Content uploaded by Tricia Yurak
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Tricia Yurak on Apr 26, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
Individual Differences Research www.idr-journal.com
2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197 ISSN: 1541-745X
© 2010 Individual Differences Association, Inc.
184
The Phenomenology of the Impostor Phenomenon
Rory O’Brien McElwee* & Tricia J. Yurak
Rowan University
*Rory O’Brien McElwee; Department of Psychology; Rowan University; Glassboro, NJ; 08028
mcelwee@rowan.edu (email).
ABSTRACT - In the Impostor Phenomenon (IP), people who are objectively competent and
successful report feeling secretly inadequate and fear detection of their incompetence. The IP has
typically been studied using personality scales, but recent research has raised concerns about
conceptualizing the IP as a personality trait. In the present work we approached the IP instead as
an affective experience that can arise in anyone in response to certain evocative situations.
Undergraduates (N = 122) described experiences in which they felt like impostors. Qualitative
and quantitative analyses showed impostor episodes to be widely experienced and related to
certain situational factors such as the relative status of the perceiver. Additionally, IP personality
scales are shown to predict some features of people’s impostor experiences. Results are discussed
in light of IP theory and research as well as self-verification theory.
Researchers who study the Impostor Phenomenon (IP) attempt to understand
individuals who are perceived as successful by others, yet fail to perceive themselves as
successful or intelligent (Clance, 1985; Clance & Imes, 1978). Objectively, these
“impostors” are successful and competent, but they have not internalized these attributes
and instead feel like a failure, about to be “found out” by others (Clance, 1985; Kolligian
& Sternberg, 1991). Research to date has addressed the IP as a stable individual
difference variable and measured it using personality scales designed for this purpose.
However, some research has documented concerns with the construct validity of scales
used to measure impostorism and even with the fundamental view of impostorism as a
personality trait (Leary, Patton, Orlando, & Funk, 2000; McElwee & Yurak, 2007). In
this paper, we argue and present evidence that although impostorism is not best
conceptualized as a stable personality trait, it is a true psychological experience that can
arise in response to situations with certain features. Many people in those situations
would experience feelings of impostorism, and thus impostor feelings can be present or
absent in a given individual at a given time as a function of the situation. Our purpose is
to examine what it feels like to be viewed more positively by others than one views
oneself.
Several personality scales measure impostorism, including those by Clance (1985),
Kolligian and Sternberg (1990), and Leary et al. (2000). All have acceptable
psychometric properties such as reliability and convergent and discriminant validity as
measured by the scales’ correlations with other measures (cf. Cozzarelli & Major, 1990;
Kolligian & Sternberg, 1991; Leary et al., 2000). However, reports from two sets of
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
185
researchers demonstrate that conceiving of impostorism as a stable personality trait is
inadequate and is unsupported in important ways.
One central tenet of Impostor Theory is that impostors believe that they have others
fooled, that others think they are more competent or intelligent than they know
themselves to be. Empirical tests of this tenet have not yielded supportive data. Leary and
his colleagues (2000) found that when people described on five dimensions their self-
appraisals (how they see themselves) and their reflected appraisals (how others see them),
there was no evidence that people with high scores on impostor scales actually thought
others viewed them more positively than they viewed themselves. In fact, impostors
reported low self-appraisals as well as low reflected appraisals, indicating an overall lack
of positive regard for the self rather than the discrepancy predicted by the theory. These
findings were affirmed and extended by McElwee and Yurak (2007) who found the same
result using three different methodologies to measure discrepancies between self- and
reflected appraisals. The lack of evidence that impostors actually think others view them
positively weakens the concept of impostorism as a personality variable.
Support for impostorism as a personality variable is further weakened by evidence
that impostors use self-deprecating statements as a self-presentation strategy. For
example, Leary et al. (2000) found that people with high scores on impostor scales
reported they would do poorly on an upcoming intelligence test, but only when they
thought their responses would be public. In a private response condition, they did not
predict failure. These findings were bolstered by McElwee and Yurak (2007) who found
that impostor scales correlated significantly with other measures of self-presentation. The
data supported the notion of impostor scales measuring a self-deprecating self-
presentation strategy akin to self-handicapping. The link to self-handicapping among
impostors is documented further by Cowman and Ferrari (2002) and Ferrari and
Thompson (2006).
In response to evidence opposing the Impostor Phenomenon as a personality trait,
McElwee and Yurak (2007) concluded that while the sum of the previous research is that
impostor scales assess a stable individual difference attribute, that attribute seems to be
more related to general negative affect and self-deprecating self-presentation than to
actual impostor feelings. Although such evidence may lead some to discount the
existence of the Impostor Phenomenon (cf. Tice & Wallace, 2003), we believe instead
that a change of approach is warranted. Instead of conceptualizing the IP as a stable
personality trait that people have to varying degrees, we argue that it is better
conceptualized as an aversive affective state that can affect many people in certain
situations (cf. Kolligian & Sternberg, 1991; Leary et al., 2000, who also suggested this
possibility). This phenomenological approach to impostor episodes has not yet been
studied empirically.
Our work documents the phenomenology of impostor episodes, in which people feel
that others perceive them as more competent or accomplished than they perceive
themselves to be. We examined individuals’ descriptions of such episodes to identify
their affective content and the situational and social antecedents and consequences. We
also explored whether features of these experiences are related to individual differences
on IP scales. That is, might people who endorse IP statements as self-descriptive report
differing experiences of impostor episodes than do people who do not do so?
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
186
Although this was an exploratory study, we offered some predictions. Certainly,
negative affect was expected to be inherent in the IP experience (McElwee & Yurak,
2007). Studies addressing the IP as a personality variable indicate that people who report
feeling inadequate and fraudulent on these scales also experience myriad negative
affective states including depressive symptoms (Kolligian & Sternberg, 1991), fear and
anxiety for evaluation (Kolligian & Sternberg, 1991; Thompson, Foreman, & Martin,
2000), and low self-esteem (Cozzarelli & Major, 1990; Kolligian & Sternberg, 1991;
McElwee & Yurak, 2007; Thompson et al., 2000).
We also expected negative affect to be central to the phenomenology of impostor
episodes because people feel uncomfortable when others do not perceive them as they
perceive themselves. Although common sense suggests people want to be viewed
favorably, self-verification research shows this to be true only for people who view
themselves favorably. People with negative self-concepts prefer interaction and
relationship partners who confirm their negative self-views, or they feel uncomfortable
and reject the positive feedback (Swann, Rentfrow, & Guinn, 2003; Swann, Stein-
Seroussi, & Giesler, 1992). This provides another rationale for expecting these episodes
to be couched in negative affect because by definition, in an impostor experience the
perceptions of others are discrepant from one’s self-view. Additionally, we expected
people with high IP scores to report being more upset during the incident, perhaps due to
the discomfort felt due to self-verification concerns (Swann et al., 2003; Swann et al.,
1992) as well as their generally higher levels of negative affect (McElwee & Yurak,
2007).
Overview of the Present Study
Participants were instructed to think of a time when they experienced impostor
feelings, that is, when someone else viewed them more positively than they viewed
themselves, and they feared being “found out.” They responded to open-ended and
response-scale questions to describe the situation, events preceding the occurrence, their
feelings at that time and in the present while recalling it, and information about the other
person involved. The methodology used to ascertain the details of the event and
participants’ affective and behavioral responses to it was modeled after that used by
Leary, Springer, Negel, Ansell, and Evans (1998) in their study of another negative
affective experience, that is, interpersonal situations involving hurt feelings.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Undergraduates (N = 122) enrolled in psychology courses at a state university
completed questionnaire booklets in the laboratory. Data from an additional 21
participants were excluded either because they did not complete the materials in a serious
manner (n = 1) or because they described experiences that were not consonant with the
Impostor Phenomenon (n = 20). These omitted experiences included some in which
participants described episodes in which others viewed them less positively than they
viewed themselves and some in which the participants intentionally deceived others
about their qualifications (e. g., lied about their SAT scores). Of the remaining 122
participants, ages ranged from 17 to 44 years, M = 19.64, SD = 3.01; 90 were female and
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
187
32 were male; 79% were European-American, 7% were African-American, 7% were
Latino/a, 4% were Asian-American and 3% reported “other” or did not respond.
Participants were informed that the study addressed interpersonal relationships and
completed informed consent forms. Following completion of the questionnaire booklets,
they were debriefed and thanked for their participation.
Materials
The first page of the questionnaire booklet began with the following instructions:
Think about a time that sticks out in your memory in which you felt that you
were less intelligent, less competent, or less capable than other people perceived
you to be. Maybe someone complimented your abilities in some particular area or
commented about your accomplishments, but instead of feeling positive about
those comments, you might have felt like a “phony.” Perhaps you felt inadequate
or had a fear that others would “find you out” and think that you aren’t as
capable as they originally thought.
Participants responded to open-ended questions in which they described the situation
and experience, including how long ago the experience occurred; the setting in which it
occurred (suggested examples were a job, academic setting, extracurricular club, etc.);
the events that led up to or preceded the event; and the participant’s view of how the
other person perceived his or her abilities, competence, or intelligence.
Participants then rated on the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS;
Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) how they felt during the event. The PANAS contains
10 positive (e.g., strong, inspired) and 10 negative (e.g., guilty, stressed) terms to
describe affective states, each rated on a 5-point scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely).
Following the PANAS was an open-ended question where participants could list
additional affective states they experienced that were not included on the rating scale.
The next questions addressed participants’ response to and perceptions of the
perceiver. Participants described any verbal responses they gave to the perceiver. They
wrote onto a blank line their relationship with the perceiver (co-worker, boss, teacher,
friend, etc.). Next, they compared the perceiver to themselves on 5 dimensions:
competent, confident, intelligent, accomplished, and in a position of authority (rated on 5-
point scales indicating the other person was much less competent to much more
competent than the participant). Participants then rated how important the perceiver’s
view was to them on a 5-point scale from very slightly or not at all to extremely. The next
question addressed whether the participant and the perceiver were alone during the event
or whether others were present and could overhear the event.
Participants then described their feelings about the perceiver’s mischaracterization.
First, participants indicated the degree to which they wanted to correct the perceiver’s
perception at the time (from very slightly or not at all to extremely on a 5-point scale) and
could check one or both of two reasons to explain their responses (“It felt good to have
other people believing good things about me” and “I did not want any negative
consequences to my job/grade/status to result from them knowing the truth.”) A blank
section was also included into which participants could write other reasons or thoughts
regarding the issue of discovery and disclosure. Next participants indicated the degree to
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
188
which, if given the opportunity now, they would choose to correct the other person (again
on the 5-point rating scale).
Participants then completed a second PANAS scale to reflect how remembering the
situation made them feel at the current moment. Participants next indicated how often
they experienced this type of situation and how often they believe the typical college
student encounters such situations (on a 5-point scale from very rarely or not at all to a
lot). The final section on this part of the questionnaire measured demographic variables
including gender, age, and year in college.
To explore the role of stable individual differences in participants’ experiences, the
questionnaire also contained two measures of impostorism. First was the 7-item Leary
Impostor Scale (LIS; Leary et al., 2000) that addresses specifically the inauthenticity
aspects of impostorism. Respondents rated items such as “I am afraid people important to
me may find out that I’m not as capable as they think I am” on 5-point scales from not at
all characteristic to extremely characteristic (in the present study, α = .91). Second was
the Clance Impostor Phenomenon Scale (CIPS; Clance, 1985). Respondents rated
agreement with 20 items such as “When people praise me for something I have
accomplished, I am afraid I won’t be able to live up to their expectations of me in the
future” on a 5-point scale, from not at all true to very true (in the present study, α = .90).
Research reported by Chrisman, Pieper, Clance, Holland, and Glickhauf-Hughes (1995)
indicated the Clance scale assesses three constructs: Fear of Failure, Attributing Success
to Luck, and Discounting Achievements. We replicated that factor structure in the present
study. A principal components analysis (PCA, using Varimax rotation) on the Clance
scale defined three sets of items representing the constructs noted above. The Fear of
Failure factor accounted for 26.78% of the variance (α = .89), the Attributing Success to
Luck factor accounted for 19.59% of the variance (α = .74), and the Discounting
Achievement factor accounted for 14.87% of the variance (α = .76). To form separate
variables for each construct, we averaged the items defining the factors. The impostor
measures were counterbalanced and always appeared after the questionnaire on which
participants described their impostor experience.
Results
Descriptions of Impostor Episodes
We begin by providing two episode descriptions to illustrate the level of detail and
affect with which some participants reported their impostor experiences.
A 19-year-old female:
“. . .my program/major is perceived to be ‘the best’ with the highest-
quality students enrolled in it. On numerous occasions, the girls in my
program have been told we’re ‘so bright and outstanding’ by professors,
advisors, etc. Many feel only the brightest students make it into the
program and by being here, it proves our intelligence and character. They
assume we’re all responsible, organized, hard-working, dedicated
students. However, that’s not the case. . .I felt ashamed. I was with 2
dozen girls who were bright and great people and I felt like I didn’t
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
189
measure up to them. Like I shouldn’t be here and I’m probably wasting
somebody’s time.”
A 19-year-old male:
“I was talking to my history teacher and he mentioned that my JROTC
instructors had said to him that I was one the best commanding officers
that they had ever had. He told me he believed it because he had seen the
others, and some of them were not so good. I asked him if what he was
saying was true, because it sounded unreal to me. I used to feel inadequate
compared to my predecessors because I felt like they did a good job, and I
felt like I had big shoes to fill. It was hard for me to judge how well I was
doing in comparison to them since my perspective changed from follower
to leader.”
Recency and Setting. The first analyses addressed the basic features of the episodes by
evaluating their recency and the setting in which they occurred. Participants recounted
episodes which occurred in a time range of “today” reaching back as long as 15 years. Of
the 117 responses classifiable for chronological distance (excluding those such as
“recently”), 71% described events within the past year; 29% of the total sample described
events within the past month. All but 4 participants (3% of the total sample) described
events within the past four years. Thus, participants described relatively recent events.
However, 29% mentioned events that occurred more than one year ago, demonstrating
that these events stand out in memory. When asked how often they experience such
events, 4.7% reported very rarely or not at all, 36.9% reported a little, 42.6% reported
sometimes, 14.8% reported quite a bit, and 1.6% reported a lot. When asked how often
they think the average college student has these experiences, the percentages were 0.8%,
13.9%, 54.9%, 25.4%, and 4.1%, respectively. Overall, these students viewed these
experiences as moderately frequent and tended to recall an episode that occurred within
the past year or so.
Participants also described the setting in which the experience occurred: 42.6%
reported being alone with the perceiver, while 56.6% reported being in a group or where
others could overhear. Academic settings or skills were featured in 52% of responses, of
which 11% mentioned writing ability; 8% mentioned artistic ability or knowledge; 10%
mentioned math or science ability; and 66% mentioned experiences in which others
viewed them as more intelligent, more accomplished, or exerting more effort in less
specific academic domains (e.g., family members being overly impressed with their
admission to college or classmates overestimating their knowledge in a given course).
Employment settings or skills were featured in 16% of responses, while 21% involved
extracurricular activities or skills (of which 56% involved sport/athletic activities and
28% involved artistic, music, or theatre activities). An additional 15% of the sample
described experiences in which the other gave them too much credit for social skills or
psychological strength. Thus, participants reported episodes in a wide variety of settings
which are common in college students’ lives.
Perceiver’s Status. The next analyses focused on the perceiver’s status relative to the
participant. The perceiver was of equal/peer status in 58% of the episodes; perceivers
were persons of authority in 52% of the epsiodes (percentages add up to more than 100%
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
190
because 7% of participants reported episodes involving both equal and higher status
perceivers). One participant described an event in which the perceiver was a subordinate.
Recall that participants completed structured-response questions regarding how the
perceiver compared to the participant on five status dimensions. For the first dimension,
39.3% of participants indicated the perceiver was equally competent, while 18.0%
reported the perceiver to be less competent and 42.6% reported him or her to be more
competent. The second dimension, confident, yielded 14.7% of participants viewing the
perceiver as less confident, 31.3% as equally confident, and 54.1% as more confident.
The third, intelligent, showed percentages of 13.9% as less intelligent, 47.5% as equally
intelligent, and 38.6% as more intelligent than the self. Next was accomplished, for
which 17.2% reported the perceiver was less accomplished, 16.4% reported the perceiver
was equally accomplished, and 66.4% reported the perceiver was more accomplished.
The last dimension, in a position of authority, yielded 9.8% reporting the perceiver of
less authority than the self, 30.3% of equal authority, and 59.8% of higher authority than
the self. Additionally, 9.0% reported the perceiver’s view of them was not at all or
slightly important, 19.7% reported it was moderately important, and 70.4% reported it
was quite a bit or extremely important. Overall, participants reported the perceiver as
being of higher status than the self on the five dimensions, though often of peer status in
terms of social role, and whose impression was of considerable importance. Table 1
displays the means and standard deviations for these and other variables.
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Between
IP Scales and Impostor Episode Experiences
Note: * p < .05 ** p < .01
Participant Responses to the Perceiver and Desire to Correct. The third set of
analyses evaluated participants’ reported actual and desired responses to the perceiver.
For the verbal response the participants gave to the perceiver, 32% were coded as “thank
you,” 24% were coded as a denial of the compliment, and 13% indicated both of those
responses. An additional 6% agreed with the other, 5% responded that their
EPISODE FEATURE
M
SD
Leary IP Scale
ClanceFear
Failure
Clance
Success /Luck
ClanceDisc.
Ach.
R
eported Frequency of Impostor Episodes for the Self 2.73 0.83 .45 ** .47 ** .30 ** .34 **
R
eported Frequency of Impostor Episodes for the Typical College Student 3.18 0.76 .13 .22 * .09 -.04
A
ffect at the Time of the Episode
Negative: Fear/Di stress 2.01 0.81 .30 ** . 26 ** .20 * .15
Negative: Guilt/S hame 1.93 0.85 .34 ** . 35 ** .24 ** .17
Negative: Hostile/Irritable 1.51 0.63 .14 .08 .09 .05
Positive: Interest 2.91 0.88 .02 -.01 -.11 -.10
Positive: Determine d 2.70 0.89 .08 .07 . 07 -.08
A
ffect at the Time of Survey Completion
Negative: Fear/Di stress 1.38 0.70 .50 ** . 40 ** .30 ** .25 **
Negative: Guilt/S hame 1.65 0.78 .48 ** . 42 ** .27 ** .33 **
Negative: Hostile/Irritable 1.26 0.67 .34 ** .27 ** .18 * .16
Positive: Interest 2.06 0.96 .21 * .21 * .22 * .04
Positive: Determined 2.23 0.99 -.05 -.08 -.15 -.24 **
N
umber of Weeks Since Episode 66.38 104.44 -.10 -.10 -.06 -.08
D
esire to Correct at the Time of the Episode 2.69 1.18 .22 * .18 * .12 .25 **
D
esire to Correc t at the Time of the Survey 2.07 1.12 .38 ** . 25 ** .27 ** .29 **
Importance of Perceiver’s Impression 3.88 0.96 .11 .05 .06 -.14
R
elative Status of Perceiver (Higher numbers indicate Perceiver is Higher than Self)
Competent 3.48 1.12 .08 .17 .11 -.07
Confident 3.60 1.10 .05 .2 1 * .08 .01
Intelligent 3.41 0.99 .14 .13 .09 -.04
Accomplished 3.79 1.11 .03 .12 .05 -.13
In a Position of Authority 3.89 1.15 -.02 .02 .02 -.12
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
191
accomplishment was some version of “no big deal,” 10% reported no verbal response.
The remainder gave miscellaneous responses such as “I hope so,” “I will try my best,” or
explained their approach to the issue at hand, etc.
In response to a structured-response question regarding how much they wanted to
correct the other’s impression at the time of the episode, 15.6% indicated very slightly or
not at all, 32.8% indicated a little, 27.9% indicated moderately, 14.8% indicated quite a
bit and 9.0% indicated extremely. Participants later indicated how much they would want
to correct the perceiver’s view at the time of survey completion; 40.5% indicated not at
all, 26.1% indicated a little, 19.8% indicated moderately, 10.1% indicated quite a bit, and
2.5% indicated extremely. Thus, between the time of the episode and the time of the
survey, participants’ tendency to want to correct the perceiver’s impression decreased.
Participants were also given the option to indicate whether they may not want to correct
the perceiver’s opinion because “It felt good to have other people believe good things
about me,” or “I did not want any negative consequences to my grade/job/status to result
from them knowing the truth.” Participants had the option of checking neither, one, or
both reasons: 38.0% checked the first one only, 19.0% checked the second one only,
38.0% checked neither, and 5.0% checked both.
Emergent Themes in the Narratives. Participants had three open-ended opportunities
to describe their thoughts about correcting the perceiver’s impression or any additional
thoughts about their experience. We describe here a summary of those explanations,
collapsing across the three questions in light of our observation that participants wrote
about similar concepts in the three places. After reading all of the descriptions, we
developed a set of four coding categories capturing the central themes of the narratives.
Each category and a few example statements are presented. Note that only explicit
mentions of the category were tallied; other participants may have also felt these ways
but did not spontaneously, explicitly mention them.
Desire for truth/self-verification: “I would rather have him know the truth”; “I did not
want people to think I thought I was something I was not”; “[I would want to tell] just so
they know how I feel about myself”; “I just didn’t want them to think I was smarter than I
actually am”; “I want people to just take what I am for face value.” Statements fitting in
this category were made by 21% of participants.
Fear of Excessive Future Expectations: “Now she has greater expectations which I
don’t feel I can meet”; “I had some self-doubt about the level I’d have to rise to”; “My
family expects me to make some massive impact upon this world and I live in constant
fear of disappointing them.” Statements fitting in this category were made by 19% of
participants.
Positive Affect as a Result of the Perceiver’s View: “It felt really good”; “I was
elated”; “I felt good and impressed with her high expectations of me”; “It felt good to
hear I did a good job and had made a ‘managerial decision’”; “I liked being respected by
others for being good at two sports instead of one. It helped build me a solid reputation. I
wasn’t about to mess that up.” Statements within this category were made by 18% of
participants.
Motivating for future improvement: “It made me want to do better so I could be proud
of myself”; “It made me want to work harder so that the next time I get a compliment, I
would know I deserved it”; “It inspired me to be a math major”; “I felt that having their
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
192
confidence in me and not wanting to let them down helped me do better.” Statements
fitting in this category were made by 7% of participants.
Affective Content of Impostor Episodes The fourth set of analyses evaluated the
affective content of the episodes. A principal components analysis on the negative
PANAS items identified three factors accounting for 68.40% of the total variance. Items
such as nervous, scared, and afraid accounted for 28.88% of the variance and were
averaged to form the Fear/Distress variable (α = .83). Items such as hostile, irritable, and
upset accounted for 21.13% of the variance and were averaged to form the
Hostile/Irritable variable (α = .70). Finally, the items guilty and ashamed were averaged
to form the Guilt/Shame variable (α = .69), accounting for 18.40% of the variance. The
analysis of the positive items identified two factors accounting for 60.03% of the total
variance. Items such as interested, excited, and enthusiastic accounted for 34.24% of the
variance and were averaged to form the Interest variable (α = .84). Items such as inspired,
determined, and attentive accounted for 25.79% of the variance and were averaged to
form the Determined variable (α = .80). See Table 1 for means and standard deviations.
A repeated-measures ANOVA on the five means for affective state at the time of the
episode revealed significant differences, F (4, 480) = 64.32, p < .0001, η2 = .35. Pairwise
comparisons revealed that all five means differed significantly from each other, ps < .01,
with the exception of the Fear/Distress and Guilt/Shame factors of negative affect. As can
be seen by examining the means in Table 1, the Interested/Excited positive affect factor
had the highest mean, followed by the positive affect factor of Inspired/Determined,
followed by the statistically equal Fear/Distress and Guilt/Shame factors of negative
affect; lowest was the Hostility/Irritability factor of negative affect. For the PANAS
describing participants’ affective state at the time of the survey, a repeated measures
ANOVA revealed significant differences, F (4, 480) = 37.80, p < .0001, η2 = .24.
Pairwise comparisons revealed that all means differed significantly with the exception of
Fear/Distress and Hostility/Irritability; these were both significantly lower than the
Guilt/Shame factor, and all three negative affect factors were significantly lower than the
two positive affect factors. Here, Inspired/Determined was higher than Interested/Excited.
These patterns make sense such that at the time of the incident, Fear/Distress and
Guilt/Shame were equal, whereas Interested/Excited was higher than Determined. Upon
later reflection, Guilt/Shame and Determined are more predominant emotions. For all 5
PANAS factors (3 negative affect and 2 positive affect), the feelings reported at the time
of the episode were more intense than the feelings reported at present while recalling the
episode, paired ts (121) > 3.40, ps ≤ .001, and each pair of measures (at the time of the
episode and at survey completion) was correlated positively, rs (120) ≥ .24, ps < .01 (see
Table 1 for descriptive statistics). Thus, participants felt the same patterns of emotion
during the episode and at survey completion, but emotions were more intense during the
episode rather than later recalling it. Note, however, that all of the affect means are below
the midpoint of the 5-point scale, indicating a relative lack of strong emotion tied to these
experiences. However, the differences in affective pattern between the time of the
episode and the time of survey completion are meaningful and illuminative.
Readers may question whether the finding that positive affect factors are rated higher
than negative affect factors indicate that impostor episodes are more fundamentally
positive experiences. Two observations fail to support this interpretation. First, in the
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
193
content of participants’ narratives and in the additional affect words listed after
completing the PANAS initially, a clear tendency toward discomfort and stress is
apparent. A total of 46 responses were listed by 36 participants. Of these 46, 5 were
positive emotions, including happy, accomplished, and confident. The remaining 41 were
negative emotions; 9 were feeling inadequate or worried about failure, 6 were
embarrassed, 4 reported feeling anxious and 4 reported feeling confused or skeptical.
Three reported feeling shocked or surprised. Other emotions included depressed,
uncomfortable, guilty, weak, deceptive, disappointed, overwhelmed, under pressure, and
jealous. Second, research studying other negative experiences has found the same
pattern: for people being actively socially excluded (Buckley, Winkle, & Leary, 2004)
and for cancer patients (Cole, Hopkins, Tisak, Steele, & Carr, 2008), positive affect is
higher than negative affect.
The Relation of IP Scales to the Phenomenology of Impostor Episodes
The fifth set of analyses evaluated the relation of stable individual differences as
measured by IP scales to participants’ impostor experiences. Correlation coefficients can
be seen in Table 1. As is apparent there, individual differences as measured by IP scales
(Leary and the three Clance factors) were predictive of the reported frequency of IP
episodes: those participants who experienced these episodes more often obtained higher
scores on the IP measures. These measures did not predict the frequency with which
participants indicated that typical college students experience such episodes with the
exception of the Clance Fear of Failure factor.
The IP scales were also associated with participants’ reported affective state at the
time of the episode. In negative affect, the Fear/Distress and Guilt/Shame factors on the
PANAS were predicted by scores on the Leary scale and the Fear of Failure and
Attributing Success to Luck factors on the Clance scale, but were not predicted by the
Discounting Achievement factor on the Clance scale. None of the IP scales predicted the
amount of Hostile/Irritable negative affect reported or either positive affect factor on the
PANAS. Thus, those scoring higher on IP scales reported more fear/distress and
guilt/shame but did not differ on positive affect nor on hostility, compared to those
scoring relatively low on the IP scales, at the time they experienced an IP episode.
Affective state at the time of survey completion was also correlated with IP measures,
although the pattern was somewhat different than it had been during the episode.
Specifically, the Leary scale and three Clance factors again predicted the Fear/Distress
and Guilt/Shame negative affect factors. However, Hostility/Irritability and the Interest
factor for positive affect were also correlated with the Leary scale and with the Fear of
Failure and Attributing Success to Luck factor of the Clance scale. Additionally, the
Clance factor of Discounting Achievement was associated with significantly lower scores
on the Determined factor for positive affect. Thus, affective experience was more intense
during the episode than when later recalling it, and individual differences in IP as
measured by the traditional personality scales were associated with affective experience
at both times, but in a more diverse pattern at the time of survey completion than during
the actual episode. At the core of these findings is that people who score high on IP scales
experience more fear/distress and guilt/shame in response to an actual IP episode than do
people who score lower.
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
194
IP scores were positively associated with the desire to correct the perceiver’s
impression, both at the time of the episode and at the time of survey completion.
Participants with higher Leary IP, Fear of Failure, and Discounting Achievement scores
were more likely to want to correct the perceiver’s impression at the time of the episode
and at the time of survey completion. Scores on the Clance Attributing Success to Luck
factor positively correlated with the desire to correct the perceiver’s impression at the
time of the survey but not at the time of the incident. Just as the affective pattern analyses
showed that people who scored high on the IP measures experienced more distress at the
time of the episode and at the time of the survey, these analyses also support that these
people experience more of a desire to correct the perceiver’s impression. This is
consistent with a self-verification view of the Impostor Phenomenon, in which people
who score higher on measures of IP report more discomfort with others’ false views of
them. However, endorsement of the two possible reasons why participants may not wish
to correct the perceiver’s impression (it felt good to have others think positive things
about them, or they did not want any negative consequences to result from the perceiver
knowing the truth) was not associated with any individual difference variable, rs < .21, ps
> .15.
Additional analyses explored relationships between IP scales and affect variables and
other features of impostor episodes. No significant relationships were found between
chronological distance (excluding outliers of more than 4 years) and any IP or affect
variable. The participants’ rating of the importance of the perceiver’s perception did not
correlate significantly with any IP measure, but did correlate positively with heightened
fear and distress at the time of the episode, r (118) = .25, p < .01, and at recalling the
episode, r (118) = .24, p < .01. No other PANAS factor correlated significantly with this
variable, rs < .10, ps > .28, with the exception of the PANAS factor of guilt and shame,
which showed a marginally significant positive correlation at the time of the survey, r
(118) = .18, p = .052. None of the IP scales were significantly correlated with
participants’ ratings of the perceiver’s relative standing on the status dimensions (e.g.,
intelligent; competent) with the exception that those who scored higher on Clance’s Fear
of Failure factor reported that the perceiver was relatively more confident than the self.
Discussion
Documenting the phenomenology of impostor episodes is important because past
research on the Impostor Phenomenon had yielded data largely nonsupportive of the IP as
a stable personality attribute. Thus, this study explored the IP as an affective state
resulting from circumstances in which one is evaluated “too highly.” The rich data
resulting from the qualitative and quantitative analyses herein suggest this is a fruitful
approach.
Intuition might suggest that being viewed positively by others would always be
desirable and satisfying. However, our data showed instead that negative affect is a
common reaction to feeling that one’s abilities are being overestimated. Most participants
in this sample were able to describe in detail an episode in which they felt another was
overestimating their abilities in an academic, employment, extracurricular, or social
setting. Generally, the perceiver was of an equal or higher status than the participant in
terms of role (e.g., fellow classmate or professor/ boss) but participants viewed the
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
195
perceiver as of higher status in terms of perceived attributes (e.g., competence,
confidence). Some clear themes of the narratives included feeling pressure to perform in
the future; feeling uncomfortable with the lack of truth; becoming motivated to fulfill the
perceiver’s expectation; and feeling good that one is perceived highly by an important
other.
Some participants expressed discomfort with having someone believe untrue things
about them specifically because they were untrue, even when those beliefs were positive.
This is consistent with Swann’s (Swann et al., 2003; Swann et al., 1992) self-verification
theory. Contrary to the expectation in much of social psychology that people want to be
viewed positively, support for self-verification theory shows that even more so, people
want to be viewed accurately. People want to feel consistency and coherence between
their self-views and feedback from others. They also want to be sure that future
interactions will go smoothly, which may not happen if others hold false views of them or
later find out how incompetent they really are (Swann et al., 2003). This theme of fear of
later discovery was reported by many of our participants.
One contribution of the present study is that it documents the pervasiveness of
impostor experiences. Most participants easily recalled experiencing an impostor episode.
Thus, these do not seem to be unusual experiences or ones confined to certain highly-
specific external situations. However, future research could profitably explore these
feelings to an even deeper extent by focusing on diverse adult samples and on people
going through role transitions. As Clance (1985) and Topping and Kimmel (1985) state,
new jobs are a prime time to experience feelings of impostorism. To be a new college
professor or a new doctor (or, as a minister friend told one of us, a new clergyperson)
means to have your “clientele” looking at you as if you are a competent, accomplished
actual member of that profession. The feelings one experiences in the new role, however,
are unlikely to match that characterization. We expect that during these times of role
transition, the affective experience of impostorism would be structurally the same as what
we found here but more intense. Future work should also study IP experiences in other
populations. Our college student sample described experiences typical in college and high
school. Work with more diverse or older populations would likely yield more
descriptions of IP episodes in employment settings.
As previously discussed, past research yielded data unsupportive of the IP as a stable
personality attribute and questions the validity of IP scales. Should researchers conclude
that the IP is not useful to study from a personality perspective? Not at all: Ample
evidence indicates that those scoring high on IP scales experience myriad negative
affective states (Henning, Ey, & Shaw, 1998; McElwee & Yurak, 2007), predict failure in
future situations (Cozzarelli & Major, 1990), and hold themselves to perfectionistic
standards (Henning et al.,1998; Kolligian & Sternberg, 1991; Thompson et al., 2000).
The scales are valid in the sense that they differentiate effectively between people
identified through other means such a clinical interviews as impostors or nonimpostors
(Holmes, Kertay, Adamson, Holland, & Clance, 1993) and predict affective and cognitive
reactions to hypothetical situations (Kolligian & Sternberg, 1990).
Thus, scales measuring the IP identify a group of people who appear to suffer from
self-doubt and other aversive affective states. Furthermore, in the present study, higher
scores on IP measures were associated with reporting a greater frequency of impostor
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
196
episodes and with greater negative affect during and while reflecting about an actual
episode. However, future work on the IP as an individual difference variable should be
careful to heed the findings of Leary et al. (2000) and McElwee and Yurak (2007) about
the lack of a true belief in “impostors” that others see them positively and about the self-
presentational aspects of impostorism (see also Ferrari and Thompson, 2006). Our
summative view is that the scales can be used to identify people who report impostor
feelings, but researchers should not assume that their underlying beliefs actually reflect
the content of the statements they endorse on the scales. Researchers need to consider the
possibility that respondents may be more concerned with self-deprecating self-
presentation in order to lower expectations and provide an excuse for failure, in the
(probably unlikely) event it were to occur.
This study represents a first step in a new approach to learning about the impostor
phenomenon. It has been our experience that when we discuss our work on this topic
informally, people readily understand the emotional experience we describe and
spontaneously and animatedly recount stories of occasions they felt that way. Certainly
this experience happens to people with a wide variety of personality attributes and is not
limited to those who score high on IP scales. We encourage future research, especially
research conducted with participants experiencing role transitions.
References
Buckley, K. E., Winkle, R. E., & Leary, M. R. (2004). Reactions to acceptance and
rejection: Effects of level and sequence of relational evaluation. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 14-28.
Chrisman, S. M., Pieper, W. A., Clance, P. R., Holland, C. L., & Glickhauf-Hughes, C.
(1995). Validation of the Clance Impostor Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment,
65, 456-467.
Clance, P. R. (1985). The impostor phenomenon. Atlanta: Peachtree.
Clance, P. R., & Imes, S. A. (1978). The impostor phenomenon in high achieving
women: Dynamics and therapeutic intervention. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research,
and Practice, 15, 241-247.
Cole, B. S., Hopkins, C. M., Tisak, J., Steele, J. L., & Carr, B. I . (2008). Assessing
spiritual growth and spiritual decline following a diagnosis of cancer: Reliability and
validity of the Spiritual Transformation Scale. Psycho-Oncology, 17, 112-121.
Cowman, S. E., & Ferrari, J. R. (2002). Am I for real? Predicting impostor tendencies
from self-handicapping and affective components. Social Behavior and Personality,
30, 119-126.
Cozzarelli, C., & Major, B. (1990). Exploring the validity of the imposter phenomenon.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 401-417.
Ferrari, J. R., & Thompson, T. (2006). Impostor fears: Links with self-presentational
concerns and self-handicapping behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences,
40, 341-352.
McElwee & Yurak / Individual Differences Research, 2010, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 184-197
197
Henning, K., Ey, S., & Shaw, D. (1998). Perfectionism, the imposter phenomenon and
psychological adjustment in medical, dental, nursing, and pharmacy students.
Medical Education, 32, 456-464.
Holmes, S. W., Kertay, L., Adamson, L. B., Holland, C. L., & Clance, P. R. (1993).
Measuring the impostor phenomenon: A comparison of Clance’s IP scale and
Harvey’s I-P scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 60, 48-59.
Kolligian, J., & Sternberg, R. J. (1991). Perceived fraudulence in young adults: Is there
an “impostor syndrome?” Journal of Personality Assessment, 56, 308-326.
Leary, M. R., Patton, K. M., Orlando, A. E., & Funk, W. W. (2000). The impostor
phenomenon: Self-perceptions, reflected appraisals, and interpersonal strategies.
Journal of Personality, 68, 725-756.
Leary, M. R., Springer, C., Negel, L., Ansell, E., & Evans, K. (1998). The causes,
phenomenology, and consequences of hurt feelings. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 1225-1237.
McElwee, R. O., & Yurak, T. J. (2007). Feeling versus acting like an impostor: Real
feelings of fraudulence or self-presentation? Individual Differences Research, 5, 201-
220.
Swann, W. B., Rentfrow, P. J., & Guinn, J. S. (2003). Self-verification: The search for
coherence. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of Self and Identity (pp.
367-383). New York: Guilford.
Swann, W. B., Stein-Seroussi, A., & Giesler, R. B. (1992). Why people self-verify.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 392-401.
Thompson, T., Foreman, P., & Martin, F. (2000). Impostor fears and perfectionistic
concern over mistakes. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 629-647.
Tice, D. M., & Wallace, H. M. (2003). The reflected self: Creating yourself as (you
think) others see you. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of Self and
Identity (pp. 91-105). New York: Guilford.
Topping, M. E., & Kimmel, E. B. (1985). The impostor phenomenon: Feeling phony.
Academic Psychology Bulletin, 7, 213-226.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070.
Copyright of Individual Differences Research is the property of IDR Publishing and its content may not be
copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.