ArticlePDF Available

Social Media Use and Perceived Emotional Support Among US Young Adults

Authors:

Abstract

Low emotional support is associated with poor health outcomes. Engagement with face-to-face social networks is one way of increasing emotional support. However, it is not yet known whether engagement with proliferating electronic social networks is similarly associated with increased emotional support. Thus, the purpose of this study was to assess associations between social media use and perceived emotional support in a large, nationally-representative sample. In October 2014, we collected data from 1796 U.S. adults ages 19-32. We assessed social media use using both total time spent and frequency of visits to each of the 11 most popular social media platforms. Our dependent variable was perceived emotional support as measured by the brief Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS) emotional support scale. A multivariable model including all sociodemographic covariates and accounting for survey weights demonstrated that, compared with the lowest quartile of time on social media, being in the highest quartile (spending two or more hours per day) was significantly associated with decreased odds of having higher perceived emotional support (AOR 0.62, 95 % CI 0.40, 0.94). However, compared with those in the lowest quartile, being in the highest quartile regarding frequency of social media use was not significantly associated with perceived emotional support (AOR 0.70, 95 % CI 0.45, 1.09). In conclusion, while the cross-sectional nature of these data hinder inference regarding directionality, it seems that heavy users of social media may actually feel less and not more emotional support.
ORIGINAL PAPER
Social Media Use and Perceived Emotional Support Among US
Young Adults
Ariel Shensa
1,2
Jaime E. Sidani
1,2
Liu yi Lin
1
Nicholas D. Bowman
4
Brian A. Primack
1,2,3
ÓSpringer Science+Business Media New York 2015
Abstract Low emotional support is associated with poor
health outcomes. Engagement with face-to-face social
networks is one way of increasing emotional support.
However, it is not yet known whether engagement with
proliferating electronic social networks is similarly asso-
ciated with increased emotional support. Thus, the purpose
of this study was to assess associations between social
media use and perceived emotional support in a large,
nationally-representative sample. In October 2014, we
collected data from 1796 U.S. adults ages 19–32. We
assessed social media use using both total time spent and
frequency of visits to each of the 11 most popular social
media platforms. Our dependent variable was perceived
emotional support as measured by the brief Patient-Re-
ported Outcomes Measurement Information System
(PROMIS) emotional support scale. A multivariable model
including all sociodemographic covariates and accounting
for survey weights demonstrated that, compared with the
lowest quartile of time on social media, being in the highest
quartile (spending two or more hours per day) was sig-
nificantly associated with decreased odds of having higher
perceived emotional support (AOR 0.62, 95 % CI 0.40,
0.94). However, compared with those in the lowest quar-
tile, being in the highest quartile regarding frequency of
social media use was not significantly associated with
perceived emotional support (AOR 0.70, 95 % CI 0.45,
1.09). In conclusion, while the cross-sectional nature of
these data hinder inference regarding directionality, it
seems that heavy users of social media may actually feel
less and not more emotional support.
Keywords Emotional support Social media Social
networks PROMIS (patient reported outcomes
measurement information system) Nationally-
representative data Young adults
Introduction
Low emotional support has been associated with poor
physical and mental health outcomes and increased overall
mortality risk [15]. Increased emotional support is also a
protective factor for vulnerable populations against nega-
tive health outcomes such as pre- and postnatal maternal
depression [6], stress, anxiety, and depression in HIV-
positive men [7], and increased survival rates among
African-American and White breast cancer patients [8].
While demographic characteristics such as age and gender
moderate the relationship between emotional support and
well-being [9,10], emotional support is consistently asso-
ciated with an overall beneficial effect on health [11].
One of the most effective ways of increasing emotional
support is through social network affiliation [12,13]. For
example, a 20-year longitudinal study found that one’s
happiness depends on the happiness of other individuals
with whom they are connected [14]. Similarly, results from
&Ariel Shensa
shensaa@upmc.edu
1
Division of General Internal Medicine, Department of
Medicine, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine,
Pittsburgh, PA, USA
2
Center for Research on Media, Technology, and Health,
University of Pittsburgh, 230 McKee Place Suite 600,
Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
3
Division of Adolescent Medicine, Department of Pediatrics,
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA,
USA
4
Department of Communication Studies, West Virginia
University, Morgantown, WV, USA
123
J Community Health
DOI 10.1007/s10900-015-0128-8
Table 1 Whole sample
characteristics and bivariable
associations with perceived
emotional support (N =1785)
Independent variables Whole
sample
a
Perceived emotional support
a
pvalue
b
Low
(n =1096)
High
(n =689)
Social media use
Time per day, min .06
Q1 (0–30) 29.8 27.8 32.8
Q2 (31–60) 20.8 19.2 23.3
Q3 (61–120) 24.0 24.6 23.0
Q4 (121 and above) 25.5 28.5 20.8
Site visits per week
c
.64
Q1 (less than 9) 28.3 27.1 30.2
Q2 (9–30) 25.1 25.4 24.6
Q3 (31–57) 24.1 23.6 24.8
Q4 (58 and above) 22.5 23.9 20.4
Sociodemographic
Age, years .16
19–23 33.7 31.3 37.4
24–26 24.8 24.8 24.7
27–32 41.6 43.9 37.9
Sex .91
Female 50.3 50.5 50.0
Male 49.7 49.5 50.0
Race .004
White, non-Hispanic 57.5 53.3 63.9
Black, non-Hispanic 13.0 12.7 13.4
Hispanic 20.6 22.2 18.1
Other
d
8.9 11.8 4.6
Relationship status \.001
Single
e
44.5 51.1 34.1
In a committed relationship
f
55.6 48.9 65.9
Living situation .10
Parent/guardian 34.0 34.9 32.7
Significant other 35.6 32.7 40.1
Other
g
30.4 32.5 27.2
Household income .047
Low (under $30,000) 22.9 25.9 18.2
Medium ($30,000–$74,999) 38.4 36.7 41.2
High ($75,000 and above) 38.7 37.5 40.6
Education level .24
High school or less 36.0 37.3 33.8
Some college 38.3 39.0 37.2
Bachelor’s degree or higher 25.7 23.7 29.0
a
Values may not total 100 due to rounding. Column percentages are based upon survey weighted data,
therefore may not be congruent with the cell frequency proportion of total N
b
Pvalue derived using Chi square analyses comparing proportion of users in each category
c
Includes Facebook, Twitter, Google?, YouTube, LinkedIn, Instagram, Pinterest, Tumblr, Vine, Snap-
chat, and Reddit
d
Includes multiracial
e
Includes widowed, divorced, and separated
f
Includes engaged, married, and in a domestic partnership
g
Defined as not living with a parent/guardian or significant other
J Community Health
123
a 32-year longitudinal study indicated that positive health
behaviors appear to spread via social ties in a large social
network [15]. Moreover, a study on religiosity and life
satisfaction offered strong evidence that increased life
satisfaction stems from the social aspects involved in
religious identity such as building congregational social
networks [16].
With the substantial increase of Internet and social
media use in the 21
st
century, opportunities for connect-
edness and support have become more plentiful yet more
complex. From 2005 to 2013, the percentage of online
adults ages 18–29 who use social media has increased from
9 to 90 %, with over 74 % of all online adults currently
reporting social media use [17]. It may be that electronic
social networks mimic face-to-face social networks, which
are known to increase emotional support. Consistent with
this, some findings suggest that larger social networks and
perceived audiences predict higher levels of life satisfac-
tion [18]. Similarly, greater intensity of social network use
has been associated with increased social capital [19,20].
Additionally, in a study assessing Facebook user responses,
positive emotions were found to be more prevalent than
negative emotions, suggesting Facebook use may be
associated with happiness [21]. Furthermore, another
recent study of Facebook found that users report higher
perceived emotional support than other internet users of
similar demographic characteristics [22]. One possibility
for social media’s potential positive impact on emotional
support is the medium’s ability to help foster both strong
and weak social ties [23]. These ties may be important for
emotional strength or securing novel sources of informa-
tion, depending upon the strength of the tie [24]. Others
have argued that social media usage creates an ambient
awareness among users, keeping them loosely aware of
each other’s day-to-day social activities [25], which may
also enhance a user’s perception of emotional support.
However, increased online social media use may not
necessarily translate into increased emotional support. For
example, results from one study suggested that total time
spent on social media and a larger online social network
was neither associated with having a larger offline network
nor with feeling emotionally closer to one’s offline net-
works [26]. Additionally, a literature review of 43 empir-
ical studies found that—while most individuals are initially
motivated to use social media sites to stay connected with
established offline social networks—increased use was
associated with a decrease in real life social community,
lower academic achievement, as well as relationship
problems [27]. Furthermore, findings from another recent
study indicated that—while online social networking was
weakly associated with decreased depression—social sup-
port from Facebook had less of an association with reduced
depression than did face-to-face social network support
[28]. Some have argued that social media use may be
disruptive in that it pulls individuals away from their
physical surroundings, including people who occupy that
space. This may result in displacement or a tension of
attention [29,30].
It is important to note that the above-mentioned often
conflicting studies were limited in several respects. For
example, these studies nearly all used relatively small
convenience samples and/or focused on youth [19,20,31].
Due to the ubiquitous nature of social media use, including
use by young adults [32], it would be valuable to study
associations between social media use and perceived
emotional support in a large national sample more gener-
alizable to the population of U.S. users. Prior studies also
generally assessed social media using a single platform
(e.g., Facebook) [18,21,28,31]. However, multi-platform
use is on the rise, with 52 % of online adults and 71 % of
teens using two or more social media sites [32]. Hence, it
would be beneficial to assess social media use more com-
prehensively. According to Glanz, Rimer, and Viswanath,
both social network and social support are inter-related
theoretical constructs within a larger conceptual model of
health behavior and health outcomes [12]. Therefore, the
purpose of this study was to assess associations between
social media use and perceived emotional support in a
large, nationally-representative sample of young adults.
Based on prior literature [1822] and theory [12], we
hypothesized that increased social media use would be
associated with greater perceived emotional support.
Methods
Participants and Procedures
Participants were recruited from a nationally-representative
probability-based online non-volunteer access panel known
as the KnowledgePanel
Ò
. This panel, which consists of
approximately 55,000 members ages 18 and older, is
recruited and maintained by Growth from Knowledge
(GfK) [33]. It was populated through both address-based
sampling and random digit dialing, allowing for a sampling
frame that covers approximately 97 % of U.S. households
[33]. Panel members are invited to participate in web-based
surveys via personal or GfK-provided e-mail addresses. In
March–April 2013, a total of 3254 panel members ages
18–30 (response rate =54 %) completed a web-based
survey about various health behaviors. In October 2014,
GfK sent a follow-up survey about these and other health
behaviors to the 3254 panel members who had completed
the baseline survey, who were then ages 19–32. Those who
completed the surveys were given a $15 cash-equivalent
incentive. Data from the current study were collected as
J Community Health
123
part of this follow-up wave of the longitudinal study;
baseline data were not used because social media use and
the dependent variable of perceived emotional support
were not collected at that time. This study was approved by
the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board and
was granted a Certificate of Confidentiality from the
National Cancer Institute at the National Institutes of
Health.
Measures
Perceived Emotional Support (Dependent Variable)
We assessed perceived emotional support using a 4-item
scale developed by the Patient-Reported Outcomes Mea-
surement Information System (PROMIS). PROMIS is a
National Institutes of Health (NIH) Roadmap initiative
aiming to provide precise, reliable, valid, and standardized
questionnaires measuring patient–reported outcomes across
the domains of physical, mental, and social health [3436].
The PROMIS emotional support item bank specifically
aims to assess perceived feelings of being cared for and
valued as a person [37]. Participants were presented with
the following items: ‘I have someone who will listen to me
when I need to talk’’; ‘I have someone to confide in or talk
to about myself or my problems’’; ‘I have someone who
makes me feel appreciated’ and ‘‘I have someone to talk
with when I have a bad day.’ Each item was followed by a
Likert-type response scale with possible responses of Never
(1), Rarely (2), Sometimes (3), Often (4), and Always (5).
We calculated a raw summary score ranging from 4 to 20,
using only the respondents who answered all 4 items.
Because of the non-normal distribution of the data, the
outcome was treated as categorical instead of continuous.
While our original analytic plan involved collapsing the
dependent variable into tertiles, this was ultimately not
appropriate because of violation of the proportional odds
assumption [38]. Therefore, the most appropriate way to
operationalize this variable for primary analyses was
dichotomously into low and high categories based upon the
distribution of the data.
Social Media Use (Independent Variables)
Social media use was assessed with multiple items devel-
oped to capture use in terms of both total time spent on
social media and frequency of use. The first item asked
participants to estimate total time per day spent on social
media for personal use. This item explicitly instructed
participants to not include work-related use in their esti-
mates. Participants were provided with open-ended boxes
for hours and minutes, and total time was converted to
minutes for analysis. Eleven subsequent items prompted
participants to indicate how frequently they visit the fol-
lowing 11 social media platforms: Facebook, Twitter,
Google?, YouTube, LinkedIn, Instagram, Pinterest, Tum-
blr, Vine, Snapchat, and Reddit. These platforms were
selected based on their popularity with the young adult age
group at the time of the study [32,39]. Seven response
categories were based on the Pew Research Center items
[32] and included: I don’t use this platform (0), less than
once a week (1), 12 days a week (2), 36 days a week (3),
about once a day (4), 24 times a day (5), and 5 or more
times a day (6). This item was used to calculate partici-
pants’ social media site visits per week (frequency) by
converting the response categories into numeric averages
based on a standardized unit of measurement rather than
general frequency. For example, 12 days a week was
recoded as 1.5 site visits per week and 24 times a day was
recoded as 21 site visits per week. Both independent
variables were collapsed into quartiles for primary analyses
to improve the interpretability of results.
Socio-demographic Factors (Covariates)
GfK maintains certain socio-demographic information
about panel members, including age, sex, race/ethnicity,
household income, and education level. For this study, age
was collapsed into three categories (19–23; 24–26; 27 and
above) based on the distribution of the data. Race/ethnicity
was divided into four categories (White, non-Hispanic;
Black, non-Hispanic; Hispanic; Other, non-Hispanic),
while household income and education level were each
divided into three categories (low, under $30,000; medium,
$30,000–74,999; high, $75,000 and above and high school
or less; some college; bachelor’s degree or higher,
respectively). Relationship status (single; in a committed
relationship) and living situation (with parent/guardian;
with significant other; other) were obtained via self-report
from participants.
Data Analysis
We included all participants who had complete data on the
dependent variable (perceived emotional support). Because
\1 % of participants had missing data for this variable,
removal of incomplete data is unlikely to have affected our
results. We first calculated descriptive statistics of the
dependent variable, each of the 2 independent variables
(time and frequency), and each of the 7 covariates.
We then used Chi square tests to determine bivariable
associations between our dependent variable and each of
our independent variables and covariates. We also used Chi
square tests to assess bivariable associations between each
covariate and each independent variable.
J Community Health
123
We then used logistic regression to determine bivariable
and multivariable associations between each independent
variable and our dependent variable. It was decided a priori
to adjust for all sociodemographic variables in our primary
multivariable models. Additionally, we used post-estima-
tion orthogonal polynomial tests to examine the overall
linear trend of each ordered categorical independent vari-
able in relation to our dependent variable.
All primary analyses were conducted using survey
weights provided by GfK in order to estimate effects for
the general U.S. population. Statistical analyses were per-
formed with Stata 12.1 [40], and two-tailed pvalues of
\.05 were considered to be significant.
To examine the robustness of our results, we conducted
three sets of sensitivity analyses. First, we conducted all
multivariable analyses without survey weights. Second, we
conducted all multivariable analyses using only a parsi-
monious set of covariates that had a bivariable association
of p\.10 with the dependent variable. Third, we con-
ducted all analyses with independent variables as contin-
uous instead of ordered categorical variables.
Results
Participants
Our final sample consisted of 1785 individuals with only
11 (\1 %) omitted for incomplete data on our outcome
variable. The sociodemographic characteristics of our
sample are reported in Table 1.
Perceived Emotional Support
Individual items demonstrated high internal consistency with
Cronbach’s alpha =0.96. Data for perceived emotional
support were non-normal and heavily skewed right. The
median score for perceived emotional support was 16 (in-
terquartile range [IQR] =12–20). As described above in the
methods section, analysis of this variable as continuous was
not viable because of non-normality and no appropriate
transformation. Additionally, collapsing in tertiles was ulti-
mately not appropriate because of violation of the propor-
tional odds assumption [38]. Thus, perceived emotional
support was dichotomized into ‘low’ and ‘high’ groups
based on the distribution of the data and correspondence with
T-scores. In particular, the 39 % of participants with raw
scores of 19 or 20 were defined as ‘‘high’ emotional support
and the remaining 61 % were placed in the ‘low’ category.
These categories corresponded well with T-scores, which
were recommended benchmarks for other PROMIS measures
under conditions of normality. For example, our median score
of 16 corresponded to a T-score of 49, which is only 1 point
below the standardized mean of 50. Our ‘high’ group, scores
of 19 and 20, corresponded to T-scores of 55.6 and 62. These
scores are equivalent to greater than half and greater than one
standard deviations above the mean, respectively.
Social Media Use
Participants reported a median of 61 min (IQR =30–135)
for time per day on social media and a median of 30
(IRQ =9–57) site visits per week. The lowest quartile for
Table 2 Bivariable associations between sociodemographic covari-
ates and time per day on social media for personal use
Covariate Time per day, min
a
pvalue
b
0–30 31–60 61–120 121?
Age, years \.001
19–23 26.7 27.6 37.2 43.3
24–26 27.4 20.3 26.1 23.2
27–32 45.9 52.1 36.8 33.5
Sex
Female 42.7 43.4 53.4 61.0 \.001
Male 57.3 56.6 46.6 39.0
Race/ethnicity
White, non-Hispanic 63.5 63.7 54.0 48.4 .13
Black, non-Hispanic 10.5 10.4 15.0 16.6
Hispanic 16.5 17.3 23.3 25.4
Other
c
9.4 8.6 7.8 9.6
Relationship status .09
Single
d
41.3 38.3 46.8 50.5
Committed
relationship
e
58.7 61.7 53.2 49.5
Living situation .13
Parent/guardian 31.3 29.5 36.9 37.7
Significant other 41.0 40.4 31.2 29.1
Other
f
27.7 30.1 32.0 33.3
Household income .17
Under $30,000 18.2 20.7 24.4 28.0
$30,000–$74,999 41.4 36.2 41.4 34.1
$75,000 and above 40.4 43.2 34.1 37.9
Education level .003
High school or less 31.9 26.3 38.4 45.0
Some college 37.1 41.7 39.1 36.9
B.A. or higher 31.0 32.0 22.5 18.2
a
Values may not total 100 due to rounding. Numerals represent
column percentages
b
Pvalue derived using Chi square analyses comparing proportion of
users in each category
c
Includes multiracial
d
Includes widowed, divorced, and separated
e
Includes engaged, married, and in a domestic partnership
f
Defined as not living with a parent/guardian or significant other
J Community Health
123
time per day included 0–30 min; the second quartile
included 31–60 min; the third quartile included
61–120 min; and the fourth and highest quartile included
121 or more minutes. For site visits per week, the lowest
quartile included less than 9 site visits; the second quartile
included 9–30; the third quartile included 31–57; and the
fourth and highest quartile included 58 or more site visits.
Bivariable Analyses
We found no significant associations between any of the
social media use variables and perceived emotional sup-
port. However, bivariable analyses did show significant
associations between race/ethnicity, relationship status, and
household income and perceived emotional support
(pranging from \.001 to .05) (Table 1). Bivariable anal-
yses also showed significant associations between three
covariates (age, sex, and education level) and time per day
on social media (pranging from \.001 to .003) (Table 2).
Only age showed a significant association with site visits
per week (p\.001) (data not shown).
Multivariable Analyses
In fully-adjusted multivariable models, respondents in the
highest quartile of time per day on social media had sig-
nificantly decreased odds of high perceived emotional
support (AOR 0.62; 95 % CI 0.40–0.94). There was no
significant association between site visits per week and
perceived emotional support (AOR 0.70; 95 % CI
0.45–1.09) (Table 3).
Post-estimate tests using orthogonal polynomials
showed significant linear trends for time per day with
perceived emotional support (p=.02), whereas site visits
per week did not (p=.18) (Table 3). Results from all
sensitivity analyses were consistent with those from pri-
mary analyses.
Discussion
This study of a nationally-representative sample of young
adults found that participants who spend the most time per
day on social media sites had significantly lower odds of
reporting higher levels of perceived emotional support.
Additionally, we found a significant linear trend between
time and perceived emotional support; as time per day
spent on social media increased, perceived emotional
support decreased. A second independent variable, which
quantified the frequency of social media visits per week,
was not associated with perceived emotional support.
While there is conflicting literature on the association
between social media use and feeling emotionally sup-
ported, our results are consistent with research that sug-
gests that social media use may undermine subjective well-
being [41] and be associated with factors counteractive to
perceived emotional support such as loneliness [26,42,43]
and depression [44]. However, they do not support the
findings that state the psychological benefit of a social
media presence presented in the literature review. While
our study did not probe the psychological mechanisms
underlying observed effects, some have argued that social
Table 3 Bivariable and
multivariable associations
between social media use and
perceived emotional support
Social media use Perceived emotional support
a
OR (95 % CI) P
b
AOR
c
(95 % CI) P
b
Time per day, min .009 .02
Q1 (0–30) 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference]
Q2 (31–60) 1.03 (0.69–1.54) 0.96 (0.65–1.42)
Q3 (61–120) 0.79 (0.53–1.19) 0.78 (0.50–1.21)
Q4 (121 ?) 0.62 (0.41–0.92) 0.62 (0.40–0.94)
Site visits per week
d
.30 .18
Q1 (less than 9) 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference]
Q2 (9–30) 0.87 (0.58–1.30) 0.77 (0.52–1.16)
Q3 (31–57) 0.94 (0.63–1.42) 0.86 (0.56–1.33)
Q4 (58 and above) 0.77 (0.50–1.17) 0.70 (0.45–1.09)
OR odds ratio, AOR adjusted odds ratio, CI confidence interval
a
Perceived emotional support is divided into 2 categories; the upper level representing greater support
b
Pvalue derived using orthogonal polynomial tests for trend
c
Adjusted for age, sex, race, relationship status, living situation, household income, and education level
d
Includes Facebook, Twitter, Google?, YouTube, LinkedIn, Instagram, Pinterest, Tumblr, Vine, Snap-
chat, and Reddit
J Community Health
123
media platforms—despite their ability to help foster both
strong (emotionally supportive) and weak (information-
seeking) social ties [24]—can greatly disrupt the user’s
normal sense of place including the people in it [29,30].
Moreover, social media platforms require users to con-
stantly monitor and co-produce information with their
peers, which might place additional strain on their cogni-
tive, emotional, and social resources [45].
It is interesting to note that, while we found significant
associations between increased time on social media and
lower perceived emotional support, we did not find sig-
nificant associations between increased frequency of use
and lower emotional support. Because the point estimate
was substantially below 1.0 (it was 0.7), it is possible that
we simply did not have enough power for this result to be
significant. However, it is also possible that total time spent
is indeed more closely tied to lower perceived emotional
support for other reasons. For example, this might support
the ‘displacement hypothesis’’ whereby increased overall
time on social media simply makes less time available for
more beneficial face-to-face relationships. Similarly,
because it is generally an acceptable social convention
among young adults to check social media frequently—
even when also engaging in an in-person social activity
[30,46]—this frequency may not interfere with benefits
afforded by other social relationships. Conversely, it could
also be the case that frequent social media checks are more
functional than they are distracting. For example, one
might make frequent checks of their Twitter or Facebook
accounts throughout the day, but these checks could also be
both short and performed in spaces otherwise free from
social interaction. That is, one might share and view social
media content that are valuable without being demanding.
It would be beneficial for future studies to continue to
examine potentially differential effects of time and fre-
quency of use.
Because our data were cross-sectional, we could not
infer temporality. One explanation of our overall findings
may be that individuals who feel less emotionally sup-
ported in offline relationships subsequently spend more
time on social media to fill this void. Alternately, it could
be that individuals who first spend substantial time on
social media subsequently feel less emotional support.
This could be an effect of what is happening online or
increased exposure to negative social support. For
example, envy, fear of missing out, and contentious
interactions with others may occur with increased time
spent on social media and adversely affect one’s per-
ceived emotional support [4750]. As noted above,
increasing time spent online may also result in being less
engaged in truly therapeutic face-to-face relationships
and/or interactions. Regardless of directionality, however,
it is an important and somewhat paradoxical finding that
those with increased social media use tend to perceive
less emotional support. Even if this is because individuals
with low perceived emotional support subsequently spend
increased time online, it is interesting that they do not
seem to find the emotional support they lack in that
medium. As is often the case, it may be that both direc-
tions are applicable—individuals with low perceived
support spend more time online, which subsequently
limits their ability to engage in more potentially valuable
in-person activities that might better serve their emotional
support needs. Qualitative work may be valuable in
helping describe these types of complex processes, as
well as longitudinal research such as cohort and panel
designs.
It is important to note that we only assessed overall time
and frequency, and that there are many qualitatively dif-
ferent ways of interacting on social media. For example,
some individuals have more ‘passive’ experiences by
simply viewing posts; this is also known as browsing or
lurking. However, other individuals have substantially
more ‘active’ experiences characterized by writing private
messages, commenting, posting, and/or actively searching
for old friends or acquaintances. It follows that users who
actively engage content—co-creating their and others’
stories through frequent and ritualized interactions—might
see these spaces as vehicles for expression and social
support [51]. Because the degree of passivity/activity may
indeed be associated with ultimately feeling emotional
support, it would be valuable for future work to assess the
character of online interactions in a more fine-grained
manner.
Similarly, there are a variety of different degrees of
emotionality that can be experienced online. Some indi-
viduals may have only relatively benign interactions, while
as noted above, others may tend to have aggressive inter-
actions. Still others may be prone to ‘overshare,’ given the
disinhibited nature of mediated social interaction. This
phenomenon of over-sharing, especially among users with
low self-esteem, ultimately can lead to negative responses
from friends [52] and decreased relationship satisfaction
[53]. One theoretical mechanism underlying these effects is
that of communication privacy management, wherein
social media users often and unintentionally violate their
own boundary rules around content and information con-
sidered to be highly personal, but once divulged online
becomes essentially public domain [54]. Because the
specific ways in which individuals use social media was
not measured in this study, these could be valuable areas
for future research.
It was an important contribution of our study that we
assessed social media broadly—using the 11 most popular
platforms at the time—to measure frequency of use
instead of using a single platform such as Facebook. This
J Community Health
123
will be even more important in the future, as multi-plat-
form use among online adults increases about 10 % per
year (Pew Research Center, 2015). While we did not find
a significant association between overall frequency and
perceived emotional support, there are other aspects of
multi-platform use that may be interesting to assess in the
future. For example, media multi-tasking, especially
among adolescents, has been associated with negative
cognitive and/or emotional outcomes [5558]. It may
therefore be interesting to determine if the diffusion of
social media use across multiple platforms is an inde-
pendent risk factor for negative outcomes such as lower
perceived emotional support.
Limitations
As noted above, the cross-sectional nature of the study
limits inference of directionality. Future longitudinal work
may improve assessment of temporality. Additionally,
although our study used a nationally-representative sample
of young adults, these findings cannot be generalized to a
younger or older population. It would be beneficial for
future research to assess other populations. This includes
older adult social media users, given this group’s rapidly
growing social media presence [32].
Another limitation of our study is that data were self-
reported. However, we assured all participants that their
responses were confidential and protected by a Certifica-
tion of Confidentiality, making it unlikely responses would
not be truthful.
Conclusion
This study of a nationally-representative sample of
young adults found that individuals who spent more than
two hours per day on any combination of the 11 most
popular social media platforms had significantly
decreased odds of reporting higher levels of perceived
emotional support, even when controlling for a com-
prehensive set of related sociodemographic characteris-
tics such as sex, relationship status, and living situation.
Individuals with low perceived emotional support may
subsequently spend more time on social media, social
media users may paradoxically begin to feel lower
emotional support, or both may be true. It would be
valuable for future research to examine these associa-
tions longitudinally and to more carefully assess differ-
ent types of social media interactions.
Acknowledgments Dr. Primack is supported in part by a grant from
the National Cancer Institute (R01-CA140150).
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict
of interest.
References
1. Reblin, M., & Uchino, B. N. (2008). Social and emotional sup-
port and its implication for health. Current Opinion in Psychiatry,
21(2), 201–205.
2. Strine, T. W., Chapman, D. P., Balluz, L., & Mokdad, A. (2008).
Health-related quality of life and health behaviors by social and
emotional support. Their relevance to psychiatry and medicine.
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43(2), 151–159.
3. Sorkin, D., Rook, K. S., & Lu, J. L. (2002). Loneliness, lack of
emotional support, lack of companionship, and the likelihood of
having a heart condition in an elderly sample. Annals of Behav-
ioral Medicine, 24(4), 290–298.
4. Berkman, L. F., Leo-Summers, L., & Horwitz, R. I. (1992).
Emotional support and survival after myocardial infarction. A
prospective, population-based study of the elderly. Annals of
Internal Medicine, 117(12), 1003–1009.
5. Krumholz, H. M., Butler, J., Miller, J., et al. (1998). Prognostic
importance of emotional support for elderly patients hospitalized
with heart failure. Circulation, 97(10), 958–964.
6. Pilkington, P. D., Milne, L. C., Cairns, K. E., Lewis, J., &
Whelan, T. A. (2015). Modifiable partner factors associated with
perinatal depression and anxiety: A systematic review and meta-
analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 178, 165–180.
7. Gordillo, V., Fekete, E., Platteau, T., Antoni, M. H., Schneider-
man, N., & No
¨stlinger, C. (2009). Emotional support and gender
in people living with HIV: Effects on psychological well-being.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32(6), 523–531.
8. Soler-Vila, H., Kasl, S. V., & Jones, B. A. (2003). Prognostic
significance of psychosocial factors in African-American and
white breast cancer patients: A population-based study. Cancer,
98(6), 1299–1308.
9. Segrin, C. (2003). Age moderates the relationship between social
support and psychosocial problems. Human Communication
Research, 29(3), 317–342.
10. Knoll, N., & Schwarzer, R. (2002). Gender and age differences in
social support: A study of East German migrants. Heart disease:
Environment, stress and gender (pp. 198–210). Berlin: IOS Press.
11. Siedlecki, K. L., Salthouse, T. A., Oishi, S., & Jeswani, S. (2014).
The relationship between social support and subjective well-be-
ing across age. Social Indicators Research, 117(2), 561–576.
12. Glanz, K., Rimer, B. K., & Viswanath, K. (2008). Health
behavior and health education: Theory, research and practice
(4th ed.). San Francisco, CA: Wiley & Sons.
13. Santini, Z. I., Koyanagi, A., Tyrovolas, S., Mason, C., & Haro, J.
M. (2015). The association between social relationships and
depression: A systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders,
175, 53–65.
14. Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2008). Dynamic spread of
happiness in a large social network: Longitudinal analysis over
20 years in the Framingham Heart Study. BMJ, 337(a2338), 1–9.
15. Rosenquist, J. N., Murabito, J., Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A.
(2010). The spread of alcohol consumption behavior in a large
social network. Annals of Internal Medicine, 152(7), 426–433.
16. Lim, C., & Putnam, R. D. (2010). Religion, social networks, and
life satisfaction. American Sociological Review, 75(6), 914–933.
17. Pew Research Center. (2014). Social networking factsheet. Pew
Research Center. Retrieved November 6, 2015, from http://www.
webcitation.org/6bXTEQ2Xk.
J Community Health
123
18. Manago, A. M., Taylor, T., & Greenfield, P. M. (2012). Me and
my 400 friends: The anatomy of college students’ Facebook
networks, their communication patterns, and well-being. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 48(2), 369–380.
19. Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of
Facebook ‘friends:’’ Social capital and college students’ use of
online social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication, 12(4), 1143–1168.
20. Steinfield, C., Ellison, N. B., & Lampe, C. (2008). Social capital, self-
esteem, and use of online social network sites: A longitudinal analysis.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29(6), 434–445.
21. Lin, R., & Utz, S. (2015). The emotional responses of browsing
Facebook: Happiness, envy, and the role of tie strength. Com-
puters in Human Behavior, 52, 29–38.
22. Hampton, K., Goulet, L.S., Rainie, L., & Purcell, K. (2011).
Social networking sites and our lives. Retrieved November 6,
2015, from http://www.webcitation.org/6cqHMG4DH.
23. Keitzmann, J. H., Hermkens, K., McCarthy, I. P., & Silvestre, B. S.
(2011). Social media? Get serious! Understanding the functional
building blocks of social media. Business Horizons, 54, 241–251.
24. Granovetter, M. S. (1973). The strength of weak ties. American
Journal of Sociology, 78(6), 1360–1380.
25. Thompson, C. (2006). Brave new world of digital intimacy. The
New York Times. Retrieved November 6, 2015, from http://www.
webcitation.org/6cUlQtC7z.
26. Pollet, T. V., Roberts, S. G. B., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2011). Use of
social network sites and instant messaging does not lead to
increased offline social network size, or to emotionally closer
relationships with offline network members. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(4), 253–258.
27. Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2011). Online social networking
and addiction—A review of the psychological literature. Inter-
national Journal of Environmental Research & Public Health,
8(9), 3528–3552.
28. Wright, K. B., Rosenberg, J., Egbert, N., Ploeger, N. A., Bernard,
D. R., & King, S. (2013). Communication competence, social
support, and depression among college students: A model of
Facebook and face-to-face support network influence. Journal of
Health Communication, 18(1), 41–57.
29. Meyrowitz, J. (1985). No sense of place: The impact of electronic
media on social behavior. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
30. Turkle, S. (2012). Alone together: Why we expect more from tech-
nology and less from each other.NewYork,NY:BasicBooks.
31. Chan, T. H. (2014). Facebook and its effects on users’ empathic social
skills and life satisfaction: A double-edged sword effect. Cyberpsy-
chology, Behavior and Social Networking, 17(5), 276–280.
32. Pew Research Center. (2015). Social media update 2014.
Washington DC. Retrieved November 6, 2015, from http://www.
webcitation.org/6ajEhvS11.
33. GfK KnowledgePanel
Ò
. (2013). KnowledgePanel Design Sum-
mary. Retrieved November 6, 2015, from http://www.webcita
tion.org/6ajEWO5mb.
34. Cella, D., Riley, W., Stone, A., et al. (2010). The Patient-Re-
ported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS)
developed and tested its first wave of adult self-reported health
outcome item banks: 2005–2008. Journal of Clinical Epidemi-
ology, 63(11), 1179–1194.
35. Cella, D., Gershon, R., Lai, J., & Choi, S. (2007). The future of
outcomes measurement: item banking, tailored short-forms, and
computerized adaptive assessment. Quality of Life Research,
16(1), 133–141.
36. Hahn, E. A., DeWalt, D. A., Bode, R. K., et al. (2014). New
English and Spanish social health measures will facilitate eval-
uating health determinants. Health Psychology, 33(5), 490–499.
37. PROMIS. (2012). Domain framework—social health. Retrieved
November 6, 2015, from http://www.webcitation.org/6bdksQYLz.
38. Long, J. S., & Freese, J. (2006). Regression models for categorical
dependent variables using stata. College Station, TX: Stata Press.
39. Nielsen. (2012). State of the media: The social media report 2012.
Retrieved November 6, 2015, from http://www.webcitation.org/
6bXTvRwTJ.
40. StataCorp. (2011). Stata statistical software: Version 12. College
Station, TX: StataCorp LP.
41. Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., et al. (2013). Facebook use
predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PLoS
ONE, 8(8), e69841.
42. Morahan-Martin, J., & Schumacher, P. (2003). Loneliness and social
uses of the Internet. Computers in Human Behavior, 19(6), 659–671.
43. Burke, M., Marlow, C. & Lento, T. (2010). Social network
activity and social well-being. In CHI 2010, April 1015, 2010,
Atlanta, Georgia, USA. (pp. 1909–1912).
44. Lin, L., Sidani, J. E., Shensa, A., & Primack, B. A. (in press).
Associations between social media use and depression among
U.S. young adults. Depression and Anxiety.
45. Bowman, N. D. (2016). Video gaming as co-production. The
intesection of audiences and production in a digital world (pp.
107–123). New York, NY: Peter Lang.
46. Carrier, L. M., Cheever, N. A., Rosen, L. D., Benitez, S., &
Chang, J. (2009). Multitasking across generations: Multitasking
choices and difficulty ratings in three generations of Americans.
Computers in Human Behavior, 25(2), 483–489.
47. Tandoc, E. C., Ferrucci, P., & Duffy, M. (2015). Facebook use,
envy, and depression among college students: Is facebooking
depressing? Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 139–146.
48. McCloskey, W., Iwanicki, S., Lauterbach, D., Giammittorio, D.
M., & Maxwell, K. (2015). Are Facebook ‘‘friends’ helpful?
Development of a Facebook-based measure of social support and
examination of relationships among depression, quality of life,
and social support. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Net-
working, 18(9), 499–505.
49. Przybylski,A.K.,Murayama,K.,DeHaan,C.R.,&Gladwell,V.
(2013). Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of
missing out. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1841–1848.
50. Best, J.C. (2014). Don’t fight flames with flames. Social media
arguments: Can’t win propositions. New York Times. Retrieved
November 6, 2015, from http://www.webcitation.org/6cqMsx3Ix.
51. Boyd, D. (2014). It’s complicated: The social lives of networked
teens. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
52. Forest, A. L., & Wood, J. V. (2012). When social networking is
not working: Individuals with low self-esteem recognize but do
not reap the benefits of self-disclosure on Facebook. Psycholog-
ical Science, 23(3), 295–302.
53. Rains, S. A., Brunner, S. R. & Oman, K. (2014). Self-disclosure and
new communication technologies: The implications of receiving
superficial self-disclosures from friends. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships.doi:10.1177/0265407514562561.
54. Petronio, S. (2002). Boundaries of privacy: Dialects of discourse.
Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
55. Junco, R., & Cotten, S. R. (2012). No A 4 U: The relationship
between multitasking and academic performance. Computers &
Education, 59(2), 505–514.
56. Becker, M. W., Alzahabi, R., & Hopwood, C. J. (2013). Media multi-
tasking is associated with symptoms of depression and social anxiety.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16(2), 132–135.
57. Rosen, L. D., Mark Carrier, L., & Cheever, N. A. (2013). Face-
book and texting made me do it: Media-induced task-switching
while studying. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(3), 948–958.
58. Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A.D. (2009). Cognitive control in
media multitaskers. In Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences (vol. 106, pp. 15583–15587).
J Community Health
123
... However, previous research on social support found that in-person support is superior to CMSS in terms of mental health outcomes. [26][27][28][29][30] CMSS has been described as somewhat inferior and weaker than real-life social support due to the limiting of social and emotional cues, as well as the inability to provide materialistic support. 21,31,32 However, this presumes that the population under study has many in-person ties. ...
... Previous research has demonstrated that only in-person support and not CMSS is protective of mental health. [26][27][28][29][30] Crucially, the results of the present study provide an alternative narrative to those found with samples that are composed of mostly domestic students. International students are a group for whom in-person support might be lower quality compared to their mediated support, due to the fact that their strongest social ties are located physically far away in their home countries. ...
... However, it is known from previous research that domestic students tend to benefit more from in-person social support and not from CMSS. [26][27][28][29][30] Future studies could consider domestic or other student groups to compare with international students. ...
Article
Objectives: The current study explored whether social support received from either mediated or in-person contact varied among international students depending on the level of social ties (stronger/weaker) with existing support groups. This study also examined whether in-person or computer mediated social support (CMSS) is a better predictor of acculturative stress which might influence mental health of international students. Methods: An online survey was conducted among 179 international students across three U.S. universities. Results: Results showed that international students perceive stronger online social ties compared to in-person social ties, t(125)=- 3.57, p < .001. Stronger ties with CMSS significantly negatively influence acculturative stress compared to in-person social support for international students, (b = -0.24, 95% CI [-0.45, -0.03]). This indicates that as CMSS increases, acculturative stress decreases among international students. Conclusions: This study concludes that online social support groups can positively influence mental health of college student populations, particularly those who feel stressed out.
... To compute the total negative affective state score for each participant, the scores for all items are summed. The final score on the DASS-21 questionnaire is then obtained by multiplying this total score by 2. Depression scores are categorized as follows: normal (0-9), mild (10-13), moderate (14)(15)(16)(17)(18)(19)(20), severe (21)(22)(23)(24)(25)(26)(27), and extremely severe (28 and above). Anxiety scores are classified as normal (0-7), mild (8-9), moderate (10)(11)(12)(13)(14), severe (15)(16)(17)(18)(19), and extremely severe (20 and above). ...
... Anxiety scores are classified as normal (0-7), mild (8-9), moderate (10)(11)(12)(13)(14), severe (15)(16)(17)(18)(19), and extremely severe (20 and above). Stress scores fall into the following categories: normal (0-14), mild (15)(16)(17)(18), moderate (19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25), severe (26)(27)(28)(29)(30)(31)(32)(33), and extremely severe (34 and above). ...
Article
Full-text available
Excessive smartphone use and dependence on social media give rise to multiple issues that negatively affect the overall well-being of individuals. Nomophobia is characterized as a “digital disease” due to the unlimited use of smartphone devices. The aim of this study is to examine smartphone use and social media involvement in association with nomophobia and psychological traits (i.e., depression, anxiety, stress, and self-esteem) of young adults. A cross-sectional investigation was conducted on 1408 Greek undergraduate students aged 18 to 25 years, using an anonymous self-reported questionnaire. Study data were collected through the Nomophobia Questionnaire (NMP-Q), Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21), and Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES). Participants’ social media engagement was summarized through four variables: use of social media applications, number of friends, number of followers, and number of messages exchanged daily on social media. High levels of nomophobia were associated with high engagement in social media and, thereby, with a high number of friends. The same applies to participants with high/normal self-esteem compared to those with low. Regarding stress, anxiety and depression were not associated with digital network use, while elevated stress and anxiety seemed to play a negative role in the number of online followers. In addition, high levels of anxiety were correlated to an increased number of messages exchanged through social media applications.
... On the one hand, it facilitates social connectivity, enabling students to maintain relationships with friends and family, even across long distances, and to expand their networks by connecting with like-minded individuals. It also provides emotional support, as students can share their feelings and experiences and often receive encouragement from their peers (Shensa et al., 2016). In addition, social media grants students access to an abundance of information and resources, helping them stay updated on current events, educational opportunities, and tools that foster learning and personal growth (Westerman et al., 2014). ...
Article
This study investigates the effects of sports participation and social media use on high school students' mental health and self-perception, with a focus on understanding their unique contributions to happiness and academic confidence. Structural equation modeling was applied to analyze the relationships between sports participation, time spent on social media, and self-reported levels of happiness and confidence, while accounting for potential gender differences. The results indicate that sports participation is positively associated with happiness, but does not significantly affect academic confidence. In contrast, the use of social media is negatively associated with academic confidence, although it does not significantly impact happiness. Gender differences were observed, with female students reporting a lower level of happiness but a higher level of academic confidence. These findings suggest that while extracurricular activities, such as sports varsity involvement, can support students' well-being, the excessive use of social media apps may undermine their academic confidence.
... Older adults rely more on social media to maintain supportive connections with strong ties (families and friends), which serves as an essential emotional resource in isolated situations (Rosen et al., 2022). Thus, the relevance of social media engagement to immigrants' perceived social support is subsequently validated by several empirical investigations (Lu and Hampton, 2017;Shensa et al., 2016;Taiminen and Taiminen, 2016;Sitar-Taut et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Surging numbers of older parents have migrated with adult children to developed urban areas under China’s internal family migration, experiencing several difficulties in social integration in local society. Social media has become an essential tool for older adults to gather information, socialize, and entertain. However, few studies have focused on the relationship between social media use and social integration among older migrants and the underlying psychological mechanisms. Drawing on the Media system dependency (MSD) theory and the Stress-buffering hypothesis, this study developed and tested a conceptual research model by conducting a cross-sectional online survey study (N = 1001). The results indicate that understanding and orientation, rather than entertainment, are two primary motivations for trailing parents to use social media. The activeness of social media engagement (exposure condition), rather than the intensity of social media use (selective exposure), can positively influence the social integration of older migrants. Psychological resilience cannot mediate the relationship between social media engagement and social integration, which is proved to be mainly achieved through perceived social support and the partial chain mediation of perceived social support and psychological resilience. These findings shed theoretical and practical light on the design of older migrant-friendly social media apps and the efforts of adult children and communities to enhance the social integration of older internal migrants through social media engagement.
... [16,[19][20][21] Similarly, the association between social media addiction and mental health problems observed in this study aligns with the findings of numerous previous studies. [22][23][24][25] The study's strengths include the use of validated instruments for assessing cyberbullying, social media addiction, and mental health issues, as well as the adjustment for potential confounding factors in the regression analyses. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Cyberbullying and excessive social media use are emerging issues among medical students, with potential implications for mental health. This study aimed to investigate the prevalence of cyberbullying, social media addiction, and their associated mental health conditions, as well as to explore the associated factors among medical students. Methods: A cross‑sectional study was conducted among 418 medical students in Gujarat using a self‑administered questionnaire. Cyberbullying was assessed using the Revised Cyberbullying Inventory (RCI‑R), social media addiction was measured using the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS), and mental health issues were evaluated using the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS‑21). Descriptive statistics and binary logistic regression analyses were performed. A P value of <0.05 was considered significant. Results: The prevalence of cyberbullying, social media addiction, depression, anxiety, and stress among participants was 27.5% (95% CI: 23.4%–31.9%), 32.1% (95% CI: 27.8%–36.7%), 37.6% (95% CI: 33.1%–42.2%), 41.9% (95% CI: 37.3%–46.6%), and 46.2% (95% CI: 41.6%–50.9%), respectively. Factors associated with increased risk of being a cyber victim included older age, female gender, later years of study, increased daily mobile and social media usage, social media as the preferred mobile usage, and social media addiction. Factors associated with being a cyberbully were similar, except for the male gender. Both cyber victimization and social media addiction were significantly associated with higher odds of depression [aOR‑2.5 (1.6–3.9) and 2.1 (1.4–3.2)], anxiety [aOR–2.2 (1.4‑3.4) and 1.9 (1.3–2.8)] and stress [aOR–2.8 (1.8‑4.3) and 2.4 (1.6–3.6)]. Conclusions: Cyberbullying, social media addiction, and mental health issues are prevalent among medical students. Targeted interventions addressing excessive social media use, promoting responsible online behaviour, and supporting mental well‑being are crucial for this population. Further research is needed to establish causal relationships and develop effective prevention and support strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Background: In the digital age, safeguarding children’s mental health (CMH) has emerged as one of the most pressing challenges. The rapid evolution of social media (SM) from a basic networking platform to a multifaceted tool has introduced numerous conveniences. However, it has also posed significant challenges to children’s mental well-being. Methods: Given the intricate relationship between the widespread use of SM and mental health issues in children, this study conducted a systematic scoping review to examine the literature on the impact of SM on CMH from 2014 to 2024. Literature searches were performed across five databases (Web of Science, ScienceDirect, Scopus, PubMed, and APA PsycInfo), and the retrieved studies were screened, extracted, and analyzed by the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis Extension for Scoping Review (PRISMA-ScR) guidelines. Results: The review identified a complex relationship between SM use and CMH. Different SM platforms exhibited varying effects on children. Frequent SM use was strongly associated with lower self-esteem, depressive symptoms, anxiety, and other mental health challenges in children. Conversely, moderate use of SM facilitated social interactions and emotional expression, which may have a positive influence on mental health. Additionally, factors such as social support from family and school were found to play a critical role in mitigating the negative effects of SM on CMH. Conclusions: To enhance CMH, it is essential to guide children in the appropriate use of SM, promote awareness of privacy protection, and ensure adequate family and social support. Future research should further investigate the specific mechanisms underlying SM use and its differential effects on children across varying age groups and regions.
Chapter
Full-text available
As an entertainment medium, video games are unique in that they are one of the only technologies that are designed to demand near-constant user input and feedback in order to be realized as complete experiences. This interactivity (user control over the form and content of on-screen content) has the potential to allow for unprecedented levels of involvement in a given digital experience, but it also results in great variability in the gaming experiences both between and within gamers (and even discrete gaming sessions). Yet, as games continue to evolve from more basic challenge and skill puzzles to comparatively intricate narrative and social digital spaces, the level of control afforded to the individual player in shaping the final product places great demand on the attentional, emotional and behavioral resources on the individual player. In some respects, this heightened user demand results in players at times serving more as co-producers than players: involving themselves in elements of the digital experience from character design to level creation to story-writing, among others. At its best, this co-production allows for the creation of remarkably enjoyable and meaningful experiences customized to suit each gamer; at its worst, such co-production places an enormous burden on players wanting to engage in gaming as a diversionary or relaxing activity. Moreover, given that people do not have an unlimited ability to process any given set of stimuli, one might challenge the extent to which players carefully attend to and process all on-screen content in a similar fashion, which might have implications for how players enact and are affected by the at-times highly demanding task of video game play. The following chapter draws from theory and empirical results applying decision-making, selective exposure and retention, learning and mental modeling, habit and intuitive response research as well as larger perspectives on entertainment theory to explain the co-productive elements of video game play.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Social media (SM) use is increasing among U.S. young adults, and its association with mental well-being remains unclear. This study assessed the association between SM use and depression in a nationally representative sample of young adults. Methods: We surveyed 1,787 adults ages 19 to 32 about SM use and depression. Participants were recruited via random digit dialing and address-based sampling. SM use was assessed by self-reported total time per day spent on SM, visits per week, and a global frequency score based on the Pew Internet Research Questionnaire. Depression was assessed using the Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS) Depression Scale Short Form. Chi-squared tests and ordered logistic regressions were performed with sample weights. Results: The weighted sample was 50.3% female and 57.5% White. Compared to those in the lowest quartile of total time per day spent on SM, participants in the highest quartile had significantly increased odds of depression (AOR = 1.66, 95% CI = 1.14-2.42) after controlling for all covariates. Compared with those in the lowest quartile, individuals in the highest quartile of SM site visits per week and those with a higher global frequency score had significantly increased odds of depression (AOR = 2.74, 95% CI = 1.86-4.04; AOR = 3.05, 95% CI = 2.03-4.59, respectively). All associations between independent variables and depression had strong, linear, dose-response trends. Results were robust to all sensitivity analyses. Conclusions: SM use was significantly associated with increased depression. Given the proliferation of SM, identifying the mechanisms and direction of this association is critical for informing interventions that address SM use and depression.
Article
Full-text available
Greater social support is predictive of lower depression and higher quality of life (QOL). However, the way in which social support is provided has changed greatly with the expanding role of social networking sites (e.g., Facebook). While there are numerous anecdotal accounts of the benefits of Facebook-based social support, little empirical evidence exists to support these assertions, and there are no empirically validated measures designed to assess social support provided via this unique social networking medium. This study sought to develop an empirically sound measure of Facebook-based social support (Facebook Measure of Social Support [FMSS]) and to assess how this new measure relates to previously established measures of support and two outcome variables: depression and QOL. Following exploratory factor analysis, the FMSS was determined to assess four factors of social support on Facebook (Perceived, Emotional, Negative, Received/Instrumental). The Negative Support factor on the FMSS was most strongly related to both depression and QOL with magnitudes (and direction of relationships) comparable to a traditional measure of perceived social support. However, two FMSS factors (Received/Instrumental and Perceived) were unrelated to both mental health outcomes. Contrary to expectations, elevations in one FMSS factor (Emotional) was associated with worse symptoms of depression and poorer psychological QOL. When taken together, only the absence of negative social support on Facebook is significantly predictive of mental health functioning. Consequently, those hoping to use Facebook as a medium for reducing depression or improving QOL are unlikely to realize significant therapeutic benefits.
Book
Full-text available
What is new about how teenagers communicate through services such as Facebook, Twitter, and Instagram? Do social media affect the quality of teens' lives? In this eye-opening book, youth culture and technology expert danah boyd uncovers some of the major myths regarding teens' use of social media. She explores tropes about identity, privacy, safety, danger, and bullying. Ultimately, boyd argues that society fails young people when paternalism and protectionism hinder teenagers' ability to become informed, thoughtful, and engaged citizens through their online interactions. Yet despite an environment of rampant fear-mongering, boyd finds that teens often find ways to engage and to develop a sense of identity. Boyd's conclusions are essential reading not only for parents, teachers, and others who work with teens but also for anyone interested in the impact of emerging technologies on society, culture, and commerce in years to come. Offering insights gleaned from more than a decade of original fieldwork interviewing teenagers across the United States, boyd concludes reassuringly that the kids are all right. At the same time, she acknowledges that coming to terms with life in a networked era is not easy or obvious. In a technologically mediated world, life is bound to be complicated.
Article
Full-text available
On Facebook, users are exposed to posts from both strong and weak ties. Even though several studies have examined the emotional consequences of using Facebook, less attention has been paid to the role of tie strength. This paper aims to explore the emotional outcomes of reading a post on Facebook and examine the role of tie strength in predicting happiness and envy. Two studies – one correlational, based on a sample of 207 American participants and the other experimental, based on a sample of 194 German participants – were conducted in 2014. In Study 2, envy was further distinguished into benign and malicious envy. Based on a multi-method approach, the results showed that positive emotions are more prevalent than negative emotions while browsing Facebook. Moreover, tie strength is positively associated with the feeling of happiness and benign envy, whereas malicious envy is independent of tie strength after reading a (positive) post on Facebook.
Article
The reported study examined the implications of receiving superficial self-disclosures from a friend. A total of 199 adults reported on communication episodes initiated by a friend during the previous 7 days via five communication technologies and completed measures of liking, relationship satisfaction, and willingness to provide social support to their friend. The results revealed significant interactions between the total volume of self-disclosures received and proportion of superficial disclosures for liking and relationship satisfaction. Among respondents who received a relatively greater volume of self-disclosures, the proportion of superficial disclosures received was inversely associated with relationship satisfaction and liking. Perceived costs mediated the preceding relationships.
Article
Article
Background: Alcohol consumption has important health-related consequences and numerous biological and social determinants. Objective: To explore quantitatively whether alcohol consumption behavior spreads from person to person in a large social network of friends, coworkers, siblings, spouses, and neighbors, followed for 32 years. Design: Longitudinal network cohort study. Setting: The Framingham Heart Study. Participants: 12 067 persons assessed at several time points between 1971 and 2003. Measurements: Self-reported alcohol consumption (number of drinks per week on average over the past year and number of days drinking within the past week) and social network ties, measured at each time point. Results: Clusters of drinkers and abstainers were present in the network at all time points, and the clusters extended to 3 degrees of separation. These clusters were not only due to selective formation of social ties among drinkers but also seem to reflect interpersonal influence. Changes in the alcohol consumption behavior of a person's social network had a statistically significant effect on that person's subsequent alcohol consumption behavior. The behaviors of immediate neighbors and coworkers were not significantly associated with a person's drinking behavior, but the behavior of relatives and friends was. Limitations: A nonclinical measure of alcohol consumption was used. Also, it is unclear whether the effects on long-term health are positive or negative, because alcohol has been shown to be both harmful and protective. Finally, not all network ties were observed. Conclusion: Network phenomena seem to influence alcohol consumption behavior. This has implications for clinical and public health interventions and further supports group-level interventions to reduce problematic drinking.
Article
Perinatal distress is a significant public health problem that adversely impacts the individual and their family. The primary objective of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to identify factors that partners can modify to protect each other from developing perinatal depression and anxiety. In accordance with the PRISMA statement, we reviewed the risk and protective factors associated with perinatal depression and anxiety symptoms that partners can potentially modify without professional assistance (PROSPERO reference CRD42014007524). Participants were new or expectant parents aged 16 years or older. The partner factors were sub-grouped into themes (e.g., instrumental support) based on a content analysis of the scale items and measure descriptions. A series of meta-analyses were conducted to estimate the pooled effect sizes of associations. We included 120 publications, reporting 245 associations with depression and 44 with anxiety. Partner factors with sound evidence that they protect against both perinatal depression and anxiety are: emotional closeness and global support. Partner factors with a sound evidence base for depression only are communication, conflict, emotional and instrumental support, and relationship satisfaction. This review is limited by the lack of generalizability to single parents and the inability to systematically review moderators and mediators, or control for baseline symptoms. The findings suggest that future prevention programs targeting perinatal depression and anxiety should aim to enhance relationship satisfaction, communication, and emotional closeness, facilitate instrumental and emotional support, and minimize conflict between partners. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.