ArticlePDF Available

The Psychology of Obesity

Authors:

Abstract

This chapter provides a discussion on the psychology of obesity. Four major individual factors have been explored as causes of overeating: interoceptive awareness, response to emotional experience, cognition, and biology. Current research is examining how underlying differences in neurobiology guide eating behavior. The past 50 years of research on the psychological causes of obesity have helped to isolate many important predictive factors and to clarify that obesity results from a variety of intra-and inter-individual factors. The factors that may be consequences of obesity are explained, such as stigmatization, discrimination, mental health problems, and physical health problems. Research on exercise interventions indicate positive health benefits, even in the absence of weight loss. It is clear that the mental health consequences of obesity should not take a back seat to treatments aimed solely at the physical health consequences.
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1  6
THE PSYCHOLOGY
OF OBESITY
ashley moskovich , jeffrey
hunger , and traci mann
The fi eld of psychology traditionally focuses on the study of individuals, their
internal mental processes, and their behavior; as such, it has adopted this approach
in the study of obesity. Rather than examining larger groups, such as communities,
or distal factors, such as laws, policies, or environmental barriers, psychologists
have explored people’s emotions, beliefs, goals, and behaviors as causes and conse-
quences of obesity. In recent years, however, and paralleling the direction of the
eld as a whole, psychologists have broadened their focus to include the infl uence
of relationships and social networks on obesity, as well as the role of genetic
and neurological factors in the development of obesity. With regard to treatment,
psychologists have focused primarily on behavioral rather than medical interven-
tions, and recently have begun to explore whether weight loss is a reasonable or
even necessary goal in treatments for obesity.
Causes of Obesity
A substantial body of work in psychology has been dedicated to exploring the causes
of obesity. Psychology originally viewed obesity as a result of psychopathology or as
a response to a signifi cant trauma in a person’s past (Kolata 2007 ). However, sup-
port for these assumptions has been mixed, and these explanations are not thought
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 8706-Cawley-Ch06.indd 87 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
88 disciplinary perspectives on obesity
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1to account for most cases of obesity (Stunkard and Wadden 1992 ). Rather, the focus
has been on individual factors such as thoughts, behaviors, and biology, as well as
aspects of the individual’s social environment such as familial and peer infl uences.
Many early theories of obesity continue to be refi ned and incorporated into current
etiological models.
The Individual
In 1957 it was proposed that obesity was the result of overeating a behavioral fac-
tor and was not exclusively due to impaired metabolic functioning (Kaplan and
Kaplan 1957 ). Much of the work within the eld has since focused on behavioral,
rather than biological, causes of obesity, and on overeating in particular. Early
research focused on differences in eating between obese and non-obese individuals,
but over time it became clear that many of the factors that promote overeating in
obese individuals also promote overeating in some non-obese individuals. Interest
has not only focused on determining who o v e r e a t s , b u t a l s o when a n d why overeating
occurs. Four major individual factors have been explored as causes of overeating:
interoceptive awareness (i.e., sensitivity to internal bodily states), response to
emotional experience, cognition, and biology.
Schacter’s internal-external theory of eating holds that different factors guide
eating for obese individuals from those that guide eating for non-obese individuals
(Schacter 1968 ; Schacter, Goldman, and Gordon 1968 ). According to this theory,
non-obese individuals use the internal sensations of hunger and satiety to guide
their eating, whereas obese individuals are less interoceptively aware and instead
rely on external cues such as the time of day or the presence of appetizing foods to
regulate eating. Although this theory is intuitive and parsimonious, original sup-
port for it was mixed and its basic premises have since been questioned (Rodin
1981 ). In particular, some have challenged the assertion that an overreliance on
external cues by obese individuals necessarily indicates that they are less interocep-
tively aware than non-obese individuals (Rodin 1981 ; Herman and Polivy 2008 ).
Furthermore, it has also been argued that non-obese individuals are not better than
obese people at using internal cues to regulate their eating, and that they are also
infl uenced by external cues (Rodin 1981 ; Herman and Polivy 2008 ).
Although it has been challenged, the internal-external hypothesis has not been
abandoned; rather, recent work has instead been expanding upon it. For example,
Herman and Polivy ( 2008 ) posit that the confl icting ndings from past research
actually demonstrate a clear pattern of eating behavior. They argue that some types
of external cues (e.g., portion size), which the authors refer to as normative cues,
infl uence everyone, whereas other cues (e.g., palatability), called sensory cues , only
impact the eating of obese and dieting individuals. Thus the internal-external
hypothesis is still present in the way in which we view the causes of obesity.
Differences in the way individuals respond to emotional experience is another
factor that is thought to play a role in the development of obesity. According to the
psychosomatic hypothesis (Kaplan and Kaplan 1957 ), obese people overeat as a way
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 8806-Cawley-Ch06.indd 88 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
the psychology of obesity 89
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1of coping with emotional distress. Obese individuals are thought to have learned
that eating reduces feelings of anxiety and therefore overeat as a way to self-soothe.
This results in a continuous cycle in which overeating aimed at reducing distress
actually promotes further distress and subsequent overeating (Kaplan and Kaplan
1957 ). Similarly, and foreshadowing the internal-external hypothesis, Bruch theo-
rized that obese individuals have diffi culties with interoceptive awareness and
therefore misinterpret emotional states as hunger (Bruch 1961 ).
Although these early theories have received only mixed support (Greeno and
Wing 1994 ), eating in response to emotional experience is still viewed as an impor-
tant contributor to overeating and obesity (Canetti, Bachar, and Berry 2002 ).
Research shows that some individuals, not exclusively obese individuals, exhibit a
tendency to overeat when in the presence of strong emotion, particularly negative
affect (e.g., Van Strien and Ouwens 2003 ; Polivy, Herman, and McFarlane 1994 ).
While emotional eating is well documented, it is still unclear why such behavior
occurs. Many studies have been unable to demonstrate that emotional eating atten-
uates negative affect (i.e., negative emotion states such as anxiety) (e.g., Herman
and Polivy 1975 ). Recent work, however, found that eating palatable food improved
mood among individuals classifi ed as emotional eaters (Macht and Mueller 2007 ).
Replication of this effect and clarifi cation of moderators and mechanisms will be
essential to this growing body of work.
The third individual factor thought to contribute to overeating, cognitive con-
trol, was introduced in a study by Herman and Mack ( 1975 ). This study highlighted
what has been termed the disinhibition effect the phenomenon that restrained
eaters, or dieters, engage in overeating after a perceived diet violation. This phe-
nomenon is considered a cognitive one (as opposed to a physiological one, for
example) because what matters is not whether restrained eaters actually violated
their diets, but whether they think that they did. Research supporting the role of
cognition shows that restrained eaters will overeat if they believe they have violated
their diet when in fact they did not, but they will not overeat after an actual diet
violation if they are led to believe that the food they consumed did not actually
violate their diet (Polivy and Herman 1985 ).
An individual’s self-imposed restraint essentially functions as a form of self-
control and is therefore susceptible to a broad range of factors known to contribute
to regulatory failures. These factors, such as ego threats (Heatherton, Herman, and
Polivy 1991 ), stress (Greeno and Wing 1994), and distraction (Mann and Ward
2000 ), have been shown to lead to overeating among restrained eaters, but not
among non-restrained individuals. Although restraint status has proven to be a
reliable indicator of overeating and has generated a considerable amount of research,
there is controversy over how it is best conceptualized and measured (Ruderman
1986 ); many believe that restraint only leads to self-regulation failure among
individuals who are also disinhibited eaters.
The fourth factor that has been explored as a source of overeating is individual
differences in biology. In an effort to explain discrepant results of externally based
eating behavior among obese individuals, Nisbett proposed a biological explanation
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 8906-Cawley-Ch06.indd 89 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
90 disciplinary perspectives on obesity
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1known as set-point theory (Nisbett 1972 ). According to Nisbett, everyone has
a biologically determined and physiologically defended weight set-point that ulti-
mately guides eating behavior. Regardless of current weight status, obese and non-
obese individuals may at times be below their actual set-point, causing them to be
in a state of deprivation that promotes externally guided eating. When individuals
are at their set-point, deprivation ceases and externally guided eating diminishes.
Set-point theories suggest that biology is a key predictor of obesity, but they
are impossible to prove or disprove and cannot account for a variety of eating phe-
nomenon, ultimately limiting their utility (Pinel, Assanand, and Lehman 2000 ).
Current research is examining how underlying differences in neurobiology
guide eating behavior. For example, there are promising results in neuroimaging
research on impulsivity, craving, binge eating, and restraint (Lowe et al. 2009 ). Such
work may help explain confl icting results found in behavioral research and may
provide necessary insight into the transactional relationship between internal and
external variables.
The Environment
In addition to individual variables that contribute to overeating and the development
of obesity, psychologists also focus on environmental causes, including familial and
social infl uences.
Because obese children are likely to become obese adults (Epstein, Wing, and
Valoski 1985 ), factors associated with the development of pediatric obesity, includ-
ing the family environment, are the focus of much research. Parents affect the eating
of their children both directly and indirectly. Parental infl uence, whether by model-
ing eating behavior, directly controlling their children’s eating behavior, or provid-
ing feedback regarding the child’s weight and shape, has been shown to foster
restrained and disinhibited eating in children (e.g., Cutting et al. 1999 ; Pike and
Rodin 1991 ; Carper, Fisher, and Birch 2000 ; Francis and Birch 2005 ). A restrained
eating style, as mentioned above, promotes vulnerability to disinhibition and over-
eating, increasing the likelihood that obesity will result.
Preliminary evidence suggests that chronic levels of psychological stress in the
family are related to obesity in children (Koch, Sepa, and Ludvigsson 2008 ).
Psychological stress encountered in childhood may lead to the development of an
emotional eating style. A recent study conducted by Greenfi eld and Marks ( 2009 )
found that individuals who reported experiencing physical and psychological vio-
lence from their parents when they were children had a greater risk for adult obesity
than those who had not experienced such violence. Moreover, the association
between a past history of physical and psychological violence and obesity was par-
tially mediated by the use of food as a coping response to stress. Family stress levels,
like parental infl uences over eating, appear to contribute to obesity by fostering
individual behaviors that are associated with overeating.
In addition to familial infl uences on overeating and obesity, researchers have
also looked at other social infl uences. A recent study by Christakis and Fowler ( 2007 )
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9006-Cawley-Ch06.indd 90 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
the psychology of obesity 91
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1investigated the “social spread” of obesity; that is, they examined whether weight
gain in one individual was correlated with weight gain in members of their social
networks. Their results suggest that obesity can spread through social ties. While
the fi ndings are not able to speak to mechanisms through which this may occur, the
authors suggest that social infl uences may promote obesity by changing norms for
acceptability of being overweight and also by directly infl uencing individual behav-
iors such as eating. A study exploring social infl uences of obesity in adolescents
found similar results as overweight adolescents were more likely to have overweight
friends than their normal weight peers (Valente et al. 2009 ). Recent work, however,
has critiqued the methodology used to investigate social network effects and has
argued that the studies should be interpreted with caution (Cohen-Cole and
Fletcher 2008 ).
The past 50 years of research on the psychological causes of obesity have helped
to isolate many important predictive factors and to clarify that obesity results from
a variety of intra- and inter-individual factors. We hope that these factors (and
other newly isolated factors) can ultimately be combined into an integrated model
that can predict who will become obese and why.
Correlates or Consequences of Obesity
Because research on the causes and consequences of obesity is correlational by
nature, any variable that is found to be associated with obesity can technically only
be considered a correlate, as opposed to a predictor or a consequence. However, the
literature does divide the correlates of obesity into those that have been shown to
precede obesity in time and are thought to be causes, and those that follow obesity
and are thus considered to be consequences. In this section, we focus on factors that
may be consequences of obesity, or perhaps only correlates, but that we are reason-
ably certain are not causes.
Stigma and Discrimination
Overweight and obese individuals face stigmatization and unfair treatment simply
because of their weight. It is often assumed that denigration and derision of over-
weight individuals can only serve to help motivate weight loss, justifying prejudice
and discrimination as “for their own good” (Brownell 2005 ). In actuality, perceived
weight discrimination and stigmatization likely have negative effects on an indi-
viduals’ psychological well-being (Hatzenbuehler, Keyes, and Hasin 2009 ) and may
contribute to the physical health problems (Maclean et al. 2009 ) that are usually
directly attributed to obesity.
Implicit bias against overweight is particularly strong (Teachman et al. 2003 ),
equivalent if not greater than bias due to age, race, and gender (Nosek, Banaji,
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9106-Cawley-Ch06.indd 91 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
92 disciplinary perspectives on obesity
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1and Greenwald 2002 ). Moreover, Puhl, Andreyeva, and Brownell ( 2008 ) demon-
strated that approximately 5 percent of overweight (BMI between 25 and 30) men
and 10 percent of overweight women experienced weight discrimination on a
daily basis. For individuals with a BMI greater than 35, these numbers jumped to
28 percent and 45 percent for men and women, respectively. Among this segment
of society, daily interpersonal discrimination through decreased civility and
increased harassment and rejection is the agonizing norm.
Physicians and other health care professionals routinely endorse negative ste-
reotypes about their overweight and obese patients. In a recent survey involving
primary care physicians, more than half of the physicians in the sample believed
that their obese patients were awkward, unattractive, and non-compliant, and a
third believed that they were weak-willed, lazy, and sloppy (Foster et al. 2003 ).
Given the sensitive nature of explicitly admitting such beliefs, this number is
undoubtedly an underestimation. Similar stigmatizing attitudes are also expressed
by the next generation of doctors, medical students (Wear et al. 2006 ). Particularly
concerning is how this biased perception translates into diminished care for
overweight patients. Nearly 70 percent of overweight and obese women reported
weight stigmatization from their physician (e.g., inappropriate comments about
their weight), and 46 percent reported similar experiences from nurses (Puhl
and Brownell 2006 ). Given how likely it is for obese patients to view the medical
setting as stigmatizing or threatening, it is not surprising that many patients avoid
or delay treatment (Drury and Louis 2002 ). This under-utilization of health care
services may lead to some of the physical health problems that are attributed
to obesity.
Overweight individuals also face noticeable discrimination in employment set-
tings, where they tend to be stereotyped as less conscientious and agreeable than
non-overweight employees (Polinko and Popovich 2001 ). A meta-analytic review
of weight discrimination in employment concluded that overweight applicants
were at a particular disadvantage when applying for jobs with considerable contact
with the public, and that they were rated less favorably when being evaluated as a
potential coworker (Roehling et al. 2008 ). In a survey of over 2,000 overweight
employees, 54 percent reported experiences of weight stigma by their fellow employ-
ees, and 43 percent reported such experiences by their employer (Puhl and Brownell
2006 ). This type of workplace discrimination ranges from inappropriate comments
and abusive joking to denied promotions and termination (Puhl and Heuer 2009 ).
Weight bias can also have a negative effect on romantic relationships (Chen
and Brown 2005 ), customer service interactions (King et al. 2006 ), and the educa-
tional environment (e.g., Puhl and Latner 2007 ). Given the unmistakably harmful
impact that weight bias has on the lives of millions of overweight and obese indi-
viduals, and the lack of success in reducing obesity itself, social and behavioral
scientists should consider it a priority to develop and test effective strategies
for eliminating this pervasive bias. Attempts to reduce weight bias have involved
providing information about the etiology of obesity or trying to create empathy
for the daily struggles of obese people, but these efforts have had mixed results
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9206-Cawley-Ch06.indd 92 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
the psychology of obesity 93
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1(e.g., Teachman et al. 2003 ) . I t m a y b e t h e c a s e t h a t u s i n g m u l t i p l e s t i g m a - r e d u c t i o n
approaches in concert will be necessary to combat such a strong bias.
Mental Health Consequences
Given the myriad ways in which obese people are discriminated against and stigma-
tized in our society, it would not be surprising if mental health problems resulted.
And because it was thought that obesity was caused by psychopathology, many
studies explored whether obesity and a variety of mental disorders co-occurred.
Thus resulted several decades of cross-sectional studies that were similar in that
they included a measure of obesity and a measure of at least one emotional prob-
lem, but that varied according to how they defi ned obesity, and how they defi ned
and measured emotional problems (Friedman and Brownell 1995 ). In that genera-
tion of studies, individuals usually self-reported their weights, and trained clini-
cians almost never used diagnostic criteria to assess mental disorders, which were
often measured with a single self-reported item. Findings from these studies rarely
found evidence for the relationship between obesity and psychopathology, but
because of the overall inconsistency of the results, it was possible to use them to
support whatever conclusion one desired. The majority of reports concluded that
obesity was not related to mental illness (see Stunkard and Wadden 1992 for a
review), but some suggested that obesity was associated with increases in the preva-
lence of mental illness, while still others argued, in what became known (rather
condescendingly, in our view) as the “jolly fat hypothesis,” that obesity was actually
related to improvements in emotional health (Crisp and McGuinness 1976 ).
In more recent years, a number of studies (Hasler et al. 2004 ; Mather et al.
2009 ; Onyike et al. 2003 ; Pickering et al. 2007 ; Scott et al. 2007 ; Simon et al. 2006 )
have addressed this question with carefully attained nationally representative sam-
ples and clinical measurement of mental illness. Like the prior generation of
research, the results of these studies were inconsistent, with some fi nding evidence
for associations between obesity and increased risk for depression or anxiety,
and others nding no signifi cant relationships (or in one case, the opposite: that
obesity was associated with a lower risk of anxiety; Hasler et al. 2004 ). Even when
signifi cant associations are found, they tend to be small effects, with odds ratios
around 1.3.
The most rigorous tests of the link between obesity and mental illness come
from prospective longitudinal studies, and these have focused primarily on depres-
sion (Bjerkeset et al. 2008 ; Herva et al. 2006 ; Roberts et al. 2000 , 2003 ). These stud-
ies measured BMI at baseline and then depression from one to 17 years later. All
three found a signifi cant relationship between baseline obesity and increased
depression at follow-up, although in the study with the 17-year follow-up, the rela-
tionship was found for men, but not women. Importantly, the one prospective
study that controlled for baseline depression still found a signifi cant relationship
between obesity and later depression (Roberts et al. 2003 ). In addition, that study
also tested the reverse path, and found that baseline depression did not predict later
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9306-Cawley-Ch06.indd 93 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
94 disciplinary perspectives on obesity
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1obesity (controlling for baseline obesity), suggesting, in what appears to be the most
rigorous test to date, that the causal direction of the relationship may be obesity
preceding depression rather than the reverse.
Considering the fi ndings from the nationally representative cross-sectional
studies and the prospective longitudinal studies, it seems that a reasonable conclu-
sion is that obesity does seem associated with later depression, but that this rela-
tionship is not particularly strong. Only one prospective study looked at anxiety,
and it did not fi nd evidence for a link between obesity and anxiety (Bjerkeset et al.
2008 ). In general, the evidence for a link between obesity and anxiety seems weaker
and more tenuous than that between obesity and depression.
What does seem clear, however, is that regardless of whether obesity causes
diagnosable mental disorders, it does have psychological consequences. Research
with children and adolescents generally nds that obesity is related to body dissat-
isfaction (Ricciardelli and McCabe 2001 ; Wardle and Cooke 2005 ) and low self-es-
teem (French, Story, and Perry 1995 ; Wardle and Cooke 2005 ). When individuals of
all ages and genders are studied, it appears that the relationship between obesity
and low self-esteem is stronger in women than in men, gets stronger from child-
hood through college age, and then starts to weaken (but remains signifi cant)
among adults (Miller and Downey 1999 ).
The relationship between obesity and self-esteem may be mediated by the stig-
matizing behaviors of others, such as, among children, weight-related teasing
(Davison and Birch 2002 ; Thompson et al. 1995 ). It may also be mediated by indi-
viduals’ own internalization of stigma, such as believing their obesity is entirely
under their control (Pierce and Wardle 1997 ), or by the emotional toll of chronic
and repeated diet failures (Miller and Downey 1999 ).
Physical Health Consequences
It has long been thought that obesity leads to physical health consequences, and the
particular ailments attributed to obesity have included cardiovascular disease,
strokes, hypertension, many cancers, diabetes, gallstones, chronic renal failure, fatty
liver disease, gout, osteoarthritis, migraines, dementia, carpal tunnel syndrome,
asthma, infertility, pregnancy complications, polycystic ovaries, erectile dysfunc-
tion, hirsutism, sleep apnea, and incontinence (NHLBI Expert Panel 1998 ). It has
not been shown, however, that obesity per se actually causes these ailments, because
the primary type of study that can show such causal links cannot be conducted for
obesity. That type of study, the randomized controlled trial, is not possible because
it requires randomly assigning individuals to either be obese or not obese, and then
watching those individuals over the next several decades to see which diseases they
contract.
Researchers must settle for the next best form of evidence, prospective longitu-
dinal studies, in which individuals who happen to be obese (or not obese) are
observed over many decades. Health differences found between obese and non-
obese people in these studies may appear to have been caused by obesity, but in fact
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9406-Cawley-Ch06.indd 94 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
the psychology of obesity 95
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1may have actually been caused by some additional factor that also varies between
obese and non-obese people. It is all too easy for people not versed in research
methods to mistakenly assume that such studies are convincing evidence of obesity’s
causal role in many diseases.
In the last 20 years, scientists (as well as activists and journalists) have begun to
explore a variety of confounding factors that may account for the relationship
between obesity and physical health problems (Campos et al. 2005 ). This has led to
a contentious and politicized debate (described in Kolata 2007 ) that is unlikely to be
easily resolved. The confounding factors that may play an important role in this
relationship are obese people’s greater likelihood (than non-obese people) of lead-
ing sedentary lifestyles, repeatedly gaining and losing weight (known as “weight
cycling”), avoiding health care, and being in lower socioeconomic groups (Campos
2004 ; Ernsberger and Koletsky 1999 ). These factors have been shown to be associ-
ated with poor health and, to the extent that they are more likely to occur among
obese people than non-obese people, may account for the relationship between
obesity and disease.
Another factor that muddies the evidence for obesity as a cause of health prob-
lems is data suggesting that obesity does not shorten an individual’s lifespan, or at
least not until extremely high levels of obesity are reached (Flegal et al. 2005 ). It is
diffi cult (although not impossible) to convincingly argue that obesity causes such a
long list of ailments but somehow does not shorten one’s life. The evidence is also
called into question because studies supporting the relationship between obesity
and health outcomes tend to be conducted by researchers with signifi cant confl icts
of interest (Fraser 1998 ).
As researchers with no vested interests on either side of this debate (as we study
psychological outcomes of diets and obesity rather than physical ones), we have
observed this debate from the outside. It is our conclusion that the links between
obesity and some diseases are quite convincing, such as that between obesity and
type 2 diabetes (Colditz et al. 1995 ; Ford, Williamson, and Liu 1997 ) and osteoarthri-
tis (Hochberg et al. 1995). However, it is equally clear to us that the strength of the
links between obesity and many other ailments has been overstated (Olshansky
et al. 2005 ). Even if these relationships cannot be entirely explained away by con-
founding variables, they do not seem strong enough to warrant the amount of alarm
they receive from the media, scientists, and the government.
Treatment of Obesity
Despite imperfect models regarding its etiology, strong beliefs in the physical health
consequences of obesity have led researchers to develop treatment interventions.
Most of these interventions have been diets designed to promote weight loss.
More recent efforts, however, have included interventions aimed at treating the
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9506-Cawley-Ch06.indd 95 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
96 disciplinary perspectives on obesity
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1self-esteem and body dissatisfaction that result from obesity, without necessarily
focusing on weight loss.
Treating the Physical Health Consequences of Obesity
Given the fi eld of psychology’s long history of research on behavior change, it is not
surprising that its approach to the treatment of obesity is to try to alter people’s
eating habits by teaching them diet strategies. Over the past 60 years, psychologists
have studied dozens of behavioral weight loss techniques to see which are effective
(Leon 1976 ; Ayyad and Andersen 2000 ). They have taught dieters how to select
appropriate foods to eat, count calories, resist temptation, monitor how much food
they eat, and to reward (or punish) themselves for eating the right (or wrong)
amount of food (see Leon 1976 for a review). In some treatments they gave the diet-
ers all the food that they were allowed to eat, and in some cases the dieters were
required to do all their eating in a researcher’s laboratory or clinic (e.g., Wing and
Jeffrey 2001 ; Musante 1976 ). Psychologists have also explored the effectiveness of
having dieters give some of their own money to researchers and then trying to earn
it back by losing certain amounts of weight (e.g., Harris and Bruner 1971 ). They also
tested whether using social pressure could get obese people to reduce their con-
sumption (e.g., Wollersheim 1970 ). Finally, in efforts to create negative associations
with desired foods, researchers have paired dieters’ favorite foods with horrible
odors, and in other cases have actually jolted obese patients with electric shock
whenever they tried to eat a food they craved (e.g., Foreyt and Kennedy 1971 ; Meyer
and Crisp 1964 ).
As silly (or cruel) as some of these treatments may sound, by and large, what-
ever psychologists attempted generally led to at least some weight loss during the
early months of the diet (Jeffery et al. 2000 ; Perri and Fuller 1995 ). Across over 200
diet studies conducted from 1966 up until 2000, participants lost from 8 to 22
pounds in as many weeks about a pound a week (Wing 2002 ). For a diet to truly
be considered successful, however, individuals must not just lose weight in the short
term, but they also must keep it off. The majority of diet studies do not follow par-
ticipants long enough to see if that happened. In fact, only 6 percent of over 800 diet
studies found by researchers in the year 2000 had follow-ups of three years or more,
and two-thirds of those had such serious fl aws that they offered little useful infor-
mation (Ayyad and Andersen 2000 ).
Across 14 studies with long-term follow-ups, participants initially lost an aver-
age of 30 pounds, but by four or ve years later, they had gained back all but 7 of
those pounds (Mann et al. 2007 ). These results likely infl ate the success of these
diets because the studies have multiple sources of systematic bias all of which
make the diets look more successful than they were. For example, most of the people
who start these diet studies do not nish them, and the people who do nish are the
ones who do the best on the diets. In addition, many of the people in these studies
are not actually weighed by the researchers, but rather just tell the researchers their
weight over the phone or by mail. Since most people lie about their weight and
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9606-Cawley-Ch06.indd 96 1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM1/25/2011 3:49:31 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
the psychology of obesity 97
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1say they weigh less than they actually do this fl aw causes the diets to look like they
led to more weight loss than they really did. Another source of systematic bias in
these studies is that many of the participants went on additional diets during the
long follow-up period. This makes it appear as if the original diet had led to sus-
tained weight loss, when, in fact, these dieters likely gained back a lot if not all of
the weight before they started a new diet.
If the goal of obesity treatment is to have obese people lose enough weight to
make them non-obese, and to maintain that weight loss in the long term, then it
seems clear to us that behavioral treatments for obesity are unsuccessful. However,
it is the association between obesity and physical health problems that makes effec-
tive obesity treatment a high priority in our country’s research agenda. Given that,
we suggest that it doesn’t make sense to use weight loss an indirect and imperfect
measure of health as a marker of successful obesity treatment when we could
be measuring improvements in physical health more directly (Ernsberger and
Koletsky 1999 ). The next generation of obesity treatment research should focus on
physical health outcomes such as heart rate, blood pressure, and cholesterol levels,
rather than weight change. Indeed, research on exercise interventions show positive
health benefi ts on all of these outcomes, even in the absence of weight loss (Caudwell
et al. 2009 ).
Treating the Mental Health Consequences of Obesity
Early psychological interventions focused on altering eating behavior to the neglect
of mental health sequelae. It has even been argued that some mental health conse-
quences of obesity, especially body distress, are important motivators for behavior
change and that addressing them would be detrimental to weight-loss efforts.
While there has been some support for this, the negative social and psychological
effects of body distress and obesity are important areas of concern (Heinberg,
Thompson, and Matzon 2001 ; Schwartz and Brownell 2004 ; Wilson 1996 ). Body
image and self-acceptance themselves became primary aims of treatment due to the
combination of these negative consequences and ineffective long-term weight loss
treatments (Wilson 1996 ; Schwartz and Brownell 2004 ; Rosen et al. 1995 ).
It has been assumed that weight loss is a necessary prerequisite for improve-
ment in body image. However, research fi ndings have produced mixed results (see
Schwartz and Brownell 2004 ). A randomized control trial comparing a cognitive
behavioral body image therapy (CBT) or a no-treatment condition found that
although weight status remained unchanged, individuals who received CBT showed
signifi cant improvement in body image as well as improvement in psychological
symptoms, self-esteem, overeating, and eating guilt (Rosen et al. 1995 ). Another
study found that the promotion of body and self-acceptance in a non-diet wellness
intervention improved not only body image, but also physical health parameters,
without changes in weight (Bacon et al. 2002 ). These positive psychological and health
outcomes were sustained at a two-year follow-up, whereas initial improvements in
the diet group were generally not maintained (Bacon et al. 2005 ). In contrast,
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9706-Cawley-Ch06.indd 97 1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
98 disciplinary perspectives on obesity
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1a study comparing a combined body image treatment and weight control program
to a weight control program alone did not fi nd any additional improvement in
body image (Ramirez and Rosen 2001 ).
In addition to being targets of treatment, body image and weight acceptance
may also promote successful weight maintenance after weight loss occurs. A cogni-
tive behavioral therapy intervention for obesity created by Cooper and Fairburn
( 2001 ) encourages a shift from weight loss to acceptance of weight stability follow-
ing a pre-set period of time. Such promotion of weight- and self-acceptance is
thought to prevent psychological barriers, including focusing on unattainable
weight loss, from interfering with weight maintenance. In support of this, satisfac-
tion with body weight has been associated with improved maintenance of weight
loss, but fi ndings have been inconsistent and more research is needed (Foster et al.
2004 ; Byrne, Cooper, and Fairburn 2004 ; Ames et al. 2005 ; Gorin et al. 2007 ).
Targeting body image dissatisfaction and self-acceptance either in isolation or
in conjunction with behavior change therapy aimed at weight loss appears to be a
promising component of treatment. While continued research is needed to explore
the effects of these treatments, it is clear that the mental health consequences of
obesity should not take a back seat to treatments aimed solely at the physical health
consequences.
Conclusion
In 1999, psychologists, nutritionists, and other experts proposed a paradigm shift in
research and policy on dieting and obesity (Cogan and Ernsberger. 1999 ). They sug-
gested a health-centered, rather than a weight-centered approach to obesity. This
approach would encourage obesity researchers to design their interventions with
the goal of promoting tness rather than weight loss, and to use health markers as
their outcome variables, rather than relying on weight as the primary and critical
measure of success. Work consistent with this approach is accumulating, and
although the proposed shift has not occurred, we believe that it would be a positive
direction for the eld. It seems to us that even though interventions based on this
new paradigm may not reduce the prevalence of obesity, they are likely to improve
the nation’s health, and by helping to unlink the association between obesity and
illness, may even reduce the stigma of obesity.
REFERENCES
Ames , G. E. , M. G. Perri , L. D. Fox , E. A. Fallon , N. De Braganza , M. E. Murawski , et al .
2005 . “ Changing Weight-Loss Expectations: A Randomized Pilot Study .” Eating
Behaviors 6 : 259 269 .
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9806-Cawley-Ch06.indd 98 1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
the psychology of obesity 99
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 Ayyad , C. , and T. Andersen . 2000 . “ Long-term Effi cacy of Dietary Treatment of Obesity: A
Systematic Review of Studies Published between 1931 and 1999 .” Obesity Reviews
1 : 113 119 .
Bacon , L. , N. L. Keim , M. D. Van Loan , M. Derricote , B. Gale , A. Kazaks , and J. S. Stern.
2002 . “ Evaluating a ‘Non-Diet’ Wellness Intervention for Improvement of Metabolic
Fitness, Psychological Well-Being and Eating and Activity Behaviors .” International
Journal of Obesity 26 : 854 865 .
Bacon , L. , J. S. Stern , M. D. Van Loan , and N. L. Keim . 2005 . “ Size Acceptance and
Intuitive Eating Improve Health for Obese, Female Chronic Dieters .” Journal of the
American Dietetic Association 105 : 929 936 .
Bjerkeset , O. , P. Romundstad , J. Evans , and D. Gunnell . 2008 . “ Association of Adult
Body Mass Index and Height with Anxiety, Depression, and Suicide in the General
Population .” American Journal of Epidemiology 167 : 193 202 .
Bruch , H . 1961 . “ The Transformation of Oral Impulses in Eating Disorders: A Conceptual
Approach .” Psychiatric Quarterly 35 : 458 481 .
Byrne , S. M. , Z. Cooper , and C. G. Fairburn . 2004 . “ Psychological Predictors of Weight
Regain in Obesity .” Behaviour Research and Therapy 42 : 1341 1356 .
Brownell , Kelly. 2005 . “ The Social, Scientifi c, and Human Context of Prejudice and
Discrimination Based on Weight .” In Weight bias: Nature, Consequences, and
Remedies , ed. Kelly Brownell , Rebecca Puhl , Marlene Schwartz , and Leslie Rudd , 1 11 .
New York : Guilford Press .
Campos , P . 2004 . The Obesity Myth . New York : Gotham .
Campos , P. , A. Saguy , P. Ernsberger , E. Oliver , and G. Gaesser.
2005 . The Epidemiology
of Overweight and Obesity: Public Health Crisis or Moral Panic?” International
Journal of Epidemiology 35 : 55 60 .
Canetti , L. , E. Bachar , and E. M. Berry. 2002 . “ Food and Emotion .” Behavioural Processes
60 : 157 164 .
Carper , J. L. , J. O. Fisher , and L. L. Birch. 2000 . “ Young Girls’ Emerging Dietary Restraint
and Disinhibtion Are Related to Parental Control in Child Feeding .” Appetite
35 : 121 129 .
Caudwell , P. , M. Hopkins , N. A. King , R. J. Stubbs , and J. E. Blundell. 2009 . “ Exercise
Alone Is Not Enough: Weight Loss Also Needs a Healthy (Mediterranean) Diet?”
Public Health Nutrition 12 : 1663 1666 .
Chen , E., and M. Brown . 2005 . Obesity Stigma in Sexual Relationships .” Obesity Research
13 : 1393 1397 .
Christakis , N. A., and J. H. Fowler . 2007 . “ The Spread of Obesity in a Large Social Network
over 32 Years .” The New England Journal of Medicine 357 : 370 379 .
Cogan , J. C. , and P. Ernsberger . 1999 . “ Dieting, Weight, and Health: Reconceptualizing
Research and Policy .” Journal of Social Issues 55 : 187 205 .
Cohen-Cole , E. , and J. M. Fletcher . 2008 . Detecting Implausible Social Network Effects in
Acne, Height, and Headaches: Longitudinal Analysis .” British Medical Journal
337 : a2533 .
Colditz G. A. , W. C. Willett , A. Rotnitzky , and J. E. Manson. 1995 . Weight Gain as a Risk
Factor for Clinical Diabetes Mellitus in Women .”
Annals of Internal Medicine
122 : 481 486 .
Cooper , Z., and C. G. Fairburn . 2001 . “ A New Cognitive Behavioural Approach to the
Treatment of Obesity .” Behaviour Research and Therapy 39 : 499 511 .
Crisp , A. H., and B. McGuinness . 1976 . Jolly Fat: Relation between Obesity and
Psychoneurosis in the General Population .” British Medical Journal 1 : 7 9 .
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 9906-Cawley-Ch06.indd 99 1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
100 disciplinary perspectives on obesity
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 Cutting , T.M. , J.O Fisher , K. Grimm-Thomas , and L.L. Birch . 1999 . Like Mother, Like
Daughter: Familial Patterns of Overweight Are Mediated by Mothers’ Dietary
Disinhibtion .” American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 69 : 608 613 .
Davison , K. K. , and L. L. Birch . 2002 . “ Processes Linking Weight Status and Self-Concept
among Girls from Ages 5 To 7 Years .” Developmental Psychology 38 : 735 748 .
Drury , C., and M. Louis , M . 2002 . Exploring the Association between Body Weight,
Stigma of Obesity, and Health Care Avoidance .” Journal of American Academy of
Nurse Practitioners 14 : 554 560 .
Epstein , L. H. , R. R. Wing , and A. Valoski. 1985 . Childhood Obesity .” Pediatric Clinics of
North America 32 ( 2 ): 363 379 .
Ernsberger , P. , and R. J. Koletsky . 1999 . “ Biomedical Rationale for a Wellness Approach
to Obesity: An Alternative to a Focus on Weight Loss .” Journal of Social Issues
55 : 221 259 .
Flegal , K. , B. I. Graubard , D. F. Williamson , and M. H. Gail . 2005 . “ Excess Deaths
Associated with Underweight, Overweight, and Obesity .” Journal of the American
Medical Association 293 : 1861 1867 .
Ford , E. S. , D. F. Williamson , and S. Liu. 1997 . Weight Change and Diabetes Incidence:
Findings from a National Cohort of US Adults .” American Journal of Epidemiology
146 : 214 222 .
Foreyt , J. P., and W. A. Kennedy . 1971 . “ Treatment of Overweight by Aversion Therapy .”
Behaviour Research and Therapy , 9 : 29 34 .
Foster , G. D. , S. Phelan , T. A. Wadden , D. Gill , J. Ermold , and E. Didie . 2004 . “ Promoting
More Modest Weight Losses: A Pilot Study .” Obesity 12 : 1271 1277 .
Foster , G. D. , T. A. Wadden , A. P. Makris , D. Davidson , R. S. Sanderson , D. B. Allison ,
and A. Kessler. 2003 . “ Primary Care Physicians’ Attitudes about Obesity and Its
Treatment .” Obesity Research 11 : 1168 1177 .
Francis , L. A., and L. L. Birch . 2005 . “ Maternal Infl uences on Daughters’ Restrained Eating
Behavior .” Health Psychology 24 : 548 554 .
Fraser , L . 1998 . Losing It: False Hopes and Fat Profi ts in the Diet Industry . New York : Plume .
French , S. A. , M. Story , and C. L. Perry. 1995 . Self-esteem and Obesity in Children and
Adolescents: A Literature Review .” Obesity Research 3 : 479 490 .
Friedman , M. A. , and K. D. Brownell . 1995 . “ Psychological Correlates of Obesity: Moving
to the Next Research Generation .” Psychological Bulletin 117 : 3 20 .
Gorin , A. A. , A. Marinilli Pinto , D. F. Tate , H. A. Raynor , J. L. Fava , and R. R. Wing .
2007 . “ Failure to Meet Weight Loss Expectations Does Not Impact Maintenance in
Successful Weight Losers .” Obesity 15 : 3086 3090 .
Greeno , C. G., and R. R. Wing . 1994 . “ Stress-induced Eating .” Psychological Bulletin
115 : 444 464 .
Harris , M. B., and C. G. Bruner . 1971
. “ A Comparison of a Self-Control and a Contract
Procedure for Weight Control .” Behavior Research and Therapy 9 : 347 354 .
Hasler , G. , D. S. Pine , A. Gamma , G. Milos , V. Ajdacic , D. Eich , W. Rossler , and J. Angst .
2004 . “ The Associations between Psychopathology and Being Overweight: A 20-Year
Prospective Study .” Psychological Medicine 34 : 1047 1057 .
Hatzenbuehler , M. L. , K. M. Keyes , and D. S. Hasin. 2009 . Associations between
Perceived Weight Discrimination and the Prevalence of Psychiatric Disorders in the
General Population .” Obesity 17 : 2033 2039 .
Heatherton , T. F. , C. P. Herman , and J. Polivy. 1991 . Effects of Physical Threat and
Ego Threat on Eating Behavior .” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
60 : 138 143 .
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 10006-Cawley-Ch06.indd 100 1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
the psychology of obesity 101
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 Heinberg , L. J. , J. K. Thompson , and J. L. Matzon. 2001 . “ Body Image Dissatisfaction as
a Motivator for Healthy Lifestyle Change: Is Some Distress Benefi cial?” In Eating
Disorders: Innovative Directions in Research and Practice , eds. R. H. Striegel-Moore and
L. Smolak. Washington, DC : American Psychological Association .
Herman , C. P., and D. Mack . 1975 . Restrained and Unrestrained Eating .” Journal of
Personality 43 ( 4 ): 647 660 .
Herman , C. P., and J. Polivy . 1975 . “ Anxiety, Restraint, and Eating Behavior .” Journal of
Abnormal Psychology 84 : 666 672 .
Herman , C. P., and J. Polivy . 2008 . External Cues in the Control of Food Intake in
Humans: The Sensory-Normative Distinction .” Physiology and Behavior 94 : 722 728 .
Herva , A. , J. Laitinen , J. Miettunen , J. Veijola , J. T. Karvonen , K. Lasky , et al . 2006 .
Obesity and Depression: Results from the Longitudinal Northern Finland 1966 Birth
Cohort Study .” International Journal of Obesity (London) 30 : 520 527 .
Hochberg , M. C. , M. Lethbridge-Cejku , W. W. Scott , R. Reichle , C. C. Plato , and J.
D. Tobin . 1995 . “ The Association of Body Weight, Body Fatness and Body Fat
Distribution with Osteoarthritis of the Knee: Data from the Baltimore Longitudinal
Study of Aging .” Journal of Rheumatology 22 : 488 493 .
Greenfi eld , E. A. , and N. F. Marks . 2009 . “ Violence from Parents in Childhood and
Obesity in Adulthood: Using Food in Response to Stress as a Mediator of Risk .” Social
Science and Medicine 68 : 791 798 .
Jeffery , R. W. , A. Drewnowski , L. H. Epstein , A. J. Stunkard , G. T. Wilson , R. R. Wing ,
et al . 2000 . “ Long-term Maintenance of Weight Loss: Current Status .” Heath
Psychology 19 (Supp.):
5 6 .
Kaplan , H. I., and H. S. Kaplan . 1957 . “ The Psychosomatic Concept of Obesity .” Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease 125 : 181 201 .
King , E. B. , J. R. Shapiro , M. R. Hebl , S. L. Singletary , and S. Turner. 2006 . “ The Stigma
of Obesity in Customer Service: A Mechanism for Remediation and Bottom-Line
Consequences of Interpersonal Discrimination .” Journal of Applied Psychology 91 :
579 593 .
Koch , F.S. , A. Sepa , and J. Ludvigsson. 2008 . “ Psychological Stress and Obesity .” The
Journal of Pediatrics 153 : 839 844 .
Kolata , Gina. 2007 . Rethinking Thin . New York : Farrar Straus and Giroux .
Leon , Gloria R . 1976 . “ Current Directions in the Treatment of Obesity .” Psychological
Bulletin 83 : 557 578 .
Lowe , M. R. , J. van Steenburgh , C. Ochner ,, and M. Coletta . 2009 . “ Neural Correlates of
Individual Differences Related to Appetite .” Physiology and Behavior 97 : 561 571 .
Macht , M., and J. Mueller . 2007 . “ Immediate Effects of Chocolate on Experimentally
Induced Mood States .” Appetite 49 : 667 674 .
Maclean , L. , N. Edwards , M. Garrard , N. Sims-Jones , K. Clinton , and L. Ashley . 2009 .
Obesity, Stigma and Public Health Planning .” Health Promotion International
24 : 88 93 .
Mann , T. , A. J. Tomiyama , E. Westling , A. Lew , B. Samuels , and J. Chatman . 2007 .
Medicare’s Search for Effective Obesity Treatments: Diets Are Not the Answer .”
American Psychologist 62 : 220 233 .
Mann , T., and A. Ward . 2000 . Don’t Mind If I Do: Disinhibited Eating under Cognitive
Load .” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78 ( 4 ): 753 763 .
Mather , A. A. , B. J. Cox , W. E. Murray , and J. Sareen . 2009 . “ Associations of Obesity with
Psychiatric Disorders and Suicidal Behaviors in a Nationally Representative Sample .”
Journal of Psychosomatic Research 66 : 277 285 .
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 10106-Cawley-Ch06.indd 101 1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
102 disciplinary perspectives on obesity
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 Meyer , V., and A. H. Crisp . 1964 . “ Aversion Therapy in Two Cases of Obesity .” Behaviour
Research and Therapy 2 : 143 147 .
Miller , C. T. , and K. T. Downey . 1999 . “ A Meta-Analysis of Heavyweight and Self-Esteem .”
Personality and Social Psychology Review 3 : 68 84 .
Musante , G. J . 1976 . “ The Dietary Rehabilitation Clinic: Evaluative Report of a Behavioral
and Dietary Treatment of Obesity .” Behavior Therapy 7 : 198 204 .
NHLBI Expert Panel . 1998 . Clinical Guidelines on the Identifi cation, Evaluation, and
Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults: The Evidence Report . NIH Publication
Number 98 4083 .
Nisbett , R. E . 1972 . “ Hunger, Obesity, and the Ventromedial Hypothalamus .” Psychological
Review 79 ( 6 ): 433 453 .
Nosek , B. A. , Banaji , M. , and A. G. Greenwald . 2002 . “ Harvesting Implicit Group Attitudes
and Beliefs from a Demonstration Web Site .” Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and
Practice , 6 : 101 115 .
Onyike , C. U. , R. M. Crum , H. B. Lee , C. G. Lyketsos , and W. W. Eaton. 2003 . “ Is Obesity
Associated with Major Depression? Results from the Third National Health and
Nutrition Examination Survey .” American Journal of Epidemiology 158 : 1139 1147 .
Olshansky , S. J. , D. J. Passaro , R. C. Hershow , et al . 2005 . A Potential Decline in Life
Expectancy in the United States in the 21st Century .” New England Journal of Medicine
352 : 1138 1145 .
Perri , M. G. , and P. R. Fuller . 1995
. “ Success and Failure in the Treatment Of Obesity:
Where Do We Go from Here? Medicine, Exercise, Nutrition, and Health
4 : 255 272 .
Pickering , R. P. , B. F. Grant , S. P. Chou , et al . 2007 . Are Overweight, Obesity, and
Extreme Obesity Associated with Psychopathology? Results from the National
Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions .” Journal of Clinical
Psychology 68 : 998 1009 .
Pierce , J. W. , and J. Wardle . 1997 . “ Cause and Effect Beliefs and Self-Esteem of Overweight
Children .” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38 : 645 650 .
Pike , K. M., and J. Rodin . 1991 . “ Mothers, Daughters, and Disordered Eating .” Journal of
Abnormal Psychology 100 : 198 204 .
Pinel , J. P. J. , S. Assanand , and D. R. Lehman. 2000 . Hunger, Eating and Ill Health .”
American Psychologist 55 ( 10 ): 1105 1116 .
Polinko , N. K., and P. M. Popovich . 2001 . “ Evil Thoughts but Angelic Actions: Responses
to Overweight Job Applicants .” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 31 : 905 924 .
Polivy , J., and C. P. Herman . 1985 . Dieting and Binging: A Causal Analysis .” American
Psychologist 40 : 193 201 .
Polivy , J. , C. P. Herman , and T. McFarlane. ( 1994 ). “ Effects of Anxiety on Eating: Does
Palatability Moderate Distress-Induced Overeating in Dieters? Journal of Abnormal
Psychology 103 : 505 510 .
Puhl , R. M. , T. Andreyeva , and K. D.Brownell.
2008 . Perceptions of Weight
Discrimination: Prevalence and Comparison to Race and Gender Discrimination in
America .” International Journal of Obesity 32 : 992 1000 .
Puhl , R. M., and J. D. Latner . 2007 . “ Obesity, Stigma, and the Health of the Nation’s
Children .” Psychological Bulletin 133 : 557 580 .
Puhl , R. M., and K. D. Brownell . 2006 . “ Confronting and Coping with Weight Stigma: An
Investigation of Overweight and Obese Individuals .” Obesity 14 : 1802 1815 .
Puhl , R. M., and C. A. Heuer . 2009 . The Stigma of Obesity: A Review and Update .”
Obesity 17 : 941 964 .
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 10206-Cawley-Ch06.indd 102 1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
the psychology of obesity 103
49
48
47
46
45
44
43
42
41
40
39
38
37
36
35
34
33
32
31
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
17
16
15
14
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 Ramirez , E. M., and J. C. Rosen . 2001 . “ A Comparison of Weight Control and Weight
Control Plus Body Image Therapy for Obese Men and Women .” Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology 69 : 440 446 .
Ricciardelli , L. A. , and M. P. McCabe . 2001 . “ Children’s Body Image Concerns and Eating
Disturbance: A Review of the Literature .” Clinical Psychology Review 21 : 325 344 .
Roberts , R. E. , G. A. Kaplan , S. J. Shema , and W. J. Strawbridge . 2000 . “ Are the Obese at
Greater Risk for Depression?” American Journal of Epidemiology 152 : 163 170 .
Roberts , R. E. , S. Deleger , W. J. Strawbridge , and G. A. Kaplan . 2003 . “ Prospective
Association between Obesity and Depression: Evidence from the Alameda
County Study .” International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders
27 : 514 521 .
Roehling , Mark V. , Shawn Pilcher , Fred Oswald , and Tamara A. Bruce . ( 2008 ). “ The
Effects of Weight Bias on Job-Related Outcomes: A Meta-Analysis of Experimental
Studies .” Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management in
Anaheim, CA , 2008 .
Rodin , J . 1981 . “ Current Status of the Internal-External Hypothesis for Obesity: What
Went Wrong?” American Psychologist 36 : 361 372 .
Rosen , J. C. , P. Orosan , and J. Reiter . 1995 . “ Cognitive Behavior Therapy for Negative Body
Image in Obese Women .” Behavior Therapy 26 : 25 42 .
Ruderman , A. J . 1986 . “ Dietary Restraint: A Theoretical and Empirical Review .”
Psychological Bulletin 99 : 247 262 .
Schacter , S . 1968 . “ Obesity and Eating .” Science 16 : 751 756 .
Schacter , S. , R. Goldman , and A. Gordon . 1968 . “ Effects of Fear, Food Deprivation, and
Obesity on Eating .” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 10 : 96 106 .
Schwartz , M. B., and K. D. Brownell . 2004 . “ Obesity and Body Image .” Body Image
1 : 43 56 .
Scott , K. M. , R. Bruffaerts , G. E. Simon , J. Alonso , M. Angermeyer , G. de Girolamo , et al .
2007 . “ Obesity and Mental Disorders in the General Population: Results from the
World Mental Health Surveys .” International Journal of Obesity 32 : 192 200 .
Simon , G. E. , M. Von Korff , K. Saunders , D. L. Miglioretti , P. K. Crane , G. van Belle ,
et al . 2006 . “ Association between Obesity and Psychiatric Disorders in the US Adult
Population .” Archives of General Psychiatry 63 : 824 830 .
Stunkard , A. J., and T. A. Wadden . 1992 . “ Psychological Aspects of Severe Obesity .”
American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 55 : 524S 532S .
Teachman , B. A. , K. D. Gapinski , K. D. Brownell , M. Rawlins , and S. Jeyaram. 2003 .
Demonstrations of Implicit Anti-Fat Bias: The Impact of Providing Causal
Information and Evoking Empathy .” Health Psychology 22 : 68 78 .
Thompson , J. K. , M. Coovert , K .J. Richards , S. Johnson , and J. Cattarin. 1995 .
Development of Body Image and Eating Disturbance in Young Females: Covariance
Structure Modeling and Longitudinal Analyses .” International Journal of Eating
Disorders 18 : 221 236 .
Valente , T. W. , K. Fujimoto , C. Chou , and D. Spruijt-Metz . 2009 . “ Adolescent Affi liations
and Adiposity: A Social Network Analysis of Friendships and Obesity .” Journal of
Adolescent Health 45 :
202 204 .
Van Strien , T. , and M. A. Ouwens . 2003 . “ Counterregulatio in Female Obese Eaters:
Schacter, Goldman and Gordons (1968) Test of Psychosomatic Theory Revisited .”
Eating Behaviors 3 : 329 340 .
Wardle , J. , and L. Cooke . 2005 . “ The Impact of Obesity on Psychological Well-Being .”
Best Practice and Research Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 19 : 421 440 .
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 10306-Cawley-Ch06.indd 103 1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
104 disciplinary perspectives on obesity
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1 Wear , D. , J. M. Aultman , J. D. Varley , and J. Zarconi . 2006 . “ Making Fun of Patients:
Medical Students’ Perceptions and Use of Derogatory and Cynical Humor in Clinical
Settings .” Academy of Medicine 81 : 454 462 .
Wilson , T. G . 1996 . “ Acceptance and Change in the Treatment of Eating Disorders and
Obesity . Behavior Therapy 27 : 417 439 .
Wing , Rena R . 2002 . “ Behavioral Weight Control .” In Handbook of Obesity Treatment , ed.
Thomas A. Wadden and Albert J. Stunkard , 301 316 . New York : The Guilford Press .
Wing , R. R. , and R. W. Jeffrey . 2001 . “ Food Provision as a Strategy to Promote Weight
Loss .” Obesity Research 9 : s271 s275 .
Wollersheim , J. P . 1970 . “ Effectiveness of Group Therapy Based upon Learning Principles
in the Treatment of Overweight Women .” Journal of Abnormal Psychology
76 : 462 474 .
06-Cawley-Ch06.indd 10406-Cawley-Ch06.indd 104 1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM1/25/2011 3:49:32 PM
OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF - FIRST PROOF / 01/25/2011 GLYPH
... anxiety, depression) may experience it challenging to control the intake of food, making healthy food choices and control subsequent weight control 25 . To elaborate, individuals with obesity may overeat (excessive intake of palatable food) as a means to cope with (alleviate) emotional distress 26 . The resulting alleviation from distress (negative reinforcement) enhances the likelihood that this behaviour occurs more frequently through the known process of operant conditioning. ...
... In other words, irrespective of internal signals (hunger), obese individuals are more susceptible to social and environmental stimuli that are related to (especially palatable) foods 36 . In addition, individuals with obesity are more likely to engage in comfort eating in response to negative mood 26 . This comfort eating is associated with a shift towards preference of palatable food, foods high in fats and sugars 44 . ...
Chapter
This chapter provides an overview of the main and most consistently reported psychological factors that play a role in the onset and persistence of obesity. Taking psychological factors into account is crucial, as a pure biomedical model does not explain sufficiently the sizeable individual variability of weight gain, and persistence of abnormal weight. From a biopsychosocial perspective, we focus on eating behaviour, how eating behaviour is affected by psychological factors and consequences of eating behaviour. We discuss the role of emotional and cognitive factors, mood and emotional regulation, stigma and discrimination and personality traits in relation to obesity. Studies show that individuals with obesity have a stronger sensitivity (attentional bias) for, and motivational drive ("wanting") towards foods rich in fat and sugar (palatable food) coupled with deficient impulse control in contexts of anticipated palatable food. Negative mood has been shown to be implicated in obesity and to induce compensatory (excessive) intake of palatable food. It should be noted that negative mood has a bidirectional relation with obesity; obesity has also been shown to result in negative mood. Certainly, adding to this is the fact that obesity is associated with stigma, discrimination, bullying and stereotypical media portrayals. Importantly, it should be emphasized that there is a considerable overlap of the condition of obesity with addiction, both in terms of phenomenology as well as with respect to the brain mechanism that drives maladaptive behaviour. Indeed, it has been suggested that obesity should be characterized a mental disorder. Currently, obesity is not classified as a mental disorder mainly because of the heterogeneity and uncertainty with respect to its etiology. This may be surprising as this is the case with several other included disorders, and the debate continues. At least part of the issue of current suboptimal treatment approaches is a lack of understanding of the key mechanism implicated in obesity. Hence, increased insight into the main psychological mechanisms could assist in future treatment directions.
Article
Full-text available
The objective of this study was to examine the construct validity of the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) Revised-18, an instrument designed to measure: Uncontrolled Eating, Emotional Eating and Cognitive Restraint, in a sample of Mexican adults of different weights. 342 man and women, with age range 19-79 years old (M=40), were recruited. TFEQ was emailed to the participants who earlier had their weight and height measured. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis was applied to examine TFEQ structure. The original three factor structure was corroborated, with nine items loading high on Uncontrolled Eating factor, three on Emotional Eating and four out of the original six on Cognitive Restraint. Two items were excluded due to low item-total correlations. Higher levels of Cognitive Restraint were associated with higher BMI (r=.13, p < .05), we found no connection between Uncontrolled Eating or Emotional Eating and body weight. Our findings suggest that the abbreviated TFEQ (16 items), is a psychometrically valid measure, and can be used to evaluate the tendencies of Cognitive Restraint, Uncontrolled Eating and Emotional Eating in the population of Mexican adults with different body weights.
Article
Full-text available
This study provides the first meta‐analytic testing of (1) several proposed moderators of the relationship between employee weight and job‐related outcomes (e.g., target gender, target qualifications, and rater gender); and (2) the effect of specific design features on the variation in results obtained across relevant experimental studies (e.g., weight manipulation and amount of job‐relevant information). The results indicate that there are important moderators of the relationship between people's weight and job‐related outcomes. However, design feature may significantly influence the obtained results. In particular, inconsistent experimental findings regarding the hypothesized moderating role of target sex appear to be explained by limitations in the way in which target weight was manipulated in some studies. Directions for future research are identified.
Article
Full-text available
This study is a meta-analysis of the relation of heavyweight and self-esteem. We examined this relation in studies thatfocused on participants' actual body weight (assessed by physical measures or self-reports of weight and height) and studies thatfocused on self-perceived degree of heavyweight or body dissatisfaction. The overall mean effect size was moderate (r = -.18, d = -.36), with lower self-esteem associated with heavier weight. The correlation between self-esteem and weight was higher for studies of self-perceived weight than for studies of actual weight. Consistent with predictions about cultural and group differences, effect sizes were smaller for low socioeconomic status (SES) samples, ethnic minority samples, and nonclinical samples than for high SES, nonminority, and clinical samples, respectively. In addition, effect sizes were larger for women than for men and for high school and college students than for children. Discussion centered on cultural, group, and individual differences that may influence the consequences on self-esteem of violating standards for appropriate body weight.
Article
The prevalence of obesity has increased substantially over the past 30 years. We performed a quantitative analysis of the nature and extent of the person-to-person spread of obesity as a possible factor contributing to the obesity epidemic. We evaluated a densely interconnected social network of 12,067 people assessed repeatedly from 1971 to 2003 as part of the Framingham Heart Study. The body-mass index was available for all subjects. We used longitudinal statistical models to examine whether weight gain in one person was associated with weight gain in his or her friends, siblings, spouse, and neighbors. Discernible clusters of obese persons (body-mass index [the weight in kilograms divided by the square of the height in meters], > or =30) were present in the network at all time points, and the clusters extended to three degrees of separation. These clusters did not appear to be solely attributable to the selective formation of social ties among obese persons. A person's chances of becoming obese increased by 57% (95% confidence interval [CI], 6 to 123) if he or she had a friend who became obese in a given interval. Among pairs of adult siblings, if one sibling became obese, the chance that the other would become obese increased by 40% (95% CI, 21 to 60). If one spouse became obese, the likelihood that the other spouse would become obese increased by 37% (95% CI, 7 to 73). These effects were not seen among neighbors in the immediate geographic location. Persons of the same sex had relatively greater influence on each other than those of the opposite sex. The spread of smoking cessation did not account for the spread of obesity in the network. Network phenomena appear to be relevant to the biologic and behavioral trait of obesity, and obesity appears to spread through social ties. These findings have implications for clinical and public health interventions.
Article
Negative body image is a major concern of overweight persons, yet current obesity treatment programs have not addressed this problem. In the present study, 51 obese women were randomly assigned to cognitive behavioral body image therapy (CBT) or no-treatment. Patients were treated in small groups for 8 two-hour sessions. Therapy included information to challenge negative stereotypes of obesity, modification of intrusive thoughts of body dissatisfaction and overvalued beliefs about physical appearance, exposure to avoided body image situations, and elimination of body checking. No assistance was provided to change eating or exercise behaviors. CBT subjects showed significantly improved body image. Psychological symptoms, self-esteem, overeating, and eating guilt also improved. Weight was unchanged for most subjects and unrelated to treatment outcome overall.
Article
Hypothesizes that body image dissatisfaction is not always a negative process. It is argued that some degree of dissatisfaction may be helpful and necessary to motivate individuals to engage in healthy behaviors such as exercise and restricting fats and calories. The assumption that dissatisfaction with weight is universally deleterious is questioned. It is suggested that to further analyze the role of body image dissatisfaction in eating disorders, one might examine how body dissatisfaction interacts with motivation and participation in healthy behaviors. Theoretical background and empirical support for these hypotheses are offered. The following issues are reviewed: the relationship between body image dissatisfaction and dieting behavior; the public health problem of obesity, body image, and dieting behavior in overweight and obese populations; the research examining the possible beneficial aspects of body image dissatisfaction in predicting weight loss and exercise behavior; and theories from the health psychology literature supporting the notion that distress may serve as a motivating factor for engaging in health behaviors. Implications for this reconceptualization of the hazards of body image dissatisfaction are offered and research directions are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)