ArticlePDF Available

How to support meta-cognitive skills for finding and correcting errors?

Authors:
  • Deutsches Forschungszentrum für Künstliche Intelligenz Saaarbrücken

Abstract

Meta-cognitive skills to be developed in learning for the 21st century is the detection and correction of errors in solu-tions. These meta-cognitive skills can help to detect errors the learner has made her/himself as well as errors others have made. Our investigations in learning from errors have the ultimate goal to adapt the selection and presentation to the learner so that he/she can better learn from erroneous examples others have made. In our experiments we found that (1) erroneous examples with help provision can promote students skill of find errors, (2) the benefit from erroneous examples depends on the relation between the student's level and the example's difficulty, i.e. if the student is prepared for the problem, (3) for many students it is very difficult to correct errors. Copyright ©2010, Association for the Advancement of Artificial Intelligence. All rights reserved.
How to Support Meta-Cognitive Skills for Finding and Correcting Errors?
Erica Melis
German Research Center for Artificial Intelligence (DFKI)
Stuhlsatzenhausweg 3 (Building D3 2)
D-66123 Saarbr¨
ucken
melis@dfki.de
Andreas Sander
Universit¨
at des Saarlandes
D-66123 Saarbr¨
ucken
andreas.sander@dfki.de
Dimitra Tsovaltzi
German Research Center for Artificial Intelligence (DFKI)
Stuhlsatzenhausweg 3 (Building D3 2)
D-66123 Saarbr¨
ucken
Dimitra.Tsovaltzi@dfki.de
Abstract
Meta-cognitive skills to be developed in learning for the 21st
century is the detection and correction of errors in solu-
tions. These meta-cognitive skills can help to detect errors
the learner has made her/himself as well as errors others have
made.
Our investigations in learning from errors have the ultimate
goal to adapt the selection and presentation to the learner so
that he/she can better learn from erroneous examples others
have made. In our experiments we found that (1) erroneous
examples with help provision can promote students skill of
find errors, (2) the benefit from erroneous examples depends
on the relation between the student’s level and the example’s
difficulty, i.e. if the student is prepared for the problem, (3)
for many students it is very difficult to correct errors.
Introduction
Some of the meta-cognitive skills to be developed in learn-
ing for the 21st century are the detection and correction of
errors in problem solutions. These meta-cognitive skills can
help to detect errors the learner has made her/himself as well
as errors others have made, e.g., errors hidden in solutions
on the web or in other instructions.
However, confronting students with erroneous solutions
of mathematical problems and asking them to find the error
is rather unusual in most schools (except Japanese schools)
and many teachers are ambivalent about – or even hostile to
– discussing errors in the classroom (Tsamir & Tirosh 2003).
Our ultimate goal is to use adaptive technology to help
students to learn by dealing with errors. That is, help stu-
dents learn mathematics by presenting erroneous examples
together with exercises in an adaptive fashion within the
TEL system ACTIVEMATH, a web-based learning environ-
ment for mathematics. We hope to improve students’ cog-
nitive competencies in math learning, as well as their meta-
cognitive competencies in error discovery, error correction
and self-monitoring.
Erroneous examples are worked solutions that include one
or more errors that the student is asked to detect and/or cor-
rect. Technology opens new possibilities of instruction with
erroneous examples. For example, erroneous examples can
Copyright c
2010, Association for the Advancement of Artificial
Intelligence (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved.
be presented in a variety of ways, with different kinds of
feedback, diverse tutorial strategies and diverse sequencing
of learning material. Adapting such features to the needs of
individual students could contribute to better learning.
We conjecture that erroneous examples will give students
the opportunity to find and reflect upon errors in a way that
will lead to deeper learning, while at the same time not
causing students to feel ashamed or demotivated, as is more
likely when their own errors are exposed.
During the last years we conducted a number of experi-
ments, some of them strictly controlled lab and classroom
studies, other more preliminary less controlled classroom
experiments, and a Wizard-of-Oz experiment.
Finding Errors in Erroneous Examples
Our experiments (Melis & Kriesell 2009; Tsovaltzi et al.
2009; 2010) tested whether erroneous examples in the do-
main of fractions can help students learn in the context
of Technology-Enhanced Learning (TEL). This could spare
students the embarrassment and demotivation of confronting
their own errors. We conducted lab studies and classroom
studies with students of different curriculum levels to mea-
sure the effects of learning through erroneous examples. We
summarize the results of these studies that compare learning
gains in three experimental conditions: a control condition, a
condition in which students were presented with erroneous
examples without help, and a condition in which students
were provided with help for error detection and correction.
Empirical Results
The erroneous examples’ benefit depends on the relation be-
tween the student’s level and the example’s difficulty, i.e.
the student’s readiness statement. and needs, (similar to the
results of investigations dealing with more vs. less interac-
tivity in (vanLehn et al. 2007)).
We hypothesize that the effect of erroneous examples de-
pends on when and how they are introduced to the students
and whether students are supported in finding and correcting
the error.
Design
Erroneous examples in ACTIVEMATH include instances of
typical errors students make, which address standard prob-
lems students face with rule-application, or errors which tar-
get common misconceptions and address more fundamental
conceptual understanding infractions. Erroneous examples
consist of two phases: error detection and error correction.
Based on pilot studies, we designed feedback for help-
ing students understand and correct the errors. There are
four types of unsolicited feedback: minimal feedback (flag
feedback), error-awareness and detection (EAD) feedback,
self-explanation feedback (realized as MCQs) and error-
correction scaffolds.
We conducted lab studies with 6th, 7th and 8th-graders
and school studies with 9th and 10th-graders. The partici-
pants came from both, urban and suburban, German schools.
All studies used three experimental conditions. The con-
dition No-Erroneous-Examples (NOEE) was the control and
included standard fraction exercises with minimal feedback
and the correct solution, but no erroneous examples in the in-
tervention. The condition Erroneous-Examples-With-Help
(EEWH) included standard exercises, and erroneous exam-
ples with provision of help (EAD, error detection/correction
MCQs, and error-specific help). The condition Erroneous-
Examples-Without-Help (EEWOH) included standard ex-
ercises, and erroneous examples but without additional
help. The design included a pre-questionnaire, a pretest,
a familiarization, an intervention, a posttest and a post-
questionnaire. The pre- and posttests were counterbalanced.
Results
In the studies with different levels of students we had results
that showed differences in how erroneous examples with
error detection and -correction help influence mathematics
learning. We found that, at least, more advanced students
(9th- and 10th-grade) benefit from erroneous examples with
help in terms of cognitive skills in general, as opposed to
erroneous examples without help, or no use of erroneous ex-
amples at all. We also found that erroneous examples as a
whole and error detection and -correction help can promote
conceptual understanding for the same students.
Erroneous examples can also influence the metacogni-
tive skill of error detection for less advanced (6th-grade) but
highly competent students.
We did not find evidence for the use of erroneous ex-
amples for medium-advanced students (7th- and 8th-grade).
However, this can be a result of the combined high level and
high competence which our participants had in comparison
to the students of the 9th and 10th-grade.
In those experiments we found that most students
do not have the meta-cognitive skill of correcting
errors. Why is this the case? Therefore, we
wanted to employ the Wizard-of-Oz (WOz) technique
(cf. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wizard of Oz experiment)
in order to find out more about reasons of correction weak-
nesses.
Correcting Errors in Erroneous Examples
In a WOz experiment we addressed the question, why many
students – even those who can find the errors or can solve
a (fraction) problem on their own – have great difficulties
when asked to correct the errors in erroneous examples. We
wanted to find reasons for these difficulties.
Experimental Set-Up
The experiment took place at a secondary school in
Heusweiler, Germany. 35 7th grade students (8 male and 27
female students) participated in this experiment and for each
students the experiment took 45 minutes. Before the main
experiment started, each subject received a short introduc-
tion about the user interface. During all experiments a total
of 102 instances of erroneous examples were processed and
for 65 instances, students detected the error at first request.
We used a WOz environment developed in our group in
which the student and the Wizard can communicate. The
Wizard (the second author) selected one to four predefined
erroneous examples. The selection criteria included: (1) not
too difficult to find the error, (2) errors are close to errors or
misconceptions the student made in the pretest. Always, the
first question to the subject was whether the computations in
the exercise are correct.
The Wizard’s plan was to first, ask the student why the
result cannot be correct, then the subject should show where
the error actually is and tell what the error is and why it is
an error. Then the student was supposed to correct the error
and simultaneously write down what was in her/his mind. In
cases where subjects had problems with this method during
problem solving, a retrospective protocol after the problem
solving process was used. Both the thinking aloud method
and retrospective protocol did not yet provide much input.
Results
At first, many students claimed to see a correct solution and
did not find the error at first try. This was the case for 47
out of the 102 instances. In 27 out of 65 cases the partici-
pants could not correct the error properly at all. That is, in
more than 40% of the cases, in which the students spotted
the errors, they could not correct them .
In the following, we present two typical interaction ex-
cerpts which were based on the erroneous example contain-
ing a wrong computation. The translation of the task in En-
glish is
The rain barrel: Ulli collects rain water in a rain barrel in
his garden and waters the plants in his garden. After several
days of rain the 320 liter barrel was filled. The day after the
rains Ulli asks: yesterday I had 320 liter in the barrel. Then I
used 5 1
2liter for my plants. How much is still in the barrel?
and he computes...
320 51
2
= 320
11
2
=320 11
2
=309
2
... did he compute correctly? Please answer this question
with ”YES” or ”NO”
Log-Excerpt 1
Original Log-Excerpt of a participant (behind the
German you find the English translation)
student:Ja En: yes
WOz:Nein, es wurde nicht richtig gerech-
net/argumentiert. En: no, the calcula-
tion/argumentation is incorrect
WOz:Warum kann das errechnete Ergebnis nicht
stimmen? En: why can the result not be correct?
student:Emm weil er die (320-11)/2 nicht soo rechnen
darf. En: emm, because you cannot compute like this
WOz:Genau, warum darf er das nicht? En: correct,
why must he not?
student:Weil er nicht die 320 auf die 11/2 mit drauf
setzten darf. En: because he must not put the 320 on
top of 11/2
WOz:Was muss er machen, damit er das auf einen
Bruchstrich schreiben darf? En: what should he do
before writing this in the nominator?
student:Er muss die Zahl 320 als Bruch umwandeln
entweder 320/1 oder soo un dann den Hauptnenner
rechnen. En: he needs to extend the number 320 to
the fraction 320/1 or so and then compute the common
denominator
WOz:Richtig! Du hast den Fehler gefunden! En:
Correct, you’ve found the error
WOz:Korrigiere nun (in deinem Eingabefenster) den
ersten fehlerhaften Schritt und f¨
uhre die Berechnungen
zu Ende. [...] En: now correct the first incorrect step
and complete the computation
student:keine ahnung En: no idea
WOz:Wie w¨
urdest du vorgehen, um den fehlerhaften
Schritt zu korrigieren? Beschreibe in Worten. En: what
would you do to correct the wrong step? describe in
your own words.
student:Also ich w¨
urde die 320/1 mit der 11/2
zusammen auf den Hauptnenner bringen dann das
minus rechnen. En: well, I’d compute the common
denominator of 320/1 and 11/2 and then substract
WOz:Richtig, versuche dies nun rechnerisch umzuset-
zen! En: correct, now try to compute this
WOz:Korrigiere nun (in deinem Eingabefenster) den
ersten fehlerhaften Schritt und f¨
uhre die Berechnungen
zu Ende. [...] Please correct (in the input window) only
the first incorrect step
student: wie bringt man es wieder auf den Hauptnen-
ner? En: how is the common denominator computed?
In this interaction the participant first answered, that all
computations in the exercise are correct, but then (after a
short hint) the student found the error. Nevertheless, the stu-
dent could not correct the mistake (compute the common de-
nominator and adapt the two fractions), because the student
did not know how the procedure works practically.
This is a good example for the dissociation between ex-
ecution (action) and monitoring (evaluation) or, the dis-
tinction between knowing that and knowing how (Ohlsson
1996), which we encountered in our earlier studies (Tso-
valtzi et al. 2010). The student knew that the fractions have
to have the same denominator before subtraction of two frac-
tions is possible. However, the student did not know how
to calculate a common denominator for the fractions. This
makes a big difference.
If a student knows what, he/she often only remembers the
(name of the) algorithm that needs to be applied. This does
not mean he/she knows exactly what to do/the single steps.
That is, the student may not even remember the definition
(rather than a vague notion) of a common denominator.
Log-Excerpt 2
Original Log-Excerpt of another participant
student:Ja En: yes
WOz: Nein, es wurde nicht richtig gerech-
net/argumentiert. En: no the solution is incorrect
student: lies dir die Aufgabe noch mal genau durch.
En: please read the problem again
Woz:Nein En: no
WOz:Warum kann das errechnete Ergebnis nicht
stimmen? En: why can the result not be correct?
WOz:Hast du Fragen? En: do you have questions?
student:nein En: no
WOz:Warum kann das errechnete Ergebnis nicht
stimmen? En: why can the result not be correct?
student:weil ulli die 320 liter auch in einen bruch
umrechnen muss En: because ulli needs to transform
the 320 liter into a fraction
WOz:Wo ist der Fehler? [...] En: where is the error?
student:Schritt 3;weil ulli die 320 liter auch in einen
bruch umrechnen muss En: step 3; because ulli needs
to transform the 320 liter into a fraction
WOz:Richtig! Du hast den Fehler gefunden! En:
correct! you found the mistake
WOz:Was genau hat er falsch gemacht? En: what
exactly did he make wrong?
student:Schritt 3;er hat die 320 liter nicht in einen
bruch umgerechnet. En: step3; he didn’t transform 320
liter to a fraction
WOz:Wie w¨
urdest du vorgehen, um den fehlerhaften
Schritt zu korrigieren? En: How would you proceed in
order to correct the incorrect step?
student:Schritt 3;ich w¨
urde aus den 320 liter 160/2
machen und dann die 11/2 davon abziehen En: step3;
I’d transform 320 liter to 160/2 and then substract 11/2
WOz: Wie w¨
urdest du vorgehen. Erkl¨
are mit deinen
eigenen Worten. Ohne genaue Rechnungen. En: how
would you proceed? explain in your own words.
without exact computations.
WOz:Hast du Fragen? En: do you have questions?
student:Schritt 3; ich w¨
urde die 320 liter nehmen und
in 160/2 liter umwandeln, dann die 5 1/2 liter in 11/2
liter um wandeln und dann die 11/2 liter von den 160/2
liter abziehen so w¨
are das ergebnis: 149/2 liter En:
step 3; I’d take the 320 liter and transform it to 160/2
liter, then transform the 5 1/2 liter into 11/2 liter, and
then substract 11/2 liter from 160/2 liter with the result
149/2 liter.
In this interaction the student found the mistake (subtrac-
tion of two fractions with different denominators without
first computing the common denominator) and had an idea
how to correct the error (expansion of 320 to a fraction with
the denominator two), which means he/she was a step ahead
but still could not complete the computation of the common
denominator, i.e., did not fully understand the principle of
expanding fractions to equal denominators..
Would understanding the principle conceptually allow
students to perform the necessary actions automatically?
What exactly it is that prevents them from correcting the er-
ror once they recognize it?
Related Work
Erroneous examples in mathematics have rarely been in-
vestigated or used as a learning intervention, either within
a technology-enhanced learning (TEL) system or within
the classroom. Overall, the related previous studies did
not target adaptation by the technology. The empirical re-
sults (sometimes inconclusive) evidence that studying errors
can promote student learning (Borasi 1994; Siegler 2002;
Curry 2004; Grosse & Renkl 2004; Monthienvichienchai &
Melis 2005; Grosse & Renkl 2007; Siegler & Chen 2008).
The skill of correcting errors was not addressed before.
A controlled comparison of the study of correct and incor-
rect examples was done by Siegler and colleagues (Siegler
2002; Siegler & Chen 2008). They investigated whether
self-explaining both correct and incorrect examples is more
beneficial than self-explaining correct examples only. They
found that when students studied and self-explained both
correct and incorrect examples they learned better. They
hypothesized that self-explanation of correct and erroneous
examples strengthened correct strategies and weakened in-
correct problem solving strategies, respectively. Grosse and
Renkl studied whether explaining both correct and incorrect
examples makes a difference to learning and whether high-
lighting errors helps students learn from those errors. Their
empirical studies in which no help was provided showed
some learning benefit of erroneous examples, but unlike the
less ambiguous Siegler et al results, the benefit they uncov-
ered was only for learners with strong prior knowledge and
for far transfer learning. Research in other domains, such as
medical education, has shown benefits of erroneous exam-
ples in combination with elaborate feedback in the acquisi-
tion of problem-solving schemata.
More relevant to the work presented in this paper is Ohls-
son’s theory (Ohlsson 1996) argues that errors occur due to
overgeneralization (applying rules to situations where they
do not apply). Error-detection requires declarative knowl-
edge (e.g.knowledge of rules) in the domain. The error
causes a conflict between this knowledge which the learner
believes to be true and what the learner perceives as the
current situation. The way to avoid errors is by special-
izing knowledge structures to trigger only in the situations
where they apply. Ohlsson distinguished between declar-
ative knowledge (knowing that) and practical knowledge
(knowing how), which he defines as the knowledge required
to choose the appropriate action in a perceive-decide-act cy-
cle.
The contribution of our work are empirical studies that
explicitly tackle this dissociation, in particular, in the do-
main of fractions. This in turn can inform the design of
erroneous examples and intruction not only in the context
of technology-enhanced learning, which we focus on, but in
general.
Conclusion
Our goal is a cognitive tool that supports learning from er-
ror, in particular from erroneous examples and that increases
monitoring alert. As the results show, the tool needs to
adapt to the student’s mastery level for sequencing and meta-
cognitive help provision.
This article describes results on our way to support stu-
dents in developing the meta-cognitive skills of error detec-
tion and error correction in ACTIVEMATH.
Acknowledgement This work was supported by Deutsche
Forschungsgemeinschaft in the project ME 1136/7-1.
References
Borasi, R. 1994. Capitalizing on errors as ”springboards for
inquiry”: A teaching experiment. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education 25(2):166–208.
Curry, L. 2004. The effects of self-explanations of cor-
rect and incorrect solutions on algebra problem-solving per-
formance. In D. Gentner, K. F., and Regier, T., eds., 26th
Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 1548.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grosse, C., and Renkl, A. 2004. Learning from worked ex-
amples: What happens if errors are included? In Gerjets, P.;
Elen, J.; Joiner, R.; and Kirschner, P., eds., Instructional de-
sign for effective and enjoyable computer-supported learn-
ing. Tuebingen: Knowledge Media Research Center. 356–
364.
Grosse, C., and Renkl, A. 2007. Finding and fixing er-
rors in worked examples: Can this foster learning outcomes?
Learning and Instruction 17:612–634.
Melis, E., and Kriesell, D. 2009. Effects of erroneous exam-
ples, results of a preliminary school experiment. In V. Dim-
itrova, R. Mizoguchi, B. d. B., and Graesser, A., eds., AIED
2009, volume 200 of Frontiers in Artificial Intelligence and
Applications, 578–580. IOS press.
Monthienvichienchai, R., and Melis, E. 2005. Implementing
courseware to support learning through real-world erroneous
examples: Student’s perceptions of tertiary courseware and
obstacles to implementing effective delivery through vle. In
Remenyi, D., ed., Proceedings of European Conference on
eLearning (ECEL-2005), 281–289. Academic Conferences
International.
Ohlsson, S. 1996. Learning from performance errors. Psy-
chological Review 103:241–262.
Siegler, R., and Chen, Z. 2008. Differentiation and inte-
gration: Guiding principles for analyzing cognitive change.
Developmental Science 11:433–448.
Siegler, R. 2002. Microgenetic studies of self-explanation.
In Granott, N., and Parziale, J., eds., Microdevelopment,
Transition Processes in Development and Learning. Cam-
bridge University Press. 31–58.
Tsamir, P., and Tirosh, D. 2003. In-service mathematics
teachers’ views or errors in the classroom. In International
Symposium : Elementary Mathematics Teaching.
Tsovaltzi, D.; Melis, E.; McLaren, B.; Dietrich, M.;
Goguadze, G.; and Meyer, A.-K. 2009. Erroneous exam-
ples: A preliminary investigation into learning benefits. In
Ulrike Cress, Vania Dimitrova, M. S., ed., European Con-
ference for Technology-Enhanced Learning, EC-TEL 2009,
LNCS 5794, 688–693. Springer-Verlag.
Tsovaltzi, D.; Melis, E.; McLaren, B.; Meyer, K.; Dietrich,
M.; and Goguadze, G. 2010. Learning from erroneous ex-
amples: When and how do students benefit from them? In ,
ed., EC-TEL 2010, volume - of LNCS, –. Springer-Verlag.
vanLehn, K.; Graesser, A.; Jackson, G.; Jordan, P.; and
C.P.Rose. 2007. When are tutorial dialogues more effec-
tive than reading? Cognitive Science 31:3–62.
... Research has found that collaboration in the mathematics classroom can be a successful approach to foster mathematics learning. A host of positive outcomes that include higher achievement ( Melis et al. 2010;Oner 2016), greater effort to learn (Yang et al. 2016), more positive relationships among students (Chaviaris and Kafoussi 2010) and more positive attitudes towards subject matter (e.g., Edwards and Jones 2003;Johnson and Johnson 1986;Rohrbeck et al. 2003;Roseth et al. 2006; Slavin 1995) have been associated with collaboration. Thus, there appears to be a consensus that collaboration should be included in classroom practices. ...
Article
Full-text available
Collaboration is an increasingly popular topic in mathematics education due to its potential to foster students’ learning. The purpose of this article is to draw attention to the semantic philosophical theory of inferentialism and its value for investigating students’ collaboration. We suggest that Brandom’s inferentialism can serve as a valuable theoretical resource to overcome certain issues of existing theoretical viewpoints on student collaboration. In particular, we argue that inferentialism may help to understand the individual and social nature of collaboration as intertwined. We illustrate our inferentialist approach using data from two scenes taken from video-recorded group work sessions from a fifth and seventh grade primary school class in Sweden. The topic in both classes was data generation in statistics.
... Thus, attributing the mistake to a fictitious student named "Sammy" is likely to have provided a non-threatening environment, and encouraged all students to express their ideas in the class discussion. This proposal echoes similar suggestions made by Melis, Sander & Tsovaltzi (2010). ...
Article
We compared the materialization of knowledge integration processes in class discussions that followed troubleshooting (TS) and problem-solving (PS) tasks and examined the impact of these tasks on students’ conceptual understanding. The study was conducted in two sixth-grade classes taught by the same teacher, in six lessons that constituted a third of a unit on simple electric circuits. In these lessons, one class was assigned PS tasks where students were asked to solve conceptual problems. Later they were asked to share their work in a class discussion. The other class was assigned TS tasks where students were asked to identify, explain, and correct the mistakes in “teacher-made” erroneous solutions to these same problems. They were also engaged later in a class discussion. We found that students’ performance on subsequent transfer problems was significantly higher for the TS class, in particular for students with low prior knowledge. We account for the difference in learning outcomes by the differences in the learning process: the TS tasks elicited more naïve ideas both in students’ worksheets as well as in class discussions, and the TS discussions involved more episodes where students developed criteria to discern scientifically acceptable and naive ideas. We also found differences in the format of students’ participation, where lower-achieving students participated in the TS discussions. The implications of these findings for future research are discussed.
... She offered a teaching approach based on problem exploration, errors, conflict, doubt and reflection, for elaborating on mathematical concepts and problem solving. Melis et al. [7,8] examined the pedagogical value of erroneous examples. She posed to students erroneous examples of others, both directly, and using ActiveMath -a webbased, user-adaptive learning environment. ...
Conference Paper
We developed an approach of learning from errors in CS1, taught in OOP. The approach is based on a set of principles, which include: cognitive conflict, constructivist characteristics, error attribution, self explanation, reflection, and refinement of concept images of fundamental OOP terms and mechanisms. The approach is presented, and some of its activities are illustrated. The developed activities aim at erroneous OOP conceptions that we have observed in a previous study. We applied the approach with CS1 college students, and conducted a comparative study, with an experiment group and a control group. We display a summary of the study's results, together with the students' opinions about the applied approach.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to examine prospective science teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge in terms of their approach to error. For this purpose, a data collection tool consisting of 5 questions for conceptual understanding of electrical circuits and steps containing correct and incorrect explanations for each question was prepared and applied to prospective science teachers. This study was carried out using case study method, one of the qualitative research approaches. The participants consisted of 30 prospective science teachers studying in 4th Grade in a state university. The data were analyzed using descriptive analysis method. The findings showed that the rate of prospective teachers who provided correct explanations in the solution of the questions about electrical circuits was high. In addition, the rate of detecting the solution steps given incorrectly was at a moderate level. It was revealed that very few participants who identified the wrong solution steps were able to make correct explanations for the solution of the question. Majority of the participants had some misconceptions such as “Generators are constant current sources”, “Local reasoning” and “Regardless of how the generator and light bulbs are connected in the circuit, if the number of generators increases, the brightness of the bulb increases”. These misconceptions were evaluated as an indication that prospective teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge was not at a level that could be considered sufficient in terms of understanding the student. In this context, error-based activities can be included in teaching in order to prevent the formation of misconceptions, incomplete and/or incorrect information in students or prospective teachers, and to correct existing misconceptions. The effects of error-based activities on individuals’ learning can be investigated in future studies.
Chapter
Understanding of the object concept in Object Oriented Programming (OOP) is obviously the center of the paradigm. Many educators and researchers explored students’ difficulties and developed teaching materials targeted at this central concept. The paper presents a diagnostic tool we developed that aims to reveal students’ perception and understanding about the current object, referring to it by the this annotation. Proper conceptualization of this indicates an understanding of objects in general, and involves aspects of memory allocation and programming approaches. The tool contains five questions, each devoted to covering different aspects in various frameworks, such as: using this in constructors, using this as a visible parameter, using this in inheritance, or making necessary changes in transition from a non-static context that uses this to a static context. The questionnaire combines closed questions with a request to explain the answers and open questions. In the paper we present the purpose of each question, and address what it comes to examine. The diagnostic tool is based on known educational approaches: Bloom’s taxonomy, assessment for, as, and of learning and learning from errors. The tool can be used by educators at high school or academic levels as a teaching tool, as a base for discussions, or as an evaluation tool. A short report on the use of the tool with different populations, including high school teachers, is presented. The paper uses Java as the programming language, but it easily can be translated to other OOP languages.
Article
Research indicates that learning from erroneous examples (EE) is superior to correct examples because errors may provide students with a stimulus to spontaneously produce more self-explanations, leading to better learning outcomes. However, because most studies were conducted in individual settings, it remains an open question whether the advantage of EE could be reproduced in collaborative settings. Therefore, this study used a 2 × 2 factorial design varying in example types (correct vs. erroneous examples) and learning settings (individuals vs. collaboration). An experimental study of a 2-week course on subtraction for third grade students was conducted (N = 109). Results indicated that collaborative learning may enhance the transfer knowledge of subtraction. The improvements were still maintained over 2 weeks. In addition, collaboration also facilitated students’ confidence in handling errors. However, there was no significant difference in promoting isomorphic and transfer knowledge between learning from correct and erroneous examples. Additionally, this study also explored the absence of the intended benefit of erroneous examples from a cognitive load perspective.
Article
Full-text available
Learning from worked examples is a very effective learning method. But what happens if er-rors are included? In two experiments it was explored if a combination of correct and incorrect worked examples enhances learning outcomes, and if incorrect examples are more effective when the error is highlighted. In addition, the effectiveness of fostering self-explanations was assessed. In Experiment 1, the participants learned to solve probability problems under six conditions that constituted a 2x3-factorial design (Factor "incorrect solutions": incorrect solutions with highlighting the error vs. incorrect solutions without highlighting the error vs. correct solutions; Factor "self-explanations": prompting written self-explanations vs. no prompting). It was found that incorrect solutions can foster far transfer performance if learners have favorable domain-specific learning prerequisites. Experiment 2 replicated this effect. Thus, incorrect examples can enhance learning outcomes, but only for "good" learners.
Article
Full-text available
Microdevelopment is the process of change in abilities, knowledge and understanding during short time-spans. This book presents a new process-orientated view of development and learning based on recent innovations in psychology research. Instead of characterising abilities at different ages, researchers investigate processes of development and learning that evolve through time and explain what enables progress in them. Four themes are highlighted: variability, mechanisms that create transitions to higher levels of knowledge, interrelations between changes in the short-term scale of microdevelopment and the crucial effect of context. Learning and development are analysed in and out of school, in the individual's activities and through social interaction, in relation to simple and complex problems and in everyday behaviour and novel tasks. With contributions from the foremost researchers in the field Microdevelopment will be essential reading for all interested in cognitive and developmental science.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
In this work, we investigate the e!ect of presenting students with common errors of other students and explore whether such erro- neous examples can help students learn without the embarrassment and demotivation of working with one's own errors. The erroneous examples are presented to students by a technology enhanced learning (TEL) sys- tem. We discuss the theoretical background of learning with erroneous examples, describe our TEL setting, and discuss initial, small-scale stud- ies we conducted to explore learning with erroneous examples.
Article
Although teachers and researchers have long recognized the value of analyzing student errors for diagnosis and remediation, students have not been encouraged to take advantage of errors as learning opportunities in mathematics instruction. The study reported here was designed to explore how secondary school students could be enabled to capitalize on the potential of errors to stimulate and support mathematical inquiry. The article provides a case study of the proposed strategy of "using errors as springboards for inquiry" in action, identifies some important variations within the strategy, and discusses its potential contributions to mathematics instruction.
Article
Various strategies such as self-explanation (Chi, 2000), collaborative problem solving (Ellis, Klahr, & Siegler, 1993), scaffolding (Vygotsky, 1978), reciprocal teaching (Brown & Palinscar, 1989), and learning from worked-out examples (Mwangi & Sweller, 1998), have been used successfully to facilitate learning and understanding. Psychologists are particularly interested in the cognitive processes underlying and affected by these methods, the varying effectiveness of each across different domains, and the mechanisms that are associated with the learning that results from the utilization of each. Although these techniques are different in form, each one encourages the student to engage in learning during which knowledge is actively processed, and mental models and schema are constructed and reconstructed. The goal of this study was to extend our knowledge of the mechanisms by which students acquire knowledge and the strategies that could be used to facilitate these processes. The effects of feedback and self-explanation have been examined under various conditions, and within various domains (Chi, de Leeuw, Chiu, & LaVancher, 1994; Mwangi & Sweller, 1998; Tudge, Winterhoff, & Hogan, 1996). Because both have shown to have advantageous effects under many circumstances, they were used together in this study of algebra problem solving. To extend prior research, both the self-explanation of correct and incorrect solutions was elicited and compared to the condition in which only the correct answer was self-explained. It was hypothesized that students who received feedback and were asked to explain both correct and incorrect solutions would demonstrate the most improvement in solving algebra word problems. Method Participants included 80 college students (60 females, mean age = 19.73 years, SD = 2.05), including 50 Caucasians, 12 African Americans, 10 Hispanics, 6 Asians, and 2 "Others". An algebra pretest consisting of 14 multiple-choice compare word problems (4 simple-direct, 5 simple-indirect, and 5 complex) was used to assess algebra problem-solving abilities. Participants then participated in a directed practice session during which they were randomly assigned to one of four experimental conditions (No feedback/"Explain own" (Control), Ambiguous feedback/"Explain own and alternative", Feedback/Explain correct, and Feedback/Explain correct and incorrect"). Students were asked to provide algebraic equations for each of 10 problems, and to explain why they thought these equations were correct (or incorrect). Finally, an algebra post-test, identical in form to the pretest, was administered.
Article
Learning from worked examples is an effective learning method in well-structured domains. Can its effectiveness be further enhanced when errors are included? This was tested by determining whether a combination of correct and incorrect solutions in worked examples enhances learning outcomes in comparison to correct solutions only, and whether a mixture of correct and incorrect solutions is more effective when the errors are highlighted. In addition, the effectiveness of fostering self-explanations was assessed. In Experiment 1, the participants learned to solve probability problems under six conditions that constituted a 2×3-factorial design (Factor 1: correct and incorrect solutions with highlighting the errors vs. correct and incorrect solutions without highlighting the errors vs. correct solutions only; Factor 2: prompting written self-explanations vs. no prompts). An aptitude-treatment interaction was found: providing correct and incorrect solutions fostered far transfer performance if learners had favourable prior knowledge; if learners had poor prior knowledge correct solutions only were more favourable. Experiment 2 replicated this interaction effect. Thus, a mixture of correct and incorrect solutions in worked examples enhanced learning outcomes only for “good” learners. In addition, Experiment 2 showed that confronting learners with incorrect solutions changed the quality of their self-explanations: on the one hand, new types of effective self-explanations could be observed, but on the other hand the amount of the very important principle-based self-explanations was substantially reduced. A possible measure to prevent this negative side effect of incorrect solutions is discussed.
Article
This paper presents a study in a UK university that investigated how first-year (freshman) Information Systems undergraduates perceive learning through courseware containing real-world erroneous examples derived from their peers and what obstacles had to be overcome to implement effective e-Learning support for using and creating such courseware. The study finds that students find the courseware very effective in dealing with their personal misconceptions while also providing other secondary pedagogic benefits for both students and lecturers.
Article
A theory of how people detect and correct their own performance errors during skill practice is proposed. The basic principles of the theory are that errors are caused by overly general knowledge structures, that error detection requires domain-specific declarative knowledge, that errors are experienced as conflicts between what the learner believes ought to be true and what he or she perceives to be the case, and that errors are corrected by specializing faulty knowledge structures so that they become active only in situations in which they are appropriate. A computer simulation model that embodies the theory learns cognitive skills in ecologically valid domains. The theory generates novel and testable predictions about error correction. It is also consistent with learning phenomena that are seemingly unrelated to errors, including transfer of training and the learning curve. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Differentiation and integration played large roles within classic developmental theories but have been relegated to obscurity within contemporary theories. However, they may have a useful role to play in modern theories as well, if conceptualized as guiding principles for analyzing change rather than as real-time mechanisms. In the present study, we used this perspective to examine which rules children use, the order in which the rules emerge, and the effectiveness of instruction on water displacement problems. We found that children used systematic rules to solve such problems, and that the rules progress from undifferentiated to differentiated forms and toward increasingly accurate integration of the differentiated variables. Asking children to explain both why correct answers were correct and why incorrect answers were incorrect proved more effective than only requesting explanations of correct answers, which was more effective than just receiving feedback on the correctness of answers. Requests for explanations appeared to operate through helping children notice potential explanatory variables, formulate more advanced rules, and generalize the rules to novel problems.