ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

There is convergent evidence that natural environments allow restoration from stress. This randomised, cross-over, field-based trial compared psychological and physiological responses of unstressed individuals to self-paced 30-min walks in three pleasant environments: residential (urban), natural (green), and natural with water (blue). Changes from baseline (T1) to T2 (end of 30-min walk), and T3 (30 min after leaving environment) were measured in terms of mood, cognitive function, restoration experiences, salivary cortisol, and heart rate variability (HRV). In the final sample (n = 38; 65% male; mean age 40.9 ± 17.6 years), mood and cortisol improved at T2 and T3 in all environments. Green and blue environments were associated with greater restoration experiences, and cognitive function improvements that persisted at T3. Stress reduction (mood and cortisol changes) in all environments points to the salutogenic effect of walking, but natural environments conferred additional cognitive benefits lasting at least 30 min after leaving the environment.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Where to put your best foot forward: Psycho-physiological responses to
walking in natural and urban environments
*Christopher J Gidlow,1 Marc V Jones, 1 Gemma Hurst, 1 Daniel Masterson, 1 David Clark-
Carter, 1 Mika P Tarvainen,2 Graham Smith, 1 Mark Nieuwenhuijsen3
1 Staffordshire University, Stoke on Trent, United Kingdom
2 University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio, Finland
3 Centre for Research in Environmental Epidemiology (CREAL), Barcelona, Spain
CORRESPONDING AUTHOR
* Christopher J Gidlow, Centre for Sport, Health and Exercise Research, Staffordshire
University, Stoke on Trent, United Kingdom, c.gidlow@staffs.ac.uk
KEYWORDS
Environment; nature; walking; stress; cognitive
Please cite article in press as: Gidlow, C.J., et al., Where to put your best foot forward:
Psycho-physiological responses to walking in natural and urban environments. Journal of
Environmental Psychology (2015), http:// doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.11.003
Please note: this version is pre-final proofing and withouth publishers formatting. For final
version please see: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0272494415300438
2
HIGHLIGHTS
Non-stressed adults walked in natural (with/without water) and pleasant urban
environments
Mood improved in natural and urban environments
Salivary cortisol reduced in natural and urban environments
Restorative experience was higher in natural environments
Greater cognitive benefits of natural environments were seen 30 min after leaving the
environment.
3
ABSTRACT
There is convergent evidence that natural environments allow restoration from stress. This
randomised, cross-over field-based trial compared psychological and physiological responses
of unstressed individuals to self-paced 30-minute walks in three pleasant environments:
residential (urban), natural (green), and natural with water (blue). Changes from baseline (T1)
to T2 (end of 30-minute walk), and T3 (30 minutes after leaving environment) were measured
in terms of mood, cognitive function, restoration experiences, salivary cortisol, and heart rate
variability (HRV). In the final sample (n=38; 65% male; mean age 40.9±17.6 years), mood and
cortisol improved at T2 and T3 in all environments. Green and blue environments were
associated with greater restoration experiences, and cognitive function improvements that
persisted at T3. Stress reduction (mood and cortisol changes) in all environments points to the
salutogenic effect of walking, but natural environments conferred additional cognitive benefits
lasting at least 30 minutes after leaving the environment.
4
1. Introduction
There is a convergent evidence for a positive relationship between natural environment
exposure and health (Hartig, Mitchell, de Vries, & Frumkin, 2014). Commonly posited
explanatory mechanisms include increased opportunities for physical activity and social
interaction from active use of natural environments, stress reduction and cognitive restoration
of passive or active use, and mitigation of environmental pollutants, such as noise and air
pollution (Bowler, Buyung-Ali, Knight, & Pullin, 2010; Hartig et al., 2014; Nieuwenhuijsen et
al., 2014).
Experimental studies to characterise the benefits of engaging with natural environments
have typically departed from one of two theoretical standpoints: attention restoration theory
(ART), which proposes that nature allows restoration from directed attention fatigue and enable
more effective cognitive performance (Kaplan, 1995); stress reduction theory (SRT), where
natural environments are thought to influence affective states by promoting recovery from
stress, and diminishing arousal and negative thoughts through psycho-physiological pathways
(Ulrich et al., 1991; Ulrich, 1983).
ART is well supported by data from laboratory-based image viewing studies (Berto,
2005; Staats, Kieviet, & Hartig, 2003; van den Berg, Koole, & van der Wulp, 2003) and some
field studies (Berman, Jonides, & Kaplan, 2008; Hartig, Evans, Jamner, Davis, & Gärling,
2003; Sonntag-Öström et al., 2014; Tennessen & Cimprich, 1995) showing that attention,
measured as performance at cognitive tasks, is better when individuals are exposed to natural
rather than urban environments. For SRT, evidence to date suggests that viewing or visiting
natural environments can result in better affective outcomes measured through self-reported
mood scales (Bowler et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2011; Tsunetsugu et al., 2013; Tyrväinen et al.,
2014), perhaps more so when the natural environment contains water, such as a river, lake or
coast (Barton & Pretty, 2010).
5
There is growing interest in measuring corresponding physiological responses to
natural environments, particularly in field (rather than laboratory) studies, where the
experiences of participants in the environments can be more ecologically valid and typically
involve walking (sometimes in addition to sitting) in urban and natural environments. To date,
the evidence remains less conclusive (Bowler et al., 2010). Physiological measures in field
experiments have been used to characterise cardiovascular (e.g., heart rate, blood pressure,
heart rate variability) and neuroendocrinal (e.g., cortisol, salivary amylase) responses, and
more recently, brain activity (Aspinall, Mavros, Coyne, & Roe, 2015). There is evidence that
walking in natural environments can lower blood pressure (Hartig et al., 2003), but evidence
for stress lowering effects of walking in natural environments measured through reduced
cortisol concentrations, is inconsistent (Beil & Hanes, 2013; Bowler et al., 2010; Lee et al.,
2011; Park, Tsunetsugu, Kasetani, Kagawa, & Miyazaki, 2010). Similarly, despite support
from laboratory studies involving walking in simulated environments (Annerstedt et al., 2013)
and from seated image viewing studies (Brown, Barton, & Gladwell, 2013; Gladwell et al.,
2012), there is not strong support for beneficial heart hate variability (HRV) responses to
natural versus urban environments from field experiments (Bowler et al., 2010). Overall, our
understanding of the cardiovascular and neuroendocrinal responses to natural environments in
field studies remains limited.
We present data from a field-based, randomised, cross-over experiment that addresses
the call for robust experimental examination of psycho-physiological responses to natural
environments (Bowler et al., 2010) and contributes to the emerging literature in a number of
ways. First, it is not uncommon for field studies in this area to use comparator urban
environments that are inherently stressful. We avoided using busy commercial areas and main
roads with high traffic volumes for our urban comparator, to reduce the risk of detecting
negative responses to the urban condition, rather than positive responses to natural
6
environments (Hartig et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2011; Park et al., 2010; Tsunetsugu et al., 2013;
Tyrväinen et al., 2014). Second, we explored instorative effects of natural environments, which
have been described as the benefits that ‘do not necessarily follow a reduction in adaptive
capacities nor involve restoring diminished capacities’ (Hartig, 2007, p.2). Rather than
focusing on restoration from a stressed or depleted baseline state, our interest was the potential
for natural environments to promote psycho-physiological improvements in a non-stressed,
healthy adult sample, as engagement with natural environments may have wider public health
benefits than facilitating recovery from stress or directed attention fatigue. Third, we controlled
for potential confounding effects of social interaction and physical activity. Finally, our
experimental design enabled enquiry beyond the immediate exposure effects to understand
whether any immediate benefits were sustained once people left the environment.
The overarching study aims were to: (i) to compare psycho-physiological responses to
natural environments with and without water, and a pleasant urban environment; (ii) explore
the immediate and sustained instorative potential of natural environments. In the context of
existing research, it was hypothesised that natural environments would confer more favourable
responses that urban, with strongest effects in natural environments with water present.
However, given our focus on healthy, unstressed individuals, and a pleasant urban comparator
environment, such effects might be less pronounced that reported elsewhere.
2. Method
2.1 Participants
Participants were adults who lived, worked or studied in a medium size conurbation in
the West Midlands region of the UK. Inclusion criteria were: aged ≥18 years; non-smokers;
not pregnant; no chronic medical conditions; not taking medication that will influence cortisol
7
or heart rate variability (Granger, Hibel, Fortunato, & Kapelewski, 2009); ‘healthy’ based on
self-report (SF12v2) (Quality Metric, 2006); and able to walk for 30 minutes.
2.2 Design and procedure
Data were collected between June and October 2013. We used a randomised, cross-
over, within-subjects design, whereby all participants walked for 30 minutes in each of three
environments (Figure 1) at the same time on three days. Data were always collected on
weekdays (Mon-Fri) and, where possible, all three days completed within two weeks
(depending on participant availability). The order of environmental exposures was individually
randomised by ascribing a number of 1 to 6 to represent each possible environment
combination, which was then assigned to the participant using a random number generator (MS
Excel). Although participants were not given prior warning of the order in which environments
would be visited, blinding of allocation was not relevant given the nature of the trial (i.e.,
exposures involved visiting an environment; all participants visited all environments).
Environment visits were only conducted in temperate conditions and were re-arranged in the
event of rain/inclement weather conditions. Two researchers (GH, DM) controlled these
procedures, with the same researcher being assigned to participants for all of their
environmental visits.
Participants were recruited through local media, advertising in and around the
University campus, and a mail shot to households within 1 km of the campus, with eligibility
screening via an online survey. This approach was intended to improve generalisability, rather
than delimiting to a student sample. Eligible participants were invited to attend the University
at either 12:00 or 14:00 on each day, and to refrain from consuming caffeine or food for at least
60 minutes prior to arrival. Figure 2 illustrates the procedures on each of the three days of data
collection. Briefly, following baseline measures (T1), participants were transported to the
8
environment to walk along a pre-designated route, at a self-directed pace. Self-pacing was used
to optimise participant experience and ecological validity, and walking helped to ensure that
exercise intensity was of mostly light intensity (approaching moderate intensity for some). A
researcher (GH or DM) accompanied the participant on the walk, remaining half a stride behind
to allow the participant to determine the pace. Participants were asked for their Rate of
Perceived Exertion (RPE) at five-minute intervals, with no other social interaction. After 30
minutes, follow-up measures were taken (T2), and repeated a further 30 minutes later after
returning to the University (T3). Participants were offered a £40 retail voucher in appreciation
of their time. All study procedures were approved by the University Ethics Committee.
2.3 Environments
The three environmental conditions were: urban - quiet residential streets with low
levels of traffic; green - country park within the city; blue - footpath besides a canal with a
range of natural vegetation (Figure 1). Environments were selected on the basis of several
criteria: less than 15-minute drive from the University; rated as ‘natural’ or ‘urban’ (depending
on condition) and ‘pleasant’ based on responses to a separate online image survey (Gidlow,
Jones, Hurst, & Masterson, 2013); comparable gradient to minimise differences in exercise
intensity; and unlikely to have large numbers of people to reduce the risk of incidental social
interaction. The roads in the urban environment were residential, had traffic levels that were
too low for official measurement and classification in the UK (i.e., <500 cars/day), and noise
levels comparable to the natural environments (urban 50.56±4.25 dbA, green 47.47±2.94 dbA,
blue 45.60±1.46 dbA, average of two 15-minute recordings at representative points in each
environment measured by a Precision Gold N05CC Sound level meter). The low levels of noise
in all environments provided confidence the slightly higher readings in the blue and urban
9
environments should not have been sufficient to promote a stress response in the urban
environment.
2.4 Measures
Baseline profiling. Participants completed a series of questionnaires to profile: socio-
demographics (age, gender, ethnicity, education, employment status, postcode); self-reported
health using the Short-Form 12 (SF12v2 used to determine Physical and Mental Component
Scores, where scores below/above 50 indicate health that is below/above average,
respectively);(Quality Metric, 2006) Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) (Cohen & Williamson,
1988); nature-relatedness using the NR-6 Nature-relatedness scale (Nisbet, Zelenski, &
Murphy, 2009). The latter was included to explore whether the extent to which individuals felt
connected with the natural world could explain differential responses to natural environments.
Responses to the Natural Environment.
- Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) was assessed using the BRUMS (Terry, Lane, &
Fogarty, 2003), a widely used and validated abbreviated version of the Profile of Moods States
(McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1971), where lower values indicate better mood.
- Cognitive function was assessed through the Backward Digit Span (BDS), a measure
of working memory used by others in this area (Berman et al., 2008). Participants were read a
sequence of three to nine digits and asked to repeat them in reverse order. This was repeated
up to 14 times (two repetitions of each digit span) and stopped after two consecutive failures,
with the length of the longest correct sequence providing a measure of cognitive function (i.e.,
higher scores indicate better cognitive function).
- Restoration experience was measured through an abbreviated six-item version of the
Restoration Outcome Scale (Korpela, Ylén, Tyrväinen, & Silvennoinen, 2008), where higher
10
scores indicate a more restorative experience. The scale included items, such as ‘I feel calmer’,
‘After visiting this place I feel restored and relaxed’, and ‘My concentration and alertness
clearly increased’, to provide a measure of the perceived restorative experience of visiting each
environment.
- Salivary cortisol was measured as a physiological marker of stress. Cortisol
concentrations were determined from saliva samples collected using synthetic swabs, which
participants placed beneath their tongue for two minutes. All samples were centrifuged at
3000rpm for 15 minutes, divided into two samples and stored separately at -80°C until batch
analysis (Salimetrics Ltd. High Sensitivity Salivary Cortisol Enzyme Immunoassay Kit).
Salivary cortisol concentrations (nmol/l) provided an objective measure of stress, with higher
values indicating higher stress.
- Ambulatory Heart Rate (HR) and HRV data were collected using the eMotion monitor
(http://www.megaemg.com/products/emotion-hrv) (Heikkinen, 2012). HR was included as an
objective measure of exercise intensity. HRV reflects the interplay between the excitatory
sympathetic nervous system, which is dominant at times of stress and the inhibitory
parasympathetic nervous system, which is dominant in periods of relative safety and restoration
(Appelhans and Luecken, 2006). Data processing in Kubios software (http://kubios.uef.fi)
included: artefact correction (Tarvainen, Niskanen, Lipponen, Ranta-Aho, & Karjalainen,
2014) removal of very low frequency trend components using a smoothness priors detrending
method (Tarvainen, Ranta-Aho, & Karjalainen, 2002), and interpolation of the RR series. HRV
indicators used in analysis included mean RR interval (time between each heart beat), standard
deviation of normal-to-normal intervals (SDNN), and low frequency (LF, 0.04-0.15 Hz) and
high frequency (HF, 0.15-0.4 Hz) powers from Fourier spectra. For each HRV variable, the
percentage coefficient of component variance (%CCV) was calculated to obtain %CCV to
11
control for differences in RR interval as a result of different exercise intensities (Højgaard,
Holstein-Rathlou, Agner, & Kanters, 1998).
- Rate of Perceived Exertion (RPE) was measured at five-minute intervals during the walks
using the Borg Scale (Borg, 1990), to provide a measure of participant-rated exercise intensity.
2.5 Statistical analysis
For each of the outcome measures, with the exception of restoration (measured T2
only), a three-by-three repeated measures ANOVA was used to investigate the effects of time
(baseline, immediately post and 30 minutes after leaving the environment), environment
(urban, green, blue) and time by environment interactions. If there was a significant interaction,
this was explored using simple main effects via one-way ANOVA comparing environments,
and where environment was significant, this was explored using paired contrasts, via t-tests
with Tukey’s HSD used to control for multiple testing. Where there was a main effect of
environment, but no interaction with time, this was again explored via paired contrasts.
Physiological measurements required transformation to meet parametric analysis assumptions
(cortisol concentration was square-root transformed and HRV parameters were log-
transformed). Where assumptions of sphericity were violated, adjusted degrees of freedom
using the Greenhouse-Geisser estimate were reviewed. The target sample size was 40. This
was based on: an estimated 20-50 participants required to detect a medium-large effects, using
a within subjects ANOVA with three conditions (df=2), power of .8 and an alpha level of .05;
the practical considerations of collecting all data in temperate conditions. Analyses were
undertaken in IBM SPSS version 22.
12
3. Results
3.1 Participants
Eighty-eight screening questionnaires were completed. Seventeen individuals were
excluded as ineligible (unable to attend appointments n=5; on medication n=6; long-term
illness n=2; medication and long-term illness n=3; smoker n=1) and 31 declined to take part or
did not respond to further communication. Of the 40 participants who started the study, two
were lost to follow-up and did not visit all environment; 38 completed and were included in
analysis (23 male, 15 female; Mage = 40.9±17.6). Participants were predominantly White
British (92.1%) and the majority were in full-time work (29%), students (29%) or retired
individuals (24%); the remainder comprised unemployed, home-keepers and those in part-time
work. Relatively good health of the sample was confirmed in terms of self-reported health
(Physical Component score=55.5±5.08; mean Mental Component Score=52.3±7.8), Body
Mass Index (25.4±5.0 kgm-2) and mean PSS score (11.1±6.5).
3.2 Walking exposure
Mean RPE was slightly higher in the green (10.2±1.5) and urban (10.0±1.5), compared
with the blue environment (9.0±1.5), but all were in the very light to light intensity range. Heart
rate data confirmed that participations were walking at moderate intensity; i.e., 55-69% of
maximal HR (Pollock et al., 1998). As a percentage of theoretical maximal heart rate
(HRmax=220-age), mean %HRmax was comparable in each environment (urban 59.97±8.86%;
green 62.18±9.56%; blue 58.67±9.35%).
3.3 Psychological responses to environments
Mood improved from baseline (T1) in all environments, with a significant main effect
of time on TMD, F(1.37, 46.41)=6.40, p=.009, η2=.07, post-walk (T1 to T2), F(1, 34)=8.77,
13
p=.006, η2=.23, and 30-mintues after leaving the environment (T1 to T3), F(1, 34)=5.29,
p=.028, η2=.16. There was no significant main effect for environment, F(2, 68)=1.77 p=.178,
η2= .02 and no significant environment*time interaction effect, F(2.91, 98.89)=1.01, p=.389,
η2=.005 (Table 1).
There was also a significant main effect of time on cognitive function, F(2, 74)=3.39,
p=.039, η2=.01, but no significant main effect for environment F(2, 74)=.926, p=.401, η2=.01.
Additionally, a significant environment*time interaction effect was evident, F(4, 148)=2.89,
p=.024, η2=.02, such that improvements in cognitive task performance persisted at T3
following exposure to both natural environments, but reduced to below baseline levels in the
urban condition: Blue vs. Urban (T1 vs. T3), F(1, 37)=9.26, p=.004; Green vs. Urban (T2 vs.
T3), F(1, 37)=4.35, p=.044 (Table 1, Figure 3).
A significant difference in restoration experiences was also found between
environments following the 30-minute walk, F(2, 72)=28.54, p<.001, η2=.21 (Figure 4).
Follow-up contrasts showed that ratings of restoration experiences were significantly higher in
the natural compared with urban environments: Green vs. Urban, t(36)=5.87, p<.001, η2=.49;
Blue vs. Urban, t(36)=-6.43, p <.001, η2=.57.
The influence of participants’ trait nature-relatedness was explored for outcomes where
a significant environment effect was observed. Nature-relatedness was not significantly
correlated with restorative experience in the green or blue environments (all p>.069), or with
changes in cognitive function (all p >.224). Collectively, the results suggested no potential
relationship linking nature-relatedness with restorative experience or cognitive function, so
further analyses were not warranted.
14
3.4 Physiological responses to environments
A significant main effect of time on cortisol concentration was found, F(1.37,
48.01)=82.83, p<.001, η2=.29. Significant reductions were observed in all environments at T2,
F(1, 35)=83.80, p<.001, η2=.71, and T3, F(1, 35)=98.23 , p<.001, η2=.73 (Table 1). There was
no significant main effect for environment, F(2, 70)=.195, p=.823, η2=.002 and no significant
environment*time interaction effect, F(2.52, 88.30)=.801, p=.478, η2=.004. Tests were run for
males and females separately, but there was no evidence of differential cortisol response by
gender (data not shown).
Heart rate variability data were inconclusive, showing no consistent patterns by time or
between environments for any of the HRV indicators from baseline to T3 (Table 1). Differences
in respiration rate were explored through HF Peak Power (Hz) to determine possible
confounding effects, but environment*time effects were not evident (all p>.194).
4. Discussion
Our data suggest that walking in a natural environment confers greater benefit for
restorative experience and cognitive function, which persisted for at least 30 minutes after
leaving the environment, when compared with a similar walk in a pleasant urban environment.
However, no differences in self-reported mood, or physiological indicators of stress (cortisol
and HRV), were observed.
Perceptions of greater restoration following natural environment visits are consistent
with the literature (Beil & Hanes, 2013; Bodin & Hartig, 2003; Korpela, Ylén, Tyrväinen, &
Silvennoinen, 2010). Significant environmental differences in cognitive function did not
manifest until 30-minutes later when participants had left the environment. In relation to the
ART, researchers have shown changes in performance on cognitive tasks do not consistently
emerge within 15-20 minutes of natural environment exposure (Laumann, Gärling, &
15
Stormark, 2003), but are manifest after 50 minutes of engagement (Berman et al., 2008; Hartig
et al., 2003). In our study, these changes were observed 60 minutes after first engaging with
the natural environment (30 minutes after leaving). This has practical implications; for
example, a lunchtime walk in a natural environment could help to improve cognitive
performance at work for at least 30 minutes after returning.
We did not find environmental differences in indicators of stress assessed by self-
reported mood and cortisol. The lack of mood effect consistent with some studies comparing
responses to walking in natural and urban environments (Johansson, Hartig, & Staats, 2011;
Kinnafick & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, 2014). It also supports data showing mood benefits of
walking at a self-directed pace (Ekkekakis et al. 2011). Where field-based studies have reported
greater improvements in mood following exposure to natural, compared with urban
environments, these are potentially attributable to a negative response to the urban condition
(Hartig et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2011; Tsunetsugu et al., 2013). Reductions in cortisol were also
observed in all environmental conditions, which is consistent with the lack of environmental
effects found in the published literature (Beil & Hanes, 2013; Lee et al., 2011; Tyrväinen et al.,
2014). Where beneficial cortisol responses have been reported, there are recognised limitations,
such as absence of baseline data (e.g., Park et al., 2010), or other potential confounders, such
as differences in activity levels across environments (e.g., Van den Berg & Custers, 2010). The
magnitude of reductions in cortisol we observed over 30 minutes were greater than would be
expected from the typical diurnal pattern (Doane, Chen, Sladek, Van Lenten, & Granger, 2015;
Edwards, Clow, Evans, & Hucklebridge, 2001), which suggests that there were instorative
effects although this remains speculative in the absence of an inactive control condition. In any
case, as improvements were observed across all conditions, our cortisol data did not support
the natural versus urban benefits reported elsewhere.
16
Heart rate variability data did not show any differences in environmental response, or
any consistent patterns over time. Significant natural-urban environment differences in HRV
have been reported in laboratory settings using images and 3-D simulations (Annerstedt et al.,
2013; Brown et al., 2013; Gladwell et al., 2012). Field-based research has suggested favourable
responses to walking and sitting in forest versus busy urban environments (higher HF; lower
LF:HF). However, in the absence of baseline data, caution is required (Park et al., 2010;
Tsunetsugu et al., 2013), and where change from baseline has been reported, a negative
response to the busy urban environment, rather than positive natural response, is evident (Lee
et al., 2011). Based on our data, 30-minutes of walking or low intensity physical activity in
natural or pleasant urban environments does not significantly alter HRV. However, as noted
below, the amount of noise in HRV when taking measurements in the field, rather than in
laboratory conditions, might negate our ability to detect subtle environmental responses using
HRV.
In our sample of non-stressed, healthy adults who visited natural and pleasant urban
environments, our findings are better explained by ART than SRT. Specifically, the
environmental differences that emerged were for restorative experience and cognitive function,
the latter emerging 60-minutes post-initial exposure. That we observed stress-reducing effects
in all environments points to the salutogenic effects of being physically active, even at low
intensity. The improvements to mood across all environments and of cognitive function in the
natural environments may reflect the motivational properties of physical activity (Ekkekakis,
2003). Also, the physical activity might have such a dominant influence that more subtle
environmental effects were not detected. This potential limitation is particularly relevant to
HRV. Although we used the %CCV indices to account for differences in heart rate (i.e.,
exercise intensity) and explored differences in respiration rate, additional controls were not
feasible. Further we recognise that it was not possible to adhere to recommendations for HRV
17
measurement. Efforts to make the participant experience as normal as possible and to avoid
boredom, meant that we were unable to measure HRV during timed breathing for five-minute
periods, and used one-minute segments of untimed breathing as used elsewhere (Lee et al.,
2011).
Other limitations are acknowledged. First, it was not feasible to explore the full range
of different environments or to recruit a fully representative sample, which limits
generalisability. Rather, we recruited a range of heathy, non-stressed adults and selected
environments with specific attributes (as detailed earlier). Second, while environment visits
were only conducted in temperate conditions, some variability in weather between days could
have influenced participant experiences. Third, it would have been preferable, but not feasible
to standardise exercise intensity across all participants and environments. As detailed earlier,
self-paced walking was chosen to optimise participant experience and ecological validity,
whilst helping to ensure that exercise mostly light intensity (so avoiding problems of psycho-
physiological impacts of higher intensity activity). Fourth, the researcher walking with the
participant was necessary, but could have made the experience feel somewhat unusual.
Measures to limit this included having the same researcher-participant pairing throughout and
randomising the order of exposures (to negate issues of the experience feeling less unusual on
subsequent days).
Our study was concerned with the potential instorative effects in individuals who are
not stressed or cognitively fatigued (Hartig, 2007), and used a comparator urban environment
that was not stressful or unpleasant. This comes from a public health perspective, that
engagement with natural environments may have wider public health benefits than facilitating
recovery from stress. The applicability of research in this area could be enhanced by replicating
how people typically engage with natural environments. In particular, people tend to visit the
same natural environments repeatedly so understanding whether the benefits of a single
18
exposure are attenuated, maintained or increased is an area of future focus. Further research
could include a broader range of urban and natural environments to begin mapping
environmental characteristics to stress-reducing capacity. For example, a recent study has
shown that the relationship between tree coverage and stress recovery was non-linear, such that
stress recovery improved with increasing tree coverage up to a point, but decreased thereafter
(Jiang et al. 2014). Exploring the longevity of the positive effects on cognitive function from
being physically active in a pleasant natural environment would be another area for future
research, specifically to determine if these extend beyond the 60 minutes post-engagement
shown in the present study.
Overall, our findings indicate that light intensity physical activity in a natural
environment confers greater benefit for restorative experience and cognitive function, which
persisted for at least 30 minutes after leaving the environment, when compared with a similar
walk in an urban environment. For immediate improvements in measures of well-being you
can put your best foot forward in a pleasant urban or natural environment, but for the additional
improvement in cognitive function, choose a natural environment.
19
5. References
Annerstedt, M., Jönsson, P., Wallergård, M., Johansson, G., Karlson, B., Grahn, P., . . .
Währborg, P. (2013). Inducing physiological stress recovery with sounds of nature in a
virtual reality forest - results from a pilot study. Physiology & Behavior, 118, 240-250.
Appelhans, B. M., & Luecken, L. J. (2006). Heart rate variability as an index of regulated
emotional responding. Review of General Psychology, 10, 229.
Aspinall, P., Mavros, P., Coyne, R., & Roe, J. (2015). The urban brain: Analysing outdoor
physical activity with mobile EEG. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 49, 272-276.
Barton, J., & Pretty, J. (2010). What is the best dose of nature and green exercise for
improving mental health? A multi-study analysis. Environmental Science & Technology,
44, 3947-3955.
Beil, K., & Hanes, D. (2013). The influence of urban natural and built environments on
physiological and psychological measures of stress - A pilot study. International Journal
of Environmental Research and Public Health, 10, 1250-1267.
Berman, M., Jonides, J., & Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting with
nature. Psychological Science, 19, 1207-1212.
Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25, 249-259.
Bodin, M., & Hartig, T. (2003). Does the outdoor environment matter for psychological
restoration gained through running? Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4, 141-153.
Borg, G. (1990). Psychophysical scaling with applications in physical work and the
perception of exertion. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health,
16(Suppl 1), 55-58.
20
Bowler, D., Buyung-Ali, L., Knight, T., & Pullin, A. (2010). A systematic review of evidence
for the added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health,
10, 456.
Brown, D. K., Barton, J. L., & Gladwell, V. F. (2013). Viewing nature scenes positively
affects recovery of autonomic function following acute-mental stress. Environmental
Science & Technology, 47, 5562-5569.
Cohen, S., & Williamson, G. (1988). Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United
States. In S. Spacapan, & S. Oskamp (Eds.). The Social Psychology of Health. The
Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology (pp. 31-67). Thousand Oaks, CA,
US: Sage Publications.
Doane, L. D., Chen, F. R., Sladek, M. R., Van Lenten, S. A., & Granger, D. A. (2015). Latent
trait cortisol (LTC) levels: Reliability, validity, and stability. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
55, 21-35.
Edwards, S., Clow, A., Evans, P., & Hucklebridge, F. (2001). Exploration of the awakening
cortisol response in relation to diurnal cortisol secretory activity. Life Sciences, 68, 2093-
2103.
Ekkekakis, P. (2003). Pleasure and displeasure from the body: Perspectives from exercise.
Cognition & Emotion, 17, 213-239.
Ekkekakis, P., Parfitt, G., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2011). The pleasure and displeasure people
feel when they exercise at different intensities. Sports Medicine, 41, 641-671.
Gidlow, C. J., Jones, M. V., Hurst, G. L., & Masterson, D. Individual responses to natural and
urban environments. Conference of the International Society of Environmental
Epidemiology (ISEE) 2013. Basel, Switzerland,
21
Gladwell, V. F., Brown, D. K., Barton, J. L., Tarvainen, M. P., Kuoppa, P., Pretty, J., . . .
Sandercock, G. R. H. (2012). The effects of views of nature on autonomic control.
European Journal of Applied Physiology, 112, 3379-3386.
Granger, D. A., Hibel, L. C., Fortunato, C. K., & Kapelewski, C. H. (2009). Medication
effects on salivary cortisol: Tactics and strategy to minimize impact in behavioral and
developmental science. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 1437-1448.
Hartig, T. (2007). Three steps to understanding restorative environments as health resources.
Open Space: People Space, 163-179.
Hartig, T., Evans, G. W., Jamner, L. D., Davis, D. S., & Gärling, T. (2003). Tracking
restoration in natural and urban field settings. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23,
109-123.
Hartig, T., Mitchell, R., de Vries, S., & Frumkin, H. (2014). Nature and health. Annual
Review of Public Health, 35, 207-228.
Heikkinen, O. (2012). Development and validation of an ambulatory HRV measurement
system (MSc).
Højgaard, M. V., Holstein-Rathlou, N., Agner, E., & Kanters, J. K. (1998). Dynamics of
spectral components of heart rate variability during changes in autonomic balance.
American Journal of Physiology - Heart and Circulatory Physiology, 275, H213-H219.
Jiang, B., Chang, C. Y., & Sullivan, W. C. (2014). A dose of nature: Tree cover, stress
reduction, and gender differences. Landscape and Urban Planning, 132, 26-36.
Johansson, M., Hartig, T., & Staats, H. (2011). Psychological benefits of walking:
Moderation by company and outdoor environment. Applied Psychology: Health and
Well-being, 3, 261-280.
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 169-182.
22
Kinnafick, F., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2014). The effect of the physical environment and
levels of activity on affective states. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 38, 241-251.
Korpela, K. M., Ylén, M., Tyrväinen, L., & Silvennoinen, H. (2008). Determinants of
restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Health & Place, 14, 636-652.
Korpela, K. M., Ylén, M., Tyrväinen, L., & Silvennoinen, H. (2010). Favorite green,
waterside and urban environments, restorative experiences and perceived health in
Finland. Health Promotion International, 25, 200-209.
Laumann, K., Gärling, T., & Stormark, K. (2003). Selective attention and heart rate responses
to natural and urban environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 125-134.
Lee, J., Park, B. J., Tsunetsugu, Y., Ohira, T., Kagawa, T., & Miyazaki, Y. (2011). Effect of
forest bathing on physiological and psychological responses in young Japanese male
subjects. Public Health, 125, 93-100.
McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Manual for the profile of mood states.
San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Services.
Nieuwenhuijsen, M. J., Kruize, H., Gidlow, C., Andrusaityte, S., Antó, J. M., Basagaña, X., .
. . Grazuleviciene, R. (2014). Positive health effects of the natural outdoor environment
in typical populations in different regions in Europe (PHENOTYPE): A study
programme protocol. BMJ Open, 4(4).
Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J. M., & Murphy, S. A. (2009). The nature relatedness scale.
Environment and Behavior, 41, 715-740.
Park, B. J., Tsunetsugu, Y., Kasetani, T., Kagawa, T., & Miyazaki, Y. (2010). The
physiological effects of shinrin-yoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing):
Evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across japan. Environmental Health and
Preventive Medicine, 15, 18-26.
23
Pollock, M., Gaesser, G., Butcher, J., Després, J., Dishman, R., Franklin, B., & Garber, C. E.
(1998). American college of sports medicine position stand. The recommended quantity
and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory and muscular
fitness, and flexibility in healthy adults. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,
30, 975-991.
Quality Metric. (2006). The SF-12®: An even shorter health survey. Available at:
http://www.sf-36.org/tools/sf12.shtml.
Sonntag-Öström, E., Nordin, M.,- Lundell, Y., Dolling, A., Wiklund, U., Karlsson, M., . . .
Slunga Järvholm, L. (2014). Restorative effects of visits to urban and forest
environments in patients with exhaustion disorder. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening,
13, 344-354.
Staats, H., Kieviet, A., & Hartig, T. (2003). Where to recover from attentional fatigue: An
expectancy-value analysis of environmental preference. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 23, 147-157.
Tarvainen, M. P., Niskanen, J., Lipponen, J. A., Ranta-Aho, P. O., & Karjalainen, P. A.
(2014). Kubios HRVheart rate variability analysis software. Computer Methods and
Programs in Biomedicine, 113, 210-220.
Tarvainen, M. P., Ranta-Aho, P. O., & Karjalainen, P. A. (2002). An advanced detrending
method with application to HRV analysis. IEEE Transactions on Bio-Medical
Engineering, 49, 172-175.
Tennessen, C. M., & Cimprich, B. (1995). Views to nature: Effects on attention. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 15, 77-85.
Terry, P. C., Lane, A. M., & Fogarty, G. J. (2003). Construct validity of the profile of mood
states-adolescents for use with adults. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4, 125-139.
24
Tsunetsugu, Y., Lee, J., Park, B., Tyrväinen, L., Kagawa, T., & Miyazaki, Y. (2013).
Physiological and psychological effects of viewing urban forest landscapes assessed by
multiple measurements. Landscape and Urban Planning, 113, 90-93.
Tyrväinen, L., Ojala, A., Korpela, K., Lanki, T., Tsunetsugu, Y., & Kagawa, T. (2014). The
influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 38, 1-9.
Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., & Zelson, M. (1991).
Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 11, 201-230.
Ulrich, R. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. Behaviour and the
Natural Environment (pp. 85-125). US: Springer.
Van den Berg, A. E., & Custers, M. H. G. (2010). Gardening promotes neuroendocrine and
affective restoration from stress. Journal of Health Psychology, June 3.
van den Berg, A. E., Koole, S. L., & van der Wulp, N. Y. (2003). Environmental preference
and restoration: (How) are they related? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 135-
146.
25
1
2
3
FIGURES
4
5
Urban
Green
Blue
Figure 1. Example images of exposure environments (urban; green; blue)
6
7
8
9
26
Participant recruitment (online
screening questionnaire) Excluded if:
1. Current smoker
2. Pregnant
3. Taking medication
4. Suffering from a chronic
medical condition
Baseline profiling questionnaire
(completed online or at University)
Baseline measures (lab)
30-minute walk in;
T1 (0 mins):
Baseline measures: affect (BRUMS); cognitive function (backwards digit span task); and cortisol. Ambulatory
HRV monitoring begins (end of T1).
30 min walk:
CalFit monitoring starts. RPE collected at 5-minute intervals throughout exposure and social interaction
monitored.
T2 (30 mins):
Follow-up measures:affect; cognitive function; and cortisol. CalFit monitoring ends (start of T2).
T3 (60 mins):
Follow-up measures (lab): affect; cognitive function; and cortisol. HRV monitoring ends (start of T3).
Urban
environment Green
environment Green + Blue
environment
T1:
T2:
T3: Follow-up measures (lab)
Transport to environment (10-15 min drive)
Follow-up measures (environment)
Transport to University (10-15 min drive)
10
Figure 2. Flow diagram for each day of data collection
11
12
13
14
27
15
16
Figure 3. Mean change in cognitive function (backwards digit span task) from baseline to 60-
17
minute post exposure follow-up by environment
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
28
25
Figure 4. Mean restorative experience, as measured by the ROS, by environment
26
29
27
28
Table 1. Mean (SD) values for psychological and physiological variables from baseline (T1) to 30- (T2) and 60-minute (T3) post-exposure
29
follow-up by environment
30
Cognitive
Mood
Cortisol
Heart Rate Variability
Function
(TMD)
(nmol/l)
% CCV
HFCCV
LFCCV
LFCCV:HFCCV
N
38
35
37
34
34
34
34
T1
5.82 (2.68)
-4.43 (4.90)
5.86 (3.59)
7.05 (3.50)
0.03 (0.02)
0.04 (0.03)
1.45 (1.05)
Blue
T2
6.53 (2.48)
-6.54 (4.18)
3.69 (1.63)
-
-
-
-
T3
6.71 (2.54)
-5.63 (3.77)
3.37 (1.55)
7.03 (3.66)
0.02 (0.01)
0.04 (0.20)
1.99 (0.96)
T1
6.21 (2.93)
-3.00 (6.31)
5.71 (3.14)
6.45 (3.29)
0.03 (0.02)
0.03 (0.02)
1.79 (1.28)
Green
T2
6.37 (2.57)
-5.34 (4.03)
3.87 (2.17)
-
-
-
-
T3
6.82 (2.59)
-5.26 (3.23)
3.53 (1.88)
6.67 (4.07)
0.02 (0.02)
0.04 (0.03)
2.17 (1.02)
T1
6.68 (5.79)
-4.00 (5.43)
5.21 (2.36)
6.89 (3.07)
0.03 (0.01)
0.04 (0.02)
2.08 (1.39)
Urban
T2
6.84 (2.52)
-5.57 (4.10)
3.77 (1.79)
-
-
-
-
T3
6.45 (2.35)
-5.34 (3.67)
3.41 (1.64)
6.13 (3.48)
0.02 (0.01)
0.03 (0.02)
2.13 (0.77)
TMD, Total Mood Disturbance; %CV, percentage coefficient of component variance; HFCCV, percentage coefficient of component variance for
31
high frequency powers; LFCCV, percentage coefficient of component variance for low frequency power; LFCCV:HFCCV, ratio between
32
LFCCV and HFCCV
33
34
... The restorative and cognitive benefits of natural environments can be well explained and supported by Attention Restoration Theory (ART) 9,10 . ART 4,11 , which suggests that natural environments enable people to restore depleted attention and improve cognitive functioning. ...
... For EEG analysis, we calculated the anterofrontal EEG responses by averaging the data from AF3, AF4, F3, F4, F7, and F8, and occipital EEG responses by averaging the data from O1 and O2. These calculated EEG responses were then subjected to a fast fourier transformation to calculate the power spectral density (PSD) of the delta (2-4 Hz), theta (4-7 Hz), alpha (8)(9)(10)(11)(12)(13), and beta (13-30 Hz) bands. DTR was calculated by dividing the PSD in delta by that in theta, DAR was calculated by dividing the PSD in delta by that in alpha, TBR was calculated by dividing the PSD in theta by that in beta, and ABR was calculated by dividing the PSD in alpha by that in beta. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates the effects of natural exposure in an indoor environment on restorative quality and cognitive ability. Thirty participants were shown nature at three different indoor sites: baseline, indoor (some vegetation), and semi-indoor (a large amount of vegetation and view to sky) for five minutes. After viewing, they completed an assessment of restoration and a cognitive task, and their electroencephalography (EEG) was recorded. Compared to the baseline, the sites with nature resulted in restorative (higher perceived restoration scores) and cognitive (higher working memory performance and lower delta-to-theta ratio (DTR), delta-to-alpha ratio (DAR), theta-to-beta ratio (TBR), and alpha-to-beta ratio (ABR) responses) benefits. These findings further our understanding of the effects of exposure to nature on restorative and cognitive benefits in an indoor environment, and help to build guidance for future research on the effects of nature indoors and designing restorative- and cognitive-enhancing indoor spaces.
... Early researchers proposed that the environment can contribute to the health benefits of individuals and society by reducing stress, alleviating anxiety, and reducing feelings of fear [1][2][3]. Even just taking walks in natural environments has been found to be beneficial for health [4]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explored the relationship between mental and physical therapeutic effects through three dimensions: man–environment relationships, a sense of place, and symbolic landscapes. The study used a combination of qualitative and quantitative research methods. Local residents living in the coastal area of Xinglin Bay were the research objects. Quantitative data analysis revealed that the frequency of residents’ visits was an important variable affecting their physical and treatment perceptions. For those who visit frequently, these visits can evoke memories, which can better express their sensory experience. The text analysis showed that residents picked up two major landscape elements to form the sense of place and symbolic landscape: one is the water body in the coastal zone, and the other is the cultural symbol of the peninsula. Based on untoward event experience, the residents assembled the elements into a new spatial relationship with therapeutic affordance.
... Previous research has demonstrated that spending time in blue spaces improves mental health, such as by viewing water bodies [21] and walking around a waterside [51]. So far, there has been a great deal of discussion about the causes of the mental restoration of blue space. ...
Article
Full-text available
Urban Blue Spaces (UBS) have been found to be beneficial to people’s mental health. Yet, the empirical evidence for how and why different types of urban blue spaces could promote residents’ mental health is still limited. Accordingly, 164 observation samples were collected for this experiment relating to the restorative perception of environmental exposure. The effects of two exposure behaviors (15 min of viewing and 15 min of walking) on psychological recovery in three different urban blue spaces settings (Urban River, Urban Canal, Urban Lake) were investigated in a field experiment. These are the main findings of this current study: (1) all three UBSs increased vitality, feelings of restoration, and positive emotions, and decreased negative emotions; (2) the mental restoration effects between walking and viewing among the three UBSs showed no significant differences; (3) of the three UBSs, urban rivers and urban lakes were the most restorative, while urban canals were less so; (4) the concept of “natural health dose” is proposed, where the health experiences of different UBSs in urban settings can show differences depending on the natural components and their levels of the environment (blue, blue + green, blue + blue). The results of this experiment can provide fundamental evidence that can contribute to building healthy cities through the management and design of different blue spaces.
... Working adults provide an excellent test for investigating the effect of self-efficacy beliefs on diurnal indicators of physiological strain, as they are generally healthy, handle a range of potential social and work-related stressors, yet are quite homogeneous in the sense of excluding relevant potential confounding variables (e.g., severe poverty, unemployment). Of interest, literature suggests that work itself occupies more than half of adults waking time and workrelated stressors are a major source of daily stress for most adults (Goh et al., 2016), such that being employed is related to an enhanced cortisol response (Gidlow et al., 2016). Moreover, in adulthood, work plays a central role in most people's lives by satisfying basic psychological needs (e.g., needs of belongingness) and being a main source of intrinsic worth and value, social exchanges and affiliation, and individual identities (Bakker et al., 2023). ...
Article
In the present ecological study, we analyzed the relations of a set of self-efficacy beliefs at work to parameters of diurnal cortisol variation. Specifically, using data collected during two consecutive working days from 166 workers, we tested a mediation model positing social and work-related self-efficacy beliefs as mediators of the relations between self-regulatory emotional self-efficacy beliefs in managing negative emotions and cortisol indicators. Results from the multilevel mediation analyses supported the proposed model for work-related self-efficacy, which resulted as a significant mediator of the relation between self-regulatory emotional self-efficacy beliefs in managing negative emotions and the overall cortisol daily production indexed by computing the area under the curve with respect to the ground. Findings suggest the importance of self-efficacy beliefs for workers' physiological adjustment. Theoretical and practical contributions of the findings are discussed.
... The comparison of psycho-physiological restoration responses between urban and natural environments showed that the restoration response increased in both waterfront and green spaces compared with urban environments. This is consistent with previous findings (Hartig, Mang & Evans, 1991;Gidlow et al., 2016;Deng et al., 2020;Park, Lee, Jung & Swenson, 2020). As seen in the PRSS results, when exposed to a natural environment, the participants felt a great sense of relief from their responsibilities and hurdles in urban life. ...
Article
Full-text available
The literature lacks a model of the specific environmental element that induces restoration. Thus, this study contributes to the development of sustainable and healthy environments by examining the effect of the soundscape and landscape experience on psycho-physiological restoration from the audiovisual interaction perspective. It examined various urban and natural environments to propose a restoration model for environmental design application. Sixty participants evaluated three each of urban, waterfront, and green environments using virtual reality technology. The audiovisual characteristics of environments were quantified, and environmental perception was assessed per 14 audiovisual element identifications, six perceived affective qualities, and six overall environmental qualities. The restoration responses were classified as psychological and physiological. Accordingly, 10 restoration models that can interpret design factors of the urban environment with a maximum correlation of 0.80 and an explanatory power of 22% emerged from urban audiovisual design elements and psycho-physiological responses. Consequently, to improve restoration response, visually attractive and spatial-natural landscapes and acoustically supportive and tranquil soundscapes should be created. Moreover, urban designers can use the models to clearly identify physical characteristics that should be prioritized when promoting the health improvement of urbanites. Furthermore, the study findings can serve as basic data for the development of healthy environments.
Article
Objectives To identify how quality and design of streets impacts urban stress. Background Few studies have comprehensively addressed environmental factors affecting stress in urban public spaces. However, a remarkable portion of our everyday life is spent in public spaces, particularly streets. Method This study seeks to evaluate the effect of three types of streets as major public spaces on stress. These include a street with the dominance of green spaces (A), a motorist-oriented street (B), and a pedestrian street (C). For this purpose, we selected a group of participants ( n = 16) aged 20-30, with an equal number of men and women who were generally healthy and had normal stress levels. Participants were asked to wear an electroencephalogram (EEG) headset, walk different streets, and answer the Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) and urban design qualities questionnaires. Results According to the results, participants experienced the highest stress in street type B and the lowest in type A. Conclusions Green space and vegetation, a sense of security, privacy and coziness, climatic comfort, and safety of space had the most positive effect on stress reduction. Whereas noise pollution, vehicle traffic, and crowdedness were the most critical factors of stress. Finally, our findings suggest that the component of green space has a more significant effect on stress reduction compared with the elimination of vehicle traffic.
Article
The use of virtual reality (VR) stimulation in clinical settings has increased in recent years. In particular, there has been increasing interest in the use of VR stimulation for a variety of purposes, including medical training, pain therapy, and relaxation. Unfortunately, there is still a limited amount of real-world 360-degree content that is both available and suitable for these applications. Therefore, this tutorial paper describes a pipeline for the creation of custom VR content. It covers the planning and designing of content; the selection of appropriate equipment; the creation and processing of footage; and the deployment, visualization, and evaluation of the VR experience. This paper aims to provide a set of guidelines, based on first-hand experience, that readers can use to help create their own 360-degree videos. By discussing and elaborating upon the challenges associated with making 360-degree content, this tutorial can help researchers and health care professionals anticipate and avoid common pitfalls during their own content creation process.
Article
Full-text available
Green space matters for mental health but is under constant pressure in an increasingly urbanising world. Often there is little space available in cities for green areas, so it is vital to optimise the design and usage of these available green spaces. To achieve this, experts in planning, design and nature conservation need to know which types and characteristics of green spaces are most beneficial for residents' mental health. A scoping review of studies that compare different green space types and characteristics on mental health was conducted. A total of 215 (experimental, observational and qualitative) papers were included in the scoping review. This review highlights a high level of heterogeneity in study design, geographical locations, mental health outcomes and green space measures. Few of the included studies were specifically designed to enable direct comparisons between green space types and characteristics (e.g. between parks and forests). The included studies have predominantly experimental research designs looking at the effects of short‐term exposure to green space on short‐term mental health outcomes (e.g. affect and physiological stress). More studies enabled only indirect comparisons, either within the same study or between different studies. Analysis of the direction of the mental health outcomes (positive, neutral, negative) from exposure to various types and characteristics of green space found positive (i.e. beneficial) effects across all green space types. However, green space characteristics did appear to render more diverse effects on mental health, which is especially the case for vegetation characteristics (e.g. higher vegetation density can be negative for mental health). The scoping review reveals gaps in the present evidence base, with a specific need for more studies directly comparing green space types and characteristics within the same study. Proposed future research directions include the use of longitudinal research designs focusing on green space characteristics, considering actual exposure and systematically addressing heterogeneity in factors influencing the relation between green spaces and mental health (e.g. type of interaction, user experience). Read the free Plain Language Summary for this article on the Journal blog.
Article
Streets are important public spaces in daily life, and their stress-relieving abilities help to improve people’s physical and mental health. To investigate the effects of different types of street vegetation on people’s stress recovery, this study used virtual reality technology to establish five street scenes with different vegetation types, including a non-vegetated street, a street with trees, a street with trees and grass, a street with trees and hedges, and a street with trees, grass, and flowers. Twenty-four participants completed the Trier Social Stress Test and then watched the five street scenes for stress recovery. Participants rated the vegetated streets as significantly better at reducing stress than the non-vegetated street. Compared with the non-vegetated street, the participants’ POMS scores decreased by 2.59–12.09 and ROS scores increased by 0.83–3.67 after watching the vegetated streets, indicating significant improvement in mood (P < 0.001). HRV data showed that the combination of trees, grass, and flowers was the most effective for stress recovery (LF/HF = 0.67 ± 0.42; pNN50 = 27.41% ± 16.32%). EEG data showed that participants’ alpha power was 0.05–0.66 µV2 higher and mental stress scores based on brainwave power were 0.23–0.37 points lower in the vegetated streets than in the non-vegetated street. The occipital and frontal regions showed the most positive responses to changes in vegetation elements, and alpha brainwaves in the O2 channel were the most active. Therefore, the streets with vegetation were more conducive to stress recovery than the non-vegetated street. It is thus suggested to integrate trees, grass, flowers, and other vegetation types along streets.
Article
Full-text available
Growing evidence suggests that close contact with nature brings benefits to human health and well-being, but the proposed mechanisms are still not well understood and the associations with health remain uncertain. The Positive Health Effects of the Natural Outdoor environment in Typical Populations in different regions in Europe (PHENOTYPE) project investigates the interconnections between natural outdoor environments and better human health and well-being. The PHENOTYPE project explores the proposed underlying mechanisms at work (stress reduction/restorative function, physical activity, social interaction, exposure to environmental hazards) and examines the associations with health outcomes for different population groups. It implements conventional and new innovative high-tech methods to characterise the natural environment in terms of quality and quantity. Preventive as well as therapeutic effects of contact with the natural environment are being covered. PHENOTYPE further addresses implications for land-use planning and green space management. The main innovative part of the study is the evaluation of possible short-term and long-term associations of green space and health and the possible underlying mechanisms in four different countries (each with quite a different type of green space and a different use), using the same methodology, in one research programme. This type of holistic approach has not been undertaken before. Furthermore there are technological innovations such as the use of remote sensing and smartphones in the assessment of green space. The project will produce a more robust evidence base on links between exposure to natural outdoor environment and human health and well-being, in addition to a better integration of human health needs into land-use planning and green space management in rural as well as urban areas.
Article
The regulation of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis has received empirical attention as a mechanism contributing to individual differences in health and human development. A variety of sampling tactics and strategies index daily HPA axis functioning including the cortisol awakening response (CAR), the diurnal slope, and the area under the curve (AUGg). In an ethnically diverse sample (54% European-American, 23% Latino) of 82 adolescents (24% male, M age=18.05 years), we assessed salivary cortisol 45 times over the transition to college: 5 times per day, over 3 sequential days, across 3 waves (initially, 5, and 9 months later). Samples were collected at waking; 30min, 3, and 8h post waking; and bedtime. Latent state-trait modeling indicated that the waking and 30min post waking samples contributed to indices of within and across wave latent trait cortisol (LTC) levels. As such, a latent trait factor of cortisol was derived to reflect both within- and across-wave trait components of the variance in cortisol. LTC was distinct from the CAR, differentially predicted components of the diurnal profile across the day, and was highly stable across assessment waves (months). As preliminary evidence for convergent validity of LTC levels, childhood trauma was positively associated with LTC. Findings document the reliability, divergent and convergent validity, and stability of a latent trait factor of individual differences in HPA axis activity that may provide a cost efficient alternative to existing strategies and minimize participant burden. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Article
Urbanization, resource exploitation, and lifestyle changes have diminished possibilities for human contact with nature in urbanized societies. Concern about the loss has helped motivate research on the health benefits of contact with nature. Reviewing that research here, we focus on nature as represented by aspects of the physical environment relevant to planning, design, and policy measures that serve broad segments of urbanized societies. We discuss difficulties in defining "nature" and reasons for the current expansion of the research field, and we assess available reviews. We then consider research on pathways between nature and health involving air quality, physical activity, social cohesion, and stress reduction. Finally, we discuss methodological issues and priorities for future research. The extant research does describe an array of benefits of contact with nature, and evidence regarding some benefits is strong; however, some findings indicate caution is needed in applying beliefs about those benefits, and substantial gaps in knowledge remain. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Public Health Volume 35 is March 18, 2014. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/catalog/pubdates.aspx for revised estimates.