembled those of victims rather than perpe-
trators, as they showed heightened antisocial behavioural tendencies against
Palestinians, whereas their prosocial behavioural tendencies remained unchanged
(see Tab l e 1). These ndings reveal that even though duals are motivated to
restore both their agency and their moral image, the need for agency takes
precedence and exerts greater inuence on their behaviour.
The ndings that members of groups involved in dual conicts experience
threats, and consequent heightened needs, to restore both their agency and moral
image can also shed light on conicting-groups memberstendency to engage in
competitive victimhood (i.e., strive to establish that their in-group has been
subjected to more injustice and suffering at the hands of the out-group; Noor,
Brown, Gonzalez, Manzi, & Lewis, 2008). Specically, because the victims role
is associated with innocence (Gray & Wegner, 2009) such acknowledgment may
restore the in-groups positive moral image. This possibility is consistent with
ndings suggesting that groups strategically engage in competitive victimhood to
protect their moral identity in response to accusations by out-groups (Sullivan,
Landau, Branscombe, & Rothschild, 2012). At the same time, acknowledgment
of the in-groups victim status implies entitlement for redress, increases the in-
groups cohesiveness, and can facilitate third-party supportall forms of social
empowerment (Noor, Shnabel, Halabi, & Nadler, 2012). Moreover, when such
acknowledgment is offered by the perpetrator group, it may serve as admission of
responsibility and consequent moral debt (Minow, 1998), which further empow-
ers the victim group. Thus, winningthe victim status can potentially satisfy
dualsheightened needs for both agency and restoration of positive moral image
at the same time.
A study that used the context of the IsraeliPalestinian conict supported our
theorising about the dual motivations leading group members to engage in
TABLE 1
Means and standard deviations of agency and morality needs, and anti- and prosocial
behavioural tendencies among Israeli Jews assigned to the victim, perpetrator, and
dual conditions
Condition
In-groups role Agency need Moral need
Antisocial
tendencies
Prosocial
tendencies
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Victims 2.03 1.38 7.15 1.10 4.84 1.60 6.42 1.83 3.81 2.49
Perpetrators 4.64 1.73 5.16 1.16 6.61 1.49 5.04 1.99 6.30 1.97
Duals 3.19 1.27 6.46 1.17 5.92 1.54 6.06 1.98 5.01 2.21
N= 79 Israeli Jewish participants. In-groups role was measured on a 7-point scale, such that lower
means indicate greater victimhood and higher means indicate greater perpetration. Reprinted from
SimanTov-Nachlieli and Shnabel (2014). © 2014 Sage Publications. Reproduced by permission of
Sage Publications. Permission to reuse must be obtained from the rightsholder.
110 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
competitive victimhood (Shnabel, Halabi, & Noor, 2013; Study 2). Participants
were Israeli Arab (N= 78) and Jewish (N= 99) students who were recruited
through ads placed on the campus of Haifa University and received payment in
exchange for their participation. Participants were randomly assigned to read a
text that constituted one of four experimental conditions. The text in the com-
mon regional identitycondition highlighted the cultural commonalities between
the two groups (see Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000, for similar manipulations of a
common, superordinate identity). The text in the common victim identity
condition highlighted the fact that both groups experienced great suffering and
loss due to the conict (see Vollhardt, 2009, for a similar conceptualisation of
inclusive victim identity). The text in the common perpetrator identity
condition highlighted that both groups actively inicted substantial harm upon
each other. Finally, the text in the neutral, control condition was unrelated to the
conict. Our primary outcome variables were measures of group members
engagement in competitive victimhood, as well as their willingness to forgive
the out-group, previously found to be negatively predicted by competitive
victimhood (Noor et al., 2008).
As expected, the common regional identity, which was found to increase mutual
prosocial tendencies in contexts of intergroup conicts not characterised by com-
petitive victimhood (see Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000), failed to reduce competitive
victimhood and increase forgiveness among Arabs and Jews, as it did not address
their pressing need for acknowledgement of their in-groups victimisation. By
contrast, both the common victim identity and the common perpetrator identity
conditions reduced group membersengagement in competitive victimhood and
increased their readiness for mutual forgiveness, yet they did so through different
routes. Specically, as illustrated in Figure 4, because belonging to a victimised
group implies moral superiority (Noor et al., 2012), the common victim identity
condition reduced participantssense of threat to their in-groupspositivemoral
image. Consequently, their need to protect the in-groups moral image at any cost
(i.e., moral-defensiveness) was experienced as less pressing, leading to reduced
engagement in competitive victimhood and greater forgiveness. As illustrated in
Figure 5, because the perpetrator role is associated with power and agency (Gray &
Wegner, 2009), the common perpetrator identity condition increased group mem-
berssense of agency, leading, in turn, to reduced engagement in competitive
victimhood and greater forgiveness. Interestingly, there were substantial differences
between the groups (i.e., possibly due to the gap in terms of relative power,
Palestinians showed greater impairment to their sense of agency, as well as more
competitive victimhood and less forgiveness than Jews); nevertheless, there were
no interactive effects, suggesting that similar processes took place in both groups.
Beyond their practical implications, these ndings are theoretically consistent with
our theorising that group membersengagement in competitive victimhood stems
from their wish to restore their agency and moral imagewhich are both threa-
tened due to the conict.
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 111
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
The effects of identity restoration outside the victim
perpetrator dyad on intergroup reconciliation
As mentioned in the previous section, our original formulation of the needs-
based model referred to identity restoration processes as socio-emotional recon-
ciliationand used the apologyforgiveness cycle as a paradigmatic example of
such processes. Consequently, our initial empirical tests of the model focused
exclusively on the exchange of empowering and accepting messages within the
victimperpetrator dyad (Shnabel & Nadler, 2008; Shnabel et al., 2009; Shnabel,
Ulrich, et al., 2013). The models formal statement, however, has gone beyond
this interactive emphasis to imply that identity restoration by itself is sufcient to
–.45**
.17*
–.20*
Competitive
Victimhood
Forgiveness
Moral-
Defensiveness
Common Victim
Identity Intervention .08 (.20*)
Figure 4. Serial mediation model for the common victim identitymoral defensivenesscompetitive
victimhoodforgiveness path (Shnabel, Ulrich, et al., 2013, Study 2). N= 99 Jewish and 78 Palestinian
citizens of Israel. Standardized regression coefcients (betas) are presented. For the path between common
victim identity intervention and forgiveness the coefcients shown inside versus outside the parentheses
represent the total and direct effects, respectively. Coefcients with one or two asterisks indicate beta
weightssignicance level of p<.05orp< .001, respectively. Bootstrapping analysis (1000 re-samples)
revealed a signicant indirect effect, the 95% condence interval = .002 to .166.
–.34**
–.45**
.22*
Competitive
Victimhood
Forgiveness
Sense of Agency
Common
Perpetrator
Identity
Intervention .19*
(
.33**
)
Figure 5. Serial mediation model for the common perpetrator identitysense of agencycompetitive
victimhoodforgiveness path (Shnabel, Ulrich, et al., 2013, Study 2). N= 99 Jewish and 78 Palestinian
citizens of Israel. Standardized regression coefcients (betas) are presented. For the path between common
victim identity intervention and forgiveness the coefcients shown inside versus outside the parentheses
represent the total and direct effects, respectively. Coefcients with one or two asterisks indicate beta
weightssignicance level of p<.05orp< .001, respectively. Bootstrapping analysis (1,000 re-samples)
revealed a signicant indirect effect, the 95% condence interval = .030 to .212.
112 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
increase conicting partiesreadiness to reconcile (see the last two rows in
Figure 1). The research in this section explored whether the restoration of
positive identity by itself, even when the source of such restoration is not the
other conict party, can indeed increase conicting group memberswillingness
to reconcile.
The rst study in this line of research (Shnabel et al., 2014) explored the
potential of third parties to promote reconciliation in contexts of interpersonal
transgressions. Even though it did not study intergroup relations, we discuss it
because of its critical theoretical implications and because, so far, research on the
needs-based model found consistent patterns across the interpersonal and inter-
group levels (compare, for example, Shnabel & Nadler, 2008; Shnabel et al.,
2009). Specically, two experiments used role-playing scenarios to examine the
effects of messages from either the other conict party or a noninvolved third
party, compared to a no-message control condition, on victimsand perpetrators
sense of power, moral image, trust in the positive intentions of the other conict
party, and willingness to reconcile. Participants were university students (N= 173
in Study 1, N= 318 in Study 2) who volunteered to take part in the studies in
exchange for rafe participation.
We found that a message of empowerment for the victim from the perpetrator
(e.g., acknowledgement of their competence and value) increased both their
sense of power and their level of trust in the perpetrators good intentions;
sense of power and trust, in turn, led to victimsincreased willingness to
reconcile (i.e., the two indirect paths, through both sense of power and trust
were signicant). The same empowering message from a third party increased
victimssense of power, but not their trust; therefore, whereas the indirect effect
of such message through sense of power was signicant, the indirect effect
through trust was not. Overall, whereas an empowering message from the
perpetrator signicantly increased victimsreadiness to reconcile compared to
the control condition, an identical message from a third party failed to do so.
For perpetrators, messages of acceptance from the victims (e.g., expressions
of liking) increased both their positive moral image and their level of trust in the
victims good intentions; moral image and trust, in turn, led to perpetrators
increased readiness for reconciliation (i.e., the two indirect paths, through both
moral image and trust, were signicant). The same accepting message from a
third party increased perpetratorsmoral image, but not their trust; therefore,
whereas the indirect effect of such a message through moral image was signi-
cant, the indirect effect through trust was not. Overall, and consistent with the
pattern obtained among victims, whereas an accepting message from the victim
signicantly increased perpetratorsreadiness to reconcile compared to the con-
trol condition, an identical message from a third party did not.
These ndings suggest that the needs-based model in its original formulation
has overlooked the fact that empowering and accepting messages from the other
conict party successfully bring about reconciliation not only because they
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 113
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
restore victimsand perpetratorsimpaired identities, but also because they
constitute trust-building gestures. That is, empowering messages for the victim
from the perpetrators signal to the victims that the perpetrators would not repeat
the transgression, and accepting messages from the victims signal to the perpe-
trators that the victims would not hold a grudge or try to take revenge.
Empowering and accepting messages by third parties do not carry a similar
meaning, as they do not signal the other conict partys positive future intentions
(which are the basis for trust building). These ndings underscore the interde-
pendence between relational changes, expressed in greater trust, and identity-
related changes, expressed in rehabilitated identity dimensions, in achieving
reconciliation.
Additional research (Harth & Shnabel, 2015) focusing on contexts of inter-
group transgressions examined the potentially differential effects of neutral third
parties versus third parties who share common identity with the other party to the
conict on reconciliation. One study was in the context of fraud between
competing German universities (N= 124 university students) and the second in
the context of the IsraeliPalestinian conict. To illustrate the experimental
design, in Study 2, 177 Israeli Jewish participants who were recruited through
snowball sampling to participate in an online study were randomly assigned to
the role of either victims or perpetrators, by reading about historical incidents in
which their in-group had been victimised by or had victimised Palestinians. They
were then randomly assigned to one of four experimental conditions: a no
message, control condition; a message from a UN representativethat is, a
neutral third party; a message from a Jordanian representativethat is, a third
party that shares common identity with the other party to the conict; and a
message from a Palestinian representativethat is, the other party to the conict.
Participants in the victim condition were exposed to an empowering message,
and participants in the perpetrator condition were exposed to an accepting
message from these different sources.
Consistent with our ndings at the interpersonal level, messages from the
other party to the conict, but not from a neutral third party, increased victims
and perpetratorswillingness to reconcile compared to the control condition.
Interestingly, messages from a third party who shared a common identity with
the other party to the conict effectively promoted reconciliation: In Study 1 they
were less effective than messages from the other party to the conict, whereas in
Study 2 they were just as effective, yet in both contexts they were more effective
than messages from a neutral third party. Further analyses showed that the
ofcial from the adversarial out-group was viewed as more representative of
the adversarial out-group than the one from the common identity third party, who
was nevertheless viewed as more representative than the ofcial from a neutral
third party. Perceived representativeness, in turn, mediated the effects of message
source on reconciliation. Given that in intergroup contexts conciliatory messages
114 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
are typically conveyed via group representatives (Blatz & Philpot, 2010), this
nding has important applied implications.
Furthermore, the ndings that third parties who shared common identity
features with the other conict party were able to effectively promote reconcilia-
tion suggest that under certain conditions identity restoration outside the victim
perpetrator dyad can effectively facilitate reconciliation. Consistent with this
possibility, research on interpersonal conicts indicates that offenders who re-
afrm their commitment to the moral values that their offence had violated show
greater self-forgiveness and readiness to reconcile with the Other (Woodyatt &
Wenzel, 2014). The possibility that the afrmation of a threatened identity out-
side the victimperpetrator dyad can positively affect readiness for reconciliation
has important implications for intergroup conicts. Such afrmation strategies
have the potential to circumvent partiesreluctance to convey empowering or
accepting messages to each other due to their concern that such positive gestures
would not be reciprocated or even be used against them (Shnabel & Noor, 2012).
Hence, some of our ongoing research focuses on such self-afrmation strategies.
In particular, SimanTov-Nachlieli, Shnabel, Aydin and Ullrich (2015) found that
the afrmation of their in-groups agency increased dual conicting parties
mutual prosocial tendencies and behaviour. Moreover, Barlow and colleagues
(2015) found that offering an apology to the victim group increased perpetrating
group memberspositive moral image and consequent willingness to reconcile
with and redress the victims.
CONTRIBUTION, LIMITATIONS, AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The needs-based model has contributed to the trend, observed both within and
outside our eld, of paying greater attention to socio-emotional aspects of
conicts by introducing a novel theoretical framework for understanding
reconciliation. While previous conceptual analysis generally viewed reconci-
liation as a process of identity change (Kelman, 2008), our model identies
the two specic identity dimensionsnamely, agency and moralitywhich
need to be changed as part of this process. The model highlights the fact that
threats to these dimensions are experienced asymmetrically by victimised and
perpetrating groups, and that their restoration is critical for reconciliation.
Although restoration of identity dimensions by itselfthat is, outside the
interaction between adversariescan have positive effects on reconciliation,
an optimal process consists of direct interaction between the adversaries. Such
interaction consists of an exchange in which the perpetrating group empowers
the victim group, and the latter expresses moralsocial acceptance of the
former. The emphasis of our model on the interactional element in reconcilia-
tion and its link to identity changes is consistent with Gopins(2004)observa-
tion that what goes on between people cannot be separated from what is
going on within people(p. 14). The needs-based model thus offers
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 115
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
integration, typically missing in socialpsychological analysis (Semin, 1997)
between intergroup processes on one hand and internal processes of identity
change on the other.
Also, whereas previous research has typically studied the effects of transgres-
sions on either the perpetrator group (e.g., work on collective guilt; Wohl et al.,
2006) or the victim group (e.g., work on collective trauma; Volkan, 2001)
separately, the needs-based model has investigated the dynamics between victims
and perpetrators by examining their responses to messages from each other. As
such, the model emphasises the reciprocal nature of reconciliation processes in
the sense that both victims and perpetrators have the ability, albeit in different
ways, to promote reconciliation. One implication of this emphasis concerns the
onus of responsibility for reconciliation. Whereas the common view is that the
perpetrators should be responsible for reconciling by redressing past wrong-
doings, our model offers a more balanced approach by underscoring the fact
that beyond being the morally culpable party, perpetrators are also psychologi-
cally vulnerable social actors whose basic identity-related needs must be con-
sidered and restored, together with the those of the victims, if reconciliation is to
be achieved and maintained. It should be claried, however, that this insight is
not intended to undermine perpetratorsmoral (and possibly legal) responsibility
for their acts, but rather to identify an existing socialpsychological dynamic. To
illustrate, without denying the Nazisresponsibility for the Holocaust, Jews who
participate in dialogue groups (i.e., structured encounters) with Germans must
express (at least some) empathy for Germansdistress if they want to facilitate
reconciliation (see Maoz & Bar-On, 2002).
Finally, the needs-based model has practical implications for the type of
messages that adversaries can convey to each other to promote reconciliation.
Intuition may suggest that in contexts of intergroup conict, in which positive
gestures by the adversary are relatively unexpected (Osgood, 1962), any positive
message from the out-group can effectively open group members to reconcilia-
tion. However, the model reveals that only a positive message that meets the
specic emotional needs of perpetrators and victims effectively promotes recon-
ciliation. The models recent extensions provide additional practical insights by
identifying how processes of recategorisation (i.e., into a common victim or
perpetrator group; Shnabel, Halabi & Noor., 2013), self-afrmation (e.g., of the
values breached by the transgression, Woodyatt & Wenzel, 2014), and third-party
interventions (i.e., by groups who share common identity features with the
adversarial group; Harth & Shnabel, 2015) can facilitate socio-emotional
reconciliation.
Of course, despite its theoretical and practical contribution, the research
conducted within the needs-based models framework is not without limitations.
First, the experimental nature of our research, which called for exposing victims
and perpetrators to the same message from the adversary, did not allow us to
directly manipulate exposure to apologies and forgiveness because it would not
116 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
make sense to expose victims to a message of forgiveness from the perpetrators,
and perpetratorsto a message of apology from the victims. We therefore used
empowering and accepting messages that could be plausibly conveyed to both
victims and perpetrators (e.g., acknowledgment of the groups value and heritage
or morality and sociability). As a result of this experimental approach, although
our theorising has used the apologyforgiveness cycle as a paradigmatic example
for socio-emotional reconciliation, the assumption that apology empowers the
victim, and forgiveness makes perpetrators feel accepted has not been empirically
examined. This is a critical limitation of our theorising in light of the consistent
ndings that group apologies, especially, as is often the case, when their content
focuses on the perpetratorsfeelings rather than on the victimssuffering
(Berndsen, Hornsey, & Wohl, 2015), are ineffective in promoting forgiveness
among members of the victim group (Philpot & Hornsey, 2008). In fact, group
apologies might even backre (i.e., produce feelings of anger among the victim
group) under certain circumstances (e.g., when status relations are perceived as
unstable, Shnabel, Halabi, & SimanTov-Nachlieli, 2015).
Future research is therefore needed to assess the full progression from expres-
sions of apology and forgiveness to feelings of empowerment and acceptance,
respectively, and the resultant readiness for reconciliation. For example, it is
possible that due to a strong social norm that victims should forgive their
perpetrators following an apology (see Harth, Hornsey, & Barlow, 2011), victims
might, at least under certain circumstances, feel disempowered when offered an
apology. Similarly, it is possible that under certain circumstances expressions of
forgiveness by the victims might signal to the perpetrators that they can close the
lid on the past and sweep the long-term implications of past injustices under the
proverbial carpet, leading to less conciliatory behaviour (e.g., readiness to com-
pensate the victims).
Another possible criticism of our research regards its use of individual-level
measures (i.e., of group membersreadiness to reconcile with their out-group)
even though, by our very own denition, reconciliation involves macro-level,
structural processes that are perhaps impervious to the opinions and actions of
individual group members; a related criticism may be that individual group
membersconciliatory attitudes may not necessarily translate into, or might
even impede, support for equality promoting structural changes (see Dixon,
Durrheim, & Tredoux, 2005, for similar criticisms on research conducted within
the framework of the contact hypothesis). We argue, however, that it is likely that
the larger the number of changed individuals, the higher the likelihood that social
change in intergroup relations will occur. For example, in democratic societies a
large number of pro-reconciliation individuals may translate into an election
result that will put a more conciliatory government in power (see Hameiri,
Porat, Bar-Tal, Bieler, & Halperin, 2014, for an individual-level intervention
that increased actual dovish voting behaviour). Moreover, as the study of
Shnabel, Ulrich, and colleagues (2013) reveals, the restoration of advantaged
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 117
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
and disadvantaged group memberspositive identities (i.e., the identity-related
aspect of reconciliation) increased their readiness to work for a more equal social
arrangement (i.e., the structural aspect of reconciliation). Hence, even though we
acknowledge the issues pointed out by Dixon and colleagues (2005), we believe
they may be less troubling than there seems at rst glance.
Another limitation of our work is that although participants had different
backgrounds (e.g., Israelis, Palestinians, Germans, Liberians) in many of our
studies they were university students, and the assessment of dependent measures
relied primarily on either paper and pencil measures or relatively simple beha-
viours (e.g., allocation of credit points; SimanTov-Nachlieli & Shnabel, 2014).
Future research should aim to recruit more diverse populations and observe more
complex behaviours. For example, it may examine whether and how the inter-
ventions developed in our research (e.g., the induction of a common victim or
perpetrator identity) affect conicting group membersemotional responses such
as tone of voice or facial expressions (see Butler, 2011) within face-to-face
dyadic interactions. Future research may also examine the models hypotheses
outside of the lab. For example, it may be interesting to explore whether
restorative justice procedures (e.g., TRCs) that involve encounters between
victims and perpetrators (see Boyes-Watson, 2008) are more effective to the
extent that victims feel empowered, and perpetrators feel accepted following the
encounter.
Another limitation of the existing research is that it has not yet tried to
integrate the models predictions with the literature on collective emotions. An
important distinction in this regard is the one between shame, which emanates
from perceptions of the in-group as dispositionally awed (e.g., innately cruel or
weak and inadequate), and guilt, which emanates from feeling culpability due to
the groups past behaviour (Branscombe & Doosje, 2004). Lickel and colleagues
phrased this distinction eloquently by stating that people feel guilty for what
they have done, and ashamed for who they are(2004, p. 41). With regards to
perpetrators, it is possible that the amelioration of threat to their moral image
through an accepting message from their victims transforms feelings of shame
into feelings of guilt, and this transformation, in turn, promotes readiness for
reconciliation. This possibility is consistent with the nding that perpetrators
guilt, but not shame, predicted reparative intentions towards the victim group
(Brown, González, Zagefka, Manzi, & Čehajić,2008). As for victims, it is
possible that an empowering message from their perpetrators, which places
both groups on more equal footing, reduces their experience of group-based
anger (Pennekamp, Doosje, Zebel, & Fischer, 2007), humiliation, and shame
(Branscombe & Doosje, 2004) and consequently increases their conciliatory
tendencies.
Another direction for future research would be to examine the type of message
that group members choose to convey to their adversarial out-group. Because
group members often project their own groups needs and views on the out-group
118 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
(Pearson et al., 2008), they might fail to realise that the out-groups motivations
and responses are fundamentally different from their own. Hence, it is important
to identify the factors that lead group members to convey the type of message
that would effectively satisfy the needs of their out-groups members and facil-
itate reconciliation. Work by Ditlmann, Purdie-Vaughns, Dovidio, and Naft
(2015), has made the rst step in this direction. Specically, Ditlmann and
colleagues found that in interracial dyadic interactions, African-American parti-
cipants who were highly motivated to act as agents of social change (i.e., with a
high implicit power motive; Winter, 1991) conveyed more afliative messages
to White participants with whom they discussed the implications of slavery for
contemporary American society. These afliative messages led, in turn, to
reduced anxiety and greater support for social change among the White partici-
pants. We are currently examining whether parallel effects would be obtained
among members of the perpetrator group. That is, we test whether members of a
perpetrator group who are high on implicit power motive intuitively understand
the needs of members of the victim group and choose to convey to them
empowering messages in discussions of historical injustices.
Finally, an additional direction for future research involves exploring the
psychological needs of victims of social exclusion. Specically, theorising on
gross human rights violations suggests that these incidents represent the denial
by perpetrators of their victimsmembership in the human community; victims
are therefore subjected to moral exclusion and dehumanisation, as they are
outside of [the perpetrators] scope of justice, barred from the protections of
community membership(Janoff-Bulman & Werther, 2008, p. 148). If so, they
may not only experience need for empowerment, as predicted by our model, but
also need to be included in the moral community whose members are entitled to
basic rights. Supporting this possibility, research in South Africa found that
White South Africans had a stronger need for acceptance than empowerment,
whereas Black South Africans experienced both needs equally. Whites were also
more willing to reconcile after their need for acceptance, compared to empower-
ment, was addressed, whereas Blacks were equally willing to reconcile following
the addressing of either need (Meyer & Ferraz, 2015). Thus, future research
should consider the circumstances under which victims experience not only
anger and a sense of injustice (resulting in heightened need for empowerment),
but also a deep sense of rejection.
In conclusion, compared to the study of conict resolution, the scientic study
of reconciliation, both within and outside social psychology (e.g., in primatol-
ogy; Silk, 2002), is relatively young. Because reconciliation in general, and
intergroup reconciliation in particular is a highly complex phenomenon, which
involves structural, relational, and identity-related aspects, much more research is
required to fully understand the processes that inhibit or facilitate its achieve-
ment. The needs-based model, presented in the present article, has attempted to
make a rst step in this direction. We hope that the joint efforts of ourselves, as
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 119
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
well as other researchers of this topic, will build a large body of knowledge that
may contribute not only to the theoretical understanding of reconciliation, but
also to the development of practical interventions to promote it.
REFERENCES
Abele, A. E., Cuddy, A. J. C., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2008). Fundamental dimensions of
social judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology,38, 10631065. doi:10.1002/ejsp.574
Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2013). The Big Two in social judgment and behavior. Social
Psychology,44,6162.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Avruch, K., & Vejerano, B. (2002). Truth and reconciliation commissions: A review essay and
annotated bibliography. Online Journal of Peace and Conict Resolution,4,3776.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social
Psychology Review,3, 193209. doi:10.1207/pspr.1999.3.issue-3
Barlow, F. K., Thai, M., Wohl, M. J. A., White, S., Wright, M.-A., & Hornsey, M. J. (2015).
Perpetrator groups can enhance their moral self-image by accepting their own intergroup apolo-
gies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,60,3950.
Bar-Tal, D. (2009). Reconciliation as a foundation of culture of peace. In J. de Rivera (Ed.),
Handbook on building cultures for peace (pp. 363377). New York, NY: Springer.
Bar-Tal, D. (2013). Intractable conicts: Socio-psychological foundations and dynamics. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Evil: Inside human cruelty and violence. New York, NY: W.H. Freeman/
Holt.
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach.
Psychological Bulletin,115 , 243267. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1995). Personal narratives about guilt: Role in
action control and interpersonal relationships. Basic and Applied Social Psychology,17, 173198.
doi:10.1080/01973533.1995.9646138
Becker, J. C. (2012). Virtual special issue on theory and research on collective action in the European
Journal of Social Psychology. European Journal of Social Psychology,42,1923. doi:10.1002/
ejsp.v42.1
Bergsieker, H. B., Shelton, J. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2010). To be liked versus respected: Divergent
goals in interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,99, 248264.
doi:10.1037/a0018474
Berndsen, M., Hornsey, M. J., & Wohl, M. J. A. (2015). The impact of a victim-focused apology on
forgiveness in an intergroup context. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations,18, 726739.
doi:10.1177/1368430215586275
Blader, S. L., & Chen, Y.-R. (2012). Differentiating the effects of status and power: A justice
perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,102, 9941014. doi:10.1037/
a0026651
Blatz, C. W., & Philpot, C. (2010). On the outcomes of intergroup apologies: A review. Social and
Personality Psychology Compass,4, 9951007. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00318.x
Blatz, C. W., Schumann, K., & Ross, M. (2009). Government apologies for historical injustices.
Political Psychology,30, 219241. doi:10.1111/pops.2009.30.issue-2
Boyes-Watson, C. (2008). Peacemaking circles and urban youth: Bringing justice home. St. Paul,
MN: Living Justice Press.
120 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
Braithwaite, J. (2002). Linking crime prevention to restorative justice. In J. Perry (Ed.), Repairing
communities through restorative justice (pp. 6783). Lanham, MD: American Correctional
Association.
Branscombe, N. R., & Cronin, T. (2010). Confronting the past to create a better future: The
antecedents and benets of intergroup forgiveness. In A. Azzi, X. Chryssochoou, B.
Klandermans, & B. Simon (Eds.), Identity and participation in culturally diverse societies (pp.
338358). New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell.
Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (Eds.). (2004). Collective guilt: International perspectives. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Brown, R., González, R., Zagefka, H., Manzi, J., & Čehajić, S. (2008). Nuestra culpa: Collective guilt
and shame as predictors of reparation for historical wrongdoing. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,94,7590. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.75
Brown, R. P., Wohl, M. J. A., & Exline, J. J. (2008). Taking up offenses: Secondhand forgiveness and
group identication. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,34, 14061419. doi:10.1177/
0146167208321538
Butler, E. A. (2011). Temporal interpersonal emotion systems: The TIESthat form relationships.
Personality and Social Psychology Review,15, 367393. doi:10.1177/1088868311411164
Campbell, D. T. (1965). Ethnocentric and other altruistic motives. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska
symposium on motivation (pp. 283301). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Cehajic, S., Brown, R. J., & Castano, E. (2008). Forgive and forget? Antecedents and consequences
of intergroup forgiveness in Bosnia and Herzegovina. Political Psychology,29, 351367.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00634.x
Choshen-Hillel, S., & Yaniv, I. (2011). Agency and the construction of social preference: Between
inequality aversion and prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,101,
12531261. doi:10.1037/a0024557.
Corkalo, D., Ajdukovic, D., Weinstein, H. M., Stover, E., Djipa, D., & Biro, M. (2004). Neighbors
again? Intercommunity relations after ethnic cleansing. In E. Stover & H. M. Weinstein (Eds.),
Justice and community in the aftermath of mass atrocity (pp. 143162). New York, NY:
Cambridge University Press.
Ditlmann, R. K., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Dovidio, J. F., & Naft, M. J. (2015). Bridging awareness about
slavery across the racial divide: Implicit power, African Americans, and interracial interactions.
Manuscript submitted for publication.
Dixon, J., Durrheim, K., & Tredoux, C. (2005). Beyond the optimal contact strategy: A reality check
for the contact hypothesis. American Psychologist,60,697711. doi:10.1037/0003-
066X.60.7.697
Doosje, B., Branscombe, N. R., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Guilty by association: When
ones group has a negative history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,75, 872886.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.872
Du Toit, F., & Doxtader, E. (Eds.). (2010). In the balance: South Africans debate reconciliation.
Johannesburg: Jacana Media.
Exline, J. J., & Baumaister, R. F. (2000). Expressing forgiveness and repentance: Benets and
barriers. In M. E. McCullough, K. I. Pargament, & C. E. Thoresen (Eds.), Forgiveness: Theory,
research and practice (pp. 133155). New York, NY: Guilford.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P. S., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype
content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,82, 878902. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878
Foster, C. A., & Rusbult, C. E. (1999). Injustice and power seeking. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin,25, 834849. doi:10.1177/0146167299025007006
Frijda, N. H. (1994). Varieties of affect: Emotions and episodes, moods, and sentiments. In P. Ekman
& R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 263289). New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 121
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The common intergroup identity
model. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
Galtung, J. (1969). Violence, peace, and peace research. Journal of Peace Research,6, 167191.
doi:10.1177/002234336900600301
Gibney, M., Howard-Hassmann, R. E., Coicaud, J. M., & Steiner, N. (2008). The age of apology:
Facing up to the past. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Gobodo-Madikizela, P. (2003). A human being died that night: A South African woman confronts the
legacy of apartheid. New York, NY: Houghton-Mifin.
Gopin, M. (2004). Healing the heart of conict: 8 crucial steps to making peace with yourself and
others. Emmaus, PA: Rodale Books.
Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2009). Moral typecasting: Divergent perceptions of moral agents and
moral patients. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,96(3), 505520. doi:10.1037/
a0013748
Gross, J. J., Halperin, E., & Porat, R. (2013). Emotion regulation in intractable conicts. Current
Directions in Psychological Science,22, 423429. doi:10.1177/0963721413495871
Hameiri, B., Porat, R., Bar-Tal, D., Bieler, A., & Halperin, E. (2014). Paradoxical thinking as a new
avenue of intervention to promote peace. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
USA,111, 1099611001. doi:10.1073/pnas.1407055111
Harth, N. S., Hornsey, M. J., & Barlow, F. K. (2011). Emotional responses to rejection of gestures of
intergroup reconciliation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,37, 815829. doi:10.1177/
0146167211400617
Harth, N. S., & Shnabel, N. (2015). Third-party intervention in intergroup reconciliation: Examining
the role of neutrality and common identity with the other conict-party. Group Processes and
Intergroup Relations,18, 676695. doi:10.1177/1368430215583151
Hewstone, M., Lolliot, S., Swart, H., Myers, E., Voci, A., Al Ramiah, A., & Cairns, E. (2014).
Intergroup contact and intergroup conict. Peace and Conict:Journal of Peace Psychology,20
(1), 3953. doi:10.1037/a0035582
Hughes, E., Scabas, W. A., & Thakur, R. (Eds.). (2007). Atrocities and international accountability:
Beyond transitional justice. New York, NY: United Nations University.
Iyer, A., & Leach, C. W. (2010). Helping disadvantaged out-groups challenge unjust inequality: The
role of group-based emotions. In S. Sturmer & M. Snyder (Eds.), New directions in the psychol-
ogy of helping: Group-level perspectives on motivation, consequences and interventions (pp. 337
353). Oxford: Blackwell.
Janoff-Bulman, R., & Werther, A. (2008). The social psychology of respect: Implications for
delegitimization and reconciliation. In A. Nadler, T. E. Malloy, & J. D. Fisher (Eds.), The social
psychology of inter-group reconciliation: From violent conict to peaceful co-existence (pp. 145
171). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Kelman, H. C. (2008). Reconciliation from a social-psychological perspective. In A. Nadler, T.
Malloy, & J. D. Fisher (Eds.), Social psychology of intergroup reconciliation (pp. 1532). New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Kriesberg, L. (2007). Aspects, growth and sequences. International Journal of Peace Studies,12,122.
Leach, C. W., Ellemers, N., & Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: The importance of morality (vs.
competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,93, 234249. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.234
Lederach, J. P. (1997). Building peace: Sustainable reconciliation in divided societies. Washington,
DC: U.S. Institute of Peace.
Lickel, B., Schmader, T., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The evocation of moral emotions in intergroup
contexts: The distinction between collective guilt and collective shame. In N. Branscombe & B.
Doosje (Eds.), Collective guilt: International perspectives (pp. 3555). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Lindner, E. (2006). Making enemies: Humiliation and international conict. London: Praeger.
122 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
Long, W. J., & Brecke, P. (2003). War and reconciliation: Reason and emotion in conict resolution.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Maoz, I., & Bar-On, D. (2002). From working through the Holocaust to current ethnic conicts:
Evaluating the TRT group workshop in Hamburg. Group,26,2948. doi:10.1023/
A:1015595027902
Mazziotta, A., Feuchte, F., Gausel, N., & Nadler, A. (2014). Does remembering past ingroup harm-
doing promote postwar cross-group contact? Insights from a eld-experiment in Liberia.
European Journal of Social Psychology,44,4352. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1986
Meierhenrich, J. (2008). Varieties of reconciliation. Law and Social Inquiry,33, 195231.
Meyer, I., & Ferraz, L. (2015). The needs-based model of reconciliation in the South African context:
Arst investigation. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Minow, M. (1998). Between vengeance and forgiveness: Facing history after genocide and mass
violence. Boston: Beacon Press.
Mummendey, A., Kessler, T., Klink, A., & Mielke, R. (1999). Strategies to cope with negative social
identity: Predictions by social identity theory and relative deprivation theory. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,76(2), 229245. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.229
Nadler, A. (2002). Post-resolution processes: Instrumental and socio-emotional routes to reconcilia-
tion. In G. Salomon & B. Nevo (Eds.), Peace education worldwide: The concept, principles and
practices around the world (pp. 127143). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nadler, A. (2011). Reconciliation: Instrumental and socio-emotional aspects. In D. J. Christie (Ed.),
Encyclopedia of peace psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.
Nadler, A. (2012). Intergroup reconciliation: Denitions, processes and future directions. In L. Tropp
(Ed.), The Oxford book of intergroup conict (pp. 291309). New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Nadler, A., & Saguy, T. (2004). Trust building and reconciliation between adversarial groups: A social
psychological perspective. In H. Langholtz & C. E. Stout (Eds.), The psychology of diplomacy
(pp. 2947). New York, NY: Praeger.
Nadler, A., & Shnabel, N. (2008). Intergroup reconciliation: The instrumental and socio-emotional
paths and the needs-based model of socio-emotional reconciliation. In A. Nadler, T. Malloy, & J.
D. Fisher (Eds.), Social psychology of intergroup reconciliation (pp. 3756). New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Nadler, A., & Shnabel, N. (2011). Promoting intergroup reconciliation in conicts involving direct
and structural violence: Implications of the needs-based model. In L. R. Tropp & R. K. Mallett
(Eds.), Beyond prejudice reduction: Pathways to positive intergroup relations (pp. 201219).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Newberg, A. B., dAquili, E. G., Newberg, S. K., & deMarici, V. (2000). The neuropsychological
correlates of forgiveness. In M. E. McCullough, K. I. Pargament, & C. E. Thoresen (Eds.),
Forgiveness: Theory, research and practice (pp. 91110). New York, NY: Guilford.
Noor, M., Brown, R., Gonzalez, R., Manzi, J., & Lewis, C. A. (2008). On positive outcomes: What
helps groups with a history of conict to forgive and reconcile with each other? Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin,34, 819832. doi:10.1177/0146167208315555
Noor, M., Shnabel, N., Halabi, S., & Nadler, A. (2012). When suffering begets suffering: The
psychology of competitive victimhood between adversarial groups in violent conicts.
Personality and Social Psychology Review,16, 351374. doi:10.1177/1088868312440048
North, J. (1998). The idealof forgiveness: A philosophers exploration. In R. E. Enright & J. North
(Eds.), Exploring forgiveness (pp. 1934). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
Osgood, C. E. (1962). An alternative to war or surrender. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Pearson, A. R., West, T. V., Dovidio, J. F., Powers, S. R., Buck, R., & Henning, R. (2008). The
fragility of intergroup relations: Divergent effects of delayed audiovisual feedback in intergroup
and intragroup interaction. Psychological Science,19, 12721279. doi:10.1111/psci.2008.19.
issue-12
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 123
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
Pennekamp, S. F., Doosje, B., Zebel, S., & Fischer, A. H. (2007). The past and the pending: The
antecedents and consequences of group-based anger in historically and currently disadvantaged
groups. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations,10(1), 4155. doi:10.1177/1368430207071339
Philpot, C. R., & Hornsey, M. J. (2008). What happens when groups say sorry: The effect of
intergroup apologies on their recipients. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,34, 474
487. doi:10.1177/0146167207311283
Psaltis, C. (2011). Intergroup trust and contact in transition: A social representations perspective on
the Cyprus conict. In I. Markova & A. Gillespie (Eds.), Trust and conict: Representations,
culture and dialogue (pp. 83104). London: Routledge.
Rouhana, N. (2004). Group identity and power asymmetry in reconciliation processes: The Israeli-
Palestinian case. Peace and Conict: Journal of Peace Psychology,10,3352. doi:10.1207/
s15327949pac1001_3
Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic
women. Journal of Social Issues,57, 743762. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00239
Sahdra, B., & Ross, M. (2007). Group identication and historical memory. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin,33, 384395. doi:10.1177/0146167206296103
Scheff, T. J. (1994). Bloody revenge: Emotions, nationalism and war. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Semin, G. (1997). The relevance of language for social psychology. In C. McGarty & S. A. Haslam
(Eds.), The message of social psychology: Perspectives on mind in society (pp. 291304).
Blackwell: Oxford.
Sherif, M. (1958). Superordinate goals in the reduction of intergroup conict. American Journal of
Sociology,63, 349356.
Sherif, M. (1967). Group conict and cooperation. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Shnabel, N., Halabi, S., & Noor, M. (2013). Overcoming competitive victimhood and facilitating
forgiveness through re-categorization into common victim or perpetrator identity. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology,49, 867877. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.04.007
Shnabel, N., Halabi, S., & SimanTov-Nachlieli, I. (2015). Group apology under unstable status
relations: Perceptions of insincerity hinder reconciliation and forgiveness. Group Processes and
Intergroup Relations,18,716725. doi:10.1177/1368430214546069
Shnabel, N., & Nadler, A. (2008). A needs-based model of reconciliation: Satisfying the differential
emotional needs of victim and perpetrator as a key to promoting reconciliation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,94,116132. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.116
Shnabel, N., Nadler, A., & Dovidio, J. F. (2014). Beyond need satisfaction: Empowering and
accepting messages from third parties ineffectively restore trust and consequent reconciliation.
European Journal of Social Psychology,44, 126140. doi:10.1002/ejsp.v44.2
Shnabel, N., Nadler, A., Ullrich, J., Dovidio, J. F., & Carmi, D. (2009). Promoting reconciliation
through the satisfaction of the emotional needs of victimized and perpetrating group members:
The needs-based model of reconciliation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,35, 1021
1030. doi:10.1177/0146167209336610
Shnabel, N., & Noor, M. (2012). Competitive victimhood among Jewish and Palestinian Israelis
reects differential threats to their identities: The perspective of the needs based model. In R. J.
Jonas & T. A. Merton (Eds.), Restoring civil societies: The psychology of intervention and
engagement following crisis (pp. 192207). New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell.
Shnabel, N., Ulrich, J., Nadler, A., Dovidio, J. F., & Aydin, A. L. (2013). Warm or competent?
Improving intergroup relations by addressing threatened identities of advantaged and disadvan-
taged groups. European Journal of Social Psychology,43, 482492. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1975
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Siem, B., Von Oetingen, M., Mummendey, A., & Nadler, A. (2013). Divergent motives of high- and
low-status group members: The moderating role of perceived legitimacy of status differences.
European Journal of Social Psychology,43, 137148. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1929
124 NADLER AND SHNABEL
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
Silk, J. B. (2002). The form and function of reconciliation in primates. Annual Review of
Anthropology,31,2144.
SimanTov-Nachlieli, I., & Shnabel, N. (2014). Feeling both victim and perpetrator: Investigating
duality within the needs-based model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,40, 301314.
doi:10.1177/0146167213510746
SimanTov-Nachlieli, I., Shnabel, N., Aydin, A. L., & Ullrich, J. (2015). Agents of Morality: Afrming
conicting groupsagency promotes mutual prosociality. Manuscript submitted for publication.
SimanTov-Nachlieli, I., Shnabel, N., & Nadler, A. (2013). Individualsand groupsmotivation to
restore their impaired identity dimensions following conicts. Social Psychology,44, 129137.
doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000148
Staub, E. (2006). Reconciliation after genocide, mass killing, or intractable conict: Understanding
the roots of violence, psychological recovery, and steps toward a general theory. Political
Psychology,27, 867894. doi:10.1111/pops.2006.27.issue-6
Sullivan, D., Landau, M. J., Branscombe, N. R., & Rothschild, Z. K. (2012). Competitive victimhood
as a response to accusations of ingroup harm doing. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,102, 778795. doi:10.1037/a0026573
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In W. G. Austin & S.
Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 724). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J. B., Cairns, E., Marinetti, C., Geddes, L., & Parkinson, B.
(2008). Postconict reconciliation: Intergroup forgiveness and implicit biases in Northern Ireland.
Journal of Social Issues,64, 303320. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00563.x
Tavuchis, N. (1991). Mea Culpa: A sociology of apology and reconciliation. Redwood City, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Blackwell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the
social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (2008). Toward an integrative social identity model of
collective action: A quantitative research synthesis of three socio-psychological perspectives.
Psychological Bulletin,134, 504535. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.504
Volkan, V. D. (2001). Transgenerational transmissions and chosen traumas: An aspect of large group
identity. Group Analysis,34,7997. doi:10.1177/05333160122077730
Vollhardt, J. R. (2009). The role of victim beliefs in the Israeli-Palestinian conict: Risk or potential
for peace? Peace and Conict: Journal of Peace Psychology,15, 135159. doi:10.1080/
10781910802544373
Whitman, J. (2003). A plea against retributivism. Buffalo Criminal Law Review,7,85107.
doi:10.1525/nclr.2003.7.1.85
Winter, D. G. (1991). A motivational model of leadership: Predicting long-term management success
from TAT measures of power motivation and responsibility. The Leadership Quarterly,2,6780.
doi:10.1016/1048-9843(91)90023-U
Wohl, M. J. A., Branscombe, N. R., & Klar, Y. (2006). Collective guilt: Emotional reactions when
ones group has done wrong or been wronged. European Review of Social Psychology,17,137.
doi:10.1080/10463280600574815
Wohl, M. J. A., Hornsey, M. J., & Philpot, C. R. (2011). A critical review of ofcial public apologies:
Aims, pitfalls and a staircase model of effectiveness. Social Issues and Policy Review,5,70100.
doi:10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01026.x
Woodyatt, L., & Wenzel, M. (2014). A needs-based perspective on self-forgiveness: Addressing threat
to moral identity as a means of encouraging interpersonal and intrapersonal restoration. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology,50, 125135. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.09.012
INTERGROUP RECONCILIATION 125
Downloaded by [Tel Aviv University] at 04:12 03 August 2017
... According to the experimental research of [50], the advantaged Israeli group tends to focus on intergroup reconciliation, commonality, and acceptance, while the disadvantaged Palestinian group wishes to get empowered by acknowledging the asymmetrical power relations between the groups and confronting them directly [51]. An extended model [52] introduces a 'dual' form, in which the advantaged group also experiences victimhood and seeks to empower its sense of agency when confronting the outgroup. To evaluate these differences, we added open questions about how participants envision the telerobotic encounter. ...
... We observe that not only do Palestinians see their national identity as a means of empowerment, but so do Israeli Jew participants. This corresponds to the 'dual' model of Nadler and Shanbel [52], in which both the advantaged and the disadvantaged groups feel a threat to their agency and identity and wish to restore it through contact. Religion, however, did not play a strong role in the desire to portray an identity to the outgroup. ...
Article
Full-text available
We explore telerobotics as a novel form of intergroup communication. In this form, remotely operated robots facilitate embodied and situated intergroup contact between groups in conflict over long distances, potentially reducing prejudice and promoting positive social change. Based on previous conceptual frameworks and design hypotheses, we conducted a survey on the acceptance and preferences of the telerobotic medium in Israel and Palestine. We analyzed the responses using a mixed-method approach. The results shed light on differences in attitudes between the groups and design considerations for telerobots when used for intergroup contact. This study serves as a foundation for the implementation of a novel method of technology-enhanced conflict resolution in the field.
... Moreover, marginalized groups who are victims of past historical atrocities (such as Jewish individuals living in Germany) prefer empowering messages (such as messages that note the importance of elevating their power and position in society) to messages that focus on acceptance between groups 124 . Thus, particularly in instances of historical and ongoing conflict, marginalized individuals want activism-related actions that restore their power and enhance their status 124,125 . Advantaged individuals can support these aims in two ways: by engaging in activism to remove the structures oppressing marginalized groups and by taking supportive, low-power positions in social movements 15,65,98 . ...
... For instance, learning about privilege can undermine esteem for the ingroup, but engaging in supportive actions for a marginalized outgroup helps to restore positive feelings about the ingroup 81,172 . Indeed, advantaged individuals care about being moral and accepted during interactions with marginalized individuals 30,174 , and engaging in ally behaviours provides one route to fulfilling these needs 125 . Thinking about ways in which marginalized groups are disadvantaged can also be motivating because it increases perceptions that society is unjust and prompts a desire to undo this injustice 175 . ...
... belong to, identify with, and derive value from groups (Cikara et al., 2011); just as we care about our own reputations, so too do we care about the image of the groups with which we are closely identified (Cialdini et al., 1976;Snyder et al., 1986). Although understudied, a concern with the reputation of one's group can be a force for good, for example, leading people to avoid engaging in immoral behavior that reflects poorly on their group (e.g., Doosje et al., 1998) or to act in conciliatory or restorative ways to improve the ingroup's image (Kardos et al., 2019;Lickel et al., 2011;Nadler & Shnabel, 2015). But might group-based reputational incentives sometimes conflict withand potentially offset-other valued considerations? ...
... relatively prosocial implications. For example, research shows that individuals care about their group being seen as moral and can become more motivated (including via moral emotions like guilt or shame) to act in conciliatory or restorative ways to improve the ingroup's image (e.g., Nadler & Shnabel, 2015;Kardos et al., 2019;Lickel et al., 2011). But humans are also highly attuned to their group's status relative to competitive outgroups (Cikara et al., 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
People’s concern with maintaining their individual reputation powerfully drives judgment and decision making. But humans also identify strongly with groups. Concerns about group-based reputation may similarly shape people’s psychology, perhaps especially in contexts where shifts in group reputation can have strategic consequences. Do individuals allow their concern with their group’s reputation to shape their reactions to even large-scale societal suffering versus benefits? Examining both affective responses and financially incentivized behavior of partisans in the United States, five preregistered experiments (N = 7,534) demonstrate that group-based reputational incentives can weaken—and sometimes nearly eliminate—affective differentiation between present-term societal harms and benefits. This can occur even when these societal harms and benefits are substantial—including economic devastation and national security threats—and when the consequences impact ingroup members. Individuals’ sensitivity to group-based reputation can even cause them to divert resources from more effective to less effective charities. We provide evidence that partisans care about group-based reputation in part because it holds strategic value, positioning their group to improve its standing vis-a-vis the outgroup. By allowing group-based reputational incentives to reduce their sensitivity to societal outcomes, partisans may play into the other side’s cynical narratives about their disregard for human suffering, damaging bridges to cooperation.
... As groups' moral examples, victims are playing an increasingly core role in transition processes towards peace (Druliolle & Brett, 2018). In this sense, meeting their demands is imperative, and in the needs-based reconciliation model (Nadler & Shnabel, 2015), this includes restoring their sense of agency, telling them the truth about what happened and enabling them to perceive justice. Victims' voices, that is, their testimonies and opinions, are one way of restoring their social competence, plus they become a powerful instrument for generating changes in societies in conflict (Ibreck, 2018;Shepherd, 2016). ...
... Las víctimas, como ejemplos morales de los grupos, tienen un rol cada vez más central en los procesos de transición hacia la paz (Druliolle & Brett, 2018). En ese sentido, satisfacer sus demandas es imperativo, y en términos del modelo de reconciliación basado en las necesidades (Nadler & Shnabel, 2015), eso incluye restaurar su sentido de agencia, que conozcan la verdad sobre lo ocurrido y que perciban justicia. La voz de las víctimas, es decir, sus testimonios y opiniones, emerge como una forma de restaurar su competencia social y, además, se convierte en un instrumento poderoso para generar cambios en las sociedades en conflicto (Ibreck, 2018;Shepherd, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Victims’ testimonies have emerged as an interesting tool for generating changes in societies that are seeking to emerge from a violent conflict. However, victims are not a homogeneous group and their stances regarding the perpetrators can different according to whether they accept or reject intergroup forgiveness processes, as was the case of J. Améry, a writer and essayist who survived the Holocaust. In this article, we inquire into the impact of victims’ testimonies on attitudes towards intergroup forgiveness in Spain ( N = 274). An experimental study with three conditions was designed using victims’ testimonies that were favourable (Condition 1) and unfavourable (Condition 2) to intergroup forgiveness, along with a condition with statistical data on violence (Condition 0). The results show a differential impact on attitudes based on the victim’s testimony, in addition to emotional activation that is not inherently related to attitudes in favour of forgiveness but instead to the victim’s attitude towards that process.
Article
Because of the growing evidence pointing to the adverse impact of competitive victimhood on intergroup relations, research has focused on revealing what motivates conflicting groups to engage in competitive victimhood. Whereas Sullivan et al. showed that need for morality—that is, protecting ingroup's moral identity—predicted engagement in competitive victimhood, Kahalon et al. found that when considered simultaneously, need for power was the primary motivator of competitive victimhood. The main objective of the present research was to replicate Kahalon et al.'s Study 1 findings, testing the robustness of their results by conducting it in the context of a unique threat (i.e., COVID‐19). Our results, involving a well‐powered sample of Jews ( N = 205) and Arabs ( N = 152) living in Israel, demonstrated that while need for morality and need for power individually related to competitive victimhood, when included simultaneously in a regression need for power but not need morality predicted competitive victimhood among members of both a disadvantaged group (Arabs living in Israel) and an advantaged group (Jews living in Israel). Replicating the results from Kahalon et al. in the unique context of the COVID‐19 indicates the persistent position that competitive victimhood plays in Arab‐Jewish intergroup relations and helps to illuminate its underlying dynamics.
Article
In many societies around the world, segments of the public strongly distrust legal and political authorities. Regardless of how the distrust arises, it lessens the possibilities for future social cohesion, democratic governance, and successful economic development—factors that define strong communities. How can authorities build trust amid a legacy of distrust? In this review, the authors focus on relations between the police and communities and draw on two psychological literatures that articulate evidence-informed trust-building strategies. One, the procedural justice approach, concentrates on the fair and respectful exercise of authority during everyday interactions between individuals. The other, reconciliation, involves gestures that are carried out at the community level with the expressed intention of addressing past injustice and that promise changes in an authority's future relations with a community. This review concludes with policy recommendations, drawn from both literatures, describing a process of trust building that involves substantive improvements in procedural justice combined with reconciliatory gestures that signal a sincere intent to increase trust through service to communities.
Article
Many governments have invested in interventions that foster social cohesion to promote peace and prevent the rise of violent extremism. Yet, we know little about the relationship between social cohesion and support for violent extremism. This research ( N = 5016) examined cross‐sectional relationships between different dimensions of social cohesion and violent extremism in 132 villages in the Sahel region of Burkina Faso (Study 1). In addition, using longitudinal data from 66 villages (Study 2: N = 2508), we assessed whether social cohesion predicts violence justification six months later. At the individual level, sense of community, outgroup trust and institutional trust were associated with lower justification of violence. Community participation was associated with higher justification of violence in villages with high levels of community participation. Between‐village effects revealed that justification of violence was lower in villages with higher aggregate levels of ingroup and outgroup trust but lower levels of community participation.
Article
Within‐group apologies are effective for governments to address injustices, but they often hesitate due to public reception concerns. With justice theory as the starting point, this study investigated the psychological effects of within‐group apologies, excuses and silence between the empathetic group towards victims' experiences (i.e. victim‐empathic group) and the observers (Study 1) and between the actual victims and the observers (Study 2). Findings indicate that within‐group apologies restore justice and social engagement for all participants, while excuses and silence are ineffective. Victims' justice restoration was notably weaker than observers when encountering the apology (Study 2), while the victim‐empathic group had similar justice restoration as observers (Study 1). Thus, within‐group apologies are more effective at restoring a sense of justice for observers than victims.
Article
The Nakba, which means “the catastrophe” in Arabic, is the most controversial historical topic in Israeli history education. Despite the Nakba’s significance to the history of Israel and the ongoing Israeli-Palestinian conflict, until the last decade it has traditionally been excluded from Israeli public discourse and school curriculum. This article analyzes the different types of ethical judgments about the Nakba included in authorized curricula, teaching resources, and national exams currently used in the Israeli Jewish public education systems. Our data analysis reveals that the Nakba is explicitly mentioned in 40% of teaching materials used in Israeli Jewish schools, and the teaching materials include six types of implied ethical judgments. We developed a typology of three ethical justifications commonly utilized in the teaching materials: denial; acknowledging suffering, limited responsibility; and complex engagement. While some of the teaching materials promote critical engagement with the Nakba, others continue to deny its existence or minimize the negative consequences experienced by Palestinians. This research highlights how political beliefs influence the ethical judgments made in teaching materials and the importance of teaching about ethical judgments for helping students critically engage with and understand difficult histories.
Article
Full-text available
Stereotype research emphasizes systematic processes over seemingly arbitrary contents, but content also may prove systematic. On the basis of stereotypes' intergroup functions, the stereotype content model hypothesizes that (a) 2 primary dimensions are competence and warmth, (b) frequent mixed clusters combine high warmth with low competence (paternalistic) or high competence with low warmth (envious), and (c) distinct emotions (pity, envy, admiration, contempt) differentiate the 4 competence-warmth combinations. Stereotypically, (d) status predicts high competence, and competition predicts low warmth. Nine varied samples rated gender, ethnicity, race, class, age, and disability out-groups. Contrary to antipathy models, 2 dimensions mattered, and many stereotypes were mixed, either pitying (low competence, high warmth subordinates) or envying (high competence, low warmth competitors). Stereotypically, status predicted competence, and competition predicted low warmth.
Article
Full-text available
This chapter focuses on the role of respect in intergroup reconciliation. It first distinguishes between two types of respect - one largely intergroup and the other primarily intragroup in nature - and discusses the attributional components of these appraisals. It then moves to a discussion of disrespect and its implications for delegitimization, from invisibility to dehumanization, which is of paramount importance in the course and escalation of social conflict; the attributional elements of respect provide an important window for viewing these degrading processes. The chapter concludes with some implications of the analysis for reconciliation, focusing particularly on respectenhancing strategies in intense social conflicts.
Article
Full-text available
The chapter begins by considering the concept of reconciliation in scientific study in general, and research on intergroup relations in particular. On the basis of this review it proposes a distinction between structural, relational and identity-related aspects of intergroup reconciliation and suggests that these aspects are hierarchically organized. It continues to focus on relational and identity-related aspects of intergroup reconciliation by discussing the distinction between processes of instrumental and socio-emotional reconciliation. It continues to analyze the psychological dynamics in identity-related reconciliation and describes the need based model of reconciliation and relevant empirical support. Subsequent sections consider the effects of 'truth-telling' in the post-conflict era on societal and individual healing. A final chapter focuses on the distinction between false and genuine reconciliation and reviews scholarly work that emphasizes the place of victim resentment in genuine intergroup reconciliation.
Book
This book is the first systematic examination of the impact of reconciliation on restoring and maintaining peace following civil and international conflicts. Through eleven comparative case studies of civil war and eight of international conflict, it constructs a surprising explanation for when and why reconciliation restores social order.The civil war cases reveal that successful reconciliation is associated with a process of national forgiveness, not merely negotiated settlement. All successful cases followed a four-step pattern of public truth telling, justice short of revenge, redefinition of the identities of former belligerents, and a call for a new relationship. The book argues that success is not solely the result of rational choice decision making. It proposes a hypothesis, grounded in evolutionary psychology, that to restore social order we use emotional/cognitive techniques that have evolved to ensure human survival. On the international level, however, successful reconciliation was not a part of a forgiveness process. Reconciliation was successful in bringing about sustained peace when it was associated with a signaling process—an exchange of costly, novel, voluntary, and irrevocable concessions in a negotiated bargain. This result is consistent with realist notions of the limits of international society and illustrates the context in which a rational choice model is appropriate. The book's approach, integrating emotion with reasoning and linking political science to scientific research in other disciplines, particularly biology and neuroscience, has broad implications for social science theory.
Article
How can survivors of wars inflamed by ethnic hatred rebuild their lives? How do they describe their former enemies, now their neighbors? What role does justice play in the process of rebuilding communities? And what will it take to re-establish trust among former neighbors torn apart by communal violence? In the summer of 2000, we set out to answer those questions through a series of long-term studies in three war-ravaged cities in Bosnia and Herzegovina (Mostar and Prijedor) and Croatia (Vukovar). As Weinstein and Stover have noted in the Introduction to this volume, these cities, once vibrant and thriving urban centers, are now deeply divided along ethnic lines. While the “ethnic divide” in Mostar is facilitated by the River Neretva, with Bosniaks living on the east bank and Croats living on the west bank, neither Vukovar nor Prijedor has such a physical demarcation. Instead, a “psychological wall” exists in both these cities, separating Croats from Serbs in Vukovar, and Bosniaks from Serbs in Prijedor. In all three cities, people from opposing ethnic groups who once lived together peacefully now harbor deep-seated resentments and suspicions of one another, making it difficult to renew social relationships or to form new ones. Our studies examined the views of residents in these three cities regarding war, justice, and the prospects for reconciliation. In Mostar and Vukovar, we studied the daily lives of residents over a two-year period.
Article
Conflict, in many cases extending to war, genocide, enslavement, and other forms of violence and domination, is the unfortunate hallmark of much of intergroup relations. Nonetheless, though humans tend to view even the most violent acts of their group against outsiders as morally justified, there are exceptions. Some individuals, in some situations, experience compunction for what people in their group have done to outsiders. Understanding when such reactions are likely to occur, and the consequences of such responses, is a potentially crucial element in attenuating intergroup conflict. Our interest in understanding collective guilt concerns when individuals will take responsibility for and make reparations for their group's mistreatment of another group. It is important to recognize that the word “guilt” can be used to refer to both an emotional response (“I feel guilty”) and a judgment of responsibility (“I am guilty”). Obviously, the two usages are related, however, our work is derived from a conceptualization of guilt first and foremost as an emotional experience. As such, our approach is grounded in basic research on emotion and strives to describe the interpretative process by which a person proceeds from a simple awareness that a negative intergroup event has taken place to having an emotional reaction and a behavioral response to that event. Our primary objective in this chapter is to outline some of the main steps in this interpretive process.