ArticlePDF Available

The interplay between Facebook use, social comparison, envy, and depression

Authors:
Appel, H., Gerlach, A. L., & Crusius, J. (2016). The interplay between Facebook use, social comparison, envy, and depression. Current
Opinion in Psychology, 9, 4449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.006
1
Accepted version. CC BY-NC-ND. Access typeset publisher version here.
The interplay between Facebook use, social comparison, envy, and depression
Helmut Appel, Alexander L. Gerlach, and Jan Crusius, University of Cologne
Highlights
Facebook use has recently been linked to depression and
negative affectivity.
Unflattering social comparison and envy are common experi-
ences for Facebook users.
Envy and social comparison mediate correlations between
Facebook use and depression.
Facebook research sheds new light on the relationship be-
tween envy and depression.
Further experiments and experience sampling should cor-
roborate causal links.
Abstract
In their Facebook profiles, users communicate abundant social
comparison information conveying mainly positive self-portrayals.
Thereby, social networking sites like Facebook provide a fertile
ground for envy. This has been proposed as a mechanism for the
potential negative effects of Facebook use on well-being and de-
pression. This article reviews research on this process. Studies
show that (especially passive) Facebook use indeed predicts dif-
ferent measures of social comparison as well as envy. In several
studies social comparison or envy mediate a positive association
between Facebook use and undesirable affective outcomes such
as depression. However, causal relationships have not yet been
sufficiently established. Methodological and conceptual variety
across studies limits their comparability, but reveals viable ideas
for future research.
Keywords: Depression, self-esteem, social comparison, envy, jeal-
ousy, online social media, Facebook
Introduction
The term ‘Facebook Depression’ has become a mass media head-
line [1] suggesting detrimental effects of social networking sites
(SNSs). The available empirical evidence is equivocal however,
sometimes suggesting a positive [2, 3, 4, 5], a non-existent [6], or
even a negative [7, 8, 9] relationship between Facebook use and
depression. Given that sites such as Facebook serve a diversity of
purposes [10, 11], treating them as a one-dimensional construct
is likely an oversimplification [4, 5] resulting in conceptual impre-
cision (for an overview, see [12**]). Investigating the specific pro-
cesses at work may disentangle these disparate findings. Recently,
social comparison and envy have been suggested to be relevant
in the association between Facebook and depressed mood. Here,
we review the evidence, consider methodological issues, and dis-
cuss open questions.
Comparison and envy in an SNS context
Social comparison is a fundamental psychological process govern-
ing how people think about themselves or others [13]. In SNSs,
social comparison is ubiquitous because comparison information
(e.g., friend count) is more salient than offline [14, 15]. SNS self-
presentation is frequently motivated by impression management
[16] and the presented information is positively skewed [17, 18].
Consequently, the probability of unflattering social comparisons
increases [12**]. Negative social comparisons are at the core of
envy [19], so the chance for envy to thrive is high [20*]. Addition-
ally, users mainly interact with friends and peers on Facebook [7,
21]. This implies both similarity to comparison standards and high
personal relevance, two factors fueling social comparisons and
envy alike [19]. Indeed, painful social comparison and envy are
among the most often reported stressors [22] and sources of
frustration on Facebook [23].
Most SNS-related studies conceptualize envy or negative social
comparisons as causal factors of depression [24, 25*, 26*]. In-
deed, envy and depression have been linked outside of Facebook
research [27, 28, 29], but the causal relationship is far from clear
[19]. Arguably, the lack of self-esteem [30] and the tendency to
compare themselves negatively with others [31] might make de-
pressed individuals also particularly prone to envy. This should be
kept in mind when drawing conclusions from the studies reviewed
below.
Cross-sectional studies
Cross-sectional evidence demonstrates a positive correlation be-
tween the amount of Facebook use and the frequency of social
comparisons on Facebook [15, 24, 32, 33*]. A similar pattern
emerges for the impression of being inferior [34] and ‘Facebook
envy’ [20*, 23]. Some of these studies [20*, 23, 24, 32] have doc-
umented an association between social comparison or envy and
negative affective outcomes. In three studies, envy and social
comparison, respectively, mediated the relationship between Fa-
cebook use and depression-related outcomes [15, 20*, 23].
Because of the cross-sectional designs, no claims can be made
about causal relations. While it is tacitly assumed that SNS envy
causes depressed mood, the reverse pathway of depression caus-
ing envy is equally plausible [26*]. In addition, the assessment of
both Facebook use and social comparison has mostly relied on
unvalidated, retrospective self-report measures, sometimes
based on only one item [24]. The fact that these measures do not
differentiate between specific Facebook activities and are suscep-
tible to recall biases calls them further into question (e.g. [2,
12**]).
Prospective studies
Prospective studies investigated the predictive value of SNS social
comparison for depression. In a sample of high school students,
the tendency to socially compare oneself in SNSs predicted
changes in depressive symptomatology one year later [25*]. Sim-
ilarly, the tendency to compare oneself unfavorably to others on
Facebook, but not the general habit to socially compare oneself,
predicted depressive symptoms at three weeks follow-up in a
study by Feinstein and colleagues [26*]. In both studies, the re-
verse pathway was not investigated.
2
Diary and experience sampling (ES) studies
Diary studies address several weaknesses associated with retro-
spective self-reports. For example, over a 2-week period Steers
and colleagues [32] asked participants about their daily Facebook
experiences. Any type of social comparison, including favorable
comparison, mediated a positive correlation between frequent
Facebook use and depressed mood. Depression, in contrast, did
not predict Facebook use. However, analyses of correlations were
not lagged. Thus, a causal influence of depression on negative Fa-
cebook comparison is still conceivable, as are other constella-
tions.
In an ES study by Verduyn and colleagues [35**], participants re-
ported their Facebook use, envy, and well-being 5 times per day
for 6 days. Lagged analyses revealed that passive (e.g., viewing
others’ profiles), but not active Facebook use (e.g., posting) pre-
dicted subsequent declines in well-being, which was fully medi-
ated by envy. The results were not influenced by baseline depres-
sion or life-satisfaction and life-satisfaction was unrelated to Face-
book-use. The finding that passive Facebook use predicted
changes in well-being over time while the opposite pathway was
not significant supports a causal chain from passive Facebook use
through envy to depression. However, variables confounded with
Facebook use might still have contributed to the effect. For exam-
ple, people potentially log into Facebook more often when they
are bored [36]. The findings demonstrate that depressiveness is
associated with higher levels of envy, especially when comparison
standards are high.
Experimental studies
Causal relationships between Facebook use, social comparison,
envy, and depression have also been established experimentally.
For example, in a study about women's body image [37], women
instructed to spend ten minutes looking at their Facebook page
rated their mood lower than those looking at control websites.
Furthermore, participants in the Facebook condition who had a
strong tendency to compare their attractiveness to others were
less satisfied with their physical appearance, providing indirect
support for the role of social comparison processes.
Similar results were obtained by Vogel and colleagues [33*].
Mood, self-esteem, and positive self-views of individuals with a
strong social comparison orientation dropped significantly after
briefly looking at a casual friend's Facebook profile compared to
participants seeing their own profile or those visiting a non-social
website. Mediation by social comparison was not tested.
Haferkamp and Krämer [14] found that constructed SNS profiles
representing highly attractive comparison standards caused
worse mood and less satisfaction with one's appearance com-
pared to unattractive standards. Note that social comparison was
not directly measured, yielding only indirect evidence for compar-
ative processes. Moreover, personal relevance, a prerequisite for
social comparison and envy [19], was likely limited because pro-
files were not realistic-looking and featured unfamiliar owners. Fi-
nally, the lack of a neutral control group makes it possible that low
comparison standards led to better outcomes instead of high
standards having a negative impact.
Focusing on self-esteem, Vogel and colleagues [15] presented
constructed SNS profiles depicting comparison targets high or low
in healthiness or popularity, respectively. The results suggested
detrimental effects of being confronted with high standards, but
were not clear-cut. On some measures, participants’ self-
evaluation was lower after seeing the popular compared to the
unpopular target, other measures were affected by the healthi-
ness-manipulation.
Appel and colleagues [38*] also used constructed, but realistic-
looking and gender-matched Facebook profiles. Depressed and
non-depressed participants saw one of two profiles conveying ei-
ther a highly desirable or an undesirable life of the profile owner.
Compared to unattractive profiles, attractive profiles caused par-
ticipants to perceive themselves as inferior and to feel more envy,
with inferiority predicting envy. Depressed participants felt more
inferior and envious than controls, especially in the high standard
condition.
Finally, Verduyn and colleagues [35**] conducted an experiment
explicitly investigating the complete causal chain of Facebook use
on well-being mediated by social comparison. Participants who
browsed their own Facebook feed passively for ten minutes re-
ported lower well-being than those using it actively. To capture
social comparison, participants also rated their quality of life in
comparison to others but no effects were observed here.
Summary of reviewed evidence
In summary, available evidence is largely consistent with the no-
tion that Facebook use encourages unfavorable social compari-
sons and envy, which may in turn lead to depressed mood. How-
ever, although prospective and experience sampling studies as
well as statistical modeling make this causal link plausible, these
findings largely remain subject to the constraints of correlational
data. Experiments have not yet shown the complete causal chain
of Facebook use eliciting depressed mood mediated by unflatter-
ing comparison or envy. Therefore, taking this pathway as a given
may be premature. Note that other mechanisms potentially re-
sponsible for negative mood effects of Facebook use have re-
ceived empirical support, for example, the impression to waste
one's time [3], or communication overload [39]. There is also
some evidence suggesting that favorable social comparison on
SNSs is used to repair negative mood [40*], implying positive ef-
fects of Facebook comparisons.
Methodological considerations
Some methodological suggestions can be derived from this re-
view. Experimental control of SNS content grants internal but not
external validity [40*] given that real-life Facebook use is mainly
about interacting with people one knows [7]. At least, profiles
should look realistic and feature profile owners that participants
can relate to [38*]. When participants use their own Facebook
page instead [35**, 37], idiosyncratic Facebook behavior should
be tracked [35**] which allows coding certain aspects and con-
trolling for them (e.g., cf. [33*, 41, 42]).
One advantage of experimental exposure lies in the immediate
experience of Facebook use and its affective consequences,
thereby avoiding problems associated with retrospective self-re-
ports [3]. However, some effects of Facebook use may not mani-
fest immediately, but require repeated exposure [2, 15], especially
when considering more far-reaching consequences. Here, experi-
ence sampling (ES) may add valuable insights by tracking effects
over time. To minimize recall biases [43], ES studies should cap-
ture activities within a narrow time frame or encourage actual Fa-
cebook use ad-hoc.
Open research questions and conclusions
3
Several open questions remain for future research. As for individ-
ual differences, most studies focused on college populations.
However, younger individuals should be even more likely to en-
gage in or be affected by SNS social comparisons, given their de-
veloping identities [44] and their heavy SNS use [45]. Similarly,
gender differences concerning the links between Facebook usage,
comparison, and well-being have been documented, but in oppo-
site directions [25*, 32]. Other studies did not find gender to mod-
erate this relationship [32, 35**]. Looking at psychopathology, the
evidence for depressiveness predicting Facebook envy is mixed.
Verduyn and colleagues [35**] found that depression scores did
not moderate the relationship between Facebook use and well-
being. However, in the experiment by Appel and colleagues [38*]
depressed participants were more negatively affected by a high
comparison target. Thus, the notion of depression predisposing
individuals to envy needs to be further investigated because it
may help identify a population that is especially vulnerable to the
envy provoking potential of Facebook comparison.
Moreover, the large variety of operationalizations across studies
raises questions. For example, differentiating between passive
and active Facebook behavior is important in predicting envy and
depression [23, 35**]. Consequently, researchers should not limit
observations to global Facebook use. Various measures have also
been used to capture negative affect, ranging from mood shifts
[14, 35**, 37] to more drastic consequences like clinical depres-
sion [20*, 24, 25*]. In a similar vein, social comparison has been
defined inconsistently, sometimes looking at the frequency of
comparisons in general [33*], other times concentrating on neg-
ative comparison and resulting inferiority [32]. This is problematic
because inferiority may be associated with depressed mood by
mere semantical overlap [26*]. Note, however, that even flattering
SNS comparison is correlated with depression [32]. Furthermore,
different comparison dimensions may be more or less impactful
[37]. Conflicting results have been found with regard to the im-
portance of comparison domains (e.g., healthiness vs. popularity
[15], or career success vs. attractiveness [14, 40*]).
Other research may investigate the characteristics of people's so-
cial networks. For example, the total number of friends was either
negatively [23, 34] or not related [20*, 24, 35**] to social compar-
ison, envy, depressed mood, or their association, respectively. De-
sirable correlates of Facebook friend count have also been ob-
served in other contexts [8]. However, a longer friend list usually
entails a high proportion of strangers or distant acquaintances
[46], which predicts negative social comparison outcomes [34,
47], maybe because overly positive self-presentations are less
easily disproved by real-life information [12**].
Valuable insight may also be gained by investigating specific cog-
nitive and affective processes. There is evidence to suggest that
ruminating about negative Facebook experiences enhances its
harmful effects [48, 49, 50]. One study demonstrated a mediating
role of rumination with respect to Facebook social comparison
[26*]. Thus, rumination or worrying should be considered as rel-
evant psychopathological mechanisms involved in social compar-
ison and envy on Facebook [35**].
Furthermore, recent evidence [51, 52] shows that unflattering so-
cial comparisons can elicit two distinct forms of envy. Malicious
envy entails the motivation to harm the envied person and is as-
sociated with resentment, low personal control, and feelings of in-
justice. Even though equally frustrating, benign envy motivates to
keep up with the superior person through effort and is associated
with admiration, not hostility. Disentangling the two may shed light
on the conflicting findings concerning SNS use. For example, given
that lack of control is associated with malicious envy [51] and de-
pression alike [53], depressed individuals may be particularly
prone to malicious rather than benign envy when using SNSs. So-
cial context may also shape envy on Facebook. For example, envy
is modulated by how achievements are presented. Specifically,
hubristic pride (attributing success to talent) results in malicious
envy, whereas authentic pride (attributing success to effort) fos-
ters benign envy [54]. Indeed, Facebook postings of narcissistic
users, which can be assumed to convey hubristic pride [55], elicit
less positive feedback [56], potentially reflecting malicious envy.
The specific relationship to the comparison standard may also de-
termine the kind of envy on Facebook. For example, stronger re-
lational ties increase benign but not malicious envy in response to
a friend's post [57].
A lot remains to be learned about the relationship between envy
and depression in general and on Facebook in particular. Re-
search should not focus on global affective changes depending on
general Facebook use, but on which processes cause which ef-
fects in which populations (e.g. [58]). More rigorous scrutiny of
causal relationships, the differentiation of constructs and their
measurements, and the inclusion of personality and psycho-
pathology moderators are called for to advance our understand-
ing of social comparison, envy, and depression in an SNS environ-
ment.
References and recommended reading
Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:
* of special interest
** of outstanding interest
References
(1) L. Guernsey Garbled in translation: getting media research to the press and public J Child Media, 8 (2014), pp. 87-94,
https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2014.863486
(2) E. Kross, P. Verduyn, E. Demiralp, J. Park, D.S. Lee, N. Lin, H. Shablak, J. Jonides, O. Ybarra Facebook use predicts declines in
subjective well-being in young adults PLoS One, 8 (2013), pp. 1-7, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841
(3) C. Sagioglou, T. Greitemeyer Facebooks emotional consequences: why Facebook causes a decrease in mood and why people
still use it Comput Hum Behav, 35 (2014), pp. 359-363, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.003
(4) M. Kalpidou, D. Costin, J. Morris The relationship between Facebook and the well-being of undergraduate college students
Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 14 (2011), pp. 183-189, https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0061
(5) C.J. Blomfield Neira, B.L. Barber Social networking site use: linked to adolescents’ social self-concept, self-esteem, and depressed
mood Aust J Psychol, 66 (2014), pp. 56-64, https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12034
4
(6) L.A. Jelenchick, J.C. Eickhoff, M.A. Moreno ‘Facebook depression?’ Social networking site use and depression in older adolescents
J Adolescent Health, 52 (2013), pp. 128-130, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.05.008
(7) N.B. Ellison, C. Steinfield, C. Lampe The benefits of Facebook ‘friends:’ social capital and college students’ use of online social
network sites J Comput Med Commun, 12 (2007), pp. 1143-1168, https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 1083-6101.2007.00367.x
(8) J. Kim, J.R. Lee The Facebook paths to happiness: effects of the number of Facebook friends and self-presentation on subjective
well-being Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 14 (2011), pp. 359-364, https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0374
(9) S. Valenzuela, N. Park, K.F. Kee Is there social capital in a social network site? Facebook use and college students’ life satisfaction,
trust, and participation J Comput Med Commun, 14 (2009), pp. 875-901, https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 1083-6101.2009.01474.x
(10) R.E. Wilson, S.D. Gosling, L.T. Graham A review of Facebook research in the social sciences Pers Psychol Sci, 7 (2012), pp. 203-
220, https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612442904
(11) A. Nadkarni, S.G. Hofmann Why do people use Facebook? Pers Ind Diff, 52 (2012), pp. 243-249,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.007
(12) ** C.R. Blease Too many Friends, too few Likes? Evolutionary psychology and Facebook Depression Rev Gen Psychol, 19 (2015),
pp. 1-13, https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000030
Annotation: reviews studies linking Facebook use and depression highlighting the role of social comparison. Focus on social rank
theories of depression.
(13) K. Corcoran, J. Crusius, T. Mussweiler Social comparison: motives, standards, and mechanisms D. Charee (Ed.), Theories in Social
Psychology, Wiley (2011), pp. 119-139
(14) N. Haferkamp, N.C. Krämer Social comparison 2.0: examining the effects of online profiles on social-networking sites
Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 14 (2011), pp. 309-314, https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0120
(15) E.A. Vogel, J.P. Rose, L.R. Roberts, K. Eckles Social comparison, social media, and self-esteem Psychol Pop Med Cult, 3 (2014), pp.
206-222, https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000047
(16) J.B. Walther Selective self-presentation in computer-mediated communication: hyperpersonal dimensions of technology,
language, and cognition Comput Hum Behav, 23 (2007), pp. 2538-2557, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2006.05.002
(17) A.H. Jordan, B. Monin, C.S. Dweck, B.J. Lovett, O.P. John, J.J. Gross Misery has more company than people think: underestimating
the prevalence of others’ negative emotions Pers Soc Psychol Bull, 37 (2010), pp. 120-135,
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210390822
(18) R.J. Lee-Won, M. Shim, Y.K. Joo, S.G. Park Who puts the best ‘face’ forward on Facebook? Positive self-presentation in online social
networking and the role of self-consciousness, actual-to-total friends ratio, and culture Comput Hum Behav, 39 (2014), pp. 413-
423, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.08.007
(19) R.H. Smith, S.H. Kim Comprehending envy Psychol Bull, 133 (2007), pp. 46-64, https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.46
(20) * E.C. Tandoc, P. Ferrucci, M. Duffy Facebook use, envy, and depression among college students: is facebooking depressing?
Comput Hum Behav, 43 (2015), pp. 139-146, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.053
Annotation: In this cross-sectional study, Facebook use was not directly associated with depression. However, Facebook use
exhibited a direct and negative correlation with depression when Facebook envy was entered as a mediator. Passive use of
Facebook was associated with more depression for participants with Facebook envy, but less depression for participants without
Facebook envy.
(21) K.F. Hew Students’ and teachers’ use of Facebook Comput Hum Behav, 27 (2011), pp. 662-676,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.20https://doi.org/10.11.020
(22) J. Fox, J.J. Moreland The dark side of social networking sites: an exploration of the relational and psychological stressors
associated with Facebook use and affordances Comput Hum Behav, 45 (2015), pp. 168-176,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.083
(23) H. Krasnova, H. Wenninger, T. Widjaja, P. Buxmann Envy on Facebook: a hidden threat to users’ life satisfaction? Paper presented
at the 11th International Conference on Wirtschaftsinformatik (WI) (2013)
(24) S.Y. Lee How do people compare themselves with others on social network sites? The case of Facebook Comput Hum Behav, 32
(2014), pp. 253-260, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.009
(25) * J. Nesi, M.J. Prinstein Using social media for social comparison and feedback-seeking: gender and popularity moderate
associations with depressive symptoms J Abnorm Child Psychol, 43 (2015), pp. 1427-1438, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-
0020-0
Annotation: Adolescents’ excessive technology-based social comparison and reassurance seeking predicted depression one
year later when controlling for baseline depression and offline reassurance seeking. The only study looking at Facebook envy
and depression in an adolescent sample.
(26) * B.A. Feinstein, R. Hershenberg, V. Bhatia, J.A. Latack, N. Meuwly, J. Davila Negative social comparison on Facebook and
depressive symptoms: rumination as a mechanism Psychol Pop Med Cult, 2 (2013), pp. 161-170,
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033111
Annotation: Found negative social comparison when using Facebook to predict depression scores three weeks later, which was
not true for a general tendency for comparison, including offline comparison. Included rumination as a potential mediator.
Studied effects of social comparison frequency and direction.
5
(27) Y. Cohen-Charash Episodic envy J Appl Soc Psychol, 39 (2009), pp. 2128-2173, https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 1559-
1816.2009.00519.x
(28) B.T. Gold Enviousness and its relationship to maladjustment and psychopathology Pers Indiv Differ, 21 (1996), pp. 311-321,
https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(96)00081-5
(29) R.H. Smith, T.J. Turner, R. Garonzik, C.W. Leach, V. Urch-Druskat, C.M. Weston Envy and schadenfreude Pers Soc Psychol B, 22
(1996), pp. 158-168, https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167296222005
(30) J. Sowislo, U. Orth Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies Psychol Bull, 139
(2013), pp. 213-240, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028931
(31) B.P. Buunk, V.B. Brenninkmeyer Social comparison processes among depressed individuals: evidence for the evolutionary
perspective on involuntary subordinate strategies? L. Sloman, P. Gilbert, Lawrence Erlbaum (Eds.), Subordination and Defeat: An
Evolutionary Approach to Mood Disorders and their Therapy, vol 1 (2000), pp. 7-164
(32) M.N. Steers, R.E. Wickham, L.K. Acitelli Seeing everyone elses highlight reels: how Facebook usage is linked to depressive
symptoms J Soc Clin Psychol, 33 (2014), pp. 701-731, https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2014.33.8.701
(33) * E.A. Vogel, J.P. Rose, B.M. Okdie, K. Eckles, B. Franz Who compares and despairs? The effect of social comparison orientation
on social media use and its outcomes Pers Indiv Differ, 86 (2015), pp. 249-256, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.026
Annotation: Includes two correlational studies showing a positive relationship between a general tendency for comparison and
Facebook use. Moreover, in an experiment, participants high but not low in social comparison orientation experienced drops in
mood, self-esteem, and positive self-views after viewing a friends profile as compared to control participants. Underlines the
importance of both social comparison and individual differences when predicting depression from Facebook use.
(34) H.T.G. Chou, N. Edge They are happier and having better lives than I am: the impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others’
lives Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 15 (2012), pp. 117-121, https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0324
(35) ** P. Verduyn, D.S. Lee, J. Park, H. Shablack, A. Orvell, J. Bayer, O. Ybarra, J. Jonides, E. Kross Passive Facebook usage undermines
affective well-being: experimental and longitudinal evidence J Exp Psychol Gen, 144 (2015), pp. 480-488,
https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000057
Annotation: Combines experimental evidence showing that passive, but not active Facebook browsing leads to reduced well-
being with an experience sampling trial finding similar results. Passive, but not active Facebook use predicted declines in well-
being, which was fully mediated by envy. Lagged analyses suggest a causal path from Facebook use to depressed affect through
envy. Provides an example of how to track Facebook experiences in real life.
(36) G. Gilbert, H. Barton The motivations and personality traits that influence Facebook usage A. Power, G. Kirwan (Eds.),
Cyberpsychology and New Media: A Thematic Reader, Psychology Press (2014), pp. 26-37
(37) J. Fardouly, P.C. Diedrichs, L.R. Vartanian, E. Halliwell Social comparisons on social media: the impact of Facebook on young
womens body image concerns and mood Body Image, 13 (2015), pp. 38-45, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.12.002
(38) * H. Appel, J. Crusius, A.L. Gerlach Social comparison, envy, and depression on Facebook: a study looking at the effects of high
comparison standards on depressed individuals J Soc Clin Psychol, 34 (2015), pp. 277-289,
https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2015.34.4.277
Annotation: Found that attractive Facebook profiles produced more envy than unattractive profiles. This finding was qualified by
a significant interaction with depression, such that depressed participants felt particularly envious in response to the attractive
profile. Highlights special risks for depressed users.
(39) W. Chen, K. Lee Sharing, liking, commenting, and distressed? The pathway between Facebook interaction and psychological
distress Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 16 (2013), pp. 728-734, https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0272
(40) * B.K. Johnson, S. Knobloch-Westerwick Glancing up or down: mood management and selective social comparisons on social
networking site Comput Human Behav, 41 (2014), pp. 33-39, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.009
Annotation: Found that experimentally induced negative mood lead participants to look longer at unattractive and unsuccessful
profiles than participants in a positive mood. This is interpreted as mood repair through flattering social comparison, suggesting
that SNS comparison may also have positive consequences.
(41) M.K. Underwood, L.H. Rosen, D. More, S.E. Ehrenreich, J.K. Gentsch The BlackBerry project: capturing the content of adolescents’
text messaging Dev Psychol, 48 (2012), pp. 295-302, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025914
(42) K. Wise, S. Alhabash, H. Park Emotional responses during social information seeking on Facebook Cyberpsychol Soc Netw, 13
(2010), pp. 555-562, https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0365
(43) S. Shiffman, A.A. Stone, M.R. Hufford Ecological momentary assessment Ann Rev Clin Psychol, 4 (2008), pp. 1-32,
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415
(44) R.W. Robins, K.H. Trzesniewski Self-esteem development across the lifespan Curr Dir Psychol Sci, 14 (2005), pp. 158-162,
https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 0963-7214.2005.00353.x
(45) T. Correa, A.W. Hinsley, H.G. de Zúñiga Who interacts on the web? The intersection of users’ personality and social media use
Comput Hum Behav, 26 (2010), pp. 247-253, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2009.09.003
(46) A.M. Manago, T. Taylor, P.M. Greenfield Me and my 400 friends: the anatomy of college students’ Facebook networks, their
communication patterns, and well-being Dev Psychol, 48 (2012), pp. 369-380, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026338
6
(47) K. Lup, L. Trub, L. Rosenthal Instagram #Instasad? Exploring associations among Instagram use, depressive symptoms, negative
social comparison, and strangers followed Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 18 (2015), pp. 247-252,
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2014.0560
(48) J. Davila, R. Hershenberg, B.A. Feinstein, K. Gorman, V. Bhatia, L.R. Starr Frequency and quality of social networking among young
adults: associations with depressive symptoms, rumination, and corumination Psychol Pop Med Cult, 1 (2012), pp. 72-86,
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027512
(49) S.M. Locatelli, K. Kluwe, F.B. Bryant Facebook use and the tendency to ruminate among college students: testing mediational
hypotheses J Educ Comput Res, 46 (2012), pp. 377-394, 10.2190/EC.46.4.d
(50) A.M. Shaw, K.R. Timpano, T.B. Tran, J. Joormann Correlates of Facebook usage patterns: the relationship between passive
Facebook use, social anxiety symptoms, and brooding Comput Hum Behav, 48 (2015), pp. 575-580,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.003
(51) J. Lange, J. Crusius Dispositional envy revisited: unraveling the motivational dynamics of benign and malicious envy Pers Soc
Psychol Bull, 41 (2015), pp. 284-294, https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000026
(52) N. Van den Ven, M. Zeelenberg, R. Pieters Leveling up and down: the experience of benign and malicious envy Emotion, 3 (2009),
pp. 419-429, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015669
(53) J.M. Burger Desire for control, locus of control, and proneness to depression J Pers, 52 (1984), pp. 71-89,
https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 1467-6494.1984.tb00551.x
(54) J. Lange, J. Crusius The tango of two deadly sins: the socialfunctional relation of envy and pride J Pers Soc Psychol, 109 (2015),
pp. 453-472, https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000026
(55) J.L. Tracy, R.W. Robins Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: a theoretical model Psychol Inq, 15 (2004), pp. 103-125,
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1502_01
(56) M. Choi, E.T. Panek, Y. Nardis, C.L. Toma When social media isn’t social: friends’ responsiveness to narcissists on Facebook Pers
Indiv Differ, 77 (2015), pp. 209-214, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.056
(57) R. Lin, S. Utz The emotional responses of browsing Facebook: happiness, envy, and the role of strength Comput Hum Behav, 52
(2015), pp. 29-38, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.064
(58) P.M. Valkenburg, J. Peter Social consequences of the internet for adolescents: a decade of research Curr Dir Psychol Sci, 18
(2009), pp. 1-5, https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 1467-8721.2009.01595.x
Author Note
Helmut Appel, Applied Social Psychology, University of Cologne, Herbert-Lewin-Str. 10, 50931 Cologne, Germany; e-mail: helmut.ap-
pel@uni-koeln.de
Alexander L. Gerlach, Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Cologne, Pohligstr. 1, 50969 Cologne, Germany; e-mail:
alexander.gerlach@uni-koeln.de
Jan Crusius, Social Cognition Center Cologne, University of Cologne, Richard-Strauss-Str. 2, 50931 Cologne, Germany; e-mail: jan.cru-
sius@uni-koeln.de
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Helmut Appel, University of Cologne, Department of Psychology, Ap-
plied Social Psychology, Herbert-Lewin-Str. 10, 50931 Cologne, Germany; e-mail: helmut.appel@uni-koeln.de, tel.: +49 (0)221 470
6877
... A research concluded the higher usage of Instagram was interconnected with social comparison. In past few years it is observed that social networks /platforms have caused troubles to mental health and it is a sign of worry for most (H & Gerlach A. L, 2016). Also because of the popularity it has expanded, it has augmented the ratio of mental health issues (Stapleton et al., 2017). ...
... As individual receives any information they interpret it and they form an idea and assumption about others around, with in no-time this spreads out. This social networking features and prominent and deep to the grass root levels and has huge impact on individual's life and whereas the offline networks and comparatively weaker and doesn't affects this much (H & Gerlach A. L, 2016). Moreover, the social media helps and impacts people in various ways. ...
Article
Full-text available
Instagram has a very high number of monthly-active users that is more than 400 million, the number of shared faces is around 80 million and nearly about 3.5 likes daily. A high percentage of people post their lives on Instagram so that they can be viewed, seen and admired. This causes people to judge, comment and at times try to copy what they see on Instagram. Thus this comparing causes "mental health issues, anxiety, depression, lowered self-esteem and social-comparison". As social media, Instagram, has become most important part of every individual especially among youth. It is important to study and analyze the impact that it brings among youth and culture it imparts. Instagram have grown rapidly in past few years. Its effects are yet unclear whether it is creating a sense of social-comparison among people and leading to mental health issues. The main idea of carrying out this study is to look for the effect of Instagram usage among individual, social depression/anxiety and comparing themselves with others. We have used the basics of social-comparison theory and Five-system model. We carried out a online-survey for this study (N=404). The whole set of data was transferred to SPSS software of Instagram users. The recent studies affirms and proves that the high usage of social platforms has major consequences as it leads to anxiety, stress and various health related issues. To lower the bad effects the research shows the role that self-esteem and social-comparison can play. The people with high self-esteem and tends to be less effected by social media usage. Thus proposes that to higher self-esteem could be used improve the social-comparison developed among youth. The research is aimed to find whether Instagram usage is leading youth towards social comparisons and to identify the correlation between factors, i.e. mental health and social comparison caused by Instagram usage. Also, it has been observed that social-comparison can cause mental health issues and colorism. As per studies nearly 88% of youth are engaged in social comparisons caused by Instagram and most of these are suffering with upward-comparison. Moreover, this research study suggests that there is a strong relation between Instagram usage and social comparison, causing mental health issues i.e social anxiety and depression. Instagram usage appeared to influence the psychological health of individual, the individuals with more usage faced higher mental issues and with high self-esteem the social comparison effect showed a change. The relationship between Instagram usage and social-comparison was studied and it regulated to extent as it created the direct relation among the users."
... This was a Cross-Sectional study utilizing a Network-Based Digital Recruitment Technique (12) in preparation for South Africa's World TB Day Commemoration in Sedibeng District. This approach utilizes various digital platforms and the inherent social connections within these platforms, employing networkbased sampling techniques where initial participants (seeds) recruit peers from their social networks, thereby creating a chain-referral system (12,13). ...
... This was a Cross-Sectional study utilizing a Network-Based Digital Recruitment Technique (12) in preparation for South Africa's World TB Day Commemoration in Sedibeng District. This approach utilizes various digital platforms and the inherent social connections within these platforms, employing networkbased sampling techniques where initial participants (seeds) recruit peers from their social networks, thereby creating a chain-referral system (12,13). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Tuberculosis (TB) continues to be a public health issue of concern in South Africa. The study aims to investigate the epidemiology of TB screening among people aged 18-64 years in South Africa. Methods This is a cross-sectional study with a study population of 132 adults in 6 provinces in South Africa. Data was collected through an online questionnaire. Outcome variable was self-reported TB screening within the past year. Descriptive statistics, multivariate logistic regression analysis were performed to determine prevalence and factors associated with TB screening, p-value <0.05 considered statistically significant. Results A third 37(38%) participants reported to have been screened for TB. Factors such as gender (AOR 1.54; p=0.490), participants in the age group 26-45 (AOR 7.80; p=0.010), Ever been tested for TB (AOR 23.17; p=0.000), and TB testing yearly (AOR 21.69; p=0.000) were positively associated with TB screening. Participants that had information on TB (AOR 0.20, p=0.031), and those who seek healthcare services (AOR 0.28, p=0.035) were significantly less likely to report TB screening. Conclusion Only a quarter of adults aged 18-64 in South Africa had been screened for TB, with significant associations observed for age, gender, previous TB screening, and recent TB testing. The structural barriers and the need for comprehensive, person-centered screening strategies to meet the WHO's 90% target are needed. The study recommends understanding the low utilization of health services, capacity building for healthcare providers, increasing awareness of TB symptoms, and conducting research on comprehensive TB screening approaches.
... Correspondingly, social media platforms like Facebook can lead to social comparison and feelings of envy due to highlighting idealized lifestyles and achievements. These comparisons may contribute to a sense of inadequacy and dissatisfaction among users and may lead to depression (Appel et al., 2016). ...
... Twitter enables educators to share resources, news, and scholarly articles, facilitating continuous professional development and the sharing of best practices [30]. LinkedIn offers networking opportunities and access to webinars and professional groups that can enrich both teaching and learning experiences [24]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social media has become an integral part of contemporary society, profoundly transforming communication, social behavior, political engagement, and cultural norms. This paper presents a comprehensive literature review exploring the multifaceted impact of social media across these dimensions. The study synthesizes findings from 32 peer-reviewed articles, case studies, and authoritative reports published between 2016 and 2024. The review reveals that social media platforms facilitate global connectivity and foster community building, yet they also pose significant challenges such as superficial connections, social isolation, and the spread of misinformation. In politics, social media has revolutionized campaign strategies and public discourse but has also contributed to polarization and the dissemination of false information. Culturally, social media influences trends, promotes diversity, and shapes societal norms, while also perpetuating issues like cultural appropriation and unrealistic self-presentation. The impact on mental health is particularly complex, with social media providing valuable support networks and mental health resources but also contributing to anxiety, depression, and cyberbullying. In education, social media enhances access to resources and fosters collaboration but can be a source of distraction and academic dishonesty. This paper underscores the need for balanced strategies that promote media literacy, critical thinking, and ethical digital communication to maximize the positive potential of social media while mitigating its negative effects. Future research should focus on developing technological and policy solutions to address these challenges and harness the benefits of social media for societal advancement
... This relativized the previously often found and recently criticized positive effect of active SMU on well-being (Verduyn et al., 2017;Valkenburg et al., 2022). Our research thus joins a series of many papers that clearly foreground the disadvantages of use (cf., Appel et al., 2016;Brailovskaia et al., 2020a, b, c;Ozimek & Bierhoff, 2019;Verduyn et al., 2015Verduyn et al., , 2017. It shows that the risks of using social media seem to outweigh the opportunities that arise from it. ...
Article
Full-text available
Fear of Missing Out is a focus of research in social sciences, especially with respect to social media use. This study examined the extent to which social media use (active and passive, respectively) elicits Fear of Missing Out due to greater perceived bridging and bonding social. In addition, the extent to which Fear of Missing Out affects feelings of stress and life satisfaction was investigated. In an online survey 1230 participants were included. Within the framework of a path model, the assumption of an indirect effect of both active and passive social media use via social capital, Fear of Missing Out, and stress on life satisfaction was confirmed. Finally, further study results as well as implications and limitations of this research are discussed.
... Das ist ein häufiger Befund in der Forschung zu den Effekten digitaler Medien: Nicht die Nutzung ist intrinsisch schädlich, sondern die exzessive Nutzung bestimmter, zum Beispiel nicht-kommunikativer, Plattform-Affordanzen (Przybylski & Weinstein, 2017 (Appel et al., 2016). In Übereinstimmung damit haben wir soziale Aufwärtsvergleiche auf Dating-Apps nachgewiesen und auch gezeigt, dass schädliche Aufwärtsvergleiche mit der Aktivität des exzessiven Durchstöberns von Profilen verbunden war, nicht aber mit der Gesamtnutzungshäufigkeit von Dating-Apps (Thomas et al., 2023a). ...
Article
Full-text available
What distinguishes mobile dating apps from online matchmaking agencies and social networking sites is their swipe-function, meaning that users accept or reject profiles by thumb-brushing right or left. Swiping consists of (a) the chance of receiving peer feedback in the form of matches, (b) cognitively processing an abundance of dating options, and (c) decision-making. Informed by social psychological theories, i. e., sociometer (Leary, 1995), tyranny of choice (Schwartz, 2000), and regulatory mode (Kruglanski, 2000), we examined psychological effects of swiping-specific aspects in six studies (three experimental , two cross-sectional, one longitudinal). Across studies, the frequency of dating app use was unrelated to detrimental outcomes. Excessive swiping, swiping in assessment mode, and evaluating abundant dating options were linked to an increased fear of being single and being overwhelmed with partner choices. Receiving matches induced partner choice overload but failed to decrease fear of being single. In sum, effects depend on how dating app users swipe.
Article
Advertisers typically publish in‐feed ads with two types of authorship: brand or influencer, yet little is known about the effectiveness of in‐feed ads between these two authors. In this study, we investigated the interactions and mechanisms of ad authorship (brand vs. influencer) and brand type (luxury vs. mass) on advertising effectiveness, and tested the moderating effect of upward social comparison based on the stereotype content model. A pilot study, by coding the secondary data from Most Liked WeChat Moment Ads, found that a greater proportion of luxury (vs. mass) brands were authored by brands (vs. influencers). Study 1 used eye tracking technique to identify the interactive effect of ad authorship and brand type on visual attention. Study 2 further identified perceived competence and warmth as mediators. Study 3 verified the moderating effect of upward social comparison on the above effects. This paper contributes to the theoretical literature on in‐feed advertising by showing the interactive effect of advertising authorship and brand types on advertising effectiveness. It also offers valuable insights for luxury or mass brands on strategically leveraging the brand itself or influencer for advertising.
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the relationship between social media use and identity formation among adolescents aged 14 to 21. A quantitative survey (n = 131) assessed social comparison, online identity, and the influence of social media feedback on selfesteem. Results show a significant correlation between social comparison and the importance of online identity. Adolescents who spend more time on social media report a stronger influence of online reactions on their self-esteem. While social media fosters identity exploration, it also encourages validation-seeking behavior. These findings underscore the need for mindful social media use among adolescents.
Article
Full-text available
Assessment in clinical psychology typically relies on global retrospective self-reports collected at research or clinic visits, which are limited by recall bias and are not well suited to address how behavior changes over time and across contexts. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) involves repeated sampling of subjects' current behaviors and experiences in real time, in subjects' natural environments. EMA aims to minimize recall bias, maximize ecological validity, and allow study of microprocesses that influence behavior in real-world contexts. EMA studies assess particular events in subjects' lives or assess subjects at periodic intervals, often by random time sampling, using technologies ranging from written diaries and telephones to electronic diaries and physiological sensors. We discuss the rationale for EMA, EMA designs, methodological and practical issues, and comparisons of EMA and recall data. EMA holds unique promise to advance the science and practice of clinical psychology by shedding light on the dynamics of behavior in real-world settings.
Article
Full-text available
People vary in their tendencies to compare themselves to others, an individual difference variable called social comparison orientation (SCO). Social networking sites provide information about others that can be used for social comparison. The goal of the present set of studies was to explore the relationship between SCO, Facebook use, and negative psychological outcomes. Studies 1a and 1b used correlational approaches and showed that participants high (vs. low) in SCO exhibited heavier Facebook use. Study 2 used an experimental approach and revealed that participants high in SCO had poorer self-perceptions, lower self-esteem, and more negative affect balance than their low-SCO counterparts after engaging in brief social comparisons on Facebook. SCO did not have as strong or consistent effects for participants engaging in control tasks. Results are discussed in the context of extant literature and the impact of social media use on well-being.
Article
Full-text available
On Facebook, users are exposed to posts from both strong and weak ties. Even though several studies have examined the emotional consequences of using Facebook, less attention has been paid to the role of tie strength. This paper aims to explore the emotional outcomes of reading a post on Facebook and examine the role of tie strength in predicting happiness and envy. Two studies – one correlational, based on a sample of 207 American participants and the other experimental, based on a sample of 194 German participants – were conducted in 2014. In Study 2, envy was further distinguished into benign and malicious envy. Based on a multi-method approach, the results showed that positive emotions are more prevalent than negative emotions while browsing Facebook. Moreover, tie strength is positively associated with the feeling of happiness and benign envy, whereas malicious envy is independent of tie strength after reading a (positive) post on Facebook.
Article
Full-text available
Psychologists (and subsequently the media) have defined 'Facebook depression' as the affective result of spending too much time on the social networking site (Selfhout et al., 2009; Kross et al., 2013). Some social psychologists have denied that Facebook is causally implicated in any such negative affect (Jelenchick et al., 2013). This article argues that if we want to understand modern mass media and new social media, we need a better understanding of the (old) psychology bequeathed us by natural selection (Barkow et al., 2012). Disentangling the relationship between social media and depression using evolutionary social competition theories of depression, I argue that the mismatch between current social milieu and the environment of evolutionary adaption affords some predictions about the use of social media as a trigger for mild depression or dysphoria. I hypothesize that users of Facebook may be more susceptible to causal triggers for mild depression under the following (specific) circumstances: (a) the greater the number of 'friends' that the user has online; (b) the greater the time that the user spends reading updates from this wide pool of friends; (c) the user does so regularly; and (d) the content of the updates tends to a bragging nature. I hypothesize that the frequency and the number of displays of higher status cues observed by the user may incur the perception of low relative social value among users (automatically triggering this response). The article concludes with directions for future research on the behavioral and cognitive effects of social media sites such as Facebook.
Article
Full-text available
Envy stems from a social comparison with a superior standard. Its 2 distinct forms are directed at changing this situation in different ways, either by becoming as successful as the envied person (in benign envy) or by lowering the envied person's advantage (in malicious envy). In essence, envy is thus a social phenomenon. Nevertheless, most previous research has focused on its underlying intrapersonal processes, overlooking envy's interpersonal core. In contrast, we show in 6 studies (N = 1,513) that envy and pride are intertwined in a social-functional relationship. Envy and pride often co-occur (Study 1) and pride displays enhance envious feelings (Studies 2 and 3). Specifically, authentic (success attributed to effort) and hubristic pride (success attributed to talent) modulate envious intentions and behavior toward their benign and malicious form (Study 2 to 6). This effect is mediated via liking, perceived prestige, and perceived dominance (Study 4). In accordance with a social-functional approach, the effects emerge only when authentic and hubristic pride are expressed by the superior person and not when the respective information about the superior person's feelings is simply available in the environment (Study 5). These effects are present when participants recall envy situations (Study 1), when they imagine being in a competitive situation (Studies 3, 4, and 5), or when envy is elicited in situ (Studies 2 and 6). Our findings show the value of studying envy as a social phenomenon and open up numerous avenues for research on envy at the interpersonal and intergroup level. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
As the use and influence of social networking continues to grow, researchers have begun to explore its consequences for psychological well-being. Some research suggests that Facebook use can have negative consequences for well-being. Instagram, a photo-sharing social network created in 2010, has particular characteristics that may make users susceptible to negative consequences. This study tested a theoretically grounded moderated meditation model of the association between Instagram use and depressive symptoms through the mechanism of negative social comparison, and moderation by amount of strangers one follows. One hundred and seventeen 18-29 year olds completed online questionnaires containing demographics, frequency of Instagram use, amount of strangers followed on Instagram, the Center for Epidemiological Resources Scale for Depression, and the Social Comparison Rating Scale. Instagram use was marginally positively associated with depressive symptoms, and positive social comparison was significantly negatively associated with depressive symptoms. Amount of strangers followed moderated the associations of Instagram use with social comparison (significantly) and depressive symptoms (marginally), and further significantly moderated the indirect association of Instagram use with depressive symptoms through social comparison. Findings generally suggest that more frequent Instagram use has negative associations for people who follow more strangers, but positive associations for people who follow fewer strangers, with social comparison and depressive symptoms. Implications of negative associations of social networking for people who follow strangers and the need for more research on Instagram use given its increasing popularity are explored.
Article
Full-text available
Social networking sites (SNSs), such as Facebook, provide abundant social comparison opportunities. Given the widespread use of SNSs, the purpose of the present set of studies was to examine the impact of chronic and temporary exposure to social media-based social comparison information on self-esteem. Using a correlational approach, Study 1 examined whether frequent Facebook use is associated with lower trait self-esteem. Indeed, the results showed that participants who used Facebook most often had poorer trait self-esteem, and this was mediated by greater exposure to upward social comparisons on social media. Using an experimental approach, Study 2 examined the impact of temporary exposure to social media profiles on state self-esteem and relative self-evaluations. The results revealed that participants’ state self-esteem and relative self-evaluations were lower when the target person’s profile contained upward comparison information (e.g., a high activity social network, healthy habits) than when the target person’s profile contained downward comparison information (e.g., a low activity social network, unhealthy habits). Results are discussed in terms of extant research and their implications for the role of social media in well-being.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined specific technology-based behaviors (social comparison and interpersonal feedback-seeking) that may interact with offline individual characteristics to predict concurrent depressive symptoms among adolescents. A total of 619 students (57 % female; mean age 14.6) completed self-report questionnaires at 2 time points. Adolescents reported on levels of depressive symptoms at baseline, and 1 year later on depressive symptoms, frequency of technology use (cell phones, Facebook, and Instagram), excessive reassurance-seeking, and technology-based social comparison and feedback-seeking. Adolescents also completed sociometric nominations of popularity. Consistent with hypotheses, technology-based social comparison and feedback-seeking were associated with depressive symptoms. Popularity and gender served as moderators of this effect, such that the association was particularly strong among females and adolescents low in popularity. Associations were found above and beyond the effects of overall frequency of technology use, offline excessive reassurance-seeking, and prior depressive symptoms. Findings highlight the utility of examining the psychological implications of adolescents' technology use within the framework of existing interpersonal models of adolescent depression and suggest the importance of more nuanced approaches to the study of adolescents' media use.
Article
Full-text available
The co-occurrence of depression and envy is both plausible and empirically established. However, little is known about the mechanisms underlying this correlation. An account is proposed according to which low self-esteem in depressed individuals leads to upward social comparison and thus makes envy more likely. This effect should frequently occur in online social networks like Facebook because they allow for easy impression management and hence provide high comparison standards. In a quasi-experimental online study, depressed and non-depressed participants indicated their self-esteem and were then presented with specifically set up Facebook profiles that were either attractive or unattractive. Participants were asked to compare themselves to the profile owner and to report their resulting feelings of inferiority and envy. Depressed participants were more envious, especially after seeing the attractive profile. Envy was associated with higher self-reported inferiority and also correlated negatively with self-esteem. The connection between depression and envy is demonstrated with an experimental elicitation of envy for the first time. The results strongly suggest that low self-esteem and consequent feelings of inferiority play a crucial role in depressed individuals’ elevated levels of envy. Practical implications and limitations are discussed.