ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Lilliputian hallucinations are infrequent but reported in a variety of states ranging from toxic metabolic states, medical, neurological and psychiatric conditions. We report three cases with different psychiatric diagnoses all presenting witb vivid Lilliputian hallucinations. An attempt to apply the current biological mechanisms that explain complex visual hallucinations to Lilliputian hallucinations is made. The phenomenon of size constancy and a functional break between the visual cortex and association cortices explains the phenomena of micropsia, a sensory distortion. We propose whether this principle could also determine the form of hallucinations and produce Lilliputian hallucination.
Reprinted from the German Journal of Psychiatry · http://www.gjpsy.uni-goettingen.de · ISSN 1433-1055
CASE REPORT
Understanding a Strange Phenomenon:
Lilliputian Hallucinations
Prabhat K. Chand1, Pratima Murthy2
1Department of Psychiatry, Kasturba Medical College, Manipal-576104
2National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, Bangalore-560029, Karnataka, India
Corresponding author: Dr Prabhat K Chand, Department of Psychiatry, Kasturba Medical College, Manipal,
Udupi, Karnataka, 576104, India, Fax no +91 820 2571930, Email: prabhatkumar_chand@yahoo.com
Abstract
Lilliputian hallucinations are infrequent but reported in a variety of states ranging from toxic metabolic states, medi-
cal, neurological and psychiatric conditions. We report three cases with different psychiatric diagnoses all presenting
with vivid Lilliputian hallucinations. An attempt to apply the current biological mechanisms that explain complex
visual hallucinations to Lilliputian hallucinations is made. The phenomenon of size constancy and a functional break
between the visual cortex and association cortices explains the phenomena of micropsia, a sensory distortion. We pro-
pose whether this principle could also determine the form of hallucinations and produce Lilliputian hallucination
(German J Psychiatry 2007; 10: 21-24).
Keywords: Lilliputian Hallucination, schizophrenia, micropsia
Received: 27.7.2006
Revised version: 17.11.2006
Published: 23.1.2007
Introduction
illiputian hallucinations are formed complex visual
hallucinations of people, objects or animals greatly
reduced in size (Asaad and Shapiro 1986). The hallu-
cinations are vivid and evoke varied responses including fear,
anxiety or even pleasure (Leroy 1909; Yawgner 1925; Savit-
sky and Tarachow 1941). The earliest descriptions of this
strange phenomenon are attributed to Macarius, who in 400
AD described such hallucinations as ‘little strangers’ (Cohen
et al.1994). The widespread use of the term Lilliputian Hal-
lucination dates back to its description in Gulliver’s Travels.
Leroy (1909) introduced this term into the scientific litera-
ture.
Lilliputian hallucinations have been described in a variety of
conditions i.e. delirium, dementia, toxoplasmosis, basilar
migraine, mesencephalic lesions, schizophrenia and alcohol
withdrawal states (Harper and Knothe 1973; Lishman 1987;
Cohen et al.1994; Hendrickson and Adityanjee 1996; Taka-
oka and Takata 1999; Podoll and Robinson 2001). What
underlies the formation of these special types of complex
visual hallucinations in such diverse conditions? We attempt
to address this issue by first reporting three unusual cases
seen in our psychiatric practice, followed by a review at-
tempting to integrate literature from different specialties.
CASE REPORTS
Case 1
Mr. R, a forty-year married male, with 10 years of formal
education was brought to the emergency with complaints of
irritability, wandering and running away from home, abusive
and violent behavior of six months duration. History also
revealed hallucinatory behavior and gross decline in personal
care.
During serial mental status examinations, he complained of
hearing multiple male voices discussing him. He also re-
L
CHAND & MURTHY
22
ported that his thoughts were being broadcast in television
and radio but did not elaborate further.
In addition, the patient reported seeing small sized people
“looking like dolls” all over his food. He was very frightened
that these small sized people would choke him. He therefore
stopped taking food and on being forced, would vomit it
out. After a few days, he saw these ‘doll like’ people chasing
him and trying to mutilate him. He was very fearful and on
few occasions tried to run away from the ward. His mood
was flat. His orientation and cognitive functions were intact.
Patient’s past psychiatric history was unremarkable. Patient’s
mother had a history of post-partum psychosis, from which
she had recovered following treatment. His elder brother
had history suggestive of alcohol abuse.
General and systemic examination at the time of admission
did not reveal any significant abnormality. Baseline investiga-
tions including blood counts and serum biochemistry were
normal. Electroencephalography showed occasional sharp
waves in frontal areas. Magnetic Resonance Imaging of brain
did not reveal any pathological findings.
A diagnosis of paranoid schizophrenia was made and the
patient received tablet risperidone up to 8mg over the next
four weeks. As his response was poor, he was shifted to
olanzapine up to 20mg without significant change. He then
received a course of seven electroconvulsive therapies with
some behavioral improvement, but psychopathology per-
sisted. Inj. zuclopenthixol decanoate 200mg once in two
weeks was added with tablet olanzapine and patient showed
considerable improvement.
Case 2
Mr. B, a twenty-five year, middle class educated married
male was brought to hospital with excessive consumption of
alcohol and abnormal behavior. History revealed that he was
a regular drinker since four years and had increased the
amount of alcohol for the last two years. His drinking pat-
tern fulfilled International Classification of Disorders-10
criteria for alcohol dependence syndrome. His usual quantity
of daily drink was around one to one and half liters of alco-
hol (approximately 50 units).
Two weeks prior to admission, the patient had decreased the
amount to half to one fourth of regular intake. Following
this, his sleep had markedly reduced and he started seeing
little people all over the house. They were about a foot high,
with funny colorful dresses, weird faces, big eyes and
mouths. Some of them were also wearing spectacles. They
would follow him all around the house and he could hear
their footsteps. Patient would also see them drinking his
blood (did not elaborate further) and complained of physical
weakness as a result. Initially, patient attributed his experi-
ences to some evil spirits present in the house and changed
the house. But the experience continued. Informants did not
report any past or current history of withdrawal seizures or
any physical problems.
Mental status examination revealed that patient was partially
oriented to time and place. He complained of visual halluci-
nations of Lilliputian nature and secondary delusions of
persecution. He denied any other psychopathology and had
impaired insight. General physical examination showed
tremors, which were more marked in the extremities. Physi-
cal investigation showed raised liver enzymes. Computerized
Axial Tomography of brain was normal. His hallucinations,
along with other withdrawal symptoms subsided completely
within three to four days of abstinence along with tablet
lorazepam 8mg/day on first day and tapering down 2mg
every alternate day.
Case 3
Mrs. C, a case seen by author 2 in the UK, was an eighty-
year-old widow with an eighteen-month history of diminu-
tion of vision and visual hallucinations. She reported seeing
very short men, women and little girls wearing blue and
/gray leaves, whom she referred to as “blue-bottles”. They
would peep at her from cupboards and doors, or hide be-
hind the curtains or lie on the carpets. She would see them
eating yellow, black fish like creatures and they would enjoy
watching her undress. Sometimes she would see little black
cats roaming inside the house. Apart from these, she also
complained of multiple tactile and olfactory hallucinations.
She was initially afraid of the blue-bottles and could not sleep
at night.
Historical and cross sectional mental status examination did
not reveal any features suggestive of cognitive decline, mood
symptoms or Schneiderian first rank symptoms. Physical
examination revealed bilateral decrease in visual acuity i.e.
left eye 0.5ft, right eye one foot and osteoarthritis of left
knee. Computerized Axial Tomography scan of head re-
vealed age related changes.
On mental status examination patient reported visual hallu-
cinations and auditory hallucinations detailed earlier. She also
looked fearful and had ideas of persecution secondary to the
perceptual abnormalities. She had intact cognitive functions
with partial insight to her problem. She was diagnosed as
suffering from Charles Bonnet Syndrome. Patient was ini-
tially treated with tablet flupenthixol 1mg/day, thioridazine
50mg/day and diazepam5mg/day, which were later with-
drawn in next one week. She was detected to be suffering
from glaucoma with retinal artery occlusion and prescribed
timoptol eye drops by the ophthalmologist. Gradually her
visual hallucinations subsided dramatically both in frequency
and in severity. She was discharged after one month of hos-
pital stay.
Discussion
All the three patients reported here with diverse psychiatric
syndromes (schizophrenia, complicated alcohol withdrawal
and Charles Bonnet Syndrome) experienced visual hallucina-
tions of Lilliputian nature along with other psychopathology.
In the first case, the patient suffered from schizophrenia.
Visual hallucinations have been fairly frequently reported in
LILLIPUTIAN HALLUCINATIONS
23
schizophrenia, as much as in 72 % of patients interviewed,
along with hallucinations in other modalities (Bracha et al.
1989; Mueser et al. 1990). This contradicts conventional
thinking that visual hallucinations are indicative of underly-
ing structural damage on the one hand, and on the other,
supports that schizophrenia is a brain disease. Although
Lilliputian hallucinations were first described in schizophre-
nia by Lewis (1961) and have been subsequently reported
(Hendrickson and Adityanjee 1996), hallucinations of this
nature are rare in schizophrenia.
In case of alcohol withdrawal syndrome, illusions and hallu-
cinations have been reported in the literature. Rats, snakes
and other small animals of Lilliputian nature can appear in
vivid and colorful forms. They may have a lot of movement
i.e. dancing or playing are quite common (Lishman 1987). In
our index second case, patient had similar hallucinations of
seeing little funny people following him and demanding his
blood.
In the third case, an elderly lady with partially impaired vi-
sion without any cognitive dysfunction presented with Lilli-
putian hallucinations along with other perceptual abnormali-
ties. This clinical presentation is suggestive of Charles-
Bonnet Syndrome, which is characterized by vivid, complex
and recurrent visual hallucinations associated with eye pa-
thology in a cognitively intact person (Gold and Rabins
1989). The characteristic findings of the index case were that
Lilliputian hallucinations subsided with the treatment of
glaucoma.
The above three cases have three different diagnosis but
interestingly all presented with visual hallucinations of Lilli-
putian nature, suggesting a possible dysfunction or involve-
ment of either the same areas or pathways in the central
nervous system to produce similar phenomenology.
Complex Visual Hallucinations - Un-
derpinnings
Manfred and Andermann (1998) have published an extensive
review on the pathogenesis of complex visual hallucinations
occurring in different conditions i.e. hypnagogic and hyp-
nopompic states, peduncular hallucinosis, narcolepsy-
cataplexy syndrome, Parkinson’s disease, Lewy-Body De-
mentia, Charles Bonnet Syndrome, schizophrenia, delirium
tremens and epilepsy. In Charles Bonnet Syndrome, the
hallucinations occur in psychologically healthy individuals
who have recently become blind or have impaired vision.
These are called release hallucinations because it is thought
that they are ‘released’ or instigated by the ‘removal of nor-
mal visual afferent input into the association cortex”. It is
found that direct stimulation of visual cortex area in epilepsy
and loss of inhibition on this area in vascular stroke involv-
ing occipital area produce similar visual hallucinations. Most
of these conditions are associated with disturbance in the
sleep-wake cycle indicating involvement of thalamus and
fibers radiating to reticular activating system.
Another insight in understanding these condition has come
from the use of hallucinogenic substance i.e. LSD and mes-
caline, potent serotonin agonists (5HT2) which on intoxica-
tion produce similar complex hallucinations. Apart from
serotonin, GABA is also implicated in some of these condi-
tions (Manfred and Andermann 1998).
Extending the theory to Lilliputian
Hallucinations
Lilliputian hallucinations are also a type of complex visual
hallucinations and can be interpreted on the above theories.
At this point of time there is no separately defined neuro-
logical basis for this strange phenomenon.
Traditional phenomenology differentiates hallucinations
(without afferent sensory signals) from illusory percepts
(false percepts with afferent sensory signals) but it is unlikely
that these phenomenons have different neural substrates
(Ffytche and Howard 1999). On the same lines, we also
suggest that an analogy could be drawn between Lilliputian
hallucinations and the so called ‘Lilliputian Sight’ or microp-
sia which is a sensory distortion rather than a false percep-
tion.
Micropsia or the ‘Alice in Wonderland Syndrome’ where
objects are perceived substantially smaller than in reality is a
condition of altered perception where the mechanics of the
eye are not affected, only the brain’s interpretation of infor-
mation passed from the eyes (Takaoka and Takata 1999).
The gestalt psychologists have demonstrated that retinal
afterimages change their size depending on where the image
is projected (Emmert’s Law) (Emmert 1881). Micropsia, an
illusion is hypothesized as a dysfunction of the mechanism
that underlies the “size constancy” phenomenon. Size con-
stancy denotes the tendency of animals and humans to see
familiar objects as having standard shape, size, color, or
location regardless of changes in the angle of perspective,
distance, or lighting. The impression tends to conform to the
object as it is or is assumed to be, rather than to the actual
stimulus. Perceptual constancy is responsible for the ability
to identify objects under various conditions, which seem to
be "taken into account" during a process of mental reconsti-
tution of the known image. Even though the retinal image of
a receding automobile shrinks in size, the normal, experi-
enced person perceives the size of the object to remain con-
stant. Indeed, one of the most impressive features of per-
ceiving is the tendency of objects to appear stable in the face
of their continually changing stimulus features. Though a
dinner plate itself does not change, its image on the retina
undergoes considerable changes in shape and size as the
perceiver and plate move. What is noteworthy is stability in
perception despite gross instability in stimulation. Such
matches between the objects as it is perceived and the object
as it is understood to actually exist (regardless of transforma-
tions in the energy of stimulation) are called perceptual con-
stancies (Tovee et al. 1996).
Precise neurobiological details on the locus of the lesion
responsible for micropsia are lacking in most of the reported
cases. An overview of the literature reveals that the deficit
is much more common after damage of the visual associa-
tion cortex (Frassinetti et al. 1999). More recently, Cohen et
al. (1994) described two cases of hemimicropsia resulting
CHAND & MURTHY
24
from a lesion affecting the lower part of areas 18 and 19 and
the underlying white matter. In the patients described by
Ceriani et al. (1998), who complained of seeing objects
smaller than they were in the whole visual field, the lesion
apparently involved the right temporoparietal cortical junc-
tion and the occipital white matter.
It is proposed that anything that produces a functional im-
balance between intact and damaged visual areas is likely to
produce micropsia (Bender and Teuber 1949). If this ex-
plains the dysfunction of size constancy for objects in the
external objective space, the same theory can explain deci-
sions of size the hallucinating brain makes, due to functional
disconnections between primary and association visual corti-
ces. So hallucinations of Lilliputian nature might be de-
scribed as micropsia for internally generated images or per-
cepts. After all, hallucinations themselves are a behavioral
manifestation of brain function, just as is interpreting reality
the way we do. The different conditions that produce Lilli-
putian hallucinations, all have evidence suggestive of under-
lying neurobiological disturbances, the details of which are
still poorly understood.
We have attempted to postulate a common mechanism for
strikingly similar phenomena in three different conditions.
More studies in the form of functional neuroimaging are
necessary to understand and establish the pathophysiology
of this strange phenomenon.
References
Asaad G, Shapiro B. Hallucinations: theoretical and clinical
overview. Am J Psychiatry 1986; 143:1088-1097.
Bender M, Teuber HL. Psychopathology of vision. In
Spiegel (Ed) Progress in Neurology and Psychiatry.
New York: Grune and Stratton: pp163-192, 1949.
Bracha HS, Owen MW, Lohr JB, Karson CN, Bigelow LB.
High prevalence of visual hallucinations in research
subjects with chronic schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry
1989; 146: 526-528.
Ceriani F, Gentileschi V, Muggia S, Spinnler H. Seeing
objects smaller than they are: Micropsia following
right temporo-parietal infarction. Cortex 1998; 34:
131-138.
Cohen MA, Alfonso CA, Haque MM. Lilliputian Hallucina-
tions and Medical Illness. Gen Hosp Psychiatry
1994;16: 141-143.
Emmert E. Größenverhaltnisse der Nachbilder. Klin Mbl
Augenheilk 1881;19: 443-450.
Ffytche DH, Howard RJ. The perceptual consequences of
visual loss: ‘Positive’ pathologies of vision. Brain
1999;122: 1247-1260.
Frassinetti F, Nichelli P, Pellegrin GD. Selective horizontal
dysmetropsia following prestriate lesion. Brain
1999;122: 339-350.
Gold K, Rabins PV. Isolated visual hallucinations and the
Charles Bonnet Syndrome: a review of the literature
and presentation of six cases. Compr Psychiatry
1989; 30: 90-98.
Harper RW, Knothe BV. Coloured Lilliputian Hallucinations
with amantadine. Med J Aust 1973;1: 444-445.
Hendrickson J, Adityanjee. Lilliputian hallucinations in
schizophrenia: Case report and review of literature.
Psychopathology1996; 29: 35-38.
Leroy R. Les hallucinations lilliputiennes. Ann Med-psychol
1909;10: 278-289.
Lewis DJ. Lilliputian hallucinations in the functional psycho-
ses. Can Psychiat Ass J 1961; 6:177-201
Lishman WA. Organic Psychiatry: The psychological conse-
quences of cerebral disorder. Ed 3rd Blackwell Sci-
ence, London. 1987.
Manford M, Andermann F. Complex visual hallucination:
clinical and neurobiological insights. Brain 1998;
121:1819-1840.
Mueser KT, Bellack AS, Brady EU. Hallucinations in
schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1990; 82: 26-29.
Podoll K, Robinson D. Recurrent Lilliputian hallucinations
as visual aura symptom in migraine. Cephalgia 2001;
21 :990-992.
Savitsky N, Tarachow S. Lilliputian hallucinations during
convalescence of scarlet fever. J Nerv Ment Disord
1941; 93: 310-311.
Takaoka K, Takata T. ‘Alice in Wonderland’ Syndrome and
Lilliputian hallucinations in a patient with substance-
related disorder. Psychopathology 1999; 32: 47-49.
Tovee MJ, Rolls ET, Ramachandran VS. Rapid visual learn-
ing in neurones of the primate temporal visual cortex.
Neuroreport 1996; 7: 2757- 2760.
Yawgner NS. Diminutive visual hallucinations in a hysterical
convict. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1925; 14: 64-65.
The German Journal of Psychiatry · ISSN 1433-1055 · http:/www. gjpsy.uni-goettingen.de
Dept. of Psychiatry, The University of Göttingen, von-Siebold-Str. 5, D-37075 Germany; tel. ++49-551-396607; fax:
++49-551-392004; e-mail: gjpsy@gwdg.de
... Metamorphopsias or Lilliputian visual hallucinations, so named in honor of the imaginary island of Liliput described in the book "Gulliver's Travels" by J. Swift in 1726, include micropsia or macropsia, depending on whether objects appear smaller or larger, teleopsia if objects appear distant, pelopsia if objects appear close, and zoopsia if visions of animals occur. There may also be alterations in the perception of time (slow-fast) and the distortion of sounds that can either be perceived as distorted or amplified [53,60]. Lastly, visual or auditory hallucinations and dyschromatopsia can be part of the AIWS. ...
... Todd also defined hyperschematia as a condition of left-sided distortion with the perception of the disproportionate expansion of space towards the left [61]. Memory loss, emotional instability, anxiety, abnormal movements of limb, persistence of hearing sounds have been also described [60]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Migraine is a complex neurological disorder with partially unknown pathophysiological mechanisms. The prevalence in childhood ranges from 7.7% to 17.8%, thus representing the most frequent primary headache. In half of the cases, migraine is accompanied or preceded by various neurological disturbances, among which the visual aura is the best known. In literature, other conditions, such as Alice in Wonderland Syndrome and Visual Snow syndrome, are characterized by visual manifestations and are often associated with migraine. The aim of this narrative review is to describe the spectrum of visual disturbances in pediatric migraine and their pathophysiological mechanisms.
... These alterations in velocity perception can lead to severe, overwhelming disorientation. 29 Individuals can also experience sound distortion, such that soft sounds may appear amplified or even a misinterpretation of common sounds. This can often lead a person with Alice in Wonderland syndrome to experience paranoia. ...
... Other less commonly experienced symptoms may include loss of limb control and discoordination, memory loss, lingering touch and sound sensations, and emotional instability. 29 Patients who experience Alice in Wonderland syndrome are at times reticent about discussing their hallucinations for fear of being labeled with a psychiatric disorder. J. J. Corbett, a neuro-ophthalmologist, had noted that patients with these types of complex illusions during their migraine headaches were hesitant about describing their symptomatology until he reassured them. ...
Article
Alice in Wonderland syndrome is a disorienting neurological condition that affects human perception to the senses of vision, hearing, touch, sensation, and the phenomenon of time. Individuals affected with Alice in Wonderland syndrome can experience alterations in their perception to the size of objects or their own body parts, known as metamorphopsias. It is known to occur in conditions including migraine, epilepsy, as well as certain intoxicants and infectious diseases. The name refers to Lewis Carrol's well-known children's book Alice's Adventures in Wonderland, in which the title character experiences alterations of sensation in which she felt that her body had grown too tall or too small, or parts of her body were changing shape, size, or relationship to the rest of her body. The syndrome was described in 1952 by Caro Lippman, and given its name in 1955 by John Todd. The metamorphopsias characteristic of this condition are also sometimes referred to as Lilliputian hallucinations, as a reference to the fictional island of Lilliput in the novel Gulliver's Travels, written by Jonathan Swift in 1726. As such, many literary and medical publications have roots in the description of this syndrome. The purpose of this review is to summarize the literary and historical significance of Alice in Wonderland syndrome, as well as to provide the reader with a medical overview of the condition.
... These changes in speed perception can lead to disorientation in individuals.These patients may perceive sounds differently than they actually are. In addition, these patients may experience memory loss, problems with limb control and coordination disorders, perceptual disorders related to the senses of touch and sound, and emotional instability (Chand & Murthy, 2007). ...
Article
Alice in Wonderland Syndrome is characterized by distortions in perception, especially with signs of micropsia or macropsia, peropia, teleopsia.The etiology of this syndrome includes migraine attacks, epilepsy, infections or substance abuse.In this case study, a 15-year-old adolescent girl was admitted to our child and adolescent psychiatry clinic with complaints of seeing objects and people as larger or smaller than they are and/or perceiving objects closer or farther than they are, before her migraine attacks.It was also stated that his father had similar complaints during his adolescence..In this case report, we present a case of migraine with aura.
... Visual hallucinations in schizophrenia are perceived as vivid experiences and usually involve normal-sized people and objects; however, rarely visual hallucinations of giants and Lilliputians have been reported. Some researchers explained AIWS through a functional disconnection between primary and association visual cortices as a result of schizophrenic neurobiology and circuits (Chand and Murthy, 2007). ...
... [1] We describe a unique resizing of Lilliputian and Brobdingnagian hallucinations in a case of pemphigus vulgaris. [2] ...
Article
Full-text available
Pemphigus vulgaris is an autoimmune disorder characterized by intraepithelial, blistering lesions affecting the skin and mucous membrane. Psychiatric manifestations of pemphigus vulgaris are generally described secondary to steroid and immunosuppressant therapy though recent studies highlight independent association of pemphigus with psychotic disorders. We describe a unique development of Lilliputian hallucinations, their transformation into Brobdingnagian hallucinations on treatment with steroids and gradual resolution back to Lilliputian status on treatment with risperidone. Earlier Lilliputian hallucinations have been described in a case of Charles Bonnet syndrome, Balint syndrome, alcohol withdrawal delirium, head injury and dementia. This unique phenomenon carries it significance in the literature from psychopathological perspective.
... For example, during Lilliputian hallucinations, very small people are hallucinated and seem to be seen in the environment that the subjects otherwise accurately perceive. For example, small people might be experienced as on the carpet, or peeking out from behind the curtains, or on the subject's food (Chand and Murthy 2007). ...
Chapter
This chapter seeks to establish whether the cognitive penetration of experience is compatible with experience having nonconceptual content. Cognitive penetration occurs when one’s beliefs or desires affect one’s perceptual experience in a particular way. This chapter examines two different models of cognitive penetration and four different accounts of the distinction between conceptual and nonconceptual content. It argues that one model of cognitive penetration—‘classic’ cognitive penetration—is compatible with only one of the accounts of nonconceptual content that It identifys. The chapter then considers the other model of cognitive penetration—cognitive penetration ‘lite’. It provides reasons to think that this is compatible with three accounts of nonconceptual content. Moreover, it argues that the account of nonconceptual content that it is not compatible with is a spurious notion of nonconceptual content that ought to be abandoned. Thus, it claims that cognitive penetration lite is compatible with all reasonable specifications of nonconceptual content.
... See e.g. Chand and Murthy (2007).) Such a person might be able to learn what was real and what was hallucinated by learning that her hallucinations were only of little people, and knowing that she was unlikely to be in an environment that contained such little people. ...
Book
Reflection on the nature of hallucination has relevance for many traditional philosophical debates concerning the nature of the mind, perception, and our knowledge of the world. In recent years, neuroimaging techniques and scientific findings on the nature of hallucination, combined with interest in new philosophical theories of perception such as disjunctivism, have brought the topic of hallucination once more to the forefront of philosophical thinking. Scientific evidence from psychology, neuroscience, and psychiatry sheds light on the functional role and physiology of actual hallucinations; some disjunctivist theories offer a radically new and different philosophical conception of hallucination. This volume offers interdisciplinary perspectives on the nature of hallucination, offering essays by both scientists and philosophers.Contributors first consider topics from psychology and neuroscience, including neurobiological mechanisms of hallucination and the nature and phenomenology of auditory-verbal hallucinations. Philosophical discussions follow, with contributors first considering disjunctivism and then, more generally, the relation between hallucination and the nature of experience. Contributors István Aranyosi, Richard P. Bentall, Paul Coates, Fabian Dorsch, Katalin Farkas, Charles Fernyhough, Dominic H. ffytche, Benj Hellie, Matthew Kennedy, Fiona Macpherson, Ksenija Maravic da Silva, Peter Naish, Simon McCarthy-Jones, Matthew Nudds, Costas Pagondiotis, Ian Phillips, Dimitris Platchias, Howard Robinson, Susanna Schellenberg, Filippo Varese. © 2013 Massachusetts Institute of Technology. All rights reserved.
... See e.g. Chand and Murthy (2007).) Such a person might be able to learn what was real and what was hallucinated by learning that her hallucinations were only of little people, and knowing that she was unlikely to be in an environment that contained such little people. ...
Article
This first chapter introduces the topics to be discussed in this book. Few phenomena have played such a vital role in shaping philosophical theories as hallucination, especially theories in the areas of the philosophy of mind, perception, and epistemology. The traditional philosophical conception includes perceptual experiences, identical in nature to experiences that could be had while perceiving the world, except only that they are had while not perceiving. Two emerging strands of research have brought to light different conceptions of hallucination. One of these is scientific evidence about people who actually hallucinate. Evidence from psychology, neuroscience, and psychiatry has shed light on the functional role and physiology of actual hallucinations. The second strand is the development of a philosophical theory of perception known as disjunctivism. Some disjunctivist theories have as part of their ontology a radically new and different conception of hallucination. It is with these issues this book engages.
Book
Full-text available
'Malingering' is Chapter 5 of a recently launched online forensic psychiatry textbook. It is the initiative of Prof. Sean Z. Kaliski, the previous head of the Forensic Mental Health Service, Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town and Valkenberg Hospital. The title of the book is 'Forensic Mental Health: From Assessment to Recovery'. Published by UCT, and edited by Sean Kaliski. With kind acknowledgements to Prof Kaliski for editing this chapter. eBook accessible at https://vula.uct.ac.za/access/content/group/9c29ba04-b1ee-49b9-8c85-9a468b556ce2/Forensics/index.html.
Article
Full-text available
We describe a patient (P.S.) who, following a right prestriate lesion, reported that objects in the left visual field appeared distorted and smaller than those on the right. Other aspects of visual processing were remarkably unaffected. We carried out a series of size comparison tests using simple or complex stimuli and requiring different types of behavioural responses. We found that P.S. significantly underestimated the size of stimuli presented in her left visual field. When comparison tasks involved stimuli placed along the vertical axis or in the right visual field, P.S. performed well. The vertical and horizontal components of size distortion were found to be differentially affected. We conclude that size processing may be dissociated from other aspects of visual processing, such as form or colour processing, and depends critically on part of the occipital, prestriate areas (Brodmann areas 18–19).
Article
Full-text available
The authors examined the prevalence of visual hallucinations in severely ill hospitalized (NIMH intramural) research subjects with carefully diagnosed chronic schizophrenia and found it to be high. A chart review of 100 discharged subjects revealed documentation of visual hallucinations in 32% . A prospective examination of 43 additional subjects revealed a history of visual hallucinations in 56% (N = 24). Also, ------------> THE FACT THAT IN 43% OF THE PATIENTS WITH VISUAL HALLUCINATIONS THE HISTORY OF VISUAL HALLUCINATIONS WAS FIRST DOCUMENTED DURING THE [INTRAMURAL NIMH] RESEARCH WARD WORK-UP , SUGGESTS THAT CLINICIANS FREQUENTLY DO NOT INQUIRE ABOUT VISUAL HALLUCINATIONS IN PATIENTS WITH CHRONIC SCHIZOPHRENIA. . See the comments which include Clinically useful informal observations about olfactory and tactile hallucinations. The presence of which should count heavily against the diagnosis of schizophrenia.
Article
We report the case of an 84-year-old lady who, after a right temporo-parietal infarction, complained of seeing things smaller than she expected. She also related that straight lines appeared distorted and described seeing colours as if they were a badly mixed assemblage of hues. Her visual field was normal except for a transient left field extinction. No spatial neglect emerged. The patient's micropsia remained unchanged during the course of the six month follow-up.
Article
The prevalence of different types of hallucinations and their clinical correlates were examined in 117 DSM-III-R schizophrenic or schizoaffective disorder patients. Auditory hallucinations were by far the most common, followed by visual hallucinations, and then by tactile and olfactory or gustatory hallucinations. Auditory hallucinations were associated with an earlier age of first hospitalization among the schizophrenics. Global severity of the illness for schizophrenics was related to the presence of visual hallucinations, but not other types of hallucinations. Tactile and olfactory or gustatory hallucinations were strongly correlated with each other and with the severity of delusions for both schizophrenic and schizoaffective patients. The results suggest that important clinical differences exist between patients with different types of hallucinations, and that these clinical variables need to be controlled for in cross-cultural studies of hallucinations.
Article
Vivid visual hallucinations without other psychopathology have been reported for several hundred years. However, there is no consensus on whether these hallucinations should be considered a separate syndrome, the Charles Bonnet syndrome, or whether eye disease or brain disease is a necessary or exclusionary criterion for diagnosing this syndrome. In an attempt to resolve these issues, this article reviews the literature on isolated visual hallucinations and presents follow-up data from six cases. Criteria for diagnosing the Charles Bonnet syndrome are suggested.
Article
The authors review the literature on hallucinations; provide theoretical background on these phenomena from physiological, biochemical, and psychological points of view; and discuss the presentations of hallucinations in different diagnostic categories. The longstanding notion that hallucinations are to be equated with schizophrenia, they conclude, is clearly unfounded, and hallucinations are never pathognomonic of any given disorder but can be relatively specific for some conditions. Current knowledge and methods of research have produced no single mechanism to account for the etiology or pathogenesis of hallucinations. The authors present an integrated approach toward viewing the etiology and clinical presentation of hallucinations that involves concepts of biological vulnerability and psychological influences.
Article
Lilliputian hallucinations have been described in patients with delirium, schizophrenia, seizure disorders, visual disturbances, and brain tumors. The authors report two cases of patients with lilliputian hallucinations, one with AIDS-dementia complex and the other with dementia following head trauma. This is the first time that lilliputian hallucinations have been described in association with such medical conditions.
Article
A patient with chronic schizophrenia who had visual hallucinations of lilliputian nature is described. The authors review the studies on prevalence of visual hallucinations in schizophrenic patients and suggest that visual hallucinations may be more common in schizophrenia than previously acknowledged. The authors suggest a routine enquiry about visual hallucination in clinical practice and emphasize the need for further research under controlled conditions.