ArticlePDF Available

Weight Loss Is Not the Answer: A Well-being Solution to the “Obesity Problem”

Authors:

Abstract

Americans have been gaining weight in recent decades, prompting widespread concern about the health implications of this change. Governments, health practitioners, and the general public all want to know: What is the best way to reduce the health risks associated with higher body weight? The dominant weightloss solution to this “obesity problem” encourages individuals to lose weight through behavior change. This solution rests on the assumptions that higher body weight causes health problems, that permanent weight loss is attainable, and that weight loss improves health. But comprehensive reviews of the scientific evidence find mixed, weak, and sometimes contradictory evidence for these premises.We suggest that a different solution to the “obesity problem” is needed – a solution that acknowledges both the multifaceted nature of health and the complex interaction between person and situation that characterizes the connection between weight and health. Thus, we use the lens of social psychological science to propose an alternative, well-being solution to the “obesity problem”. This solution has the potential to improve health by encouraging eating and exercising for optimal health rather than weight loss, by developing interventions to reduce weight stigma and discrimination, and by helping higher body-weight people cope with the stress of stigma and discrimination.
Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
Weight Loss Is Not the Answer: A Well-being Solution to the
Obesity Problem
Christine Logel
1
*, Danu Anthony Stinson
2
and Paula M. Brochu
3
1
Renison University College, University of Waterloo
2
University of Victoria
3
Nova Southeastern University
Abstract
Americans have been gaining weight in recent decades, prompting widespread concern about the health
implications of this change. Governments, health practitioners, and the general public all want to know:
What is the best way to reduce the health risks associated with higher body weight? The dominant weight-
loss solution to this obesity problemencourages individuals to lose weight through behavior change.
This solution rests on the assumptions that higher body weight causes health problems, that permanent
weight loss is attainable, and that weight loss improves health. But comprehensive reviews of the scientific
evidence find mixed, weak, and sometimes contradictory evidence for these premises. We suggest that a
different solution to the obesity problemis needed a solution that acknowledges both the multifac-
eted nature of health and the complex interaction between person and situation that characterizes the
connection between weight and health. Thus, we use the lens of social psychological science to propose
an alternative, well-being solution to the obesity problem. This solution has the potential to improve
health by encouraging eating and exercising for optimal health rather than weight loss, by developing in-
terventions to reduce weight stigma and discrimination, and by helping higher body-weight people cope
with the stress of stigma and discrimination.
obesity is one of todays most blatantly visible yet most neglected public health problemsIf immediate action
is not taken,millions will suffer from an array of serious health disorders.
World Health Organization (2015
a
)
Body weight is indeed blatantly visible, as noted by the World Health Organization
(WHO, 2015a) above. With a single glance, people can size up one another and categorize each
other as thinner or fatter. More than ever, people are categorizing each other as fatter. In the
United States, the focus of the present review, the majority of adults now weigh more than
health authorities agree is optimal for health (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
[CDC], 2014b, September 9; WHO, 2015b). Indeed, the prevalence of weights that are con-
sidered obeseaccording to medical classification systems and thus are considered to pose
a significant health risk has increased from 13% to 32% over the past 50years (e.g., Wang &
Beydoun, 2007).
However, the extent to which higher body weight is actually a neglected public health
problemthat has not yet been targeted with immediate actionin the United States is debat-
able. The American government has taken action. They have created a dedicated division of the
CDC with an annual budget of $49.5 million (The State of Obesity, 2015) and made higher
body weight among children the priority issue of the First Ladys office (see www.letsmove.
gov). Individuals are also taking action. The most common New Yearsresolution is to lose
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
weight (Norcross, Mrykolo, & Blagys, 2002), and Americans spend $50 billion annually on
weight-loss products (Weiss, Galuska, Khan, & Serdula, 2006). Despite these and other actions,
as the WHO notes above, most people still weigh more than what is considered optimal for
health by health authorities.
So it may not be lack of action that makes higher body weight a neglected public health
problem, but a lack of effective action. Yet the dominant response to the ineffectiveness of past
efforts has been to take the same actions, but just try harder: Governments increase funding
for weight-loss initiatives (The State of Obesity, 2015); encourage employers to monitor their
employeesweight and implement weight-loss programs in the workplace (CDC, 2014a,
November 2012); and introduce taxes on foods thought to contribute to higher body weight
(Mandaro, 2014). Individuals, for their part, attempt one diet after another (Montani, Schutz,
& Dulloo, 2015). Yet body weights remain high.
Rather than continuing to pour money and energy into actions that have not been effective,
we suggest that researchers, health practitioners, and lay people pause to ask themselves a tough
question: What is the best way to reduce the health risks that are associated with higher body
weight? We call this the obesity problem, and in this paper, we use the lens of social psycho-
logical science to evaluate the currently dominant solution, and then to propose an alternative.
With its focus on the complex interactions between the person and the social situation, and its
emphasis on experimental methods to determine causality, social psychological science is
uniquely suited to tackle the obesity problem.
Throughout this paper, we use the colloquial term body weight to refer to a personsrelative
fatness or leanness. Higher body-weight people are those who would be deemed overweight
or obeseaccording to the common medical classification system for weight, or by themselves
or important others. We use quotation marks around these weight terms because they reflect
arbitrary classifications and many higher body-weight people find them stigmatizing. Beyond
the scope of this article are issues pertaining to the one in 200 people who are classified as ex-
tremely obese(Sturm & Hattori, 2013), to childrens weight, or to clinically significant eating
disorders.
Evaluating the Weight-loss Solution
Obesity is a national epidemic,causing higher medical costs and a lower quality of life.
CDC (2010)
This quote from a CDC pamphlet exemplifies a common belief: That a high volume of ad-
ipose tissue causes costly and debilitating health problems. This belief about the obesity prob-
lemleads logically to what we term the weight-loss solution: If higher body weight causes serious
health problems, the appropriate solution is to encourage weight loss. Implicit to this solution is
the assumption that permanent weight loss is attainable with individual efforts, and that it will
improve health. Does the evidence support these premises?
Evidence that higher body weight causes health problems
Evidence is strong that higher body weight predicts health problems over time. Studies in high-
caliber epidemiological and biomedical journals identify higher body weight as a risk factor for
type 2 diabetes, cardiovascular disease, osteoarthritis, and several types of cancer (e.g., Calle,
Thun, Petrelli, Rodriguez, & Heath, 1999; Mokdad, Marks, Stroup, & Gerberding, 2004).
Large-scale longitudinal studies demonstrate moderate correlations between higher body
A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem679
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
weight and both disease and all-cause risk of mortality (e.g., Flegal, Graubard, Williamson, &
Gail, 2005; Hedley et al., 2004). If body fat actually causes these outcomes, then the United
States is experiencing a true public health crisis.
However, articles in many of the same high-caliber epidemiological and biomedical journals
call for caution in concluding that this link is causal. Virtually all research linking higher body
weight to longer-term negative health consequences is, necessarily, correlational, and many
studies fail to adequately control for third variables known to predict, and sometimes cause, both
higher body weight and poor health. One such third variable is socioeconomic status (SES;
e.g., Kassirer & Angell, 1998). People who are of lower SES are higher in body weight and also
experience worse health outcomes than their higher SES counterparts. Another is physical ac-
tivity (e.g., LaMonte & Blair, 2006). Studies that distinguish between physical activity and body
weight reveal that fitness, not fatness, is the strongest predictor of health (e.g., Farrell, Braun,
Barlow, Cheng, & Blair, 2002; Grundy et al., 1999; Lee, Blair, & Jackson, 1999; Wie et al.,
1999). Additional and potentially important third variables include powerful social determinants
of health (Wilkinson & Marmot, 2003) such as social rejection, poor social support, and stress,
each of which can lead to weight gain and poor health (e.g., Björntorp, 2001; Cohen, 2004;
Cohen, Tyrrell, & Smith, 1991; Dallman, 2010; Logel et al., 2014; Stinson et al., 2008). These
confounding variables may explain the so-called obesity paradox, the finding in many large-scale,
multi-country epidemiological studies that body fat predicts lower mortality risk (Flegal, Kit,
Orpana, & Graubard, 2013; Grabowski & Ellis, 2001; Orpana et al., 2010; Stessman, Jacobs,
Ein-Mor, & Bursztyn, 2009; Tamakoshi et al., 2010), and better outcomes following pneumo-
nia infection (Corrales-Medina, Valayam, Serpa, Rueda, & Musher, 2011), kidney disease
(Kopple, 2005), and cardiovascular disease (Lavie, Milani, & Ventura, 2009).
When taken together, evidence suggests that it is premature to conclude that higher body
weight causes health problems. What are the implications of this uncertain causality for the
weight-loss solution to the obesity problem? If a given public health action has potential to
benefit health, is easy to implement, and carries few costs, then it make sense to implement it
while scientists continue to seek causal evidence. This appears to be the perspective adopted
by public health and governmental organizations like the WHO and CDC, whose scientists
have surely been aware of the problem of inferring causal associations from correlational
evidence, and who support ongoing research in this area. Some of this research has investigated
the degree to which weight loss is easy to attain and has costs. Does this research support
continued implementation of the weight-loss solution?
Evidence that weight loss is attainable
So here I stand,40 pounds heavier than I was in 2006.Im mad at myself.Im embarrassed.Icant believe that after
all these years,all the things I know how to do,Im still talking about my weightHow did I let this happen again?
Oprah Winfrey (2009)
Oprah Winfrey conquered childhood poverty and prejudice to become one of Time
Magazines100 Most Inf luential People in the Worldand win the Presidential Medal of
Freedom (Turner, 2011). But as she poignantly describes, she has been unable to consistently
maintain a body weight in the range considered optimal by health authorities. Her failure is
not for want of trying. Ms Winfrey has famously lost and then regained weight numerous times
during her long tenure in the public eye.
How can a highly successful philanthropist and business woman have so much difficulty
maintaining a lower body weight? The formula for weight loss seems simple consume less
680 A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
energy than you burn, creating an energy deficit, and forcing your body to use stored fat for
fuel. Ms Winfreys website includes tips to create such an energy deficit by consuming
13001500kilocalories per day (Oz, 2012), an amount consistent with public health recom-
mendations for weight loss (American Heart Association, 2014) and much less than the 2500ki-
localories necessary to be well-nourishedaccording to the Food and Agricultural
Organization of the United Nations (FAO, 2000). Ms Winfreys guidelines are less extreme
than popular commercial diet plans like Nutrisystem (1250 to 1500 kilocalories) and Jenny
Craig (1200 kilocalories).
Public health communications typically recommend attaining this reduced energy intake by
eating smaller portions that include more fruits and vegetables and exercising to burn additional
fat stores (e.g., American Heart Association, 2014; CDC, 2014). However, this behavioral for-
mula for weight loss can only work if body weight is a predominantly controllable characteristic
(Friedman, 2003, 2004; ORahilly & Farooqi, 2008). It is not. Large-scale studies of families,
including twins who have been reared apart, reveal that fully 70% of individual variance in body
weight can be explained by genetics (Stunkard, Harris, Pedersen, & McClearn, 1990; see also
Sørensen, Price, Stunkard, & Schulsinger, 1989; Stunkard et al., 1986), a degree of heritability
commensurate with traits like height (e.g., ORahilly & Farooqi, 2008). This means that most of
the populations variance in body weight is determined by individual differences in genetic
heritage, not individual differences in behavior. Furthermore, although environmental factors
like greater availability of calorie-dense foods (e.g., Kessler, 2010), increasing portion sizes
(Wansink & Chandon, 2014), and less active work lives (Prentice & Jebb 1995; but see
Westerterp & Speakman, 2008) may explain recent weight gains across the entire American
population, the same large-scale studies of families we just reviewed reveal that some people
are genetically more susceptible to such environmental factors, and this susceptibility is generally
uncontrollable by the individual. Thus, many experts conclude that weight is a largely genetic
trait that is highly responsive to the environment (e.g., Friedman, 2003, 2004).
Once genes and the environment interact to settle a person at a given body weight,
automatic, homeostatic biological processes work to maintain it as a minimum body weight
or more specifically, a minimum fat volume that only varies within a relatively narrow range
(e.g., Keesey & Powley, 2008). Thus, decreases in energy intake, such as those proscribed by
weight loss plans, prompt a cascade of biological changes that resist weight loss. These include
shifts in appetite-regulatory hormones that increase subjective appetite (Sumithran et al.,
2011) and changes in metabolism that reduce energy expenditure (Brownell, 1991; Leibel,
Rosenbaum, & Hirsch, 1995), which can persist up to a year after weight loss occurs.
Moreover, the level of energy intake recommended by many popular diets (i.e., 1200 to
1500 kilocalories per day) is comparable to that of the most undernourished global regions,
where severe hunger interferes with individualsability to thrive and make meaningful contri-
butions to society (FAO, 2000). It is no wonder, then, that dieting for weight loss is psycholog-
ically and physiologically stressful (Tomiyama et al., 2010). This stress further undermines
weight loss by prompting increased caloric consumption (see Torres & Nowson, 2007) and
physiological changes that facilitate weight gain (see Björntorp, 2001; Dallman, 2010).
Pitted against these genetic, environmental, biological, and psychosocial factors, how well do
people fare in their efforts to lose weight? Independent comprehensive reviews of behavioral
weight-loss interventions all reach the same conclusions: In the short term, some weight loss
is possible on any diet program, but nearly every dieter regains the weight they lost within three
to five years, and as many as two-thirds of dieters regain more weight than they initially lost
(Katz, 2005; Mann et al., 2007; see also Aphramor, 2010; Bacon, 2008; Bacon & Aphramor,
2014; Garner & Wooley, 1991; Kassirer & Angell, 1998). Permanent weight loss, at least via
individual behavioral efforts, is exceptionally rare.
A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem681
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
But can those rare individuals who do maintain significant weight losses over time serve as
exemplars for the potential success of the weight-loss solution? Perhaps. But such individuals
might also be counted as statistical outliers. Moreover, in a sample drawn from the National
Weight Control Registry, participants reported that they maintained their 30-pound weight
losses for five years or more by continuing, against biological, environmental, and psychosocial
pressures, to follow a starvation-level diet 1685 kilocalories daily for men; 1300kilocalories daily
for women (Shick et al., 1998). And even then, on average the womens weights were barely
into healthy weightclassification, and the men were still classified as overweight.
Proponents of the weight-loss solution to the obesity problemmight argue that the
weight-loss solution should not be abandoned, but merely adjusted that new methods of
weight loss should be sought for the sake of peopleshealth.Butdoesweightlossimprovehealth?
Evidence that weight loss improves health
Researchers have documented some modest, short-term health benefits of weight loss. A re-
view of 21 randomized-controlled trials of weight-loss interventions concluded that participants
in the weight-loss conditions enjoyed small decreases in coronary morbidity and mortality risk,
as well as slight improvements in blood pressure, use of diabetes medication, fasting blood glu-
cose, levels of cholesterol and triglycerides, and incidence of diabetes and stroke compared to
the control participants (Tomiyama, Ahlstrom, & Mann, 2013). However, none of these effects
were correlated with the amount of weight that participants lost, meaning that participants in
the intervention conditions enjoyed these health benefits regardless of their level of weight loss,
which varied widely across participants. Given that most interventions increased exercise, social
support, healthcare access, and consumption of fruits and vegetables in addition to manipulating
calorie intake, the authors of the review could find little convincing evidence that weight loss
itself played any causal role in the (mostly small) positive health outcomes observed in the treat-
ment conditions. Indeed, health interventions that include all of these beneficial factors but do
not limit caloric intake demonstrate equally positive health outcomes in the absence of weight
loss (Bacon & Aphramor, 2011).
Not only does evidence that weight loss directly improves health appear to be weak, but ev-
idence that weight-loss efforts can be harmful appears to be stronger, and includes correlational
but also experimental evidence. Dieting requires sustained acts of willpower which can
eventually break down, resulting in overeating and a lack of self-regulatory capacity needed
for other tasks (Kahan, Polivy, & Herman, 2003; Vohs & Heatherton, 2000). The stress of
dieting increases the stress hormone cortisol, potentially compromising dietersimmune func-
tioning (Tomiyama et al., 2010). Dieting also predicts elevated rates of depression and negative
self-image (Markowitz, Friedman, & Arent, 2008), and in adolescents, predicts future weight
gain and binge eating (Field et al., 2003). And it appears that the more often people attempt
weight loss, the more their health is at risk frequency of dieting predicts decreased markers
of immune functioning among higher-weight, currently-healthy women (Shade et al., 2004).
The costs of the weight-loss solution are not borne by higher body-weight people exclu-
sively. Theportrayal of higher weight as unhealthy and weight loss asboth desired and attainable
has likely contributed to the fear of fat that extends across the weight spectrum. In one study,
nearly 60% of participants classified as non-overweightagreed that they would give up at least
one year of their lives rather than be obese, and 15% agreed that they would give up ten years
or more (Schwartz, Vartanian, Nosek, & Brownell, 2006). Weight dissatisfaction is so pervasive
among women that it is referred to as a normative discontent(Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-
Moore, 1983). Among young people, body dissatisfaction prospectively predicts a host of
682 A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
unhealthy behaviors for both higher- and lower-weight people, including extreme dieting,
binge eating, smoking, lower fruit and vegetable intake, and lower rates of exercise
(Neumark-Sztainer, Paxton, Hannan, Haines, & Story, 2006). The difference between a per-
sons actual and ideal weight is a stronger predictor of physically and mentally unhealthy days
than is actual body weight (Muennig, Jia, Lee, & Lubetkin, 2008), and the negative health con-
sequences of weight loss followed by weight regain (i.e., weight cycling) are most severe for
lower body-weight people (Montani et al., 2015). Moreover, negative body image predicts
weight gain over time independent of body weight (van den Berg & Neumark-Sztainer, 2007).
The research evidence regarding body weight and health is perhaps best summed up by the
editors of the New England Journal of Medicine:Unfortunately, the data linking overweight and
death, as well as the data showing the beneficial effects of weight loss, are limited, fragmentary,
and often ambiguous(1998, p. 52). Our own reading of the empirical literature supports that
conclusion and adds that evidence suggests that long-term weight loss is extremely rare, and that
efforts to attain long-term weight loss can be harmful to health and well-being.
And when viewed through a social psychological lens, this research points to another ques-
tion that must be asked of the weight-loss solution: Could it be helping to maintain stigma
and discrimination against higher body-weight individuals?
Weight stigma and discrimination
In 2008, Representative W. T. Mayhall Jr proposed legislation requiring restaurants to weigh
prospective diners and refuse service if their weight met Mississippiscriteriaforobesity
( Junkfood Science, 2008). This bill did not pass, but its very proposal offers a stark reminder
of the pervasive and socially acceptable weight stigma that dominates American culture
(Puhl & Brownell, 2001; Puhl & Heuer, 2009).
Higher body-weight people are commonly stereotyped as unhealthy, unhappy, lazy, unat-
tractive, and slow, and they are perceived to have poor eating habits, to lack physical fitness,
and to lack self-control or willpower (e.g., Brochu & Esses, 2011). At three years of age, children
describe their chubbypeers as mean, stupid, lazy, and ugly (Cramer & Steinwert, 1998), and
5th and 6th grade students like obesechildren less than any other stigmatized group (Latner &
Stunkard, 2003). As potential marriage partners, higher body-weight people are deemed less
desirable than embezzlers, cocaine users, and shoplifters (Venes, Krupka, & Gerard, 1982).
Physicians report that obesepatients are awkward, unattractive, ugly, and noncompliant
(Foster et al., 2003) and display less warmth in their interaction with higher body-weight
patients (Gudzune, Beach, Roter, & Cooper, 2013). Sadly, higher body-weight people cannot
always rely on their family members or close friends for support in the face of such negativity,
because such close relationships are often a major source of weight stigmatization in peoples
lives (Puhl & Brownell, 2006).
In an age where blatant bias or discrimination is becoming less and less acceptable, weight bias
remains one of the last socially acceptable forms of discrimination (Puhl & Brownell, 2001).
Higher body-weight people face discrimination in employment, health care, education, media,
and interpersonal relationships (Puhl & Heuer, 2009). Higher body-weight job candidates are eval-
uated less favorably than lower body-weight job candidates who are unqualified for the position
(Sartore & Cunningham, 2007), and such discrimination continues throughout hiring, promotion,
earning, disciplinary decisions, and f iring (Roehling, 1999; Roehling, Pichler, & Bruce, 2013).
Higher body-weight people are also underrepresented in college and are less likely to receive
post-interview offers of admission to psychology graduate programs, despite high standardized test
scores, strong recommendation letters, and aspirations to attend college (Burmeister, Kiefner,
A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem683
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
Carels, & Musher-Eizenman, 2013). It is no surprise, then, that they earn approximately 90 cents
for every dollar earned by their lower body-weight counterparts (Baum & Ford, 2004).
Thus, the negative stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination experienced by higher body-
weight people in America permeate their daily lives. And evidence suggests that the weight-loss
solution to the obesity problemserves to perpetuate this discrimination. Of course, weight
stigma and discrimination existed long before the recent upsurge in anti-obesityactions
carried out as part of the weight-loss solution. Yet stigmatizing comments and discriminating
behavior are commonly framed as concern about the targets health, a tactic termed concern
trolling (e.g., warn a friend theyre fat day; Connelly, 2015). Size-acceptance activists devote
considerable attention to helping higher body-weight people protect themselves from this
subtle, yet damaging, form of aggression (e.g., Harding & Kirby, 2009). Representative
Mayhalls proposed bill banning higher body-weight people from restaurants, putatively to help
them avoid temptation to overeat, is one example of this type of discrimination. Thus, the
weight-loss solution to the obesity problemworks together with existing weight biases to
foster the belief that higher body weight is a personal moral failure.
This likely-unintended cost of the weight-loss solution undermines its goal of helping higher
body-weight people prevent health problems. Cultural weight bias exposes higher body-
weight people to both acute and chronic stressors, taxing their ability to cope and negatively
impacting the cardiovascular system, the neuroendocrine system, and the bodys restorative
processes (Major, Mendes, & Dovidio, 2013). For example, the frequency of weight stigma that
people experience and their awareness of weight stigma in their daily lives each predict
biological markers of stress (cortisol) and cellular aging (F
2
-isoprostane), suggesting that weight
stigma may contribute to the development of chronic disease(Tomiyama et al., 2014, p. 862).
Because financial security, access to healthcare, education, and social support all cause better
health (Wilkinson & Marmot, 2003), discrimination against higher body-weight people in these
domains likely undermines health. Indeed, higher body-weight people who experience weight
stigma are more likely to avoid preventive healthcare screenings and cancel medical appoint-
ments (Amy, Aalborg, Lyons, & Keranen, 2006).
Weight stigma also triggers social identity threat, a concern that one is being judged in light of
negative group stereotypes (Hunger, Major, Blodorn, & Miller, 2015; Steele, Spencer, &
Aronson, 2002). In such situations, higher body-weight people show reduced executive control,
increased blood pressure, cortisol reactivity, and subjective stress (Major, Eliezer, & Rieck, 2012;
Schvey, Puhl, & Brownell, 2011), resulting in increased calorie consumption and feelings of loss
of self-control (Brochu & Dovidio, 2013; Major, Hunger, Bunyan, & Miller, 2014; Schvey et al.,
2011). Indeed, perceived weight stigma explains why higher body-weight people report worse
health outcomes than lower body-weight people (Hunger & Major, 2015; Jackson, Beeken, &
Wardle, 2015), and perceived weight discrimination predicts weight gain over time in nationally
representative samples ( Jackson, Beeken, & Wardle, 2014; Sutin & Terracciano, 2013).
If weight loss was attainable and improved health, then weight loss would be the solution to
this problem of stigma and discrimination. But as the evidence reviewed in the previous section
demonstrates, weight loss efforts tend to be ineffective and harmful. Clearly, a different answer
to the obesity problemis needed a solution that draws on social psychological science.
Proposing a Well-being Solution
Fat people already are ashamedNo further manpower needed on the shame frontMy question is,what if they
try and try and try [to lose weight]and still fail?What if they are still fat?What if they are fat forever?What do you
do with them then?Do you really want millions of teenage girls to feel like theyre trapped in unsightly lard prisons
684 A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
that are ruining their lives,and on top of that its because of their own moral failure,and on top of that they are
ruining America with the terribly expensive diabetes that they dont even have yet?
Lindy West (2011)
In the above quote, Lindy West, an award-winning journalist and self-identified fat person,
gives voice to the urgent need for a solution to the obesity problemthat acknowledges the
toll of weight stigma and the elusiveness of permanent weight loss. Scientists, health practi-
tioners, and activists from diverse backgrounds share Ms Wests concerns and have rallied to
propose alternative, weight-inclusive models of health (see Tylka et al., 2014). Perhaps the best-
known and most influential of these alternatives is the Health at Every Size
approach
(HAES
; www.haescommunity.org; Bacon, 2008), which emerged from the feminist and
size-acceptance movements in the 1970s and has grown in scope and inf luence in the interven-
ing decades (see Bruno, 2013). HAES
promotes a model of health care that honors the prin-
ciples of accepting and respecting all bodies, engaging in compassionate self-care by listening to
the bodys hunger and satiation cues and engaging in joyful activity, and critically challenging
scientific and cultural assumptions about weight and health.
We draw upon this weight-inclusive health movement as well as social psychological theory
and research to propose a well-being solution to the obesity problem. This solution uses an
evidence-based approach to improve overall health and to reduce the negative health outcomes
associated with higher body weight. Importantly, a well-being solution defines health as “… a
state of complete physical, mental and social well-being and not merely the absenceof disease or
infirmity(WHO, 2003, para. 1). Hence, actions that might positively affect physical health but
might negatively affect mental or social health are not supported. Moreover, a well-being solu-
tion evolves as new evidence supports or fails to support its key elements. Currently, some pro-
posed elements are supported directly by weight and health research, and others are suggested
based on research in other literatures.
Promote healthful and weight-neutral approaches to eating and exercise
Proponents of the weight-loss solution may fear that any focus away from weight loss might en-
courage malnutrition, extreme weight gain, and inactivity. But a well-being solution includes
attention to nutrition, weight stability, and physical activity and does so in a publically appealing
way people perceive health-focused public health campaigns to be more motivating, less stig-
matizing, and more promoting of self-efficacy for health behavior change than weight-loss fo-
cused campaigns (Puhl, Luedicke, & Peterson, 2013; Puhl, Peterson, & Luedicke, 2013).
Indeed, a recent review of six randomized and controlled HAES
interventions concluded that
weight-inclusive approaches to health can decrease blood pressure and cholesterol, reduce in-
stances of binge eating, increase physical activity, and improve psychological well-being across
a range of dimensions, all without dieting or weight loss (Bacon & Aphramor, 2011). But what
does a weight-inclusive approach to eating and exercise look like?
Encourage eating for health, not weight loss. Evidence suggests that longevity and vitality can be sup-
ported by eating fewer processed foods and more plants, according to a comprehensive review
(Katz & Meller, 2014). None of the popular, specific recommendations for proportioning mac-
ronutrients (e.g., low carb, low fat, Paleolithic) have been consistently found superior to the
others, nor has an ideal intake amount been identified, perhaps because ideal food intake should
vary according to the needs of the individual in each situation.
One evidence-based strategy for healthful nutrition intake is intuitive eating, which emphasizes
attention to internal cues of hunger and fullness and an attitude of body acceptance (in contrast
A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem685
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
with dieting, which directs attention away from hunger and satiety cues onto external rules;
e.g., Orbach, 2006). A review of 20 interventions found that intuitive eating decreased reports
of disordered eating behaviors like binge eating and dietary restraint, increased physical activity,
body satisfaction, self-esteem, and quality of life and reduced depression and anxiety (Schaefer
& Magnuson, 2014). Furthermore, intuitive eating can lead to better nutrition, such as reduced
fat intake and increased consumption of fruits and vegetables. Many of these improvements in
health lasted longer than the temporary weight loss resulting from traditional diets.
Encourage exercising for health, not weight loss. The evidence is strong that regular physical activity is
effective in preventing chronic diseases and premature death (Warburton, Nicol, & Bredin,
2006). Exercise also appears to have psychological benefits, predicting decreased symptoms of
anxiety and depression (Jayakody, Gunadasa, & Hosker, 2014; Penedo & Dahn, 2005; Scully,
Kremer, Meade, Graham, & Dudgeon, 1998) and improved body image (Hausenblas & Fallon,
2006). However, much of this research concerning psychological health is correlational. The
experimental studies, although promising, have not yet clarified the precise mechanisms
through which exercise benefits psychological well-being (some theorized causal paths include
increasing endorphins and providing a time outfrom stressors; Scully et al., 1998); nor have
they established what type, duration, and frequency of activity is optimal for psychological
health. However, it seems plausible that exercising for immediate and long-term improvements
in well-being may be more rewarding than exercising while hungry in the hopes of achiev-
ing a potential, future weight loss. This shift in purpose could potentially result in higher levels
of exercise participation and improved population health. Indeed, there is some experimental
evidence that people who exercise for health, but not weight loss, are more likely to maintain
their new exercise regime than people who exercise for weight loss (Bacon, Stern, Van Loan, &
Keim, 2005). Furthermore, research suggests that framing physical activity as fun and enjoyable
(e.g., ascenicwalk) reduces the compensatory hedonic eating that typically follows physical
activity when it is framed as exercise(Werle, Wansink, & Payne, 2014).
Reduce weight stigma and discrimination
If weight stigma impairs health, then one action in the well-being solution to the obesity
problemmust be to reduce the stigma. Doing so is no easy task. The social acceptability,
pervasiveness, and cognitive and affective underpinnings of anti-fat prejudice such as beliefs
about weights controllability, and disgust towards fat make it particularly intractable
(e.g., Daníelsdóttir, OBrien, & Ciao, 2010). Still, social psychological science suggests some
promising strategies.
Develop and implement social psychological interventions. A meta-analysis concludes that social psy-
chological interventions that address controllability beliefs, foster empathy, or use social consen-
sus and informational social inf luence can produce a small, positive effect on negative beliefs and
attitudes towards higher body-weight people (Lee, Ata, & Brannick, 2014). Such prejudice-
reduction efforts may be especially worthwhile when aimed at healthcare professionals, because
their biases can constrain higher body-weight peoples access to healthcare (Puhl & Heuer,
2010). Especially promising are psychoeducational interventions that emphasize the uncontrol-
lable aspects of body weight, which have been shown to reduce implicit and explicit anti-fat
prejudice among healthcare students (OBrien, Puhl, Latner, Mir, & Hunter, 2010).
Promote non-stigmatizing media portrayals of higher body-weight people. Higher body-weight people
are rarely seen in the media, and when they are seen, they are stigmatized (Greenberg, Eastin,
Hofschire, Lachlan, & Brownell, 2003; Hilbert & Ried, 2009). One content analysis of online
686 A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
news stories about obesityobserved that72% of news images depict higher body-weight peo-
ple in stigmatizing ways, by focusing on their stomachs, portraying them eating or drinking, or
cutting their heads from the frame (Heuer, McClure, & Puhl, 2011). Reducing stigmatizing
portrayals and introducing positive portrayals could help to reduce weight bias. Non-
stigmatizing media portrayals of higher body-weight people improve weight attitudes and re-
duce support for discriminatory weight-based medical policy (Brochu, Pearl, Puhl, & Brownell,
2014; Pearl, Puhl, & Brownell, 2012).
Include weight as a protected category in federal legislation. Inclusion of weight as a protected category
has potential not only to reduce systematic weight-based discrimination and establish nondis-
criminatory social norms but also to reduce weight-based health disparities and improve the ef-
fectiveness of public health efforts. Civil rights legislation in the 1950s and 1960s is at least
partially credited with increasing awareness of racism, reducing tolerance for overt discrimina-
tion, improving racial attitudes, and spawning interest in intergroup relations research (Bobo,
2001; Dovidio, Newheiser, & Leyens, 2011). Furthermore, in the 12months after the legaliza-
tion of same-sex marriage in Massachusetts, sexual minority men had fewer medical care visits,
mental healthcare visits, and mental healthcare costs compared to the 12 months before the law
change (Hatzenbuehler et al., 2012). However, there is currently no federal legislation that pro-
tects people from discrimination based on their body weight (Pomeranz & Puhl, 2013). Among
states, only Michigan prohibits weight discrimination, and among cities, only six (Washington,
DC; San Francisco, CA; Santa Cruz, CA; Binghamton, NY; Urbana, IL; and Madison, WI)
include weight and/or physical characteristics as protected categories in their human rights
law or municipal code (National Association to Advance Fat Acceptance, n.d.). It is encourag-
ing, however, that public opinion polls demonstrate overwhelming support for legislation that
prohibits weight discrimination, particularly in employment decisions (Puhl & Heuer, 2010;
Puhl, Heuer, & Sarda, 2011; Suh, Puhl, Liu, & Fleming Milici, 2014).
Promote strategies for coping with weight stigma
Stigma is highly stressful (Miller & Major, 2000), and stress damages health. Social psychological
science has identified numerous strategies that can reduce the pernicious effects of the stress of
stigma. Not only could these strategies support higher body-weight peoples health, they could
also benefit lower body-weight people, whose well-being is compromised by fear of fat and
body dissatisfaction.
Theory-driven psychological interventions. Positive psychology interventions are treatment methods
or intentional activities that cultivate positive feelings, behaviors, or cognitions. A review sug-
gests they can enhance well-being and reduce depression symptoms (Sin & Lyubomirsky,
2009). Mindfulness interventions, which can be effectively carried out in therapy, groups, or in-
dividually with workbooks and computer programs (Cavanagh, Strauss, Forder, & Jones, 2014),
teach people to intentionally focus their attention, in an accepting and non-judgmental way, on
the emotions, thoughts, and sensations of the present moment. Mindfulness increases subjective
well-being, reduces psychological symptoms and emotional reactivity, and improves behavioral
regulation (Keng, Smoski, & Robins, 2011). A review of 14 interventions showed that mind-
fulness decreases binge eating and emotional eating among populations engaging in these be-
haviors (Katterman, Kleinman, Hood, Nackers, & Corsica, 2014).
A related intervention fosters self-compassion, which entails treating the self with kindness
during emotional setbacks, recognizing the shared humanity of ones f laws and negative
experiences, and adopting a mindful, non-judgmental perspective towards the self (Neff, 2011).
A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem687
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
Self-compassion reduces guilt after overeating among chronic dieters (Adams & Leary, 2007) and
buffers young women against body shaming from family (Daye, Webb, & Jafari, 2014), benefits
that may explain why self-compassion is already a mainstay of the size-acceptance community
(e.g., Baker, 2015). Thus, self-compassion may be a potential antidote to the negative self-
evaluations that higher body-weight individuals often experience as a result of weight stigma
(i.e., Crocker, Cornwell, & Major, 1993; Puhl, Moss-Racusin, & Schwartz, 2007).
Finally, self-affirmation is a guided-writing task in which individuals contemplate a cherished
personal value, such as relationships, creativity, or education (e.g., Sherman & Cohen, 2006).
Such contemplation affirms peoples sense of personal worth and self-integrity and allows them
to more effortlessly cope with both mundane stressors and experiences of discrimination. These
benefits can persist for months and even years after the initial intervention, because immediate
changes in self-integrity positively inf luence behavior and experiences over time, which further
reinforce self-worth. Thus, self-affirmation may be an effective tool to buffer higher body-
weight people against the stress of weight stigma.
Changing attributions. Higher body-weight people may be able to improve their well-being by
(accurately) attributing their weight status to factors that are primarily out of their control, such
as genes. Similarly, higher body-weight people may benefit from learning to attribute their neg-
ative experiences to the offending partys discrimination and prejudice, rather than their own
personal and moral shortcomings. Higher body-weight people typically attribute negative ex-
perience to their weight, and most importantly, they do not blame an offending party for his
or her transgression (Crocker et al., 1993). As a consequence of this attribution style, higher
body-weight people experience heightened negative mood, depression, and anxiety. In con-
trast, members of stigmatized groups who attribute their negative experiences to discrimination
and prejudice are buffered from many of the negative psychological consequences of discrimi-
nation (Crocker & Major, 1989). Thus, higher body-weight people who learn to embrace the
size-acceptance mantra my body is fine, its the world that is messed upmay also come to
benefit from these self-protective aspects of stigma.
Identifying with the stigmatized majority.Because most higher body-weight people do not want
to identify with fat peopleand have internalized the premises of the weight-loss solution,
higher body-weight people do not currently enjoy the stigma-buffering effects of group mem-
bership (Crocker & Major, 1989; Wang, Brownell, & Wadden, 2004). Thus, another strategy
for coping with weight stigma is to build social networks with other in-group members and
allies from the outgroup. The online size-acceptance community is doing just that in blogs
(see The Militant Baker, militantbaker.com; Dances with Fat, danceswithfat.wordpress.com;
and The Body is Not an Apology, thebodyisnotanapology.com). Research is needed to
examine the degree to which participation in the size-acceptance community supports higher
body-weight peoples well-being by encouraging them to reject the premise that thinness is a
prerequisite for beauty or self-acceptance and reduce the importance of body shape and size
for self-worth. The size-acceptance community also may help people to reaffirm their worth,
for example, by reappropriating the term fat(e.g., Galinsky et al., 2013; Wann, 1998) and cel-
ebrating the positive aspects of higher body weight, like softness and strength. By encouraging
social activism and confronting prejudice in daily life, these communities may increase feelings
of empowerment and self-confidence.
One aspect of group identification that is particularly applicable to higher body-weight peo-
ple is that of satisfaction ones positive attitudes about the group and ones membership in it
(Leach et al., 2008). This type of weight satisfaction confers numerous benefits, including pos-
itive health behaviors (e.g., less likely to diet and more physically active) and physical health
688 A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
outcomes (e.g., lower rates of hypertension, diabetes, and hypercholesterolemia), regardless of
body weight (Blake et al., 2013). Preliminary research suggests that, as a component of group
identity, weight satisfaction buffers the negative consequences of discrimination and increases
perceptions of injustice (Brochu & Jones, 2014).
Conclusion
People are tired of diets,tired of feeling like failures,and tired of being scared of food.They are excited to nd a par-
adigm that respects the diversity of human bodies and starts from the very basic premise that they can trust themselves,
a paradigm that respects pleasure rather than denial.
Linda Bacon (n. d.)
Nutrition professor and HAES
pioneer Linda Bacon, quoted above, describes an approach
towards weight and health that has, thus far, been uncommon in North America. As population
weights have risen, and the association between higher body weight and health problems has
been identified, researchers, health practitioners, and individuals have been striving to solve this
obesity problem. The approach so far has been to pursue weight loss. But this approach has
generally failed to produce lasting weight loss or health improvements and appears to contribute
to weight gain, weight cycling, weight bias, and body image dissatisfaction and preoccupation
ultimately undermining health and well-being. These costs are especially lamentable given that
research has not convincingly shown that higher body weight is the key cause of its associated
health problems.
We conclude that it is time to abandon the weight-loss solution. It is time for researchers,
health practitioners, and individuals to stop trying to find the rightdiet, and instead focus
on supporting health, broadly defined, by encouraging eating and exercising for well-being
rather than weight loss, by reducing weight bias and stigma, and by protecting higher body-
weight people from the damaging effects of discrimination.
Acknowledgements
This work was completed with the support of research grants awarded to Drs Logel and Stinson
by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC). We thank our
research assistants, Steven Hallman and Christopher Lok, for their help.
Short Biographies
Christine Logels research seeks to understand and address social-psychological barriers to
health, well-being, and educational success. She has authored or coauthored papers in Psycholo-
gical Science, Educational Psychologist, Journal of Educational Psychology, Journal or Personality and
Social Psychology, Journal of Experimental Psychology, and others. She is currently an assistant pro-
fessor in the Department of Social Development Studies at Renison University College, affili-
ated with the University of Waterloo in Ontario, Canada. She holds a BA in Psychology and
PhD in Social Psychology from the University of Waterloo and previously held postdoctoral
fellowships at the University of Colorado Boulder, Stanford University, and the University of
Waterloo. Her research is funded by SSHRC, HEQCO, and the University of Waterloos
Centre for Teaching Excellence.
Danu Anthony Stinsons research seeks to identify the consequences of different regulatory
strategies for peoples psychological, social, and physical well-being. Her recent publications ex-
amine the role of self-esteem and social relationships in health and well-being. Dr Stinsons
A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem689
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
SSHRC-funded work is published in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Psychological Sci-
ence, Social Psychology and Personality Science, Personal Relationships,andSelf and Identity.DrStinson
is an associate professor in the Psychology Department at the University of Victoria. She did her
undergraduate degree at the University of British Columbia and her PhD in Social Psychology
at the University of Waterloo, both in Canada, and a postdoctoral fellowship at State University
of New York, Buffalo.
Paula M. Brochus research examines the processes underlying the expression of prejudice, as
well as the consequences of stigma on human functioning. With obesity levels high and weight
bias rampant, she finds weight bias to be an informative domain to test, extend, and formulate
psychological theories of prejudice and stigma. Her research has been published in Social Psycho-
logical and Personality Science, Health Psychology, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Journal of
Applied Social Psychology,andGroup Processes and Intergroup Relations, among others. Dr Brochu
earned a BA in Psychology at the University of Saskatchewan and a PhD in Social Psychology
at the University of Western Ontario, both in Canada. She completed a postdoctoral fellowship
at Yale University, before accepting a position as an assistant professor at Southeastern Nova
University in Florida.
Note
*Correspondence: Department of Social Development Studies, Renison University College, University of Waterloo,
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G4. Email: clogel@uwaterloo.ca
References
Adams, C. E., & Leary, M. R. (2007). Promoting self-compassionate attitudes toward eating among restrictive and guilty
eaters. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,26,11201144.
Amy, N. K., Aalborg, A., Lyons, P., & Keranen, L. (2006). Barriers to routine gynecological cancer screening for White and
African-American obese women. International Journal of Obesity,30,147155.
American Heart Association. (2014, March 14). Understanding the American Obesity Epidemic. Retrieved on August 10,
2015, from http://www.heart.org/HEARTORG/GettingHealthy/WeightManagement/Obesity/Understanding-the-
American-Obesity-Epidemic_UCM_461650_Article.jsp
Aphramor, L. (2010). Validity of claims made in weight management research: A narrative review of dietetic articles.
Nutrition Journal,9,19.
Bacon, L. (n. d). The Pledge Concept. HAES Connections. Retrieved August 6, 2015, from http://www.haescommunity.
org/about.php
Bacon, L. (2008). Health at Every Size: The Surprising Truth About Your Weight. Dallas, TX: BenBella Books Inc.
Bacon, L., & Aphramor, L. (2014). Body Respect: What Conventional Health Books Get Wrong, Leave Out, and Just Plain Fail to
Understand About Weight. Dallas, TX: BenBella Books Inc.
Bacon, L., & Aphramor, L. (2011). Weight science: Evaluating the evidence for a paradigm shift. Nutrition Journal,10,113.
Bacon, L., Stern, J. S., Van Loan, M. D., & Keim, N. L. (2005). Size acceptance and intuitive eating improve health for
obese, female chronic dieters. Journal of the American Dietetic Association,105,929936.
Baker, J. (2015). Scars, self harm, and the Saturday I stopped giving a shit. The Militant Baker. Retrieved August 10, 2015
from http://www.themilitantbaker.com/2013/10/scars-self-harm-and-saturday-i-stopped.html
Baum, C. L., & Ford, W. F. (2004). The wage effects of obesity: A longitudinal study. Health Economics,13,885899.
Björntorp, P. (2001). Do stress reactions cause abdominal obesity and comorbidities? Obesity Reviews,2,7386.
Blake, C. E., Hébert, J. R., Lee, D. C., Adams, S. A., Steck, S. E., Sui, X., etal. (2013). Adults with greater weight satisfaction
report more positive health behaviors and have better health status regardless of BMI. JournalofObesity,2013,291371.
Bobo, L. (2001). Racial attitudes and relations at the close of the twentieth century. America becoming: Racial trends and their
consequences,1,264301.
Brochu, P. M., & Esses, V. M. (2011). Whats in a name? The effects of the labels fatversus overweighton weight bias.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology,41,19812008.
Brochu, P. M., & Dovidio, J.(2013). Would you like fries (380 calories) with that? The effect of weight stigma threat on food
choice. Canadian Journal of Diabetes,37,S276S277.
690 A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
Brochu, P. M., & Jones, J. M. (2014, June). Shopping while fat: Does group identity buffer against stigmatizing experiences?
Paper presented at the 10thBiennial Conventionof the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues, Portland, OR.
Brochu, P. M., Pearl, R. L., Puhl, R. M., & Brownell, K. D. (2014). Do media portrayals of obesity inuence support for
weight-related medical policy? Health Psychology,33,197200.
Bruno, B. A. (2013, April 30). History of the Health at Every Size® Movement, Part I. the HAES® les. Retrieved August 6,
2015, from http://healthateverysizeblog.org/2013/04/30/the-haes-les-history-of-the-health-at-every-size-movement-
part-i/
Brownell, K.D. (1991). Dieting and the search for the perfect body: Where physiology and culture collide. Behavior Therapy,
22,112.
Burmeister, J. M., Kiefner, A. E., Carels, R. A., & Musher-Eizenman, D. R. (2013). Weight bias in graduate school
admissions. Obesity,21,918920.
Calle, E. E., Thun, M. J., Petrelli, J. M., Rodriguez, C., & Heath, C. W. Jr. (1999). Body-mass index and mortality in a
prospective cohort of U.S. adults. New England Journal of Medicine,341,10971105.
Cavanagh, K., Strauss, C., Forder, L., & Jones, F. (2014). Can mindfulness and acceptance be learnt by self-help? A systematic
review and meta-analysis of mindfulness and acceptance-based self-help interventions. Clinical Psychology Review,34,
118129.
Centers for Disease Control (2014a, November 12). Workplace Health Promotion: Obesity Prevention and Control.
Retrieved August 6, 2015, from http://www.cdc.gov/workplacehealthpromotion/implementation/topics/obesity.html
Centers for Disease Control (2014b, September 9). Adult Obesity Facts. Retrieved April 2, 2015, from http://www.cdc.
gov/obesity/data/adult.html
Centers for Disease Control. (n. d). Losing Weight. Retrieved April 2, 2015, from http://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/
losing_weight/index.html
Centers for Disease Control: Vital Signs. (2010, August 3). Adult Obesity. Retrieved April 2, 2015, from http://www.cdc.
gov/vitalsigns/pdf/2010-08-vitalsigns.pdf
Cohen, S. (2004). Social relationships and health. American Psychologist,59,676.
Cohen, S., Tyrrell, D. A., & Smith, A. P. (1991). Psychological stress and susceptibility to the common cold. New England
JournalofMedicine,325,606612.
Corrales-Medina, V. F., Valayam, J., Serpa, J. A., Rueda, A. M., & Musher, D. M. (2011). The obesity paradox in
community-acquired bacterial pneumonia. International Journal of Infectious Diseases,15,5457.
Connelly, C.(2015, January 9). Tell a Friend Theyre Fat Day. The Social: Health. Retrieved August 10, 2015, from http://
www.thesocial.ca/wellness/health/tell-a-friend-they-re-fat-day
Cramer, P., & Steinwert, T. (1998). Thin is good, fat is bad: How early does it begin? Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology,19,429451.
Crocker, J., Cornwell, B., & Major, B. (1993). The stigma of overweight: Affective consequences of attributional ambiguity.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,64,6070.
Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review,
96,608630.
Dallman, M. F. (2010). Stress-induced obesity and the emotional nervous system. Trends in Endocrinology & Metabolism,21,
159165.
Daníelsdóttir, S., OBrien, K. S., & Ciao, A. (2010). Anti-fat prejudice reduction: A review of published studies. Obesity Facts,
3,4758.
Daye, C. A., Webb, J. B., & Jafari, N. (2014). Exploring self-compassion as a refuge against recalling the body-related
shaming of caregiver eating messages on dimensions of objectied body consciousness in college women. Body Image,
11,547556.
Dovidio, J. F., Newheiser, A., & Leyens, J. P. (2011). A history of intergroup relations research. In A. W. Kruglanski & W.
Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of the History of Social Psychology (pp. 407429). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Farrell, S. W., Braun, L., Barlow, C. E., Cheng, Y. J., & Blair, S. N. (2002). The relation of body mass index,
cardiorespiratory tness, and all-cause mortality in women. Obesity Research,10,417423.
Field, A. E., Austin, S. B., Taylor, C. B., Malspeis, S., Rosner, B., Rockett, H. R., et al. (2003). Relation between dieting
and weight change among preadolescents and adolescents. Pediatrics,112,900906.
Flegal, K. M., Graubard, B. I., Williamson, D. F., & Gail, M. H. (2005). Excess deaths associated with underweight,
overweight, and obesity. JAMA,293,18611867.
Flegal,K.M.,Kit,B.K.,Orpana,H.,&Graubard,B.I.(2013).Associationofall-causemortalitywithoverweightandobesityusing
standard body mass index categories: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association,309,7182.
FAO (2000). The State of Food Insecurity in the World (2nd edn). Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.
Retrieved April 2, 2015, from http://www.fao.org/hunger/en/
Foster, G. D., Wadden, T. A., Makris, A. P., Davidson, D., Sanderson, R. S., Allison, D. B., et al. (2003). Primary care
physiciansattitudes about obesity and its treatment. Obesity Research,11,11681177.
Friedman, J. M. (2004). Modern science versus the stigma of obesity. Nature Medicine,10,563569.
A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem691
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
Friedman, J. M. (2003). A war on obesity, not the obese. Science,299,856858.
Galinsky, A. D., Wang, C. S., Whitson, J. A., Anicich, E. M., Hugenberg, K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2013). The reappropriation
of stigmatizing labels: The reciprocal relationship between power and self-labeling. Psychological Science,24, 20202029.
Garner, D. M., & Wooley, S. C. (1991). Confronting the failure of behavioral and dietary treatments for obesity. Clinical
Psychology Review,11,729780.
Grabowski, D. C., & Ellis, J. E. (2001), High body mass index does not predict mortality in older people: Analysis of the
Longitudinal Study of Aging. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,49,968979.
Greenberg, B. S., Eastin, M., Hofschire, L., Lachlan, K., & Brownell, K. (2003). Portrayals of overweight and obese
individuals on commercial television. American Journal of Public Health,93, 13421348.
Grundy, S. M., Blackburn, G., Higgins, M., Lauer, R., Perri, M. G., & Ryan, D. (1999). Roundtable consensus statement:
Physical activity in the prevention and treatment of obesity and its comorbidities. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise
(pp. S502S508). Indianapolis: American College of Sports Medicine.
Gudzune, K. A., Beach, M. C., Roter, D. L., & Cooper, L. A. (2013). Physicians build less rapport with obese patients.
Obesity,21,21462152.
Harding, K., & Kirby, M. (2009). Lessons from the Fat-o-sphere: Quit Dieting and Declare a Truce with Your Body. NY: Penguin.
Hatzenbuehler, M. L., OCleirigh, C., Grasso, C., Mayer, K., Safren, S., & Bradford, J. (2012). Effect of same-sex marriage
laws on health care use and expenditures in sexual minority men: A quasi-natural experiment. American Journal of Public
Health,102,285291.
Hausenblas, H. A., & Fallon, E. A. (2006). Exercise and body image: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Health,21,3347.
Hedley, A. A., Ogden, C. L., Johnson, C. L., Carroll, M. D., Curtin, L. R., & Flegal, K. M. (2004). Prevalence of overweight
and obesity among US children, adolescents, and adults, 19992002. JAMA,291,28472850.
Heuer, C. A., McClure, K. J., & Puhl, R. M. (2011). Obesity stigma in online news: A visual content analysis. Journal of
Health Communication,16,976987.
Hilbert, A., & Ried, J. (2009). Obesity in print: An analysis of daily newspapers. Obesity Facts,2,4651.
Hunger, J. M., & Major, B. (2015). Weight stigma mediates the association between BMI and self reported health. Health
Psychology,34,172175.
Hunger, J. M., Major, B., Blodorn, A., & Miller, C. (2015). Weighed down by stigma: How weight-based social identity
threat inuences weight gain and health. Social Psychology and Personality Compass,6,255268.
Jackson, S. E., Beeken, R. J., & Wardle, J. (2014). Perceived weight discrimination and changes in weight, waist
circumference, and weight status. Obesity,22,24852488.
Jackson, S. E., Beeken, R. J., & Wardle, J. (2015). Obesity, perceived weight discrimination, and psychological well-being in
older adults in England. Obesity. doi: 10.1002/oby.21052
Jayakody, K., Gunadasa, S., & Hosker, C. (2014). Exercise for anxiety disorders: Systematic review. British Journal of Sports
Medicine,48,187196.
Jenny Craig FAQs. (n. d). Retrieved March 31, 2015, from http://www.jennycraig.com/site/how-it-works/help#thirty
Junkfood Science. (2008, January 31). No Fat People Allowed: Only the Slim Will Be Allowed to Dine in Public! Retrieved
on August 10, 2015, from http://junkfoodscience.blogspot.ca/2008/01/no-fat-people-allowed-only-slim-will-be.html
Kahan, D., Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2003). Conformity and dietary disinhibition: A test of the ego-strength model of
self-regulation. International Journal of Eating Disorders,33,165171.
Kassirer, J. P., & Angell, M. (1998). Losing weight An ill-fated New Years resolution. The New England Journal of Medicine,338,52.
Katterman, S. N., Kleinman, B. M., Hood, M. M., Nackers, L. M., & Corsica, J. A. (2014). Mindfulness meditation as an
intervention for binge eating, emotional eating, and weight loss: A systematic review. Eating Behaviors,15,197204.
Katz, D. L. (2005). Competing dietary claims for weight loss: Finding the forest through truculent trees. Annual Review of
Public Health,26,6188.
Katz, D. L., & Meller, S. (2014). Can we say what diet is best for health? Annual Review of Public Health,35,83103.
Keesey, R. E., & Powley, T. L. (2008). Body energy homeostasis. Appetite,51,442445.
Keng, S. L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of empirical
studies. Clinical Psychology Review,31, 10411056.
Kessler, D. (2010). The End of Overeating: Taking Control of the Insatiable American Appetite. New York, Macmullan.
Kopple, J. D. (2005). The phenomenon of altered risk factor patterns or reverse epidemiology in persons with advanced
chronic kidney failure. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition,81,12571266.
LaMonte, M. J., & Blair, S. N. (2006). Physical activity, cardiorespiratory tness, and adiposity: Contributions to disease risk.
Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition & Metabolic Care,9,540546.
Latner, J. D., & Stunkard, A. J. (2003). Getting worse: The stigmatization of obese children. Obesity Research,11,452456.
Lavie, C. J., Milani, R. V., & Ventura, H. O. (2009). Risk factor, paradox, and impact of weight loss. Obesity and
Cardiovascular Disease,53,19251932.
Leach, C. W., Zomeren, M. V., Zebel, S., Vliek, M. L. W., Ouerkerk, J. W., & Spears, R. (2008). Group-level self-
denition and self-investment: A hierarchical (multicomponent) model of in-group identication. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,95,144165.
692 A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
Lee, M., Ata,R. N., & Brannick, M. T. (2014). Malleability of weight-biasedattitudes and beliefs: A meta-analysis of weight
bias reduction interventions. Body Image,11,251259.
Lee, C. D., Blair, S. N., & Jackson, A. S. (1999). Cardiorespiratory tness, body composition, and all-cause and
cardiovascular disease mortality in men. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition,69,373380.
Leibel, R. L., Rosenbaum, M., & Hirsch, J. (1995). Changes in energy expenditure resulting from altered body weight. New
England Journal of Medicine,332,621628.
Logel, C., Stinson, D. A., Gunn, G. R., Wood, J. V., Holmes, J. G., & Cameron, J. J. (2014). A little acceptance is good for
your health: Interpersonal messages and weight change over time. Personal Relationships,21,583598.
Mandaro, L. (2014, November 5). Nations First Soda Tax Is Passed. Retrieved March 27, 2015, from http://www.
usatoday.com/story/news/nation-now/2014/11/05/berkeley-passes-soda-tax/18521923/
Mann, T., Tomyama, A. J., Westling, E., Lew, A. M., Samuels, B., Chatman, J. (2007). Medicares search for effective
obesity treatments: Diets are not the answer. American Psychology,62,220233.
Major, B., Eliezer, D., & Rieck, H. (2012). The psychological weight of weight stigma. Social Psychological and Personality
Science,3,651658.
Major, B., Hunger, J. M., Bunyan, D. P., & Miller, C. T. (2014). The ironic effects of weight stigma. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology,51,7480.
Major, B., Mendes, W. B., & Dovidio, J. F. (2013). Intergroup relations and health disparities: A social psychological
perspective. Health Psychology,32,514524.
Markowitz, S., Friedman, M. A., & Arent, S.M. (2008). Understanding the relation between obesity and depression: Causal
mechanisms and implications for treatment. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,15,120.
Miller, C. T., & Major, B. (2000). Coping with stigma and prejudice. In T. Heatherton, R. Kleck, M. R. Hebl & J. G. Hall
(Eds.), The Social Psychology of Stigma (pp. 243272). New York, NY: Guilford.
Mokdad, A. H., Marks, J. S., Stroup, D. F., Gerberding, J. L.(2004). Actual causes of death in the United States, (2000).
JAMA,291, 12381245.
Montani, J. P., Schutz, Y., & Dulloo, A. G. (2015). Dieting and weight cycling as risk factors for cardiometabolic diseases:
Who is really at risk? Obesity Reviews,16,718.
Muennig, P., Jia, H., Lee, R., & Lubetkin, E. (2008). I think therefore I am: Perceived ideal weight as a determinant of
health. American Journal of Public Health,98,501506.
National Association to Advance Fat Acceptance. (n. d). Weight Discrimination Laws. Retrieved on August 10, 2015, from
http://www.naafaonline.com/dev2/education/laws.html
Neff, K. D. (2011). Self-compassion, self-esteem, and well-being. Social and Personality Compass,5,112.
Neumark-Sztainer, D., Paxton, S. J., Hannan, P. J., Haines, J., & Story, M. (2006). Does body satisfaction matter? Five-year
longitudinal associations between body satisfaction and health behaviors in adolescent females and males. The Journal of
Adolescent Health,39,244251.
Norcross, J., Mrykolo, M., Blagys, M. (2002). Auld Lang Syne: Success predictors, change processes, and self-reported
outcomes of New Years resolves and non-resolvers. Journal of Clinical Psychology,58,397405.
Nutrisystem: Information for Your Doctor. (n. d). Retrieved March 31, 2015, from http://www.nutrisystem.com/pdf/
Information_for_your_doctor.pdf
OBrien, K., Puhl, R., Latner, J., Mir, A., & Hunter, J. (2010). Reducing anti-fat prejudice in preservice health students: A
randomized trial. Obesity Journal,18,21382144.
ORahilly, S. & Farooqi, I. S. (2008). Human obesity: A heritable neurobehavioral disorder that is highly sensitive to
environmental conditions. Diabetes,57,29052910.
Orbach, S. (2006). Commentary: There is a public health crisis Its not fat on the body but fat in the mind and the fat of
prots. International Journal of Epidemiology,35,6769.
Orpana, H. M., Berthelot, J. M., Kaplan, M. S., Feeny, D. H., McFarland, B. & Ross, N. A. (2010), BMI and mortality:
Results from a national longitudinal study of Canadian adults. Obesity,18,214218.
Oz, M. (2012, April). Dr. Oz Reveals the 5 Diet Myths Making You Gain Weight. Retrieved April 2, 2015, from http://
www.oprah.com/health/Dr-Oz-Weight-Loss-Advice-Diet-Myths-Surprise-Weight-Gain
Pearl, R. L., Puhl, R. M., & Brownell, K. D. (2012). Positive media portrayals of obese persons: Impact on attitudes and
image preferences. Health Psychology,31,821829.
Penedo, F. J., & Dahn, J. R. (2005). Exerciseand well-being: A review of mental and physical health benets associated with
physical activity. Current Opinion in Psychiatry,18(2), 189193.
Pomeranz, J. L., & Puhl, R. M. (2013). New developments in the law for obesity discrimination protection. Obesity,21,
469471.
Prentice, A. M., & Jebb, S. A. (1995). Obesity in Britain: Gluttony or sloth? British Medical Journal,311,437439.
Puhl, R. M., & Brownell, K. D. (2006). Confronting and coping with weight stigma: An investigation of overweight and
obese adults. Obesity,14, 18021815.
Puhl, R. M. & Brownell, K. D. (2001), Bias, discrimination, and obesity. Obesity 9,788805.
Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: A review and update. Obesity,17,941964.
A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem693
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2010). Obesity stigma: Important considerations for public health. American Journal of Public
Health,100,10191028.
Puhl, R. M., Heuer, C., & Sarda, V. (2011). Framing messages about weight discrimination. Impact on public support for
legislation. International Journal of Obesity (2005), 35,863872.
Puhl, R., Luedicke, J., & Peterson, J. L. (2013). Public reactions to obesity-related health campaigns: A randomized
controlled trial. American Journal of Preventive Medicine,45,3648.
Puhl, R. M., Moss-Racusin, C. A., & Schwartz, M. B. (2007). Internalization of weight bias: Implications for binge eating
and emotional well-being. Obesity,15,1923.
Puhl, R., Peterson, J. L., & Luedicke, J. (2013). Fighting obesity or obese persons? Public perceptions of obesity-related
health messages. International Journal of Obesity,37,7.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1983, December). Women and weight: A normative discontent. In
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.Nebraska Symposium on Motivation,32,267307.
Roehling, M. V. (1999). Weight-based discrimination in employment: Psychological and legal aspects. Personnel Psychology,
52,9691016.
Roehling, M. V., Pichler, S., & Bruce, T. A. (2013). Moderators of the effect of weight on job-related outcomes: A meta-
analysis of experimental studies. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,43,237252.
Sartore, M. L., & Cunningham, G. B. (2007). Weight discrimination, hiring recommendations, person-job t, and
attributions: Fitness-industry implications. Journal of Sport Management,21,172193.
Schaefer, J. T., & Magnuson, A. B. (2014). A review of interventions that promote eating by internal cues. Journal of the
Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics,114,734760.
Schvey, N. A., Puhl, R. M., & Brownell, K. D. (2011). The impact of weight stigma on caloric consumption. Obesity,19,
19571962.
Schwartz, M. B., Vartanian, L. R., Nosek, B. A., Brownell, K. D. (2006). The inuence of ones own body weight on
implicit and explicit anti-fat bias. Obesity,14,440447.
Scully, D., Kremer, J., Meade, M. M., Graham, R., & Dudgeon, K. (1998). Physical exercise and psychological well-being:
Acriticalreview.British Journal of Sports Medicine,32,111120.
Shade, E. D., Ulrich, C. M., Wener, M. H., Wood, B., Yasui, Y., Lacroix, K., et al. (2004). Frequent intentional weight loss
is associated with lower natural killer cell cytotoxicity in postmenopausal women: Possible long-term immune effects.
Journal of the American Dietetic Association,104,903912.
Sherman D. K., Cohen G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense: Self-afrmationtheory. In Zanna M. P. (Ed.), Advances
in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 183242). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Shick, S. M., Wing, R. R., Klem, M. L., McGuire, M. T., Hill, J. O., & Seagle, H. (1998). Persons successful at long-term
weight loss and maintenance continue to consume a low-energy, low-fat diet. Journal of the American Dietetic Association,98,
408413.
Sin, N. L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms with positive psychology
interventions: A practice-friendly meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology,65,467487.
Sørensen, T. I., Price, R. A., Stunkard, A. J., & Schulsinger, F. (1989). Genetics of obesity in adult adoptees and their
biological siblings. The British Medical Journal,298,8790.
Stessman, J., Jacobs, J. M., Ein-Mor, E., & Bursztyn, M. (2009). Normal body mass index rather than obesity predicts
greater mortality in elderly people: The Jerusalem Longitudinal Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,57,
22322238.
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social
identity threat. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,(Vol.34,pp.379440). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Stinson, D. A., Logel, C., Zanna, M. P., Holmes, J. G., Cameron, J. J., Wood, J. V., et al. (2008). The cost of lower self-
esteem: Testing a self- and social-bonds model of health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,94,412428.
Stunkard, A. J., Harris, J. R., Pedersen, N. L., & McClearn, G. E. (1990). The body-mass index of twins who have been
reared apart. New England Journal of Medicine,322,14831487.
Stunkard, A. J., Sørensen, T. I., Hanis, C., Teasdale, T. W., Chakraborty, R., Schull, W. J., & Schulsinger, F. (1986). An
adoption study of human obesity. New England Journal of Medicine,314,193198.
Sturm, R., & Hattori, A. (2013). Morbid obesity rates continue to rise rapidly in the US. International Journal of Obesity,37,
889891.
Sumithran, P., Prendergast, L. A., Delbridge, E., Purcell, K., Shulkes, A., Kriketos, A. et al. (2011). Long-term persistence of
hormonal adaptations to weight loss. New England Journal of Medicine,365, 15971604.
Suh, Y., Puhl,R., Liu, S., & Fleming Milici, F. (2014). Support for laws to prohibit weight discrimination in the united states:
Public attitudes from 2011 to 2013. Obesity,22,18721879.
Sutin, A. R., & Terracciano, A. (2013). Perceived weight discrimination and obesity. PloS One,8,e70048e70048.
Tamakoshi, A., Yatsuya, A., Lin, Y., Tamakoshi, K., Kondo, T., Suzuki, S. et al.(2010). BMI and all-cause mortality among
Japanese older adults: Findings from the Japan Collaborative Cohort Study. Obesity,18,362369.
694 A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
The State of Obesity. (2015). Federal funding for obesity prevention. Retrieved March 31, 2015 from http://stateofobesity.
org/federal-funding-obesity-prevention/
Tomiyama, A. J., Ahlstrom, B., & Mann, T. (2013). Long-term effects of dieting: Is weight loss related to health? Social and
Personality Psychology Compass,7,861877.
Tomiyama, A. J., Mann, T., Vinas, D., Hunger, J. M., DeJager, J., & Taylor, S. E. (2010). Low calorie dieting increases
cortisol. Psychosomatic Medicine,72,357364.
Tomiyama, A. J., Epel, E. S., McClatchey, T. M., Poelke, G., Kemeny, M. E., McCoy, S. K., et al. (2014). Associations of
weight stigma with cortisol and oxidative stress independent of adiposity. Health Psychology,33,862867.
Torres, S. J., & Nowson, C. A. (2007). Relationship between stress, eating behavior, and obesity. Nutrition,23,887894.
Turner, T. (2011, April 21). The 2011 TIME 100. Retrieved April 2, 2015, from http://content.time.com/time/specials/
packages/article/0,28804,2066367_2066369_2066094,00.html
Tylka, T. L., Annunziato, R.A., Burgard, D., Danielsdottir, S., Shuman, E., Davis, C., et al. (2014). The weight-inclusive vs.
weight-normative approach to health: Evaluating the evidence for prioritizing well-being over weight. JournalOfObesity,
118. doi: 10.1155/2014/983495
van den Berg, P., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2007). Fatn happy 5 years later: Is it bad for overweight girls to like their bodies?
JournalofAdolescentHealth,41,415417.
Venes, A. M., Krupka, L. R., & Gerard, R. J. (1982). Overweight/obese patients: An overview. The Practitioner,226,
11021109.
Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science,11,
249254.
Wann, M. (1998). Fat! So?: Because You Dont Have to Apologize for Your Size!. New York: Random House LLC.
Wang, Y., & Beydoun, M. A. (2007). The obesity epidemic in the United States Gender, age, socioeconomic,
racial/ethnic, and geographic characteristics: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Epidemiologic Reviews,29,
628.
Wang, S. S., Brownell, K. D., & Wadden, T. A. (2004). The inuence of the stigma of obesity on overweight individuals.
International Journal of Obesity,28, 13331337.
Wansink, B., & Chandon, P. (2014). Slim by design: Redirecting the accidental drivers of mindless overeating. Journal of
Consumer Psychology,24,413431.
Warburton, D. E., Nicol, C.W., & Bredin, S. S. (2006). Health benets of physical activity: The evidence. Canadian Medical
Association Journal,174,801809
Weiss, E. C., Galuska, D. A., Khan, L. K., Serdula, M. K. 2006. Weight control practices among U.S. adults, 20012002.
American Journal of Preventive Medicine,31(1), 1824.
Werle, C. O., Wansink, B., & Payne, C. R. (2014). Is it fun or exercise? The framing of physical activity biases subsequent
snacking. Marketing Letters,2,112.
West, L. (2011, February 11). The Stranger: Hello, I Am Fat. Retrieved on August 10, 2015, from http://slog.thestranger.
com/slog/archives/2011/02/11/hello-i-am-fat.
Westerterp, K. R., & Speakman, J. R. (2008). Physical activity energy expenditure has not declined since the 1980s and
matches energy expenditures of wild mammals. International Journal of Obesity,32,12561263.
Wie, M., Kampert, J. B., Barlow, C. E., Nichaman, M. Z.,Gibbons, L. W., Paffenbarger, R.S., Jr, et al. (1999). Relationship
between low cardiorespiratory tness and mortality in normal-weight, overweight, and obese men. Journal of the American
Medical Association,282,15471553.
Wilkinson, R. G., & Marmot, M.G. (Eds.). (2003). Social Determinants of Health: The Solid Facts. World Health Organization.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Winfrey, O. (2009, January). How did I let this happen again? Oprah Magazine. Retrieved Feb 25, 2015 from http://www.
oprah.com/spirit/Oprahs-Battle-with-Weight-Gain-O-January-2009-Cover
World Health Organization. (2015a). Controlling the Global Obesity Epidemic. Retrieved April 2, 2015, from http://
www.who.int/nutrition/topics/obesity/en/
World Health Organization. (2015b). Overweight and Obesity. Retrieved April 2, 2015, from http://www.who.int/gho/
ncd/risk_factors/overweight_text/en/
World Health Organization. (2003, para. 1). WHO Denition of Health. Retrieved April 1, 2015 from http://www.who.
int/about/denition/en/print.html
A Well-being Solution to the Obesity Problem695
© 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9/11 (2015): 678695, 10.1111/spc3.12223
... weight stigma and discrimination at broad, societal levels as well as in health care settings and interactions (Brochu, Pearl, & Simontacchi, 2018;Hunger, Smith, & Tomiyama, 2020;Logel, Stinson, & Brochu, 2015;Pearl, 2018). These include, for example, health care professionals reflecting on self-assessment of their own beliefs and attitudes about higher-weight people, creating inclusive public spaces (e.g., comfortable seating, navigable doorways and halls, properly sized gowns, and blood pressure cuffs), and engaging in respectful communication (e.g., assessment of language preferences; Brochu et al., 2018). ...
... More work is needed to reduce weight bias at the societal level and weight bias internalization at the individual level. The adoption of weight-inclusive health care approaches that focus on health and well-being for all people regardless of size, rather than weight and weight loss, represents one foundational approach that supports these efforts Logel et al., 2015;Tylka et al., 2014). Such approaches hold promise for reducing weight bias among health care professionals and self-blame among higher-weight patients. ...
... Moreover, this definition of normal was based on a measurement that had significant limitations as it related to measuring health-related outcomes and mortality. Studies had found that BMI did not correlate with increases in mortality (Logel, Stinson, & Brochu, 2015;Major et al., 2014;Major et al., 2018;Tomiyama et al., 2018), rendering the claim that an increase in BMI was problematic or an "epidemic" tenuous. Guthman (2013) also cited evidence that the "epidemic" claim may be a conflation of the phenomenon that larger individuals are getting larger, increasing the mean BMI found in the population. ...
... It is well-documented that being encouraged to diet, receiving weight-related teasing or comments, or being labeled "too fat" predicts disordered eating behaviors and lower psychosocial well-being (Berge et al., 2019;Eisenberg et al., 2012;Hunger & Tomiyama, 2018). Whereas weight-normative approaches to health emphasize weight and weight loss when defining health, weight-inclusive approaches to health emphasize holistic aspects of health while seeking to improve health access and reduce weight stigma (Logel et al., 2015;Tylka et al., 2014). More research on weight stigma in relationships is needed from researchers who adopt weight-inclusive perspectives. ...
Article
Romantic relationship quality is a robust predictor of health and well‐being. With increasing awareness of the pervasiveness and harm of weight stigma, it is important to understand the role of weight stigma within romantic relationships. This systematic review sought to synthesize the findings of research examining the association between weight stigma and relationship functioning. Following PRISMA guidelines, 32 relevant articles were identified. Only nine assessed or manipulated weight stigma directly; most measured body mass index (BMI) and examined associations with relationship outcomes. Although, the association between BMI and relationship functioning was inconsistent across studies, weight stigma, most notably in the form of weight criticism between partners, was consistently associated with poorer relationship functioning, including lower relationship satisfaction, sexual intimacy, relationship stability, and constructive communication during conflict. The existing literature is limited by convenience samples of primarily White, heterosexual adults in individualistic countries. Several studies reinforced and expressed weight stigmatizing beliefs due to reliance on weight‐normative perspectives on health to interpret findings. Future research is encouraged to examine the association between weight stigma and relationship functioning and underlying mechanisms using dyadic, longitudinal designs that incorporate weight‐inclusive approaches. The development of couples‐based interventions to address weight stigma in relationships is sorely needed.
... In the years since the BMI classification system was adopted by the World Health Organization, it has received criticism regarding its validity and usefulness. There is a widespread belief that higher BMI leads to poorer health [15,42]; however, critics of the BMI have noted that the relationship between BMI and health may be confounded by third variables, including socioeconomic status, stress, metabolic dysfunction, and physical activity [43,44]. Accordingly, many individuals who fall into the overweight and obese categories can be accurately classified as "healthy" based on a variety of cardiometabolic indicators, though misclassifications are common [45,46]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of Review Body mass index (BMI) outside of the “normal” range is commonly cited as a predictor of adverse health outcomes and has been identified as a potential risk factor for suicidal thoughts and behaviors (STBs). This meta-analysis provides a descriptive and quantitative summary of the literature evaluating the longitudinal relationship between BMI/weight status and STBs. Recent Findings The longitudinal literature examining the relationship between BMI/weight status and STBs is small and methodologically constrained. Within the existing literature, BMI and weight status are generally weak or nonsignificant risk factors for STBs. It is possible that body weight has a complex relationship with physical and mental health, including STBs, which may not be possible to accurately capture with a singular metric such as BMI. Summary BMI and weight status do not appear to robustly predict STBs, at least within the methodological constraints of the existing literature.
... Parents were informed that the study was focused on factors that may be associated with parental feeding practices and aimed to examine the interaction between attitudes towards weight, thoughts about food, 1 In line with previous work, we have used the term body-weight (i.e., higher body-weight, lower body-weight) throughout this paper to describe the "relative fatness or leanness" of people (Blodorn et al., 2016;Logel et al., 2015, p. 4). Alternative terms such as "overweight" and "obese" will be presented within quotation marks because they are stigmatizing and represent arbitrarily defined classification categories (Blodorn et al., 2016;Logel et al., 2015). and parental feeding styles. ...
Article
Parental restriction of food intake has been associated with heightened eating disorder psychopathology in some longitudinal research. Yet, relatively little is known about the determinants of restrictive feeding practices. This cross-sectional study explored the association between parents' anti-fat attitudes and their use of restrictive feeding practices in a mixed British (41.10% England, 39.90% Scotland, 4.20% Other) and Irish (14.80%) sample. Parents and caregivers (N = 472; 94.10% female; 70.90% university level education) of children between the ages of 4-8 (48.20% female; 91.10% rated as "normal weight" by their parents) completed self-report questionnaires assessing their anti-fat attitudes (dislike, fear, and blame subscales), use of restrictive feeding practices (for weight control, health purposes, and covert restriction), and how influential their child's body-weight and -shape is for their perception of themselves as parents. Overall, our hypothesis that parental anti-fat attitudes would be significantly associated with restrictive feeding practices was supported. Anti-fat attitudes related to disliking higher body-weight people and blaming parents for their child's weight were significant predictors of all forms of restrictive feeding (all ps < .05). However, anti-fat attitudes related to fearing being a higher body-weight were not significant predictors of restrictive feeding for the purposes of health nor for covert restriction (ps > .05). Additionally, our hypothesis that the associations between anti-fat attitudes and restrictive feeding practices would be stronger for parents for whom their child's body-weight and -shape more strongly influenced how they judged themselves as parents was not supported (the interaction term was not significant in two out of three analyses). Future research is needed to investigate these associations across time and in samples of higher body-weight children.
... Based on the findings that acceptance and settlement with body size, weight and physical capability added to the participants' embodied dwelling and mobility we argue that well-being in identity can develop and facilitate change. This study's emphasis on dwelling and mobility is a contribution to existing research suggesting that promotion of well-being among persons with obesity should replace the existing focus on body size and weight [40,41]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Maintaining a healthy living after the end of a lifestyle intervention is a challenge for persons with severe obesity. Measurable outcomes are often emphasised, but there is a need for understanding the process of lifestyle change and the long-term perspectives among persons with severe obesity. Aim and objective: To describe and deepen the understanding of how persons with severe obesity experience making and maintaining lifestyle changes in everyday life three years after lifestyle intervention. Methodology and methods: The study used a hermeneutic phenomenological approach. A purposive sample of seven adults with BMI ≥ 40 was recruited from a lifestyle intervention programme. Data were generated through individual follow-up interviews. The analysis was based on theoretical framework on dwelling and mobility. Ethical approvals were received from the Danish Data Protection Agency and the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki were followed. Results: One overarching theme emerged: ‘The journey of ups and downs’. Three sub-themes were: ‘Living with and tackling the demands of life's hassles’, ‘Deliberating the fight for weight loss’ and ‘Needing a trusted person to feel met as a human being’. Conclusions: Everyday life among persons with severe obesity is experienced as a dynamic process of shifting experiences of dwelling and mobility. Sustained lifestyle changes require ongoing adjustments of action, which healthcare providers can influence in ways that either support or obstruct. Collaboration and a humanised approach across disciplines and sectors are suggested to promote sustained healthy living.
Article
We offer a primer for researchers who seek to carry out studies that evaluate the lived experience of larger‐bodied workers. We use objectification theory to describe the process by which intraculturally‐determined body size preferences impact how observers think about and react to larger‐bodied colleagues, and how these larger‐bodied colleagues internalize and cope with these judgements. Arguing that exploration of the objectification of larger‐bodied professionals is incomplete without the use of multidisciplinary lenses, we describe mechanisms that reinforce weight stigma, including the role of healthism‐based value systems, intersectionality, and body image. We conclude the primer by outlining areas for new research that highlights burgeoning applied demand for more nuanced, evidence‐based discussions of weight at work. In professional spaces, many workers feel comfortable “objectifying” their colleagues who occupy larger (i.e., “overweight”/”obese”) bodies. This means that workers (a) constantly compare their colleagues’ bodies to a “thin” standard, (b) feel a certain comfort in remarking on their larger‐bodied colleagues’ size, and (c) this judgment feeds into a cycle of self‐consciousness and self‐dislike that many larger‐bodied workers feel about themselves. This objectification process can be doubly harmful to the well‐being of larger‐bodied workers when they, too, occupy a secondary marginalized identity/ies, such as being female or of a minority ethnicity. The motivation for objectification comes from broader Western culture, which views the pursuit of health as a value that all people should pursue. Workers often assume their larger‐bodied colleagues are not pursuing health simply because of their size and can feel punitive toward them as a result. Organizational remedies for this objectification process include educational programming and training to talk about the myriad ways “health” may be realized; more precise information about the complex origin’s of one’s body size; education around how marginalization of larger‐bodied colleagues does not stimulate effective action, but instead harms these colleague’s mental health (and beyond).
Article
Full-text available
It is not known whether psychological stress suppresses host resistance to infection. To investigate this issue, we prospectively studied the relation between psychological stress and the frequency of documented clinical colds among subjects intentionally exposed to respiratory viruses. After completing questionnaires assessing degrees of psychological stress, 394 healthy subjects were given nasal drops containing one of five respiratory viruses (rhinovirus type 2, 9, or 14, respiratory syncytial virus, or coronavirus type 229E), and an additional 26 were given saline nasal drops. The subjects were then quarantined and monitored for the development of evidence of infection and symptoms. Clinical colds were defined as clinical symptoms in the presence of an infection verified by the isolation of virus or by an increase in the virus-specific antibody titer. The rates of both respiratory infection (P less than 0.005) and clinical colds (P less than 0.02) increased in a dose-response manner with increases in the degree of psychological stress. Infection rates ranged from approximately 74 percent to approximately 90 percent, according to levels of psychological stress, and the incidence of clinical colds ranged from approximately 27 percent to 47 percent. These effects were not altered when we controlled for age, sex, education, allergic status, weight, the season, the number of subjects housed together, the infectious status of subjects sharing the same housing, and virus-specific antibody status at base line (before challenge). Moreover, the associations observed were similar for all five challenge viruses. Several potential stress-illness mediators, including smoking, alcohol consumption, exercise, diet, quality of sleep, white-cell counts, and total immunoglobulin levels, did not explain the association between stress and illness. Similarly, controls for personality variables (self-esteem, personal control, and introversion-extraversion) failed to alter our findings. Psychological stress was associated in a dose-response manner with an increased risk of acute infectious respiratory illness, and this risk was attributable to increased rates of infection rather than to an increased frequency of symptoms after infection.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to examine the impact of weight discrimination on perceived attributions, person-job fit, and hiring recommendations. Three experiments were undertaken to investigate these issues with people applying for positions in fitness organizations (i.e., aerobics instructor and personal trainer). In all three studies qualified people who were overweight, relative to their qualified and sometimes unqualified thin counterparts, were perceived to have less desirable attributes (e.g., lazy), were thought to be a poorer fit for the position, and were less likely to receive a hiring recommendation. These relationships were influenced by applicant expertise and applicant sex in some cases. Implications for the fitness industry are discussed.
Article
Attributing negative outcomes to prejudice and discrimination may protect the mood and self-esteem of some stigmatized groups. Thus, the overweight may be low in self-esteem because they blame their weight, but not the attitudes of others, for negative outcomes based on their weight. In an experiment, 27 overweight and 31 normal weight college women received either positive or negative social feedback from a male evaluator. Relative to other groups, overweight women who received negative feedback attributed the feedback to their weight but did not blame the evaluator for his reaction. This attributional pattern resulted in more negative mood for these overweight women in comparison with other groups. Dimensions of stigma that may account for differences in the tendency to attribute negative outcomes to prejudice, and implications of these findings for weight loss programs and psychotherapy for the overweight, are discussed.
Article
Objective: To examine associations between perceived weight discrimination and changes in weight, waist circumference, and weight status. Methods: Data were from 2944 men and women aged ≥50 years participating in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Experiences of weight discrimination were reported in 2010-2011 and weight and waist circumference were objectively measured in 2008-2009 and 2012-2013. ANCOVAs were used to test associations between perceived weight discrimination and changes in weight and waist circumference. Logistic regression was used to test associations with changes in weight status. All analyses adjusted for baseline BMI, age, sex, and wealth. Results: Perceived weight discrimination was associated with relative increases in weight (+1.66 kg, P < 0.001) and waist circumference (+1.12 cm, P = 0.046). There was also a significant association with odds of becoming obese over the follow-up period (OR = 6.67, 95% CI 1.85-24.04) but odds of remaining obese did not differ according to experiences of weight discrimination (OR = 1.09, 95% CI 0.46-2.59). Conclusions: Our results indicate that rather than encouraging people to lose weight, weight discrimination promotes weight gain and the onset of obesity. Implementing effective interventions to combat weight stigma and discrimination at the population level could reduce the burden of obesity.