ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Children with speech disorders often present with systematic speech error patterns. In clinical assessments of speech disorders, evaluating the severity of the disorder is central. Current measures of severity have limited sensitivity to factors like the frequency of the target sounds in the child's language and the degree of phonological diversity, which are factors that can be assumed to affect intelligibility. By constructing phonological filters to simulate eight speech error patterns often observed in children, and applying these filters to a phonologically transcribed corpus of 350K words, this study explores three quantitative measures of phonological impact: Percentage of Consonants Correct (PCC), edit distance, and degree of homonymy. These metrics were related to estimated ratings of severity collected from 34 practicing clinicians. The results show an expected high correlation between the PCC and edit distance metrics, but that none of the three metrics align with clinicians' ratings. Although these results do not generate definite answers to what phonological factors contribute the most to (un)intelligibility, this study demonstrates a methodology that allows for large-scale investigations of the interplay between phonological errors and their impact on speech in context, within and across languages.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Ranking severity of speech errors by their phonological impact in context
Sofia Strömbergsson1, Christina Tånnander2, Jens Edlund1
1 Department of Speech, Music and Hearing, KTH, Stockholm, Sweden
2 Swedish Agency for Accessible Media, Johanneshov, Sweden
sostr@kth.se, christina.tannander@mtm.se, edlund@speech.kth.se
Abstract
Children with speech disorders often present with systematic
speech error patterns. In clinical assessments of speech
disorders, evaluating the severity of the disorder is central.
Current measures of severity have limited sensitivity to factors
like the frequency of the target sounds in the child’s language
and the degree of phonological diversity, which are factors
that can be assumed to affect intelligibility. By constructing
phonological filters to simulate eight speech error patterns
often observed in children, and applying these filters to a
phonologically transcribed corpus of 350K words, this study
explores three quantitative measures of phonological impact:
Percentage of Consonants Correct (PCC), edit distance, and
degree of homonymy. These metrics were related to estimated
ratings of severity collected from 34 practicing clinicians. The
results show an expected high correlation between the PCC
and edit distance metrics, but that none of the three metrics
align with clinicians’ ratings. Although these results do not
generate definite answers to what phonological factors
contribute the most to (un)intelligibility, this study
demonstrates a methodology that allows for large-scale
investigations of the interplay between phonological errors and
their impact on speech in context, within and across languages.
Index Terms: speech disorders, intelligibility, child speech
1. Introduction
Children with speech sound disorders (SSDs) often exhibit
systematic errors, affecting groups of sounds or sound
patterns. For example, a child with SSD can display patterns
of stopping of fricatives, velar fronting, or consonant cluster
reductions [1]. Children with SSD may exhibit only one error
pattern, but more often, children display combinations of
speech errors [2]. In speech-language therapy, these different
types of errors need to be prioritized. Considering that the
ultimate goal of speech-language therapy is to optimize
communicative functioning, it makes sense to focus
intervention on those speech error patterns that have the most
detrimental effects on communication.
In clinical descriptions of SSDs, “severity” is a central
concept, referring to the degree to which the speech disorder
affects the child’s communication skills [3]. The most
commonly used metric of severity is the Percentage of
Consonants Correct (PCC) [4]. However, there are several
limitations associated with using the PCC as an index of
severity. For example, the measure is based on binary
evaluations of correct/incorrect production of consonants, and
therefore not sensitive to different types and degrees of
distortions/errors [5]. Although variants of the PCC metric
addressing this limitation have been suggested [6], together
with alternative measures like the Weighted Speech Sound
Accuracy (WSSA) score [5], no measure to date is based on
statistically motivated weighting of speech errors. For
example, although it may make intuitive sense to weight
deletions or additions of weak segments (e.g. glottals and
glides) half as much as errors involving strong segments (e.g.
orally articulated consonants and vowels) as in [5], both the
classification and the weighting are arbitrary. Moreover,
existing measures of severity have almost exclusively been
evaluated with regards to English; hence, they are not
necessarily directly applicable to other languages.
Misarticulation of sounds that occur frequently has more
pervasive effects than misarticulation of sounds that are less
frequent [7]. Moreover, the types of words that are affected
also plays a role; misarticulation of content words has more
detrimental effects on intelligibility than misarticulation of
function words [8]. Hence, measures of severity should also be
sensitive to what types of words are affected by speech errors.
Another potential source of diminished intelligibility is
neutralization of phonological contrasts, or homonymy [9].
This is apparent in cases where, for instance, a child’s
production of the word key is indistinguishable from his or her
production of the word tea. Hence, a metric of severity should
also be sensitive to the extent to which the speech error(s)
cause homonymy. The phonological distance between a
speech target and another version e.g. a dialectal version, or
a misarticulated version of the same word may be measured
by means of the Levenshtein metric [10]. Although it shares
several features with the PCC metric, the interrelation between
PCC and phonological distance as measured by the
Levenshtein metric has not yet been described. By considering
the relative impact of each of these factors, and of all of them
combined, currently used measures of severity may be
extended to better reflect aspects of the impact of specific
speech errors in a communicative context.
The severity of speech disorders is closely related to
intelligibility. Although the relation between different types of
speech errors and their impact on intelligibility has been
studied, it is not yet fully understood; suggestions of
phonetic/phonological correlates to (un)intelligibility are often
not based on empirical evidence [11]. However, by exploring
the distribution of different speech errors across children
grouped by level of intelligibility, indirect links between
different speech errors and levels of intelligibility have been
observed [12]. This way, omissions of speech sounds have
been concluded as being more damaging to intelligibility than
phonetic distortions [12]. More direct links between errors and
(un)intelligibility may be described by simulating error
patterns, and exploring outcomes with regards to intelligibility.
A rare example of using this approach is described in [11],
where three different speech error patterns final consonant
deletion, stopping, and velar fronting were ranked with
regards to their impact on intelligibility. By applying three
different “phonological filters” (each representing one speech
error pattern) to a phonetically transcribed text, speech errors
were simulated and read aloud by an adult male speaker.
Although the results from this study e.g. that final consonant
deletion has the most detrimental effects on intelligibility
have important clinical implications, the ecological validity in
having an adult male producing speech errors from transcribed
texts may be questioned. However, using phonological filters
to represent speech errors is still a viable approach to studying
the impact of different speech errors in context, particularly
for large-scale text-based studies.
There is an apparent value in knowing how specific speech
error patterns contribute to decreased intelligibility, as therapy
targeting those error patterns that are most detrimental to
intelligibility will potentially be most rewarding in terms of
functional gains. Moreover, by relating objective measures of
severity to practicing clinicians’ intuitive estimates of severity,
the measures may be compared with regards to how they
reflect clinical intuition on severity thus examining their
construct validity. This study constitutes the first report of
such a comparison, by addressing the following questions:
1. How does the PCC relate to two other measures
describing the impact of speech errors: the degree of
homonymy and Levenshtein distance?
2. How are different speech errors ranked by their impact on
severity by these different metrics?
3. How do these ranks compare to practicing clinicians’
intuitive rankings of how different speech errors affect
intelligibility?
2. Method
2.1. Data
With the aim of gathering text material as representative of
children’s speech production as possible, reflecting expected
vocabularies in preschool-aged children, a corpus of children’s
books was collected from Språkbanken1. Two versions of this
corpus were used: the full corpus (Corpusfull), and a version
where all frequent function words (occurring among the 100
most common words) had been excluded (Corpuscontent). Word
statistics for both corpora are presented in Table 1.
Table 1. Word statistics describing the two corpora.
Corpus
# word
tokens
# word
types
Type/token
ratio
Corpusfull
351 094
18 962
.054
Corpuscontent
192 957
18 904
.098
Phonological transcriptions of all words in the corpora,
available from the Swedish Agency for Accessible Media,
were on a broad level, reflecting frequent reductions in
colloquial speech. For example, the final consonant of the
word “och” (Eng. and) and of adjectives ending in “-ig” (e.g.
gullig, Eng. cute) is omitted.
2.2. Procedure
Based on a description of speech error patterns observed in
Swedish children with PD [13], eight error patterns were
selected for analysis. This selection was restricted to context-
independent speech error patterns that could be expressed as
paradigmatic substitutions. Context-dependent patterns like
1 A subset of the corpus Läsbart (available from
http://spraakbanken.gu.se/eng/resource/lasbart), where only
children’s literature was included.
assimilations and metatheses were not included in the analysis.
Thus, the included patterns could all be straight-forwardly
represented as phonological filters. Table 2 presents a list of
the filters/error patterns examined.
Table 2. The speech error patterns examined, as
expressed as phonological filters.
Speech error
Substitution(s)
Stopping Fricatives substituted by stops, while
retaining (approx.) place of
articulation. E.g. /s/
/t/, /f/
/p/.
Cluster
reductions Syllable-initial consonant sequences
reduced,
a) In cases of CC, where one C is
a plosive, the other is omitted.
E.g. /pl/ /p/, /st/ /t/
b) In other cases of CC, where the
first is /s/, /s/ will be omitted.
Eg. /sn/ /n/, /sl/ /l/
c) In other cases of CC, where the
second is /l/, /r/ or /j/, the
second C will be omitted. E.g.
/fl/ /f/, /mj/ /m/
d) In cases of CCC, the last C is
omitted. E.g. /str/
/st/
/h/-zation Initial voiceless consonants (and /r/)
substituted by /h/.
Backing /t, d, n/
/k, g, ŋ/
Fronting /k, g, ŋ/
/t, d, n/
Labialization /
ɧ
/
/f/
/r/-weakening /r/
/j/
/s/-distortion /s/
/θ/
Target transcriptions representing expected (correct)
production of all words in the corpus were available in the
lexicon. Different versions of these transcriptions, each
representing the expected production assuming a specific
speech error type, were generated by constructing
“phonological filters” and applying these to the target
transcriptions. For example, when the target transcription is
passed through the phonological filter representing velar
fronting (substitution of [t, d, n] for the targets [k, g, ŋ]), a
filtered transcription is generated, representing the expected
pronunciation of a child exhibiting consistent velar fronting.
The eight filters were applied to both corpora.
2.3. Effect measures
Three different metrics were used, each representing a
different aspect of the phonological effect of applying a
specific phonological filter to the target transcription:
1. The Percentage of Consonants Correct (PCC): the
proportion of consonants that are not produced in
error. Here, this refers to the proportion of
consonants that are unaffected by the application of
the phonological filters.
2. Degree of homonymy (HOMONYMY): the proportion
of words that share the same phonological
transcription as at least one other word. A low
degree of homonymy corresponds to high
phonological diversity.
3. Overall phonological distance (EDITDIST): the
average Levenshtein (or edit) distance (i.e. the
minimum number of insertions, deletions or
substitutions required to transform one string into
another) between target transcriptions and error
transcriptions, across all word tokens in the corpus.
2.4. Clinical survey
34 speech-language pathologists (SLPs) were recruited to
provide their estimated evaluations of the impact of each of
the error types on intelligibility. The participants all reported
having at least 5 years of clinical experience with childhood
speech and language disorders. Responses were collected by
means of a web form, in which the SLPs rated the eight speech
error types with regards to a 5-step scale, where 0 indicates
“no impact” and 5 indicates “severe impact”. Inter-rater
reliability was estimated by means of an intra-class
correlation, which revealed a high level of consistency:
ICC(2,34) = .98 (95% CI: .962-.996).
3. Results
3.1. Relation between metrics
In order to explore the correlation between the three different
effect metrics, three separate Pearson’s correlations were
conducted one for each pair of metrics. When estimated on
Corpusfull, these analyses showed a strong negative correlation
between PCC and EDITDIST (r = -.99, p < .001), whereas
HOMONYMY was found to correlate neither with PCC (r = -.14,
p = .74), nor with EDITDIST (r = .26, p = .53). Although the
numeric details were not identical, the same correlation pattern
was observed also for Corpuscontent. Table 3 displays the
outcomes of the different effect measures for all speech error
patterns, for both corpora. As indicated in the table, the speech
errors are ranked similarly across the corpora, although the
numeric details are not identical.
Table 3. Phonological impact of the eight error
patterns, as measured by the three different metrics.
Corpus
full
content
Error
PCC
E
DIT
H
OM
PCC
E
DIT
H
OM
Backing
66
1,3
0,02
69
1,0
0,02
Stopping
81
1,0
0,05
80
0,8
0,05
/h/-zation
86
0,9
0,11
86
0,8
0,11
/r/-weak.
88
0,8
0,01
86
0,7
0,01
/s/-dist.
90
0,8
0,01
89
0,7
0,01
Fronting
91
0,7
0,02
89
0,7
0,02
ClustRed.
92
0,7
0,04
90
0,7
0,04
Labial.
100
*
0,5
0,01
99
0,5
0,01
* Here, the PPC value 99.598 is rounded up to 100.
In accordance with the results of the correlation analyses,
the numbers in Table 3 illustrate the close inverse relation
between PCC and EDITDIST, and that the HOMONYMY metric
yields a more disparate ranking of speech errors. Three
separate Pearson’s correlation analyses (one per metric)
showed strong significant correlations across the two corpora,
all three being r(8) > .98, p < .001. However, three paired-
samples t-tests (one per metric) revealed corpus-dependent
differences for EDITDIST: t(7) = 3.69, p < .01 and for
HOMONYMY: t(7) = 17.36, p < .001, such that phonological
impact was generally higher in Corpusfull than in Corpuscontent.
For the PCC metric, no difference between the corpora were
found: t(7) = 1.22, p = .26.
3.2. Clinical survey
Figure 1 displays the results of the clinical survey, where SLPs
rated the different error patterns by their impact on
intelligibility. A Kruskal-Wallis analysis exploring the
variation in rating across the different speech error patterns
showed a significant dependence: χ2(6, N = 272) = 118.44,
p < .001. Multiple pairwise comparisons revealed significant
differences between all error patterns except for those
indicated with “n.s.” in Figure 1.
Figure 1: Impact on intelligibility, for each speech
error pattern, as estimated by practicing clinicians
with reference to a scale from “no impact” (0) and
“severe impact” (5). Error bars represent 1 S.E.
3.3. Metrics vs. clinical survey
A linear regression was conducted in order to explore whether
any of the three metrics could predict the clinical estimates of
impact on intelligibility. This analysis showed that none of the
three metrics significantly predicted the clinical estimates,
neither alone, nor in combination with any of the other two.
This was found for both corpora. Figure 2 illustrates how the
rankings of the three effect metrics relate to the ranking of the
clinical estimates, for Corpuscontent. Clinicians’ estimated
ranking is indicated by the ordering of the error patterns along
the y-axis, ranging from the most severe errors on top, to the
least severe errors at the bottom. Ranking on the x-axis is
given as the ratio to the maximum values observed for PCC,
EDITDIST and HOMONYMY, respectively.
If an impact metric corresponded well to clinical estimates
of severity, a decreasing pattern from top to bottom would be
expected in figure 2. However, in congruence with the results
of the linear regression, figure 2 shows that neither of the
impact measures matches the clinical estimates of severity
very closely. For example, in relation to the clinical estimates,
both the PCC metric and the EDITDIST metric overestimate the
severity of /r/-weakening and /s/-distortions. In the same vein,
the HOMONYMY metric overestimates /h/-zation, while under-
012345
/s/-dist.
/r/-weak.
ClustRed.
Labial.
Fronting
/h/-zation
Stopping
Backing
n.s.
n.s.
n.s.
n.s.
n.s.
estimating the effect of backing. Moreover, labialization is
ranked by all three metrics as a less severe error patterns
compared to clinicians’ estimates.
Figure 2: Ranking of impact, for each speech error
pattern, presented as the ratio to the maximum value
for PCC, EDITDIST and HOMONYMY, respectively.
Clinicians’ estimates of severity are indicated by the
ordering along the y-axis, from the highest impact on
top, to the lowest impact at the bottom.
4. Discussion
We have presented an investigation of the severity of different
speech error patterns, as measured by their phonological
impact on speech in context. By simulating speech error
patterns often observed in children with speech sound
disorders, and by investigating how these error patterns are
ranked by three objective measures of phonological change,
the interrelations between these metrics were explored, as well
as how each of them relates to experienced clinicians’
estimates of how the same speech errors affect intelligibility.
Two of the measures, PCC and EDITDIST, were found to be
highly intercorrelated, whereas the third, HOMONYMY, shared
little resemblance with the other two. Considering that PCC
and EDITDIST are both based on character-by-character
comparisons between target transcriptions and error
transcriptions, their close interrelation is not surprising. Hence,
these measures both represent the average phonological
distance between target and error productions, which has been
suggested as one factor contributing to perceived
(un)intelligibility [14]. However, the fact that neither PCC nor
EDITDIST predict clinicians’ intuitive ratings of how different
speech errors affect intelligibility, accords with established
knowledge that many other factors also affect intelligibility.
The observation that phonological impact was generally
higher in Corpusfull than in Corpuscontent indicates that function
words, by virtue of occurring frequently, pose a stronger
influence on measures of phonological effects. However,
although the effect measure values are generally higher in
Corpusfull, their relative rankings of speech errors were
observed not to be different from those found for Corpuscontent.
Hence, the examined speech errors affect function words very
similarly to how they affect content words. However,
considering that misarticulation of function words is less
deteriorating for intelligibility than misarticulation of content
words, the measures as applied to Corpuscontent can be assumed
to better reflect effects on intelligibility.
To the authors’ knowledge, the present study constitutes a
first report of clinical estimates of how different speech errors
affect intelligibility for Swedish. Considering the high
agreement between raters, this may be considered a reliable
benchmark to which quantitative measures of severity may be
compared. In the present report, none of the three measures
reflect clinical estimates very closely. For one, PCC and
EDITDIST overestimate the effects of “light” problems like /r/-
weakening and /s/-distortions. (This limitation of the PCC has
been reported earlier, e.g. [6].) In this respect, the HOMONOMY
metric reflects clinical estimates better. On the other hand, the
HOMONYMY metric overestimates the severity of cluster
reductions and /h/-zation. The fact that not even the
combination of the three metrics predicts the clinical estimates
indicates that other factors need to be included in the model.
Although all error patterns examined in the present have
also been reported for other languages (e.g. English [15] and
Dutch [16]), their phonological impact on speech in context
will vary with phonotactic characteristics tied to different
languages. Therefore, a ranking of speech errors by severity
for one language will not necessarily be valid for other
languages. However, the method described may easily be
applied to other languages, allowing for exploration of cross-
linguistic differences in phonological impact of speech errors.
Using children’s literature as a proxy for children’s
expected speech production is a choice that deserves
motivation. Not only are linguistic differences expected
between spoken and written modes of language, one would
also expect different linguistic features in texts written for
children, compared to what the children themselves would
produce. However, in lack of access to a similarly-sized
corpus of children’s spoken utterances, this option was
considered the best available for the present investigation.
The idea of constructing phonological filters to simulate
speech errors involves some limitations that should be
considered. One aspect is that of describing speech errors with
reference to categorical labels, even though acoustic
evaluation of speech errors often reveals more gradual
differences between targets and errors [17]. However, when
aiming at ensuring a controlled and linguistically
representative setting, analyses require large corpora of
transcribed speech, where reduction of phonetic detail is
inevitable. Another limitation regards the application of each
phonological filter separately and across all word positions;
clearly, this is a simplification of how speech errors actually
appear in children’s speech. Refining the phonological filters,
allowing less consistent application, and exploring effects of
applying multiple filters, are evident venues for future work.
This investigation constitutes a first step towards large-
scale examination of the severity of different speech errors
with reference to controlled linguistically representative
contexts. Although the phonological filters described may be
refined, and although additional measures of phonological
impact may be introduced, the report demonstrates the
potential of ranking speech errors by their impact in context.
Clinically, such quantitative measures may extend current
recommendations regarding what specific speech error
patterns should be prioritized in therapy with information
regarding how the errors affect children’s communicative
functioning.
0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100%
/s/-dist.
/r/-weak.
ClustRed.
Labial.
Fronting
/h/-zation
Stopping
Backing
PCC
EDITDIST
HOMONYMY
5. References
[1]
Nijland, L., ”Speech perception in children with speech output
disorders,” Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 23(3): 222-
239,
2009.
[2]
Rvachew, S. and Nowak, M., ”The Effect of Target-Selection
Strategy on Phonological Learning,”
Journal of Speech Language
and Hearing Research, 44(3), 610-623, 2001.
[3]
Prezas, R. and Hodson, B., ”Diagnostic evaluation of children
with speech sound disorders,” in S. Rvachew (Ed.)
Encyclopedia
of language and literacy development
, London, Ontario,
Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network, 2007, pp. 1
-
7.
[4]
Shriberg, L. D. and Kwiatkowski, J., ”Phonological disorders III:
A procedure for assessing severity of involvement,”
Journal of
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47(3), 256-270, 1982.
[5]
Preston, J. L., Ramsdell, H. L., Oller, D. K., Edwards, M. L. and
Tobin, S. J.
, ”Developing a weighted measure of speech sound
accuracy,”
Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research,
54(1), 1-18, 2011.
[6]
Shriberg,, L. D., Austin, D., Lewis, B. A., McSweeny, J. L. and
Wilson, D. L.
, ”The percentage of consonants correct (PCC)
metric: Extensions and reliability data,” Journal of Speec
h
Language and Hearing Research, 40(4), 708-722, 1997.
[7]
Brown, A., ”Functional load and the teaching of pronunciation,”
TESOL Quarterly, 22(4), 593-606, 1988.
[8]
Lam, J. and Tjaden, K., ”Intelligibility of Clear Speech: Effect of
Instruction,”
Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research,
56(5), 1429-1440, 2013.
[9]
Ingram, D., Phonological disability in children, London, UK:
Cole and Whurr Ltd, 1989.
[10]
Heeringa, W., Measuring dialect pronunciation differences using
Levenshtein distance, PhD Thesis,
Groeningen, The Netherlands:
University of Groeningen, 2004.
[11]
Klein, E. S. and Flint, C. B., ”Measurement of intelligibility in
disordered speech,” Language, Speech,
and Hearing Services in
Schools, 37(3), 191-199, 2006.
[12]
Hodson, B. W. and Paden, E., Targeting intelligible speech: A
phonological approach to remediation, San Diego, CA: College-
Hill Press, 1983.
[13]
Nettelbladt, U., ”Fonologiska problem hos barn med
språkstörning,” in U. Nettelbladt & E.-
K. Salameh (Eds.)
Språkutveckling och språkstörning hos barn
, Lund,
Studentlitteratur, 2007, pp. 95-134.
[14]
Gooskens, C., Heeringa, W. and Beijering, K., ”Phonetic and
lexical predictors of intelligibility,”
International Journal of
Humanities and Arts Computing, 2(1-2), 63-81, 2008.
[15]
Grunwell, P., Clinical Phonology, Baltimore, MD: Williams &
Wilkins, 1987.
[16]
Beers, M., ”Phonological processes in Dutch language impaired
children,”
Scandinavian Journal of Logopedics and Phoniatrics,
17, 9-16, 1992.
[17]
Tyler, A. A., Figurski, G. R. and Langsdale, T., ”Relationships
between acoustically determined knowledge of stop place and
voicing contrasts and phonological treatment progress,”
Journal
of Speech and Hearing Research, 36(4), 746-759, 1993.
Article
Full-text available
Phonological errors in communicating will let the listener gain a different meaning and communication becomes impeded. The students' errors are due to their lack of understanding of the sound system of language, as their second-language acquisition. The problem in this research is what type of phonological errors are made by students when they are listening to the short story and will impact on their speaking. The data is conducted in the language laboratory of The National University in Jakarta by secondsemester students in the subject of Spoken English. in the Faculty of Letters, English Department. The purpose of this research study is to learnthe difficulties of students when learning Basic Spoken English. The method used is descriptive with content analysis techniques. The result showed the most the biggest of surface structure taxonomy of phonological errors created by the students when they are listening to the short story and has impacted on speaking is global errors in diphthong (61,29%). This is because in Indonesian there is no sound of a diphthong. These global errors will have an effect on good communication. The students should study the phonological system in the target language, especially diphthong sound, in addition to vowels and consonants. It is expected that the lecturer often guides students' pronunciation based on the point and the manner of articulation and improve if they are not able to.
Technical Report
Full-text available
This report is the conclusion of a workgroup, which the steering group assembled to evaluate the status of Icelandic language technology and to create a five-year project plan. The workgroup suggests that four open core-solutions are created: Speech Recogniser; Speech Synthesiser; Machine Translation System; and Spell and Grammar Checker. These will be developed and adapted to Icelandic to the point where they are fully usable, and used by the public, companies, governmental bodies, and institutions in Iceland. The principal prerequisite in building language technology tools is to have language resources and support tools in place, and the report describes the measures required to achieve this. In addition to the project schedule, suggestions are made as to how it will be carried out, with a description of how similar schedules have been executed in other countries.
Article
Full-text available
In the present investigation, the intelligibility of 17 Scandinavian language varieties and standard Danish was assessed among young Danes from Copenhagen. In addition, distances between standard Danish and each of the 17 varieties were measured at the lexical level and at different phonetic levels. In order to determine how well these linguistic levels can predict intelligibility, we correlated the intelligibility scores with the linguistic distances and we carried out a number of regression analyses. The results show that for this particular set of closely related language varieties phonetic distance is a better predictor of intelligibility than lexical distance. Consonant substitutions, vowel insertions and vowel shortenings contribute significantly to the prediction of intelligibility.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The authors investigated how clear speech instructions influence sentence intelligibility. Method Twelve speakers produced sentences in habitual, clear, hearing impaired, and overenunciate conditions. Stimuli were amplitude normalized and mixed with multitalker babble for orthographic transcription by 40 listeners. The main analysis investigated percentage-correct intelligibility scores as a function of the 4 conditions and speaker sex. Additional analyses included listener response variability, individual speaker trends, and an alternate intelligibility measure: proportion of content words correct. Results Relative to the habitual condition, the overenunciate condition was associated with the greatest intelligibility benefit, followed by the hearing impaired and clear conditions. Ten speakers followed this trend. The results indicated different patterns of clear speech benefit for male and female speakers. Greater listener variability was observed for speakers with inherently low habitual intelligibility compared to speakers with inherently high habitual intelligibility. Stable proportions of content words were observed across conditions. Conclusions Clear speech instructions affected the magnitude of the intelligibility benefit. The instruction to overenunciate may be most effective in clear speech training programs. The findings may help explain the range of clear speech intelligibility benefit previously reported. Listener variability analyses suggested the importance of obtaining multiple listener judgments of intelligibility, especially for speakers with inherently low habitual intelligibility.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose To develop a system for numerically quantifying a speaker’s phonetic accuracy through transcription-based measures. With a focus on normal and disordered speech in children, the authors describe a system for differentially weighting speech sound errors on the basis of various levels of phonetic accuracy using a Weighted Speech Sound Accuracy (WSSA) score. The authors then evaluate the reliability and validity of this measure. Method Phonetic transcriptions were analyzed from several samples of child speech, including preschoolers and young adolescents with and without speech sound disorders and typically developing toddlers. The new measure of phonetic accuracy was validated against existing measures, was used to discriminate typical and disordered speech production, and was evaluated to examine sensitivity to changes in phonetic accuracy over time. Reliability between transcribers and consistency of scores among different word sets and testing points are compared. Results Initial psychometric data indicate that WSSA scores correlate with other measures of phonetic accuracy as well as listeners' judgments of the severity of a child’s speech disorder. The measure separates children with and without speech sound disorders and captures growth in phonetic accuracy in toddlers' speech over time. The measure correlates highly across transcribers, word lists, and testing points. Conclusion Results provide preliminary support for the WSSA as a valid and reliable measure of phonetic accuracy in children’s speech.
Article
The concept of functional load has been used by various writers in various linguistic fields and has consequently received differing definitions and methods of calculation. It has not, however, been applied to the teaching of pronunciation. This article examines several aspects of functional load in English that may be relevant for assessing the relative importance of segmental features of learners' speech. Implications for the use of pronunciation drill books are discussed.
Article
Process analyses were made of the spontaneous speech of 15 Dutch language impaired children. The processes that were applied by these children are presented as a rank order of their frequency of occurrence in 24 samples of these children. This rank order was compared to a rank order of processes occurring in the speech of 32 Swedish phonologically impaired children as well as to English data. Some processes were applied equally often in the three languages (e.g. Cluster reduction, Pretonic syllable deletion, Devoicing). Since these are processes that are often mentioned in the literature it is suggested that there is a universal aspect to them. Other processes (e.g. Gliding, Stopping. Velarisation) differ in their frequency of occurrence. Sometimes this could be explained by the specific language the children were acquiring, thereby suggesting that there is influence of the phonological characteristics of the ambient language on the speech of the children.
Article
'Measuring dialect pronunciation differences using Levenshtein distance' is het proefschrift van Wilbert Heeringa. Hij heeft zich beziggehouden met een onderzoek naar methoden waarmee taalvariatie nader bestudeerd kan worden. Heeringa bespreekt eerst diverse methoden voor het meten van taalkundige afstanden tussen dialecten, waaronder de 'Levenshtein-afstand'. Deze methode staat centraal in het verdere proefschrift; er worden verschillende varianten van gepresenteerd. Vervolgens legt Heeringa uit hoe de betreffende methode precies werkt en meet. Dan volgt de beschrijving van een aantal onderzoeken waarin de methode wordt gebruikt. Een groot aantal Nederlandse dialecten wordt hierin met elkaar en met het standaard Nederlands vergeleken. Ook wordt een aantal Noorse dialecten bij het onderzoek betrokken. Het boek besluit met de conclusie, die luidt dat de Levenshtein-afstand bruikbaar is voor het berekenen van afstanden tussen taalvariëteiten. Dit is gebaseerd op de uitkomsten van gepresenteerde onderzoeken. Bron: http://taalunieversum.org/cultuur/bibliografische attenderingslijst/20/3/2/
Article
Research in the field of speech production pathology is dominated by describing deficits in output. However, perceptual problems might underlie, precede, or interact with production disorders. The present study hypothesizes that the level of the production disorders is linked to level of perception disorders, thus lower-order production problems (such as childhood apraxia of speech; CAS) are linked to lower-order perception problems and higher-order production problems (phonological disorder; PD) are linked to higher-order perception problems. For this, various perception tasks were administered (non-word auditory discrimination task, word rhyming task, categorical classification, and discrimination task) in children with CAS, PD, or a mixture. The results show that children with PD only show higher-order perception problems, whereas children with CAS have difficulties on both lower- and higher-order perception tasks. In children with CAS, difficulties at lower-order processes might affect the higher-order processes in development. Furthermore, significant correlations were found between production and perception scores at different levels. Thus, a link between perception and production seems to be evident.
Article
Data are presented to support the reliability, validity, and utility of a severity metric for phonological disorders. The metric, percentage of consonants correct (PCC), is readily derived from a continuous speech sample. PCC values are shown to reflect an ordinal severity scale that embraces the constructs of disability, intelligibility, and handicap. PCC values index four levels of "severity of involvement": Mild, mild-moderate, moderate-severe, and severe. The metric provides a means by which instructors, researchers, and speech-language pathologists working in different settings can specify subject descriptions, gauge the effects of intervention programs, and undertake cross-institutional projects. The metric is used as one component of a diagnostic classification system for phonological disorders (Shriberg & Kwiatkowski, 1982a) and within a framework for management of persons with phonological disorders (Shriberg & Kwiatkowski, 1982b), the first and second papers in this series.