ArticlePDF Available

A quantitative study of "friends with benefits" relationships

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Canadian undergraduates (146 men and 135 women; ages 18-40, M = 20.9, SD = 3.4) described their experiences with "friends with benefits" relationships (FWBRs). Responses were coded and analysed using chi-square contingency tables and multinomial regression analysis. Study results link some of the previously identified advantages and disadvantages of FWBRs to relational outcomes (whether the experience was positive, negative, or neutral; and willingness to enter a FWBR again). Most participants reported positive (38%) or neutral (37%) FWBR experiences, yet 40% indicated they would not enter a FWBR again. Developing emotional complications throughout the relationship occurred in 22% of participants, men and women equally, and strongly predicted negative outcomes (odds ratio 9.5, p < 0.001 for negative experience; odds ratio 2.8, p = 0.007 for not wanting a FWBR again). Women were also significantly more likely than men to enter the relationship, hoping it would evolve into dating (p < 0.001), and to express desire to avoid a FWBR in the future (odds ratio 3.3, p < 0.001). Results indicated that other gender differences in FWBRs are nuanced, and both confirm and depart from the traditional gender norms.
Content may be subject to copyright.
41
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
Introduction
Casual relationships are gaining increased acceptance
by young people today (Manning, Giordano, &
Longmore, 2006). One type of such relationships is
“friends with bene ts,” de ned as a “new relational
style that blends aspects of friendship and physical
intimacy” (Owen & Fincham, 2011a, p. 311).
Consequently, most academic investigation into the
phenomenon of friends with bene ts relationships
(FWBRs) occurred only within the last decade
(Weaver, MacKeigan, & MacDonald, 2011).
In the literature, FWBRs have been uniformly
characterized by (a) sexual intimacy, (b) ongoing
friendship, and (c) desire or agreement between the
participants to avoid of cial romantic commitment
(Bisson & Levine, 2009; Hughes et al., 2005;
Lehmiller, VanderDrift, & Kelly, 2011; Owen
& Fincham, 2011a). This de nition, however, is
vague. While the rst characteristic seems obvious,
it remains unclear whether “ongoing friendship”
is a requirement for a FWBR. Furthermore, does
every couple engaging in a FWBR “desire or agree”
to begin this form of relationship? How does this
account for those who simply “plunge into” this
form of connection? We prefer to de ne a FWBR
as a relationship between two people who begin as
friends or acquaintances and subsequently introduce
some degree of sexual intimacy for an undetermined
period of time, which participants themselves regard
as a non-dating relationship. Existing literature
demonstrates that approximately 50 to 60% of
young people have been involved in at least one
FWBR in their lifetime (Bisson & Levine, 2009;
Hughes, Morrison, & Asada, 2005; Puentes, Knox,
& Zusman, 2008); including adolescents as young
as 14 (Chernin, Rich, & Shing, 2010).
Virtually all research on the subject of FWBRs comes
from the U.S. However, many social differences exist
between Canadians and Americans, which could
translate into “cross-cultural differences in sexual
attitudes and behaviours” (Fischtein, Herold, &
Desmarais, 2007, p. 452). For example, Canadians
report lower religiosity compared to Americans
(measured through service attendance), and 40% of
Canadians under the age of 25 do not identify with a
particular religion (Fischtein et al., 2007). Meanwhile,
religion or morality was listed as a common reason to
avoid FWBRs in the U.S. (Bisson & Levine, 2009).
Also, church attendance was inversely correlated with
the likelihood of establishing a FWBR in the U.S.
(McGinty, Knox, & Zusman, 2007).
A quantitative study of “friends with bene ts” relationships
Inga Gusarova1, Vanessa Fraser1, and Kevin G. Alderson1
1 Division of Applied Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta
Abstract: Canadian undergraduates (146 men and 135 women; ages 18-40, M = 20.9, SD = 3.4)
described their experiences with “friends with bene ts” relationships (FWBRs). Responses were coded
and analysed using chi-square contingency tables and multinomial regression analysis. Study results
link some of the previously identi ed advantages and disadvantages of FWBRs to relational outcomes
(whether the experience was positive, negative, or neutral; and willingness to enter a FWBR again). Most
participants reported positive (38%) or neutral (37%) FWBR experiences, yet 40% indicated they would
not enter a FWBR again. D eveloping emotional complications throughout the relationship occurred in 22%
of participants, men and women equally, and strongly predicted negative outcomes (odds ratio 9.5, p <
0.001 for negative experience; odds ratio 2.8, p = 0.007 for not wanting a FWBR again). Women were also
signi cantly more likely than men to enter the relationship, hoping it would evolve into dating (p < 0.001),
and to express desire to avoid a FWBR in the future (odds ratio 3.3, p < 0.001). Results indicated that other
gender differences in FWBRs are nuanced, and both con rm and depart from the traditional gender norms.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kevin Alderson, Ph.D., Division of Applied Psychology,
University of Calgary, 2500 University Drive N.W., Calgary Alberta T2N 1N4. E-mail:alderson@ucalgary.ca
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 201242
For these reasons, FWBRs may be more common
or accepted in Canada. As such, there is value in
studying FWBRs within the Canadian cultural
landscape. To date, only two Canadian studies
examining FWBRs have been published (Weaver
et al., 2011; Wentland & Reissing, 2011). Both of
these studies utilized qualitative samples of 26 and 23
participants, respectively, suggesting that additional
research in this area is warranted.
Advantages and impacts of FWBRs
The most commonly cited reasons and advantages of
FWBRs are hardly surprising: sex, particularly with
a trusted, comfortable, and safe other, while avoiding
romantic commitment (Bisson & Levine, 2009;
Weaver et al., 2011). Additional factors, described
by the same authors, include positive emotions
and the pleasure of the experience, closeness and
companionship, non-exclusivity and a sense of
freedom, and opportunity or convenience.
The mention of pleasure is consistent with other
ndings. Participants in FWBRs were highly likely
to report hedonism (i.e., focus on pleasure rather than
relationship) as a value (Puentes et al., 2008; Richey,
Knox, & Zusman, 2009). There is also an interesting
notion that the appeal of FWBRs may be time-limited
as a good t for the current stage of life. Being at
college or university is seen as an exploratory period
of personal development, with an expectation of
cultivating more serious relationships upon growing
older (Kalish, 2009; Weaver et al., 2011).
Weaver et al. (2011) provided insight into the two-fold
justi cations for avoiding commitment. On one hand,
there is the wish to escape the drawbacks associated
with it, such as drama, complications, worry, hurt, and
“messiness” (Weaver et al., p. 46). On the other hand,
there is the desire to pursue freedom, non-exclusivity,
experimentation, and independence. Probing further,
Weaver et al. assessed how many participants were
non-exclusive in FBWRs, both through their own
study and in citing work by Patterson and Price
(2009). Patterson and Price conducted an Internet-
based survey of 297 individuals, while Weaver et
al. performed semi-structured interviews with 26
young adults. Interestingly, both studies arrived at
the exact same number: 44% of participants in each
case reported sexual relations with more than one
partner concurrently. These ndings demonstrate
that the majority of FWBR participants (i.e., 56%)
pursued non-committed yet exclusive liaisons. It
appears that steering clear of the effort required in
building traditional relationships may be slightly
more common than having multiple sexual partners.
Some researchers have questioned whether the
reasons for and experiences of people entering
FWBRs re ect a conscious decision. In other words,
“How much thought is given to the decision to start
a FWBR? Epstein, Calzo, Smiler, and Ward (2009)
conceptualized FWBRs as closely related to hook-
ups, which are primarily spontaneous in nature. Once
triggered by immediate stimuli such as sexual drive,
alcohol, or stress, hook-ups follow an established
script. These authors consider the addition of
friendship and the possibility of ongoing encounters
as the only differences between FWBRs and hook-
ups. Owen and Fincham (2011a) reported that alcohol
increased the odds of engaging in a FWBR but also
that thoughtful adults were more likely to avoid
FWBRs even when alcohol was present. These two
studies suggest that the decision to enter a FWBR is
impulsive and governed by immediate cues with little
regard for future consequences.
In contrast, Bisson and Levine (2009) and Weaver et
al. (2011) found that many participants in FWBRs
were seeking trust and comfort, as well as consciously
avoiding emotional complications and potential
hurt associated with romantic commitment. Trust
and safety also advantageously distinguish FWBRs
from hook-ups, in that hook-ups are more likely
to involve a risk of pressure into unwanted sexual
activities (Paul & Hayes, 2002). These observations
suggest that the choice of FWBRs over either random
encounters or traditional dating relationships is
an examined decision. Clearly, both scenarios for
entering a FWBR, spontaneous and examined,
seem to take place. At present, it is not known how
prominent each of these scenarios is or how gender or
situational context affect the decision-making process.
Despite concerns expressed in both academic
literature and popular culture about psychological
damage, Eisenberg, Ackard, Resnick, and Neumark-
Sztainer (2009) found that casual sex presents no
increased risk of harm. Psychological well-being
43
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
scores on body satisfaction, self-esteem, depressive
symptoms, and suicidal ideation “were notably
similar across sex partner categories, spanning less
than one point in many cases” (Eisenberg et al., p.
234). Bay-Cheng, Robinson, and Zucker (2009)
commented on the limitations of existing research,
noting that there is a difference between showing no
increased risk and obtaining bene t. These authors
claim that the academic literature is as affected by the
negative bias toward sexuality as is popular culture,
since most studies are designed to investigate the
harm rather than the good outcomes arising from
adolescent sexual practices.
Sex and gender differences
Fischtein et al. (2007) reported that a majority of
men (55%) compared to a vast minority of women
(8%) would entertain the thought of sex with
someone they just met. In terms of actual behaviours
among adolescents, Manning et al. (2006) found
that signi cantly more boys (68%) than girls (52%)
reported a non-dating sexual experience. With
respect to FWBRs in particular, gender differences
in prevalence rates have also been reported;
54% for men and 43% for women reported such
relationships (Owen & Fincham, 2011a). In contrast,
Bisson and Levine (2009) found no statistically
signi cant gender differences in prevalence of FWBR
experience although their sample of 125 participants
was much smaller than the 889 participants in the
study by Owen and Fincham.
With respect to reasons for engaging in FWBRs,
the ndings indicate that men primarily want and
value sex whereas women more often emphasize
emotional connection (Lehmiller et al., 2011;
McGinty et al., 2007). Research indicates that there
is a disproportionate distribution of costs and bene ts
for men versus women when it comes to casual sex
(Eshbaugh & Gute, 2008; Grello, Welsh, & Harper,
2006; Owen & Fincham, 2011b; Paul & Hayes,
2002). Men seem to reap the bene ts of casual sex
which is associated with the lowest symptoms of
depression or distress. Women appear to bear the
cost in that casual sex is linked to higher prevalence
of depression and greater likelihood of emotional
ambivalence and regret afterwards. That being
said, Dworkin and O’Sullivan (2005) reported on
the expressed dissatisfaction of some men with the
cultural roles assigned to them and on their expressed
desire for more egalitarian sexual dynamics within
relationships. Other studies indicating that women
do not hold a monopoly on desiring emotional
connections to their sexual partners found signi cant
numbers of men who expressed similar preferences
(Epstein et al., 2009) or displayed strong commitment
to the friendship and closeness aspects of FWBRs
(Lehmiller et al., 2011). Indeed, in the Epstein et
al. (2009) investigation, most men rejected non-
relational scripts and preferred ones that increased
emotional closeness.
The traditional “double standard” has been described
“as a prescriptive social standard, in which women
were permitted to engage in sexual relations only
within a committed love relationship, whereas men
were permitted to have as many sexual partners
as they wanted without condition” (Milhausen &
Herold, 1999, p. 361). By the time of their 1999
study of Canadian female undergraduate students,
Milhausen and Herold found that 99% of their
respondents stated that women could enjoy sex as
much as men do, 69% disagreed with women being
less interested in sex than men, and 76% stated that
sex was either very or somewhat important to them.
Despite such attitudes toward sex, the authors also
found that most women enforced the idea of a sexual
double standard and held negative attitudes toward
female promiscuity. However, a noticeable minority
(29%) described women with multiple partners
positively, using words such as “independent,”
“unashamed,” and “in touch with her own desires”
(Milhausen & Herold, p. 365).
Consistent with these observations, Lehmiller et
al. (2011) found that most women reported sexual
desire as a motive for initiating an FWBR, although
not a primary one. The authors speculated that the
sexual double standard and the need to legitimize sex
by emotional involvement might be the issues here
rather than women not actually wanting or enjoying
sex. Looking at heterosexual Canadian women and
casual sex in general, Weaver and Herold (2000)
found that sexual pleasure was the most common
reason for engaging in casual sex. They also reported
that direct experience was correlated with both
increased acceptance and expectation of enjoyment
from casual sex, which suggests that it was not
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 201244
necessarily personal experience but rather a socially
upheld double standard that held women back from
casual sex. This observation probably also pertains
to FWBRs, a speci c example of casual sex. The
continuing importance of social acceptability is also
re ected in the study by Weaver et al (2011) who
found that 77% of participants believed that women
were judged more harshly than men for taking part
in FWBRs despite the fact that the female prevalence
rate for FWBRs was 43% compared to 50% for men
(Owen & Fincham, 2011a; Puentes et al, 2008).
Bay-Cheng et al. (2009) found that FWBRs were
associated with the highest self-ratings of desire,
wanting, and pleasure compared to all other serious
and casual relationship experiences reported by
women. While the differences were large and
statistically signi cant between FWBRs and hook-ups,
they were less so between FWBRs and committed
romantic relationships. The signi cant differences
between FWBRs and hook-ups could be explained by
the companionate and repeated nature of the former.
Grello et al. (2006) partially supported this notion,
indicating that FWBRs, far more than hook ups,
were associated with affectionate sexual behaviours
potentially related to increased emotional intimacy
(e.g., kissing, holding hands, and hugging). Female
regret about casual sex has also been strongly
predicted by having sexual intercourse with a partner
only once and by knowing the sexual partner for less
than 24 hours (Eshbaugh & Gute, 2008). Thus, it is
the hook-up characteristics that carry the heaviest
emotional consequences for women compared to
those of FWBRs.
The dynamics of FWBRs
Does what happens during the course of FWBRs,
explain why some participants are satis ed with the
outcome and others are not? Common sense suggests
that one factor is the extent to which participants’
expectations are ful lled. By FWBR de nition,
these expectations include a straightforward
and simple relationship without commitment or
complications. Therefore, it is not surprising that
developing unreciprocated feelings and getting hurt,
or hurting another as a result, is the most frequently
reported drawback of FWBRs. This is followed by
the loss of friendship, and other negative feelings
re ecting emotional complications: awkwardness,
jealousy, and hurt (Bisson & Levine, 2009; Weaver
et al., 2011).
Gender may explain different levels of satisfaction
with FWBRs. Women are more likely to hope
for and discuss a change in a FWBR, while men
are more likely to wish for it to remain the same
(Grello et al., 2006; Lehmiller et al., 2011; Owen
& Fincham, 2011a). Since unreciprocated feelings
have been named as the highest source of risk and
unhappiness in FWBRs, this could contribute to the
difference in relationship satisfaction rates. If women
are more likely to enter the relationship hoping
for commitment or to develop a desire for it in the
process, they could be more likely to be unhappy
with the FWBR arrangement.
Bisson and Levine (2009) found that despite the
propensity of FWBRs to change (almost half of their
sample indicated raising questions and uncertainty
about status, future, and feelings), participants did
not engage in explicit conversations about their
relationship. A significant majority (i.e., 77%)
indicated there was no discussion of ground rules, and
conversations about relationship maintenance and
development usually weren’t initiated either. Weaver
et al. (2011) con rmed that such communication
tends to be either indirect or superficial. To the
extent that FWBRs have ground rules (Hughes et
al., 2005; Weaver et al., 2011), these rules appear
to be understood implicitly and this understanding
may therefore vary from person to person. All of
this suggests the potential for a vicious cycle for
participants in that the desire to keep things simple
prevents relationship negotiations from taking place
and, ironically, the lack of negotiation creates the very
dif culties that the participants were trying to avoid.
In such circumstances, some individuals may attempt
to avoid the topic. However, topic avoidance has
generally been found to be positively related to
relationship uncertainty and to the perception that
disclosure carries risks. That being said, “individuals
may sometimes prefer the unknown to the discovery
of undesirable information”(A & Burgoon, 1998,
p. 266). A and Burgoon found that people in
cross-sex friendships were more likely to avoid
conversations about the state of their relationship
and about relational norms than were people in
45
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
dating relationships. While topic avoidance may
be motivated by self-protection and relationship
protection, research suggests that high levels of
avoiding discussion about relationship concerns tend
to be associated with lower levels of relationship
satisfaction across both romantic and non-romantic
relationships (Dailey & Palomares, 2004).
Among the two other core components of FWBRs, sex
and friendship, the evidence is mixed on which one
plays the leading role in FWBRs. Most authors have
viewed FWBRs as primarily a sexual relationship
with a focus on bene ts (Hughes et al., 2005; Richey
et al., 2009). McGinty et al. (2007) suggest that
women’s socialization to assess relationships in terms
of emotional value re ects a gender difference in the
importance attached to the friendship component
of FWBRs. With respect to women’s experience,
McGinty et al. note that “women regard a [FWBR]
as emotional with the emphasis on friends while men
tend to view the relationship as more casual with an
emphasis on bene ts (sexual)” (p. 1130). Despite
the apparent emphasis on non-commitment and sex
in FWBRs, Lehmiller et al. (2011) unexpectedly
found that both men and women in their sample of
over 400 people displayed signi cantly stronger
commitment to friendship than to the sexual part
of the relationship. They suggested that in FWBRs,
“regardless of partner’s sex, friendship comes before
bene ts” (Lehmiller et al., p. 281).
This observation suggests that interdependence,
bonding, and avenues of social and emotional support
could be crucial to all interpersonal relationships, for
both men and women (Sprecher & Regan, 2002).
Perhaps, existing literature has focused too much on the
sexual aspects of FWBRs and somewhat overlooked the
potential support structures within these relationships.
At the same time, communication dif culties and lack
of relationship discussions, as underlined by Bisson
and Levine (2009), could compromise the effectiveness
of these support structures.
Research objectives and hypotheses
The present study examined the experiences of
Canadian female and male undergraduate university
students who had either been in or were currently
involved in a FWBR. The data originally collected by
the second author were drawn from a qualitatively-
coded questionnaire completed by 155 men and 150
women. The current quantitative study explored
whether any of the reasons for or expectations of
FWBRs reported by participants in the original
qualitative study predicted relationship outcomes.
Three primary hypotheses that emerged from the
literature review were probed in order to understand
the dynamics of FWBRs. Secondary hypotheses
related to the effects of traditional gender roles on
FWBRs were similarly tested.
Hypothesis 1: Wishfulness
We hypothesized that participants who entered a
FWBR explicitly wanting it to progress into dating
would be less likely to report a positive experience
and less likely to engage in a FWBR again compared
to participants who entered a FWBR for any other
primary reason (such as sex, fun, or avoiding
commitment). This hypothesis checks the possible
mismatch between the relationship hopes that
participants sometimes harbour and the “no-strings”
nature of FWBRs.
Hypothesis 2: Developing unreciprocated
feelings
We hypothesized that participants who indicated
that they expected no emotional commitment, but
who found that unreciprocated feelings developed
during the course of the relationship, on either side,
would be more likely to report a negative experience
and avoid engaging in a FWBR again, compared to
the participants whose “no-strings” expectations
were met. This hypothesis tests the most commonly
mentioned disadvantage of FWBRs and seeks
to determine whether this disadvantage alone is
suf cient to predict FWBR outcomes.
Hypothesis 3: Comfort
We hypothesized that those participants who listed
comfort as a salient reason for being in a FWBR would
be no more likely to report negative outcomes than
the average participant. This hypothesis considers
how well FWBRs address the need for comfort, and
predicts that the friendship, companionship, and
intimacy components should enable an adequate level
of support for such relationships.
Secondary Hypotheses
Since gender norms were only partially challenged
in this study, we expected that the results would
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 201246
be mixed but that traditional norms would still
hold for the majority of participants. We therefore
hypothesized that: (a) women would be more likely
than men to want a relationship change or would
indicate a desire for commitment as a reason to enter
a FWBR; and (b) women would be more likely than
men to view their experience as negative and avoid
FWBRs in the future.
Methods
This study took the extensive data on FWBRs collected
by the second author in a qualitative study (Fraser,
2010) and translated the ndings into a quantitative
research study. The design of the present investigation
thus has some unique aspects related to having to work
with an existing data set and its limitations.
Sampling procedure and participant
recruitment
A detailed description of sampling procedure and
questionnaire development can be found in Fraser
(2010). A briefer, but necessarily substantive,
summary is presented here to illustrate the validity
of the sampling methods involved.
Participants were recruited using the university
research participation system and compensated with a
bonus course credit. Participants were presented with
an informed consent form and an online anonymous
questionnaire. The consent form described the study
as an investigation of men and women’s perceptions,
expectations, and experiences with FWBRs, with
the intent of better understanding this type of
relationship. Anonymity was seen as a way to obtain
more honest answers on sensitive topics. To protect
anonymity, after reading the informed consent form,
the participants had to press “I agree” to participate
instead of signing. The participants were explicitly
told they could decline to answer any question or exit
the study at any time without consequence.
In an attempt to reduce the likelihood that individuals
who did not qualify for the study would proceed, those
with no experience of FWBRs were asked to refrain
from continuing. However, they were assured that they
would still receive the bonus credit. The participants
who chose to stay completed the questionnaire and
were electronically debriefed upon completion.
Participants
The data set received from Fraser (2010) included
305 participants, all of whom were undergraduate
psychology students at the University of Calgary. All
the participants reportedly had direct experience of at
least one FWBR. Seven entries were eliminated due
to duplication, two for being underage (i.e., 17-years-
old), and 15 more were dropped due to failing to
provide meaningful answers about both reasons
and expectations for engagement in the FWBR. The
remaining sample used in the analysis included 281
participants (135 women, 146 men), ranging in age
from 18 to 40 (M = 20.9, SD = 3.4).
Preliminary review of qualitative data
For the purposes of the present quantitative study,
the previously collected qualitative data (Fraser,
2010) had to be coded and translated into categories
suitable for quantitative analysis. Participants in the
qualitative study answered a total of 12 questions
but not all of the response sets could be used, e.g.,
some were dropped because no feasible research
questions emerged from either the literature review
or the review of the data itself. Given the nominal
nature of most of the data, chi-square contingency
tables test of independence was chosen as the main
method of analysis.
In order to determine how to best use secondary data
analysis to establish suitable post-hoc hypotheses, the
rst author read through the entire data set and then
consulted with the third author to create a preliminary
list of potential research questions. These questions
informed our decisions about which response sets
needed to be coded and which should be excluded.
A literature review was used to con rm and re ne
the nal hypotheses.
Retention and exclusion decisions
In order to assess responses to the question of
whether participants would ever engage in another
FWBR, the FWBR-related response information
based on gender, reasons, expectations, pros and
cons, and relationship outcomes was retained for the
analysis. We excluded response information on age,
ancestry, sexual orientation, whether the person was
in a FWBR at the time of the survey or in the past,
how long the current or past relationship lasted, and
whether the FWBR turned into a dating relationship.
47
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
Two exclusions, age and sexual orientation, re ect
the
process. With respect to age, the sample age range was
18-40 years but 83.6% of the participants (235 people)
were between 18- and 22-years-old, and only 10% of
the sample (28 people) were 25 or older. Consequently,
there was not enough statistical power to obtain valid
answers about age differences in FWBRs. Given
that 94.7% of participants (266 people) identi ed
as heterosexual, the same problem of insuf cient
statistical power applied to sexual orientation.
Coding procedure
Categorization of FWBR questions based on gender,
relationship outcomes, and the likelihood that a
participant would ever again engage in another
FWBR was relatively straightforward. In relation
to gender, all participants identi ed as either male
or female. The relationship outcomes question was
partially closed-ended (“Would you consider your
participation in a friends with bene ts relationship
overall a positive, negative, or neutral experience and
why?”) but the three response options left virtually
no ambiguity. Categorizing whether participants
would enter a FWBR again was also simple with
yes”, “no” and “uncertain/depends” for everything
in between. It was far more complex to develop
categories for reasons for and expectations of FWBRs
because the information was solicited through
open-ended
questions. The expectations question
was effectively a two-part question and was broken
down as such. i.e., expectations “What were your
expectations going into a FWBR?” and expectation
ful llment (“Was it what you expected?”). Pros and
cons were not treated as independent predictors but
were used to better understand expectation ful llment.
Hence, no categories had been developed for them.
Preliminary categories for reasons, expectations,
and expectation fulfillment were developed and
illustrative examples were found and discussed. The
resulting lists of operationalized categories informed
the actual coding procedure.
The nal categories for reasons to engage in FWBRs
were attraction, avoiding emotional commitment,
seeking comfort, convenience, fun and experience,
closeness with a friend, wishfulness (hoping that the
FWBR develops into a more committed relationship),
spontaneity (“just happened,” as several participants
described it), wanting sexual release, seeking
uncommitted sex, and other. The categories for
expectations from FWBRs were similar and involved
avoiding emotional commitment, fun and experience,
friendship, wishfulness, no speci ed expectations,
sexual release, uncommitted sex, and other. Response
categories for whether expectations were met
included (1) yes, (2) no, due to developing emotional
complications, (3) no, for any other reasons, (4)
mixed results, and (5) more work than expected.
Coding for reasons, expectations and expectation
ful llment
The first author and a second rater used the
foregoing categories to independently code reasons,
expectations, and expectation ful llment. While
participants sometimes listed several reasons or
expectations, a decision was made to identify the
most salient reason or expectation so that there was
only one reason and one expectation category per
participant for the analysis. Intercoder reliability
was determined by both the raw percentage of
agreement and Cohen’s kappa, with 86% and .84 for
reasons, 82% and .80 for expectations, and 65% and
.56 for expectation ful llment, respectively. Lower
inter-coder agreement on expectation ful llment
was attributed to scarcity of information (explained
fully below) in the discussion of missing values. All
disagreements were reconciled through discussion,
and only post-reconciliation categories were used in
the analysis.
Resolving categorizations
To illustrate the process of resolution, here is an
example of an expectation for a FWBR that caused
discussion: “She understood me better than anyone
else at the time and we were both comfortable around
each other.” One of the raters interpreted this as
close friendship, while the other focused on comfort
as the key word. It was decided that the participant
did not appear to be actively seeking comfort, and
was instead mostly describing a companionate
relationship with a friend. This reason was coded as
“closeness with a friend”.
Another example addresses the issue of salience: “No
expectations really, I hoped we could stay friends. It
was a spontaneous thing.” One rater paid attention to
the lack of expectations, while the other focused on
the stated desire to stay friends. It was decided that
the hope to remain friends represented an expectation,
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 201248
indicating that “no expectations really” was not fully
accurate. As a result, “I hoped we could stay friends”
was seen as more salient than “no expectations,”
because the participant was effectively saying, no
expectations aside from hoping to stay friends. The
nal code for this expectation was “friendship”. An
assumption was made that avoiding commitment is
a basic characteristic of FWBRs, so if a participant
decided to mention something else, it was treated as
salient. For example, “Simply attracted to the person,
wasn’t looking for a serious relationship” was coded
as “attraction” rather than “avoiding commitment”.
A decision was made to keep the two sex-related
categories for both reasons and expectations separate,
as sexual release emphasized sexual needs and desires
per se, while uncommitted sex focused on achieving
them non-exclusively or without commitment. There
were enough people in each category to enable
valid follow-up analysis to establish whether this
distinction matters.
Responses coded as “other” were treated as missing
values in the analysis. There were four of these in the
outcomes (people failed to provide an answer), three
in the expectations, and 41 in expectation ful llment.
For the latter two, “other” referred to situations where
expectations did not t any remaining category and
also to cases where participant expectations could
not be identi ed from the information provided.
Participants frequently failed to answer the “was it
what you expected?” part of the question. For 118
people (42%), answers to pros and cons in conjunction
with reasons and expectations had to be reviewed to
estimate whether their expectations were met, and in
the end, 41 (14.6%) were still left unknown.
Here is an example of using additional information
to establish expectation ful llment. The reason for
entering a FWBR was, “I was afraid of committing
to a relationship.” The expectation response stated
only, “I was not thinking in the long term. Just have
fun.” The pros and cons were listed as, “It is a nice
experience in the short term. Jealousy ultimately
came into play, as it eventually developed into
feelings. One-sided feelings.” This was coded as
the “developing emotional complications category.
Multinomial logistic regression procedure
A second analysis of relational outcome predictors
was performed using the multinomial logistic
regression (MLR). MLR is a regression model used
when both dependent and independent variables
are categorical, and the dependent variable has
three or more levels (Orme & Buehler, 2001). The
rationale behind adding the MLR analysis was its
greater statistical power compared to the chi-square
contingency tables, and as such, lower risk of Type
I error and improved ability to detect effects on the
same sample size. MLR analysis also includes odds
ratios, providing a convenient measure of effect size
and a practical interpretation for the effects. Odds
ratios are not available in SPSS chi-square crosstabs
that are greater than 2 x 2.
Several MLR analyses were run with relationship
experience assessment and willingness to enter a
FWBR again as dependent variables, with reasons,
expectations, expectation ful llment, and gender as
independent variables. SPSS output of the results of
each analysis included the overall model signi cance,
and signi cance of each parameter’s effect on the
change in dependent variable relative to a referent
category. Additionally, a post hoc analysis was
performed (willingness to enter a FWBR again by
relationship experience assessment) to help interpret
the main results.
Results
Reasons for and expectations and
outcomes of FWBRs
The most common reason to enter a FWBR was
avoiding emotional attachment; however, as an
expectation, it was only the second most common.
The most common expectation from a FWBR was the
ability to maintain a friendship. Valid percentages in
descending order for speci c categories of the most
salient reasons and expectations are summarized in
Table 1.
In cases where expectation ful llment was identi ed,
meeting expectations was the most common
result (45%), followed by developing emotional
complications (21.7%), mixed results (13.8%), no
for any other reason (12.5%), and running into more
work than expected (7.1%). Relationship experience
49
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
assessments were mostly positive and neutral (37.9%
and 36.8% respectively), with only a quarter of
participants reporting a negative experience (25.4%).
Nonetheless, 40.1% of participants indicated that
they would not enter a FWBR again, 35.4% said they
would, and 24.5% were uncertain or felt it depended
on circumstances.
To test the three primary hypotheses and examine
which factors in uenced relationship outcomes, several
chi-square contingency tables were analyzed using
IBM SPSS. The tables included: experience assessment
by reasons, expectations, and expectation ful llment;
willingness to enter a FWBR again by reasons,
expectations, and expectations ful llment; and all the
above-mentioned categories by gender. Examination
of the effect of reasons on experience assessments or
willingness to enter a FWBR again failed to produce
statistically signi cant results. It appears that the
reasons to enter a FWBR and relationship outcomes
were independent in this sample.
Expectations analysis fared better and showed that
both experience assessments and willingness to
enter a FWBR again were linked with relational
expectations,2(12) = 24.12, p = 0.020, and 2(12) =
26.13, p = 0.010, respectively. Expectation ful llment
demonstrated even greater signi cance with 2(8)
= 66.78, p < 0.001, and 2(8) = 36.78, p < 0.001,
respectively. This indicates that what the expectations
were and how they were met had a strong effect on
how the participants saw their FWBR experience and
whether they would be willing to try it again.
The MLR results (Table 2) largely confirmed
and expanded on the chi-square results, although
they threw doubt onto the role of expectations in
predicting relationship experience assessments.
While the expectations model overall showed
statistical significance with 2(12) = 23.8, p =
0.022, none of the speci c expectations parameters
turned out to have a statistically signi cant effect
on relationship experience assessments. At the
same time, both the overall model (2(12) = 26.5, p
= 0.009) and several of the expectations parameters
were statistically signi cant when evaluating whether
participants would choose to enter a FWBR again.
All the statistically signi cant results of the MLR
analyses are presented in Table 2.
Evidence for the primary hypotheses
To understand the speci c effect of expectations and
expectation ful llment on relationship outcomes, the
SPSS output crosstabs were examined in detail using
residual analysis, as suggested by Haberman (1973),
and the MLR odds ratios. Adjusted residuals (AR)
translate the difference between the expected count
and a particular observed count into z-distribution,
while taking into account the overall sample size
(Bearden, 2011). Haberman concluded that AR
indicated the importance of each cell to the nal chi-
square value in large tables better than standardized
residuals. Therefore, this method allows for direct
comparisons between cells in the crosstabs output
in tables larger than 2 x 2. The value of AR more
extreme than +/-1.96 indicates that the cell in question
has signi cant contribution to the obtained chi-square
value, with the equivalence of two-tailed p < 0.05. It
is important to remember that AR magnitude re ects
Table 1 Descriptive results for reasons for entering and expectations of FWBRs
Reasons Expectations
Avoiding emotional commitment 17.8% Friendship 19.1%
Sexual release 12.8% Avoiding emotional commitment 16.9%
Fun and experience 12.5% Fun and experience 15.5%
Uncommitted sex 11.4% No speci ed expectations 15.5%
Seeking comfort 10.3% Uncommitted sex 12.2%
Spontaneity 8.9% Sexual release 11.5%
Attraction 7.8% Wishfulness 9.4%
Wishfulness 6.4%
Closeness with a friend 6%
Convenience 5%
Other 1.1%
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 201250
only the strength of the association, while MLR odds
ratios provide a measure of the effect size.
Hypothesis 1: Wishfulness leads to
negative experiences
The results demonstrate support for this hypothesis.
Wishfulness led to a higher chance of rating the
relationship experience as negative, with AR = 3.0.
It also made the participants more likely to say no
and less likely to say yes to whether one would
enter a FWBR again, with AR = 3.1 and AR = -2.7,
respectively. In the MLR analysis, wishfulness
increased the likelihood of both saying “no” and
being uncertain by 12 and 7.5 times respectively
as compared to saying “yes” Table 2). This means
that the participants, who entered a FWBR with
expectations of progressing into dating, were indeed
much more likely to report negative experiences than
those with any other expectation.
Hypothesis 2: Unreciprocated feelings
lead to negative experiences
This hypothesis also received strong support.
Developing emotional complications produced
extreme AR values. It was associated with both
higher chances of a negative relationship assessment
and lower chances of a positive one (AR = 4.7 and
AR = -2.4 respectively), increased the likelihood of
not wanting to enter a FWBR again (AR = 2.7), and
decreased the probability of being uncertain about this
decision (AR = -2.3). Developing feelings was very
strongly associated with viewing the FWBR experience
as negative, since AR = 4.7 means p < 0.0001.
An MLR examination of the expectation ful llment
against experience assessments and willingness to
enter a FWBR again shows statistical signi cance
of both the overall models (2(8) = 68.54, p < 0.001,
and 2(8) = 38.49, p < 0.001, respectively) and some
of the speci c parameters. Developing emotional
complications increased the chances of a negative
relationship experience as compared to neutral by
9.5 times and of saying “no” rather than “yes” to
entering a FWBR again by 2.8 times. (Table 2) We
can conclude that the most commonly mentioned
disadvantage of FWBRs is indeed capable of
predicting relational outcomes.
Hypothesis 3: Comfort
Reasons for entering an FWBR were not associated
with relational outcomes in the contingency tables.
MLR analysis of relational outcomes by reasons
Table 2 Statistically signi cant parameters for FWBR expectations and expectation ful llment models in the MLR
analysis
Expectations and the willingness to enter a FWBR again model
Parameter description and referent category Odds ratio p-value B (MLR coef cient)
Wishfulness: no compared to yes 12.0 0.001 2.485
Wishfulness: uncertain compared to yes 7.5 0.028 2.015
No expectations: no compared to yes 4.0 0.016 1.386
No expectations: uncertain compared to yes 7.0 0.005 1.946
Fun and experience: uncertain compared to yes 4.2 0.034 1.430
Expectation ful llment and experience assessment model
Parameter description and referent category Odds ratio p-value B (MLR coef cient)
Emotional complications: negative compared to neutral 9.5 < 0.001 2.252
No for any other reason: negative compared to neutral 5.8 0.002 1.760
Mixed expectations: positive compared to neutral 0.3 0.007 -1.241
More work: positive compared to neutral 0.1 0.003 -2.380
No for any other reason: positive compared to neutral 0.2 0.013 -1.464
Expectation ful llment and willingness to enter a FWBR again model
Parameter description and referent category Odds ratio p-value B (MLR coef cient)
Emotional complications: no compared to yes 2.8 0.007 1.045
No for any other reason: no compared to yes 17.0 < 0.001 2.837
More work: uncertain compared to yes 3.6 0.039 1.286
No for any other reason: uncertain compared to yes 7.9 0.014 2.069
51
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
produced an error message, indicating that validity
of model t is uncertain (possibly due to small cell
sizes). Consequently, no support for Hypothesis 3
was found nor could any other conclusions about
reasons be drawn.
Gender differences
All gender differences in the sample were also
analyzed using adjusted residuals (AR) of crosstabs
output. Gender effects turned out to be signi cant in
every category considered, and secondary hypotheses
were supported.
Willingness to enter a FWBR again
The ndings on gender in uences on relationship
experience assessment and willingness to enter a
FWBR again are presented in their entirety in Figure
1. Women were more likely than men to view their
FWBR relationship experience as negative, 2(2) =
6.12, p = 0.047, AR = 2.4. While men were more
likely to say “yes” to participating in a FWBR in
the future, women appeared more likely to avoid
them, 2(2) = 20.51, p < 0.001, AR = 4.4 and AR =
3.5, respectively.
MLR analysis of relationship experience assessment
and willingness to enter a FWBR again by gender
both produced statistically significant models
(2(2) = 6.14, p = 0.046, 2(2) = 20.92, p < 0.001,
respectively), but only the latter also demonstrated
significance of specific parameters. Overall, the
effects of gender on relational outcomes verify the
ndings obtained through the chi-square contingency
tables. Men were less likely than women to be
uncertain or not want to enter a FWBR again (B =
-0.953, p = 0.004, odds ratio = 0.4, and B = -1.285, p
< 0.001, odds ratio = 0.3, respectively). These odds
ratios are equivalent to women being 2.5 times more
likely to be uncertain or 3.3 times more likely to want
to avoid future FWBRs than men.
Reasons for entering and expectations of FWBRs
In both reasons to enter a FWBR and expectations
from it, women were more likely than men to display
wishfulness; and men were more likely than women
to seek and expect sexual satisfaction and sex without
commitment, 2(10) = 40.62, p < 0.001 (AR = 3.1,
4.7, and 2.4) for reasons and 2(6) = 28.29, p <
0.001 (AR = 3.9, 3.1, and 2.0) for expectations,
respectively. At the same time, adjusted residuals
pointed to no statistically signi cant differences in
other categories of reasons and expectations. Men
and women were very closely matched in most other
reason categories but displayed more pronounced
Figure 1 Relational outcomes of FWBRs by gender
Note: Respondents indicated whether they would engage again in a FWBR (“no”, “yes” or “neutral”) and assessed their
overall experience of participation in an FWBR (“negative”, “positive” or “neutral”).
Percentage Within Gender
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 201252
Figure 2 Expectations from FWBRs by gender.
Note: Percentages for statistically signi cant results (* p < .001) were: women 7.5%, men 15.3% for uncommitted sex; women
6%, men 18.1% for sexual release; and women 16.4%, men 2.8% for wishfulness.
differences in expectations. The complete gender
distribution of expectations of FWBRs is show in
Fig.2 and of reasons for entering a FWBR in Fig.3.
Expectations of ful llment
Looking at expectation fulfillment, the only
signi cant difference was in women being more
likely than men (20.2% versus 5.6%) (Fig.4) to
report that their expectations were not met for
reasons other than developing feelings or emotional
complications, 2(4) = 12.56, p = 0.014, AR = 3.4.
Interestingly, the difference between men and women
in reporting expectations mismatch due to emotional
complications was the smallest of all and bordering
on non-existent, with AR = +/-0.2, equivalent to p =
.492. Complete breakdown of expectation ful llment
by gender is displayed in Figure 4 below, with the
statistically signi cant difference denoted with * sign.
Other results
“No speci ed expectations” was associated with a
reduction in both positive assessments and chances
of wanting to take part in a FWBR again, with the AR
= -2.7 and AR = -2.0, respectively, but no increase in
negative assessments. MLR analysis also con rmed
that this category led to reduced willingness to enter a
FWBR again, as participants were 7 times more likely
to be uncertain and 4 times more likely to say “no.”
The participants expecting “fun and experience” were
also 4.2 times more likely to be uncertain rather than
certain about wanting to be in a FWBR again.
The expectation of “sex without commitment”
produced AR = 2.8 for the willingness to participate
in a FWBR again. Despite comparable counts
(total of 32 and 34, respectively), the expectation
of “sexual satisfaction” per se did not produce any
statistically signi cant AR, and neither expectation
was linked with relationship assessments. Hence, the
participants who placed emphasis on the expectation
of non-exclusive sex were uniquely more likely to
want to enter a FWBR again than any other group,
even though it seems to have had no impact on how
they assessed the relationship experience.
53
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
Figure 3 Reasons for entering FWBRs by gender
Note: Percentages for statistically signi cant results (* p < .001) were: women 6.7%, men 15.8% for uncommitted sex;
women 3%, men 21.9% for sexual release; and women 11.1%, men 2.1% for wishfulness.
.0%
5.0%
10.0%
15.0%
20.0%
25.0%
30.0%
35.0%
40.0%
45.0%
50.0%
Yes No - Feelings No - Other* Mixed More Work
Were Expectations Met?
Male
Female
Note: Percentages for statistically signi cant results (* p < .001) were: women 6.7%, men 15.8% for uncommitted sex;
women 3%, men 21.9% for sexual release; and women 11.1%, men 2.1% for wishfulness.
Figure 4 FWBR expectation ful llment by gender
Despite the small number of respondents, “more work”
had a signi cant effect, reducing the chances of a
positive experience compared to neutral by 10 times,
and increasing uncertainty about entering a FWBR
again by 3.4 times compared to certainty. “Mixed
expectations” had a similar effect, making the chance
of a positive experience assessment one third that of
a neutral. Not meeting expectation for reasons other
than emotional complications also had strong effects.
Compared to neutral, it increased the probability of
negative experience by 5.8 times and reduced the
probability of a positive experience by 5 times. It also
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 201254
increased the chances of both being uncertain and
saying “no” to being in a FWBR again as compared
to “yes”, by almost 8 and 17 times, respectively.
While coef cient of determination does not exist
for MLR, SPSS provides three estimates of pseudo
R-square (i.e., McFadden, Cox and Snell, and
Nagelkerke estimates; UCLA, n.d.). Comparing these
values, the model for expectation ful llment was
the strongest as it was capable of explaining 13.3%
to 28.3% of variability in experience assessment
and 7.6% to 17% of variability in the willingness to
enter a FWBR again. Pseudo R-square coef cients
for expectations were much lower, explaining only
4% to 9.4% of variability in experience assessment
and 4.5% to 10.5% of variability in the willingness to
enter a FWBR again. The experience assessment by
gender model had extremely low pseudo R-squares,
1% to 2.5%. The willingness to enter an FWBR again
fared better, with pseudo R-squares of 3.5% to 8.3%.
Interpretation of differences between relational
outcomes when writing the discussion prompted
MLR analysis of willingness to enter a FWBR
again by relationship experience, in order to obtain
pseudo R-square coef cients. The overall model was
statistically signi cant (2(4) = 61.25, p < 0.001),
yet the pseudo R-square estimates range was only
11.9% – 25.6%.
Discussion
In terms of our hypotheses, the results indicated strong
support for wishfulness (wanting the relationship to
proceed to dating) and development of emotional
complications as potential predictors of negative
relationship outcomes (Hypotheses 1 and 2). These
ndings con rmed what several other researchers
(Bisson & Levine, 2009; Weaver et al., 2011) have
reported about unreciprocated attachment being
the key disadvantage of FWBRs. Almost 22% of
the participants reported development of emotional
complications, which strongly predicted negative
experiences. These individuals were 9.5 times more
likely to report a negative experience and almost 3
times less likely to express desire to enter a FWBR
again. Wishfulness was more rare, with only 9.4%
of participants expressing this expectation, but it had
a larger effect. It increased the chances of avoiding
or being uncertain about FWBRs in the future by 12
times and 7.5 times, respectively.
Examination of reasons for engaging in a FWBR
failed to yield statistically signi cant results, so
there was no support for Hypothesis 3, concerning
the role of comfort and availability of emotional
support inside FWBRs. There could be several
explanations for this result. First, the information
about comfort or support that FWBRs can deliver to
its participants may have been spread over a number
of questions and answers, such as relationship pros,
and not really captured well by reasons. Second, the
speci c reasons for entering a FWBR may not be as
relevant to outcomes, as the relationship dynamics.
Expectations and expectation fulfillment reflect
how the relationship unfolds better than reasons,
which may explain their higher predictive ability.
If true, the inability to nd statistical signi cance
of reasons could, in itself, be a signi cant nding
as it may indicate that future research should look
elsewhere for predictors of outcomes. At the same
time, insuf cient sample size could be the main
contributing factor to the result, since reasons had
some of the smallest cell counts.
Nonetheless, descriptive results can provide some
information about the role of friendship and comfort
in FWBRs. Comfort and closeness with a friend were
listed as the most salient reasons for entering the
relationship by a minority (i.e., 10.3% and 6%) of
the participants. While maintaining friendship was
the most frequently listed expectation, in absolute
numbers it seriously mattered to only one fth of
the participants (19.1%). The categories, developed
in this study, closely parallel the advantages and
disadvantages described by Bisson and Levine (2009)
and Weaver et al. (2011). Consequently, these low
frequencies likely reflect genuine differences in
prevalence, rather than in categorization between
researchers. However, the need to pick the most salient
reason or expectation prevented the present study
from discovering the total number of participants who
may have indicated that comfort was important to
them. How well the friendship component of FWBRs
supports emotional needs of participants remains a
potential direction for future research.
55
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
Gender differences and similarities in
FWBRs
Support was found for both secondary hypotheses,
which predicted that prevalence of negative relational
outcomes and wishfulness would be higher among
women than men. The similar and suf ciently large
numbers of men (n=143) and women (n=134) in
this study suggest high reliability of gender analysis.
Women were 3.3 times more likely to want to avoid
FWBRs in the future and 2.5 times more likely to be
uncertain about them than men. Women were also
signi cantly more likely than men to have a negative
FWBR experience (32% versus 19% respectively).
As predicted, wishfulness was gendered. Consistent
with previous research (Grello et al., 2006; Lehmiller
et al., 2011; Owen & Fincham, 2011a), women were
more likely to display wishfulness than men: 11.1%
as reason, 16.4% as expectation for women; 2.1% as
reason, 2.8% as expectation for men.
The low percentage of women in our study who
identified wishfulness as a primary expectation
(16.4%) is consistent with Grello et al (2006) who
found that “fewer than one fth of the females who
had had casual sex experiences reported that they
thought a romance might be imminent” (p. 264).
These numbers suggest that while some women do
agree to casual sex hoping it develops into a romantic
relationship (Fraser, 2010; Impett & Peplau, 2003)
- and the tendency to do so may be persistent - such
wishfulness is not generalizable to most women.
It should be noted that Fraser (2010) found that
47% of women and 25% of men in the same dataset
expressed a wish for the relationship to turn into a
dating one based on their response to the question
“Did your friends with bene ts relationships turn
into a dating relationship? And did you want it
to?” (Responses to this question were not used in
the present study). Fraser’s results are consistent
with much higher desire for a romantic relationship
reported by other researchers (Lehmiller et al., 2011;
Owen & Fincham, 2011a), ranging around 24% for
men and 40% to 43% for women. These differences
from our ndings may be explained by the fact
that we attempted to isolate wishfulness as a factor
present at the beginning of the relationship in the
context of reasons for and expectations of FWBRs.
The larger percentages reported by other authors
could include both the wishfulness upon entering a
FWBR and the feelings, developed over the course
of the relationship. Differences in time speci cations
in how the questions were asked in different studies
may also be relevant here.
Wishfulness could partially explain, why more
women than men (20.2% versus 5.6%) reported
unmet expectations for reasons other than developing
attachment. With wishfulness from the very beginning
of the relationship, feelings do not need to develop,
as they already exist. Lack of reciprocity then leads
to increased failure to meet expectations and hopes.
As expected, the ndings contained much nuance
about gender roles as well. Gender difference
in experience assessments came close to being
statistically insigni cant, and MLR analysis did not
identify either gender as a statistically signi cant
predictor of the experience. While relational
outcomes overall conformed to the traditional gender
expectations, significant minorities of men and
women went against this trend. For men, 20% and
30% respectively reported negative experience and
unwillingness to enter a FWBR again. And quite a
number of women appeared happy with FWBRs,
indicating positive experience (32%) and saying yes
to the prospect of a FWBR in the future (24%).
Consistent with traditional gender roles, men were
signi cantly more likely to seek uncommitted sex
and sexual release than women. However, unexpected
emotional complications occurred for both men and
women with equal prevalence. That both men and
women develop feelings is consistent with Epstein
et al. (2009) ndings about men desiring emotional
closeness. We found no statistically significant
differences between men and women for the other
reasons and expectations, e.g. fun and experience, or
emphasis on friendship. Interestingly, more women
than men reported avoiding emotional commitment
as the main reason to enter a FWBR (20% versus
15%), but more men than women indicated this as
primary expectation (19% versus 15%).
Explanations for the within-gender variability of
reasons, expectations, and outcomes in FWBRs could
be related to changing cultural norms. Yet several
frameworks of individual differences exist that could
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 201256
provide more insight into this matter and represent
promising areas for further research. Markey and
Markey (2007) linked prevalence of sexual partners
and non-committed sexual relationships to the
interpersonal warmth dimension of Interpersonal
Circumplex. The authors found that individuals, who
display both extreme coldness and extreme warmth,
tended to have more partners and uncommitted
relationships. Markey and Markey speculated that
the former might do so out of fear of mistreatment
and rejection, or sel shness; while the latter may
be motivated by “an opportunity to exchange love,
intimacy, or friendship with as many people as
possible” (p. 1210). It is possible that warm and cold
individuals would differ greatly in their ability to
provide emotional support and closeness in FWBRs,
partially accounting for outcome discrepancies.
Another framework of individual differences is
sociosexuality: a continuum between individuals
who require commitment and closeness prior to sex
and those who are fully comfortable with casual
sex and multiple occasional partners (Simpson
and Gangestad, 1991). The extent of individual’s
sociosexuality and how well partners match each
other on this dimension could be related to how
comfortable or uncomfortable they may be with the
non-committed nature of FWBRs, and how likely
they are to maintain this original lack of commitment.
Unrestricted sociosexual orientation could also
explain why individuals of both genders, who placed
emphasis on the expectation of non-exclusive sex,
were more likely to want to enter a FWBR again
than any other group.
FWBR dynamics and other ndings
It is worth noting that while both wishfulness
and development of emotional complications had
large effects, in absolute terms they only occurred
in approximately one tenth and one fifth of the
participants. Consequently, they cannot be relied
on as a major explanation for most of relational
outcomes. This study results con rm that the extent of
meeting expectations signi cantly contributes to both
relationship experience assessments and willingness
to enter a FWBR again.
The association was significant even for mixed
expectation ful llment and running into more work
than expected, despite relatively small counts of
participants, who reported these outcomes. The
expectations-based ndings make so much intuitive
sense regarding relationships in general that they
hardly teach us anything about FWBRs speci cally.
However, since the demonstrated statistical strength
of associations is extremely logical, it could be
interpreted as validation for the method itself. And as
such, it could indirectly validate other, less obvious,
ndings in this study.
It is clear that there are other influences on the
willingness to enter a FWBR again, beyond what the
experience was like. Almost 40% of the participants
had a positive experience and only 25% reported a
negative one. However, only 35% of the participants
stated that they would enter a FWBR again, while 40%
indicated they would not. Furthermore, according to
the MLR model, variability in the experience explains
only 11.9% to 25.6% of the variability in wanting to
be in a FWBR again. This percentage is much less
than could be expected. One reason for this nding
could lie in personal change experienced during the
relationship. Fraser (2010) indicated that a number
of women mentioned that “their views had changed,
and they realized that they want a real relationship”
(p. 33), while men mostly said no to a FWBR in the
future due to presently dating someone.
Combined, these results could be taken as preliminary
support for the conception of FWBRs being
something some people can grow out of, as their
expectations and desires for relationship change
(Kalish, 2009; Weaver et al., 2011). This concept and
possible gender differences within it would need to
be further veri ed. If true, it also means that personal
characteristics and life circumstances of people, who
engage in FWBRs early on as part of sexual and
relational experimentation and those who continue to
do so later in life, may be different. This presents an
interesting suggestion for future studies, especially
since investigating FWBRs in older generations has
been neglected by research to date.
Some other ndings related to relational outcomes
were unexpected. It is puzzling that participants
who reported “no speci ed expectations” or “fun
and experience” expressed reduced willingness or
higher uncertainty about entering a FWBR again.
57
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
Unless both results represent a statistical anomaly
or mis-coding, it appears that simply going along for
the experience is associated with reduced chances of
a positive outcome. Potential explanations could be
related to the lack of purpose and self-awareness, as
genuinely having no expectations is unlikely. And
such lack of awareness could contribute to the failure
to negotiate an evolving relationship, as highlighted
by Bisson and Levine (2009). These results could
also be associated with overly positive expectations,
increasing chances of disappointment. But ultimately,
all that is available at this point are speculations,
which might be re ned by future research.
While the study formulated no hypotheses about
conscientiousness of the decision to enter a FWBR,
it has something to contribute to this question. Only
8.9% of the participants listed spontaneity as a salient
reason for entering a FWBR. In the raw data, only
4 of the participants mentioned any in uence of
alcohol at all. This nding is at odds with Owen and
Fincham (2011a), but there are other studies on the
role of alcohol that it is consistent with.
Wentland and Reissing (2011) used focus groups
to explore perceptions of different types of casual
sex relationships among Canadian undergraduates.
They found that drinking could be used in FWBRs
to overcome the awkwardness of initiation, but was
not perceived as part of continued relationship.
Vélez-Blasini (2008) examined casual sex behaviours
in relation to perceived costs and internal con icts
and suggested that rational decision-making was
obvious, even when alcohol was present. Vélez-
Blasini pointed out that any behavioural research on
undergraduate populations that looks into the role
of alcohol would likely nd signi cant correlations
simply because it is such a widespread part of student
experience, while the causal link may be missing.
Study limitations
The core limitation of this study arose from the
fact that a very large qualitative dataset was used to
perform quantitative research. Consequently, some
of the research direction and methods were identi ed
working backwards from the available data, rather
than from the questions posed by literature review.
As well, some gaps identified in the literature
could not be addressed as effectively, since data
collection was not speci cally designed to do so.
The methods section provides considerable detail
on these and other challenges and the implicit and
explicit limitations they represent so we will restrict
our comments here to a few such examples. The
quantitative analysis overall relied on taking answers
to different questions and treating them as separate
predictors which lost some of the narrative aspect that
re ected FWBR dynamics. Categorical quantitative
analysis also required establishing one category per
question per participant and having to choose only
one main reason or expectation for the analysis.
Good inter-rater reliability could not eliminate the
limitations of this process. The age variability in the
sample combined with differences in the duration and
currency of FWBRs were also confounding factors.
Conclusion
This study has several important strengths. The
categories developed were based on rich qualitative
data, presented in participants’ own words, which
improved their ability to accurately reflect the
complexity of the phenomena at hand. Independent
coding by two raters contributed to accuracy and
validity of the categories. In the analysis itself,
combined use of adjusted residuals and MLR allowed
this study to zero in on speci c effects and odds ratios
of different predictors, which could be challenging
when dealing with categorical variables.
To the authors’ knowledge, this is the rst Canadian
quantitative study of FWBRs, conducted on a
relatively large sample of 281 people. As such, it
presents a signi cant contribution to the understanding
of FWBRs within the Canadian cultural landscape.
It also represents the rst attempt to explicitly link
some of the previously identi ed advantages and
disadvantages of FWBRs—operationalized through
reasons and expectations—to relational outcomes.
This study helps to re ne what is becoming better
understood about FWBRs and what remains
unknown, as even though FWBR research is
relatively new, it has already covered a lot of ground.
The authors believe that future research on FWBRs
would bene t from focusing more on why, rather than
what, happens during the relationship.
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 201258
References
A , W.A., & Burgoon, J.K. (1998). “We never talk
about that”: A comparison of cross-sex friendships
and dating relationships on uncertainty and topic
avoidance. Personal Relationships, 5, 255-272.
doi:10:1111/j.1475-6811.1998.tb00171.x
Bay-Cheng, L.Y., Robinson, A.D., & Zucker, A.N. (2009).
Behavioral and relational contexts of adolescent
desire, wanting, and pleasure: undergraduate women’s
retrospective accounts. Journal of Sex Research,
46(6), 511-524. doi:10.1080/00224490902867871
Bearden, J. (2011). Chi-square test of independence.
Retrieved from Geneseo College, NY, Sociology
211 Statistics for Social Research website on March
2, 2012: http://www.geneseo.edu/~bearden/socl211/
chisquareweb/chisquare.html
Bisson, M., & Levine, T. (2009). Negotiating a friends with
bene ts relationship. Archives of Sexual Behaviour,
38, 66-73. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9211-2
Chernin, A., Rich, M., & Shing, R. (2010). Prevalence of
adolescent friends with bene ts relationships varies by
attitude, not by age. Abstracts, 46, 35. doi:10.1016/j.
jadohealth.2009.11.084
Dailey, R.M., & Palomares, N.A. (2004). Strategic topic
avoidance: An investigation of topic avoidance
frequency, strategies used, and relational correlates.
Communication Monographs, 71, 471-496.
doi.10.1080/0363452042000307443
Dworkin, S.L., & O’Sullivan, L. (2005). Actual versus
desired initiation patterns among a sample of college
men: Tapping disjunctures within traditional male
sexual scripts. The Journal of Sex Research, 42, 150-
158. doi:10.1080/00224490509552268
Eisenberg, M.E., Ackard, D.M., Resnick, M.D., &
Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2009). Casual sex and
psychological health among young adults: Is having
“friends with benefits” emotionally damaging?
Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 41,
231-237. doi:10.1363/4123109
Epstein, M., Calzo, J., Smiler, A., & Ward, L. (2009).
“Anything for making out to having sex”: Men’s
negotiations of hooking up and friends with
benefits. Journal of Sex Research, 46, 414-424.
doi:10.1080/00224490902775801
Fischtein. D.S., Herold, E.S., & Desmarais S. (2007). How
much does gender explain in sexual attitudes and
behaviors? A survey of Canadian adults. Archives of
Sexual Behaviour, 36, 451-461. doi:10.1007/s10508-
006-9157-9
Fraser, V. (2010). A qualitative study of “friends with
benefits” relationships. (Unpublished Honours
thesis.) University of Calgary, Calgary, AB.
Available via email from alderson@ucalgary.ca or
vanessafraser33@hotmail.com
Eshbaugh, E.M., & Gute, G. (2008). Hookups and sexual
regret among college women. The Journal of Social
Psychology, 148, 77-89. doi:10.3200/SOCP.148.1.77-90
Grello, C., Welsh, D., & Harper, M. (2006). No strings
attached: The nature of casual sex in college
students. Journal of Sex Research, 43, 255-267.
doi:10.1080/00224490609552324
Haberman, S.J. (1973). The analysis of residuals in
cross-classified tables. Biometrics, 29, 205-22.
doi:10.2307/2529686
Hughes, M., Morrison, K., & Asada, K. (2005). What’s
love got to do with it? Exploring the impact of
maintenance rules, love attitudes, and network
support on friends with benefits relationships.
Western Journal of Communication, 69, 49-66.
doi:10.1080/10570310500034154
Impett, E., & Peplau, L. (2003). Sexual compliance:
Gender, motivational, and relationship perspectives.
The Journal of Sex Research, 40, 87-100.
doi:10.1080/00224490309552169
Kalish, R. (2009). Review of the book, Hooking up:
Sex, dating, and relationships on campus, by
K.A. Bogle. Gender & Society, 23, 129-131.
doi:10.1177/0891243208320408
Lehmiller, J.L., VanderDrift, L.E., & Kelly, J.R. (2011).
Sex differences in approaching friends with bene ts
relationships. Journal of Sex Research, 48, 275-284.
doi:10.1080/00224491003721694
Manning, W.D., Giordano, P.C., & Longmore, A.M.
(2006). Hooking up: The relationship contexts of
“nonrelationship’’ sex. Journal of Adolescent Research,
21, 459-483. doi:10.1177/0743558406291692
59
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 21 (1) 2012
Markey, P.M., & Markey, C.N. (2007). The interpersonal
meaning of sexual promiscuity. Journal of Research
in Personality 41, 1199-1212. doi:10.1016/j.
jrp.2007.02.004
McGinty, K., Knox, D., & Zusman, M. (2007). Friends
with benefits: Women want “friends,” men want
“bene ts.” College Student Journal, 41, 1128-1131.
Milhausen, R.R., & Herold, E.S. (1999). Does the sexual
double standard still exist? Perceptions of university
women. The Journal of Sex Research, 36, 361-368.
doi:10.1080/00224499909552008
Orme, J.G., & Buehler, C. (2001). Introduction to multiple
regression for categorical and limited dependent
variables. Social Work Research, 25, 49-61. Retrieved
from http://swr.oxfordjournals.org/
Owen, J., & Fincham, F.D. (2011a). Effects of gender
and psychological factors on “friends with bene ts”
relationships among young adults. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 40, 311-320. doi:10.1007/s10508-010-
9611-6
Owen, J., & Fincham, F.D. (2011b). Young adults’
emotional reactions after hooking up encounters.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 321-330. doi:10.1007/
s10508-010-9652-x
Patterson, H.M., & Price, E.L. (2009). More than “just”
friends? Perceived trust and sexual health behaviours
within the “friends with benefits” relationships.
Manuscript in preparation.
Paul, E., & Hayes, K. (2002). The casualties of
“casual” sex: A qualitative exploration of the
phenomenology of college students’ hookups. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 19, 639-661.
doi:10.1177/0265407502195006
Puentes, J., Knox, D., & Zusman, M. (2008). Participants
in “friends with benefits” relationships. College
Student Journal, 42, 176-180.
Richey, E., Knox, D., & Zusman, M. (2009). Sexual values
of 783 undergraduates. College Student Journal, 43,
175-180.
Simpson, J.A., & Gangestad, S.W. (1991). Individual
differences in sociosexuality: Evidence for convergent
and discriminant validity. Journal Of Personality and
Social Psychology, 60, 870-883. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.60.6.870
Sprecher, S., & Regan, P. (2002). Liking some things (in
some people) more than others: Partner preferences
in romantic relationships and friendships. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 19, 463-481.
doi:10.1177/0265407502019004048
UCLA: Academic Technology Services, Statistical
Consulting Group. (n.d.). FAQ: What are pseudo
R-squareds? Retrieved from the UCLA Academic
Technology Services website on April 16, 2012:
http://www.ats.ucla.edu/stat/mult_pkg/faq/general/
Psuedo_RSquareds.htm
Vélez-Blasini, C.J. (2008). Evidence against alcohol as a
proximal cause of sexual risk taking among college
students. Journal of Sex Research, 45, 118-128.
doi:10.1080/00224490801987408
Weaver, A.D., MacKeigan, K.L., & MacDonald, H.A.
(2011). Experiences and perceptions of young adults
in friends with bene ts relationships: A qualitative
study. The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality,
20, 41-53.
Weaver, S.J., & Herold, E.S. (2000). Casual sex and
women: Measurement and motivational issues.
Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 12, 23-41.
doi:10.1300/J056v12n03_02
Wentland, J.J., & Reissing, E.D. (2011). Taking casual
sex not too casually: Exploring definitions of
casual sexual relationships. The Canadian Journal
of Human Sexuality, 20, 75-91. Retrieved from
http://sexresearchandthecity.com/wp-content/
uploads/2010/06/Taking-casual-sex-not-too-casually
pdf on April 16, 2012.
... Researchers have frequently tried to characterize individuals who enter FWBRs, though most studies focus on U.S. college students, which limits the generalizability of findings [7,9,21,22,23,28]. These studies suggest that men, urban residents, and less religious individuals-those typically holding more liberal views on sexuality-are more likely to engage in FWBRs [7,9,21,22,23,28]. ...
... Researchers have frequently tried to characterize individuals who enter FWBRs, though most studies focus on U.S. college students, which limits the generalizability of findings [7,9,21,22,23,28]. These studies suggest that men, urban residents, and less religious individuals-those typically holding more liberal views on sexuality-are more likely to engage in FWBRs [7,9,21,22,23,28]. One study [22] found that people who prefer casual social encounters and focus on sexual pleasure are more likely to engage in FWBRs. ...
... Some studies have also found that FWBRs occur among teenagers [7,28], single professionals in their 30s focused on their careers [7,22,27], and even seniors [29]. Each of these age groups likely has distinct motivations for entering FWBRs: ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction and Objective :"Friends with benefits" relationships (FWBR) are becoming increasingly common. Defined as friendships involving sexual activity without romantic commitment, FWBRs blur the boundaries between friendship and romance. The aim of this article is to gather the most up-to-date information on "friends with benefits" relationships.Review Methods: A review of studies available on the PubMed platform (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/) was conducted, including articles with free full-text access that used the keywords: “friends with benefits relationship”, “friends with benefits,” "FWB," and "FWBR."Brief Description of the State of Knowledge: Many individuals involved in friends with benefits relationships establish their own rules, and one surprising trend is the frequent observance of sexual exclusivity. Research on the prevalence of FWBRs has focused largely on students in the USA, but there is limited data on FWBRs in Poland, where they appear to be rising in popularity, influenced by dating sites and population migrations. Studies indicate that men, residents of larger cities, and less religious individuals are more likely to participate in FWBRs.Summary: FWBRs offer a unique dynamic where individuals can avoid the emotional commitments and negative traits of traditional romantic relationships. However, these relationships also present challenges, such as differing expectations between the partners and the societal stigma that may be associated with them.
... Some possible reasons are due to its accessibility, safety, being easier to understand their partner's feelings, trust, being able to still engage in friendly activities with their FWB partner, and not having to be exclusive (Bisson & Levine, 2009;Erlandsson et al., 2013;Letcher et al., 2022). Additionally, studies conducted in Canada and the USA appeared to not show any significant On the one hand, studies conducted in the USA and Canada reported that women are more likely than men to have negative experiences, to feel uncertain, and to avoid FWBR, while others report that both men and women have mostly positive experiences in this type of relationship (Gusarova et al., 2012;Letcher et al., 2022;Owen & Fincham, 2011;Weaver et al., 2011;Williams & Jovanovic, 2015). In addition, a study conducted in Spain by García et al. (2014) concluded that women show more positive emotional reactions than men in these relationships, which may be a result of modern transformations in gender norms. ...
... Regarding expectations in FWBR, although some women are more likely than men to discuss the possibility of the relationship becoming serious, this desire is not generalizable to most women (Grello et al., 2006;Gusarova et al., 2012;Lehmiller et al., 2011;Owen & Fincham, 2011). Overall, emotional complications seem to occur in both men and women with equal prevalence, going against the social expectation that women are more likely than men to develop romantic feelings in a casual relationship (Amaro et al., 2022;Gusarova et al., 2012). ...
... Regarding expectations in FWBR, although some women are more likely than men to discuss the possibility of the relationship becoming serious, this desire is not generalizable to most women (Grello et al., 2006;Gusarova et al., 2012;Lehmiller et al., 2011;Owen & Fincham, 2011). Overall, emotional complications seem to occur in both men and women with equal prevalence, going against the social expectation that women are more likely than men to develop romantic feelings in a casual relationship (Amaro et al., 2022;Gusarova et al., 2012). Once again, friends with benefits seem to fight against gender stereotypes that modern feminist issues have been trying to deconstruct. ...
... Niezobowiązujący seks ze znaną sobie osobą doceniają nastolatki, dla których "przyjaźń z bonusem" bywa źródłem pierwszych doświadczeń seksualnych, skoncentrowani na karierze zawodowej single z dużych miast, którzy nie mają czasu na zabieganie o stałego partnera, a także osoby do 65. roku życia oraz starsze, które chcą uniknąć samotności, ale zarazem unikają angażowania się w oficjalny związek (Levine, Mongeau, 2010;Lehmiller, VanderDrift, Kelly, 2011;Gusarova, Fraser, Alderson, 2012;Fisher, 2017). Relacja typu friends with benefits może zaspokajać potrzeby seksualne, wypełniać czas i być okazją do testowania nowego związku (Jonason, 2013). ...
... Opórcz badań nad grupami amerykańskich i kanadyjskich studentów, niewiele jest jednoznacznych i rzetelnych ustaleń co do zasad funkcjonowania związków typu friends with benefits oraz opisu doświadczeń ich uczestników (Maticka-Tyndale, Herold, Mewhinney, 1998;Weaver, MacKeigan, MacDonald, 2011;Gusarova, Fraser, Alderson, 2012). W Polsce, poza artykułami o charakterze popularnonaukowym, wzmiankami na portalach internetowych oraz nielicznymi próbami przeglądu badań zachodnich, brakuje pogłębionych analiz naukowych na ten temat (Włodarczyk, Chanduszko-Salska, 2014). ...
... Wśród badanych przeze mnie młodych dorosłych nie istniała już tego typu dysproporcja między płciami -sześć kobiet i pięci mężczyzn praktykowało "przyjaźń z bonusem". Jednak kobiety, podobnie jak uczestniczki badań zachodnich (Owen, Fincham, 2011;Gusarova, Fraser, Alderson, 2012), oceniają ten rodzaj związku negatywnie i częściej niż mężczyźni deklarują unikanie takich relacji w przyszłości. Ta przelotna i niewymagająca emocjonalnej inwestycji strategia wydaje się idealną alternatywą dla związków romantycznych. ...
Article
Full-text available
W narracji biograficznej młodych dorosłych przedłużenie dojrzewania to świadome oddzielenie życia seksualnego od prokreacji i małżeństwa. Kultura „podrywu”, obrazująca wyraźną zmianę w kierunku akceptacji przez młodych dorosłych w zachodnich społeczeństwach przygodnego, nierelacyjnego seksu, wywołuje znaczne zainteresowanie w naukach społecznych. Prezentowany artykuł definiuje i opisuje społeczny konstrukt „przyjaźni z korzyścią/bonusem” definiowanej jako relacja, w której przyjaciele przeciwnej płci utrzymują ze sobą powtarzalne kontakty seksualne, wykluczają jednak romantyczne uczucie i zaangażowanie. Związki te postrzegane są jako krótkoterminowe strategie dobierania się w pary, niezwiązane z planem stworzenia stałego związku. Celem artykułu jest przedstawienie wyników badań jakościowych opartych na 42 na wpół ustrukturalizowanych wywiadach z kobietami i mężczyznami w wieku od 27 do 38 lat, zamieszkującymi z rodzicami w Warszawie. Analiza skupia się na zobrazowaniu stosunku młodych dorosłych do friendship with benefits jako jednej z form realizowanych przez nich samych relacji w intymnym związku, jak i oceny tego nowego wymiaru relacyjności w środowisku ich rówieśników.
... For example, men have reported sex as a stronger motive for participating in a FWBR than women while women have been more likely to report an emotional connection as a motive for entering a FWBR and were more likely than men to want the FWBR to transition to a romantic relationship (Lehmiller et al., 2011;Mongeau et al., 2013;Owen & Fincham, 2011). These differences in approaches and desired outcomes may explain why women tend to report more unmet expectations in FWBRs than men (Gusarova et al., 2012). Considering that over half of participants on multiple college campuses report experiencing FWBRs (e.g., McGinty et al., 2007;Puentes et al., 2008) and that differences between men and women in FWBRs are found inconsistently for relational, physical, and mental health outcomes (e.g., Gusarova et al., 2012;Lehmiller et al., 2014;Owen & Fincham, 2011;VanderDrift et al., 2012;Williams & Jovanovic, 2015), the current study sought to investigate the interaction between gender (men, women) and college students' commitment in FWBRs. ...
... These differences in approaches and desired outcomes may explain why women tend to report more unmet expectations in FWBRs than men (Gusarova et al., 2012). Considering that over half of participants on multiple college campuses report experiencing FWBRs (e.g., McGinty et al., 2007;Puentes et al., 2008) and that differences between men and women in FWBRs are found inconsistently for relational, physical, and mental health outcomes (e.g., Gusarova et al., 2012;Lehmiller et al., 2014;Owen & Fincham, 2011;VanderDrift et al., 2012;Williams & Jovanovic, 2015), the current study sought to investigate the interaction between gender (men, women) and college students' commitment in FWBRs. Like Machia et al. (2020) and others who have examined commitment in FWBRs, we drew from well-established and broader relationship research on commitment to determine how FWBRs are similar to and/or different from hookups and romantic relationships. ...
... Others have also found male college students are more likely than female students to participate in FWBRs (Puentes et al., 2008). Male students are also more likely than female students to say they would enter a FWBR again if they had a chance, while female students are more likely to report their expectations were not met (Gusarova et al., 2012). These gender differences in satisfaction may be explained, in part, by perceptions of partners' commitment to the FWBR. ...
Article
Friends with benefits relationships (FWBRs) are a common form of relationship for college students that combine aspects of friendship with sex, yet little is known about commitment in these relationships and whether they are more similar to casual relationships or to romantic relationships. We investigated associations between investment, alternatives, and satisfaction, with commitment (per the Investment Model) in college students' FWBRs and examined how associations between variables differ by participant and partner gender. Primary analyses were conducted with male-female FWBRs (n = 252). Male-male (n = 19) and female-female (n = 4) FWBRs are described. Investment was the strongest predictor of commitment for participants in male-female FWBRs, with commitment higher among women than men when investment was high. Additionally, participants' perceptions that their own and their partners' commitment was asymmetrical, as well as reports of extra-dyadic sexual activity (i.e., sex outside the FWBR) correlated with lower commitment to the FWBR. Overall, results suggest that FWBRs can be distinguished not only from other casual sexual relationships such as hookups but can also be differentiated from romantic relationships. Future research and intervention work should measure commitment and investment to predict how these relationships can persist, as well as transition, over time.
... In each study that measured positive and negative feelings but did not statistically test for differences, the means of positive feelings were higher than means of negative feelings (Fielder & Carey, 201a;Owen et al., 2011;Snapp et al., 2015;Wesche et al., 2018;Woerner & Abbey, 2017). Furthermore, in each study that assessed scale scores of subjective emotional reactions, the average response was higher than the scale midpoint for positive reactions and lower than the scale midpoint for negative reactions (Eshbaugh & Gute, 2008;Fielder & Carey, 2010a;Gusarova et al., 2012;Kennair et al., 2016Kennair et al., , 2018Lyons et al., 2014;Lewis et al., 2012;Owen & Fincham, 2011a, 2011bWesche et al., 2018). Although individuals reported more positive feelings than negative feelings about their most recent CSREs, many individuals reported having ever felt negatively about a CSRE. ...
... Although individuals reported more positive feelings than negative feelings about their most recent CSREs, many individuals reported having ever felt negatively about a CSRE. Between 25% and 78% of participants had ever regretted a CSRE or had a negative CSRE (Bachtel, 2013;Eshbaugh & Gute, 2008;Fisher et al., 2012;Gusarova et al., 2012), with most studies on this topic finding that over half of participants had ever regretted a CSRE (Bachtel, 2013;Eshbaugh & Gute, 2008;Fisher et al., 2012). ...
... Self-focused approach motives such as pleasure, fun, and self-affirmation were linked to more positive and less negative subjective emotional reactions (De Jong et al., 2018;Montes et al., 2017;Snapp et al., 2015). Relationship-focused motives such as entering a CSRE with the goal of forming a romantic relationship were associated with more negative emotional reactions in studies of both Canadian and American college students (Gusarova et al., 2012;Montes et al., 2016). Individuals with avoidant motives such as coping or conformity also tended to experience more negative subjective emotional reactions (Montes et al., 2017, and individuals with non-autonomous motives tended to have lower self-esteem (Vrangalova, 2015a). ...
Article
Casual sexual relationships and experiences (CSREs) are common and emotionally significant occurrences. Given the uncommitted, often emotionally complicated nature of CSREs, researchers have asked whether these experiences may have positive and/or negative emotional consequences. We reviewed 71 quantitative articles examining emotional outcomes of CSREs, including subjective emotional reactions (e.g., excitement, regret) and emotional health (e.g., depression, self-esteem). Overall, people evaluated their CSREs more positively than negatively. In contrast, CSREs were associated with short-term declines in emotional health in most studies examining changes in emotional health within a year of CSRE involvement. Emotional outcomes of CSREs differed across people and situations. Women and individuals with less permissive attitudes toward CSREs experienced worse emotional outcomes of CSREs. Alcohol use prior to CSREs, not being sexually satisfied, and not knowing a partner well were also associated with worse emotional outcomes. These findings suggest directions for prevention/intervention related to CSREs. For example, skill-building related to sexual decision-making may help individuals decide whether, and under what circumstances, CSREs are likely to result in positive or negative emotional outcomes. In addition, the limitations of extant research suggest directions for future inquiry (e.g., examining whether verbal and nonverbal consent practices predict emotional outcomes of CSREs).
... Samo zjawisko "przyjaźni z bonusem" nie jest nowe (O'Meara, 1989), ale badania na szerszą skalę dotyczące Friends with Benefits Relationships -pochodzące przede wszystkim ze Stanów Zjednoczonych -trwają od zaledwie kilkunastu lat (Gusarova i in., 2012;Knight i in., 2008;Mongeau i in., 2003;Weaver i in., 2011). Badania wskazują, że ok. ...
... W licznych badaniach podejmowana jest próba nakreślenia portretu uczestników związków Friends with Benefits (Gusarova i in., 2012;Levine, Mongeau, 2010;McGinty i in., 2007;Puentes i in., 2008). Biorąc pod uwagę dane demograficzne, wielu autorów wskazuje, że częściej angażują się w nie mężczyźni (Mongeau i in., 2003;Owen, Fincham, 2011;Puentes i in., 2008;Reis i in., 2012;Smith, Morrison, 2010). ...
... Do najczęściej deklarowanych motywów zaliczane są: unikanie zobowiązań i zaangażowania, typowych dla tradycyjnego związku romantycznego, oraz motywy seksualne (Bisson, Levine, 2009;Gusarova i in., 2012;Mongeau i in., 2013;Reis i in., 2012). Badania Karlsen i Traeen (2013) ukazują różnorodność przyczyn poszukiwania związków pozbawionych emocjonalnego zaangażowania. ...
Article
Full-text available
Związek Friends with Benefits (FWB) (w dosłownym tłumaczeniu „przyjaciele z korzyścią/bonusem”) to relacja, w której przyjaciele utrzymują ze sobą powtarzalne kontakty seksualne, wykluczająca jednak romantyczne zaangażowanie. Badania skupiły się na wybranych aspektach związków Friends with Benefits, należą do nich: rozpowszechnienie omawianych związków, motywy, oczekiwania i charakterystyka ich uczestników. Badania miały na celu określić różnice w sposobie, w jaki mężczyźni i kobiety nawiązują „przyjaźń z bonusem”. W tym celu przeprowadzono za pośrednictwem Internetu badania wśród osób zaangażowanych obecnie w relacje FWB (łącznie 134 osoby, w tym 65 mężczyzn i 65 kobiet). Wyniki wskazują na wiele podobieństw, ale także różnic, bowiem seks był częściej wymienianym motywem do rozpoczęcia takich relacji przez mężczyzn, podczas gdy motywy emocjonalne i seksualne w równym stopniu motywowały kobiety. Związki te postrzegane są jako nowość w dziedzinie relacji międzyludzkich, a w szczególności w obszarze polskich badań, i wymagają dalszej eksploracji.
... Begitu pun dalam hubungan FWB, Gusarova, Fraser, & Alderson menyatakan bahwa perempuan memiliki risiko untuk merasa tidak bahagia ketika menjalani sebuah hubungan FWB dibandingkan dengan laki-laki. Hal ini dikarenakan perempuan cenderung menginginkan sebuah komitmen dalam sebuah hubungan, sedangkan laki-laki tidak (Gusarova, Fraser, & Alderson, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Friends with Benefit (FWB) is an interpersonal relationship that only promotes mutually beneficial activities for actors. The FWB phenomenon is often carried out by students in Malang City. According to the BKKBN, 80% of people in Malang are married due to pregnancies outside of marriage (KTD). Therefore, researchers are interested in studying the phenomenon of the FWB relationship among students in Malang City. This study aims to determine the construction of meaning and the process of interpreting the FWB relationship among students in Malang City. This study uses a phenomenological approach with the theoretical framework of Peter L. Berger which focuses on the process of individual social construction of reality. There are six informants in this study. Data analysis is used through several stages, namely, data collection, data reduction, data presentation, and verification and drawing conclusions. From the results of this study, it was found that the meaning of the FWB relationship was not only limited to sexual activity, but there were other meanings. Other meanings, such as a friend to talk to entertain themselves, a friend for a walk or even just doing lecture assignments. The other findings that were found were worries and fears when undergoing FWB relationships. The concerns and fears referred to in terms of the FWB relationship being lived are known by partners and even other people and the fear of contracting HIV/AIDS.
... Although both the men and the women in their sample were equally committed to both the sexual and friendship aspects of the relationship, more women than men hoped their FWB would develop into a romantic relationship; this is a finding noted in other studies as well (Mongeau et al. 2013;Owen and Fincham 2011). Gusarova et al. (2012) found that although more women than men reported unmet expectations in their FWB, the overall percentage of women who felt this way was low and, in fact, women were equally likely as men to report having had a positive experience in their FWB relationships. Similar positive reports by women have been found in other studies (García et al. 2014;Owen et al. 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent trends suggest that friends with benefits (FWB) relationships are prevalent among emerging adults on college campuses. Notably, young women are just as likely to participate in these relationships as young men, a finding that differentiates FWBs from heterosexual hook ups, where women traditionally report less participation. As such, it has been suggested that friends with benefits relationships may provide young women an avenue to explore and achieve sexual agency. Yet, whether emerging adults actually perceive friends with benefits relationships as affording women sexual agency has not been explored explicitly. In this study, we focus on female sexual agency and examine whether college women and men perceive FWB relationships as a means of expressing women’s sexual agency. Based on focus group discussions with 71 women and 35 men at a large public university, this study explores the myriad ways that students make sense of FWB relationships. Focus group discussions focused on the themes of empowerment, control, and safety in FWB relationships; we examine these themes in order to provide a nuanced analysis of FWB relationships as an increasingly widespread sexual behavior among young people on college campuses.
Article
Full-text available
The present investigation examined the degree to which various characteristics are desired in five types of relational partners. Men and women (N = 700) indicated their prefer- ences for several attributes in either a casual sex partner, dating partner, marriage partner, same-sex friend, or opposite-sex friend (randomly assigned). Participants also indicated how important it was to obtain a partner with the desired level of each attribute. Although participants most preferred warmth and kindness, expressivity and openness, and a good sense of humor across relationship types, they clearly distinguished between romantic/sexual relationships and friendships. Specifically, participants preferred (and felt that it was more important to obtain) higher levels of many desirable characteristics - including physical attractiveness, social status attributes, and disposition or personality traits (e.g., warmth, expressiveness, humor, intelligence) - in a romantic/sexual partner than in a friend. Participants also differentiated between same-sex and opposite-sex friend- ships. In general, they preferred higher levels of and/or believed it was more important to obtain physical attractive- ness, social status, and dispositional/personality attributes from opposite-sex friends than from same-sex friends. To at least some degree, men's preferences were associated with the number of others they believed were available for a particular type of relationship; women's preferences generally were associated with perceptions of their own desirability as a partner.
Article
Friends with Benefits Relationships (FWBRs) involve ongoing sexual activity occurring between partners who do not identify the relationship as romantic. Although preliminary research suggests that FWBRs may be common, little is known about how young adults experience these relationships. In the current study, semi-structured Interviews were completed with 26 young adults with FWBR experience. Positive aspects of FWBRs included appropriateness for their life situation, safety, comfort and trust, gaining confidence and experience, closeness and companionship, freedom and having control, and easy access to sex. Negative aspects of FWBRs included getting hurt, ruining the friendship, and the relationship becoming complicated or awkward, FWBRs were characterized by limited direct communication between partners. Participants perceived little or no risk of sexually transmitted infections (STIs) within their FWBRs although 44% reported having additional sexual partners during their most recent FWBR and a third reported Inconsistent or no condom use, A majority (77%) of the participants believed that a sexual double standard exists in which women are judged more negatively than men for participating in FWBRs.
Article
Researchers are beginning to explore the variety of casual sexual relationships that individuals engage in. These relationships, and the subtle nuances that differentiate them, have not been studied collectively. The purpose of the present study was to qualitatively examine casual sexual relationships (CSRs), ranging from a single encounter to an ongoing sexual relationship with a friend. Male and female focus group participants identified a number of implicit and explicit rules that guide the initiation, maintenance, and termination of four types of casual sexual relationships: One Nights Stands, Booty Calls, Fuck Buddies, and Friends with Benefits. Participants identified these rules regardless of gender or whether they had previous personal experience with any of these CSRs. The results suggest that each of these relationship types can be placed on a continuum of casual sex according to various dimensions, including frequency of contact, type of contact (sexual and/or social), personal disclosure, discussion of the relationship, and friendship. Participants' shared understanding of CSRs suggests that young adults may have common cultural knowledge of these relationships and a fluid conceptualization of what constitutes a relationship.
Article
One of the most prominent features of the current college campus environment is the casual sex practice of the hookup. Hookups are defined as a sexual encounter between two people who are brief acquaintances or strangers, usually lasting only one night without the expectation of developing a relationship (Paul, McManus, & Hayes, 2000). Although there is a vast literature on college students' casual sexual attitudes and behavior, there is little attention to (a) subjective or experiential elements of and (b) the heterogeneity of casual sexual experiences. The goal of this study was to explore the varied phenomenology or experiential reality of college students' casual sexual hookup experiences. A structured questionnaire soliciting open responses regarding college students' views of a typical hookup and reports of their best and worst hookup experiences was administered to 187 college students. Responses were microanalytically content analyzed and globally thematically analyzed. College students' accounts of hookup experiences included behavioral, situational, cognitive, and emotional elements. As expected, although there was relative uniformity in college students' descriptions of a typical hookup, there was wide variation in college students' descriptions of their best and worst hookup experiences. Moreover, whereas there were few differences between males' and females' descriptions of what transpired, there were some sex differences in descriptions of what was felt after actual casual sexual experiences and in interpretations of why experiences were good or bad.
Article
Conflicting research findings regarding the sexual double standard have led to much debate over its existence in North American society. Using a diversity of measures, 165 women at a Canadian university were surveyed to determine their perceptions and behaviors regarding the sexual double standard. The normative belief that men are rewarded for having a high number of sexual partners, whereas women are penalized for similar behavior is one major component of the sexual double standard that was studied. The most striking finding was the discrepancy between the women's perception of the sexual double standard at the societal level and their own personal rejection of the double standard. Most respondents believed other women were more likely to enforce the double standard than were men. Women who had many previous sexual partners were more accepting of men who had many partners. The value of using a diversity of measures to study the complexities of the double standard is illustrated.
Article
Friends with benefits relationships (FWBRs) are defined as relationships between cross‐sex friends in which the friends engage in sexual activity but do not define their relationship as romantic. Relationship scholars have only recently begun to examine these relationships, despite their mention in the popular media (e.g., HBO's ‘Sex in the City,’ MTV's ‘True Life,’ ‘Seinfeld,’ and the New York Times). This study explored the relationship between FWBRs and maintenance rules, love attitudes, and network communication and support. Respondents (N = 143) completed self‐report surveys in which they described their FWBRs, their perceived rules for maintenance, perceptions of same‐sex network communication and support, and the current status of their FWBRs. They also completed the short form of the love attitude scale (Hendrick, Hendrick, & Dicke, 199815. Hendrick , C , Hendrick , SS and Dicke , A . (1998). The love attitudes scale: Short form. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15: 147–159. [CrossRef], [Web of Science ®], [CSA]View all references). The findings suggest that people tend to communicate their FWBR experiences to their same‐sex friends and, in general, receive supportive responses. Although attitudes toward love (e.g., agape) did not impact rules for maintenance of FWBRs, attitudes toward love did influence motivations for FWBRs and the outcomes of these relationships.
Article
This research stresses the need to examine the relationship between topic avoidance and relational correlates (e.g., satisfaction and emotional closeness) from a message pro-duction theoretical perspective. Our approach—strategic topic avoidance—offers addi-tional explanatory capabilities as the strategies with which interactants in close relationships avoid topics may be associated with perceptions of the relationship (after accounting for topic avoidance frequency). Moreover, relational correlates may also vary by the combination of overall topic avoidance frequency and certain topic avoidance strategies. The current research, therefore, assessed individuals' topic avoidance fre-quency levels and the frequency of using topic avoidance strategies in relation to satisfaction and closeness across three different relational types (i.e., significant others, mother–young-adult, and father–young-adult relationships). Results suggested that avoiding certain topics, such as current relational concerns, predicted levels of satisfac-tion and closeness across relationship types; however, cross-relational differences also emerged. Strategies employed to avoid topics accounted for additional variance in satisfaction and closeness for relationships with significant others and mothers but not fathers. Analyses also demonstrated that overall topic avoidance frequency interacted with topic avoidance strategy use.