ArticlePublisher preview available

Intelligence, Personality, and Interests: Evidence for Overlapping Traits

American Psychological Association
Psychological Bulletin
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

The authors review the development of the modern paradigm for intelligence assessment and application and consider the differentiation between intelligence-as-maximal performance and intelligence-as-typical performance. They review theories of intelligence, personality, and interest as a means to establish potential overlap. Consideration of intelligence-as-typical performance provides a basis for evaluation of intelligence–personality and intelligence–interest relations. Evaluation of relations among personality constructs, vocational interests, and intellectual abilities provides evidence for communality across the domains of personality of J. L. Holland's (1959) model of vocational interests. The authors provide an extensive meta-analysis of personality–intellectual ability correlations, and a review of interest–intellectual ability associations. They identify 4 trait complexes: social, clerical/conventional, science/math, and intellectual/cultural.
Psychological
Bulletin
1997, Vol.
121,
No. 2,
219-245
Copyright
1997
by the
American
Psychological
Association,
Inc.
0033-2909/97/$3.00
Intelligence,
Personality,
and
Interests:
Evidence
for
Overlapping
Traits
Phillip
L.
Ackerman
and
Eric
D.
Heggestad
University
of
Minnesota,
Twin
Cities
The
authors
review
the
development
of the
modern
paradigm
for
intelligence
assessment
and
applica-
tion
and
consider
the
differentiation
between
intelligence-as-maximal
performance
and
intelligence-
as-typical
performance.
They
review
theories
of
intelligence,
personality,
and
interest
as a
means
to
establish
potential
overlap.
Consideration
of
intelligence-as-typical
performance
provides
a
basis
for
evaluation
of
intelligence-personality
and
intelligence-interest
relations.
Evaluation
of
relations
among
personality
constructs,
vocational
interests,
and
intellectual
abilities
provides
evidence
for
communality
across
the
domains
of
personality
of J. L.
Holland's
(1959)
model
of
vocational
inter-
ests.
The
authors
provide
an
extensive
meta-analysis
of
personality-intellectual
ability
correlations,
and
a
review
of
interest-intellectual
ability
associations.
They
identify
4
trait
complexes:
social,
clerical/conventional,
science/math,
and
intellectual/cultural.
In
this article,
we
briefly
review theoretical approaches
to
intellect,
personality,
and
interests that make contact
across
these
seemingly
disparate
domains.
We
also review
the
empirical evi-
dence
and
theoretical arguments
for an
approach
to
adult intel-
lect that goes beyond
the
traditional paradigm.
The
review
first
focuses
on the
description
of the
traditional paradigm
for
intelli-
gence assessment
of
children.
We
next
consider
the
extension
of
the
paradigm
to
adult
intellectual assessment. From this
foun-
dation,
we
review
a
separation
of the
constructs
of
inteUigence-
as-maximal
performance
and
intelligence-as-typical perfor-
mance.
In an
attempt
to
bridge
the
separation
of
maximal
and
typical performance,
we
review
the
literature
on the
commonal-
ity
among personality constructs
and
intellectual abilities
in
adults
and
provide
a set of
meta-analytic
results.
We
also review
the
literature
on the
relations between interest constructs
and
intellectual abilities
in
adults.
Intelligence
Testing
as a
Paradigm
Assessment
of
Intelligence
of
Children
A
comprehensive review
of the
early history
of
intelligence
testing
is
beyond
the
scope
of
this article (although
see an
Phillip
L.
Ackerman
and
Eric
D.
Heggestad,
Department
of
Psychol-
ogy,
University
of
Minnesota,
Twin
Cities.
This
research
was
partially
supported
by
U.S.
Air
Force
Office
of
Scientific
Research
Grant
F49620-93-1-0206.
Phillip
L.
Ackerman
would
like
to
gratefully
acknowledge
early
dis-
cussions
and
correspondence
with
J. B.
Carroll,
L. 3.
Cronbach,
C.
Hert-
zog,
and
F.
L.
Schmidt
about
some
of the
ideas
contained
in
this
article.
We
thank
A. T.
Church
for
providing
raw
correlations
from
his
1994
article,
"Relating
the
Tellegen
and
Five-Factor
Models
of
Personality
Structure.''
We
also
thank
A.
Tellegen
for
several
productive
discussions
on the
structure
of
personality
and for his
help
in
sorting
personality
scales
into
trait
classes.
We
also
thank
M.
Goff
for
collecting
and re-
viewing
many
of the
studies
used
in the
personality-intelligence
meta-
analysis.
Correspondence
concerning
this
article
should
be
addressed
to
Phillip
L.
Ackerman,
Department
of
Psychology,
University
of
Minnesota,
N218
Elliott
Hall,
75
East
River
Road,
Minneapolis,
Minnesota
55455.
Elec-
tronic
mail
may be
sent
via
Internet
to
ackerman@maroon.tc.umn.edu.
extensive review
by
Peterson,
1925).
However, while there were
many
earlier instances
of
psychologists making
use of
mental
tests
(e.g.,
J.
McK.
Cattell,
1890;
Gallon,
1883/1928),
the be-
ginnings
of the
modern paradigm
for
intelligence testing
can be
identified
in two of
Binet
and
Simon's
(1905/1961,
1908/1961)
classic
articles. They described
a set of
higher order mental tests
that could
be
administered
to
children
of
various ages
in an
effort
to
predict academic success
or
failure
(e.g.,
see the
abridged translation reprinted
by
lenkins
&
Paterson
in
Binet
&
Simon,
1908/1961;
also
see the
translation
by
Town
in
Binet
&
Simon,
1911/1915).
The
term paradigm
is too
eagerly applied
and
often
overused
in
psychology,
but the
kind
of
mental testing
proceduralized
by
Binet
and
Simon
(1905/1961,
1908/1961)
and
their followers
readily meets many
of the
classificatory
requirements
for the
existence
of
a
scientific
paradigm (Kuhn, 1970). Binet
and
Simon
described
the
type
of
procedures that allow assessment
of
intelligence, specifically they distinguished between three
different
methods: medical, which focuses
on
physiology
and
pathology;
pedagogical,
which determines intelligence
on the
basis
of the
examinee's knowledge;
and
psychological, which
makes
direct
observations
of
intelligence
(as
translated
by
Kite
in
Binet
&
Simon,
1908/1961).
Binet
and
Simon
(1905/1961,
1908/1961) argued that they
wished
to
separate
"natural
intelligence
from
instruction"
by
"disregarding,
in so far as
possible,
the
degree
of
instruction
which
the
subject
possesses"
(as
translated
by
Kite
in
Binet
&
Simon,
1908/1961,
p.
93). That
is,
Binet
and
Simon
attempted
to
provide
an
estimate
of
individual differences
in
intellectual
ability, which was,
to a
great
degree,
separated
from
influences
of
experience, social privilege,
and
other confounds
of
socioeco-
nomic
status. Binet
and
Simon provided cogent arguments
for
the
psychological method over
the
pedagogical method
to
assess
school
children's
intelligence.
In
an
earlier article, Binet
and
Simon
(1896,
as
cited
by
Carroll, 1993) considered
the use of a
wide variety
of
tests,
such
as
simple
psychophysical
procedures (like those
of
Gallon,
1883/1928;
and J.
McK. Cattell,
1890),
but
ultimately rejected
those
in
favor
of
tests
of
higher mental processes, which
had
219
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... When exploring the relationship between bright-side traits and intelligence, a meta-analysis by Ackerman and Heggestad (1997) demonstrated the relationship between both higher-and lower-order personality traits (e.g., the Big Five traits and their narrower facets; Costa and McCrae, 1985) and intelligence. Test anxiety showed the strongest relationship with lower IQ scores, while Openness exhibited the strongest relationship with higher IQ scores. ...
Article
Full-text available
The current study explored the relationship between measures of “bright-side” and “dark-side” personality traits and business reasoning (BR)/judgment using the Hogan Business Reasoning Inventory (HBRI). Participants were a global sample (N = 2,342) who completed the Hogan Personality Inventory (HPI), a bright-side trait measure; the Hogan Development Survey (HDS), a dark-side trait measure; and the HBRI, which is similar to a measure of general cognitive ability. The analyses showed gender effects (men scored higher) but not age effects. Correlation and regression analyses showed that Learning Approach and Adjustment traits were positively associated with business reasoning, while Prudence and Inquisitive traits were negatively associated with business reasoning. In cases where significant dark-side factor relationships were observed, they were negatively associated with business reasoning, except for Reserved and Imaginative traits. However, these traits accounted for relatively little of the variance (approximately 5%) in business reasoning. Stable, ambitious, and intellectually curious individuals who are not high on Conscientiousness and have few dark-side traits appear to be better at business reasoning.
... For example, Sylvia et al. (2023) tested the cognitive processes, including trait anxiety and fluid reasoning, through which test anxiety may affect reading comprehension performance, and no significant contribution was found from trait anxiety. Although it may seem counterintuitive, a few studies have even evidenced that students classified as highly trait-anxious surpassed their less anxious peers on certain reading exam questions or tasks requiring close passage analysis (Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to compare trait anxiety and test anxiety among Chinese undergraduates with typical development, high-functioning specific learning disabilities (SLD), and typical-functioning SLD, and to examine the mediating role of test anxiety in the relationship between trait anxiety and academic performance across these three groups. The study included 239 s-year undergraduate students from universities in Taiwan, with 134 typically developing students, 54 students with typical-functioning SLD, and 51 students with high-functioning SLD. Our results indicated that the high-functioning SLD group reported significantly lower levels of trait anxiety and test anxiety compared to both the typically developing and typical-functioning SLD groups. The typical-functioning SLD group exhibited the highest levels of test anxiety. Mediation analyses revealed that test anxiety significantly mediated the relationship between trait anxiety and academic performance in the high-functioning SLD group, while only a direct effect of trait anxiety on academic performance was found in the typical-functioning SLD group. No significant direct or indirect effects were found in the typically developing group. Our findings highlight distinct anxiety profiles and differential patterns of direct and indirect effects of trait anxiety on academic performance among Chinese undergraduates with high-functioning SLD, typical-functioning SLD, and typical development.
... Savickas (citado en Noronha, Barros y Nunes, 2009) lo define como una respuesta de gusto, repulsión o neutralidad a estímulos ocupacionales. Los intereses vocacionales guían el proceso de elección vocacional, dirigiendo el esfuerzo cognitivo hacia determinadas áreas y determinando la posterior satisfacción laboral (Ackerman y Heggestad, 1997). ...
Article
La concepción de orientación vocacional ha evolucionado con los años, siendo entendida hoy como un proceso que incluye aspectos aptitudinales, motivacionales y de personalidad. Los intereses vocacionales del alumnado ACI comienzan a despertarse de forma precoz, siendo necesaria una intervención planificada desde las primeras etapas educativas. La multipotencialidad, las expectativas sociales y las diferencias de género son aspectos a tener en cuenta al abordar la elección vocacional de esta población. El presente estudio se plantea si la superdotación se relaciona con un perfil vocacional alto y plano, y si hay correlaciones entre aptitudes e intereses. Para la consecución de los objetivos se utiliza el cuestionario EXPLORA, la escala de inteligencia de Weschler, la batería de aptitudes BADyG y el test de creatividad Torrance. El análisis de los datos revelan correlaciones negativas significativas entre el interés social-asistencial y las aptitudes lógica y numérica.
... Previous research has shown inconsistent results regarding the importance of previous knowledge for the effectivity of contrast and discrimination processes (for an overview, see . ) that students with a lower prior knowledge benefitted just as much as those with a higher prior knowledge from comparing when more possibilities to practice were provided and the pace of instruction was decelerated. On the other hand, motivational variables (e.g., attitude, goal orientations, self-efficacy) and the cognitive motivation of 162 students (need for cognition), i.e., the enjoyment of being involved in cognitive activities, seem to be dispositions of students that could moderate the effect of classroom instructions (e.g., Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997;Preckel et al., 2006;Dalbert & Radant, 2008;Hughes et al., 2013;Preckel, 2014;. The effect of these variables might be even more substantial for desirably difficult classroom instructions since they hamper learning in the short-term and therefore, require a higher cognitive effort from the individuals before learning successes occur. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This thesis explores the potential of interleaved practice to improve primary students' strategy proficiency in three-digit subtraction tasks. An experimental classroom study with 236 German third graders compared the effectiveness of interleaved practice, including between-comparison prompts, with blocked practice, including within-comparison prompts, in teach-ing the adaptive use of different subtraction strategies. Across four articles, this research high-lights the superiority of interleaved practice in promoting flexible and adaptive strategy use. The results show that interleaved practice better fosters students' ability to adaptively select subtraction strategies based on task characteristics than blocked practice, leading to improved problem-solving accuracy. This effect was observed immediately post-intervention and sustained five weeks later, indicating that the benefits of interleaving are long-lasting. Notably, the positive effects of interleaving on adaptive strategy use were found to be independent of students' prior knowledge, indicating its broad applicability across a diverse range of learners. The effectiveness of interleaving was influenced by students' need for cognition. However, this influence did not differ significantly from its impact on blocked practice. Moreover, the results show that interleaved practice helps to reduce overreliance on the standard written al-gorithm and encourages using more efficient strategies, such as indirect addition and compen-sation strategy. Cluster analysis further revealed that interleaved practice predicts inclusion in a high-adaptivity cluster, highlighting its role in fostering comprehensive adaptive strategy use. Additionally, interleaved practice promotes the development of flexible calculators—stu-dents who employ various strategies with few errors. This is in sharp contrast to the blocked condition, which tends to lead to a greater reliance on the standard algorithm with associated errors. This finding highlights the potential of interleaved practice to cultivate a more versatile and accurate problem-solving approach in students. This thesis contributes to the understanding of effective strategy instruction in mathematics. It advocates for the integration of interleaved practice as a means of cultivating adaptive expertise and accuracy in problem solving.
Article
Personality and cognition offer robust frameworks to understand the individual differences associated with externalizing behaviors. However, these literatures have historically been separated in the study of the externalizing spectrum. In the present study, we used comprehensive tests of additive and interactive effects to examine the utility of integrating models of personality and cognition to understand externalizing behaviors in young adulthood ( N = 1,179; Human Connectome Project) and late childhood ( N = 11,448; Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study). Generally, the average effects of personality traits in young adulthood and the additive effects of impulsigenic traits and cognition in late childhood demonstrate the utility of integrating personality and cognition to understand incidence of early onset externalizing-spectrum behaviors. Although interaction effects were detected, they were small and practically negligible in their explanation of variance in externalizing behaviors. Results support primarily focusing on additively integrating these literatures to understand the individual differences related to externalizing behaviors.
Chapter
This volume provides the most comprehensive and up-to-date compendium of theory and research in the field of human intelligence. Each of the 42 chapters is written by world-renowned experts in their respective fields, and collectively, they cover the full range of topics of contemporary interest in the study of intelligence. The handbook is divided into nine parts: Part I covers intelligence and its measurement; Part II deals with the development of intelligence; Part III discusses intelligence and group differences; Part IV concerns the biology of intelligence; Part V is about intelligence and information processing; Part VI discusses different kinds of intelligence; Part VII covers intelligence and society; Part VIII concerns intelligence in relation to allied constructs; and Part IX is the concluding chapter, which reflects on where the field is currently and where it still needs to go.
Chapter
This volume provides the most comprehensive and up-to-date compendium of theory and research in the field of human intelligence. Each of the 42 chapters is written by world-renowned experts in their respective fields, and collectively, they cover the full range of topics of contemporary interest in the study of intelligence. The handbook is divided into nine parts: Part I covers intelligence and its measurement; Part II deals with the development of intelligence; Part III discusses intelligence and group differences; Part IV concerns the biology of intelligence; Part V is about intelligence and information processing; Part VI discusses different kinds of intelligence; Part VII covers intelligence and society; Part VIII concerns intelligence in relation to allied constructs; and Part IX is the concluding chapter, which reflects on where the field is currently and where it still needs to go.
Chapter
This volume provides the most comprehensive and up-to-date compendium of theory and research in the field of human intelligence. Each of the 42 chapters is written by world-renowned experts in their respective fields, and collectively, they cover the full range of topics of contemporary interest in the study of intelligence. The handbook is divided into nine parts: Part I covers intelligence and its measurement; Part II deals with the development of intelligence; Part III discusses intelligence and group differences; Part IV concerns the biology of intelligence; Part V is about intelligence and information processing; Part VI discusses different kinds of intelligence; Part VII covers intelligence and society; Part VIII concerns intelligence in relation to allied constructs; and Part IX is the concluding chapter, which reflects on where the field is currently and where it still needs to go.
Article
Full-text available
High school students face critical psychological challenges during adolescence, including academic pressures and educational decision-making. Dabrowski’s Theory of Positive Disintegration provides a framework for understanding growth through disintegration and reintegration, with perfectionistic traits acting as intrinsic motivators for self-improvement. This study examined the psychological profiles of 641 Taiwanese high school students: 207 mathematically and scientifically talented students (MSTS), 187 verbally talented students (VTS), and 247 regular students (RS). Using the ME III, refined from the ME II, and the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale, our research assessed overexcitabilities (OEs) and perfectionism traits. MSTS and VTS scored significantly higher in Intellectual and Imaginational OEs than RS, with VTS also overperforming in sensual OE. MSTS and VTS showed higher personal standards, while VTS excelled in organization, and RS reported higher parental criticism. Emotional OE correlated with perfectionism, such as concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, and parental criticism, while Intellectual OE positively correlated with personal standards and negatively with parental criticism. Intellectual and Emotional OEs jointly predicted personal standards and organization; while Sensual, Intellectual, and Emotional OEs predicted doubts about actions, etc. These findings underscore the importance of tailored educational and counseling strategies to address the unique needs of gifted students, fostering environments that enhance their abilities and overall well-being.
Article
Full-text available
Unfortunately, Block's brilliant critique is terribly biased, much like a legal brief that presents only one side of the issues at suit. It does not distinguish between the Big Five model of phenotypic personality attributes from alternative models of the causal underpinnings of personality differences. Ironically, it attempts to explain away the extensive evidence for the Big Five model as largely the result of data prestructuring, with no acknowledgement of the unique contribution of the lexical approach to minimizing such problems. Even more seriously, it omits a good deal of crucial evidence favorable to the Big Five model, including studies of Block's own Q-set and independent investigations of personality-related terms in other languages. Sadly, Block's closing suggestions provide little in the way of specific proposals for alternatives that he would have us use instead.
Article
Full-text available
The five-factor model (FFM) of personality offers a structural organization of personality traits in terms of 5 broad factors. J. Block's (1995) critique of the FFM failed to recognize the utility of a trait taxonomy and the intent of research designed to test the 5-factor hypothesis. In a number of instances he omitted reference to empirical evidence that addresses concerns he raised; this evidence shows strong support for the FFM beyond the lexical and questionnaire traditions he reviews. Many of his suggestions for improving the quality of personality research are valuable, but are likely to be more fruitful when used in conjunction with established knowledge about the structure of personality traits: the FFM.
Article
Full-text available
Nash has argued that individuals will perform better on cognitive tasks when their self-concepts match the gender stereotyping of the tasks. To evaluate this hypothesis, we reviewed studies on the relation between gender self-concept and performance on spatial, mathematical, and verbal tasks. Meta-analytic techniques were used to estimate the average effect sizes and to determine the significance of the combined probabilities. The influence of subjects' sex and age, date of study, type of spatial task, and type of self-concept measure on these associations was also examined. In general, the results from spatial and mathematical tasks, which are usually stereotyped as masculine, supported Nash's hypothesis. Higher masculine and lower feminine self-concept scores were associated with better performance. These relations were observed more consistently for female than for male subjects. Most notably, there was some evidence of better spatial and mathematical performance among adolescent boys who described themselves as feminine. Nash's hypothesis was not supported for verbal tasks. Finally, there was no evidence that androgyny, defined either as high masculine and high feminine scores or as a balance between masculine and feminine scores, is associated with better cognitive performance.
Article
Full-text available
52 male and 27 female college students completed the Worry-Emotionality Inventory just prior to taking the Miller Analogies Test. Worry and emotionality were significantly correlated. State test anxiety, worry, and emotionality were inversely related to performance, but partial correlations indicated that only worry was correlated with performance when the common variance between worry and emotionality was partialed out. High worriers performed less well than low worriers. The effects of emotionality were nested within worry level; at low levels of worry, emotionality was unrelated to performance, but at high levels of worry, high emotionality was associated most negatively with performance. No sex differences were found. Results support the conceptual distinction between worry and emotionality.
Article
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of instructions, anxiety, and locus of control on scores obtained on a group-administered intelligence test. One hundred and thirteen fifth-grade children were randomly assigned to three conditions in which they were told that they were taking either an “intelligence,” “routine,” or “achievement” test. Prior to taking the Kuhlmann-Anderson test, children's level of anxiety and perceived locus of control were assessed by means of the Test Anxiety Scale for Children and the Intellectual Achievement Responsibility Questionnaire, respectively. Results indicated that differences in perceived locus of control were reflected in test scores obtained by boys but not in those obtained by girls, while differences in anxiety level were reflected by scores of children of both sexes. It was also demonstrated that boys and girls manifesting heightened anxiety and external locus of control scored lower than children who were less anxious and more internal. Results suggested that boys were more sensitive than girls to the differential demands of testing instructions.