Content uploaded by Andrew J King
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Andrew J King on May 13, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
<Verso Running Head>Section III / Research and Applications Hearing and
Auditory Function in Ferrets
<Recto Running Head>Research and ApplicationsChapter 29 / Hearing and
Auditory Function in Ferrets
Hearing and Auditory
Function in Ferrets 29
Wellcome Trust
WT076508AIA
Fernando R. Nodal and Andrew J. King
In recent years, ferrets have become increasingly popular as an animal model for
studying hearing, and are now used for this purpose in a number of laboratories
around the world. There are several reasons for this. First, as with their ability to
see, the functional onset of hearing takes place in ferrets several weeks after birth,
allowing postnatal access to an immature nervous system at a stage that can only
be studied in utero in some other more precocial species. Second, the range of
sound frequencies that are audible to ferrets (approximately 20 Hz–40 kKHz)
entirely overlaps, and extends beyond, that of humans (Fig. 29.1). In contrast to
many rodents, particularly mice, whose audible range is shifted to higher
frequencies, the sensitivity of ferrets to low- frequency sounds allows the study of
various aspects of hearing, including pitch perception, sound localization, and the
processing of communication calls, within a similar frequency range to that used
by humans. Finally, because of their inquisitive nature and intelligence, ferrets can
be readily trained to carry out a variety of auditory behavioral tasks, which has led
to the development of paradigms for measuring their ability to detect, discriminate,
and localize sounds. Moreover, for some aspects of hearing, their performance in
these tasks closely resembles that of human listeners, providing an impetus for
using ferrets to investigate the neural basis of auditory perception as well as the
effects of hearing loss and its restoration.
Behavioral Studies of Hearing in Ferrets
The first auditory behavioral study in ferrets used a positive conditioning paradigm
to measure the range and sensitivity of the animals to tones of different frequency
[1]. They were trained to initiate a trial by touching a spout positioned in front of a
central start platform and to approach a peripheral water spout located at either 30°
to the right when they heard a sound or to the left when they heard no sound.
Correct responses were reinforced by the delivery of a water reward. The same
principle was then adopted to measure <termDef>minimum audible
angle</termDef>s (<abbrev>MAA</abbrev>s), i.e.that is, the ability of ferrets to
discriminate between sounds presented from one of two speakers separated by
progressively smaller angles in the horizontal plane [2,3]. Subsequent studies
expanded this to a 12-speaker task, measuring both the accuracy of the approach to
target responses and of the initial head orienting response made following sound
presentation [4]. Ferrets are very easy to train in this type of task and it has even
proved possible, using a left-–right response paradigm, to measure MAAs in the
vertical plane [5,6].
The horizontal plane MAAs measured in ferrets are comparable to with those
reported for other carnivores [7]. Their spatial acuity varies with the location of
the stimulus from ∼10° to –15° when animals are tested with brief noise bursts
around the midline to ∼25–30° at more lateral locations [3,8]. This spatial
dependence in localization accuracy is also seen when ferrets are trained to
approach the source of 1 of 12 loudspeakers arranged at 30° intervals in the
horizontal plane [4]. The accuracy of approach-to-target responses declines as the
duration of the stimulus is reduced (Fig. 29.2A,B), particularly for lateral and
posterior target locations, although responses to sound, presented in the frontal
region of space and directly behind the animal, remain largely unchanged (Fig.
29.2C,E). Sound-evoked head movements made by the animal before it leaves the
central start platform have a latency of ∼200 milliseconds ms and vary
systematically in magnitude with the direction of the stimulus (Fig. 29.2D,F). In
contrast to the approach-to-target behavior, the accuracy of the head orienting
responses is less affected by changes in stimulus duration (Fig. 29.2B), suggesting
that these are triggered by the onset of the sound and that the improved
performance observed when animals are approaching longer duration sounds may,
at least, in part, be due to re-sampling of the stimulus after the initial head turn has
been made.
The ability of animals to localize sound depends on the geometry of the head and
external ears [9]. Because of the physical separation of the ears on either side of
the head, sounds originating from one side will arrive at the closer ear first.
Depending on their wavelength, they may also be attenuated at the far ear as a
result of the acoustic shadow cast by the head, while interactions with the folds of
the external ears can modify the spectral shape of the sound. Localization of a
sound source therefore relies on the sensitivity of the auditory system to a
combination of <termDef>interaural time difference</termDef>s
(<abbrev>ITD</abbrev>s), i<termDef>interaural level difference</termDef>s
(<abbrev>ILD</abbrev>s), and spectral cues. In order to understand the basis for
localization behavior in ferrets, closed-field techniques have been developed to
measure their sensitivity to binaural cues [10]. This involves attaching earphones
to lightweight titanium holders that are mounted on the animal’'s head, so that they
can be easily disconnected and consistently repositioned just in front of the ear
canals (Fig. 29.3A). Ferrets are extremely sensitive to both ITDs and ILDs (Fig.
29.3B,C), with thresholds that can largely account for their ability to localize
stimuli in the horizontal plane [11]. Indeed, their behavioral sensitivity to these
cues closely resembles that found in other species, including humans, confirming
the suitability of ferrets as a model for studying spatial hearing.
Ferrets are also used to investigate aspects of sound identification. Although little
is currently known about how they perceive their own vocalizations (Fig. 29.4),
their ability to discriminate the periodicity and timbre of artificial vowels that
cover the same frequency range has been measured. Vowel identification involves
picking out the formant peaks in the spectral envelope of the sound, and is
therefore a timbre discrimination task. Ferrets readily learn to distinguish different
vowels and, like humans, are able to generalize across a range of stimulus
periodicities and to maintain their behavior in the presence of background noise
[12].
The periodicity of a sound corresponds to its perceived pitch and conveys
information about speaker identity (males tend to have lower pitch voices than
females), age, and emotional state. Because pitch is a perceptual attribute and is
not related in a straightforward way to the physical properties of sound, it is hard
to demonstrate conclusively that other species experience pitch in the same way as
humans. Nevertheless, there is now plenty of evidence that non-human animals are
sensitive to the same periodicity cues as humans [13]. For example, ferrets are
sometimes able to spontaneously distinguish harmonic complex tones, which, in
humans, have a clear pitch associated with them, from inharmonic tones [14], and
can be trained to discriminate the relative pitch of pairs of tones that either rise orf
fall in frequency [15]. Walker et al. [16] showed that ferrets can be trained to
discriminate two consecutive artificial vowels by indicating whether the pitch of
the second sound was higher or lower than that of the first (Fig. 29.5). In this case,
however, their thresholds were much poorer than those found in humans and other
species. Moreover, the animals appeared to perform the two2-alternative forced
choice task by attending to the absolute pitch height of the second sound, rather
than the direction of the pitch change between the two vowels. This does not
mean, however, that ferrets are unsuitable species to use for studying pitch
perception, since much lower thresholds have been found when they perform a
go/no-go change detection task.
Finally, auditory temporal processing has been examined behaviorally in ferrets by
measuring their thresholds for detecting short gaps in a sound (Ref. 40Kelly et al.,
1996), while other studies have tested their hearing abilities in more complex
acoustic environments [17–20]. This work has shown that, like humans, ferrets use
binaural cues to help them pick out sounds against other sounds originating from
different directions in space [17], can suppress echoes when localizing sounds
[19], and appear to be able to perceptually organize sound sequences into auditory
streams [18].
Organization of the Auditory System
The anatomy of the ear and the organization of the central auditory pathway of the
ferret follow the same principles that have been described in other mammals.
Although relatively little research has been carried out on the middle ear or inner
ear in this species, other than in the context of studying the effects of upper
respiratory infections (e.g., Ref. [21]), a great deal is known about how its
immobile external ears modify incoming sounds to provide spectral cues for
source location [22–24]. All the auditory nuclei or relay stations in the brain,
which have been characterized in numerous mammalian species (for a review of
the neuronanatomy of the auditory system, see Ref. [25]), are well developed and
easily distinguishable in the ferrets (Fig. 29.6). Thus, after leaving the cochlea, the
hearing part of the inner ear, the auditory nerve fibers enter the <termDef>cochlear
nucleus</termDef> (<abbrev>CN</abbrev>) in the brainstem [26,27], which in
turn sends bilateral projections to the <termDef>superior olivary
complex</termDef> (<abbrev>SOC</abbrev>) [28]. This is where convergence of
information from both ears is registered for the first time. Both the CN and SOC
project to the <termDef>inferior colliculus</termDef> (<abbrev>IC</abbrev>) in
the midbrain for further signal processing [26,29]. The IC and other brainstem
areas provide the principal auditory input to the <termDef>superior
colliculus</termDef> (<abbrev>SC</abbrev>) [30,31], another midbrain nucleus.
The IC also projects to the medial geniculate body or auditory thalamus [32],
which constitutes the gateway to the auditory cortex located in the
<termDef>ectosylvian gyrus</termDef> (<abbrev>EG</abbrev>) (Fig. 29.6B,C).
Some species-specific differences have been noted in these anatomical studies,
such as the proportion of ipsi versus contralateral projections from the lateral
superior olive to the IC in ferrets and cats (Moore et al., 1988). Overall, however,
the connectivity between the different auditory regions of the ferret’'s brain is
broadly consistent withhich thoseat found in other species.
In addition to the afferent connections from cochlea to cortex, the auditory
pathway is characterized by extensive efferent or descending connections (Fig.
29.6C), the size of which often outweighs those carrying information in the
ascending auditory pathway. The functions of these descending pathways remain
unclear, but they are thought to modulate the processing of acoustical signals at
lower levels in the pathway [33]. As in other species, descending projections in
ferrets link the auditory cortex with the IC [34] and SC [35], and the SOC with the
cochlea [36]. As we shall discuss later in this chapter, by combining methods for
selectively lesioning these pathways with behavioral testing, studies in ferrets have
provided the first functional evidence for the role that descending connections
might play in hearing [36,37].
Physiological Studies of Auditory Processing in Ferrets
The functional organization of the central auditory pathways has been studied by
measuring the responses of neurons in each region to a variety of sounds
(reviewed in Refs. [9 and ,38]). The animal species most commonly used are
guinea pigs, gerbils, and cats, with macaque monkeys and marmosets being the
subject of many of the studies on auditory cortical processing. The use of ferrets in
this field dates back to the 1980s [1,39,40], and has focused mainly on the
midbrain and cortex. Indeed, the only investigation so far of the response
properties of neurons at more peripheral levels was carried out by Sumner and
Palmer [41], who described the sound frequency tuning, spontaneous firing rate,
relationship between firing rate and sound level, and phase locking of auditory
nerve fibers (Fig. 29.7). To a large extent, the coding of information about sound
frequency and level by auditory nerve fibers reflects the properties of the cochlear
inner hair cells to which they are connected. Perhaps surprisingly, Sumner and
Palmer [41] found that the auditory nerve in the ferret more closely resembles that
of the guinea pig or chinchilla than that of the cat.
Several studies have examined the response properties of neurons in the IC of the
ferret (e.g., Refs. [39, 42, and 43]), sometimes as part of a wider investigation of
how the representation of natural sounds changes at higher levels of the auditory
system (Schnupp and King, 1997) [44,45]. This species has also been used
extensively for investing the auditory response properties of neurons in the SC
[46]. Unlike other parts of the auditory pathway, the spatial receptive fields of SC
neurons are organized topographically to form a map of auditory space, which lies
in spatial register with the representations of other sensory modalities. This
arrangement allows the different sensory signals associated with a particular object
to be registered at the same location, and often by the same neurons, within the
SC, so that they can trigger orienting movements that help to redirect attention
toward the object [47]. Although studies of how multisensory inputs interact to
determine the responses of SC neurons have been carried out primarily in other
species [48], experiments in ferrets have helped to reveal how a map of auditory
space is constructed in the brain from the localization cues generated by the way
sounds interact with the head and external ears (King et al., 1994) [23,49].
In recent years, interest in the ferrets as a model for auditory cortical processing
has greatly increased. A common feature of all sensory systems is that they
comprise multiple cortical areas that can be defined both physiologically and
anatomically, and which are collectively involved in the processing of the world
around us. As in other mammalian species, a number of different auditory cortical
fields with distinct functional properties are found in the ferrets [50–52] (Fig.
29.6B). The middle portion of the EG comprises the primary auditory cortex (A1)
and the <termDef>anterior auditory field</termDef> (<abbrev>AAF</abbrev>):
the neurons found there have short latency responses that are most sensitive to
particular sound frequencies, which vary systemically in value with neuron
location within each cortical area to form “tonotopic” maps. There is little doubt
that an equivalent area to the region designated as A1 is found in many different
mammalian species, including humans, and the same is probably true of AAF.
Drawing homologies between the other fields described in the ferret auditory
cortex and those described in other species has, however, proved to be less
straightforward. Two more tonotopically organized, secondary cortical areas are
found on the posterior EG, and the neurons found there can be distinguished from
those in the primary areas by the temporal characteristics of their responses.
Finally, two further fields have been described in the anterior EG, where neurons
respond to sound but lack the tonotopic order that is characteristic of the rest of
auditory cortex.
Each of the auditory fields in the ferret cortex has also been shown to receive
visual inputs, which are likely to arise from different parts of the visual cortex
[53]. In fact, the region labeled in Fig. 29.6B as the <termDef>anterior ventral
field</termDef> (<abbrev>AVF</abbrev>) is probably more accurately
designated a higher- level multisensory area, since it contains a particularly high
incidence of visually responsive neurons and lies close to a somatosensory field
[54]. Other reports have also highlighted the multisensory nature of neurons in the
anterior EG [55,56]. Inputs from other sensory modalities to auditory cortex have
also been observed in several a growing number of other species too, including
primates [57]. Although their functions are not well understood, they appear to
enhance the capacity of cortical neurons to signal the location [58] or identify
identity [59,60] of of a sound source, and are therefore likely to contribute to the
well known effects of vision on auditory perception.
Recording studies in ferrets have provided novel insights into the way in which
various sound attributes, including periodicity, timbre, and spatial location are
represented in the cortex [61–63], and have also contributed to our understanding
of how the brain encodes natural sounds such as speech (Rabinowitz et al., 2013)
[64,65]. Using the same artificial vowel sounds used to measure pitch and timbre
perception in the behavioral studies described in an earlier section of this chapter,
Bizley et al. [62] found that sensitivity to both spatial and non-spatial sound
attributes is widely distributed across different cortical fields. Regional differences
do exist, however, with the firing patterns of neurons in certain areas being more
informative about the periodicity or timbre of the sound, whereas those in other
areas are more sensitive to its location in space. This is broadly consistent with
evidence in other species for distinct cortical processing streams for different
aspects of the auditory scene [66,67]. At the same time, the ferret recordings have
revealed that these stimulus attributes are not only coded by different spatial
patterns of activation but also by different temporal patterns within the same
population [63]. In other words, the firing rate of a neuron can be modulated by
the identity (i.e., spectral timbre) of an artificial vowel sound in one time window
and the periodicity of the vowel in a later time window. This is known as
“multiplexing” and allows the neurons to carry mutually invariant information
about these two perceptual features. In keeping with this neurophysiological
finding, ferrets were found to detect changes in vowel identity faster than they
detected changes in the periodicity of these sounds [63]. This cortical processing
strategy potentially allows for a more dynamic representation of perceptual
attributes or even the acquisition of representations of new attributes that become
relevant or are learned through behavioral training.
The primary reason for exploring the response properties of neurons in the brain is
to attempt to identify the neural basis for perception. One approach to this is to
make quantitative comparisons of the psychometric performance of an animal in a
given detection or discrimination task with “neurometric” measures of how well
single neurons or neural ensembles can detect a change in the stimulus. Using this
approach, Bizley et al. [68] found that relatively small networks of neurons
throughout the auditory cortex of anesthetized ferrets can account for the ability of
trained animals to detect the direction of a pitch change. This appears to be at odds
with studies in other species that have reported the existence of a localized pitch
center in the cortex, where neurons are tuned to different periodicities [69]. This
remains controversial, however, and establishing a more direct link requires
making simultaneous behavioral and electrophysiological recordings from the
same subject. This approach has mainly been restricted to larger animals,
particularly non-human primates, and has rarely been used in the auditory system.
Recently, however, it has been demonstrated in ferrets trained to perform a pitch
discrimination task that auditory cortical activity represents not only the
periodicity of the sound, but also the pitch that the animals appear to hear as
assessed by their behavioral responses [70].
In order to demonstrate that a given area of the brain is involved in a particular
aspect of perception, it is also necessary to show that silencing or manipulating
neural activity in that area alters the animal’'s behavior. A commonly used
approach is to lesion the brain region in question, which, in ferrets, has been used
to show that the SOC [71], IC [8], and auditory cortex [2,72] are both all necessary
for accurate sound localization. These results support the widely accepted idea that
although the different cues for sound localization are extracted and computed in
the brainstem, higher-level areas, and the auditory cortex, in particular, play a
critical role in determining the location of a sound source. A reversible
pharmacological inactivation method that utilizes subdural placement of a polymer
to release the GABAA agonist muscimol over a period of weeks has been
developed in ferrets [73], and used to confirm that both primary- and higher- level
auditory fields contribute to localization accuracy when animals make their
responses by approaching the source of the sound for a fluid reward [73,74]. The
accuracy of the initial head orienting response is preserved, however, following
aspiration lesions [72] or pharmacological inactivation [73], suggesting that the
neural circuitry involved in mediating these responses may be different.
Development and Plasticity of the Auditory System
Interest in the use of ferrets for studying the auditory system was driven initially
by their suitability for developmental studies. Compared withto most other
mammals, ferrets are particularly immature at birth and do not begin to hear until
near the end of the first postnatal month (Moore and Hine, 1992). Despite their
relative immaturity, young ferrets are robust and highly suitable for
electrophysiological recording experiments. Several studies have examined the
maturation of auditory circuits at different ages (Gabriele et al., 2000; Henkel
et al., 2007) [31], or have documented how the response properties of auditory
neurons change following hearing onset. For example, recordings from the SC
[75] and auditory cortex [76] have shown that the spatial tuning of the neurons is
much broader in young ferrets than it is in adults. It turns out this can be explained
by changes in the localization cue values that take place as the head and ears grow
(Schnupp et al., 2003). Thus, presenting infant ferrets with stimuli via earphones
that replicate how sounds are filtered by the head and ears of adult animals led to
an immediate sharpening of the spatial receptive fields (Fig. 29.8). At the same
time, there is considerable evidence that central auditory circuits undergo
experience-dependent changes during development and studies in ferrets have
provided valuable insights into this process.
One commonly used approach for investigating the plasticity of auditory circuits
in the brain is to change the balance of inputs from the two ears, either by ablating
the cochlea or by introducing a reversible conductive hearing loss or some other
manipulation on one side. Unilateral cochlear removal in ferrets results in an
increase in the number of neurons that project from the CN on the non-operated
side to the ipsilateral IC [77], and an increase in the proportion of IC neurons that
are excited by the intact ear [78]. This anatomical reorganization is also seen when
ferrets are raised with one ear occluded by an earplug [79].
Although comparable results have been obtained in other species, these studies
have been extended in ferrets to examine the functional consequences of hearing
loss at different ages. Plugging one ear, either in infancy or adulthood, results in an
impairment in binaural hearing when assessed by measuring the ability of ferrets
to detect a tone in the presence of noise originating from other directions, with
binaural unmasking gradually recovering following earplug removal (King et al.,
2000) [80] (Fig. 29.9). While these findings provide little indication of any
adaptation to the partial loss of hearing in one ear, a different result was obtained
when the ability of adult ferrets that had been raised with a unilateral earplug was
measured (King et al., 2000) [24]. Even though the earplug was still in place, these
animals were able to localize sounds accurately (Fig. 29.10A) and the auditory
spatial tuning of neurons in the SC adjusted so that a near-normal map of auditory
space emerged (Fig. 29.10B). This adaptive plasticity does not involve a retuning
of neurons to the altered binaural cues, but is instead brought about by developing
a greater dependence on the unchanged spectral localization cues provided by the
intact ear [24].
Plasticity of auditory spatial processing in the SC has also been demonstrated by
manipulating other sensory cues. As mentioned in an earlier section of this chapter,
a characteristic feature of the organization of the SC across different species is the
presence of topographically aligned sensory maps for each of the modalities
represented there. Bringing together spatial signals that are registered by different
sense organs is challenging, but studies in ferrets and barn owls have been at the
forefront in showing that vision, which generally provides more precise and
reliable spatial information, plays an instructive role in merging multisensory
spatial information during development through its influence on the maturation of
the auditory space map (reviewed in Ref. [81]) (Fig. 29.11). These studies suggest
that visual inputs to the SC might provide a template for guiding the development
and plasticity of the auditory responses (King et al., 1988) [30,82], so that
information from the different senses can be synthesized appropriately.
Dynamic Neural Coding and Plasticity in the Adult Brain
A growing number of studies have demonstrated that sensory systems can adjust
their sensitivity to compensate for the often considerable changes in input statistics
between different sensory scenes. Studies in ferrets have shown that the response
properties of neurons in the IC [42] and auditory cortex [83–85] are not fixed, but
can change rapidly over time. For example, cortical neurons rescale their gain to
compensate for the contrast between a sound and its background: when the
contrast is low, the gain of the neurons increases so that they became more
sensitive to level changes, but when contrast is high, they become less sensitive
[83,84]. The stimulus values to which auditory neurons are tuned can also change
in response to the recent history of stimulation [42,86].
Another remarkable example of the plasticity of the adult brain has been
demonstrated by studies in ferrets showing that the <termDef>spectrotemporal
receptive field</termDef>s (<abbrev>STRF</abbrev>s) of auditory cortical
neurons can change rapidly when animals are engaged in a behavioral task
[87,88]. In these experiments, ferrets were trained by aversive conditioning to
refrain from licking a spout when they detected a target tone or a change in the
frequency of the sound in order to avoid a mild electric shock. The selectivity of
the neurons was found to change rapidly, sharpening the representations of the
stimuli that were relevant to the specific demands of the task. These changes,
which were readily observed after just a few trials and sometimes persisted for
several hours [88], are thought to reflect the influence of attention on cortical
processing. However, the nature of the task is not the only factor that determines
how the STRFs change. Its consequences are also important, as experiments in
which ferrets were trained to make different behavioral responses in the same
sound discrimination task have revealed [89] (Fig. 29.12). The animals either had
to suppress their licking behaviourbehavior to avoid a mild electrical shock or
were rewarded for licking a spout when they correctly identified the target sound.
Changes in the STRFs were observed in both paradigms, but occurred in opposite
directions with responses to the target sound becoming enhanced in the avoidance
task, but suppressed when the animals were rewarded. Both changes promote
better discriminability, but it seems that the responses of cortical neurons to stimuli
associated with aversive outcomes are selectively amplified.
Plasticity of cortical responses is also associated with the perceptual improvements
associated with long-term training. The sensitivity of neurons to the trained
stimulus can be enhanced altered in various ways. While some studies have
focused on changes in the proportion of neurons that respond to behaviorally
relevant aspects of the sound (e.g., Refs. [90 and ,91]), recordings in ferrets have
shown that learning on a complex sound discrimination task is associated with
increases in the amount of information that cortical neurons transmit about these
stimuli in their temporal firing patterns [92]. Moreove,r, behavioral studies in adult
ferrets have demonstrated that the capacity of the auditory system to compensate
for abnormal spatial cues is not restricted to a sensitive period of development. As
in humans, pPlugging one ear changes the binaural cue values corresponding to
each direction in space and results in a profound deficit in sound localization,
which is characterized by an increase in the number and magnitude of the errors
made with a notable bias towards the side of non-occluded ear. If provided with
appropriate training, however, adult ferrets with wearing a unilateral earplug
rapidly recover their ability to localize sound, with the extent and rate of that
recovery being determined by the frequency with which they are trained [93] (Fig.
29.13A). This capacity to compensate for a conductive hearing loss in one ear has
considerable implications for rehabilitation strategies following the loss or
restoration of hearing, and led directly to studies that demonstrated an equivalent
capacity for learning-induced plasticity of spatial hearing in humans [94].
In keeping with the physiological evidence in other species that perceptual
learning is associated with changes in the response properties of neurons in early
sensory cortex, the ability of adult ferrets to adapt to the altered spatial cues
produced by plugging one ear is lost if the auditory cortex is silenced [72,74] (Fig.
29.13B) or if cholinergic inputs to the cortex from the basal forebrain are removed
[95]. The finding that adaptation depends on cholinergic neuromodulation implies
a possible role for attention in this process and is consistent with other work,
suggesting that acetylcholine plays a critical role in sensory processing under
challenging conditions. The experience-dependent recalibration of sound
localization in adult ferrets has also been used to demonstrate the behavioral
importance of descending projections in the auditory system. Thus, unilateral
ablation of the projection from A1 to the IC by chromophore-targeted laser
photolysis impaired the spatial plasticity that normally occurs after plugging one
ear (Fig. 29.13C), without affecting the ability of the animals to localize sound
under normal hearing conditions [37]. Behavioral adaptation by adult ferrets to a
unilateral earplug also depends on the integrity of the olivocochlear bundle, the
descending projection from the SOC, where sensitivity to binaural localization
cues is first derived, to the cochlea [36].
Cochlear Implant Model
With the advent of cochlear implants, it is possible to partially restore hearing in
the deaf by directly stimulating the inner ear. Although traditionally fitted in one
ear only, bilateral implantation is becoming more common, particularly in
children, in an attempt to improve sound localization and hearing in noisy
environments, which both rely heavily on having two functioning ears. However,
results from patients have been quite variable and it is likely that age at onset of
deafness, duration of implant use, and targeted auditory training can influence the
performance of cochlear implant users. There is, a therefore, a pressing need for
animal models that can be used to investigate both the behavioral and
physiological outcomes of cochlear implantation and the importance of experience
in restoring auditory function. Although a range of species have been used to study
the effects of intracochlear electrical stimulation, the size of the speech processors
that are currently available commercially restricts their use in behavioral studies to
larger mammals, such as primates or carnivores.
The development of a ferret model of cochlear implantation [96] (Fig. 29.14) has
been driven by the substantial amount of physiological and behavioral data
available on numerous aspects of hearing in this species, and, in particular, by the
evidence described in the previous sections that ferrets are particularly suitable
animals for investigating the development and plasticity of auditory function.
Protocols have been developed for deafening ferrets with aminoglycoside
administration, followed by implantation and chronic stimulation of intracochlear
electrical arrays in one or both ears. By connecting the electrodes to clinical
processors via modified stimulator-–receivers worn within a custom-made jacket
(Fig. 29.14), behaviorally -relevant ILDs and ITDs can be provided during chronic
stimulation, which are used by the animals to orient towards acoustic stimuli [96].
These experiments provide a unique opportunity for investigating the role of
experience and training in establishing and maintaining auditory spatial abilities,
and should also contribute to the improvement of neuroprosthetic therapies in
humans.
Concluding Remarks
In this chapter, we have attempted to provide an overview of our current
knowledge about hearing in ferrets and how this species is contributing to
advances in our understanding of many aspects of auditory function. Thirty years
ago, the viability of the ferret as a model of auditory processing was questioned
(D.R. Moore, personal communication), whereas today they are one of the most
commonly used species for studying the higher auditory system. It is likely that
the popularity of the ferret as a model for auditory research will continue to
increase thanks to the technical advances that allow neural activity to be recorded
in freely moving, behaving animals. The prospect of combining the wide
repertoire of behavioral tasks that can be performed by ferrets with chronic
recording of neural activity will undoubtedly further our understanding of complex
functions like such as speech recognition or scene analysis and is likely to be key
to revealing the nature of the changes that give rise to highly prevalent conditions
such as tinnitus. Moreover, the advent of new ways of controlling neural activity
by light through the use of optogenetics, which are now being used in a range of
species, including ferrets, is rapidly expanding the methods available for studying
neural circuits, and may pave way toward new treatment possibilities for a range
of neurological conditions.
Acknowledgements
The authors’' research is supported by a Wellcome Trust Principal Research
Fellowship (WT076508AIA) to AJK.
References
1. <journal>Kelly JB, Kavanagh GL, Dalton JC (1986) Hearing in the ferret
(Mustela putorius): tthresholds for pure tone detection. Hear Res 24: 269–
275.</journal>
2. <journal>Kavanagh GL, Kelly JB (1987) Contribution of auditory cortex to
sound localization by the ferret (Mustela putorius). J Neurophysiol 57: 1746–
1766.</journal>
3. <journal>King AJ, Parsons CH (1999) Improved auditory spatial acuity in
visually deprived ferrets. Eur J Neurosci 11: 3945–3956.</journal>
4. <journal>Nodal FR, Bajo VM, Parsons CH, Schnupp JW, King AJ (2008)
Sound localization behavior in ferrets: ccomparison of acoustic orientation and
approach-to-target responses. Neuroscience 154: 397–408.</journal>
5. <journal>Parsons CH, Lanyon RG, Schnupp JW, King AJ (1999) Effects of
altering spectral cues in infancy on horizontal and vertical sound localization by
adult ferrets. J Neurophysiol 82: 2294–2309.</journal>
6. <journal> Bizley JK, Nodal FR, Parsons CH, King AJ (2007) Role of auditory
cortex in sound localization in the midsagittal plane. J Neurophysiol 98: 1763–
1774.Bizley JK, Nodal FR, Bajo VM, Nelken I, King AJ (2007) Physiological and
anatomical evidence for multisensory interactions in auditory cortex. Cereb Cortex
17: 2172–2189.</journal>
7. <journal>Heffner RS, Heffner HE (1992) Visual factors in sound localization in
mammals. J Comp Neurol 317: 219–232.</journal>
8. <journal>Kelly JB, Kavanagh GL (1994) Sound localization after unilateral
lesions of inferior colliculus in the ferret (Mustela putorius). J Neurophysiol 71:
1078–1087.</journal>
9. <book>Schnupp J, Nelken I, King A (2010) Auditory neuroscience: mmaking
sense of sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.</book>
10. <journal>Nodal FR, Keating P, King AJ (2010) Chronic detachable
headphones for acoustic stimulation in freely moving animals. J Neurosci Methods
189: 44–50.</journal>
11. <journal>Keating P, Nodal FR, Gananandan K, Schulz AL, King AJ (2013)
Behavioral sensitivity to broadband binaural localization cues in the ferret. J Assoc
Res Otolaryngol 14: 561-572 (in press).</journal>
12. <journal>Bizley JK, Walker KM, King AJ, Schnupp JW (2013) Spectral
timbre perception in ferrets: ddiscrimination of artificial vowels under different
listening conditions. J Acoust Soc Am 133: 365–376.</journal>
13. <journal>Walker KM, Bizley JK, King AJ, Schnupp JW (2011) Cortical
encoding of pitch: rrecent results and open questions. Hear Res 271: 74–
87.</journal>
14. <journal>Kalluri S, Depireux DA, Shamma SA (2008) Perception and cortical
neural coding of harmonic fusion in ferrets. J Acoust Soc Am 123: 2701–
2716.</journal>
15. <journal>Yin P, Fritz JB, Shamma SA (2010) Do ferrets perceive relative
pitch? J Acoust Soc Am 127: 1673–1680.</journal>
16. <journal>Walker KM, Schnupp JW, Hart-Schnupp SM, King AJ, Bizley JK
(2009) Pitch discrimination by ferrets for simple and complex sounds. J Acoust
Soc Am 126: 1321–1335.</journal>
17. <journal>Hine JE, Martin RL, Moore DR (1994) Free-field binaural
unmasking in ferrets. Behav Neurosci 108: 196–205.</journal>
18. <journal>Ma L, Micheyl C, Yin P, Oxenham AJ, Shamma SA (2010)
Behavioral measures of auditory streaming in ferrets (Mustela putorius). J Comp
Psychol 124: 317–330.</journal>
19. <journal>Tolnai S, Litovsky RY, King AJ (2014) (2010) Investigation of tThe
precedence effect and its build-up and breakdown in ferrets and humans: ddo they
experience a build-up of echo suppression?. J AcoustA sSsoc Res Otolaryngol
AbsAm 833: ••–••. In press</journal>
20. <journal>Alves-Pinto A, Sollini J, Sumner CJ (2012) Signal detection in
animal psychoacoustics: aanalysis and simulation of sensory and decision-related
influences. Neuroscience 220: 215–227.</journal>
21. <journal>Rarey KE (1985) The ferret as a model for inner ear research. Lab
Anim Sci 35: 238–241.</journal>
22. <journal>Carlile S (1990) The auditory periphery of the ferret. II: tthe spectral
transformations of the external ear and their implications for sound localization. J
Acoust Soc Am 88: 2196–2204.</journal>
23. <journal>Carlile S, King AJ (1994) Monaural and binaural spectrum level
cues in the ferret: aacoustics and the neural representation of auditory space. J
Neurophysiol 71: 785–801.</journal>
24. <journal>Keating P, Dahmen JC, King AJ (2013) Context-specific
reweighting of auditory spatial cues following altered experience during
development. Curr Biol 23: 1291-1299 23, (in press).</journal>
25. <book>Webster DB, Popper AN, Fay RR, eds. (1992) The mammalian
auditory pathway: nneuroanatomy. New York: Springer-Verlag.</book>
26. <journal>Moore DR (1988) Auditory brainstem of the ferret: ssources of
projections to the inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 269: 342–354.</journal>
27. <journal>Fuentes-Santamaría V, Alvarado JC, Henkel CK, Brunso-Bechtold
JK (2007) Cochlear ablation in adult ferrets results in changes in insulin-like
growth factor-1 and synaptophysin immunostaining in the cochlear nucleus.
Neuroscience 148: 1033–1047.</journal>
28. <journal>Henkel CK, Gabriele ML (1999) Organization of the disynaptic
pathway from the anteroventral cochlear nucleus to the lateral superior olivary
nucleus in the ferret. Anat Embryol (Berl) 199: 149–160.</journal>
29. <journal>Henkel CK, Brunso-Bechtold JK (1993) Laterality of superior olive
projections to the inferior colliculus in adult and developing ferret. J Comp Neurol
331: 458–468.</journal>
30. <journal>King AJ, Jiang ZD, Moore DR (1998) Auditory brainstem
projections to the ferret superior colliculus: anatomical contribution to the neural
coding of sound azimuth. J Comp Neurol 390: 342-365 Schnupp JW, Thompson
ID (1998) Signals from the superficial layers of the superior colliculus enable the
development of the auditory space map in the deeper layers. J Neurosci 18: 9394–
9408.</journal>
31. <journal>Nodal FR, Doubell TP, Jiang ZD, Thompson ID, King AJ (2005)
Development of the projection from the nucleus of the brachium of the inferior
colliculus to the superior colliculus in the ferret. J Comp Neurol 485: 202–
217.</journal>
32. <journal>Angelucci A, Clascá F, Sur M (1998) Brainstem inputs to the ferret
medial geniculate nucleus and the effect of early deafferentation on novel retinal
projections to the auditory thalamus. J Comp Neurol 400: 417–439.</journal>
33. <journal>Bajo VM, King AJ (2013) Cortical modulation of auditory
processing in the midbrain. Front Neural Circuits 6: 114.</journal>
34. <journal>Bajo VM, Nodal FR, Bizley JK, Moore DR, King AJ (2007) The
ferret auditory cortex: ddescending projections to the inferior colliculus. Cereb
Cortex 17: 475–491.</journal>
35. <journal>Bajo VM, Nodal FR, Bizley JK, King AJ (2010) The non-lemniscal
auditory cortex in ferrets: cconvergence of corticotectal inputs in the superior
colliculus. Front Neuroanat 4: 18.</journal>
36. <journal>Irving S, Moore DR, Liberman MC, Sumner CJ (2011)
Olivocochlear efferent control in sound localization and experience-dependent
learning. J Neurosci 31: 2493–2501.</journal>
37. <journal>Bajo VM, Nodal FR, Moore DR, King AJ (2010) The descending
corticocollicular pathway mediates learning-induced auditory plasticity. Nat
Neurosci 13: 253–260.</journal>
38. <book>Popper AN, Fay RR, eds. (1992) The mammalian auditory pathway:
nneurophysiology. New York: Springer-Verlag.</book>
39. <journal>Moore DR, Semple MN, Addison PD (1983) Some acoustic
properties of neurones in the ferret inferior colliculus. Brain Res 269: 69–
82.</journal>
40. <journal>Kelly JB, Judge PW, Phillips DP (1986) Representation of the
cochlea in primary auditory cortex of the ferret (Mustela putorius). Hear Res 24:
111–115.</journal>
41. <journal>Sumner CJ, Palmer AR (2012) Auditory nerve fibre responses in the
ferret. Eur J Neurosci 36: 2428–2439.</journal>
42. <journal>Dahmen JC, Keating P, Nodal FR, Schulz AL, King AJ (2010)
Adaptation to stimulus statistics in the perception and neural representation of
auditory space. Neuron 66: 937–948.</journal>
43. <journal>Shechter B, Marvit P, Depireux DA (2010) Lagged cells in the
inferior colliculus of the awake ferret. Eur J Neurosci 31: 42–48.</journal>
44. <journal>Garcia-Lazaro JA, Ahmed B, Schnupp JW (2011) Emergence of
tuning to natural stimulus statistics along the central auditory pathway. PLoS ONE
6: e22584.</journal>
45. <other>Rabinowitz NC, Willmore BDB, King AJ, Schnupp JWH (2013) (••••)
Constructing noise-invariant representations of sound in the auditory pathway.
••PLoS Biol 11: e1001710 (in press). Submitted for publication.</other>
46. <journal>King AJ, Hutchings ME (1987) Spatial response properties of
acoustically responsive neurons in the superior colliculus of the ferret: aa map of
auditory space. J Neurophysiol 57: 596–624.</journal>
47. <journal>King AJ (2004) The superior colliculus. Curr Biol 14: R335–
R338.</journal>
48. <journal>Stein BE, Stanford TR, Rowland BA (2009) The neural basis of
multisensory integration in the midbrain: iits organization and maturation. Hear
Res 258: 4–15.</journal>
49. <journal>Campbell RA, Doubell TP, Nodal FR, Schnupp JW, King AJ (2006)
Interaural timing cues do not contribute to the map of space in the ferret superior
colliculus: aa virtual acoustic space study. J Neurophysiol 95: 242–254.</journal>
50. <journal>Kowalski N, Versnel H, Shamma SA (1995) Comparison of
responses in the anterior and primary auditory fields of the ferret cortex. J
Neurophysiol 73: 1513–1523.</journal>
51. <journal>Nelken I, Bizley JK, Nodal FR, Ahmed B, Schnupp JW, King AJ
(2004) Large-scale organization of ferret auditory cortex revealed using
continuous acquisition of intrinsic optical signals. J Neurophysiol 92: 2574–
2588.</journal>
52. <journal>Bizley JK, Nodal FR, Nelken I, King AJ (2005) Functional
organization of ferret auditory cortex. Cereb Cortex 15: 1637–1653.</journal>
53. <journal> Bizley JK, Nodal FR, Bajo VM, Nelken I, King AJ (2007)
Physiological and anatomical evidence for multisensory interactions in auditory
cortex. Cereb Cortex 17: 2172–2189.Bizley JK, Nodal FR, Parsons CH, King AJ
(2007) Role of auditory cortex in sound localization in the midsagittal plane. J
Neurophysiol 98: 1763–1774.</journal>
54. <journal>Keniston LP, Allman BL, Meredith MA, Clemo HR (2009)
Somatosensory and multisensory properties of the medial bank of the ferret rostral
suprasylvian sulcus. Exp Brain Res 196: 239–251.</journal>
55. <journal>Ramsay AM, Meredith MA (2004) Multiple sensory afferents to
ferret pseudosylvian sulcal cortex. Neuroreport 15: 461–465.</journal>
56. <journal>Manger PR, Engler G, Moll CK, Engel AK (2005) The anterior
ectosylvian visual area of the ferret: aa homologue for an enigmatic visual cortical
area of the cat? Eur J Neurosci 22: 706–714.</journal>
57. <journal>Kayser C, Petkov CI, Logothetis NK (2009) Multisensory
interactions in primate auditory cortex: fMRI and electrophysiology. Hear Res
258: 80–88.</journal>
58. <journal>Bizley JK, King AJ (2008) Visual-auditory spatial processing in
auditory cortical neurons. Brain Res 1242: 24–36.</journal>
59. <journal>Ghazanfar AA, Chandrasekaran C, Logothetis NK (2008)
Interactions between the superior temporal sulcus and auditory cortex mediate
dynamic face/voice integration in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci 28: 4457–
4469.</journal>
60. <journal>Kayser C, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S (2010) Visual enhancement of
the information representation in auditory cortex. Curr Biol 20: 19–24.</journal>
61. <journal>Schnupp JW, Mrsic-Flogel TD, King AJ (2001) Linear processing of
spatial cues in primary auditory cortex. Nature 414: 200–204.</journal>
62. <journal>Bizley JK, Walker KM, Silverman BW, King AJ, Schnupp JW
(2009) Interdependent encoding of pitch, timbre, and spatial location in auditory
cortex. J Neurosci 29: 2064–2075.</journal>
63. <journal>Walker KM, Bizley JK, King AJ, Schnupp JW (2011) Multiplexed
and robust representations of sound features in auditory cortex. J Neurosci 31:
14565–14576.</journal>
64. <journal>Mesgarani N, David SV, Fritz JB, Shamma SA (2008) Phoneme
representation and classification in primary auditory cortex. J Acoust Soc Am 123:
899–909.</journal>
65. <journal>Mesgarani N, David SV, Fritz JB, Shamma SA (2009) Influence of
context and behavior on stimulus reconstruction from neural activity in primary
auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 102: 3329–3339.</journal>
66. <journal>Rauschecker JP, Tian B, Pons T, Mishkin M (1997) Serial and
parallel processing in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. J Comp Neurol 382: 89–
103.</journal>
67. <journal>Lomber SG, Malhotra S (2008) Double dissociation of ‘what’ and
‘where’ processing in auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 11: 609–616.</journal>
68. <journal>Bizley JK, Walker KM, King AJ, Schnupp JW (2010) Neural
ensemble codes for stimulus periodicity in auditory cortex. J Neurosci 30: 5078–
5091.</journal>
69. <journal>Bendor D, Wang X (2005) The neuronal representation of pitch in
primate auditory cortex. Nature 436: 1161–1165.</journal>
70. <journal>Bizley JK, Walker KM, Nodal FR, King AJ, Schnupp JW (2013)
Auditory cortex represents both pitch judgments and the corresponding acoustic
cues. Curr Biol ••: 23: ••–••. 620–625doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.003. [Epub ahead
of print].</journal>
71. <journal>Kavanagh GL, Kelly JB (1992) Midline and lateral field sound
localization in the ferret (Mustela putorius): ccontribution of the superior olivary
complex. J Neurophysiol 67: 1643–1658.</journal>
72. <journal>Nodal FR, Kacelnik O, Bajo VM, Bizley JK, Moore DR, King AJ
(2010) Lesions of the auditory cortex impair azimuthal sound localization and its
recalibration in ferrets. J Neurophysiol 103: 1209–1225.</journal>
73. <journal>Smith AL, Parsons CH, Lanyon RG, Bizley JK, Akerman CJ, Baker
GE, Dempster AC, Thompson ID, King AJ (2004) An investigation of the role of
auditory cortex in sound localization using muscimol-releasing Elvax. Eur J
Neurosci 19: 3059–3072.</journal>
74. <journal>Nodal FR, Bajo VM, King AJ (2012) Plasticity of spatial hearing:
bbehavioural effects of cortical inactivation. J Physiol 590: 3965–3986.</journal>
75. <journal>Campbell RA, King AJ, Nodal FR, Schnupp JW, Carlile S, Doubell
TP (2008) Virtual adult ears reveal the roles of acoustical factors and experience in
auditory space map development. J Neurosci 28: 11557–11570.</journal>
76. <journal>Mrsic-Flogel TD, Schnupp JWH, King AJ (2003) Acoustic factors
govern developmental sharpening of spatial tuning in the auditory cortex. Nat
Neurosci 6: 981–988.</journal>
77. <journal>Moore DR (1994) Auditory brainstem of the ferret: llong survival
following cochlear removal progressively changes projections from the cochlear
nucleus to the inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 339: 301–310.</journal>
78. <journal>McAlpine D, Martin RL, Mossop JE, Moore DR (1997) Response
properties of neurons in the inferior colliculus of the monaurally deafened ferret to
acoustic stimulation of the intact ear. J Neurophysiol 78: 767–779.</journal>
79. <journal>Moore DR, Hutchings ME, King AJ, Kowalchuk NE (1989)
Auditory brain stem of the ferret: ssome effects of rearing with a unilateral ear
plug on the cochlea, cochlear nucleus, and projections to the inferior colliculus. J
Neurosci 9: 1213–1222.</journal>
80. <journal>Moore DR, Hine JE, Jiang ZD, Matsuda H, Parsons CH, King AJ
(1999) Conductive hearing loss produces a reversible binaural hearing impairment.
J Neurosci 19: 8704–8711.</journal>
81. <journal>King AJ (1999) Sensory experience and the formation of a
computational map of auditory space in the brain. Bioessays 21: 900–
911.</journal>
82. <journal>Knudsen EI, Brainard MS (1991) Visual instruction of the neural
map of auditory space in the developing optic tectum. Science 253: 85–
87.</journal>
83. <journal>Rabinowitz NC, Willmore BD, Schnupp JW, King AJ (2012)
Spectrotemporal contrast kernels for neurons in primary auditory cortex. J
Neurosci 32: 11271–11284.</journal>
84. <journal>Rabinowitz NC, Willmore BD, Schnupp JW, King AJ (2011)
Contrast gain control in auditory cortex. Neuron 70: 1178–1191.</journal>
85. <journal>Depireux DA, Dobbins HD, Marvit P, Shechter B (2012) Dynamics
of phase-independent spectro-temporal tuning in primary auditory cortex of the
awake ferret. Neuroscience 214: 28–35.</journal>
86. <journal>Dahmen JC, Hartley DE, King AJ (2008) Stimulus-timing-dependent
plasticity of cortical frequency representation. J Neurosci 28: 13629–
13639.</journal>
87. <journal>Fritz JB, Elhilali M, Shamma SA (2005) Differential dynamic
plasticity of A1 receptive fields during multiple spectral tasks. J Neurosci 25:
7623–7635.</journal>
88. <journal>Fritz J, Shamma S, Elhilali M, Klein D (2003) Rapid task-related
plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Nat
Neurosci 6: 1216–1223.</journal>
89. <journal>David SV, Fritz JB, Shamma SA (2012) Task reward structure
shapes rapid receptive field plasticity in auditory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
109: 2144–2149.</journal>
90. <journal>Recanzone GH, Schreiner CE, Merzenich MM (1993) Plasticity in
the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination
training in adult owl monkeys. J Neurosci 13: 87–103.</journal>
91. <journal>Polley DB, Steinberg EE, Merzenich MM (2006) Perceptual learning
directs auditory cortical map reorganization through top-down influences. J
Neurosci 6: 4970–4982.</journal>
92. <journal>Schnupp JW, Hall TM, Kokelaar RF, Ahmed B (2006) Plasticity of
temporal pattern codes for vocalization stimuli in primary auditory cortex. J
Neurosci 26: 4785–4795.</journal>
93. <journal>Kacelnik O, Nodal FR, Parsons CH, King AJ (2006) Training-
iInduced plasticity of auditory localization in adult mammals. PLoS Biol 4:
e71.</journal>
94. <journal>Kumpik DP, Kacelnik O, King AJ (2010) Adaptive reweighting of
auditory localization cues in response to chronic unilateral earplugging in humans.
J Neurosci 30: 4883–4894.</journal>
95. <journal>Leach ND, Nodal FR, Cordery PM, King AJ, Bajo VM (2013)
Cortical cholinergic input is required for normal auditory perception and
experience-dependent plasticity in adult ferrets. J Neurosci 33: 6659-6671 (in
press).</journal>
96. <journal>Hartley DE, Vongpaisal T, Xu J, Shepherd RK, King AJ, Isaiah A
(2010) Bilateral cochlear implantation in the ferret: aa novel animal model for
behavioral studies. J Neurosci Methods 190: 214–228.</journal>
97. <journal>Phillips DP, Judge PW, Kelly JB (1988) Primary auditory cortex in
the ferret (Mustela putorius): nneural response properties and topographic
organization. Brain Res 443: 281–294.</journal>
98. <journal>Moore BCJ, Glasberg BR (1983) Formulae describing frequency
selectivity as a function of frequency and level, and their use in calculating
excitation pattern. Hear Res 28: 209–225.</journal>
99. <journal>Palmer AR, Russell IJ (1986) Phase-locking in the cochlear nerve of
the guinea-pig and its relation to the receptor potential of inner hair cells. Hear Res
24: 1–15.</journal>
100. <journal>Johnson DH (1980) The relationship between spike rate and
synchrony in responses of auditory-nerve fibers to single tones. J Acoust Soc Am
68: 1115–1122.</journal>
101. <journal>Moore DR (1982) Late onset of hearing in the ferret. Brain Res
253: 309–311.</journal>
Fig. 29.1. Hearing range and audiogram of the ferret. (A) The audible frequency
range of different mammals, including humans, ferrets, and some of the most
common species used for auditory research. Note the total overlap of the ferret’'s
hearing range with that of humans, which makes the ferrets a more suitable species
to study the processing of low-frequency vocalizations and sound localization cues
than rodents whose vocalizations sometimes lie beyond the range of human
hearing. (B) Typical audiogram of two ferrets, whose audible frequency range, as
indicated in panel A, extends from 20 Hz to 50 kHz with maximum sensitivity
around 7–11 kHz. <source>Reproduced with permission from Kelly et al.
[1].</source>
Fig. 29.2. Sound localization behaviourbehavior in ferrets. (A) Overall percentage
across 12, equally spaced target locations of trials classified by their error size for
different durations of a broadband stimulus (from 2000 to 40 millisecondss). The
incidence of incorrect trials and the size of the errors increased as the stimulus
duration was reduced. (B) The behavioral responses were quantified by calculating
the <termDef>mutual information</termDef> (<abbrev>MI</abbrev>) between
target location and response location. The MI values (in bits) are shown for both
the initial head orienting and the subsequent approach-to-target responses at
different stimulus durations. The MI between target and response location
increased with stimulus duration for the approach-to-target task, indicating that the
animals became more accurate, but remained constant for the relationship between
final head bearing and target location. The bBoxes represent the inter-quartile
range, the horizontal line is the median, and the vertical line is the full range of MI
values. (C, E) Stimulus-–response plots showing the distribution of approach-to-
target responses for two stimulus durations, (C) 1000 ms (C) and (E) 40 ms
(E)illiseconds. For each target location, the size of the dots is proportional to the
probability of responses to different target locations. The corresponding percent
correct and MI values for these stimulus durations (1000 and 40 milliseconds) are
shown in panels A and B, respectively. (D, F) Stimulus-–response plots showing
the distribution of final head bearings (binned in steps of 7.5°) for each target
location. The size of the dots is proportional to the response probability. The gray
lines represent the mean final bearing for each target location.
<source>Reproduced with permission from Nodal et al. [74].</source>
Fig. 29.3. Measuring sensitivity to binaural localization cues in ferrets. (A)
Photograph of the head of a ferret wearing lightweight titanium headphone holders
attached to a chronic implant on the skull, which enable earphones to be
positioned close to the entrance of the ear canal. <source>Reproduced from Nodal
et al. [7210].</source> (B) Mean interaural time difference (ITD) thresholds
across different animals are shown for noise bursts as a function of stimulus
duration. Data for individual animals are denoted by symbols of the same color
and are shown for two testing sessions (indicated by the different lines) separated
by 16 weeks to illustrate the stability of these measurements. (C) Corresponding
data for interaural level difference (ILD) thresholds obtained from the same
ferrets. <source>Data from Keating et al. [2411].</source>
Fig. 29.4. Comparison of human and ferret vocalizations. Spectrograms showing
examples of human speech (A) and ferret vocalizations (B) In each case, the sound
is produced by the repeated opening and closing of the vocal folds, resulting in
glottal pulse trains that are made up of a fundamental frequency and a number of
higher harmonics. Transmission of these pulse trains through the resonant cavities
of the vocal tract then enhances some harmonics and suppresses others. The
resulting frequency structure is visible as roughly horizontal bands in the
spectrograms. Note the difference in the scale of the y- axis in the two panels: the
ferret calls contain higher frequency components than human speech, reflecting
differences in the size and structure of the vocal apparatus between these species.
Fig. 29.5. Pitch discrimination by ferrets. The animals performed a two2-
alternative forced choice task, in which they indicated by approaching one of two
reward spouts whether a target sound was higher or lower in pitch than a
preceding reference sound. Each panel shows the psychometric functions for
individual animals. The grayscale of each curve corresponds to the fundamental
frequency of the reference vowel, as indicated to the right. The different reference
values are represented by gray circles at 50% choice probability.
<source>Reprinted, with permission, from Walker et al. [16].</source>
Fig. 29.6. Organization of the auditory pathway in the ferret. (A) Photograph of
the brainstem of a ferret after removing the cerebellum and cerebral hemispheres
to expose most of the subcortical auditory relay stations. (B) Photograph of the
ferret cortex in which the location of different auditory fields on the ectosylvian
gyrus (EG) is indicated. (C) Simplified diagram depicting the main ascending
(black) and descending (red) connections of the auditory pathway. For clarity, all
lines have been drawn with the same thickness independent of the size of the
projection they represent. Each of the connections is shown on one side of the
brain only. Abbreviations: AAF, anterior auditory field; A1, primary auditory field;
ADF, anterior dorsal field; as, ansinate sulculs; AVF, anterior ventral field; CN,
cochlear nuclei; cns, coronal sulcus; crs, cruciate sulcus; IC, inferior colliculus;
SC, superior colliculus; MGB, medial geniculate body; ls, lateral sulcus, PPF,
posterior pseudosylvian field; PSF, posterior suprasylvian field; pss, pseudosylvian
sulcus; SOC, superior olivary complex; sss, suprasylvian sulcus; VP, ventro
posterior field.
Fig. 29.7. Ferret auditory nerve physiology. (A) Frequency response areas from
six auditory nerve fibers in one animal with <termDef>characteristic
frequencies</termDef> (<abbrev>CFs</abbrev>) ranging from below 0.6 kHz to
above 17 kHz. Sound level is expressed in decibelB attenuation, where 0 dB
attenuation is ~100 dB sound pressure level (SPL). (B) Minimum thresholds as a
function of CF. The black continuous line is the ferret audiogram [1]. The gray
area is the outline of minimum thresholds reported in the inferior colliculusIC and
auditory cortex [39,40,97]. The dashed line is the outline when the frequency
tuning curves from all of the fibers are superimposed. AN, auditory nerve; CNS,
central nervous system. (C) <termDef>Equivalent rectangular
bandwidth</termDef> (<abbrev>ERB</abbrev>), a measure of frequency
selectivity, versus CF. The solid line shows the fit ERBkHz = 0.31CFkHz0.533. The
dashed line shows a fit to human psychophysical data [ERBkHz = 0.0247
(0.00437CFkHz + 1)] [98]. (D) Phase locking as a function of stimulus frequency.
Each point represents a significant vector strength value for a single stimulus
frequency in a single fibrefiber. Also shown are the range of values reported by
Palmer and Russell [99] in the guinea -pig (light gray) and by Johnson [100] in the
cat (dark gray). <source>Reproduced, with permission, from Sumner and Palmer
[41].</source>
Fig. 29.8. Identifying the factors that shape the maturation of the spatial
sensitivity of neurons in the auditory cortex of the ferret. (A) Representative
examples of spatial receptive fields recorded at a sound level of 10 dB above
neuronal threshold for different neurons in the auditory cortex of infant and adult
ferrets. These plots illustrate the region of space within which a sound can
modulate the firing rate of the neurons. The receptive fields recorded from infant
cortex (P33–P39; left column) are more irregular and larger than those recorded in
adults (right column). The cross indicates the direction of the <termDef>spatial
response field</termDef> (<abbrev>SRF</abbrev>) centroid vector. The color bar
denotes the evoked spike count per stimulus presentation, averaged over five
repetitions. The black contour line runs along the half-maximal response level,
demarcating the 50% response area. (B) Pair-wise comparisons of near-threshold
50% area and number of bits of information transmitted between the stimulus
location and response for spatial receptive fields recorded from infant cortex
through the animals’' own ears (“infant own ears”) and then through virtual adult
ears (“infant adult ears”). Note that the 50% areas become smaller, and the
transmitted information increases when mature localization cue values are
provided. The histograms show the mean 50% area and information values derived
from infant (own-ear and adult-ear) SRFs and adult (own-ear) SRFs recorded at
near threshold (5–15 dB above unit threshold) and at higher sound levels (20–
35 dB above threshold; n = 40) (n.s., not significant; *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01). Error
bars indicate S.E.M. <source>Adapted from Mrsic-Flogel et al. [76].</source>
Fig. 29.9. Effect of abnormal auditory experience on the development of binaural
unmasking in ferrets. (A) Adult ferrets can be trained to detect a 500-Hz tone
(delivered from speaker A) in the presence of a noise (delivered from speakers A
and B) when a light flashed. The noise stimuli were presented continuously. On
50% of trials, a tone was presented when the ferret contacted spout 1. Success in
the task was measured by the ferret correctly identifying the presence or absence
of the tone (by going to spouts 2 or 3, respectively). In the control condition (tones
and noise interaurally in phase), both speakers were positioned on the ferret’'s
right side. In the bilateral condition, the noise was made out of phase with the tone
by moving speaker B (noise alone) to the ferret’'s left side. The difference in tone
threshold between the two conditions measured binaural unmasking. Unilateral or
asymmetric conductive hearing loss, as seen in middle ear disease in children and
otosclerosis in adults, can lead to poor binaural hearing. In the laboratory, these
diseases are modeled using earplugs in experimental animals to control for
variability and to investigate mechanisms. (B) Mean psychometric functions from
three groups of ferrets (blue, normally hearing animals; red, animals reared with a
unilateral conductive hearing loss and tested as adults; green, animals with a
unilateral conductive hearing loss that began in adulthood). Threshold (56.5%
correct) is shown by the horizontal dashed line. Like humans, Ferrets learned to
perform this task at a high level; the dashed line shows performance that was
statistically above chance (binomial distribution). However, normal ferrets with
normal , like normal humanshearing, have thresholds that are about 10 dB better in
the bilateral condition than in the control condition, showing binaural unmasking.
Unilateral conductive hearing loss impaired bilateral thresholds without
substantially affecting control thresholds (C) Mean (and S.D.) level of binaural
unmasking is shown for normally hearing ferrets and in animals that received a
unilateral hearing loss for several months. The reduction in unmasking seen during
earplugging takes several months to recover following its removalUnilateral or
asymmetric conductive hearing loss, as seen in middle ear disease in children and
otosclerosis in adults, can lead to poor binaural hearing. In the laboratory, these
diseases are modelledmodeled using ear plugs in experimental animals to control
for variability and to investigate mechanisms. Ferrets receiving several months of
unilateral ear plugging had reduced binaural unmasking for several more months
after the plug was removed. <source>Adapted from Moore et al. [80].</source>
Fig. 29.10. Auditory experience shapes the maturation of sound localization
behavior and the map of auditory space in the superior colliculusSC. (A)
Stimulus-–response plots showing the combined data of three3 normally -reared
ferrets (“NNormal adult ferrets”), another three3 animals just after inserting an
earplug into the left ear (“Adult left earplug”), and three3 ferrets that had been
raised and tested with the left ear occluded with a plug that produced 30–40 dB
attenuation (“Reared with left earplug”). These plots illustrate the distribution of
approach-to-target responses (ordinate) as a function of stimulus location
(abscissa). The stimuli were bursts of broadband noise. The size of the dots
indicates, for a given speaker angle, the proportion of responses made to different
response locations. Correct responses fall on the diagonal (x = y). Occluding one
ear disrupts sound localization accuracy, but adaptive changes take place during
development that enable the juvenile plugged ferrets to localize sound almost as
accurately as the controls. (B) The map of auditory space in the ferret SC,
illustrated by plotting the best azimuth of neurons versus their location within the
nucleus. Occluding one ear disrupts this spatial tuning, but, as with the behavioral
data, near-normal spatial tuning is present in ferrets that were raised with one ear
occluded. Based on Hartley King et al. (2000).
Fig. 29.11. Representation of sensory space in the ferret superior colliculusSC.
(A) The superficial layers of the SC are exclusively visual, whereas neurons in the
deeper layers respond to auditory (as well as visual and tactile) stimuli. Stimulus
azimuth is mapped onto the rostro-caudal axis (black arrow), and elevation onto
the medio-lateral (gray arrow) axis of the nucleus. The portion of visual and
auditory space represented within the SC is indicated by the arrows around the
ferret’'s head, and corresponds approximately to the extent of the visual field of
the contralateral eye. Based on King and Hutchings [46]. (B) Visual calibration of
the developing auditory space map in the ferret superior colliculusSC. Removal of
the medial rectus muscle just before natural eye opening induces a small, outward
deviation of the eye. The visual field is therefore shifted laterally relative to the
head. This is shown schematically as the receptive field of a superficial- layer
neuron in the contralateral SC shifting from position V to V*. Electrophysiological
recordings in adult ferrets revealed that the auditory receptive fields of deep SC
neurons are also shifted laterally by a corresponding amount (A → A*) following
this procedure. As a result of this adaptive change, the correspondence between the
superficial-layer visual map and the deep-layer auditory map is preserved.
<source>Data source from King et al. (1988).</source>
Fig. 29.12. Cortical tuning changes during behavior. (A) Ferrets were trained to
detect a pure tone target (red) after a random number of reference noise sounds
(blue) using two different behavioral methods. During the approach behavior
(timeline, lLower), the animals were positively rewarded with water for licking a
water spout 0.1–1.0 second after target onset (green bar) and punished with a
timeout for licking earlier (red bar). During the avoidance task, the ferrets were
rewarded by licking a continuously flowing stream of water during the reference
sounds and punished with a mild tail shock if they failed to cease licking
0.4 second after target offset. (B) Data from one neuron during the approach task.
The spectrotemporal receptive field (STRF) of the neuron was estimated from
responses to reference sounds during passive listening (lLeft) and indicates which
stimulus frequencies and time lags correlated with excited increased (red) or
suppressed (blue) neural spiking. This neuron was excited broadly by 8000- to
16,000-Hz stimuli, which overlapped the target tone (11,046 Hz). During behavior
(cCenter), a notch appeared at the target frequency in the excitatory region, and
the difference between the active and passive STRFs (rRight) shows a 23%
decrease in gain at the target frequency (“X”). (C) STRF change for a second
neuron during approach, plotted as in B. Here the target frequency (11,300 Hz)
overlapped an inhibitory subregion of the passive STRF. During behavior, the
inhibition grew stronger, producing a net 7% decrease at the target frequency. (D)
STRF change for a neuron during avoidance behavior, with the target positioned
on the shoulder of an excitatory region of the passive STRF (1250 Hz). The STRF
showed a selective 10% increase at the target frequency during behavior. (E) Data
from a second neuron recorded during avoidance, with the target positioned over
an inhibitory region (1350 Hz). The inhibition was mostly abolished during
behavior, resulting in a 30% response increase at the target frequency.
<source>Reprinted, with permission, from David et al. [89].</source>
Fig. 29.13. Experience- dependent plasticity of spatial hearing in adult ferrets. (A)
Stimulus-–response plots showing the distribution of responses (ordinate) made by
a ferret as a function of stimulus location in the horizontal plane (abscissa). Prior
to occlusion of the left ear, the animal achieved 100% correct scores at all stimulus
directions (left panel), but performed poorly, particularly on the side of the
earplug, when the left ear was occluded (middle). Further testing with the earplug
still in place, however, led to a recovery in localization accuracy (right).
<source>Modified from Kacelnik et al. [93].</source> (B) Effect of cortical
inactivation on adaptation. Each panel shows data from a different group of
animals that received a subdural implant of a slow release polymer over some part
of auditory cortex. The top left panel are drug-free controls, while the other three
panels show the effects of inactivating the cortex by placing muscimol-Elvax over
the middle (MEG), anterior (AEG), or posterior (PEG) ectosylvian gyrus. The
percentage of correct trials measured every day from individual animals and
averaged across all speaker locations are shown by the dashed lines, with the mean
performance for each group indicated by the solid lines. Data are shown prior to
insertion of an earplug in the left ear (Pre), on each of the 9 days that the earplug
was in place (dDays 1–9), and following its removal (Post). Deactivating any of
these cortical areas impaired adaptation, whereas the drug-free controls learned
normally. <source>Modified from Nodal et al. [74].</source> (C) Elimination of
descending corticocollicular neurons by chromophore-targeted laser photolysis
also impaired the ability of ferrets to adapt to an earplug. Different symbols
correspond to different animals and the lines show the mean performance of
control (gray) and lesioned (black) groups. The photomicrographs on the right
illustrate the loss of layer V pyramidal cells in the lesioned cortex compared to
with intact cortex. <source>Modified from Bajo et al. [37].</source>
Fig. 29.14. Ferret cochlear implantation. (A) Nucleus ESPrit 3G speech processor
(1) attached to a modified Nucleus Cochlear implant CI24RE emulator (2).
Pockets were incorporated within the neckline of a detachable “backpack” (3) that
was attached to a jacket made from a ferret harness and elasticated tubular
bandage (4). (B) Ferrets carried their cochlear implants within the jacket, which
held the microphone of the left and right speech processor immediately posterior
to the ipsilateral pinna and enabled animals to carry on with their normal activities
during chronic stimulation. (C) Hhigh resolution micro-focus radiograph in
anterior–posterior view of a ferret skull showing bilateral intracochlear arrays
inserted to an even depth into the basal turn of both cochleas relative to the
<termDef>round window niche</termDef> (<abbrev>RWN</abbrev>) marked
with a fine wire. The apical electrode in the electrode array is marked with an
arrowhead. <source>Reproduced, with permission, from Hartley et al.
[96].</source>