ArticlePDF Available

Successfully leaving work at work: The self-regulatory underpinnings of psychological detachment

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Detaching from work – defined as mentally and physically disengaging from work during off‐hours – is an important prerequisite to effective daily recovery and psychological well‐being. However, the extant literature has yet to articulate exactly why some employees fail to detach from work and, furthermore, offers few concrete recommendations on how to increase detachment on a daily basis. I illustrate how both of these limitations may be resolved by extending the definition of psychological detachment to more clearly specify from what employees are failing to detach. Drawing from self‐regulation research, the theoretical framework developed in this study proposes that employees' minds continue to linger over goal‐related content after the workday is finished. This proposition was supported in a longitudinal sample of 103 employees pursuing 1,127 goals. Consistent with a self‐regulatory perspective, employees had more difficulty detaching from incomplete (vs. completed) work goals later in the day, especially when these goals possessed high valence. Furthermore, an experimental manipulation demonstrated that creating plans to resolve incomplete goals increased psychological detachment among employees with traits that chronically inhibit detachment. I discuss how this refined conceptualization of psychological detachment catalyses future theoretical development and provides groundwork for evidence‐based interventions. Practitioner points Creating plans at the end of the day that describe where, when, and how unfulfilled work goals will be completed is an effective, low‐cost intervention that enhances psychological detachment among employees, which will ultimately improve occupational health and performance. The planning intervention was primarily effective among employees who typically have difficulty detaching from work during leisure time, indicating that intervention efforts should be targeted at specific types of employees. When setting daily work goals, employees should be encouraged to focus on smaller, concrete goals at the end of the day in order to reduce unfulfilled work goals and facilitate psychological detachment.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology (2015)
©2015 The British Psychological Society
www.wileyonlinelibrary.com
Successfully leaving work at work: The
self-regulatory underpinnings of psychological
detachment
Brandon W. Smit*
Department of Psychological Sciences, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana, USA
Detaching from work defined as mentally and physically disengaging from work during
off-hours is an important prerequisite to effective daily recovery and psychological well-
being. However, the extant literature has yet to articulate exactly why some employees
fail to detach from work and, furthermore, offers few concrete recommendations on how
to increase detachment on a daily basis. I illustrate how both of these limitations may be
resolved by extending the definition of psychological detachment to more clearly specify
from what employees are failing to detach. Drawing from self-regulation research, the
theoretical framework developed in this study proposes that employees’ minds continue
to linger over goal-related content after the workday is finished. This proposition was
supported in a longitudinal sample of 103 employees pursuing 1,127 goals. Consistent
with a self-regulatory perspective, employees had more difficulty detaching from
incomplete (vs. completed) work goals later in the day, especially when these goals
possessed high valence. Furthermore, an experimental manipulation demonstrated that
creating plans to resolve incomplete goals increased psychological detachment among
employees with traits that chronically inhibit detachment. I discuss how this refined
conceptualization of psychological detachment catalyses future theoretical development
and provides groundwork for evidence-based interventions.
Practitioner points
Creating plans at the end of the day that describe where, when, and how unfulfilled work goals will be
completed is an effective, low-cost intervention that enhances psychological detachment among
employees, which will ultimately improve occupational health and performance.
The planning intervention was primarily effective among employees who typically have difficulty
detaching from work during leisure time, indicating that intervention efforts should be targeted at
specific types of employees.
When setting daily work goals, employees should be encouraged to focus on smaller, concrete goals at
the end of the day in order to reduce unfulfilled work goals and facilitate psychological detachment.
Although most individuals transition between the work and home domains on a daily
basis, they do not necessarily mentally disengage from their professional role simply
because the workday has ended. Work-related issues have the ability to linger on
employees’ minds and, in some cases, prevent their ability to enjoy leisure and family time.
In fact, 72% of workers report spending some time after work worrying about their job,
*Correspondence should be addressed to Brandon W. Smit, Department of Psychological Sciences, Ball State University, North
Quad Building, Muncie, IN 47306, USA (email: smitbw@gmail.com).
DOI:10.1111/joop.12137
1
and 22% report that they worry regularly (Gallie, White, Cheng, & Tomlinson, 1998). In
the last two decades, the burgeoning literature on psychological detachment defined as
the extent to which an individual disengages from work-related matters during leisure
time has illustrated that mentally and physically detaching from work during off-hours
improves occupational health and performance among employees. Several daily-diary
studies have illustrated the positive effects of detachment, such as increased positive
mood and reduced negative affective arousal and fatigue during leisure time (Sonnentag &
Bayer, 2005), as well as increased daily task performance, personal initiative, and
organizational citizenship behaviour at work (Binnewies, Sonnentag, & Mojza, 2010).
While it is clear that psychological detachment benefits employees and organizations,
important questions about psychological detachment itself have yet to be addressed. First,
it remains unclear exactly why some individuals are unable to psychologically detach from
work. That is, why do our thoughts continue to linger during our leisure time on
upcoming deadlines, inept supervisors, and last year’s performance reviews? Little
research to date has formally articulated or tested the theoretical mechanisms that explain
why some employees continue to linger over work-related content during their leisure
hours, while others do not. As a result, it is unclear why different antecedents in this
literature shape detachment. For example, job involvement decreases detachment, while
a preference for segmentation increases detachment (Sonnentag, 2012); however, are
these effects each indicators of a common underlying mechanism, or do they reflect
distinct pathways to detachment? Second, what can individuals actually do to facilitate
experiencing psychological detachment themselves? Without a clear understanding of
the mechanisms driving detachment, intervention efforts aimed towards promoting
recovery will be impeded because it is unclear what the intervention should target.
Although the extant literature has explored a handful of solutions (e.g., joint partner
activities during weekends, Hahn, Binnewies, & Haun, 2012; mindfulness, H
ulsheger
et al., 2014; volunteering, Mojza, Lorenz, Sonnentag, & Binnewies, 2010), these practical
recommendations for increasing detachment have yet to be experimentally tested, and
furthermore, many may not lend themselves to widespread implementation on a daily
basis.
This study illustrates how these limitations can be successfully addressed by
expanding the definition of detachment to develop more conceptual nuance. Psycho-
logical detachment is currently defined as ‘both refraining from job-related activities and
not thinking about job-related issues during off-job time’ (p. 114; Sonnentag, 2012). This
definition, however, does not clearly explicate exactly what individuals are detaching
from during off-job time. I argue that goal-related content is one significant dimension of
these ‘job-related’ issues. Goals are theoretically relevant to psychological detachment
because of the varied and powerful ways they shape human cognition (Fӧrster, Liberman,
& Friedman, 2007). For example, research on the Zeigarnik (1927) effect documents how
unfulfilled goals continue to capture attentional resources until the goal has been either
completed or abandoned, which may have important implications for employees’ ability
to detach.
Given that goals have a unique ability to substantively capture attention (Fӧrster et al.,
2007), detachment may be, in part, conceptualized as a goal-oriented state. This
expansion of the detachment construct highlights the possibility that a state of low
detachment may be multifaceted and consist of a variety of different types of thoughts.
Using this revised conceptualization, I draw on self-regulation theory to develop a
theoretical framework that describes the mechanisms through which goals shape
detachment. Specifically, I hypothesize that goal accessibility defined as the extent to
2Brandon W. Smit
which goals are readily available in consciousness (Kruglanski, 1996) is one mechanism
determining the extent to which employees successfully detach from work. To continue
building evidence-based practices for increasing detachment, I leverage previous
laboratory research to experimentally test the efficacy of an intervention. I hypothesize
that creating plans, in the form of implementation intentions (Gollwitzer, 1999), will help
individuals detach from work-related goals. Previous research indicates that deciding
where, when, and how to resolve unfulfilled goals will reduce their accessibility
(Masicampo & Baumeister, 2011a) and, within this framework, increase psychological
detachment.
This study makes at least three substantive contributions to the detachment literature.
First, the revised definition of psychological detachment developed here simultaneously
contributes to construct clarity and challenges our existing assumptions about
detachment. Conceptualizing detachment as a multifaceted construct adds nuance to
our understanding of this phenomenon and represents an important first step in
developing a more comprehensive understanding of what employees fail to detach from
during leisure time. Furthermore, this study highlights the possibility that a low level of
detachment may not be a monolithic state, but rather a more complex aggregation of goal-
related (and likely other) thoughts. Capturing detachment from specific goals represents
an innovative measurement approach that prompts questions about the assessment of
detachment.
Second, this study provides researchers with a new theo retical lens to explore why and
how employees fail to detach from work. Although previous research consistently finds
that goal completion reduces goal accessibility (Johnson, Chang, & Lord, 2006), these
findings have been largely confined to laboratory settings. This study replicates these
findings in an applied context and, furthermore, extends and integrates these proposi-
tions with the psychological detachment literature. Leveraging self-regulation theory not
only allows scholars to reinterpret existing research, but catalyses future theoretical
development. This framework reveals the complex ways that workplace events shape
detachment by introducing goal accessibility as a mechanism in determining employees’
detachment. This is important, because a clearer understanding of the theoretical
mechanisms that drive detachment can help scholars expand the nomological network of
psychological detachment, and help practitioners utilize this knowledge to develop
interventions that can facilitate detachment.
Lastly, this study provides supportive evidence for a specific, effective remedy for
employees who have difficulty detaching from work. This is important, because the
extant literature provides few concrete recommendations for facilitating detachment
(c.f., Hahn et al., 2012; H
ulsheger et al., 2014; Mojza et al., 2010). This study illustrates
that creating specific plans for how to complete unfulfilled tasks frees employees’
attention to be directed towards other matters. Given how often employees transition
between work and home roles, this simple, low-cost method has the potential to improve
occupational health while preserving, or even improving, job performance on a daily
basis.
Psychological detachment from work-related goals
Stemming from the burgeoning literature on recovery experiences from job stressors, the
term detachment was originally defined by Etzion, Eden, and Lapidot (1998) as an
‘individual’s sense of being away from the work situation’ (p. 579). Recently,
psychological detachment has been defined more precisely as ‘both refraining from
Successfully leaving work at work 3
job-related activities and not thinking about job-related issues during off-job time’ (p. 114;
Sonnentag, 2012). Thus, full psychological detachment from work implies that an
individual not only avoids work behaviours such as reading work-related materials or
taking work-related phone calls, but simultaneously, and perhaps most importantly, he or
she avoids thinking about work-related content, such as mentally preparing for a future
presentation. Absent in this definition, however, is a clear conceptualization of what it is
that employees actually leave behind at work. Although the content of ‘job-related issues’
that follow employees home may be vast, goal-related topics may account for a portion of
these thoughts, among other dimensions.
Goals are defined as mental representations of end states that guide behaviour, as
well as shape emotions and evaluations (Austin & Vancouver, 1996). Although
extensive research has been devoted to exploring how the structure and content of
goals impact behaviour (Locke & Latham, 2002), given their inherently cognitive
structure as mental representations, an equally important facet of goals is how they
operate in the context of human cognition (Kruglanski et al., 2002). As cognitive
representations, goals are an example of a knowledge structure. As a result, goals have a
number of important characteristics that will determine where, when, and how they
come to mind. First, like any other cognitive knowledge structure, the extent to which a
goal is accessible in memory will vary across time and circumstances (Kruglanski, 1996).
In the same way that we know the plot of our favourite novel, goal knowledge
structures are stored in memory, and not necessarily immediately accessible in working
memory at any given moment. Second, goals as cognitive structures are nested within a
wider associative network of other constructs in memory. Constructs that are
conceptually more similar, and thus frequently co-activated, are more closely linked
together in the network. When a given construct is activated, closely related constructs
in the associative network also become activated and more accessible through well-
studied excitatory and inhibitory ‘spreading of activation’ mechanisms (Anderson, 1983;
Lord & Levy, 1994).
Goal pursuit is facilitated by cognition in two main ways. First, in what can be called
activation potential (Higgins, 1996), priming a goal does, in fact, prime closely related
information in that category, where this information is both accessed more quickly and is
more likely to be accessed overall (Goschke & Kuhl, 1993; Johnson et al., 2006). The
functional view of goal accessibility argues that prior to goal completion, goal-related
constructs are prioritized in cognitive processing, and individuals experience heightened
goal accessibility to facilitate the completion of the focal goal. Second, in what has been
coined post-fulfilment inhibition, when a goal is accomplished, it and its associative
network become deactivated and less accessible (Marsh, Hicks, & Bink, 1998). From the
functional view, by inhibiting now-irrelevant goal constructs, individuals are able to divert
attention to new goals rather than succumbing to maladaptive lingering on finished
business. Given that so many mental processes align and coordinate in ways that promote
focus on a goal, an important question to address is: What happens when goals are left
incomplete?
Although the cognitive processes that promote goal focus during goal pursuit are no
doubt an important prerequisite to goal accomplishment, these same mechanisms can be
counterproductive in some circumstances. These cognitive mechanisms would hypo-
thetically work perfectly when goals could be pursued from beginning to end in a single
performance episode, and each goal could be pursued consecutively. In everyday life,
however, individuals create complex networks of goals that are pursued in tandem, and
vary in hierarchical level, interconnectedness, and scope (Austin & Vancouver, 1996).
4Brandon W. Smit
When ambitious goals require more work than can be accomplished in one sitting,
individuals are forced to table and resume goals iteratively over the course of days, weeks,
months, or even years. Given that human cognition is strongly biased towards goal
completion once a goal has been activated (Fӧrster et al., 2007), problems can ensue
when goal pursuit is tabled.
When goals remain unfulfilled, these focal goals continue to be accessible because the
mind presses towards goal completion to alleviate ‘goal tension’ (Lewin, 1951). Evidence
shows that participants better remember tasks that were interrupted rather than
completed (Zeigarnik effect; Marsh et al., 1998; Zeigarnik, 1927). This suggests
unfulfilled goals remain active and accessible, while goal fulfilment reduces accessibility
(i.e., post-fulfilment inhibition). As a consequence, goals that remain accessible, but are
not being currently pursued, can split attention and create distractions that disrupt
activities in other domains. For example, individuals may have difficulty disengaging from
a work mindset and/or difficulty engaging in other roles due to continued work-goal
accessibility.
Applying the tenets of goal accessibility to psychological detachment would suggest
that when employees leave work before completing a given goal, the goal will remain
highly accessible. This heightened goal accessibility caused by unfulfilled goals will
continue to capture attentional resources even after an employee returns home for the
evening (i.e., Zeigarnik effect; Zeigarnik, 1927). When work goals remain accessible at
home, they can be pushed into conscious awareness when given the opportunity (e.g., ‘I
need to finish tomorrow’s lecture’; Bongers, Dijksterhuis, & Spears, 2010), thereby
inhibiting detachment. Given that psychological detachment has been described as the
experience of ‘switching off’ and leaving work behind both physically and mentally,
continued activation of unresolved goals is clearly contrary to this state. Conversely,
when goals are completed, post-fulfilment inhibition occurs and reduces goal accessi-
bility.
Hypothesis 1: Individuals experience less goal-level detachment from incomplete goals than from
completed goals.
Not all goals are created equal, and consequently, there will be variability with respect
to how easily individuals are able to detach from different goals. A widely supported
proposition in the goal accessibility literature is that accessibility is proportional to the
motivational strength of a goal (Fӧrster et al., 2007). In general, a self-regulatory
perspective on psychological detachment would predict individuals will have more
difficulty detaching from goals with a stronger ‘motivational pull’. When goals have higher
valence, they will be more motivational and accessible than goals with lower valence
(F
orster, Liberman, & Higgins, 2005). There is a functional reason why more motivating
goals are more easily accessible. Accessibility facilitates goal pursuit by making individuals
more sensitive to goal-related cues in the environment, and making goal-related
information more easily retrievable to facilitate decision-making. Given that people have
limited resources and pursue multiple goals at once, the ability to focus cognitive
resources on goals that will maximize personal benefit is highly adaptive (Fӧrster et al.,
2007). Previous experimental laboratory research has observed that valence enhances
goal accessibility (F
orster et al., 2005).
Hypothesis 2: Goal valence moderates the relationship between goal type (i.e., incomplete vs.
complete) and goal-level detachment, where goal valence is negatively related to
goal-level detachment among incomplete goals.
Successfully leaving work at work 5
Global versus goal-level psychological detachment
To be clear, this revised conceptualization complements existing notions of psychological
detachment as measured in previous research (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007), referred to here
as global detachment, rather than replaces it. Given that global detachment is largely
conceptualized as the extent to which work-related thoughts linger in employees’ minds,
goal-level detachment represents a facet of global detachment by exclusively focusing on
goal-related thoughts. It is important to note that while goal-related content should
capture a wide array of work-related stimuli, other dimensions may also contribute to an
individual’s sense of detachment. For example, the justice literature illustrates the
ubiquitous value employees place on fairness (Colquitt & Rodell, 2011). Therefore,
another dimension that may shape detachment could be fairness-related events in the
workplace. Although global indices of detachment (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007) have the
advantage of comprehensively assessing an individual’s sense of detachment from work,
they also risk the possibility of conflating qualitatively different types of content under a
single label (e.g., task-focused thoughts vs. interpersonal-focused thoughts). Existing
interventions target global detachment, but neglect the possibility that detachment is
multifaceted. Given that detachment is likely multifaceted, not all interventions will be
equally relevant or effective for all employees. Thus, one purpose of this study was to
illustrate that psychological detachment may not be a monolithic state and there may be
advantages of using a more nuanced conceptualization of detachment.
Planning facilitates psychological detachment
The second broad goal of this study was to explore interventions that may help individuals
increase psychological detachment during off-work hours. While the extant literature has
explored a handful of specific, practical solutions such as volunteering (Mojza et al.,
2010), engaging in absorbing joint partner activities during weekends (Hahn et al., 2012),
and practicing mindfulness (H
ulsheger et al., 2014), these recommendations have several
limitations. Foremost is that they have only been supported using correlational designs.
Furthermore, not all employees have significant others, or the time and opportunity to
devote to volunteer work. Additionally, the nature of the joint activities that enhance
recovery has not been clearly explicated, and these effects were limited to weekends,
rather than on a daily basis. Only one study, to the author’s knowledge, has experimentally
tested the efficacy of an intervention to increase detachment (Hahn, Binnewies,
Sonnentag, & Mojza, 2011). Although effective, this intervention implemented a variety
of behavioural strategies for separating work and non-work domains, which makes it
difficult to draw firm conclusions regarding what specific strategies improve detachment.
Using a self-regulatory perspective, I leverage goals research to develop hypotheses about
a simple method that may have the ability to enhance detachment on a daily basis:
Planning.
Although the planning literature spans multiple disciplines and perspectives (Lapierre
& Allen, 2012), I focus specifically on implementation intentions (Gollwitzer, 1999) for
three reasons: (1) it has been replicated in both laboratory and field settings, (2) it has
accumulated decades of empirical support and its major propositions have been
supported with meta-analytic tests, and (3) most importantly, it is uniquely complemen-
tary to a self-regulatory perspective on psychological detachment because it addresses
goal accessibility (Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006).
Implementation intentions are plans that specify the where, when, and how of
behavioural goal pursuit (Gollwitzer, 1993). They are structured as, ‘When situation x
6Brandon W. Smit
arises, I will perform response y!’ In this way, implementation intentions link goal pursuit
to a specific situation (e.g., ‘when I finish working for the day’) and a specific behavioural
response (e.g., ‘I will drive to the gym and run for 30 min’). Implementation intentions
facilitate goal pursuit through several mechanisms. Implementation intentions remove
the ambiguity of how specific goals should be acted upon by specifying the behavioural
response to a situational prompt (‘... I will perform response y!’). Additionally, by
specifying the situational conditions that will prompt goal pursuit (i.e., ‘When situation x
arises...’), implementation intentions both provide reminder prompts to work on a goal,
and remove the ambiguity of when specific goals should be acted on. In one study on
preventative health behaviours, 47% of female participants forming the simple intention
to perform a breast self-examination failed to do so, citing ‘forgetting’ as the primary
culprit. The success rate among women with implementation intentions was 100%
(Orbell, Hodgkins, & Sheeran, 1997).
Implementation intentions may also facilitate psychological detachment through
these same mechanisms. When the situational cues to pursue goals are absent, especially
in salient cases like ‘When I am in the office...’, the cognitive processes that press for goal
completion should become relaxed because they are no longer necessary. Implementa-
tion intentions reduce the accessibility of unfinished work goals, because these goals have
been resolved, in a sense. Experimental studies have directly tested and supported this
proposition (Masicampo & Baumeister, 2011a,b). In one set of studies, participants
assigned implementation intentions were less likely to experience intrusive thoughts of
unfulfilled goals during an unrelated task (Study 1) and experienced reduced unfulfilled
goal accessibility and goal means accessibility (Study 2, Study 3; Masicampo & Baumeister,
2011a). Importantly, several alternative explanations for these effects were ruled out in
these experiments, including the ideas that planning reduces anxiety and creates a sense
of goal progress in individuals’ minds.
I hypothesize that forming implementation intentions will reduce the accessibility of
unfulfilled goals at the end of the workday. When individuals experience daily goal events
that would normally increase goal accessibility (e.g., unfulfilled goals), end-of-day planning
will buffer these individuals from goal accessibility and increase psychological detachment.
Hypothesis 3: End-of-day planning moderates the relationship between goal type (i.e., incomplete
vs. complete) and goal-level detachment, where planning will increase detachment
from incomplete goals.
Boundary conditions on planning effects
Although theory and previous research indicate planning should be an effective
intervention for increasing detachment, this intervention may not be equally relevant
to all employees. While many employees experience thoughts of work while at home,
there is between-subjects variability in this phenomenon. As a result, employees with
individual differences that predispose them to reduced detachment may benefit more
from the planning intervention than employees who normally experience high levels of
detachment. In this study, I explore how job involvement may impact the efficacy of the
planning intervention.
Job involvement reflects the extent to which a person’s job plays a central role in his or
her life. Previous research finds that increased job involvement reduces detachment
(K
uhnel, Sonnentag, & Westman, 2009). From a self-regulatory perspective, identity will
shape priorities, and as a consequence, an individual’s goal hierarchy (Carver & Scheier,
Successfully leaving work at work 7
1998). Goals that have implications for the self are typically abstract, high-level goals that
can be flexibly accomplished through different means. Furthermore, the value of mid-
level action goals may be determined according to how well they satisfy these high-level
goals (e.g., needs, values; Lord, Diefendorff, Schmidt, & Hall, 2010). In a top-down
mechanism, high-level goals will not only cause an individual to perceive and seek more
ways to satisfy them, but will also make the goals themselves more sensitive to activation.
As a result, individuals with greater job involvement may experience less detachment
cumulatively, because the structure of their goal hierarchy increases the likelihood they
will cumulatively experience more work-goal accessibility. Consequently, individuals
with high job involvement may experience more benefit from creating plans.
Hypothesis 4: The interaction between goal type (i.e., incomplete vs. complete) and planning will
be stronger among employees high in job involvement, where planning will increase
goal-level detachment from incomplete goals.
Finally, this study examined the efficacy of the planning manipulation in predicting
global levels of detachment. As noted earlier, goals represent one content domain that
may shape global detachment, and consequently, represent one facet of global
detachment rather than a substitute. If goals do represent one substantive domain of
thoughts that impair effective detachment, the planning intervention should improve
daily levels of global detachment, as well as detachment from specific goals. Extending the
rationale above, this study explores how the planning manipulation differentially affects
detachment for workers with varying levels of job involvement, given that not all
employees experience problems detaching from work.
Hypothesis 5: Job involvement moderates the relationship between planning and global
detachment, where planning will eliminate the significant differences in global
detachment between employees with low and high job involvement.
Method
Sample and procedure
Participants were recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk), an online forum
used to solicit workers to complete tasks in exchange for compensation. Previous
research has found that MTurk workers are generally representative of the larger US
population, and the quality of data gathered from this source is equivalent to other sources
(Behrend, Sharek, Meade, & Wiebe, 2011; Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011).
Respondents who listed MTurk participation as their primary occupation were not
eligible for this study. Participants were compensated $7.00. On the first day of data
collection, participants filled out a single between-subjects online survey that assessed
demographic information and personality traits. Additionally, participants selected
specific days and times to complete the daily online surveys (i.e., one survey immediately
after work and one survey before going to sleep for the night on each day) in order to
accommodate unique schedules. Submitted surveys were tracked with timestamps to
ensure the surveys were submitted at the appropriate days and times. After completing
the first survey, participants began a schedule where they filled out two online surveys per
day at the times they selected for five working days. The after-work survey collected
information on (1) daily unfulfilled goals, (2) daily goal successes, (3) goal valence, and (4)
daily workload. To collect information on goals, participants first listed any goals they did
8Brandon W. Smit
not complete that day, and then rated the valence of each goal. This sequence was
repeated for any successfully completed goals listed by the participant. The before-bed
survey was the second daily survey that participants completed just prior to going to sleep.
This survey collected information on (1) goal-level psychological detachment, (2) daily
global psychological detachment (3) daily relaxation, and (4) state negative affect.
A total of 103 individuals participated in the study. These participants completed an
average of 3.3 days of surveys. Participants provided data on a total of 1,127 goals, with an
average of 10.9 goals per participant. On average, participants were 34.6 years old
(SD =10) and 60% female. While 57% of participants reported having no children, 43%
reported having one or more. Among all participants, 34% reported they had at least one
child living at home with them. Additionally, 42.7% of participants reported that they
never work from home, while 57.3% either occasionally work from home or work from
home most of the time.
1
With regard to work status, 71% of participants worked full-time
(40 hr or more per week), and no participants were unemployed as per the screening
criteria. Finally, 47% of participants were single, 47% were married, and 6% were divorced .
Experimental manipulation
One experimental manipulation was used in this study. Participants were randomly
assigned into one of two groups, the planning condition (N=46) or the control condition
(N=57). Participants in the planning condition created concrete plans at the end of the
after-work survey according to a set of guidelines outlined in the instructions. Using the
guidelines from a previous study, participants were instructed to create a plan for (1)
where, (2) when, and (3) how they will accomplish each of their unfulfilled goals from that
day (Masicampo & Baumeister, 2011a). For example, a credit analyst listed the incomplete
goal ‘Call back customers about financial disputes’, and subsequently wrote the plan, ‘I
will go into work and start at 10:00 AM in a call center in my office. Log into my computer
and call customers back on the multi-line phone to inquire more information about the
disputes. After a thorough investigation and verifying evidence, make a final decision and
send correspondence to the customer’. Participants in the control condition completed
the exact same scales as participants in the planning condition, except for the planning
task. To ensure the manipulation was effective, the qualitative data were validated by two
raters to ensure participants created plans according to the objective guidelines (i.e.,
specifying where, when, and how). There was 99.1% agreement between the two raters
for whether plans were acceptable or not. Disagreements were resolved by referencing
the guidelines provided to participants.
Measures
Goal type (complete vs. incomplete)
Participants responded to prompts during the after-work survey to list and rate up to four
incomplete and four completed work goals from that workday. The prompts provided
instructions and these guidelines on what kinds of goals to list: (1) goals that are work-
related tasks, rather than personal; (2) specific goals, rather than broad, vague ones; (3)
1
Given the high proportion of participants who work from home, exploratory analyses were used to confirm that there were no
statistically significant differences in the two dependent variables used in this study (i.e., goal-level detachment and global
detachment) for those who worked from home and those who do not. Additionally, all hypothesis tests control for working from
home.
Successfully leaving work at work 9
short-term goals that could be finished in a week or less if possible; (4) goals you have
control over completing; and (5) rather than listing large projects, focus on the next tasks
that must be performed. These goals were coded dichotomously, where completed goals
were assigned a 0, and incomplete goals were assigned a 1.
Goal valence
Goal valence reflects participants’ anticipated satisfaction from goal completion (Mento,
Locke, & Klein, 1992). Valence was measured with a 2-item scale (a=.95), and
participants rated each goal they listed with this scale. The items were as follows: ‘How
will you feel after completing your goal: [customized goal text]’ and ‘To what extent will
you feel satisfaction from completing your goal?’ Consistent with previous research, these
items were selected to emphasize the attractiveness of the goal outcome (Mento et al.,
1992). Participants rated these items on a 5-point Likert-type scale, from Very Dissatisfied
(1) to Very Satisfied (5).
Psychological detachment
Detachment reflects the extent to which individuals psychologically distance themselves
from work during off-time hours. Daily levels of global detachment (a=.90) were
measured using the 4-item detachment subscale from the Recovery Experiences
Questionnaire (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007). Two sample items include the following: ‘I
forgot about work’ and ‘I didn’t think about work at all’. Thus, scores on the daily global
detachment scale reflect the extent to which a participant thought about work in general
during their leisure time on a given day. Goal-level detachments (a=.91) were measured
using the same scale with slight modifications to focus the item content on individual
goals, rather than work more broadly. Participants completed the modified 4-item scale in
the before-bed survey for each individual goal listed by them in the after-work survey
earlier in the day. Two sample items include the following: ‘With regard to your
(in)complete goal earlier today at work: [customized goal text]...’, ‘I forgot all about it’
and ‘I didn’t think about this goal at all’. Thus, scores on the goal-level detachment scale
reflect the extent to which a participant thought about a specific work goal during their
leisure time on a given day. Participants responded on a 5-point Likert scale for both
measures, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
Job involvement
Job involvement reflects the extent to which an individual’s job plays a central role in his
or her life. Job involvement (a=.90) was measured with a 7-item scale modified from
Kanungo (1982). A sample item includes the following: ‘The most important things that
happen to me involve my present job’. Participants responded on a 7-point Likert scale,
ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Control variables
I controlled for a variety of factors that previous research has linked to detachment,
including having children living at home (Hahn & Dormann, 2013), workload (Sonnentag,
Kuttler, & Fritz, 2010), neuroticism (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007), segmentation preferences
(Park, Fritz, & Jex, 2011), negative affect (Sonnentag, Binnewies, & Mojza, 2008), and
10 Brandon W. Smit
relaxation (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007).
2
State negative affect (a=.93) was measured with a
10-item scale (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Participants rated these emotion
descriptor items on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 5 (Very much).
Daily relaxation (a=.97) was measured with a 4-item scale developed by Sonnentag and
Fritz (2007). Daily workload (a=.84) was measured with an abbreviated 6-item scale
developed by Ilies et al. (2007; modified from Janssen, 2001). Participants rated the
relaxation and workload items on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Neuroticism (a=.94) was measured with a 10-item scale
derived from the International Personality Item Pool (Goldberg, 1999). Segmentation
preferences (a=.95) were measured with a 4-item scale developed by Kreiner (2006). To
control for any pre-existing individual differences in habitual planning behaviour,
participants answered a brief set of questions regarding whether and how they normally
plan on a day-to-day basis. Current planning behaviours (a=.90) were measured with the
problem-solving rumination scale (Cropley, Michalianou, Pravettoni, & Millward, 2012). A
sample item includes the following: ‘I find solutions to work-related problems in my free
time’. Participants responded to these individual differences scales on a 7-point Likert
scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Results
Preliminary analyses
First, I examined the extent to which the random assignment procedure was effective in
this study. To test for differences between the two experimental conditions, I conducted
nine independent-samples t-tests for the continuous variables in this study. For nine
comparisons, the probability of making a type I error would be equal to 37%. To control for
this inflated type I error rate, I used a Bonferroni correction to set the alpha level at .006.
The results indicated that there were no statistically significant differences between the
conditions for any of the variables included in this study, providing supportive evidence
that random assignment was effective. Additionally, I conducted chi-square analyses for
the categorical variables in this study (i.e., children at home, work from home). These
analyses each yielded non-significant differences for a conventional alpha level (a=.05),
which demonstrates these two variables were distributed in the same way across the two
experimental conditions.
To determine whether multilevel modelling is appropriate, intraclass correlations were
calculated with null empty-means models to estimate the proportion of between-subjects
variance in the outcome variable. With regard to goal-level detachment, approximately
16% of the variance existed at the day level (s=.15, p<.01), while 36% of the variance
existed at the person level (s=.34, p<.01). As a result, all analyses with goal-level
detachment as a dependent variable specified a 3-level hierarchical model where repeated
goal observations were nested within days, nested within individuals. The reliabilities and
intercorrelations among the study variables are listed in Table 1.
Hypothesis testing
Hypothesis 1 specified that daily unfulfilled goals, contrasted with daily goal successes,
would be associated with decreased psychological detachment. This hypothesis was
2
All hypotheses were tested controlling for negative affect and relaxation, as well as without these covariates, and the results were
equivalent.
Successfully leaving work at work 11
Table 1. Intercorrelations among study variables
MSD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 111213 14
1. Work from
home
0.57 0.50 ()
2. Children
at home
0.34 0.48 .00 ()
3. Job
involvement
3.24 1.26 .19* .04 (.90)
4. Segmentation
preferences
5.11 1.53 .34** .05 .63** (.95)
5. Neuroticism 3.40 1.37 .12 .11 .10 .01 (.94)
6. Planning
behaviours
4.06 1.40 .23* .04 .58** .59** .04 (.90)
7. Planning
manipulation
0.45 0.50 .03 .02 .06 .07 .10 .10 ()
8. Daily
workload
3.20 0.79 .07 .21* .05 .03 .05 .20* .20** (.84) .11 .16** .08**
9. Daily NA 1.49 0.64 .03 .12 .07 .01 .45** .13 .02 .14 (.93) .45** .24**
10. Daily
relaxation
3.68 0.85 .02 .03 .21* .04 .21* .07 .12 .19 .41** (.97) .32**
11. Daily global
detachment
3.71 0.90 .14 .13 .33** .22* .14 .31** .10 .29** .29** .57** (.90)
12. Goal type 0.66 0.19 .11 .02 .08 .05 .22* .07 .10 .22* .32** .22* .13 ().05** .20**
13. Goal valence 3.73 0.39 .04 .21* .07 .06 .15 .00 .04 .11 .32** .26** .23* .06 (.95) .13**
14. Goal-level
detachment
3.82 0.68 .06 .02 .28** .09 .19 .32** .14 .39** .27** .35** .58** .22* .18 (.91)
Notes. Work from Home: 0 =No, 1 =Yes; Children at Home: 0 =No, 1 =Yes. Goal Type: 0 = Complete, 1 = Incomplete; Between N= 96–103. Day-Level N=
320; Goal-Level N= 1,057–1,125.
*p<.05; **p<.01.
12 Brandon W. Smit
tested at the goal level, where goal-level detachment from individual unfulfilled goals and
successful goals was contrasted. A 3-level model was specified to control for both day- and
person-level intercept variance, along with the battery of day- and person-level control
variables (Table 2). It is interesting to note that the current planning behaviour covariate,
measured as problem-solving rumination, was actually negatively related to goal
detachment (c=.08, p<.05). Although previous research finds that planning should
reduce rumination, this negative relationship can likely be attributed to the fact that the
items for this scale emphasize problem-solving during leisure time, which is contradictory
to detachment. The results indicate that unfulfilled goals, in contrast to completed goals,
were associated with reduced goal-level psychological detachment (c=.28, p<.01;
pseudo R
2
=.22). Pseudo R
2
was calculated to create an index of effect size by calculating
the reduction in residual variance after adding hypothesized predictors. Given that effect
size calculation in multilevel modelling is still a debated topic (Nezlek, 2008), caution
should be used when interpreting these effect sizes. This pattern of results supports
hypothesis 1 and suggests that unfulfilled goals remain accessible to employees during off-
job time, consistent with the Zeigarnik effect.
Hypothesis 2 specified that goal valence would moderate the relationship between
goal type and detachment, where unfulfilled goals with higher valence are more strongly
related to goal-level detachment. Supporting hypothesis 2, an interaction term between
incomplete versus complete goals and goal valence was significant (c=.20, p<.01;
pseudo R
2
=.01). The simple slopes (Figure 1) suggest that regardless of goal type, goals
with low valence are unrelated to psychological detachment (c=.01, ns). However,
among goals with high valence, unfulfilled goals in contrast to completed goals were
associated with significantly reduced detachment (c=.21, p<.01). This pattern of
results is consistent with a self-regulatory perspective, and provides further evidence
suggesting that goal accessibility drives impairments in detachment.
Hypothesis 3 specified that the planning manipulation moderates the relationship
between goal type (incomplete vs. complete) and goal-level detachment, where the
differences in detachment due to goal type should be eliminated in the planning
Table 2. Test of Hypotheses 13
DV: Goal-level detachment
Variables Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE
Intercept 3.98** .07 3.98** .07 3.93** .08
Work from home 0.01 .09 0.01 .09 0.01 .08
Children at home 0.00 .08 0.01 .08 0.00 .08
Neuroticism 0.11** .03 0.11** .03 0.11** .03
Segmentation preferences 0.05 .04 0.05 .04 0.04 .04
Job involvement 0.14** .04 0.14** .04 0.14** .04
Planning behaviours 0.08* .04 0.08* .04 0.07* .04
Daily workload 0.08 .07 0.08 .07 0.08 .07
Goal type (1 =incomplete) 0.28** .04 0.21** .05 0.32** .06
Goal valence 0.01 .05
Planning manipulation 0.11 .08
Goal type 9Goal valence 0.20** .07
Goal type 9Planning 0.10 .09
Note.*p<.05; **p<.01.
Successfully leaving work at work 13
condition. The interaction between goal type and planning, although in the hypothesized
direction, was not statistically significant (c=.10, p=.25). As a result, I failed to find
support for hypothesis 3, which indicates the planning intervention may not be equally
effective for all employees.
To further explore this relationship, hypothesis 4 predicted that the planning
intervention would be most effective among employees with individual differences that
chronically reduce detachment. Hypothesis 4 specified a 3-way interaction between goal
type, planning condition, and job involvement, where the effect of planning for
incomplete goals would be most pronounced among employees high in job involvement.
The 3-way interaction term was added to the model (Table 3) and found to be statistically
significant (c=.18, p<.05; pseudo R
2
=.05). Consistent with hypothesis 4, the simple
slopes (Figure 2) suggested that among individuals with high job involvement, the
planning intervention was associated with increases in psychological detachment from
incomplete goals (c=.61, p<.01). As expected, the planning manipulation did not
2.5
3.0
3.5
4.0
4.5
5.0
Low valence High valence
Goal-level detachment
Complete goal
Incomplete goal
Figure 1. Interaction among goal type and goal valence.
Table 3. Test of Hypothesis 4
DV: Goal-level detachment
Variables Coefficient SE
Intercept 3.93** .08
Work from home 0.02 .08
Children at home 0.02 .08
Neuroticism 0.09** .03
Segmentation preferences 0.02 .04
Job involvement 0.11* .05
Planning behaviours 0.07 .04
Daily workload 0.08 .07
Goal type (1 =incomplete) 0.35** .05
Planning manipulation 0.14 .08
Goal type 9Planning 0.10 .09
Goal type 9Involvement 0.26** .04
Planning 9Involvement 0.12 .07
Goal type 9Planning 9Involvement 0.18* .08
Note.*p<.05; **p<.01.
14 Brandon W. Smit
impact detachment from completed (c=.01, ns) or incomplete (c=.13, ns) goals
among employees low in job involvement. This pattern of results supports the idea that
the planning intervention implemented in this study may only be effective among
employees who have chronic difficulties detaching from work.
Hypothesis 5 tested the efficacy of the planning intervention for improving global,
rather than simply goal-level, detachment (Table 4). Somewhat supportive of hypothesis
5, an interaction term between planning and job involvement was significant at the
p<.10 level (c=.19, p=.075; pseudo R
2
=.00). The simple slopes indicate that in the
control condition, individuals with high job involvement experience significantly less
detachment than those with low job involvement (c=.23, p<.01). However, when
individuals created plans at the end of the day, individuals with low and high job
involvement experienced similar levels of global daily detachment (c=.04, ns). Given
that planning exerted a much stronger effect on goal-level detachment than global
detachment, this pattern of results supports the idea that detachment may be better
conceptualized as a multifaceted construct composed of thoughts from a variety of
meaningful sources, including goals. Furthermore, the fact that a goal-centric intervention
primarily improved goal-level detachment instead of global detachment illustrates the
value of targeted interventions.
2.5
3.0
3.5
4.0
4.5
5.0
Goal-level detachment
Goal type
Low job involvement
No Plan
Plan
2.5
3.0
3.5
4.0
4.5
5.0
Complete Incomplete Complete Incomplete
Goal-level detachment
Goal type
High job involvement
No Plan
Plan
Figure 2. Three-way interaction among goal type, planning condition, and job involvement.
Table 4. Test of Hypothesis 5
DV: Global detachment
Variables Coefficient SE
Intercept 3.60** .12
Work from home 0.06 .14
Children at home 0.23* .14
Neuroticism 0.07 .05
Segmentation preferences 0.01 .06
Job involvement 0.23** .08
Planning behaviours 0.08 .06
Daily workload 0.12** .03
Planning manipulation 0.09 .13
Planning 9Involvement 0.19* .11
Note.*p<.10; **p<.01.
Successfully leaving work at work 15
Discussion
Why do some employees have difficulty mentally disengaging from work during leisure
time, and can anything be done to help them? The purpose of this study was to explore
why, how, and when employees will experience psychological detachment from work. I
developed a set of propositions designed to test the idea that detachment may be
conceptualized, in part, as a goal-oriented state characterized by low work-goal
accessibility. In line with this revised conceptualization, results indicated that daily
unfulfilled goals were associated with reduced goal detachment in contrast to completed
goals. Furthermore, individuals had more difficulty detaching from unfulfilled goals with
higher valence, consistent with the idea that goals with greater motivational force capture
attention more easily and inhibit psychological detachment. To explore practical methods
to improve detachment on a daily basis, this study tested and supported the efficacy of a
planning intervention. Although planning did not increase detachment from unfulfilled
goals among all employees, evidence indicates that it does improve detachment among
individuals with high job involvement. This observation supports the idea that employees
with individual differences that chronically predispose them to low detachment may
benefit most from intervention efforts.
This study also explored whether the planning intervention can facilitate global
detachment, in addition to goal detachment. While the results are suggestive and in the
hypothesized direction, the interaction term between planning and job involvement
failed to reach conventional levels of significance (i.e., p=.075). Thus, these findings
should be interpreted with caution. One likely explanation for these non-significant
results focuses on the differences between goal detachment and global detachment.
Specifically, because global detachment may be composed of other categories of work-
related thoughts beyond goals (e.g., justice-related events, interpersonal interactions), the
goal-based planning intervention may not be effective in increasing detachment from
thoughts in domains that are not goal-relevant. Thus, the planning intervention may
perhaps only be effective in facilitating global detachment to the extent that a given
person’s level of global detachment is largely composed of goal-related thoughts. Some of
the future research directions outlined below may be able to shed light on this question.
Overall, the findings of this study have several important theoretical implications.
Theoretical implications and future research directions
First, conceptualizing psychological detachment as a multifaceted construct addresses
important gaps in theory. In the extant literature, it is currently unclear what employees
are actually detaching from during off-job time. Although the widely accepted definition of
detachment specifies job-related content, this study has demonstrated that psychological
detachment may be explored in more depth by conceptualizing it as a multifaceted
construct. This conceptualization sheds new light on the content of psychological
detachment by illustrating that goals are at least one important facet of what employee
attention continues to linger over during off-job time. This revised perspective creates
many opportunities for future research to further explore the content of psychological
detachment. For example, a goal-oriented perspective on psychological detachment
prompts questions about what types of goals employees detach from during off-job time.
Although this study focused exclusively on task-related goals, future research could
conceivably explore other categories of goal content as well. Self-determination theory
proposes that individuals pursue goals related to autonomy, competence, and relatedness
16 Brandon W. Smit
(Ryan & Deci, 2000). It may be the case that relatedness-focused goals are either more or
less difficult to detach from than task-oriented goals.
Additionally, a multifaceted perspective on psychological detachment prompts
important questions about the structure of psychological detachment. Specifically,
there is a need for a more comprehensive understanding of the content of detachment,
because neglecting other important facets may result in deficient measures. Although
previous research has conceptualized psychological detachment a monolithic state, this
study indicates that employees’ global sense of detachment may actually represent a
complex aggregation of detachment from various sources. Future research could
continue to explore other sources of work-related thoughts in order to develop
targeted evidence-based interventions that can facilitate detachment from these
sources.
Second, apart from clarifying the content of detachment, this theoretical innovation
also allows researchers to create richer theory regarding the mechanisms underlying
detachment effects. Although some scholars have suggested that prolonged work-
derived arousal may account for reduced detachment (Sonnentag et al., 2010), no
studies to date have explicitly tested this hypothesis. The current study departs from
this perspective and indicates that goal accessibility may be one important mechanism
underlying employee detachment failures. This assertion is bolstered by the strong
correlation between goal-level and global detachment, along with the fact that goal
valence moderated the relationship between goal type and goal detachment, indicating
individuals have more difficulty detaching from goals with stronger motivational force.
Although this feature may be adaptive during goal pursuit, the evidence presented
here converges with previous findings that a failure to appropriately disengage from
goals can have negative consequences (Wrosch, Scheier, Miller, Schulz, & Carver,
2003).
Identifying goal accessibility as a mechanism underlying this process is important
because it provides a fresh perspective on detachment. Although not measured directly,
the overall pattern of results found here replicates previous research that does directly
measure goal accessibility which helps bolster confidence that goal accessibility does play
a role in these findings (F
orster et al., 2005). This self-regulatory lens can help scholars
reinterpret previous research by illustrating how goals may affect detachment through
both top-down and bottom-up mechanisms. Returning to an earlier question, individual
differences such as job involvement may reduce goal detachment by chronically making
goal constructs more accessible (i.e., top-down). This is important to note, because it may
explain why segmentation tactics, such as removing work-related items from home, may
not be effective in some cases. Although bottom-up goal cues have been removed from the
environment, individual priorities and goal hierarchies may still prompt work-related
thoughts in the absence of physical cues.
Finally, this study contributes to the growing conversation on practical methods of
enhancing psychological detachment. The findings presented here build on prior work
by demonstrating that planning, in the form of implementation intentions, is one
effective way to increase psychological detachment from incomplete work-related goals.
Future research may continue to explore the role that planning plays in shaping
psychological detachment. For example, future studies may elaborate on the different
components of plans to explore differential effects on goal detachment. Additionally,
researchers may also continue to explore individual differences that moderate the effect
of planning on detachment to determine for whom planning will increase psychological
detachment.
Successfully leaving work at work 17
Limitations
Despite its contributions to theory and practice, there are a number of limitations to this
study. For example, goal-level detachment aggregated to the daily level was only
moderately correlated with daily global detachment (r=.58, p<.001), which may create
some concerns about construct validity. It is important to note, however, that these two
constructs were measured with the exact same four items (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007).
These scales differed only in their focus, where participants rated detachment from work
as a whole for the global measure, and participants rated detachment from specific work
goals for the goal-level measure. The moderate correlation here may indicate participants
failed to list important goals in the after-work survey from which they later failed to detach.
However, this result may also lend support to one assertion in this study that other content
areas beyond goals, such as justice concerns or interpersonal interactions, also shape
global detachment.
There could also be some concern that attrition in this study has shaped the findings.
To explore this possibility, I correlated the demographic and personality characteristics
with the number of days that participants provided data in this study. If significant
relationships were observed, this might suggest the sample is biased towards these
characteristics (e.g., there are fewer neurotic participants in the sample, because they
were less likely to complete surveys). This analysis, however, yielded non-significant
correlations between number of survey days completed and demographic/personality
characteristics. Although it cannot be determined with absolute certainty, this analysis
indicates that the data are missing at random in this sample.
Practical implications
At least two substantive practical implications may be drawn from the findings reported
in this study. First, the planning intervention used here provides prescriptive guidance
for employees and organizations seeking to improve occupational health and perfor-
mance through psychological detachment. It is important, however, that planning be
implemented in a specific way. The intervention used in this study instructed
participants to list (1) where, (2) when, and (3) how they would accomplish an
incomplete goal. Employees may leverage these results by creating plans at the end of
their workday to promote detachment. Creating end-of-day plans for incomplete goals is
a low-cost option, both with regard to time and money, for improving employee
occupational health and performance. As noted earlier, however, this planning method
will likely be less useful for individuals who already easily detach from work during off-
job time.
Second, these findings also have implications for how employees set and pursue
goals at work. For example, these results illustrate the importance of setting realistic
goals within a given day, because committing to too much will likely result in more
incomplete goals at the end of the day and, consequently, less detachment. Research
on the planning fallacy indicates that individuals poorly estimate how long tasks will
take to complete (Buehler, Griffin, & Ross, 1994), so recognizing this bias and
purposefully undercommitting and overperforming may help individuals improve their
accuracy in setting and completing goals on time. One recommendation to employees
may be to structure the workday so that concrete, short-term tasks are completed near
the end of the day, to help ensure that their goals are complete before finishing work
for the day.
18 Brandon W. Smit
Conclusion
Psychological detachment plays an important role in shaping employees’ occupational
health and job performance. This study expands our knowledge by conceptualizing
detachment as a multifaceted construct that is meaningfully shaped by goal-related events.
The findings presented here indicate that employees have more difficulty detaching from
unfulfilled goals in contrast to completed goals, especially for goals characterized by high
valence. The intervention implemented in this study provides supportive evidence that
planning, in the form of implementation intentions, can increase detachment among
employees who possess individual differences that impair effective detachment (e.g., job
involvement). Overall, these results support a self-regulatory perspective on detachment,
where goals play an important role in shaping employees’ ability to disengage from work
during off-job time.
References
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and
content. Psychological Bulletin,120, 338375. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.3.338
Behrend, T., Sharek, D., Meade, A., & Wiebe, E. (2011). The viability of crowdsourcing for survey
research. Behavior Research Methods,43, 800813. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0081-0
Binnewies, C., Sonnentag, S., & Mojza, E. J. (2010). Recovery during the weekend and fluctuations in
weekly job performance: A four-week longitudinal study examining intra-individual
relationships. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology,83, 419441.
doi:10.1348/096317909X418049
Bongers, K. C. A., Dijksterhuis, A., & Spears, R. (2010). On the role of consciousness in goal pursuit.
Social Cognition,28, 262272. doi:10.1521/soco.2010.28.2.262
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994). Exploring the ‘planning fallacy’: Why people
underestimate their task completion times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
67, 366381. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.366
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk a new source of
inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science,6,35. doi:10.1177/
1745691610393980
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Colquitt, J. A., & Rodell, J. B. (2011). Justice, trust, and trustworthiness: A longitudinal analysis
integrating three theoretical perspectives. Academy of Management Journal,54, 11831206.
doi:10.5465/amj.2007.0572
Cropley, M., Michalianou, G., Pravettoni, G., & Millward, L. (2012). The relation of post work
ruminative thinking with eating behaviour. Stress and Health,28,2330. doi:10.1002/
smi.1397
Etzion, D., Eden, D., & Lapidot, Y. (1998). Relief from job stressors and burnout: Reserve service as a
respite. Journal of Applied Psychology,83, 577585. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.4.577
F
orster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology,41, 220239. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.06.009
Fӧrster, J., Liberman, N., & Friedman, R. S. (2007). Seven principles of goal activation: A systematic
approach to distinguishing goal priming from priming of non-goal constructs. Personality and
Social Psychology Review,11, 211233. doi:10.1177/1088868307303029
Gallie, D., White, M., Cheng, Y., & Tomlinson, M. (1998). Restructuring the employment
relationship. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the
lower-level facets of several five-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt &
Successfully leaving work at work 19
F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 728). Tilburg, the
Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone
(Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 141185). New York, NY: Wiley.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American
Psychologist,54, 493503. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-
analysis of effects and processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,38,69120.
doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38002-.1
Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,19, 12111226.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.5.1211
Hahn, V. C., Binnewies, C., & Haun, S. (2012). The role of partners for employees’ recovery during
the weekend. Journal of Vocational Behavior,80, 288298. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2011.12.004
Hahn, V. C., Binnewies, C., Sonnentag, S., & Mojza, E. J. (2011). Learning how to recover from job
stress: Effects of a recovery training program on recovery, recovery-related self-efficacy, and
well-being. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,16, 202216. doi:10.1037/a0022169
Hahn, V. C., & Dormann, C. (2013). The role of partners and children for employees’ psychological
detachment from work and well-being. Journal of Applied Psychology,98,2636. doi:10.1037/
a0030650
Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability and salience. In E. T. Higgins
& A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133168). New
York, NY: Guilford.
H
ulsheger, U. R., Lang, J. W., Depenbrock, F., Fehrmann, C., Zijlstra, F. R., & Alberts, H. J. (2014). The
power of presence: The role of mindfulness at work for daily levels and change trajectories of
psychological detachment and sleep quality. Journal of Applied Psychology,99, 11131128.
doi:10.1037/a0037702
Ilies, R., Schwind, K. M., Wagner, D. T., Johnson, M. D., DeRue, D. S., & Ilgen, D. R. (2007). When can
employees have a family life? The effects of daily workload and affect on workfamily conflict
and social behavior at work. Journal of Applied Psychology,92, 13681379. doi:10.1037/0021-
9010.92.5.1368
Janssen, O. (2001). Fairness perceptions as a moderator in the curvilinear relationships between job
demands, and job performance and job dissatisfaction. Academy of Management Journal,44,
10391050. doi:10.2307/3069447
Johnson, R. E., Chang, C. H., & Lord, R. G. (2006). Moving from cognition to behavior: What the
research says. Psychological Bulletin,132, 381415. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.381
Kanungo, R. N. (1982). Measurement of job and work involvement. Journal of Applied Psychology,
67, 341349. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.67.3.341
Kreiner, G. E. (2006). Consequences of workhome segmentation or integration: A person
environment fit perspective. Journal of Organizational Behavior,27, 485507. doi:10.1002/
job.386
Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Motivated social cognition: Principles of the interface. In E. T. Higgins & A.
W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 493520). New
York, NY: Guilford.
Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W. Y., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A
theory of goal systems. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,34, 331378.
doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80008-9
K
uhnel, J., Sonnentag, S., & Westman, M. (2009). Does work engagement increase after a short
respite? The role of job involvement as a double-edged sword. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology,82, 575594. doi:10.1348/096317908X349362
Lapierre, L. M., & Allen, T. D. (2012). Control at work, control at home, and planning behavior
implications for workfamily conflict. Journal of Management,38, 15001516. doi:10.1177/
0149206310385868
20 Brandon W. Smit
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science; selected theoretical papers. New York, NY: Harper
& Row.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task
motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist,57, 705717. doi:10.1037/0003-
066X.57.9.705
Lord, R. L., Diefendorff, J. M., Schmidt, A. M., & Hall, R. J. (2010). Self-regulation at work. Annual
Review of Psychology,61, 543568. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100314
Lord, R. G., & Levy, P. E. (1994). Moving from cognition to action: A control theory perspective.
Applied Psychology,43, 335367. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.1994.tb00828.x
Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., & Bink, M. L. (1998). Activation of completed, uncompleted and partially
completed intentions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition,24, 350361. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.2.350
Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2011a). Consider it done!Plan making can eliminate the
cognitive effects of unfulfilled goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,101, 667
683. doi:10.1037/a0024192
Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2011b). Unfulfilled goals interfere with tasks that require
executive functions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,47, 300311. doi:10.1016/
j.jesp.2010.10.011
Mento, A. J., Locke, E. A., & Klein, H. J. (1992). Relationship of goal level to valence and
instrumentality. Journal of Applied Psychology,77, 395405. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.77.4.395
Mojza, E. J., Lorenz, C., Sonnentag, S., & Binnewies, C. (2010). Daily recovery experiences: The role
of volunteer work during leisure time. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,15,6074.
doi:10.1037/a0017983
Nezlek, J. B. (2008). An introduction to multilevel modeling for social and personality psychology.
Social and Personality Psychology Compass,2, 842860. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.
00059.x
Orbell, S., Hodgkins, S., & Sheeran, P. (1997). Implementation intentions and the theory of planned
behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,23, 945954. doi:10.1177/
0146167297239004
Park, Y., Fritz, C., & Jex, S. M. (2011). Relationships between workhome segmentation and
psychological detachment from work: The role of communication technology use at home.
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,16, 457467. doi:10.1037/a0023594
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist,55,6878.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68
Sonnentag, S. (2012). Psychological detachment from work during leisure time: The benefits of
mentally disengaging from work. Current Directions in Psychological Science,21, 114118.
doi:10.1177/0963721411434979
Sonnentag, S., & Bayer, U. V. (2005). Switching off mentally: Predictors and consequences of
psychological detachment from work during off-job time. Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology,10, 393414. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.10.4.393
Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2008). ‘Did you have a nice evening?’ A day-level study
on recovery experiences, sleep, and affect. Journal of Applied Psychology,93, 674684.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.674
Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. (2007). The Recovery Experience Questionnaire: Development and
validation of a measure assessing recuperation and unwinding from work. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology,12, 204221. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.12.3.204
Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010). Job stressors, emotional exhaustion, and need for
recovery: A multi-source study on the benefits of psychological detachment. Journal of
Vocational Behavior,76, 355365. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2009.06.005
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of
positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54, 10631070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Successfully leaving work at work 21
Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Miller, G. E., Schulz, R., & Carver, C. S. (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of
unattainable goals: Goal disengagement, goal reengagement, and subjective well-being.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,29, 14941508. doi:10.1177/0146167203256921
Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Uber das Behalten von erledigten und unerledigten Handlungen [On the
retention of completed and uncompleted transactions]. Psychologische Forschung,9,185.
Received 6 May 2015; revised version received 14 August 2015
22 Brandon W. Smit
... It could be also described as mentally "switching off" from work, not thinking about work issues, and as a process of recovery (Sonnentag et al., 2010;Weigelt et al., 2019). Psychological detachment is thought to be one of the ideal recovery experiences to reduce or stop ruminative thinking (Martin & Tesser, 1996;Smit, 2015). To replace and recover the exhausted sources, psychological detachment can be achieved through rest, recreation, and leisure activities. ...
... Workplace nutrients, such as a supportive work environment can also be a replenishing factor for employees to improve their well-being. It was found that psychological detachment is negatively related to health issues, symptoms of depression, the need for recovery, and emotional exhaustion and is linked to increased well-being (Smit, 2015;Sonnentag et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study is twofold. First, it evaluates the psychometric properties of the Czech adaptation of the Work-Related Rumination Questionnaire (WRRQ) and Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration Scale-Work Domain (BPNSFS-W). Second, the link between work-related rumination and work-related need satisfaction was examined. Three forms of work-related rumination, namely, affective rumination, pondering, and detachment were hypothesized to have distinct associations with the satisfaction of autonomy, competence, and relatedness needs at work. There were 278 employees (67% female, Mage= 40.22, SD = 12.98) completing an online survey. CFA confirmed the three-factor model of both WRRQ and BPNSFS-W. Moreover, the measurement invariance of both scales was investigated across gender. The BPNSFS-W has measurement invariance, and WRRQ has partial measurement invariance across females and males. Hierarchical regression analyses showed that while competence did not predict any form of WRR, relatedness positively predicted detachment. Moreover, autonomy positively predicted pondering and negatively predicted affective rumination. Findings suggest that the WRRQ and BPNSFS-W are promising instruments for future research and practice in the Czech context.
... There may also be an anticipation of future stress, which then triggers a mental and emotional continuation of work (ibid.). Moreover, Smit (2016) argues that stressors can contribute to the failure to achieve goals sought during working hours. This is consistent with the Action Regulation Theory conceptualization of demands, which states that they arise from contradictions between goals and implementation conditions at work (Oesterreich et al. 2000, p. 54;Zapf and Semmer 2004;Zacher and Frese 2018). ...
... As mentioned in Sect. 2.1, unfinished goals are assumed to affect detachment (Smit 2016). Therefore, it can be inferred that aspects that promote goal achievement at work contribute to successful detachment. ...
Article
Full-text available
After the coronavirus pandemic, hybrid work has become firmly established in many companies. This study investigated the relation of hybrid work and relevant working conditions with the psychological detachment of employees. A written online survey was conducted among 250 employees of a company in the mechanical engineering industry in Germany who worked from home at least part of the time. Multiple hierarchical regression was used for the analysis. The results show that, as expected, the quality of manager-employee communication has a positive (β = 0.13, p < 0.05) and extended work availability a negative effect (β = −0.35, p < 0.001) on psychological detachment. Contrary to the assumptions, however, the proportion of work from home has a positive effect on detachment (β = 0.15, p < 0.05). Regarding the demographic variables, female employees are better able to detach from work (β = −0.36, p < 0.01). The quantitative demands initially show a significant effect, but this disappears as soon as the extended work availability and the proportion of work from home are included in the analysis. The included predictors explain 19% (p < 0.001) of the total variance. Practical Relevance Hybrid work offers the opportunity to organize work more flexibly and thus to adapt it more closely to the needs of employees. The results of this study show that work from home can foster psychological detachment from work, provided that health-promoting working conditions are in place.
... Effective work scheduling and workspace planning are linked to increased cognitive flexibility. Proactive planning can decrease the number of incomplete tasks, thus mitigating job rumination and acting as a safeguard against it (Masicampo & Baumeister, 2011;Smit, 2016;Smit & Barber, 2016). Furthermore, coordination with colleagues is associated with greater work engagement and does not inevitably lead to burnout ( Van den Broeck et al., 2008). ...
Article
The COVID-19 pandemic has accelerated the adoption of flexible work arrangements within the Italian public administration. While much of the existing research has focused on the drawbacks of such arrangements, there has been less exploration of their benefits. Cognitive demands related to the structure of work activities, planning of working hours, planning of workplaces, and coordination with others, under flexible working conditions, might be considered as job resources that act as challenging demands within the Job Demand-Resource (JD-R) model. This study aimed to explore how the “cognitive challenge of flexible work” (CCFW) impact job satisfaction through home-based performance, taking into account the role of weekly working hours on home-based performance. Furthermore, the potential moderating role of cognitive and physical job demands between CCFW and home-based performance was explored. Using structural equation modeling on data from 484 public employees, the findings confirmed the positive impact of the structure of work tasks and planning of working times on both job satisfaction and home based performance. In addition, cognitive demands (i.e., perception of cognitive work overload) played a moderating role in the mediated relationship between coordinating with others on job satisfaction and the structure of working tasks on job satisfaction through home-based performance.
... As a result, NA makes detachment difficult because employees "stay occupied with work-related tasks, try to engage in problem solving and to deal with the stressor, or continue to worry or vent about the stressor" (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2015, p. S87). Although the assumption that NA plays a central role in the stressor-detachment model is widely accepted (Chen & Li, 2019;Smit, 2016;Sonnentag, 2018;Sonnentag & Fritz, 2015), empirical evidence for this idea is surprisingly lacking (Cangiano et al., 2021;Sonnentag et al., 2017). Thus, we examined NA as a central mediator to capture the underlying processes that might contribute to change during interventions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recovering from work is essential for maintaining occupational well-being, health, motivation, and performance, but recovery is often difficult to achieve. In this study, we evaluated and compared the effectiveness of two (parallel) interventions aimed at promoting recovery: one based on mindfulness and one involving applying cognitive–behavioral strategies. Both interventions were embedded in a measurement burst design, which allowed us to examine the mechanisms underlying change or intervention success. To explore mechanisms of change, we used the stressor-detachment model as a theoretical framework. We operationalized the interventions’ effects in three ways: as changes from pretest to posttest, as changes in daily states, and as changes in daily associations. To this end, we used intensive longitudinal data to examine the roles that daily negative activation plays in detachment and strain. In a randomized controlled trial (N = 393), we administered three assessments of traits: pretest, posttest (8 weeks later), and follow-up (3 months after the posttest). We also administered 2 work weeks of experience sampling questionnaires (preintervention and postintervention). Latent change models and Bayes factor equivalence tests revealed that both interventions substantially—and to a similar extent—increased detachment. Bayesian multilevel path models showed improvements in all state variables, including improvements in negative activation, and provided some evidence that mindfulness-based and cognitive–behavioral approaches might tackle different processes at the daily level. We discuss theoretical implications for the literature on recovery from work and specifically for the stressor-detachment model.
... As a result, negative activation makes detachment difficult because employees "stay occupied with work-related tasks, try to engage in problem solving and to deal with the stressor, or continue to worry or vent about the stressor" (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2015, p. S87). Although the assumption that negative activation plays a central role in the stressor-detachment model is widely accepted (Chen & Li, 2019;Smit, 2016;Sonnentag, 2018;Sonnentag & Fritz, 2015), empirical evidence for this idea is surprisingly lacking (Cangiano et al., 2021;Sonnentag et al., 2017). Thus, we examined negative activation as a central mediator in order to capture the underlying processes that might contribute to change during interventions. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Recovering from work is essential for maintaining occupational well-being, health, motivation, and performance, but recovery is often difficult to achieve. In this study, we evaluated and compared the effectiveness of two (parallel) interventions aimed at promoting recovery: one based on mindfulness and one involving applying cognitive-behavioral strategies. Both interventions were embedded in a measurement burst design, which allowed us to examine the mechanisms underlying change or intervention success. To explore mechanisms of change, we used the stressor detachment model as a theoretical framework. We operationalized the interventions' effects in three ways: as changes from pretest to posttest, as changes in daily states, and as changes in daily associations. To this end, we used intensive longitudinal data to examine the roles that daily negative activation plays in detachment and strain. In a randomized controlled trial (N = 393), we administered three assessments of traits: pretest, posttest (8 weeks later), and follow-up (3 months after the posttest). We also administered 2 work weeks of experience sampling questionnaires (preintervention and postintervention). Latent change models and Bayes factor equivalence tests revealed that both interventions substantially—and to a similar extent—increased detachment. Bayesian multilevel path models showed improvements in all state variables, including improvements in negative activation, and provided some evidence that mindfulness-based and cognitive-behavioral approaches might tackle different processes at the daily level. We discuss theoretical implications for the literature on recovery from work and specifically for the stressor detachment model.
... According to the effort-recovery model (Meijman & Mulder, 1998;Sonnentag et al., 2022), energy expended at work must be replenished via a process of recovery to achieve balance, rather than conflict, between work and nonwork domains. Theories of recovery from work argue that the ability to psychologically detach from work in a global sense (i.e., detaching from all aspects of work, including tasks, interpersonal connections and fairness-related events; Smit, 2016) is crucial for recovery (Sonnentag et al., 2017(Sonnentag et al., , 2022 -and that segmentation can facilitate psychologically detachment (Karabinski et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Segmentation of work from nonwork life is widely presented as desirable to maximize recovery from work. Yet it involves effort which may reduce its positive effects. We present a dual-process model of segmentation based on integrating boundary theory and self-regulation theory that shows how creating and maintaining boundaries can have both positive and negative effects. Segmentation allows individuals not only to psychological detach from work, thereby reducing work–nonwork conflict, but can also deplete an individual's momentary capacity for state self-control, increasing work–nonwork conflict. We tested our model with two studies: a weekly diary study with a sample of 436 individuals and a daily diary study with data collected at two time points each day from 162 participants. Using a Bayesian approach, we find some support for our hypothesized dual pathways. In both studies, psychological detachment mediated a negative relationship between segmentation and work–nonwork conflict. In the daily study, self-control capacity mediated a positive relationship between segmentation and work–nonwork conflict, but this effect was present only when individuals worked onsite and not when working at home. This article contributes towards understanding the mechanisms explaining the relationship between segmentation and work–nonwork conflict and underscores the importance of self-regulation in this process.
... Recent research suggests that the psychological benefits of disclosure and reflection with an agent are similar to reflection with another human [18]. Moreover, reflecting on one's plans at the end of one's day can help psychologically detach from work and support a healthy work-life balance [23,37,40]. However, users need a nudge to reflect upon their activities as reflection does not come naturally to humans [13]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Efficient task planning is essential for productivity and mental well-being, yet individuals often struggle to create realistic plans and reflect upon their productivity. Leveraging the advancement in artificial intelligence (AI), conversational agents have emerged as a promising tool for enhancing productivity. Our work focuses on externalizing plans through conversation, aiming to solidify intentions and foster focused action, thereby positively impacting their productivity and mental well-being. We share our plan of designing a conversational agent to offer insightful questions and reflective prompts for increasing plan adherence by leveraging the social interactivity of natural conversations. Previous studies have shown the effectiveness of such agents, but many interventions remain static, leading to decreased user engagement over time. To address this limitation, we propose a novel rotation and context-aware prompting strategy, providing users with varied interventions daily. Our system, PITCH, utilizes large language models (LLMs) to facilitate externalization and reflection on daily plans. Through this study, we investigate the impact of externalizing tasks with conversational agents on productivity and mental well-being, and the effectiveness of a rotation strategy in maintaining user engagement.
Article
Full-text available
This paper presents the findings of a mixed-method study exploring the self-reported negative impact on wellbeing of working in policing in England and Wales during the Covid-19 pandemic. For the first time, findings relating to the wellbeing of UK police and police staff are presented from two different points in time during the pandemic period-the initial 'lockdown phase' (March 2020) and Autumn 2021 as the UK began to return to pre-pandemic social behaviours. A first study presents data collected via electronic survey, completed in June 2021 by more than 500 UK police officers and staff, exploring how they felt initially about the Covid-19 pandemic and how they felt later in 2021. For example, how it affected their work and how they dealt with the changes in working practices. A second study presents an Interpretive Phenomenological Analysis of 18 interviews capturing the individual 'life experiences' of UK police officers and staff working in variety of policing roles during the pandemic. Key themes identified include the negative effects of working at home and the worry of spreading the disease to loved ones. Recommendations are made for how police forces can better support the wellbeing of officers and staff during pandemic scenarios and increase resilience. For example, by quickly identifying those officers and staff most vulnerable to the disease and/ or most anxious generally at the start of a pandemic, to plan for how appropriate support can be provided, and acknowledge that police staff feel most isolated when working from home.
Article
Augmentation-based artificial intelligence (AI) artifacts are increasingly being incorporated into the workplace. The coupling of employees and AI tools, given their complementary strengths, expands and expedites employees’ access to information and affords important learning opportunities. However, existing research has yet to fully understand the learning-based benefits and challenges for employees in augmentation. Integrating insights from AI augmentation literature and cognitive load theory, we conducted a daily diary study to understand employees’ experience using augmentation-based AI at work on a daily basis. We theorized and found that, on the one hand, frequent usage of augmentation-based AI during a workday was associated with greater knowledge gain and subsequently better task performance at the end of the workday. On the other hand, using augmentation-based AI frequently also led employees to experience information overload, which in turn impaired their performance and recovery at the end of the workday. In addition to elucidating the countervailing mechanisms, we identified employee openness to experience as a dispositional factor, and positive affect as a momentary state that shaped the effects of using augmentation-based AI over the workday. Our research has implications for understanding AI augmentation dynamics from a learning-based perspective, as well as AI’s impact on employees at large.
Article
Full-text available
While regular physical exercise as a recovery activity has an important role regarding employees’ well-being, employees seem to engage less in physical exercise after stressful workdays—pointing to a paradoxical pattern. Extending the recovery paradox (Sonnentag, 2018) applied to physical exercise and drawing from perspectives on energetic and self-regulatory resource loss and recovery, we integrate psychological detachment right after work as an explaining psychological mechanism in the paradoxical pattern between job stressors, physical exercise, and energetic well-being. We collected daily diary data from 93 employees on 514 days over two consecutive workweeks. Results of our two-level path model suggest that psychological detachment right after work explains the paradoxical pattern between job stressors (workload and self-control demands) and time spent on physical exercise. However, we did not find a serial indirect effect from job stressors to next-morning energetic well-being (vigor and fatigue) via psychological detachment and time spent on physical exercise. Our study suggests a new perspective on the role of psychological detachment, namely as a prerequisite instead of an outcome of physical exercise after stressful workdays. This new perspective has implications for future research and practical interventions.
Article
Full-text available
In this research, we examined the role of mindfulness for recovery from work using a daily diary design (N = 121; 5 days; 3 measurement occasions per day). The first goal of the study was to investigate the relationship of mindfulness with sleep quality and the mediating role of psychological detachment from a day-level perspective. A second goal was to extend the process perspective in recovery research beyond the day level and consider systematic change trajectories in recovery variables over the course of the work week and the role of mindfulness in these trajectories. Results regarding day-level relationships confirmed that mindfulness experienced during work was related to subsequent sleep quality, and this relationship was mediated by psychological detachment from work in the evening. Furthermore, an investigation of the role of mindfulness in recovery change trajectories supported the idea that psychological detachment trajectories increase over the work week for individuals low on mindfulness while there was no systematic mean-level change for individuals high on mindfulness. In contrast, sleep quality followed a linear increase from Monday to Friday for all individuals, irrespective of their levels of trait mindfulness. Practical and theoretical implications for the mindfulness and the recovery literature are discussed in conclusion.
Book
This book presents a thorough overview of a model of human functioning based on the idea that behavior is goal-directed and regulated by feedback control processes. It describes feedback processes and their application to behavior, considers goals and the idea that goals are organized hierarchically, examines affect as deriving from a different kind of feedback process, and analyzes how success expectancies influence whether people keep trying to attain goals or disengage. Later sections consider a series of emerging themes, including dynamic systems as a model for shifting among goals, catastrophe theory as a model for persistence, and the question of whether behavior is controlled or instead 'emerges'. Three chapters consider the implications of these various ideas for understanding maladaptive behavior, and the closing chapter asks whether goals are a necessity of life. Throughout, theory is presented in the context of diverse issues that link the theory to other literatures.
Article
Despite a significant amount of theoretical and empirical attention, the connection between justice and trust remains poorly understood. Our study utilized Mayer, Davis, and Schoorman's (1995) distinction between trustworthiness (the ability, benevolence, and integrity of a trustee) and trust (a willingness to be vulnerable to the trustee) to clarify that connection. More specifically, we drew on a theoretical integration of social exchange theory, the relational model, and fairness heuristic theory to derive predictions about the relationships among justice, trustworthiness, and trust, with supervisors as the referent. A longitudinal field study stretching over two periods showed that informational justice was a significant predictor of subsequent trust perceptions, even when analyses controlled for prior levels of trust and trustworthiness. However, the relationship between justice and trustworthiness was shown to be reciprocal. Procedural and interpersonal justice were significant predictors of subsequent levels of benevolence and integrity, with integrity predicting subsequent levels of all four justice dimensions. We describe the theoretical implications of these results for future research in the justice and trust literatures.
Book
This masterly new study presents the first large-scale empirical analysis of the changes in British work experiences and employment relationships between the 1980s and 1990s. Drawing on the Employment in Britain surveya national survey providing the richest source of evidence to date about individuals' experience of employmentit examines the impact of new technologies, the emergence of new management policies, the changing forms of employment contract, and the growth of job insecurity on people's experience of employment. The authors focus on the implications these developments have for the ways in which skills and work tasks have been changing, the nature of control at work, the degree of participation in decision-making, and the flexibility demanded at work. They assess whether there has been a tendency towards either a polarization or convergence of employment experiences between men and women, and between occupational classes. They offer fresh insight into how the changing quality of work in recent years has affected employee's involvement in their jobs and organizations, the stress they experience at work, and the propensity for absenteeism and staff turnover. While the study provides strong evidence of a marked trend towards upskilling, the authors take issue with the argument that a new type of employment relationship is emerging, arguing instead that the restructuring of the employment relationship has, in fact, reinforced traditional lines of division in the workforce.