Content uploaded by Sebastian Paul Suggate
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Sebastian Paul Suggate on Jun 08, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=recr20
Download by: [University of Regensburg] Date: 29 April 2016, At: 04:55
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal
ISSN: 1350-293X (Print) 1752-1807 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/recr20
The Parable of the Sower and the long-term
effects of early reading
Sebastian P. Suggate
To cite this article: Sebastian P. Suggate (2015) The Parable of the Sower and the long-term
effects of early reading, European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 23:4, 524-544,
DOI: 10.1080/1350293X.2015.1087154
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2015.1087154
Published online: 26 Oct 2015.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 74
View related articles
View Crossmark data
The Parable of the Sower and the long-term effects of early
reading
Sebastian P. Suggate
ab
*
a
Department of Education, Alanus University of Arts and Social Sciences, Alfter, Germany;
b
Department of Education, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
Previous work on the long-term effects of early reading focuses on whether children
can read early (i.e. capability) not on whether this is beneficial (i.e. optimality).The
Luke Effect is introduced to predict long-term reading development as a function of
when children learn to read. A review of correlational, intervention, and
comparative research suggests that early reading shows only short-term effects
for reading skills that later wash out. Based on different developmental
trajectories of language and word recognition skills (WRS), it is hypothesised
that: (1) children who learn to read later acquire WRS more readily; and (2)
acquiring WRS earlier does not improve language development. Thus, reading
instruction falling ‘on good soil [produces] a hundred times as much grain’
(Luke, 8:8), but early reading skills do not improve the soil, simply withering
until children’s language is more developed.
Keywords: decoding skill; reading development; age; instruction; language;
reading comprehension
A farmer went out to sow his seed. As he was scattering the seed, some fell along the path;
it was trampled on, and the birds of the air ate it up. Some fell on rock, and when it came
up, the plants withered because they had no moisture. Other seed fell among thorns, which
grew up with it and choked the plants. Still other seed fell on good soil. It came up and
yielded a crop, a hundred times more than was sown. (Luke, 8: 5–8)
In the Parable of the Sower from the Gospel of Luke, the right conditions have to be in
place before an abundant harvest is reaped. Applying the analogy to child development
and education, we teach children certain skills because we hope that the seeds of skills
and knowledge that we plant will blossom into flower and then fruit, so that children
may in turn bestow these fruit on society. Moreover, we seek to sow the skills in the
right season when children are best able to benefit from them. To illustrate through
extremes, we would consider it futile to instruct appropriate use of the subjunctive
tense before children can form subject-verb-object sentences or we do not ask children
to learn the rules of driving long before they are legally able to drive. Here I coin the
Luke Effect to reflect the developmental principle of teaching the right skills in the right
season, so that children can derive long-lasting benefit.
The purpose of this article is to apply this principle of the Luke Effect to when
children learn to read. Although neglected (Dollase 2007), this question has a huge
© 2015 EECERA
*Email: sebastian.suggate@alanus.edu
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 2015
Vol. 23, No. 4, 524–544, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2015.1087154
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
importance in Europe in educational policy, psychological research and parenting
practice –particularly amidst growing pressure on children (Hyson et al. 1991) and the
perceived loss of childhood (House 2011). Although arguments have been made based
on Piagetian, Montessouri, Steiner, or other philosophical perspectives here the intention
is to build primarily upon contemporary and empirical reading-based evidence.
In the first section, I introduce the key terms on what is involved in learning to read
and what the hopeful outcome is. Then I turn to consider a distinction between capa-
bility and optimality with respect to the question of a right season to introduce reading
instruction. After reviewing relevant evidence, I conclude that there is an optimal
season –albeit broad and likely dependent on individual child and contextual factors
approximated by age (Rutter 1989) - and outline a developmental model to specify
the conditions for the optimal season to learn reading.
Defining key terms
To reduce this article’s scope, discussion falls specifically on whether early reading
instruction might improve later reading comprehension and language development.
Unfortunately, this entails directing the scope away from the many important factors
enabling success at reading, such as engagement in reading, motivation, and academic
self-concept (Chapman and Tunmer 1995; Morgan and Fuchs 2007; Sonnenschein and
Munsterman 2002; Wang and Guthrie 2004).
The optimal season for reading skill acquisition
A key premise of this article is that there is an optimal season to begin learning to read.
Based on the view that skilled reading involves the development of language and word
recognition skills (WRS) (e.g. Gough and Tunmer 1986; Storch and Whitehurst 2002)
that initially develop from different experiences (Suggate, Schaughency, and Reese
2011), I suggest that this season must heavily relate to the development of these two
factors. Thus, I conceptualise the optimal season as being: (1) when WRS (i.e. learning
to read) can be learnt most effortlessly; (2) in harmony with and not in place of activities
fostering language, so long as this occurs in time for: (3) children to optimally derive
benefit from having reading skills over and above language skills for their further learn-
ing (e.g. from reading science or history texts). Theoretically, if this relation were
plotted as a figure it would approximate an inverse parabola because instruction
begun before the right season would be time consuming and may reduce time for
other important activities, such as language learning. However, if left too late then chil-
dren would be deprived of important learning opportunities arising from possessing
good reading skills.
Clearly, this optimisation depends on at least two factors –on the individual child’s
capability to acquire WRS and on the learning opportunities available to children, be
they reading or language based. Therefore, educators would ideally need to always con-
sider both individual (e.g. child language skills, eagerness, home environment) and
contextual differences (e.g. low presence of learning resources, second language learn-
ing) in determining what is optimal. A noteworthy contextual factor is that the different
orthographical features of languages affect reading development (Seymour, Aro,
Erskine, and COST Action Network 2003). To reduce the complexity of this work, I
focus on children learning English and speaking this at home, although the theory pre-
sented could also be applied to other European languages.
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 525
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Defining early and later while avoiding blanket ages
Given that the optimal season might vary greatly between children due to individual
differences in aptitude and environment, it is difficult to define early and later in a sat-
isfactory blanket age manner. Accordingly, I use these ‘blanket’terms of early and later
and their corresponding references only in the sense that a statistical mean is reported in
research papers –under the premise that the reader understand that the mean is an
approximation for a larger sample, but that there are many exceptions on either side
of the mean. With this caveat in mind, the age range in which children receive
formal reading instruction in developed countries varies from around age four in the
UK or academically-focused preschools, to age seven in Sweden, Bulgaria, Brazil,
the Baltic states, and Russia, for example (Huebler 2007). To restrict considerations
to the age ranges in available data, ‘early’refers to ages four and five (preschool and
kindergarten in the US, and preschool and primary school in much of the former
British Commonwealth) and ‘later’refers to ages six and seven. Thus, the difference
between early and later is, on average, around two years.
Defining word recognition, language, and reading comprehension skills
The term WRS is preferred over decoding because the latter often refers to sounding-
out or letter-sound decoding skills, whereas it is necessary here to include a broader
term that acknowledges other more ‘non-decoding’processes operating in text deci-
phering (e.g. sight-word reading, chunked word-stem recognition, set for variability).
These WRS are restricted to those directly involving text, such as alphabet knowledge,
letter-sound correspondences, word-decoding, and –to the extent that this is possible
without top-down language skills –reading fluency skills (Adams 1990; Lonigan
and Shanahan 2008). Language encompasses vocabulary, phonology, semantics,
syntax, and pragmatics (Snow, Burns, and Griffin 1998).
It is generally accepted that the purpose of reading (i.e. engaging in the activity of
reading) is to read to comprehend and in an educational sense to learn from the text,
without precluding that the activity of reading has other purposes (e.g. to practice
reading itself, to convey meaning to a third person, for enjoyment). Reading compre-
hension in its most simple form is defined as being able to read a given text fluently
and understand to a reasonable level what was read (Stuart, Stainthorp, and Snowling
2008). However, at a level that more closely adheres to the goal of reading allowing
children to derive profit from WRS, reading comprehension becomes skilled
reading. Skilled readers not only exhibit mastery of WRS and can derive meaning
from sentences containing vocabulary items already in their lexicons, but they can
also infer and acquire meanings of unfamiliar words (Swanborn and de Glopper
1999), reflect upon the meaning of text, and acquire knowledge from reading (Biemiller
2006). Thereby, skilled readers derive profit from engaging in the activity of skilled
reading, with this then improving language, non-language, and reading comprehension
skills.
The past, present and future: from capability to optimality
Historically, much debate on early reading centred on whether children were ‘ready’to
read. Thus, in an early empirical study Morphett and Washburne (1931) explored pro-
gress in reading across grade 1 as a function of mental age and concluded that children
526 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
should learn to read at age 6.5 years based on their progress. However, this conclusion
was challenged by Gates (1937), who found that the necessary mental age depended to
a large extent on the reading programme; children as young as five could learn to read if
the programme was appropriately tailored.
From there –at the risk of oversimplification –the debate on mental requisites
shifted in two directions, although still focusing on the capability to read early. In
the first instance, the inadequacy of chronological age as an indicator of when to
learn to read was demonstrated, primarily in research on school readiness and school
entry age (e.g. Morrison, Griffith, and Alberts 1997). In the second instance, focus
shifted toward developing programmes that would teach children early reading skills
(e.g. Ehri et al. 2001). A further breach from the readiness perspective was taken in
emergent literacy, which sees a conglomeration of pre-reading factors as influencing
later reading (Whitehurst and Lonigan 2001) and these begin developing long before
children were typically considered ready (e.g. age 6.5, Morphett and Washburne 1931).
However, at this junction I wish to introduce a second facet to the consideration that
of optimality –for it may well be possible to learn to read early but it is important to
know what the benefits of doing so are. Indeed, in this respect the concluding words
of Gates (1937) over eight decades later are still relevant:
Finally, it should be made clear that the results presented in this report do not answer the
question: At what age is it best to introduce reading to pupils? Although the data seem to
indicate that it is possible to organise materials and methods to teach children to learn to
read at a mental age of 5.0 or higher, they do not, in any way, imply that it is desirable to
do so. …It would be necessary to determine the general educational, personal, and social
effects of introducing reading at different stages by methods so well adjusted to the pupils
that they would all learn to read successfully. (p. 508, emphasis in original)
In other words, the question of what effects early versus later reading has requires
making a fine distinction between what can be taught to children early (i.e. capability),
versus whether children’s long-term reading achievement is bettered by doing so (i.e.
optimality).
Evidence and arguments for early reading
At the heart of the arguments for children learning to read early are two lines of evi-
dence. The first concerns developmental continuity, and the second, the effectiveness
of early reading intervention.
Developmental continuity
Developmental continuity is the observation that children’s relative reading perform-
ance remains stable over time (Lonigan and Phillips 2012) from kindergarten into
elementary school (e.g. Hecht et al. 2000; Juel 1988; Kirby, Parrila, and Pfeiffer
2003; National Early Literacy Panel 2008). However, environmental factors operating
to lead to the initial advantage in reading skills are also likely to continue to drive sub-
sequent reading skill development (Suggate 2009,2011,2012). Second, children’s
intellectual and language aptitude is also unlikely to change drastically over time, so
this also may drive developmental continuity, not the fact that they could read earlier
per se. Third, the simple demonstration of developmental continuity between early
and later reading skills misrepresents the complex picture arising from correlational
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 527
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
research. Importantly, few correlational studies are conducted over a long period of
time and when these are, correlations amongst WRS decrease over time (e.g. Blatchford
and Plewis 1990; Hemphill and Tivnan 2008) suggesting that other factors come into
play. Moreover, over time correlational studies show that language becomes an impor-
tant predictor of reading skill (Cunningham and Stanovich 1997; Hemphill and Tivnan
2008; Kirby et al. 2003; Storch and Whitehurst 2002; Vellutino et al. 2007).
Finally, correlation coefficients in this context essentially represent a relative rank
indicating that children retain their scores relative to other children over time. They do
not indicate whether having the one skill at one point in time was necessary for receiv-
ing a particular score later on, which instead requires comparative research designs (as
are reviewed later). To drive the analogy to the extreme, correlational research might
simply allow us to conclude that our education systems do not have the effect of
taking the best readers and turning them into the worst (Suggate 2011).
Intervention research
Turning now to consider intervention research, much evidence exists showing mean-
ingful short-term effects of early reading intervention, usually with moderate effect
sizes (d≈.50) (Bus and van Ijzendoorn 1999;Ehrietal.2001;Ehrietal.2001). Unfortu-
nately, well-controlled studies of early reading intervention (i.e. conducted in preschool or
kindergarten) including follow-up beyond elementary school are largely missing, with the
result that estimates of long-term effects are inferred from studies with a short-term or
medium follow-up. Effect size estimates from immediately post-intervention to follow-
up around 12 to 18 months later indicate decreases in effect size over time (e.g. from
around d= .16 to .36). Crucially, only about 10% of original studies actually report
follow-up data (Bus and van Ijzendoorn 1999;Ehrietal.2001;Ehrietal.2001;
Suggate 2010), suggesting publication bias may be at work (Hunter and Schmidt 2004).
Moreover reading intervention studies specifically manipulate WRS development
but do not compare early WRS environments with equally rich and stimulating environ-
ments fostering, for example, language –often because intervention studies typically
compare different reading programmes with one another. Therefore, early intervention
research provides answers about how to facilitate early acquisition of WRS but
determining the long-term effect that the early acquisition of WRS has, in comparison
to stimulating non-WRS environments, necessitates a different line of comparative
long-term research.
Evidence comparing early versus later reading over the long-term
Ages three to five: evidence from preschool programmes
A large body of evidence suggests that a well-designed preschool or home intervention
programme can boost the later cognitive and social development of children raised in
poverty or otherwise at-risk environments (e.g. Gilliam and Zigler 2001; Lazar and
Darlington 1982; Magnusson, Ruhm, and Waldfogel 2007; Nelson, Westhaus, and
MacLeod 2003; Schweinhart and Weikart 1988). Unfortunately, much of this research
does not include reading outcomes reported separately (e.g. Becker and Gerstein 1982;
Gray and Klaus 1970; Lee et al. 1990; Meyer 1984; Miller and Bizzell 1983; Sprigle
and Schaefer 1985; Wasik et al. 1990). Most crucially, this research often compares
a well designed preschool programme with lower quality or no care. Thereby this evi-
dence does not isolate the issue of whether early development of WRS in comparison to
528 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
a similarly rich but non-reading programme leads to long-term advantages in reading. A
summary of studies containing an early reading component are described in Table 1.
In Table 1, only one study found evidence of an advantage in reading that reduced
over the medium-term from kindergarten to grade 1. However, this advantage existed
only for the at-risk subsample attending a preschool programme and this was associated
with greater externalising behaviour difficulties (Magnusson et al. 2007). Importantly,
most studies in Table 1 failed to show a reading advantage even though the children
were at-risk.
Ages four to seven: evidence from school entry age in international studies
There appear to be two published quantitative international analyses of reading as a
function of school entry age. Elley’s(1992) study suggested that, both before and
after controlling for social and demographic factors, by age nine differences between
children starting early and later had largely disappeared, although he did not replicate
his analysis with the 14-year-old sample. To address this limitation, Suggate (2009)
re-analysed data from the 2006 Programme for International Student Assessment
(PISA), investigating 15-year-olds’reading achievement across 55 countries. After
controlling for similar social and economic factors as Elley (1992), there was no differ-
ence in reading achievement as a function of when children entered school. Impor-
tantly, neither Elley (1992) nor Suggate (2009) accounted adequately for the
orthographic-depth or complexity of syllable structure across different languages.
Ages four to seven: evidence from school entry age in within-culture studies
To better account for orthographic features of English, research comparing reading
development from schools adopting different curriculum is insightful. Crucially, chil-
dren in also publically funded Steiner (also called Waldorf) schools in New Zealand
do not typically enter school until the year they turn seven and state school pupils
receive intensive reading instruction and support such that most read connected text flu-
ently by age six according to scores on graded readers (McNaughton, Phillips, and
MacDonald 2000). Suggate et al. (2013) compared reading development for 287 chil-
dren from state and Steiner schools across the first six years of school. Accounting for
receptive vocabulary, home literacy environment, school reading instruction, reading-
self concept, parental education and income, community economic status, second
language proficiency, and ethnicity, reading fluency and comprehension indicated
that the later starters had caught up by age 11 and, in a second part to this study,
they even overtook early starters in reading comprehension by age 12.
In Montessori education, learning is largely led by children’s individual initiative
with teachers there to direct and support children in activities. Interestingly, it is
often the case that children learn to read early under these conditions, around age
4. In a cross-sectional design carried out in the US, Lillard and Else-Quest (2006)
recruited children aged either five or 12 (N= 112) who were attending state versus
Montessori schools, with selection into the Montessori school occurring via a lottery.
The five-year-old children had greater reading skills, however this difference was not
observed in the 12-year-old sample, suggesting that the early advantage for reading
skill in Montessori education disappeared.
In a study on the long-term effect of early schooling and therewith early reading,
Kern and Friedman (2009) looked at a range of lifelong outcomes, including educational
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 529
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Table 1. Summary of selected preschool research studies including a word recognition skills component and concordant outcome measures.
Authors Year Description Design Sample Outcome Comments
Evans 1985 Looked at DI vs HS (vs no
PS)
Quasi, test of PS
programme
N= 44 (20), AR Similar G6 and G8 RA Attrition of 52%, small
sample.
Durkin 1974–75 Two-year additional PS
reading programme from
age four school from age
six.
Quasi, IQ controlled,
age four vs six
NA, N= 67 By G3-4 no
differences in RA
Reading instruction
initially whole
word, then phonics.
Magnusson
et al.
2007 Large longitudinal study
with naturally occurring
variation in PS care
Quasi, HS, PS, pre-
K, parental care,
other non-parental
care
N= 10,244, NA and
AR, fairly
representative
> RA in K but reduced
by G1. RA
advantage persists
more for AR
PS associated with >
externalising
behaviour
difficulties
Marcon 2002 Compared DI vs child-
initiated vs mixture
Quasi, PS & K AR, N= 169 G4
and N=183 G5
RA similar in G4 and
G5
Attrition of 36%,
similar across PSs
Reynolds 1994 PS with emphasis on reading
readiness and affective
development (vs
traditional kindergarten).
PS and K
intervention vs
none, quasi
N= 398, AR RA identical G3-5 Evidence of > RA for
PS int. when this ran
for 2 years in school
Schmerkotte 1978 Government test effects of
lower SEA. Academic K
vs non-academic K.
Quasi, age 5 vs. 6 100 classes with up
to 25 children in
each, NA
By G2 no differences
in RA
After study,
government did not
lower SEA
Schweinhart &
Weikart
1997 High/scope vs DI (vs
traditional nursery school)
RA N= 45 (68), AR Similar academic
achievement.
High/scope similar to
traditional nursery
school
Note: SEA = school entry age, Quasi = quasi experimental design, RA = reading age, PS = preschool, K = kindergarten, G = grade, AR = at risk sample, NA = normally
achieving, RA = reading achievement, int. = intervention, DI = direct instruction, HS = Head Start.
530 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
achievement, midlife adjustment and mortality. The participants were gifted (IQ > 135)
and, on average, aged 12 in 1922 when they entered the study. Parents provided retro-
spective information on when the children had learned to read and when they entered
school. Although early school entry and learning to read earlier were initially associated
with positive factors (i.e. higher IQ and family income), early reading only resulted in
short-term educational success and in the long-term it was associated with lower edu-
cational achievement and poorer teenage and adult adjustment. In addition to being
linked with lower overall achievement and adjustment, earlier school entry (when the
parents opted to send the child a year earlier than they could have) was linked to
earlier death, even after controlling for a host of personality and developmental variables
(Kern and Friedman 2009). Aside from the Magnusson et al.’s(2007) study in Table 1,
this is the first study that has found links between earlier reading and later maladjust-
ment. Interestingly, by virtue of including a gifted sample, the findings emphasise a
tenet of this article –that capability may not be optimality –because the children
were likely perfectly able to learn to read in that they were gifted.
Summary of evidence: the null hypothesis upheld
Traditionally, correlational and intervention research has been used to argue for early
reading. However, correlational evidence is open to many interpretations and interven-
tion research is too short-term and often lacks an appropriate language-rich control
group. Much quasi-experimental evidence exists, from preschool studies, international
studies, and comparative quasi-experimental studies. There is strong and converging
data to suggest that early reading does not lead to greater skilled reading later on,
despite initial short-term improvements.
These findings cannot easily be dismissed with the mantra absence of evidence is
not evidence of absence because the studies initially show evidence of an effect that
later shows an absence of an effect. Rather, these findings ought be interpreted and
upheld in light of one of the pillars of positivistic scientific enquiry: an intervention
has no effect unless empirically demonstrated to do so. With regards the idea of an
optimal season, given that the early advantage washes out, it can be said –according
to the definition provided earlier for what is optimal –that teaching before age six or
seven is not optimal. We do not have any evidence on whether, for example, age
eight is more or less optimal than age seven.
Building a developmental model of reading
Next I propose a theoretical model on the conditions under which learning to read has
long-term benefits for skilled reading. I first outline that skilled reading is built on word
recognition and language skills and discuss several features with respect to skill con-
straint and dual constraint. Based on this nuanced developmental view, I formulate
two hypotheses which lead to a conceptualisation of the Luke Effect as it relates to
reading development.
Two components of reading
Decades of research has identified that a large part of variance in reading comprehen-
sion development can be explained by two strands of skills, namely WRS and language
(e.g. Biemiller 2006; Scarborough 2001; Sénéchal, LeFevre, and Smith-Chant 2001;
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 531
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Storch and Whitehurst 2002). The importance of these two strands of skills is empha-
sised in the Simple View of Reading (Gough and Tunmer 1986), which proposes that
reading is the product of decoding and comprehension. The Simple View of Reading
has received reasonable support (e.g. Kendeou, Savage, and van den Broek 2009;
Kirby and Savage 2008; Stuart et al. 2008) and has a high conceptual appeal and
face validity (Kirby and Savage 2008). Indeed, other cognitive factors mediate and/
or explain additional variance in skilled reading (e.g. Ouellette and Beers 2010;
Conners 2009; Kershaw and Schatschneider 2012; Tilstra et al. 2009) but word recog-
nition and language are two key aspects.
Skill and developmental constraints in reading development
Dual-constraint in reading development
An important consequence of reading depending heavily on language and WRS is that a
dual-constraint is thereby placed on reading comprehension. Superior language devel-
opment in the absence of superior word-recognition skill development and vice versa
can logically lead only to reading at the level of the lesser developed of these two
sets of skills. The dual-constraint nature of reading is recognised in dyslexia and hyper-
lexia (deficit in word-recognition, deficit in reading comprehension, respectively,
Tunmer and Greaney 2009). Thus, good WRS in the absence of good language leads
to difficulty reading words with irregular orthography and reading for meaning.
Although exceptions may exist (e.g. Thompson et al. 2008), this dual-constraint
aspect of reading means that many early readers would likely be constrained to
reading controlled text with few exception words, in comparison to older children
with greater language ability. Perhaps more importantly, children without the language
and accompanying conceptual lexicon would be restricted when reading for meaning to
texts that contain vocabulary items they sufficiently understand, as demonstrated by the
contribution vocabulary makes in reading comprehension (e.g. Cunningham and Stano-
vich 1997).
Skill-constraint in reading development
Paris (2005) in his Constrained Skills Theory pointed out the role of skill constraint in
reading development. In terms of this article, although both language and WRS
approximate asymptotic trajectories, WRS are comparatively constrained (Paris
2005). WRS develop with initial bursts in acquisition, followed by a plateau as a
ceiling, or asymptote, is approached:
[T]raditional reading research has ignored fundamental differences in the developmental
trajectories of reading skills. These different trajectories are manifested in different times
of skill onset, different durations of acquisition, and different asymptotic levels of per-
formance. What is most important is that there are some skills that are more constrained
than others; they are learned quickly, [and] mastered entirely …(Paris 2005, 184)
For WRS, the period of time in which these develop is relatively brief, characterised by
a floor effect before development, a rapid burst in acquisition, and then a tapering
toward a ceiling. Conversely, many language skills require much longer periods to
develop, have greater ceilings, and make a conceptually less-constrained contribution
to reading. Paris termed these latter skills ‘unconstrained’but here I call them less-
532 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
constrained because all skills are ultimately constrained. According to Paris, uncon-
strained skills include vocabulary and semantics because development thereof can con-
tribute indefinitely to reading comprehension. In English, individual word reading
requires a sufficiently developed lexicon because of the irregular orthography (e.g.
Nation and Coxsey 2009), rendering reading of irregular words less constrained than
regular word decoding.
To give an example, important requisites for reading such as alphabet knowledge
and letter-sound correspondences can be quickly mastered because they are finite in
number (i.e. 26 letters in the alphabet and about 50 letter-sound correspondences in
English). Thus, alphabet knowledge, concepts about print, and, to a lesser extent, pho-
nemic awareness develop to reach a ceiling in a brief time frame, whereas vocabulary
and comprehension require much longer. It may be possible to comparatively quickly
develop the requisite WRS to read much of Shakespeare’sHamlet aloud. However,
understanding Hamlet is an entirely different matter, requiring sophisticated vocabu-
lary, semantic, and pragmatic understanding. Unlike some WRS, language develops
over a long period of time and its contribution for skilled reading is ultimately great
(Dickinson et al. 2010). Figure 1 depicts these relations graphically by plotting the
average score of the normative test sample for commonly used measures of phonemic
awareness, non-word and word reading, vocabulary, and reading comprehension.
Although the relations in Figure 1 reflect, to some extent, the constrained properties
of tests, they bear remarkable similarity to the relative constraint in skills as depicted
by Paris (2005).
Figure 1. Normative sample means on published norm-referenced tests of language and
reading skill development as a function of chronological age. Note that the raw score for
passage comprehension was multiplied by an integer (2.5) so that it lay between word identifi-
cation and vocabulary. PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, CTOPP = Comprehensive
Test of Phonological Processing, WRMT = Woodcock Reading Master Test-Normative Update.
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 533
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Two hypotheses on early WRS acquisition
Are WRS acquired more readily when children are older?
It is clear from decades of intervention studies that most children can acquire at least
rudimentary WRS at age four or five (e.g. Bus and van Ijzendoorn 1999; Ehri et al.
2001; Ehri et al. 2001; National Early Literacy Panel 2008, Suggate 2010); therefore,
it is here taken as established that, under the right conditions, many children can acquire
early WRS. However, I now consider whether this progresses as rapidly as with other
children.
Theoretical considerations
Theoretically, there are good reasons to believe that WRS acquisition might differ as a
function of when children learn to read. First, given the importance of language in
reading, phonology in phonemic awareness (e.g. Walley, Metsala, and Garlock
2003), vocabulary in irregular word decoding (e.g. Tunmer and Chapman 2012), and
in reading comprehension (e.g. Cunningham and Stanovich 1997), the acquisition of
reading skills is more likely to proceed favourably when language development is
stronger. There are indeed age-related gains in vocabulary and therewith phonemic
awareness (Walley et al. 2003), complexity and rapidity of language comprehension
(Wassenberg et al. 2008), and children’s narrative skill (Liles 1993; Suggate et al.
2011). Therefore, older beginning readers should have a greater language foundation
than younger beginning readers, thus facilitating WRS development.
Second, although this is not a major focus of the current work, non-language,
general knowledge (Neuman 2006; Vanderwood et al. 2002), and cognitive factors
(Luna and Sweeney 2001) important for reading comprehension develop with age
and experience driven maturation (e.g. Suggate 2010). Reading is an ‘unnatural act’
containing many abstract features that can seem counterintuitive to children. For
example, children confuse word length with the size of the object referred too
(Valtin 2012). Additionally, the letters themselves in alphabetic languages bear little,
if any, relationship to the objects and sounds they represent. That ‘a’makes the
sound /ah/, that ‘house’is spelt with five letters, and that ‘knight’is not spelt ‘nyte’
is abstract and often illogical. Thus, the ability to think abstractly, apply rules, and
encode and retrieve learned forms in memory would facilitate acquiring WRS. Accord-
ingly, evidence suggests that older children at school entry have greater verbal skills
(Kurdek and Sinclair 2001) and pre-literacy skills including phonemic awareness
(Crone and Whitehurst 1999). Certainly, type and intensity of instruction may exert
a more powerful influence than a few months of maturation (Crone and Whitehurst
1999; Morrison et al. 1997). However, in general, children who are one to two years
older when they begin reading instruction should learn WRS more readily.
Evidence from meta-analysis
Some reading intervention meta-analyses suggest that older children exhibit a greater
response to reading intervention (Suggate 2010; Talbott, Wills, and Tankersley
1994). For example, in a large reading intervention meta-analysis (k= 116), Suggate
(2010)investigated intervention effect size for at-risk and reading disabled children
as a function of grade, controlling for a host of moderating variables, including inter-
vention modality. Overall there was a tendency for older children to show greater inter-
vention effect sizes and this appeared most marked for WRS.
534 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Evidence from international comparisons
Findings from international studies capitalising on different ages at which children
begin schooling shed light on how the development of WRS proceed depending on
when instruction began. However, the PISA studies measure reading performance at
age 15, which is extremely distal to when instruction begins. As mentioned earlier,
Elley’s(1992) study looked at reading comprehension of nine- and 14-year-olds
across 32 countries. Surprisingly, Elley (1992) found that the relative difference in
instruction for those beginning at age seven or five was barely detectable by age
nine. Thus, similar levels of WRS were acquired in half the amount of time and
instruction.
Further evidence comes from an international study of reading development focus-
ing on WRS development in the first year of school (Seymour et al. 2003). Seymour
and colleagues found a tendency for older children to have a greater rate of WRS acqui-
sition, although the relations with age were not as great as with orthographic depth and
syllabic structure of the languages. Importantly, this pattern occurred also when two
languages (i.e. English and Danish) similar in orthographic depth were directly com-
pared (Cunningham and Carroll 2011).
Evidence from studies in one language
Although not conducted in English, one set of experimental studies directly compared
the rate of developing WRS for comparatively older and younger children (Feitelson,
Tehori, and Levinberg-Green 1982). In their report, three experiments investigated
acquisition of decoding (and also comprehension) skills in Hebrew for differently
aged children (N= 62). In all studies, older children showed greater gains in WRS
than younger children, and this relation held when conducted in a community with a
‘very low SES population’(Feitelson et al. 1982, 492).
Cunningham and Caroll (2011) compared the reading development of children in
standard versus Steiner schools in England, providing evidence from English’s
complex orthography (Seymour et al. 2003).Over the course of the year, the groups
made similar progress in the development of WRS including on phonemic awareness
tasks, even though the older children received around 17 hours of phonics instruction
in the year compared to the standard school children who received nearly 38 hours.
Thus, Cunningham and Carroll (2011) commented:
A possible explanation [for the similar progress with less instruction] is that the superior
reading related skills and greater maturity of the Steiner children (age-related factors)
made them more receptive to the instruction …than their younger counterparts. (12)
Similar evidence comes from the study discussed earlier (i.e. Suggate et al. 2013), in
which the Waldorf children had, by around age 10, made good large deficits in
WRS caused by delaying the onset of reading instruction by approximately 19
months. Again, this occurred despite the Steiner schools devoting in comparison a
little over half of the amount of time per day to text-related reading instruction –par-
ticularly in the earlier grades. Moreover, the later starting children in Durkin’s(1974–
1975) study also reached a similar level of word reading skills despite receiving instruc-
tion for two instead of four years.
Hypothesis I. In light of the above evidence, I hypothesise that: comparatively older
children acquire WRS more readily.
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 535
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Do early reading skills benefit language development?
To address whether it is beneficial to acquire WRS early, it is necessary to see how
language develops with and without early reading experiences. If having early WRS
led to improved language development, then it would here be considered optimal.
However, a fair comparison on the benefits of early reading is not comparing an effec-
tive early reading programme with a poor one –as is often the case in early intervention
research. Instead early reading programmes must be compared with those of similar
quality fostering oral language skills (e.g. through play, storytelling, singing). Approxi-
mations of this contrast can be found internationally by comparing children in play-
oriented kindergartens versus more school like environments (e.g. Finland vs New
Zealand, child-centred vs academic preschools, Lonigan and Phillips 2012; Marcon
2012, or Waldorf vs. state, Suggate et al. 2013).
Theoretical considerations. Given the already rapid expansion in preschoolers’
vocabularies –from around 50 words at 18 months to 10,000 words at six years (Die-
sendruck 2009)–it is reasonable to doubt whether having WRS substantially alters this
trajectory. Indeed, powerful influences from spoken language are exerted on children’s
vocabulary development through shared-attention, overhearing (Gampe, Liebal, and
Tomasello 2012), fast mapping (Diesendruck 2009), and of course sophistication of
vocabulary in the home environment (e.g. Weizman and Snow 2001). Depending on
the viewpoint, either reciprocal benefits of early WRS for language might exist (e.g.
Ehri 2012) or concern might be voiced at the time directed into reading instruction
taken away from imagination and play (e.g. Golinkoff, Hirsh-Pasek, and Singer
2006; House 2011).
Theoretically, differential effects of early WRS acquisition on language develop-
ment might be expected. One skill that clearly shows short-term benefits of learning
to read is phonemic awareness, which experiences a time-limited burst in development
after the beginning of reading instruction (e.g. Silvén, Poskiparta, and Niemi 2004).
Conversely, the broader skills of vocabulary, syntax, and semantics would only be
expected to improve from having early reading skills if: (1) either of these language
skills were present in texts to a greater extent than in oral language environments; or
(2) features of the language medium (either text or spoken) allowed deeper or more effi-
cient processing.
Is spoken language less complicated than early texts?
Nagy and Anderson (1984) estimated, based on the complexity of classroom texts, that
children would not begin to encounter new vocabulary from reading, that they would
not have encountered through non-reading experiences, until around grade 3 to 4. If
children are not being exposed to vocabulary from text that is richer than what they
receive from an oral non-WRS environment until middle elementary school, then it
is difficult to see how having early WRS would improve language. Additionally, the
complexity of texts would not help children unless they were more efficient at acquiring
vocabulary from reading, thus this is considered next.
Does text facilitate language learning?
Two recent studies using experimental designs investigated whether the presence of
text facilitated the acquisition of vocabulary. For both non-words (Rickets, Bishop,
and Nation 2009) and words (Rosenthal and Ehri 2008) having an orthographic
536 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
representation resulted in superior word learning, particularly for older children (grade
5 vs grade 2), that is when reading skills begin to contribute to language. However, the
conditions in both studies resembled formulaic vocabulary instruction, not incidental
word learning from either a naturalistic early WRS environment or non-WRS
conditions.
To directly test whether having early WRS results in greater vocabulary acquisition
compared to more naturalistic language environments, Suggate et al. (2013) exposed
grade 2 and 4 children to three different conditions. Children either read stories inde-
pendently, had stories read to them by experimenters, or they simply had stories told
to them freely by experimenters. The purpose of choosing the three conditions was
to not only control for the presence of text, but also to include a more naturalistic
text-free condition with a purely oral delivery. In each story, acquisition of target
words was tested. Children showed greater learning of target vocabulary items when
stories were told, followed by the shared-reading, and then the independent reading
conditions –even though all children were established readers. Thus, even skilled
grade 4 readers appear to gain greater impetus for vocabulary development through
oral language over reading.
An alternative approach is to examine incidental word acquisition from children’s
reading of texts. Swanborn and de Glopper (1999) conducted a meta-analysis of inci-
dental word learning research for children reading texts under conditions designed to
simulate normal reading experiences. Overall the probability that children learned a
previously unfamiliar word from reading text was .15. Importantly, at grade 4 this prob-
ability was only .08, but in grade 11 this was .33. Unfortunately, there were no studies
with samples younger than grade 4; however, the pattern with age suggests that younger
children might learn but few new words from reading. Moreover, in their meta-anaylsis,
Swanborn and de Glopper (1999) found evidence that non-WRS development was
more important than reading skill. Grade, but not reading ability, made a significant
contribution to word learning, explaining 46% of the heterogeneity in effect size.
Language acquisition from early reading versus non-reading environments
In the following section examining language acquisition from early reading and non-
reading environments, research on shared book reading is not discussed because this
largely concerns parents decoding the text with children being involved primarily in
a language interaction with parents.
Comparative studies
There are several studies suggesting that language skills overall develop similarly
regardless of whether children are in environments fostering early WRS or not. In a
natural experiment comparing children lying on either side of a cut-off date for entry
into school, Morrison, Griffith, and Frazier (1996) report results showing that similarly
aged children made no more progress in formal schooling versus a less formal kinder-
garten on measures of vocabulary, general knowledge, and narrative skills, despite
developing superior WRS.
One study looked cross-sectionally at samples of children in either state or Steiner
schools in New Zealand (Suggate et al. 2011). Importantly, the children had vastly
different experiences from age five to six, with the state-school children attending
formal full-day schooling and the Waldorf children attending half-day play-based
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 537
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
kindergartens. Three samples were recruited, to allow matching on age and schooling
and measures were taken of vocabulary, phonemic awareness, WRS, and oral narrative
retelling. Attending state-school related to superior WRS and phonemic awareness but
not in improved vocabulary or oral narrative skill (Suggate et al. 2013).
In an international study (Suggate et al. 2012) designed to extend the above
work, we compared the language and WRS development of children attending
either their last year of kindergarten (age five to six) or first year of school (aged
six to seven) in Germany, and their first year of state-school in New Zealand
(aged five to six). Vocabulary, listening comprehension, phonemic awareness,
WRS, and narrative story telling abilities were followed across one year. Hierarch-
ical linear models controlled for parental education, ethnicity and second-language
experiences. Data suggested that experience with schooling resulted in greater WRS
and phonemic awareness, but that this WRS development was not associated with
greater improvements in language than that afforded by age and non-schooling
experiences alone.
Hypothesis II. Children experience remarkable language development before they
begin to read, such that it is clear that the bulk of children’s language development has
nothing to do with whether they have early WRS or not. However, contention sur-
rounds the icing on the cake; there is some evidence to suggest that the highly con-
strained skill of phonemic awareness receives short-term benefit from early WRS. It
is also likely that the beginning stages of reading might expose children to different
vocabulary items compared to those that would have been encountered in purely oral
environments and vice versa but that general language differences do not exist.
Although more research is needed, based on the available evidence I hypothesise
that: Early reading skills do not, on average, provide benefits for language above
and beyond a language environment of similar quality.
The Luke Effect and early reading
The changing landscape of child development means that for reading instruction to be
optimal, it needs to be timed, or sown, in the right soil, in the right season. According to
the theory proposed here, early WRS are: (1) conceptually and developmentally con-
strained; (2) constrained by immature early language and non-language factors; (3)
acquired less readily earlier (Hypothesis I); and (4) do not initially improve the less
constrained aspects of language (Hypothesis II), which initially mature without
reading skills. Accordingly, reading instruction begun when children are able to
acquire WRS, but when these WRS do not in turn improve language or non-language
skills over and beyond language environments are destined to wither until the soil (i.e.
language and non-language skills) is developmentally ripe to be able to profit from
having WRS. Reading instruction begun when the soil is ripe yields a crop of
reading skills that –because of requiring less reading instruction to sow and
because benefits to language and learning from the activity of reading –produces,
by comparison, ‘a hundred times more than was sown’(Luke, 8: 8). The Luke
Effect as it applies to reading is depicted in Table 2, with phases used to simplify
the description, although progression from one stage to the other is of course
gradual and context dependent. Likewise, the suggested points in schooling in
column 1 are somewhat arbitrary and vary greatly as a function of a given language’s
orthographic complexity, multiple language acquisition, individual differences, and
school and home environments.
538 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Summary
The Luke Effect represents a significant paradigm shift in the way early reading is con-
ceptualised –this shift can be summarised as viewing readiness not as capability but as
long-term optimality. Certainly, there are many complicated and important factors that
lie outside the scope of this work that need to be the focus of future research. As such,
the Luke Effect itself does not provide the final account on whether children should
learn to read earlier or later but rather represents a beginning in attending to this too
often neglected question.
Acknowledgements
I would like to thank Elaine Reese, Elizabeth Schaughency, Wolfgang Schneider, and in particu-
lar Tamara Suggate for their conceptual and editorial assistance with this article. Also thanks to
Stefanie Obergrießer and Stefanie Fischer for proof reading. Substantial proportions of this
Table 2. The Luke Effect: a developmental model of the optimal age to learn to read.
Schooling Phase
Language
development
Direction of
learning
Features of WRS
development
Preschool Language
phase
Language
improved
through rich
non-WRS
environments.
•WRS are
developed more
laboriously than
language.
•Benefit for
language from
WRS is negligible
and short-term.
•Low levels of
language and WRS
constrain reading.
Early
elementary
school
WRS phase Language begins
to improve from
the activity of
skilled reading,
but not more
than from non-
reading
experiences
alone.
•Greater language
skills aid WRS
development.
•WRS become
fluent and
integrated.
•Transition to
reading to learn.
From middle
elementary
school
Reading to
learn
phase
Language begins
to derive
exclusive
benefit from the
activity of
skilled reading.
•WRS approach
ceiling and skilled
reading is thereafter
constrained by
language.
•Children with
inadequate WRS
and/or language
experience
suboptimal
learning.
Note: WRS = word recognition skills.
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 539
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
article were developed while receiving the support of a publishing grant from the University of
Otago and an Alexander-von-Humboldt research fellowship at the University of Würzburg.
References
Adams, M. J. 1990. Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. Cambridge, MA:
The MIT Press.
Becker, W. C., and R. Gerstein. 1982. “A Follow-Up Through for Follow-Through: The Later
Effects of the Direct Instruction Model on Children in 5th and 6th Grades.”American
Educational Research Journal 19: 75–92.
Biemiller, A. 2006. “Vocabulary Development and Instruction: A Prerequisite for School
Learning.”In Handbook of Early Literacy Research, edited by D. K. Dickinson and S. B.
Newman, Vol. 2, 41–51. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Blatchford, P., and I. Plewis. 1990. “Preschool Reading-related Skills and Later Reading
Achievement: further evidence.”British Educational Research Journal 16: 425–428.
Bus, A. G., and van M. H. Ijzendoorn. 1999. “Phonological Awareness and Early Reading: A
Meta-Analysis of Experimental Training Studies.”Journal of Educational Psychology 91:
403–414.
Chapman, J. W., and W. E. Tunmer. 1995. “Development of Young Children’s Reading Self-
Concepts: An Examination of Emerging Subcomponents and Their Relationships with
Reading Achievement.”Journal of Educational Psychology 87: 154–167.
Conners, F. A. 2009. “Attentional Control and the Simple View of Reading.”Reading &
Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 22: 591–613.
Crone, D. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. 1999. “Age and Schooling Effects on Emergent Literacy and
Early Reading Skills.”Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 604–614.
Cunningham, A. J., & J. M. Carroll. 2011. “The Development of Early Literacy in Steiner- and
Standard-Educated Children.”British Journal of Educational Psychology.doi:10.1348/
000709910X522474.
Cunningham, A. E., & K. E. Stanovich. 1997. “Early Reading Acquisition and Its Relation to
Reading Experience and Ability 10 Years Later.”Developmental Psychology 33: 934–945.
Dickinson, D. K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. 2010. “Speaking Out for Language: Why
Language is Central to Reading Development.”Educational Researcher 39: 305–310.
Diesendruck, G. 2009. “Mechanisms of Word Learning.”In Blackwell Handbook of Language
Development, edited by E. Hoff and M. Shatz, 257–276. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-
Blackwell.
Dollase, R. 2007. “Bildung im Kindergarten und Früheinschulung: Ein Fall von Ignoranz und
Forschungsamnesie [Learning in Kindergarten and Preschool: A Case of Ignorance and
Forgotten Results].”Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie 21: 5–10.
Durkin, D. 1974–1975. “A Six Year Study of Children Who Learned to Read in School at the
Age of Four.”Reading Research Quarterly 10: 9–61.
Ehri, L. C. 2012. “Why is it Important for Children to Begin Learning to Read in Kindergarten?”
In Contemporary Debates in Child Development and Education, edited by S. P. Suggate and
E. Reese, 171–180. London: Routledge, Taylor & Francis.
Ehri, L. C., S. R. Nunes, S. A. Stahl, and D. M. Willows. 2001. “Systematic Phonics Instruction
Helps Students Learn to Read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s Meta-Analysis.”
Review of Educational Research 71: 393–447.
Ehri, L. C., S. R. Nunes, D. M. Willows, B. V. Schuster, Z. Yaghoub-Zadeh, and T. Shanahan.
2001. “Phonemic Awareness Instruction Helps Children Learn to Read: Evidence from the
National Reading Panel’s Meta-Analysis.”Reading Research Quarterly 36: 250–287.
Elley, W. B. 1992. How in the World do Students Read? IEA Study of Reading Literacy. The
Hague, Netherlands: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational
Achievement.
Evans, E. D. 1985. “Longitudinal Follow-up Assessment of Differential Preschool Experience
for Low Income Minority Group Children.”The Journal of Educational Research 78: 197–
202.
Feitelson, D., B. Z. Tehori, and D. Levinberg-Green. 1982. “How Effective is Early Instruction
in Reading? Experimental Evidence.”Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 28: 485–494.
540 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Gampe, A., K. Liebal, and M. Tomasello. 2012. “Eighteen-Month-Olds Learn Novel Words
Through Overhearing.”First Language 32: 385–397. doi:10.1177/0142723711433584.
Gates, A. I. 1937. “The Necessary Mental Age for Beginning Reading.”The Elementary School
Journal 37: 497–508.
Gilliam, W. S., and E. F. Zigler. 2001. “A Critical Meta-Analysis of All Evaluations of State-
Funded Preschool from 1977 to 1998: Implications for Policy, Service Delivery and
Program Evaluation.”Early Childhood Research Quarterly 15: 441–473.
Golinkoff, R. M., K. Hirsh-Pasek, and D. G. Singer. 2006. “Why Play = Learning: A Challenge
for Parents and Educators.”In Play = Learning: How Play Motivates and Enhances
Children’s Cognitive and Social-Emotional Growth, edited by D. G. Singer, R. M.
Golinkoff and K. Hirsh-Pasek, 3–12. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Gough, P. B., and W. E. Tunmer. 1986. “Decoding, Reading and Reading Disability.”Remedial
& Special Education 7: 6–10.
Gray, S. W., and R. A. Klaus. 1970. “The Early Training Project: A Seventh-Year Report.”
Child Development 41: 909–924.
Hecht, S. A., S. R. Burgess, J. K. Torgesen, R. K. Wagner, and C. A. Rashotte. 2000.
“Explaining Social Class Differences in Growth of Reading Skills from Beginning
Kindergarten Through Fourth-Grade: The Role of Phonological Awareness, Rate of
Access, and Print Knowledge.”Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 12: 99–
128.
Hemphill, L., and T. Tivnan. 2008. “The Importance of Early Vocabulary for Literacy
Achievement in High-Poverty Schools.”Journal of Education for Students Placed at
Risk (JESPAR) 13: 426–451. doi:10.1080/10824660802427710.
House, R. (Ed.). 2011. Too Much, Too Soon? Early Learning and the Erosion of Childhood.
Gloucestershire, UK: Hawthorne Press.
Huebler, F. 2007. “Primary, Secondary, and Compulsory Education.”http://huebler.blogspot.
com/2007/01/official-school-ages-primary-secondary.html#download2007
Hunter, J. E., and F. L. Schmidt. 2004. Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in
Research Findings. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Hyson, M. C., K. Hirsh-Pasek, L. Rescorla, J. Cone, and L. Martell-Boinske. 1991. “Ingredients
of Parental “Pressure”in Early Childhood.”Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
12: 347–365.
Juel, C. 1988. “Learning to Read and Write: A Longitudinal Study of 54 Children from First
Through Fourth Grades.”Journal of Educational Psychology 80: 437–447.
Kendeou, P., R. Savage, and P. van den Broek. 2009. “Revisiting the Simple View of Reading.”
British Journal of Educational Psychology 79: 353–370.
Kern, M. L., and H. S. Friedman. 2009. “Early Educational Milestones as Predictors of Lifelong
Academic Achievement, Midlife Adjustment, and Longevity.”Journal of Applied
Developmental Psychology 30 (4): 419–430. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.025.
Kershaw, S., and C. Schatschneider. 2012. “A Latent Variable Approach to the Simple View of
Reading.”Reading and Writing 25: 433–464. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9278-3.
Kirby, J. R., R. K. Parrila, and S. L. Pfeiffer. 2003. “Naming Speed and Phonological Awareness
as Predictors of Reading Development.”Journal of Educational Psychology 95: 453–464.
Kirby, J. R., and R. S. Savage. 2008. “Can the Simple View Deal with the Complexities of
Reading?”Literacy 42: 75–82.
Kurdek, L. A., & Sinclair, R. J. 2001. “Predicting Reading and Mathematics Achievement in
Fourth-Grade Children from Kindergarten Readiness Scores.”Journal of Educational
Psychology 93, 451–455.
Lazar, I., and R. Darlington. 1982. “Lasting Effects of Early Education: A Report from the
Consortium for Longitudinal Studies.”Monographs of the Society of Research in Child
Development 47 (2–3): 1–151.
Lee, V. E., J. Brooks-Gunn, E. Schnur, and F. Liaw. 1990. “Does Head Start Work? A 1-Year
Follow-up Comparison of Disadvantaged Children Attending Head Start, No Preschool, and
Other Preschool Programs.”Developmental Psychology 24: 210–222.
Liles, B. Z. 1993. “Narrative Discourses in Children with Language Disorders and Children with
Normal Language: A Critical Review of the Literature.”Journal of Speech & Hearing
Research 36: 868–882.
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 541
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Lillard, A., and N. Else-Quest. 2006. “Evaluating Montessori Education.”Science 313: 1893–
1894.
Lonigan, C. J., and B. M. Phillips. 2012. “Understanding the Contributions of Early Academic
Skills to Children’s Success in School.”In Contemporary Debates in Childhood Education
and Development, edited by S. P. Suggate and E. Reese, 145–158. London: Routledge,
Taylor & Francis.
Lonigan, C. J., and T. Shanahan. 2008. “Executive Summary of the Report of the National Early
Literacy Panel.”In Developing Early Literacy: A Scientific Synthesis of Early Literacy
Development and Implications for Intervention, edited by National Early Literacy Panel,
pp. v–xii. Jessups, ML: National Center for Family Literacy, Ed Pubs.
Luna, B., and J. A. Sweeney. 2001. “Studies of Brain and Cognitive Maturation Through
Childhood and Adolescence: A Strategy for Testing Neurodevelopmental Hypotheses.”
Schizophrenia Bulletin 27: 443–455.
Magnusson, K. A., C. Ruhm, and J. Waldfogel. 2007. “Does Kindergarten Improve School
Preparation and Performance?”Economics of Education Review 26: 33–51.
Marcon, R. A. 2002. “Moving Up the Grades: Relationship Between Preschool Model and Later
School Success.”Early Childhood Research & Practice 4 (1).
Marcon, R. 2012. “The Importance of Balance in Early Childhood Programs.”In Contemporary
Debates in Childhood Education and Development, edited by S. P. Suggate and E. Reese,
159–168. London: Routledge, Taylor & Francis.
McNaughton, S., G. Phillips, and S. MacDonald. 2000. “Curriculum Channels and Literacy
Development Over the First Year of Instruction.”New Zealand Journal of Educational
Studies 35: 49–59.
Meyer, L. A. 1984. “Long-Term Academic Effects of the Direct Instruction Project Follow
Through.”The Elementary School Journal 84: 380–394.
Miller, L. B., and R. P. Bizzell. 1983. “Long-Term Effects of Four Preschool Programs: Sixth,
Seventh, and Eighth Grades.”Child Development 54: 727–741.
Morgan, P. L., and D. Fuchs. 2007. “Is There a Bidirectional Relationship Between Children’s
Reading Skills and Reading Motivation?”Exceptional Children 73: 165–183.
Morphett, M. V., and C. Washburne. 1931. “When Should Children Begin to Read?”The
Elementary School Journal 31: 496–503.
Morrison, F. J., E. M. Griffith, and D. M. Alberts. 1997. “Nature-Nurture in the Classroom:
Entrance Age, School Readiness, and Learning in Children.”Developmental Psychology
33: 254–262.
Morrison, F. J., E. M. Griffith, and J. A. Frazier. 1996. “Schooling and the 5 to 7 Shift: A Natural
Experiment.”In The Five to Seven Year Shift, edited by A. J. Sameroff and M. M. Haith,
161–186. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Nagy, W. E., and R. C. Anderson. 1984. “How Many Words are There in Printed School
English?”Reading Research Quarterly 19: 304–330.
Nation, K., and J. Coxsey. 2009. “The Relationship Between Knowing a Word and Reading it
Aloud in Children’s Word Reading Development.”Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology 103: 296–308.
National Early Literacy Panel (Ed.). 2008. Developing Early Literacy: A Scientific Synthesis of
Early Literacy Development and Implications for Intervention. National Institute for
Literacy & The Partnership for Reading.
Nelson, G., A. Westhaus, and J. MacLeod. 2003. “A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Research
on Preschool Prevention Programs for Children.”Prevention & Treatment 6: Article 31.
Neuman, S. B. 2006. “The Knowledge Gap: Implications for Early Education.”In Handbook of
Early Literacy Research, edited by D. K. Dickinson and S. B. Neuman, Vol. 2, 29–40.
New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Ouellette, G., and A. Beers. 2010. “A Not-So-Simple View of Reading: How Oral Vocabulary
and Visual-Word Recognition Complicate the Story.”Reading and Writing 23: 189–208.
doi:10.1007/s11145-008-9159-1.
Paris, S. G. 2005. “Reinterpreting the Development of Reading Skills.”Reading Research
Quarterly 40: 184–202.
Reynolds, A. J. 1994. “Effects of a Preschool Plus Follow-on Intervention for Children at Risk.”
Developmental Psychology 30: 787–804.
542 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Rickets, J., D. V. M. Bishop, and K. Nation. 2009. “Orthographic Facilitation in Oral
Vocabulary Acquisition.”The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62: 1948–
1966.
Rosenthal, J., and L. C. Ehri. 2008. “The Mnemonic Value of Orthography for Vocabulary
Learning.”Journal of Educational Psychology 100: 175–191.
Rutter, M. 1989. “Age as an Ambiguous Variable in Developmental Research: Some
Epidemiological Considerations from Developmental Psychopathology.”International
Journal of Behavioral Development 12: 1–34.
Scarborough, H. S. 2001. “Connecting Early Language and Literacy to Later Reading (dis)
Abilities: Evidence, Theory, and Practice.”In Handbook of Early Literacy Research,
edited by S. B. Neuman and D. K. Dickinson, 97–110. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Schmerkotte, H. 1978. “Ergebnisse eines Vergleichs von Modellkindergnderg und Vorklassen
in Nordrhein-Westfalen [Results from a Comparison of Typical Kindergartens and
Preschools in Northrhein-Westphalia].”Bildung und Erziehung 31: 401–411.
Schweinhart, L. J., and D. P. Weikart. 1988. “Education for Young Children Living in Poverty:
Child-Initiated Learning or Teacher-Directed Instruction?”The Elementary School Journal
89: 212–225.
Schweinhart, L. J., and D. P. Weikart. 1997. “The High/Scope Preschool Curriculum
Comparison Study Through Age 23.”Early Childhood Research Quarterly 12: 117–143.
Sénéchal, M., J. LeFevre, and B. Smith-Chant. 2001. “On Refining Theoretical Models of
Emergent Literacy: The Role of Empirical Evidence.”Journal of School Psychology 39:
439–460.
Seymour, P. H. K., M. Aro, J. M. Erskine, and COST Action Network. 2003. “Foundation
Literacy Acquisition in European Orthographies.”British Journal of Psychology 94: 143–174.
Silvén, M., E. Poskiparta, and P. Niemi. 2004. “The Odds of Becoming a Precocious Reader of
Finnish.”Journal of Educational Psychology 96: 152–164.
Snow, C. E., M. S. Burns, and P. Griffin. 1998. Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young
Children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Sonnenschein, S., and K. Munsterman. 2002. “The Influence of Home-Based Reading
Interactions on 5-Year Olds’Reading Motivations and Early Literacy Development.”
Early Childhood Research Quarterly 17: 318–337.
Sprigle, J. E., and L. Schaefer. 1985. “Longitudinal Evaluation of the Effects of Two
Compensatory Preschool Programs on Fourth Through Sixth Grade Students.”
Developmental Psychology 21: 702–708.
Storch, S. A., and G. J. Whitehurst. 2002. “Oral Language and Code-Related Precursors to
Reading: Evidence from a Longitudinal Structural Model.”Developmental Psychology
38: 934–947.
Stuart, M., R. Stainthorp, and M. Snowling. 2008. “Litearcy as a Complex Activity:
Deconstructing the Simple View of Reading.”Literacy 42: 59–66.
Suggate, S. P. 2009. “School Entry Age and Reading Achievement in the 2006 Programme for
International Student Assessment (PISA).”International Journal of Educational Research
48: 151–161.
Suggate, S. P. 2010. “Why ‘What’we Teach Depends on “When”: Grade and Reading
Intervention Modality Moderate Effect Size.”Developmental Psychology 46: 1556–1579.
doi:10.1037/a0020612.
Suggate, S. P. 2011. “Viewing the Long-Term Effects of Early Reading with an Open Eye.”In
Too Much, Too Soon? Early Learning and the Erosion of Childhood, edited by R. House,
236–246. Gloucestershire, UK: Hawthorne Press.
Suggate, S. P. 2012. “Watering the Garden Before a Rainstorm: The Case of Early Reading.”In
Contemporary Debates in Child Development and Education, edited by S. P. Suggate and E.
Reese, 181–190. London: Routledge, Taylor & Francis.
Suggate, S. P., W. Lenhard, E. Neudecker, and W. Schneider. 2013. “Incidental Vocabulary
Acquisition from Stories: Second and Fourth Graders Learn More from Listening Than
Reading.”First Language 33: 551–571. doi:10.1177/0142723713503144.
Suggate, S. P., E. Reese, W. Lenhard, and W. Schneider. 2012, September 7. “Language and
Literacy Development as a Function of Whether Children Attend Play-Oriented
Kindergartens or Formal School.”Poster presented at the Narrative, Intervention, and
Literacy conference, Paris.
European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 543
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016
Suggate, S. P., E. A. Schaughency, and E. Reese. 2011. “The Contribution of Age and Formal
Schooling to Oral Narrative and Pre-Reading Skills.”First Language 31: 379–403. doi:10.
1177/0142723710395165.
Suggate, S. P., E. A. Schaughency, and E. Reese. 2013. “Children learning to read later catch up
to children reading earlier.”Early Childhood Research Quarterly 28: 33–48. doi:10.1016/j.
ecresq.2012.04.004.
Swanborn, M. S. L., and K. de Glopper. 1999. “Incidental Word Learning While Reading: A
Meta-Analysis.”Review of Educational Research 69: 261–285.
Talbott, E., L. Wills, and M. Tankersley. 1994. “Effects of Reading Comprehension
Interventions for Students with Learning Disabilities.”Learning Disability Quarterly 17:
223–232.
Thompson, G. B., M. F. McKay, C. M. Fletcher-Flinn, V. Connelly, R. T. Kaa, and J. Ewing.
2008. “Do Children Who Acquire Word Reading Without Explicit Phonics Employ
Compensatory Learning? Isuues of Phonological Reccoding, Lexical Orthography, and
Fluency.”Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 21: 505–537.
Tilstra, J., K. McMaster, P. Van den Broek, P. Kendeou, and D. Rapp. 2009. “Simple but
Complex: Components of the Simple View of Reading Across Grade Levels.”Journal of
Research in Reading 32: 383–401. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01401.x.
Tunmer, W. E., and J. W. Chapman. 2012. “Does Set for Variability Mediate the Influence of
Vocabulary Knowledge on the Development of Word Recognition Skills?”Scientific
Studies of Reading 16: 122–140. doi:10.1080/10888438.2010.542527.
Tunmer, W., and K. Greaney. 2009. “Defining Dyslexia.”Journal of Learning Disabilities 43:
229–243. doi:10.1177/0022219409345009.
Valtin, R. 2012. “Increasing Awareness of Phonological Awareness –Helpful or Misleading?”
In Contemporary Debates on Child Development and Education, edited by S. P. Suggate
and E. Reese, 227–238. London: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.
Vanderwood, M. L., K. S. McGrew, D. P. Flanagan, and T. Z. Keith. 2002. “The Contribution of
General and Specific Cognitive Abilities to Reading Achievement.”Learning and
Individual Differences 13: 159–188.
Vellutino, F. R., W. E. Tunmer, J. J. Jaccard, and R. Chen. 2007. “Components of Reading
Ability: Multivariate Evidence for a Convergent Skills Model of Reading Development.”
Scientific Studies of Reading 11: 3–32.
Walley, A. C., J. L. Metsala, and V. M. Garlock. 2003. “Spoken Vocabulary Growth: Its Role in
the Development of Phoneme Awareness and Early Reading Ability.”Reading & Writing:
An Interdisciplinary Journal 16: 5–20.
Wang, J. H., and J. T. Guthrie. 2004. “Modeling the Effects of Intrinsic Motivation, Extrinsic
Motivation, Amount of Reading, and Past Reading Achievement on Text Comprehension
Between U.S. and Chinese Students.”Reading Research Quarterly 39: 162–186.
Wasik, B. H., C. T. Ramey, D. M. Bryant, and J. J. Sparling. 1990. “A Longitudinal Study of
Two Early Intervention Strategies: Project CARE.”Child Development 61: 1682–1696.
Wassenberg, R., P. Hurks, J. G. M. Hendriksen, F. J. M. Feron, C. J. C. Meijs, J. S. H. Vles, et al.
2008. “Age-Related Improvement in Complex Language Comprehension: Results of a
Cross-Sectional Study with 361 Children Aged 5 to 15.”Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Psychology 30: 435–448.
Weizman, Z. O., and C. E. Snow. 2001. “Lexical Output as Related to Children’s Vocabulary
Acquisition: Effects of Sophisticated Exposure and Support for Meaning.”Developmental
Psychology 37: 265–279. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.37.2.265.
Whitehurst, G. J., and C. J. Lonigan. 2001. “Emergent Literacy: Development from Prereaders
to Readers.”In Handbook of Early Literacy Research, edited by S. B. Newman and D. K.
Dickinson, Vol. 1, 11–29. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
544 S.P. Suggate
Downloaded by [University of Regensburg], [${individualUser.displayName}] at 04:55 29 April 2016