ArticlePDF Available

Constructing Autism: Unravelling the “Truth” and Understanding the Social

Authors:
  • Arizona State University West Campus

Abstract

Autism is now considered to be one of the most common developmental disorders today, yet 100 years ago the term did not exist. This book examines the historical and social events that enabled autism to be identified as a distinct disorder in the early twentieth century.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
1
Nadesan, Majia (2007). Constructing Autism: A Brief Genealogy. In M. Osteen’s (ed.),
Autism and Representation. London: Routledge.
Constructing Autism
(Conference Paper Later Published as Book Chapter)
December 27, 2005
Majia Holmer Nadesan
Communication Studies 3251
Arizona State University West
Phoenix AZ 85069
(602) 543-6668 office
majia@asu.edu
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
2
Constructing Autism
Abstract
The explosion of autism diagnoses has led to the proliferation of research studies
that aim to identify the genetic and biological conditions involved in its etiology and its
remediation. However, although the bio-medical model has relevance for understanding
the disorder, it can be limiting as it represents autism in a visual-spatial framework that
implies that the disorder is a definitive thing that can be known and manipulated,
primarily through chemical interventions. This essay is part of a larger project that
explores the social construction of the idea of autism as it is constituted within specific
matrices of ideas and practices. The specific matrices that are identified include those
that constitute the professional fields of psychiatry, psychology, childhood, and the
“environmental movement,” among others. These matrices will be briefly identified in
relation to the idea that autism is socially, albeit materially, constituted in relation to and
through them.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
3
Constructing Autism
Autism, a neurological disease, is now regarded as reaching epidemic proportions
(Evidence mounts 1) and is considered to be a major public health problem. The
dramatic increase in autistic diagnoses has paralleled a huge increase in scientific
research on autism and related disorders such as Pervasive Developmental Disorder
(PDD), Asperger’s Syndrome, and Nonverbal Communication Disorders. For example,
acting in response to the dramatic rates of increase in its autism diagnoses--up to 1000%--
the state of California recently allocated $34 million to autism spectrum disorder research
(California 1). This scientific research is complemented with a whole range of media
information directed toward popular audiences. Information about autism can be found
in a wide array of popular periodicals including such varied titles as Time (Nash and
Bonesteel 46), Newsweek (Cowley 46), The Economist (Science and Technology 93),
and Scientific American (Rodier 56) and on a seemingly infinite supply of websites (e.g.,
www.autism.com; www.autismcenter.org; www.nichd.nih.gov/autism/). Articles about
the relationship between autism and vaccinations can be found in almost every daily
newspaper within the United States (e.g., Bandler B2; Drug Lawsuits A4) and
representations of autism find wide expression within the popular media as illustrated by
the well known film, Rain Man. More recently, the popular media have speculated on a
causal relationship between autism and computer “geekiness” (Chapman 1; Nash and
Bonesteel 46).
Across many of these representation of autismboth scientific and popular--is an
unthematized but foundational premise that autism is a disease (or sometimes condition)
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
4
that will, ultimately, be known, rendered transparent through the ceaseless efforts of
scientific authorities. Typically, this quest for transparency has entailed a reductionistic
search for origins, although speculations regarding the nature of these origins have
changed across time, reflecting historically specific pre-occupations and research
paradigms. The 1950s “refrigerator mother” thesis for the ontogenesis of autism
commonly attributed to Bettelheim (Gardner 12) has been replaced by the current
positivistic pre-occupation with genetic mutations and/or faulty bio-chemistry (e.g.,
Rodier 56-63).
i
Today, efforts to spatially localize epidemiological centers for the
disorder hint at the promise of its remediation through gene therapy or pharmacological
interventions. Thus, even while estimates of the autistic population grow, the underlying
articulation of the “condition” as pathological remains relatively intact. The few voices
that argue for the disorder’s functionality in the information age do not question its
location outside the norm.
This chapter is part of a larger project published as Constructing Autism:
Unravelling the “Truth” and Understanding the Social, which attempts to redress the
positivistic biases in the vast corpus of literature on autism by addressing the social-
historical conditions and practices that enabled autism to be identified, labeled, and
therapied in the early twentieth century as well as those conditions that prevail today that
have enabled childhood autism, particularly high-functioning childhood autism, to be
labeled an “epidemic” with dramatic increases in diagnoses up to 1,000 percent.
ii
Although much maligned, “autism” as an epidemic has also captured the public
imagination as a disorder that is regarded simultaneously as both threatening and
fascinating. Therefore, exploration of the social conditions involved in the production,
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
5
interpretation, and remediation of autism is important not only for people intimately
involved with autism but also for those interested in how social institutions such as
medicine, psychology and psychiatry, and even, the popular media, constitute and shape
our ideas about normality and difference in the context of economic and political
environments. Thus, what is at stake in this exploration extends beyond “autism” as a
distinct disorder to include the ideas and practices whereby we constitute everyday life
and social institutions, to include the processes that will, ultimately, produce the
opportunities for personhood for the twenty first century.
Accordingly, this chapter provides a “genealogy” of autism, a history of the idea
of autism through present articulations, thereby contextualizing the conditions of
possibility for contemporary understandings of autism. Specifically, this chapter unpacks
the social construction of “autism” by identifying the various discourses and institutional
practices that have contributed to the naming, interpretation, and remediation of “autistic”
symptoms over the last century. This genealogy will ultimately problematize the idea
that autism is a homogeneous, pathological condition that can be exhaustively known
transparently representedby scientists and their representational technologies (e.g.,
MRIs). However, although this genealogy deconstructs the idea of autism as a uniform,
biological essence shared by all people labeled as “autistic,” it does not reject the idea
that biological phenomena contribute to the expression of “autistic” symptoms. What is
called for therefore is an approach to autism that explores how various institutional
relationships, expert authorities, and bodies of knowledge have sought to represent,
divide, understand, and act upon biologically based, but socially shaped and expressed,
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
6
behavioral and cognitive differences. What follows constitutes a preliminary and partial
response to this call.
Constructing Autism in the Medical Literature
Autism, as a meaningful diagnostic category, emerged in the early 1940s. This
timing is not accidental but ultimately reflects an emergent matrix of practices and
interpretive vocabularies that marked the transition into the twentieth century.
The emergence of autism as a diagnostic category in the 1940s must be
understood in relation to a matrix of professional and parental practices that marked the
cultural and economic transition to the 20th century just as the emergence of high-
functioning forms of autism must be understood in terms of the matrix of practices that
mark late 20th century, early 21st century life. The conditions of possibility for
diagnosing a child as autistic or high-functioning autistic are ultimately less rooted in the
biology of their conditions than they are rooted in the cultural practices and economy of
their times. For example, in the 1800s, the standards for classifying individuals as
disordered were much less nuanced, the standards of normality much broader, and the
mechanisms for social and individual surveillance that we take for granted today simply
did not exist. Prior to the late 1800s, children, in particular, would have not have been
subject to any form of “developmental” or psychological examination unless their
conditions were particularly severe and their parents particularly economically privileged.
Indeed, it was not until the 1930s that developmental guidelines were created and used in
tracking children’s “developmental” progression.
iii
Given historical contingency in the
identification of autism as a distinct disorder, one is led to question the relationship
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
7
between the biological and the cultural, the relationship between disease and social
representation.
Did autism exist prior to its naming? How did the historically embedded
frameworks used to name autism influence the manner whereby autistic people were
understood, treated, co-created? What assumptions were smuggled in when Leo Kanner
appropriated E. Bleuler’s framework of autistic thinking, which Bleuler himself
appropriated from Freud in order to describe what he saw as a characteristic of
schizophrenic thinking? Did Hans Asperger appropriate Kurt Schneider’s
iv
idea of the
personality disorder when describing the condition that now bears his name, Asperger’s
syndrome?
v
How did conceptions of autism change with the ascendancy of ego
psychology within the United States?
In the context of present circumstances one must ask how cognitive psychology
has constructed autism over the last thirty years? How has neuroscience constructed
autism? How has bio-genetic research constructed autism? And ultimately one must ask
whether a constructionist approach to autism entails denial of biological facticity? How
can representation and materiality be reconciled and how do representations shape the
unfolding/expressions of materiality?
Given limitations of space, I will here merely provide one framework, among
many I am sure, for tackling the challenges posed by representing socially a condition
that has an inescapable biological dimensionality. I then sketch how I use this framework
to address the questions raised above, as well as the representational issues and politics
stemming from current articulations of autism in academic research and popular
discourse.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
8
In our everyday thinking and communication, most of us visualize disease as
either being caused by a scientifically discernable agent such as a virus or bacterium
(e.g., AIDS or meningitis) or as emanating from a detectable, localized bodily
dysfunction (e.g., heart disease or diabetes). The disease causing agent or diseased
bodily system is seen as objective, available to visual representation (through a
microscope, electro-magnetic scan, or simply through a scientific diagram), and
ultimately, treatable (even if the “cure” eludes current medical understanding). In effect,
disease is represented in our everyday understanding as available to “empirical”
(experiential) identification, interpretation, and intervention. This everyday
understanding of disease is rooted in nineteenth century “positivistic” thought that holds
that the laws of nature can be identified and understood univocally through detached,
empirical inquiry. Positivist conceptions of medicine presume a mind/body dichotomy in
which diseases are primarily if not exclusively located in the biological body and
presume that each disease is caused by a specific and (ultimately) identifiable element.
vi
Although medicine, as a professional field, has long rejected many of the positivist
assumptions about the nature and origin of disease, our popular understandings and much
medical practice continue to invoke them in the ways we diagnose and treat various
diseases.
Although many experts reject the positivist model of autism, this formulation
lingers in the popular imagination and in expert efforts to identify “agents” responsible
for causing autism or capable of curing autism. In fact, autism is a heterogeneous
condition that is more properly a syndrome than it is a distinct disease. Moreover, the
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
9
causal pathways engendering autistic symptoms are most likely multiple and contingent
upon level upon level of loosely-coupled, synergistic, biological and social systems.
Most medical experts would agree that all, or even most, people with autism do not share
the same underlying biological condition. Moreover, people with autism, as is the case
with all people, grow and change in relation to their familial and social environments.
Built cultural environments and social practices elicit and constrain, enable and shape the
developing person, whether that person bears a label or not. And yet, despite these
truisms, autism is often represented in popular discoursein the media, in parental
accounts of autistic children, and in some medical research--as being caused by
discernable, distinct, and mechanistic agents whose operations will inevitably be
“revealed” to an anxious public. In such accounts, the complex and irreducible biological
and social synergies that operate across genetic, molecular, psychological, social, and
cultural environments are erased or marginalized in favor of reductionistic, linear, bio-
genetic formulations.
However, efforts to reject positivist models confront age old dilemmas posed by
classical western (i.e., Platonic and Cartesian) dualisms between body and mind/soul,
corporeality and symbolic representation, materialism and idealism. Ridding theorizing
of ancient but false dichotomies is not an easy task as illustrated by the challenges posed
by approaching disease as socially constituted. Too often efforts to view disease as
socially constructed oscillate between the binaries of materialism and idealism because of
the difficulties inherent in their synthesis. On the one hand, the socially built
environment contributes to disease (e.g., through diet and pollution) and yet the causes
for diseases cannot be reduced to built environments. On the other hand, although
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
10
disease may appear to present itself as brute facticity (e.g., through pain or
dehabilitation), bodily symptoms are always/already interpreted within symbolic systems
of meanings, social practices, and historically and culturally variable expert authorities.
Prevailing socially constituted ideas about disease shape and constrain medical
researchers’ observations, interpretations, and interventions while bodily symptoms and
processes can at times offer material refutation of prevailing ideas about causation and
cure. Disease, disability, bodily difference are at once material and symbolic, socially
constructed and materially inscribed.
Recently, scholars in the areas of the sociology of medicine and disabilities
studies have offered more synthetic accounts of the relationship between body and mind,
disease and representation, materialism and idealism. Fox, for example, suggests that
rather than becoming locked within the dualism of nature/biology and culture, a more
productive route would entail rejecting a search for the “truth” of how the body (or brain)
“really is” (or should be), rejecting a search for the body’s facticity. He contends that
research should focus on the becoming of the body/mind as it is constituted by and in
relation to cultural processes (Fox 9). Fox’s approach need not be construed as
necessitating a rejection of a biological component of autism. Rather, it entails viewing
the biological and the cultural as mutually constitutive, inseparable in their constitution of
personhood. An approach such as the one endorsed by Fox would not emphasize the
mechanistic and reductionistic search for definitive origins but would instead focus on the
becoming of autism as it is bio-culturally constituted, interpreted, experienced, and
resisted.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
11
In seeking to unravel the becoming of autism, I will employ the work of Ian
Hacking, who provides useful models and heuristics for exploring the social construction
of disease and bodily difference. In The Social Construction of What? Hacking
demonstrates the interaction of biology and culture, materiality and ideas, through a
variety of examples including schizophrenia, child abuse, and childhood autism. Hacking
uses the idea of “interactive kinds” to explain the mediation of socially constructed ideas
and material existence: “‘Interactive’ is a new concept that applies not to people but to
classifications, to kinds, to the kinds that can influence what is classified. And because
kinds can interact with what is classified, the classification itself may be modified or
replaced” (103). Unlike “indifferent kinds,” which refer to the classifications of entities
that are not affected by their classifications as such, “interactive kinds” are fundamentally
affectedproduced in relation tothe categories and labels used to describe them.
Interactive kinds are affected by the process of classification to such a degree that the
classification may itself require eventual modification or replacement. The construct of
child television viewers, Hacking observes, illustrates the kind of classificatory “looping”
effect specific to interactive kinds: the behavior of child television viewers is, no doubt,
irrevocably changed by the classification and research of children as child viewers.
Interactive kinds are classificatory systems that emerge within complex matrices of
institutions and practices. Once articulated, these classificatory systems engender
practices and institutions that have the effect of producing/shaping what was classified.
However, the process of producing human beings is subject to effects unintended
because, among other factors, of the reflexive nature of consciousness. Awareness of
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
12
one’s classification as a particular kind of being, a particular kind of subject, can
engender resistance and/or behavioral variation.
Hacking goes on to distinguish the kind of classificatory looping illustrated by
child viewers from another form of looping he describes as ‘biolooping’ (109), which he
illustrates in relation to the brain chemical, serotonin. Serotonin levels, as Hacking
observes, are correlated with depression. However, behavioral treatments directed
toward reducing depressive states, can be as effective as chemical therapy in raising
serotonin levels. Biolooping thus refers to the process whereby mental states, individual
comportment, and cultural practices can affect biological outcomes (e.g., serotonin
levels). Hacking argues that biolooping and classificatory looping could both be at work,
simultaneously, in some forms of psychopathologies, particularly schizophrenia and
childhood autism.
Hacking suggests that autism, like schizophrenia, is an interactive kind that may
be subject to the looping effect as a consequence of the interpretive work done by
parents, caregivers, therapists, and autistic patients. Included in the scope of Hacking’s
idea of interpretive work are experts’ very understandings of the diseases at issue.
Accordingly, Hacking suggests that understandings of the very nature and manifestations
of distinct diseases/pathologies/etc may change significantly over time, leading to
different therapeutic approaches and, consequently, disease progressions. For example,
Hacking notes that many of the symptoms identified by Kanner and Asperger are no
longer regarded as the primary symptoms that demarcate “autistic” children, as illustrated
by the decreased relevance of “flat affect,” which was observed and remarked upon by
both original researchers and yet is no longer regarded as a determinate diagnostic
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
13
criteria. Moreover, many severely impacted children who today bear the label of
classical autism would not have been so diagnosed by Kanner or Asperger, who would
have been likely to diagnose severely impaired children as either psychotic or mentally
retarded.
Consequently, autism is a particularly compelling example of the intersection of
biology and culture because although it is arguably an interactive kind, it also evidences
the characteristics of an indifferent kind in that its symptoms are in some way rooted in
genetics or molecular chemistry. These bio-genetic factors, however, do not “motivate”
fixed, uniform symptoms, such as “flat affect.” Moreover, the underlying bio-genetic
factors are not themselves fixed or uniform. The effects of parental expectations and
reinforcements, therapy programs, and the individual experiences loop back to
affect/constitute the expression of bio-genetic factors. Further, the use of drugs such as
Prozac do not merely reduce symptoms but may actually alter the brain chemistry and
neural topography of autistic patients, illustrating what Hacking describes as bio-looping.
Prozac and other drugs that target neural chemistry are themselves cultural
artifacts and thereby illustrate how cultural models of the mind/brain undergird “expert”
authorities’ efforts toward bio-engineering. Although the current bio-genetic/chemical
model of the brain/mind relationship is not exhaustive (or necessarily “valid”),
biomedical interventions generated using this model tend to alter subtly the expression of
that which they presuppose. Moreover, patient and caregiver expectations, based on the
perceived validity of the bio-genetic model of the brain, no doubt alter the experience and
expression of the effects of biomedical interventions. Although my discussion of the
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
14
interactive dimension of autism extends beyond Hacking’s formulation it demonstrates
the complex interactions among biology and culture, neurology and communication.
Hacking’s works points to many compelling avenues of research for those
interested in approaching autism from a symbolic perspective. I am most interested in
the emergence of autism in the early 20th century and the emergence of more high-
functioning forms of the disorder in the late 20th century as “niche” disorders. In
Hacking’s 1998 text, Mad Travelers, he investigates “transient” mental illness:
By a ‘transient mental illness’ I mean an illness that appears at a time, in a
place, and later fades away. It may spread from place to place and
reappear from time to time. It may be selective for social class or gender,
preferring poor women or rich women. I do not mean that it comes and
goes in this or that patient, but that this type of madness exists only at
certain times and places. The most famous candidate for a transient
mental illness is hysteria. . . . (1)
Hacking questions whether a variety of neuroses such as PMS, ADHD, and multiple
personality disorder are real or culturally produced in relation to specific socio-cultural
events and practices. The so-called “shadow syndromes” such as sub-clinical autism and
depression are particularly suspect disorders in this regard.
Although there is some discontinuity in Hacking’s formulations of sub-clinical
autism as a transient mental illness in Mad Travelers and his later formulation of it as
both an interactive and indifferent kind in The Social Construction of What, I feel that the
disorder exemplifies the niche effect that Hacking sees as operating in transient mental
illness, even while it undoubtedly has a biological component. In the section that
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
15
follows, I briefly introduce my thesis that autism is a disorder that emerged and was
created in relation to cultural practices and discourses that are specific to particular points
in time, the transition from the nineteenth to the twentieth century and, more recently, the
transition from the twentieth to the twenty-first century.
Constructing Autism: Some Preliminary Ideas
Following Hacking’s idea of a niche effect, I suggest that autism is a disorder of
the early twentieth century while the high-functioning variants of autism such as
Asperger’s Syndrome (AS), and Pervasive Development Disorder (PDD) are
fundamentally disorders of the late twentieth-early twenty-first centuries. This is to say
that as a distinct psychological disorder or psychiatric disease, autism could not have
emerged in the nineteenth century, even while I concede that there have been, no doubt,
people throughout history who have displayed the symptoms we now group and define as
autism.
vii
The idea of autism could not have emerged as a distinct disorder because
within the diagnostic categories of nineteenth century (and earlier) thought, autism was
unthinkable. If autism was unthinkable within the scientific taxonomies, medical
nosologies, and medical practices of nineteenth century thought, high functioning
variants of autism were largely unthinkable diagnostic categories until the mid to late
twentieth century.
viii
It was not until standards of normality had been formalized and
narrowed and standards of pediatric screening extended to a child’s earliest years that
children with PDD or AS (or ADD, or ADHD) could be identified, labeled, and
therapied.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
16
And so, the history of autism in all of its forms must be contextualized within the
evolution and transformation of medical practices, the development of professions such
as psychiatry, psychology, social work, and special education, all of which either
emerged or were professionalized in the early twentieth century. The history of AS and
PDD must be further understood in the context of new standards for parenting that
emerged mid-twentieth century and new economic conditions surrounding the purported
“information revolution” that began in the 1960s. As I will argue, the public’s
fascination with autism stems in large part from the idea that people with autism are
technologically gifted and are particularly adapt with computer technology.
ix
The
scientific search for understanding the essence of autism in the late twentieth century
must also be contextualized within a new matrix of practices that seek to explain social
behaviors in terms of genetic markers, which are seen as entities that will ultimately be
linked (deterministically) to the whole repertoire of human behaviors.
Finally, the “cures” for autism, the various remediations and therapies promoted
by professional practitioners and parents alike, must be understood in the context of late
twentieth century social fears about environmental contamination and pollution, fears
engendered by environmental catastrophes but also motivated by, I argue, latent cultural
taboos and anxieties. In its entirety, my project addresses the social complexity of
autism, embracing the ambivalence and contradictions associated with its interpretations
and remediations, not only to understand its social construction but also to gain insight
into some of the cultural frameworks of interpretation and social practices that are
leading us into the 21st century.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
17
For the sake of linguistic simplicity, I represent the various strands involved in the
constitution, interpretation, and remediation of autism in terms of “social discourses,”
while acknowledging that discourses cannot be severed from the institutional relations
that engender them and serve as the means for their reproduction and transformation.
The various discourses that I will briefly introduce include discourses of twentieth
century pediatric psychiatry, childhood, cognitive discourses, genetic discourses,
discourses of the environment, and discourses of advocacy in the context of social
designations of normality and difference.
The first set of discursive practices to be addressed concerns the psychiatric
articulation of boundaries between normality and pathology. Genealogies of mental
illness provided by Berrios, Berrios and Porter, Foucault, and Porter, among others,
reveal the historical contingencies and institutional matrices providing the conditions of
possibility for increasingly nuanced ideas about the nature of, and divisions between,
psychological normality and pathology in the early twentieth century. The institution of
the modern state in the nineteenth century involved the extension of governmental
practices over more domains of social life, resulting in new bodies of knowledge, social
institutions, and authorities who aimed to divide populations according to finer wrought
distinctions of health and pathology, sanity and insanity, intellectual acuity and mental
retardation (see Foucault “Governmentality”). Accordingly, by the beginning of the
twentieth century, new divisions, institutions, and authorities enabled the identification
and articulation of autistic spectrum disorders that formerly would have either escaped
the parameters of mental illness (e.g., Asperger’s syndrome) or, contrastingly, would
have resulted in designations of “idiocy.”
x
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
18
Changing ideas about childhood also enabled identification and articulation of
autism within the early twentieth century. Nineteenth century governmental practices
included expansion of compulsory education, resulting in more social and expert
surveillance of children. Moreover, the articulation and popularization of Freudian and
Darwinian theories in the second half of the nineteenth century resulted in new cultural
anxieties about childhood in general and the childhood of potential social “degenerates”
in particular (see Morss). Although much early research and social work focused on
adjusting the children of lower class immigrants in the United States, the increasing
psychiatric interest in preventing social maladjustment eventually brought middle class
children into focus by the early twentieth century. As new educational and psychological
authorities were developed to meet the new imperatives of social adjustment (see Rose
Governing the Soul), the boundaries between normality and pathology were
problematized as the binary distinction was slowly replaced by a continuum of
pathologized and pre-pathological states associated with the (Freudian) neuroses, pre-
schizophrenic or schizophrenic like-conditions (the pre-psychotic) and finally with the
articulation of personality disorders (as articulated by Kurt Schneider). Educational
psychologists, community psychiatrists, and pediatriciansall newly instituted experts
found apparent manifestations of a vast range of disorders among the children they
surveyed (also see Jones 1-37).
These historical circumstancesthe convergence of new ideas about childhood,
new systems of surveillance, new expert authorities, and new institutional
arrangementsprovided the conditions of possibility for autism to be identified, named
and interpreted. Accordingly, Hans Asperger’s pediatric practice and Leo Kanner’s child
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
19
psychiatry must be contextualized in relation to these historical circumstances.
Furthermore, efforts to understand how autism meant for each of these individuals must
be contextualized within a genealogy of Bleuler’s schizophrenia, Freudian
psychoanalysis, German phenomenology, and Kurt Schneider’s personality theory—
conceptual frameworks for the explanation of mind, perception, and pathology that
shaped Asperger’s and Kanner’s ideas about autism. Both Leo Kanner and Hans
Asperger appropriated Bleuler’s description of the schizophrenic’s “autistic” style of
thinking, which Bleuler had appropriated from Freud. This intellectual heritage
engendered the assumption that the autistic essence was characterized by aloneness,
solipsism, a turning away from the social world, resulting in a failure to develop a
“normal” ego. However, whereas Kanner viewed autistic thinking in relation to
psychosis, in relation to an underdeveloped or disintegrating ego, Asperger was more
inclined to regard the children under his care as afflicted with a particular type of (non-
psychotic but disordered) ego that afforded unique insight into the natural world even
while it created social discord (Nadesan 70-78). Both Kanner and Asperger felt that
autistic thinking resulted from inborn, biological forces; although, Kanner also noted
behavioral and personality similarities in the parents of his autistic children (50), thereby
implying the possibility for some level of social influence in the conditioning of autistic
symptoms.
The role of social forces in producing autism captured the public imagination in
the United States and Western Europe in the post-World War II context. Popularized
accounts of child development promulgated by such diverse figures as D. W. Winnicott,
Benjamin Spock, and Bruno Bettelheim all emphasized the role of the mother in fostering
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
20
stable ego development. Normal, healthy child development was viewed as a process
fraught with peril, requiring the ever present guidance of the maternal figure. Autism and
other developmental disorders were sometimes inadvertently and other times directly
attributed to maternal negligence or transgression. Earlier psychoanalytic accounts that
emphasized infant perceptions (such as those proposed by Melanie Klein) were replaced
in the post-war context by object relations accounts that centered mothers’ actual (or
purported) behaviors as explaining developmental problems. In popularized
psychological discourses, mother blaming” continued to explain many if not most forms
of child psychopathology until the late 1970s and early 1980s.
Toward the close of the twentieth century new institutional conditions, expert
authorities, and systems of knowledge acted to shape further ideas about normality and
pathology in the context of new “niche” conditions. Cognitive psychology, in particular,
replaced the psychoanalytic framework for understanding “developmental disorders.”
And so autism was reconceptualized in the popular imagination, from a ego shipwrecked
on the shores of object relations to a computer with modular dysfunctions. Accordingly,
I suggest that “high functioning” autism (e.g., Asperger’s Syndrome and Semantic
Pragmatic Disorder) and Pervasive Developmental Disorders (PDD) have been carved
out as distinct disorders by several important social/cultural discourses including the
cybernetic metaphors of cognitive psychology that view mind in terms of isolated
modules, as well as by “parent centered” bourgeois discourses that articulate childhood as
the locus of class mobility, particularly in the context of the “information” age (see
Ehrenreich 1-20; Nadesan 401).
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
21
The coupling of cognitive psychology with cognitive neuroscience, and the
popularization of this coupling in the form of a late twentieth century discourse of early
childhood “brain science” (Bruer “Brain Science” 14; Bruer, “In Search” 649) lent
urgency to the public and academic interest in “developmental disorders,” particularly
autism, further expanding the continuum of possible autistic symptoms and diagnostic
assessments.
In relation to these discourses, childhood autism has multiple valences in the
context of late 20th century life. Autism signifies pathology and difference in a historical
epoch that increasingly emphasizes physical and psychological “health,” yet it also
signifies technological aptitude as illustrated in this Los Angeles Time article on the
prototypical “computer geek”: “There is some fascinating speculation going on these
days that the well-known stereotype of the computer geek or nerd may actually be a
description of mild autism, especially a form of autism known as Asperger’s syndrome”
(Chapman 1; see also Nash and Bonesteel and Silberman). I suggest that the
simultaneous elevation and denigration of “high functioning” autistic characterizations
speak more to cultural preoccupations and anxieties with technology and masculinity
than they do to the “essential” autistic personality (see Nadesan 128-132). But most
concerning is that at times cognitive representations of high-function autism erase the
very humanity of their subjects by promulgating a vision of autistic intelligence as both
alien and machine-like.
Additionally at issue in understand contemporary preoccupations with autism are
heightened concerns about the role of the built environment in affecting disease and
disablement. Discourses of “risk” management, “environmental degradation” and, more
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
22
generally, discourses of modern “malaise” offer a range of interpretive frameworks for
understanding autism as an environmentally induced/mediated condition. Specifically,
understanding how autism as articulated as an “environmental” illness points to current
anxieties surrounding environmental contamination and government mandated medical
protocols (e.g., vaccinations) designed to manage populations and reduce risk at the level
of those populations. Framed from this perspective, “autism” sometimes signifies as a
disease of modernization whose remediation depends largely upon adopting the regimes
of alternative medicine. In a sense, autism is the outermost pole of a whole range of
“contemporary” diseases including ADHD, chronic fatigue syndrome, and fibromyalgia
that, following the argument of Morris, are illnesses that “possess the power to define or
represent an entire era” (56).
Perhaps most importantly, however, cultural anxieties about the relationship
between autism and environmental contamination have not resulted in a desire to
confront actively the source and form of environmental contaminants but rather have
resulted in research and resource allocations dedicated to identifying “susceptibility”
genes. Environmental discourses are marginalized in importance in comparison to the
new discourse of the active gene promulgated by pharmacological and bio-engineering
interests.
Therefore, the social construction of autism must also be explored in the context
of an emergent, hegemonic discourse that has been labeled (by critics) the “geneticization
thesis” (Lippman 15). Critics of this discourse argue that it operates by reifying the
“gene” as a unified biological agent and a causal principle that is used in a reductionistic
fashion to explain ever more aspects of social life (see full accounts by Keller; ten Have;
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
23
Hedgecoe; Rabinow). Some fear that the geneticization thesis leads to the resurrection of
older eugenics politics and policies, raising new concerns about the possible implications
of the search for autism’s genetic origins. While these concerns are valid, they entail that
the causal and mechanistic model of gene action will ultimately prove fruitful in
explaining autism’s etiology. And yet, this model of the gene has so far proved
unproductive in explaining autism and, indeed, most forms of mental illness, in part
because the model presupposes that complex phenotypes are reducible to clearly
definable genotypes and because the reductionistic model of the gene erases the
complexity of synergistic bodily systems, systems whose operations are always/already
mediated by environmental inputs.
However, although the discourse of genetic determinism is rent by limitations, it
has emerged as an important social discourse in that it “produces truth” by organizing “an
epistemo-political field of the visible and the expressible, which controls the diverse
forms of signification, and defines the conditions for truth and falsehood” (Lemke 553).
In other words, the new genetics discourse is a regime of truth that constructs individuals
as particular kinds of subjects.
Accordingly, Lemke suggests that the “social power” of genetic information “lies
less in the resurrection of genetic determinism and more in the construction of genetic
risks” (551). What Lemke is suggesting is that the power of the genetics discourses lies
less in its power to predict determinate outcomes than in its power to calculate “risks” in
relation to “deviations” from statistically generated profiles of normality. Thus, Novas
and Rose (486) suggest that genetic risk has been afforded a “new calculability,”
potentially leading to new strategies and technologies designed to assess and ameliorate
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
24
risky personhood. Risky personhood itself is evaluated in relation to a new and
ultimately normatively constituted “consensus genome” (Lemke 553). Riskiness is
assessed in relation to the consensus genome, leading to a new “discourse of deficiency”
that constructs individuals in relation to the disposition of their genetic variation (Lemke
553).
The desire to represent and control the human genome illustrates in the extreme
Foucault’s description of biopower—as a form of power that seeks to govern populations
through technologies of life (History of Sexuality 139). Accordingly, as a disease of
modernization, autism speaks to new practices of biopower as well as to new
technologies for the surveillance and governance of populations while, conversely, it
opens up new strategies of resistance by individuals who seek to advocate for respect and
social justice on the basis of their biogenetic differences (see Rose “The Politics of Life
Itself”).
Novas and Rose suggest that “somatic” individuals who relate to themselves on
the basis of some underlying genetic identity do so in a context of inalienable legal rights
granting them entitlements as well as obligations (501). The somatic subject is also
embedded within the humanistic psychological discourse of self-actualization that
demands certain ethical accountabilities from those providing their services or caring for
their well-being. Therefore, the bio-genetic medical discourses that construct individuals
as “somatic” selves also intersect with other legal and humanistic discourses that stress
“autonomy, self-actualization, prudence, responsibility and choice” (Novas and Rose
502). Disability and patients’ rights advocates make use of these legal and humanistic
discourses in their efforts to optimize care and social respect and accommodation.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
25
A wide and diverse range of autism advocates have seized genetic discourses,
utilized them advantageously, and simultaneously disparaged such discourses for
genetically pathologizing social difference. Understand the social construction of autism
thus entails exploring the ambiguities, contradictions, and efforts posed by efforts to
appropriate and resist genetic discourses.
I conclude by considering the various effects of the current discursive
articulations of autism. This chapter does not attempt to locate the “truth” of autism
because, as the author argues, there is no fixed, universal biological truth to be located.
Autism is articulated discursively through the nosological clustering of symptoms and
through the clinical practices of remediation. It is “produced” through the practices that
materially inscribe the various social discourses that have been briefly described. This
argument does not deny that there are biological differences: it does not deny that
genetics, ontogenetic socialization factors, and environmental chemicals shape the
emergence and expression of our experiential embodiment. Following Hacking’s
vocabulary, I argue that autism is an interactive kind and that individuals labeled autistic
are fundamentally transformed by that labeling and the subsequent interventions that
follow, leading to what Hacking has described as the looping effect. Thus, the argument
made here is that the processes of identifying, interpreting, remediating, and performing
embodied differences are cultural and historically specific. Autism is not outside of the
symbolic awaiting discovery. Autism is inscribed, produced, through it and an
investigation into these processes of inscription/production reveals current desires,
anxieties, and opportunities for personhood in the twenty-first century.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
26
i
Although Bettelheim is typically held responsible for the idea that mothers cause
their children’s autism, the idea that mothers’ behaviors lead to mental illness predates
Bettelheim’s work. Lauretta Bender suggests that Frieda Fromm-Reichmann’s 1948 idea
of the “schizophrenogenic mother” may have helped catalyze mother blaming.
Popularized renditions of objects relations theory promulgated by figures such as D. W.
Winnicott held mothers’ responsible for their infants’ successes or failures in achieving
stable egos (see Nadesan 95-102). However, the force and literary simplicity of
Bettelheim’s writing in The Empty Fortress led credence to Bettelheim’s writings in the
context of a Zeitgeist willing to blame mothers for all manner of developmental
irregularities.
ii
See “Evidence Mounts for Epidemic of Autism” in the 2000 edition of Autism
Research Review, 14(2), 1. Indeed, “autism” has become such a ubiquitous disorder that
the state of California allocated $34 million for autism spectrum disorder research in
2000 (California 1).
iii
See Rose, Governing the Soul and Armstrong A New History of Identity for
discussion of the emergence of childhood norms of development.
iv
Schneider’s 1923 text, Psychopathic Personalities, articulated ontology of distinct
personality types whose deviation from the norms of behavior result in extreme social
difficulties. That is, according to Schneider, persons with “psychopathic” personalities
exhibit such behavioral and attitudinal irregularities that they cause considerable distress
for social others and for themselves. Schneider believed that these personality types were
not caused by psychiatric diseases of the kind suggested by Kraepelin’s dementia
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
27
praecox. Moreover, Schneider strongly believed that these personality types were not
“developmental syndromes” understood in the psychoanalytic sense. In other words,
Schneider went against the prevailing tendencies to regard personality irregularities in
terms of psychotic tendencies or character neuroses (quoted in Berrios 22). Instead,
Schneider forged a new approach to understanding psychopathology: an approach that
blended aspects of phenomenology, Gestalt psychology, psychoanalysis and biological
psychiatry.
v
In 1981 Lorna Wing popularized Hans Asperger’s observations of children in the
1940, who he perceived as having a type of disorder he labeled, autistic psychopathy.
Asperger claimed that his patients with “autistic psychopathy” did not suffer from a
disease entity insomuch as they suffered from a personality disorder that was free from
the secondary psychotic expressions (e.g., delusions and hallucinations) described by
Bleuler. And from Asperger’s point of view, this personality disorder engendered certain
intellectual strengths as well as weaknesses, including peculiar interests and a “paucity”
of “expressive movements,” including deficiencies in eye-contact and expressive gestures
(69).
vi
See Freund and McGuire’s discussion of the western medical model, pp. 6-7.
vii
For a case study of an individual who has been retroactively labeled “autistic” see
Autism in History: The Case of Hugh Blair of Borgue by Rab Houston and Uta Frith.
viii
People who today exhibit symptoms that result in the label “high functioning
autism” would likely have escaped diagnosis in the early part of the twentieth century.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
28
By the mid-twentieth century, such high-functioning individuals might have been
diagnosed with a personality disorder such as the schizoid personality disorder or might
have been regarded as a sociopathic or psychopathic.
ix
For example, Wired magazine recently ran an article titled, “The Geek
Syndrome” written by Steve Silberman that speculates that some technologically “gifted”
individuals may have Asperger’s Syndrome and that autism and Asperger’s Syndrome
diagnoses are surging among the children of Silicon Valley (174-183). Silberman
speculates that “math and tech genes” may be to blame (see also Nash and Bonesteel 40).
viii For an excellent discussion of the history mental illness, see Trent Inventing the
Feeble Mind and Wright and Digby From Idiocy to Mental Deficiency.
References
Armstrong, David. A New History of Identity. Houndmills: Palgrave, 2002.
Bandler, J. “Children’s Vaccines Raise Seizure Risk, Don’t Cause Autism.” Wall Street Journal
30 August 2001: B2.
Bender, Lauretta. “The Historical Background of the Concept of Childhood Schizophrenia.” Ed.
J. G. Howells The Concept of Schizophrenia: Historical Perspectives. Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Press, 1991.
Berrios, Germane E. The History of Mental Symptoms: Descriptive Psychopathology Since the
Nineteenth Century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
29
Berrios, Germane E., and R. Porter (Eds.). A History of Clinical Psychiatry: The Origin and
History of Psychiatric Disorders. London: Athlone, 1995.
Bleuler, E. Die Prognose der Dementia Praecox (Schizophreniegruppe).” Allgemeine Zeitschrift
fur Psychiatrie und Psychisch-Gerichtliche Medizin vol 65 1908: 436-464.
Bleuler, E. “Dementia Praecox Oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien.” Ed. G. Aschaffenburg (ed.),
Handbuch der Psychiatrie. Spezieller Teil. 4 Abteilung, 1. Halfte. Leipzig und Wien:
Franz Deuticke, 1911.
Bruer, J. T. “Brain Science, Brain Fiction.” Educational Leadership 56(3) (1998): 14-18.
Bruer, J. T. “In Search of Brain-Based Education.” Phi-Delta Kappan 80(9) (1999): 649-657.
“California: $34 million allocated for autism spectrum disorders research.” Autism Research
Review 14(2) (2000): 1.
Chapman, G. “The Cutting Edge: Focus on Technology; Digital Nation: Even If “Geekiness” is
a Disorder, There’s No Rush to Find a Cure. Los Angeles Times 27 September 1999: 1.
Cowley, G. “Understanding Autism.” Newsweek 31 July 2000: 46-54.
“Drug Lawsuits Pursue Mercury Link to Autism.” The Arizona Republic 3 October 2001: A4.
Ehrenreich, Barbara. Fear of Falling: The Inner Life of the Middle Class. New York: Patheon,
1989.
“Evidence Mounts for Epidemic of Autism.” Autism Research Review 14(2) (2000): 1.
Foucault, Michel. “Governmentality.” Ed. G. Burchell, C. Gordon, and P. Miller The Foucault
Effect. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf, 1991. 97-104.
--- Madness and Civilization: A History of Insanity in the Age of Reason. Trans. Richard
Howard. New York: Vintage Books, 1988.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
30
---Mental Illness and Psychology. Trans. Alan Sheridan. Berkeley: University of California
Press, 1987.
---The History of Sexuality: An Introduction. Vol 1 Trans. Robert Hurley. New York: Vintage,
1990.
Fox, Nick. Beyond Health: Postmodernism and Embodiment. London: Free Association
Books, 1999.
Freund, P. E. S. and McGuire, M. B. Health, Illness, and The Social Body: A Critical Sociology,
Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall, 1991.
Gardner, Martin. “The Brutality of Dr. Bettelheim.” Skeptical-Inquirer 24(6) (2000)
November/December: 12-14.
Greco, Monica. Illness as a Work of Thought: A Foucauldian Perspective on Psychosomatics.
London: Routledge, 1998.
Hacking, Ian. Mad Travelers. Charlottesville: University of Virginia, 1999.
--The Social Construction of What? Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1999.
Hedgecoe, Adam. Schizophrenia and the Narrative of Enlightened Geneticization. Social
Studies of Science 31(6)(2001): 875-911.
Houston, Rab and Frith, Uta. Autism in History: The Case of Hugh Blair of Borgue. Padstow,
Cornwall: Blackwell, 2000.
Jones, K. W. Taming the Troublesome Child: American Families, Child Guidance, and the
Limits of Psychiatric Authority, Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1999.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
31
Kanner, Leo. “Autistic Disturbances of Affective Contact.” Ed. A. M. Donnellan Classic
Readings in Autism. New York: Columbia University, 1943/1985. 11-49.
Keller, Evelyn F. Refiguring Life: Metaphors of Twentieth-Century Biology. New York:
Columbia University Press, 1995.
--The Century of the Gene, Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 2000.
Lippman, A. (1991). Prenatal Genetic Testing and Screening. American Journal of Law and
Medicine 1 (1991): 15-50.
Lemke, T. Disposition and DeterminismGenetic Diagnostics in Risk Society. Sociological
Review 22(4) (2004): 550-566.
Morris, David B. Illness and Culture in the Postmodern Age. Berkeley: University of
California Press, 1998.
Morss, John R. The Biologising of Childhood: Developmental Psychology and the Darwinian
Myth. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 1990.
Nadesan, Majia. Constructing Autism: Unravelling the “Truth” and Understanding the Social.
London: Routledge, 2005.
--“Engineering the Entrepreneurial infant: Brain Science, Infant Development Toys, and
Governmentality.” Cultural Studies 16 (2002): 401-132.
Nash, Madeleine J. and Bonesteel, Amy. “The Geek Syndrome.” Time 6 May (2002): 90-94.
Novas, Carlos and Rose, Nikolas. “Genetic Risk and the Birth of the Somatic Individual.”
Economy and Society 29(4) (2000): 485-513.
Porter, R. Madness: A Brief History. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002.
Rabinow, Paul. French DNA: Trouble in Purgatory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press,
1999.
Nadesan, Constructing Autism
32
Rodier, Patricia M. “The Early Origins of Autism.” Scientific American 282(2) (2000): 56-63.
Rose, N. Governing the Soul: The Shaping of the Private Self (2nd ed.). Routledge: London,
1999.
--“The Politics of Life Itself.” Theory, Culture and Society 18 (2001): 1-30.
Schneider, Kurt. Die Psychopathischen Persoenlichkeiten. / The psychopathic personalities.
Leipzig: Thieme, 1923.
-- “Ueber die Grenzen der Psychologisierung. / The limits of psychologization’, Nervenarzt 24
(1953): 89-90
“Science and Technology: Pointing the Finger.” 24 March 2001. Economist 93.
Silberman, Steve. “The Geek Syndrome.” Wired Magazine 9, 12 (2001): 174-183. Available
online http://www.wired.com/wired/archive/9.12/aspergers_pr.html
ten Have, H. A.M.J. “Genetics and Culture: The Geneticization Thesis.” Medicine, Health Care
and Philosophy 4 (2001): 295-304.
Trent, James W. Inventing the Feeble Mind: A History of Mental Retardation in the United
States. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1994.
Wing, Lorna . “Asperger’s Syndrome: A Clinical Account.” Psychological Medicine,
11(1981): 115-130.
Wright, David and Anne Digby (Eds.) From Idiocy to Mental Deficiency: Historical Perspectives
on People with Learning Disabilities. London: Routledge, 1996.
... This is in part because some applications emphasize behavioral compliance over empowerment, immediate antecedents, and consequences in shaping behavior over histories of trauma and survival (Bascom, 2012;Behavior Analysis Certification Board, 2018;Birdmadgirl, 2017). Many autistic and neurodiverse self-advocates view interventions rooted in applied behavior analysis as attempts to "fix" something professionals view as wrong with them as opposed to attempting to understand and support them in living lives of their choosing (Nadesan, 2005;Shyman, 2016). Some adults who identify as part of a neurodiverse community report that practices rooted in behavior analysis were a source of harm and trauma (Anderson, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives To explore the role of intentional, trauma-informed program planning and evaluation in intellectual and developmental disabilities (IDD) services and to propose that the theory of change logic modeling can serve as an effective tool to enhance trauma-informed care (TIC) implementation, sustainability, and evidence-based. Methods TIC has emerged as a transformative framework within social service sectors, including disability services, to address the widespread impact of trauma and adversity. This review provides an overview of the state of TIC, identifying key areas for improvement and recommendations for implementing the theory of change logic modeling as a means of integrating the foundations of TIC from conception and planning to implementation and practice. Results Despite its potential to foster system-wide reflection and improve service outcomes, TIC implementation faces significant challenges, including insufficient evidence, an overreliance on individual practitioners, and inadequate programmatic and policy support. The disability service system, particularly for individuals with IDD, has been slower to integrate TIC despite the heightened risk of trauma among this population. Conclusions By embedding TIC principles within structured programmatic frameworks such as the theory of change logic modeling, service systems can move beyond isolated training efforts toward comprehensive, systemic change.
... Until recently, much of the research supporting diagnosis and classification has been from a development deficit basis. This has influenced generations of clinicians and the public understanding of autism, affecting the experiences of autistic individuals and their families [6][7][8]. This lack of focus on the positive aspects of parenting could therefore be strengthened through the lens of positive psychology, a field of psychology concerned with the study of positive emotions and which emphasises strengths, wellbeing, and flourishing [9]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This review aimed to identify and synthesise the evidence for the positive aspects of parenting a child with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). To date, ASD parental research has predominantly focused on the negative aspects; parents and carers are left uninformed of how to strengthen their positive mental wellbeing. Three electronic databases—PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science —were searched for literature reporting positive aspects of parenting a child with ASD, published between January 2013 and December 2023. The PRISMA 2020 checklist was used for reporting. Two hundred and fifty-four articles were retrieved from the databases. After title/abstract screening (n = 213) and full-text reviews (n = 32), six papers were included (n = 6). Sixteen positive aspects of parenting a child with ASD were identified and grouped into three themes: joyful moments, journey to resilience, and social connection. These three themes are eudaimonic wellbeing constructs and often under-represented in ASD parental studies. Focusing on joyful moments, building resilience through the parenting journey, and finding support and connection with others in similar situations can support the mental wellbeing of parents and carers of children with ASD. Future research is needed to further evaluate the positive aspects of parenting a child with ASD.
... Several authors highlight the consequences of diagnoses, their role in stigmatizing and subjectivity-shaping (Corrigan 2007;Farrugia 2009), building homogenizing standardizations (Hull et al. 2017), and an incommensurable sense of otherness (Pinchevski 2005). Ways of thinking and acting autism that traversed practices and discourses in the field, yielding direct repercussions such as pharmacological interventions (Nadesan 2009), coerced institutionalization-particularly prevalent until the 1990s (Nadesan 2013)re-education processes and therapies to redirect behavior (Bumiller 2008). These endeavors have collectively shaped the reality for many individuals with autism, perpetuating a pervasive logic of violence that portrays autism as a pathological threat (McGuire 2016). ...
... Para la autora, estos discursos tienen fuertes inclinaciones eugenésicas y capacitistas. 5 Al respecto, puede consultarse:Nadesan (2005),Cascio (2018),Rios (2018) yGrinker (2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
En un guiño a la Crítica de la razón negra de Achille Mbembe (2016), el objetivo de este escrito es adelantar algunas discusiones para delinear lo que llamo la razón autista: las prácticas, discursos, dispositivos y tecnologías que han figurado al cuerpo autista como subhumano —o protohumano—, enfermo, trastornado, deficiente y enajenado de los otros cuerpos, seres, mundos y ambientes. Desde los estudios críticos animales, los estudios críticos latinoamericanos en discapacidad, las antropologías crip-lisiadas y el paradigma de la neurodiversidad en/desde el sur global, analizaré las imbricaciones del capacitismo y el especismo que están presentes en la fabricación de los cuerpos autistas como animal(izabl)es; así como las posibilidades de giro y subversión en la politización y la resistencia autista-animalista.
Article
In the 1940s, the child welfare and mental hygiene movements merged, leading to the emergence of the category of autism, as childhood became a focus of psychiatric surveillance. This resulted in stigma placed on parents through psychoanalytic explanations attributing autism to an inappropriate upbringing. A social movement by parents emerged as a response to challenge this medical perspective, coinciding with the deinstitutionalization and community transition trend of the mid-1970s, expanding the authority of therapists rather than physicians. The redefinition of autism was made possible through collaborative efforts between parents and therapists. Against the backdrop of mid-20th-century norms in middle-class child rearing, the concept of Asperger`s syndrome spread with heightened sensitivity to minor deviations or delays. Parent groups seeking support for autistic individuals without intellectual or language impairments, and their families engaged with autism researchers, who were also parents, to influence academia, leading to conceptual expansion and geneticization. These actions provided legitimacy to parents advocating for children with language impairments within the same genetic community. The broadening of the diagnostic criteria inevitably gave rise to a group of autistic self-advocates who could assert themselves without linguistic impairments. In the early 1990s, a neurodiversity movement emerged, aiming to depathologize autism and promote inclusive societal realization, in contrast to the medical model-focused parent groups. This movement expanded in various directions and, like previous parent groups, sought to influence academia' s authority over defining “what is autism.” While many activists agree with the medical definition describing the core features of autism, some challenge the current autism concept because it is considering social communication difficulties as individual traits. Empirically grounded studies supporting these trends are also emerging. Sociology, which has studied the normativity of social communication, can potentially align with the neurodiversity movement by describing micro-level communication styles and macro-level social orders that have been customized for neurotypicals and exclude autistics.
Article
The neurodiversity movement has so far been dominated by autistic people who believe their condition is not a disease to be treated and, if possible, cured, but rather a human specificity (like sex or race) that must be equally respected. Very few studies have been conducted to examine the significance of the neurosciences and the cerebralization of autistic culture for promoting these ideas. The article explores the role of the brain and the neurosciences in projects of identity formation as illustrated in the case of the emergence of the neurodiversity movement. The movement is driven by so called high functioning autistic individuals, who argue that autism is not a disease, but a form of human difference. The article examines the development of autistic individuals and autistic socialities as well as social and community networks. It also addresses the emergence of autistic cultures and various issues concerning autistic identities. It shows that the formation of identities is associated with the cerebralization of the condition. Facts about the brain are mobilized to depict autism positively. The move toward neurosciences has to be understood in a wider social context in which a brain-based vocabulary disseminates outside the lab and penetrates different domains of contemporary societies. This vocabulary is taken up by individuals and groups to speak about themselves and their relation with others as well as in their identity claims and fights for rights.
Article
Full-text available
A child’s autism diagnosis is life-changing for their parents, due to the complex and long-term nature of the condition. Furthermore, research about the increased prevalence of autism suggests that more parents are being affected globally. However, limited studies have investigated parents’ lived experience of their child being diagnosed with autism. This study aimed to explore the first-hand experience and concerns of parents when their child has been diagnosed with autism. The study employed a qualitative, exploratory design. Parents of a child who had received an autism diagnosis at least 12 months prior were invited to a video interview. Eleven parents participated in an hour-long interview. The interviews were transcribed, and an inductive reflexive thematic analysis was performed. The analysis resulted in four main themes: “Shock and control”, “A thousand little conversations”, “Put your own oxygen mask on first”, and “Reforged identities”, orientating around the period surrounding diagnosis, ongoing communication requirements, the need for self-care and reorientation of identity. These findings identify areas of heightened concern for parents and provide professionals with practical insight when evaluating their engagement approaches with these groups of parents. Findings can help psychologists, therapists, educators and parents themselves to identify strategies to support the well-being of parents who have an autistic child.
Chapter
In this chapter, I describe how I navigated a 2-year ethnography wherein I explored how autism was performed as an interactional event among children diagnosed with autism, the therapists who worked with them, and their parents. Thinking with critical disability studies, discursive psychology, and postcritical understandings of ethnography, I argue for the value of ethnographers engaging with critical conceptions of disability, where disability may even be imagined as desirable. I offer two descriptions of tension points in this work, drawing upon data, primarily fieldnotes and researcher journal entries to illustrate challenges I face in representing this work, and my engagement with crip horizons.
Thesis
Full-text available
This thesis explores autistic people’s experiences of sexuality, intimacy, and authenticity. The cultural and scientific contexts in which autistic people and autism researchers are embedded tend to assume deficit, limitation or damage when considering social, sexual, or intimate autistic possibilities. These assumptions are omnipresent and stated as fact in much mainstream autism research, casting a pall over discussions of sexuality, intimacy, authenticity, and indeed any inter- or intra-personal endeavour undertaken by an autistic person. Working within a framework of critical autism studies, and using a qualitative and participative methodology, the main thrust of this research involved a total of 24 in-depth interviews with 16 participants, leaning on Grounded Theory for data collection and analysis. An online survey with 567 respondents was also carried out, and a research website set up. A Research Advisory Group of nine autistic people provided input regarding appropriate research methods and instruments and piloted interviews, many communicating with me and each other through the research website’s private forum. Findings show that the challenges experienced by participants as they set out on their intimate journeys had less to do with intrinsic difficulties linked to ‘being autistic’ than with the point of encounter with a hostile and alienating environment. This environment is one in which difference of any kind is policed and punished, and sexual and gender identities and behaviours other than cis-gendered heterosexuality are rendered invisible or undesirable. For many participants, an intimate future was initially unimaginable. The ways in which they move to resolve these unpromising beginnings, however, reveal themselves to be interwoven with participants’ autistic subjectivity, involving, inter alia, intense interests, accessing and creatively navigating diagnosis, and meeting and loving other autistic people. Running through and expressed within all these themes is the whisper of authenticity. When authenticity is threatened, so are intimate possibilities; when it is recognised and nurtured, it is revealed to be a highly valued quality, a central part of what it means to participants to ‘be autistic’, and an important step on the path to accessing and maintaining satisfying, safe, and meaningful intimate relationships.
Article
Full-text available
Book
We become ill in ways our parents and grandparents did not, with diseases unheard of and treatments undreamed of by them. Illness has changed in the postmodern era—roughly the period since World War II—as dramatically as technology, transportation, and the texture of everyday life. Exploring these changes, this book tells the story, or stories, of what goes into making the postmodern experience of illness different, perhaps unique. Even as it decries the overuse and misuse of the term “postmodern,” it shows how brightly ideas of illness, health, and postmodernism illuminate one another in modern culture. Modern medicine traditionally separates disease—an objectively verified disorder—from illness, a patient's subjective experience. Postmodern medicine, the book says, can make no such clean distinction; instead, it demands a biocultural model, situating illness at the crossroads of biology and culture. Maladies such as chronic fatigue syndrome and post-traumatic stress disorder signal our awareness that there are biocultural ways of being sick. The biocultural vision of illness not only blurs old boundaries but also offers a new and infinitely promising arena for investigating both biology and culture. In many ways, the book leads us to understand our experience of the world differently.
Book
Originally published in 1990, this book looks at the history of developmental psychology in order to locate and evaluate the role played by biology in its most influential formulations. First Charles Darwin's own writings on child development are examined. It is shown that Darwin endorsed such ideas as the 'recapitulation' of evolutionary ancestry in the developing child, even though this is inconsistent with his natural selection theory. The first great developmentalists - Hall, Baldwin, Freud - adopted and applied these non-Darwinian evolutionist ideas. The next generation - Vygotsky, Piaget, Werner - applied similar ideas in a variety of ways. Alongside this evolutionism, but interconnected with it, sensationist/empiricist forms of epistemology were directing developmentalists (from Rousseau onwards) to see the child as having to work himself out of sense-bound experience - to develop further and further from the 'here-and-now'. Contemporary developmental theory retains these influences: biological approaches (ethological, psychobiological) remain pre-Darwinian in spirit; lifespan theories remain attached to biology; formal/cognitive approaches remain attached to sensationism. 'Social context' approaches are rather half-hearted, and it is only the social-constructionist orientation which seems to offer a real alternative to biology. Major conclusions are stated in chapter ten, which includes a re-evaluation of Darwin's role. © 1990 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. All rights reserved.
Article
When our children act up - whether they're just moody and rebellious or taking drugs and committing crimes - our solution, so often now, is to send them to a psychiatrist or developmental psychologist for help. What makes us think this will work? how did we come to rely on psychological explanations - and corrections - for juvenile misconduct? In this book, these questions lead to the complex history of "child guidance", a specialized psychological service developed early in the 20th century. Kathleen Jones puts this professional history into context of the larger culture of age, class, and gender conflict. using the records of Boston's Judge Baker Guidance Centre from 1920 to 1945, she looks at the relationships among the social activists, doctors, psychologists, social workers, parents and young people who met in the child guidance clinic, then follows the clinicians as they adapt delinquency work to the problem of nondelinquent children - an adaptation that often entailed a harsh critique of American mothers, Her book reveals the uses to which professionals and patients have put this interpretation of juvenile misbehaviour, and the conditions that mother-blaming has imposed on social policy and private child rearing to this day.
Article
Majia Holmer Nadesan analyzes the Fukushima nuclear disaster and its radiological aftermath for the citizens of Japan and elsewhere in the context of historical and contemporary understandings of radiation-caused health and reproductive effects.
Article
To date, research regarding the neurobiological basis of autism has produced only the most preliminary of data. Although some advances have been made, the need for more definitive knowledge is crucial to more fully understand and treat this disorder. In this review, the autistic spectrum is discussed in terms of its defining symptoms. These symptoms are explained based on current understanding, and the need for more information regarding the bases of these abnormalities is noted. The critical needs of research are discussed, with an emphasis on areas in urgent need of analysis.