Available via license: CC BY-NC-ND 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
36 O.Yu. Mykhailyuk, H.Ya. Pohlod
UDC 811.111:82-83:316.473
doi: 10.15330/jpnu.2.2-3.36-41
Section:
LANGUAGE, CULTURE AND WORLD-VIEW
THE LANGUAGES WE SPEAK AFFECT OUR PERCEPTIONS OF THE
WORLD
O.YU. MYKHAILYUK, H.YA. POHLOD
Abstract. The article presents review of studies on interaction between language and thought and
the way it influences our understanding of the world. This is an interesting and important new
direction for the research on language and thought, as our society becomes increasingly global and
has an increased need for clear communication across languages and cultures. With a better
understanding of how language and thought interact to influence how we understand and
communicate about the world, we may begin to better understand how to communicate across
languages and cultures, in ways beyond simple translation, that allow us to understand the various
nuances of different languages and cultures. All new research shows us that the languages we
speak not only reflect or express our thoughts, but also shape the very thoughts we wish to
express. The structures that exist in our languages profoundly shape how we construct reality, and
help make us as smart and sophisticated as we are.
Keywords: cognitive abilities, cross-linguistic differences, perceive, gender, multilingual.
1. INTRODUCTION
New cognitive research suggests that language profoundly influences the way people see the
world.
Do the languages we speak shape the way we think? Do they merely express thoughts, or do the
structures in languages (without our knowledge or consent) shape the very thoughts we wish to
express? Do English, Spanish, Ukrainian and Japanese speakers end up attending to, understanding,
and remembering their experiences differently simply because they speak different languages?
These questions touch on all the major controversies in the study of mind, with important
implications for politics, law and religion.
The pioneering linguist Benjamin Whorf (1897–1941) grasped the relationship between human
language and human thinking: how language can shape our innermost thoughts. His basic thesis is that
our perception of the world and our ways of thinking about it are deeply influenced by the structure of
the languages we speak.
Scholars have long wondered whether different languages might impart different cognitive
abilities. In recent years empirical evidence for this causal relation has emerged, indicating that one’s
mother tongue does indeed mold the way one thinks about many aspects of the world, including space
and time.
Journal of Vasyl Stefanyk Precarpathian National University
http://jpnu.pu.if.ua
Vol. 2, No. 2-3 (2015), 36-41
The Languages We Speak Affect Our Perceptions of the World 37
The latest findings also hint that language is part and parcel of many more aspects of thought than
scientists had previously realized.
Language is a human gift. When we study language, we are uncovering in part what makes us
human. As we uncover how languages and their speakers differ from one another, we discover that
human natures too can differ dramatically, depending on the languages we speak. The next steps are to
understand the mechanisms through which languages help us construct the incredibly complex
knowledge systems we have. Understanding how knowledge is built will allow us to create ideas that
go beyond the currently thinkable. This research cuts right to the fundamental questions we all ask
about ourselves. How do we come to be the way we are? Why do we think the way we do? An
important part of the answer, it turns out, is in the languages we speak.
Of course, just because people talk differently doesn't necessarily mean they think differently. In the
past decade, cognitive scientists have begun to measure not just how people talk, but also how they
think, asking whether our understanding of even such fundamental domains of experience as space,
time and causality could be constructed by language. But on the other hand, if people learn another
language, they inadvertently also learn a new way of looking at the world. When bilingual people
switch from one language to another, they start thinking differently, too.
Clearly, languages require different things of their speakers. They must attend to and encode
strikingly different aspects of the world just so they can use their language properly.
Believers in cross-linguistic differences counter that everyone does not pay attention to the same
things: if everyone did, one might think it would be easy to learn to speak other languages.
Unfortunately, learning a new language (especially one not closely related to those you know) is never
easy; it seems to require paying attention to a new set of distinctions. Whether it's distinguishing modes
of being in Spanish, evidentiality in Turkish, or aspect in Ukrainian, learning to speak these languages
requires something more than just learning vocabulary: it requires paying attention to the right things
in the world so that you have the correct information to include in what you say.
2. ANALYSIS AND DISCUSSION
Current research examining the relationship between language and thought no longer ask the
question of whether language determines thought, but rather, focus on the ways in which language
influences thought, or vice versa (Boroditsky, 2003). This new focus is aptly referred to as Linguistic
Relativity, and much research has been conducted since Whorf’s time to investigate the effects of
linguistic relativity. Researchers in psychology, linguistics, anthropology, and cognitive science have
asked several types of questions to investigate how language affects thought or thought affects
language. The following are a few of the most popular questions asked in this area of research.
Some studies have proved how languages shape the way we think about space, time, colors, and
objects. Other studies have found effects of language on how people construe events, reason about
causality, keep track of number, understand material substance, perceive and experience emotion,
reason about other people's minds, choose to take risks, and even in the way they choose professions
and spouses Taken together, these results show that linguistic processes are pervasive in most
fundamental domains of thought, unconsciously shaping us from the nuts and bolts of cognition and
perception to our loftiest abstract notions and major life decisions. Language is central to our
experience of being human, and the languages we speak profoundly shape the way we think, the way
we see the world, the way we live our lives.
A few examples of cross-lingustic differences.
DO SPEAKERS OF DIFFERENT LANGUAGES TALK ABOUT TIME DIFFERENTLY?
Even basic aspects of time perception can be affected by language. For example, English speakers
prefer to talk about duration in terms of length (e.g., "That was a short talk," "The meeting didn't take
long"), while Spanish and Greek speakers prefer to talk about time in terms of amount, relying more on
words like "much" "big", and "little" rather than "short" and "long" Theresearch into such basic cognitive
38 O.Yu. Mykhailyuk, H.Ya. Pohlod
abilities as estimating duration shows that speakers of different languages differ in ways predicted by
the patterns of metaphors in their language. (For example, when asked to estimate duration, English
speakers are more likely to be confused by distance information, estimating that a line of greater length
remains on the test screen for a longer period of time, whereas Greek speakers are more likely to be
confused by amount, estimating that a container that is fuller remains longer on the screen.)
In English, people most commonly use words referring to front vs. back to talk about the future vs.
the past. They talk about certain people as being ahead of their time; they look forward to meeting a friend
for lunch tomorrow; they fall behind schedule; and they think about going back in time. In contrast,
speakers of other languages, such as Mandarin Chinese, use two types of time metaphors: (1) front vs.
back metaphors and (2) up vs. down metaphors. Mandarin Chinese speakers use front vs. back
metaphors in ways similar to English speakers; the up vs. down metaphors are reserved for talking
about the order of events. In these up vs. down metaphors, up refers to earlier events and down refers to
later events. Boroditsky (2001) examined whether speakers of English and speakers of Mandarin
Chinese think about time in different ways. Her studies showed that English speakers were faster to
correctly answer questions such as, “Does March come before April?” after seeing a row of items
organized horizontally; on the other hand, Mandarin Chinese speakers were faster at correctly
answering the same questions after seeing a column of items organized vertically. What these data
suggest is that speakers of different languages think about time more easily when they are primed to
think in the direction indicated by the time metaphors used in their language. Boroditsky thus argues
that language shapes how speakers of different languages conceptualize time.
DO SPEAKERS OF DIFFERENT LANGUAGES TALK ABOUT SPACE DIFFERENTLY?
Speakers of English and Dutch use relative spatial terms to describe space (e.g., left, right, front,
back). In contrast, speakers of Tzeltal (a Mayan language spoken in Mexico) use absolute references to
describe space (i.e. uphill, downhill [corresponds roughly to English south/north directions]). A study
by Levinson (1996) examined whether these linguistic differences between Dutch and Tzeltal influence
how speakers of these languages conceptualize space. In this study, speakers of Dutch and Tzeltal were
shown sets of arrows on two different occasions. On the first occasion, they saw two arrows pointing in
the same direction (e.g., two arrows pointing right/north); the participants were then rotated 180
degrees to be shown another pair of arrows: one of which pointed in the same relative direction as the
arrows from the first occasion (e.g., right [the absolute direction being south]), and the other pointed in
the same absolute direction as the arrows from the first occasion (e.g., north [the relative direction being
left]). What Levinson found when asking Dutch and Tzeltal speakers to identify the arrow that was
“like the [arrow] that they saw before,” Dutch speakers selected the arrow that was pointing in the
same relative direction (e.g., the one pointing right [the absolute direction being south]), while Tzeltal
speakers selected the arrow that was pointing in the same absolute direction (e.g., north [the relative
direction being left]). Thus, Levinson concludes that Dutch and Tzeltal speakers appear to be
influenced by their respective native languages in understanding spatial orientation.
DO SPEAKERS OF DIFFERENT LANGUAGES CATEGORIZE OBJECTS IN DIFFERENT WAYS?
Languages such as Spanish, French, and German have what is called grammatical gender, where
words (especially nouns) are assigned gender. For example, in Spanish, the word for apple is feminine,
whereas the word for dog is masculine. Other languages such as English or Japanese do not assign
gender to words and therefore do not have grammatical gender. Researchers have thus asked whether
speakers of grammatically gendered languages categorize objects in different ways from speakers of
languages without grammatical gender. Sera, Berge, and del Castillo Pintado (1994) examined this
question, by asking English- and Spanish-speaking children and adults to assign a female or male voice
to various objects. Their study revealed that Spanish-speakers assigned gendered voices to the objects
in ways that matched the Spanish grammatical gender of the word for the object (e.g., an apple would
be assigned a woman’s voice because the word for apple in Spanish is feminine). In contrast, English-
speakers assigned gendered voices to all the objects at random. This study thus provides evidence in
support of the idea that language influences how speakers categorize objects.
The Languages We Speak Affect Our Perceptions of the World 39
Unlike English, most European languages assign a gender (feminine, masculine, or neuter) to
almost every single noun. There is little relation between this so called grammatical gender and the
biological gender.
What it means for a language to have grammatical gender is that words belonging to different
genders get treated differently grammatically and words belonging to the same grammatical gender get
treated the same grammatically. Languages can require speakers to change pronouns, adjective and
verb endings, possessives, numerals, and so on, depending on the noun's gender. For example, to say
something like "my watch was old" in Ukrainian (mij hodynnyk buv staryj), you'd need to make every
word in the sentence agree in gender with "watch" (krislo), which is masculine in Ukrainian. So you'd
use the masculine form of "my", "was", and "old." These are the same forms you'd use in speaking of a
biological male, as in "my grandfather was old." If, instead of speaking of a chair, you were speaking of
a book (knyha), which is feminine in Ukrainian , or about your grandmother, you would use the
feminine form of "my", "was", and "old."
When a noun describes a woman or a female animal, they are usually a she, e.g. die Frau (she-
woman), la chatte (she-cat), when it describes a man or a male animal, it is usually a he, e.g. der Mann
(he-man), le chat (he-cat), but even this rule cannot be taken for granted. For example, das Mädchen
means “girl” in German, but it is an ‘it’ because all German -chen nouns are ‘it’; la girafe is a she in
french even if you speak about a male giraffe.
Whether a thing is a she, he, or it is mostly random. A bridge is a she in German (die Brücke) but a
he in Spanish (el puente).
Does treating watches as masculine and book as feminine in the grammar make Ukrainian speakers
think of watches as being more like men and books as more like women in some way? It turns out that
it does. In one study, scientists asked German and Spanish speakers to describe objects having opposite
gender assignment in those two languages. The descriptions they gave differed in a way predicted by
grammatical gender. For example, when asked to describe a "key" — a word that is masculine in
German and feminine in Spanish — the German speakers were more likely to use words like "hard",
"heavy", "jagged", "metal", "serrated", and "useful", whereas Spanish speakers were more likely to say
"golden", "intricate", "little", "lovely", "shiny", and "tiny." To describe a "bridge", which is feminine in
German and masculine in Spanish, the German speakers said "beautiful", "elegant", "fragile", "peaceful",
"pretty", and "slender", and the Spanish speakers said "big", "dangerous", "long", "strong", "sturdy", and
"towering." This was true even though all testing was done in English, a language without grammatical
gender. The same pattern of results also emerged in entirely nonlinguistic tasks (e.g., rating similarity
between pictures). And we can also show that it is aspects of language per se that shape how people
think: teaching English speakers new grammatical gender systems influences mental representations of
objects in the same way it does with German and Spanish speakers. Apparently even small flukes of
grammar, like the seemingly arbitrary assignment of gender to a noun, can have an effect on people's
ideas of concrete objects in the world.
In fact, you don't even need to go into the lab to see these effects of language; you can see them with
your own eyes in an art gallery. Look at some famous examples of personification in art — the ways in
which abstract entities such as death, sin, victory, or time are given human form. How does an artist
decide whether death, say, or time should be painted as a man or a woman? It turns out that in 85
percent of such personifications, whether a male or female figure is chosen is predicted by the
grammatical gender of the word in the artist's native language. So, for example, German painters are
more likely to paint death as a man, whereas Russian painters are more likely to paint death as a
woman.
DO SPEAKERS OF DIFFERENT LANGUAGES PERCEIVE COLOR IN DIFFERENT WAYS?
Different languages around the world have different numbers of words for colors. Some languages
have as little as two basic color terms (e.g., the Dani of Irian Jaya); other languages have five (e.g., the
Berinmo of Papua New Guinea); other languages historically only have a handful of basic color terms
but have since added new color terms (e.g., Japanese); and other languages have as many as twelve
40 O.Yu. Mykhailyuk, H.Ya. Pohlod
basic color terms (e.g., Russian). Differences in the number of color terms across languages has led
researchers to ask whether speakers of different languages perceive color boundaries differently. Take
for example, a rainbow: a rainbow is a spectrum of color with no distinct boundaries between the
colors, but in the United States, we perceive the rainbow as being comprised of seven colors (i.e. red,
orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, and violet). As there are no specific color boundaries in a rainbow,
would speakers of different languages with different numbers of color terms perceive a rainbow as
being comprised of a different number of colors? Davidoff, Davies and Roberson (1999) examined how
color boundaries are perceived by English-speakers and the Berinmo of Papua New Guinea, by asking
them to provide a name for 160 different colors on the color spectrum. In this task, English-speakers
reported eight basic colors categories, while the Berinmo reported five basic color categories.
Furthermore, where on the color spectrum the English-speakers and the Berinmo differentiated blues
from greens were markedly different. Results from this study suggest that language influences how
speakers perceive color boundaries.
In other words, the influence of language isn't so much on what we can think about, or even what
we do think about, but rather on how we break up reality into categories and label them. And in this,
our language and our thoughts are probably both greatly influenced by our culture.
3. CONCLUSIONS
Nearly everyone who is multilingual talks about languages changing the way they think, affecting
their personality. Each language is another personality as each language has its own distinctive way of
expressing ideas. Varying grammar structures force the speaker to rethink how they emphasise certain
ideas, and words can have different etymologies which, even if only on a subconscious level, affects the
associations you have with them. As a result, an important part of language learning is embracing these
personality changes and being comfortable with them. Experience promts that people who are ‘fluent’
in a language are generally those who adopt the mentality of a speaker of a different language, their
delivery improves, along with their grammar, pronunciation, and of course confidence.
So, can learning a different language change the way we think?
The answer to this question is complicated. To some extent, it's a chicken-and-egg question:
a) Are you unable to think about things you don't have words for, or do you lack words for
them because you don't think about them? Part of the problem is that there is more involved than just
language and thought; there is also culture. Your culture—the traditions, lifestyle, habits, and so on that
you pick up from the people you live and interact with—shapes the way you think, and also shapes the
way you talk.
b) Are cultural practices a product of language, or is language a product of cultural practices?
No matter what the case may be, learning another language is the best way to break free from whatever
psychological shackles your mother tongue has placed on you.
What is clear from the vast research literature on language and thought is that language and
thought seem to interact in some way to influence how we understand and communicate about the
world.
Moreover, if the new language is very different from your own, it may give you insight into another
culture and another way of life.
The Languages We Speak Affect Our Perceptions of the World 41
REFERENCES
[1] Levinson S.C., Wilkins D.P. (Eds.) Grammars of Space: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge
University Press, New York, 2006.
[2] Borodistky L. Linguistic Relativity. In: Nadel L. (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. UK: MacMillan
Press, London, 2003, 917-921.
[3] Casasanto D. et al. How Deep Are Effects of Language on Thought? Time Estimation in Speakers of
English, Indonesian Greek, and Spanish. In: Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the
Cognitive Science Society, 2004, 575–580.
[4] Birner B. (Ed) Does the Language I Speak Influence the Way I Think? Linguistic Society of America,
Washington, DC.
[5] How Deep Are Effects of Language on Thought? Time Estimation in Speakers of English and Greek. In:
Birner B. (Ed) Does the Language I Speak Influence the Way I Think? Linguistic Society of America,
Washington, DC.
[6] Boroditsky L. Does Language Shape Thought? English and Mandarin Speakers' Conceptions of Time.
Cognitive Psychology, 43 (1) (2001), 1–22.
[7] Barrett L.F. et al. Language as a Context for Emotion Perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11 (2007).
[8] Whorf B.L. Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. MIT Press, Cambridge,
MA, 1956.
Address: O.Yu. Mykhailyuk, H.Ya. Pohlod, Vasyl Stefanyk Precarpathian National University, 57,
Shevchenko Str., Ivano-Frankivsk, 76000, Ukraine.
E-mail: myksana@gmail.com.
Received: 14.03.2015; revised: 04.06.2015.
_____________________
Михайлюк О.Ю., Поглод Г.Я. Мови, якими ми розмовляємо, впливають на наше світосприйняття.
Журнал Прикарпатського університету імені Василя Стефаника, 2 (2-3) (2015), 36–41.
У статті проведено огляд взаємодії мови і мислення та шляхів впливу мови на наше
світобачення. Важливість та науковий інтерес до цього напрямку дослідження мови і мислення
обґрунтовано зростанням глобалізації суспільства та потреб у чіткій міжмовній та міжкультурній
комунікації. Із кращим розумінням взаємодії та взаємовпливів мови і мислення та комунікації з
довкіллям пов’язане і покращення розуміння комунікації між мовами і культурами, яка виходить за
межі простого перекладу. Це дозволяє адекватно розуміти численні змістові нюанси різних мов і
культур. Новітні дослідження засвідчують, що мови, якими ми розмовляємо, не лише виражають чи
відображають, але й значною мірою формують висловлювану думку. Структури, чинні у наших
мовах, на глибинному рівні формують наше конструювання реальності та допомагають
удосконалювати власні інтелектуальні ресурси.
Ключові слова: когнітивні здібності, міжкультурні відмінності, сприйняття, гендер,
багатомовність.