ArticlePDF Available

Long-term meditation training induced changes in the operational synchrony of default mode network modules during a resting state

Authors:
  • BM-Science - Brain & Mind Technologies Research Centre
  • Physics Foundations Society

Abstract and Figures

Using theoretical analysis of self-consciousness concept and experimental evidence on the brain default mode network (DMN) that constitutes the neural signature of self-referential processes, we hypothesized that the anterior and posterior subnets comprising the DMN should show differences in their integrity as a function of meditation training. Functional connectivity within DMN and its subnets (measured by operational synchrony) has been measured in ten novice meditators using an electroencephalogram (EEG) recording in a pre-/post-meditation intervention design. We have found that while the whole DMN was clearly suppressed, different subnets of DMN responded differently after 4 months of meditation training: The strength of EEG operational synchrony in the right and left posterior modules of the DMN decreased in resting post-meditation condition compared to a pre-meditation condition, whereas the frontal DMN module on the contrary exhibited an increase in the strength of EEG operational synchrony. These findings combined with published data on functional-anatomic heterogeneity within the DMN and on trait subjective experiences commonly found following meditation allow us to propose that the first-person perspective and the sense of agency (the witnessing observer) are presented by the frontal DMN module, while the posterior modules of the DMN are generally responsible for the experience of the continuity of 'I' as embodied and localized within bodily space. Significance of these findings is discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Below is the unedited draft of the article that has been accepted for publication
(© Cognitive Processing, 2016, Volume 17, Issue 1, P. 27-37.)
Long-Term Meditation Training Induced Changes in the Operational Synchrony
of Default Mode Network Modules During a Resting State
Andrew A. Fingelkurts a,*, Alexander A. Fingelkurts a, Tarja Kallio-Tamminen b
a BM-Science – Brain and Mind Technologies Research Centre, Espoo, Finland
b Physics Foundations Society and Society for Natural Philosophy, Helsinki, Finland
Abstract:
Using theoretical analysis of self-consciousness concept and experimental evidence on the brain default mode
network (DMN) that constitutes the neural signature of self-referential processes we hypothesized that the
anterior and posterior subnets comprising the DMN should show differences in their integrity as a function of
meditation training. Functional connectivity within DMN and its subnets (measured by operational synchrony)
has been measured in 10 novice meditators using an electroencephalogram (EEG) recording in a pre-/post-
meditation intervention design. We have found that while the whole DMN was clearly suppressed, different
subnets of DMN responded differently after four months of meditation training: The strength of EEG
operational synchrony in the right and left posterior modules of the DMN decreased in resting post-meditation
condition compared to a pre-meditation condition, whereas the frontal DMN module on the contrary exhibited
an increase in the strength of EEG operational synchrony. These findings combined with published data on
functional-anatomic heterogeneity within the DMN and on trait subjective experiences commonly found
following meditation allow us to propose that the first-person perspective and the sense of agency (the
witnessing observer) are presented by the frontal DMN module, while the posterior modules of the DMN are
generally responsible for the experience of the continuity of ‘I’ as embodied and localized within bodily space.
Significance of these findings is discussed.
Keywords:
Meditation; yoga; electroencephalogram (EEG); mind–body practice; self; self-referential processing; self-
consciousness; functional connectivity; operational synchrony; operational modules; DMN
Abbreviations:
Quantitative electroencephalogram – qEEG; default mode network – DMN; operational module – OM;
operational synchrony – OS; rapid transitional period – RTP; synchrocomplex – SC.
2
1. Introduction
For a long time the topic of self-consciousness (or self-awareness) was the exclusive domain of
philosophy and only recently has it begun to take a more prominent place in the cognitive
neuroscience and neuroimaging studies under the umbrella term of ‘self-referential processing
(Musholt, 2013). Northoff et al. (2006) have argued that self-referential processing is common to
distinct concepts of self in different knowledge domains. In this context the self-referential processing
is the core of what constitutes the ‘experiential self’. The accumulation of empirical data in
neuroscience (Craik et al., 1999; Kircher et al., 2000; Gusnard et al., 2001; Gusnard, 2005; Spreng
and Grady, 2010; Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011; Qin and Northoff, 2011), including the study of
direct causal relationship between brain activity and self-consciousness (Lou et al., 2010; Chen et al.,
2013), has lead researchers to a suggestion that the brain default mode network (DMN) is a most
probable neural correlate for the sense of self1 (Christoff et al., 2003; Wicker et al., 2003; Gusnard,
2005; Buckner and Carroll, 2007; Schilbach et al., 2008; Fingelkurts et al., 2012).
The DMN is commonly defined as a set of interacting cortex areas, encompassing mostly left and
right middle frontal gyri, bilateral frontal medial areas, left and right middle temporal and occipital
gyri, and left and right precuneus, that are mostly active and functionally synchronized across a wide
variety of self-related tasks and during the resting state when participants are engaged in self-
generated thoughts and mind-wandering (Gusnard et al., 2001; Newen and Vogeley, 2003; Christoff
et al., 2003; Northoff et al., 2006; Schilbach et al., 2008; Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011;
Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014).
Capitalizing on these studies it has been suggested that the practice of meditation (which aims at
reducing the prevalence of self-related thought chains and valuation) should reduce the activity of or
synchrony within the DMN (Fell, 2012). In the initial attempts to verify this hypothesis it was indeed
found that meditation inhibits activity of the DMN (Pagnoni et al., 2008; Ott et al., 2010; Brewer et
al., 2011) and reduces functional connectivity between areas within DMN (Faber et al., 2004; Brewer
et al., 2011; Lehmann et al., 2012; Taylor et al., 2013; Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2014). However, such
findings might be biased by treating the entire DMN as a cohesive unit (where the dynamics of
separate modules of DMN is masked). Indeed, in most DMN studies this network was considered as a
single and homogenous unit that functions as a single whole (Uddin et al., 2009). At the same time, it
1 This conclusion is further supported by evidence that DMN structured interactions do not exist in preterm infants
(Fransson et al., 2007, 2009), are underdeveloped in infants (Gao et al., 2009) and develop an adult-like structural
patterns only by age 7–9 (Fair et al., 2008; Thomason et al., 2008). The role of the DMN in supporting self-conscious
experience is also confirmed by empirical evidence from patients with disorders of consciousness (Laureys 2005;
Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2010; Fingelkurts et al., 2012), as well as during anesthesia (Greicius et al., 2008) and brain death
(Boly et al., 2009).
3
seems very unlikely that multiple spatially distinct regions that comprise DMN are responsible for
just one function, being essentially redundant (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014). Much more plausibly,
different DMN regions serve complementary functions that in combination give rise to a large variety
of self-related mentations and processes (for example, self-location, first-person perspective and
agency, self-reflection, self-referential narrative thinking, autobiographical thoughts, planning aspects
of personal future, and so on).
Recent evidence suggests that the DMN is indeed a heterogeneous brain system composed of at
least three separable yet interacting components or subnets (Uddin et al., 2009; Andrews-Hanna et al.,
2010; Spreng and Grady, 2010; Leech et al., 2011; Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011). In our own
previous studies we have found three subnets (or operational modules – OMs, as we call them) within
DMN (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011): two symmetrical occipito-parieto-temporal OMs and one
frontal OM. Comparing healthy fully-conscious subjects with patients in vegetative and minimally
conscious states, we also documented that these modules react in a slightly different manner as a
function of self-consciousness presence (Fingelkurts et al., 2012): While the strength of functional
connectivity decreased dramatically in vegetative patients (who are unconscious) in all three OMs, it
was minimal within the frontal OM that nearly ceased to exist. Such strength was intermediate in
patients who were in a minimally conscious state when compared to patients in vegetative state and
healthy fully conscious subjects (Fingelkurts et al., 2012). Moreover, only frontal OM could reliably
predict recovery of self-consciousness six years later (Fingelkurts et al., in press). Based on the
functional distinctions between these three DMN OMs (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011), one could
predict that the anterior and posterior OMs comprising the DMN should show differences in their
integrity as a function of meditation training.
Therefore, the aim of this study was to analyze the DMN OMs individually to reveal whether
there are important trait differences in the resting-state functional connectivity within different OMs
in a pre-/post- meditation intervention design. Given the functional specialization of frontal and
parietal OMs (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011), we hypothesized that after long-term meditation
training the strength of integrity (functional connectivity) within the frontal OM should increase,
while it should decrease in the occipito-parieto-temporal OMs. Furthermore, we expected to see much
stronger decrease of the functional connectivity strength in both posterior OMs, than increase of
integrity in the single frontal OM, thus resulting in an overall decrease (in congruence with the
previous studies: Faber et al., 2004; Brewer et al., 2011; Lehmann et al., 2012; Berkovich-Ohana et
al., 2014) in the functional connectivity of the DMN taken as a whole.
EEG is informationally reach signal and there are many EEG characteristics that could be
measured pre- and post-mediation, but our interest in this particular study was to investigate how
4
operational synchrony within three separate modules of DMN will be affected by the long-term
meditation training.
2. Methods
2.1. Subjects
Ten (average age = 51.7 ± 10.9, four males) healthy, right-handed subjects participated in the
study. The authors recruited participants (novices) from a four-month training course in meditation
who have passed inclusion/exclusion criteria. The subjects had no history of head trauma, no
current/past psychiatric disorders, no psychoactive medication or drug use, and also had normal or
corrected to normal vision. Inclusion criteria to be recruited for this study were (a) be in good general,
neurological and psychological/psychiatric health, (b) never practice any meditation technique before
entry to the study. Exclusion criteria comprised (a) stressful events during the course of meditation
training, (b) change of job, place of residence, or preoccupation during the course of meditation, (c)
change of life-style or a diet during the course of meditation training, (d) any serious disorder during
the course of meditation.
Participants signed an informed consent form after the experimental procedures were explained,
prior to electroencephalogram (EEG) scanning. The study complied with the Code of Ethics of the
World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and standards established by the organization
Review Board. The use of the data for scientific studies was authorized by means of written informed
consent of the subjects approved by the Review Board.
2.2. EEG Recording and Trial Design
The subjects’ EEG was recorded with a 21-channel EEG data acquisition system (Mitsar, St.
Petersburg, Russian Federation). EEG data were collected (linked earlobes as a reference electrode;
0.5–30 Hz bandpass; 50 Hz notch filter ON; 250 Hz sampling rate; 6 min closed eyes) in subjects
during a waking resting state with eyes closed from 19 electrodes positioned according to the
International 10–20 system (i.e., O1, O2, P3, P4, Pz, C3, C4, Cz, T3, T4, T5, T6, Fz, F3, F4, F7, F8, Fp1,
Fp2). The impedance of recording electrodes was always below 5-10 k. Eye movements was
registered with an electrooculogram (0.5–70 Hz bandpass), which was recorded alongside the EEG.
All EEG recordings were done in late morning for all subjects. The subjects were asked to relax
and engage in no specific mental activity, and to not apply any specific relaxation or meditation
techniques during the EEG recording. The first EEG recording (marked as a pre-meditation) was
recorded within one week before meditation training course commenced. Meditation training lasted
5
four months and required 20 minutes of daily meditation. We were not interested in the specific and
immediate effects of different meditation techniques (‘state’ effect2) but rather in a general and long-
lasting effect (‘trait’ effect3) common for any meditation such as entering into a relaxed and at the
same time alert state of mind where one feels to be really present in the moment and is able to
observe his/her body, emotions and thoughts (or physiological, emotional and mental contents) in a
fresh and approving manner (see also, Fischer, 1971; Goleman, 1996; Hinterberger et al., 2011;
Raffone et al., 2014). For that purpose, initially all of the participants were given the basics of an
ancient Kriya yoga practice which contains various meditational methods like following the breath,
noticing certain points in the body, recitation and visualization (Nash and Newberg, 2013). Within the
first week of supervised training by the experienced instructor, participants found and chose their
preferred technique/method for the rest of the course. The second EEG (marked as a post-meditation)
was recorded within one week after 4-month meditation training has been terminated. Parameters and
conditions of this second EEG recording were identical to those of the first EEG recording.
Such design of the study allowed us to avoid the multiple control conditions for possible
confounding factors and specific mediation techniques that would make this study unfeasible. All
unstructured factors (such as different life-styles, diets, stress levels, specific meditation techniques)
that did not systematically relate to a general (and common) trait effect of mediation on DMN would
cancel out when averaged between different subjects. Trait features of the resting-state condition
associated with DMN and influenced by meditation would stand out because the meditation
(irrespectively of a concrete technique) is the only common factor among all subjects during very
long time (four months). Additionally, the resting-state condition permits assessment of “pure” self-
relevant brain activity (Koenig et al., 2002) such as spontaneous processing of an internal mental
context (top-down processing) (Von Stein and Sarnthein, 2000), related episodic memory (Shulman
et al., 1997), imagery (Fletcher et al., 1995), internal ‘narrative,’ and ‘autobiographical’ self (Gusnard
et al., 2001; Johnson et al., 2002; Buckner and Carroll, 2007).
2.3. EEG-Signal Data Preprocessing
The presence of an adequate EEG signal was determined through visual inspection of the raw
signal. Epochs containing artefacts due to eye movement/opening, significant muscle activity, and
movements on EEG channels, as well as drowsy and sleep episodes were marked and then
automatically rejected from further analysis.
2 State effect refers to altered sensory, cognitive, and neurophysiological effects that can arise during meditation
practice (Cahn and Polich, 2006).
3 Trait effect refers to lasting changes in altered sensory, cognitive, and neurophysiological effects that persist in the
meditator irrespective of being actively engaged in meditation at the moment (Cahn and Polich, 2006).
6
A full EEG stream for each subject free from any artifacts was fragmented into consecutive 1-
min epochs (5-6 one-minute EEG epochs for different subjects). The division of the EEG stream into
a 1-min intervals permitted us to obtain a relatively large number of the analysis epochs (within
which we searched for the naturally accruing quasi-stationary segments, see subsection 2.4. below) –
this was important for the unbiased estimate of the operational synchronicity index (its computation
requires enough data samples). Such an approach is justified because there are no fixed positions in
the EEG, and we can therefore divide it into any number of epochs with a length appropriate for the
particular experiment and analysis. More details and justifications could be found in Fingelkurts and
Fingelkurts (2008). Further data processing was done separately for each 1-minute epoch of the
signal.
Due to the technical requirements of the tools used to process the data, EEGs were re-sampled to
128 Hz. This procedure should not affect the results since 128 Hz sampling rate meets the Nyquist
Criterion (Faulkner, 1969) of a sample rate greater than twice the maximum input frequency for the
alpha activity, thus avoiding aliasing and preserving all the information about alpha activity in the
input signal. This method was considered sufficient since the sampling rate of the source signals was
significantly higher than required.
After re-sampling and prior to further processing procedures, each EEG signal was bandpass-
filtered (Butterworth filter of the sixth order) in the alpha (7–13 Hz) frequency band. Phase shifts
were eliminated through forward and backward filtering. The alpha frequency band was chosen for
several reasons. First, it has been repeatedly demonstrated that the DMN has significant positive
correlation with alpha rhythm4 (Laufs et al., 2003; Mantini et al., 2007; Jann et al., 2009) and that the
alpha band independent component of EEG-signal showed the highest spatial correlation to the DMN
template when compared to other EEG bands (Knyazev et al., 2011). Second, alpha oscillations
dominate EEG of humans in the absence of external stimuli when internal life (mind-wandering and
spontaneous thoughts) is most pronounced (Shaw, 2003; Palva and Palva, 2007; Klimesch et al.,
2007; Basar and Guntekin, 2009; Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2010, 2014). Thirdly, it has been
shown that operational connectivity within the DMN (identified by EEG alpha band) was smallest or
even absent in patients in vegetative state, intermediate in patients in minimally conscious state and
highest in healthy fully self-conscious subjects (Fingelkurts et al., 2012).
4 Though there are some studies where correlation of DMN with other frequency bands has been found (Berkovich-Ohana
et al., 2012, 2014; Neuner et al., 2014), the positive correlation of DMN with alpha band is the most reproducible.
7
2.4. Estimation of DMN OMs and Their Strength
As it has been proposed in our earlier study (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011) and confirmed in
subsequent work (Fingelkurts et al., 2012) a constellation of nine operationally synchronized cortical
areas indexed by 3 distinct OMs (frontal OM: F3-Fz-F4; left posterior OM: T5-P3-O1; and right
posterior OM: T6-P4-O2) could account, in large part, for the DMN. Therefore, in this study the
following EEG positions (and correspondent to them cortical areas, Koessler et al., 2009) were used
to estimate the operational synchrony within such OMs: EEG positions F3 and F4 (left and right
middle frontal gyri or Brodmann’s area 8), EEG position Fz (bilateral medial areas or Brodmann’s
area 6), EEG positions T5 and T6 (left and right middle temporal gyri or Brodmann’s area 21), EEG
positions P3 and P4 (left and right precuneus or Brodmann’s area 19), and EEG positions O1 and O2
(left and right middle occipital gyri or Brodmann’s area 18). The anatomical correlations of EEG
electrode positions used were taken from the reference study of Koessler et al. (2009), where a clear
match between the EEG electrode positions and anatomical areas of the cortex was clearly established
and verified through an EEG-MRI sensor system and an automated projection algorithm (see also
Kaiser, 2000 for the correlations between EEG activity in a given electrode position and its
correspondent cortical area).
In order to estimate the operational synchrony and its strength within given DMN OMs as well as
within the ‘whole DMN’, several stages of data processing are required. The details of these
procedures could be found elsewhere (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2008, 2015). Therefore, here we
provide only a brief overview of main steps. At the first step, each local EEG signal was reduced to a
temporally organized sequence of nearly stationary (quasi-stationary) segments of various duration
(~300 ms in average for alpha rhythm). To uncover these quasi-stationary segments from the complex
nonstationary structure of local EEG signals, an adaptive segmentation procedure was used
(Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2008, 2015). The aim of the segmentation is to divide each local EEG
signal into naturally existing quasi-stationary segments by estimating the intrinsic points of ‘gluing’ –
rapid transitional periods (RTPs). An RTP is defined as an abrupt change in the analytical amplitude
of the signal above a particular threshold which is computed based on statistical procedures which
have been experimentally established in modeling and empirical studies (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts,
2008, 2015). The RTPs themselves are very short in duration when compared to the quasi-stationary
segments, and therefore can be treated as a point or near-point (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2008,
2015). It has been proposed that each homogeneous segment in the local EEG signal corresponds to a
temporary stable microstate – an operation executed by a neuronal assembly (Fingelkurts et al.,
2010). The temporal coupling (synchronization) of such segments among several local EEG
recordings then, reflects the synchronization of operations (i.e. operational synchrony) produced by
8
different neuronal assemblies, which are located in different cortex regions, into integrated and
unified patterns responsible for complex mental operations (Fingelkurts et al., 2010).
Estimation of operational synchrony signifies the second step of analysis. Measurement of
operational synchrony estimates the statistical level of RTP temporal coupling between two or more
local EEG recordings (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2008, 2015). The measure tends toward zero if
there is no synchronization between EEG segments derived from different EEG channels and has
positive or negative values where such synchronization exists. Positive values (above upper stochastic
threshold) indicate ‘active’ coupling of EEG segments (synchronization of EEG segments is observed
significantly more often than expected by chance as a result of random shuffling during a computer
simulation), whereas negative values (below lower stochastic threshold) mark ‘active’ decoupling of
segments (synchronization of EEG segments is observed significantly less than expected by chance as
a result of random shuffling during a computer simulation) (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2008, 2015).
The strength of EEG operational synchrony is proportional to the actual (absolute) value of measure:
The higher this value, the greater the strength of functional connection.
Using pair-wise analysis, operational synchrony was identified in several (more than two)
channels – synchrocomplexes (SC); these define operational modules – OMs. The criterion for
defining an OM is a sequence of the same synchrocomplexes (SC) during each 1-min epoch, whereas
a SC is a set of EEG channels in which each channel forms a paired combination with valid values of
synchrony with all other EEG channels in the same SC; meaning that all pairs of channels in an SC
have to have statistically significant synchrony linking them together (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts,
2008, 2015).
The measure of operational synchrony is sensitive to the morpho-functional organization of the
cortex rather than to volume conduction and is independent of the signal power (Fingelkurts and
Fingelkurts, 2008, 2015).
2.5. Statistics
The strength of functional connectivity within the DMN and individual DMN OMs was assessed
using EEG operational synchrony (see previous subsection). The differences in strength of
operational synchrony between different conditions (pre-meditation and post-meditation) were
assessed using Wilcoxon Matched Pairs Test, which is used in the majority of functional connectivity
studies (for the overview, see Weiss and Rappelsberger, 2000). At first, all strength values of EEG
operational synchrony were averaged within each OM for all 1-min EEGs of all subjects per
condition (pre-meditation or post-meditation). During the final stage an average of operational
9
synchrony strength values for the whole DMN (includes all three OMs) was calculated for each
condition.
3. Results
We observed a significant decrease (p < 0.01) in the average strength of EEG operational
synchrony within the DMN taken as a whole in post-meditation condition compared to a pre-
meditation condition (Fig. 1). Analysis of separate subnets of DMN (indexed as OMs) revealed that
while right and left posterior OMs had a significant decrease in the strength of EEG operational
synchrony in post-meditation condition compared to a pre-meditation condition (p < 0.001 for the
right OM; p < 0.01 for the left OM), the frontal OM on the contrary exhibited a mild but statistically
significant increase (p < 0.05) in the strength of EEG operational synchrony (Fig. 1).
Decreases or increases in the operational synchrony (congruent with a group level) have been
found in 9 participants from 10 (though, for different OMs the single participant who did not show
difference between pre-post conditions was always different). Note that the topological distribution
was always the same – three modules for each participant.
Figure 1. EEG operational synchrony strength within the DMN and its subnets (OMs) as a function of
meditation training. DMN – default mode network; OM – operational module; R – right; L - left. Y-axis
indicates the strength of EEG operational synchrony. * – p < 0.05; ** – p < 0.01; *** – p < 0.001. Due to
very small values of standard error for all means, their values presented in the legend and not on the graph.
Standard error of the means: Frontal OM before meditation: 0.01 / after meditation: 0.01; Right posterior
OM before mediation: 0.02 / after mediation: 0.03; Left posterior OM before meditation: 0.02 / after
meditation: 0.02; Full DMN before meditation: 0.02 / after meditation: 0.02.
10
4. Discussion
Using operational synchrony analyses of resting-state EEG data, we examined differentiation of
functional connectivity of subnets within the DMN as well as dynamics of the functional connectivity
of the whole DMN in a pre-/post- meditation training design. Our results indicated that while there
was an overall decrease in the functional connectivity of the whole DMN after 4-month meditation
training, – a finding compatible with previous studies (Faber et al., 2004; Brewer et al., 2011;
Lehmann et al., 2012; Taylor et al., 2013; Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2014), – a significant functional
differentiation of separate DMN OMs was present (Fig. 1). Only right and left posterior OMs had
decreased strength of EEG operational synchrony in post-meditation condition compared to a pre-
meditation condition, whereas the frontal OM on the contrary had increased strength of EEG
operational synchrony (Fig. 1). These findings reveal that the phenomenon of DMN-change as a
function of meditation is more complex than previously thought. They pointed to a conclusion that
the DMN is comprised of multiple subsystems, each contributing to specific processes that
characterize different aspects or qualities of self-referential thought. Given the functional-
topographical heterogeneity of OMs within the DMN (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011; see also
Uddin et al., 2009; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010, 2014; Doucet et al., 2011; Yeo et al. 2011) combined
with meditation training induced specific changes in the functional connectivity within separate OMs
(Fig. 1), and keeping in mind that the phenomenon of self-awareness is multi-faceted (Musholt,
2013), one could speculate about the specific qualities that contribute to different aspects of self-
referential thought and the sense of self.
The major finding that deserves explanation is the functional connectivity increase found in the
frontal OM of the DMN as a result of meditation training. To understand this result we need to
consider multiple aspects of the self. It could be plausibly divided into being a self (i.e., being a
subject of self-conscious experience) and being cognitive of being a self (i.e., being able to represent
and reflect on oneself) (Musholt, 2013). While the former consists in having a first-person
perspective, the latter requires the ability to think about oneself as such and to explicitly represent
one’s own perspective, including autobiographical memories (Musholt, 2013). Based on the available
data from literature, we argue that the first-person perspective and the sense of agency5 (the
5 Even though some researchers attribute to the term ‘agency’ just the active (volitional) control or action, the term is
much broader. Starting from the early phenomenologists like Merleau-Ponty or Husserl, the agency is understood as
mines’, or the ‘sense of ownership’ of thoughts, perceptions, and actions relevant to selfhood (Metzinger, 2004; de
Vignemont and Fourneret, 2004; Hohwy, 2007; Blanke and Metzinger, 2009). In other words agency means the sense
that it is ‘I’ who is undergoing an experience in its implicit first-person mode of givenness (Gallagher, 2000; Zahavi,
2005). It is this ‘self-ownership’ that has been claimed to be the most fundamental aspect of phenomenal selfhood
11
witnessing observer) are presented by the frontal OM, while the posterior OMs are generally
responsible for the experience of the continuity of ‘I’ as embodied and localized within bodily space
(Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011).
Indeed, the brain structures participated in the frontal OM have been shown to be involved in a
complete self-consciousness (Uhtomskiy, 1966; Andrews-Hanna, 2012; Moran et al., 2013), where
one feels directly present as the center of an externalized multimodal perceptual reality (for instance,
the objects in one’s environment or phenomenal emptiness are always presented in a certain distance
and orientation from oneself) (Sims, 2003; Revonsuo, 2006; Trehub, 2007). Interestingly, the sense of
such a ‘center’ is never lost even in deep meditation (or sleep) and is repeatedly described as ‘the
unbroken experience of existence attained by the still mind’ or a ‘samadhi’ state and is the highest
aim of many different meditation techniques (Nash and Newberg, 2013, Raffone et al., 2014). From
this perspective it is not surprising that long-term meditation leads to an increased integrity in the
frontal OM of the DMN. Several other functions of the areas involved in the frontal OM could also
explain its enhancement after the course of meditation training.
Many studies documented that meditators report the development of an unconditional feeling of
loving-kindness and compassion as a result of meditation training (Ricard et al., 2014). It is the
frontal DMN subnet that has been repeatedly shown to be involved in these experiences (Lutz et al.,
2008; Davidson, 2010; Travis and Shear, 2010; Mascaro et al., 2013; Li et al., 2014). Furthermore, a
number of neuroimaging studies have demonstrated the crucial role of the frontal brain areas in
empathy (Eslinger, 1998), positive affect (Nitschke et al., 2004; Phan et al., 2004) and extreme joy
(Blood and Zatorre, 2001; Arnow et al., 2002; Hagerty et al., 2013) all of which are reported to
emerge as the meditation effects6 (Nash and Newberg, 2013; Ricard et al., 2014).
The chief role of the frontal DMN OM in self-referential processing was documented in the study
of patients with disorders of consciousness (those who are in vegetative or minimally conscious
states) (Fingelkurts et al., 2012). It was demonstrated that the frontal DMN OM had the strongest
decrease in operational synchrony strength as a function of self-consciousness loss, when compared
with the posterior DMN OMs. As the result of these findings it has been proposed that the frontal
DMN module most likely provides a critical self-related context (experience of agency) for all human
behaviors and activities (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011). This conclusion is also in line with the
fact that the frontal brain areas are reciprocally connected with nearly all other cortical, subcortical,
(Gallagher, 2000; Aspell et al., 2009; Blanke and Metzinger, 2009). In such conceptualization the agent could be a
passive observer, who just witnesses events, perceptions or thoughts in its implicit first-person mode of givenness.
6 While usually meditation effects considered to be positive and beneficial, they could become maladaptive and negative
if overexpressed in individuals with a particular set of constitutional neurophysiological characteristics (for a detailed
analysis see Fingelkurts et al., 2015).
12
and brainstem structures (Fuster, 1993; Noack et al., 2012), and thus, represent some kind of a hub
which integrates motivational, emotional, sensory, motor, and mnemonic information (Barbas, 2000),
serving as the ‘observing self’ to maintain any conscious state (Baars et al., 2003; Fingelkurts and
Fingelkurts, 2011; Qin and Northoff, 2011; Noack et al., 2012).
Another finding of the present study that fully confirmed our prediction was the marked decrease
in the integrity of posterior DMN subnets (Fig. 1). The following interpretation of this result could be
suggested based on the available literature about subjective experiences of meditators. Many
meditators report the sense of ‘absolute unitary being’ or ‘self-boundarylessness’, or loss of bodily
perceptions (Newberg et al., 2001; Newberg and Iversen, 2003). We suggest that such subjective
experiences when practiced systematically in the course of meditation training may result in the long-
lasting diminished integrity (measured by functional connectivity) of the occipito-temporal-posterior
DMN subnets (indexed as posterior OMs) as was found in the present study. More specifically, we
propose that decreased functional connectivity within these posterior OMs is responsible for
diminished experience of embodiment and localization of the continuity of ‘I’ within bodily space.
Indeed, the brain areas comprising the left and right posterior OMs have been shown to be involved in
the body self-consciousness (Haggard et al., 2003; Jeannerod, 2007) as well as interoceptive and
exteroceptive bodily sensory processing (Damasio, 1999; Critchley et al., 2004); and the dysfunctions
in such areas have been causally associated with out-of-body experiences (Blanke et al., 2002; Ionta
et al., 2011). Such conceptualization is in line with the studies of subjective experience during the
acute intensive mediation. It has been documented that in the acute intensive meditation some
meditators have reported achieving a state of complete loss of body ownership, where the body no
longer being perceived as belonging to the ‘self’ (Dor-Ziderman et al., 2013), which is strikingly
similar to the subjective experiences during de-personalisation syndrome (Berlucchi and Aglioti,
1997).
Furthermore, since the temporal areas that participate in the posterior OMs have been shown to
contribute to the integration of an object or self into a situation model, including a particular time,
place, and context, including autobiographical memories (Patterson et al., 2007; Walker et al., 2007;
Binder and Desai, 2011; Múnera et al., 2014), we may propose that the lack of functional integration
of such areas with other areas of the posterior OMs found in the present study as a result of
meditation training is consistent with frequently reported subjective feelings of meditators such as
peaceful state of mind, diminishing of ego borders and narrative thoughts7, transcending, expanded
consciousness, all-oneness, etc (Fischer, 1971; Goleman, 1996; Hinterberger et al., 2011). Such
7 Interestingly, narrative thoughts and mind wandering has been linked to negative emotions (Smallwood et al., 2009) and
a sense of unhappiness (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; Fell, 2012).
13
subjective effects of meditation are considered useful to cope with stress and responsible for the fact
that today, elements of meditation have been implemented in several therapeutic systems, such as
stress reduction (Ludwig and Kabat-Zinn, 2008) or cognitive therapy (Shonin et al., 2014).
To summarize, it is important to keep in mind that these separate modules of DMN persistently
exist in parallel (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011) suggesting that these three OMs are functionally
integrated with one another within the common neuronal spatial–temporal network (i.e., DMN), and
they are likely to interact or co-occur during many self-generated experiences (Andrews-Hanna,
2012) to support much of the mental content underlying self-referential mentation or thought and
form a coherent framework of a sense of self (Uddin et al., 2009; Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011;
Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014).
5. Conclusion, Significance and Limitations
The study of self is very broad due to the multi-faceted nature of self-consciousness phenomenon
itself (Musholt, 2013). The accumulated evidence suggests that the core of the experiential self is
constituted by self-referential processes (Northoff et al., 2006), that are represented
(neurophysiologically) through operationally integrated DMN of the brain (Christoff et al., 2003;
Wicker et al., 2003; Gusnard, 2005; Buckner and Carroll, 2007; Schilbach et al., 2008; Qin and
Northoff, 2011). It is further suggested that such DMN could be a ‘neural signature’ of self-referential
processes with a control function of the overall behavior from a first-person perspective (Fingelkurts
and Fingelkurts, 2011); meaning that the DMN maintains a first-person perspective for any
information or stimuli for perceiving, interpreting, responding to, remembering and even predicting
environmental demands or envisioning the future (Raichle, 2006).
Since the practice of meditation aims to reduce the prevalence of self-related thought chains and
dissolution of ego borders, it is commonly hypothesized that it should reduce the activity of or
synchrony within the DMN (Fell, 2012). At the same time, in light of the recent evidence on DMN
heterogeneity (Uddin et al., 2009; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010; Spreng and Grady, 2010; Leech et al.,
2011; Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2011; Salomon et al., 2014) we expected that while the whole
DMN should be clearly suppressed, it is possible for different subnets of DMN to respond differently
to the meditation training. Considering the functional differentiation of separate DMN subnets
(Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010, 2014; Doucet et al., 2011; Yeo et al. 2011; Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts,
2011) we specifically predicted that functional connectivity within the frontal DMN module should
increase, while the functional integrity of bilateral posterior modules of DMN should decrease as a
function of four-month meditation training.
14
The results of the present study fully support our predictions (Fig. 1). Such opposing changes
induced by the meditation training in the functional connectivity within the frontal and posterior
modules of DMN integrated with published data on functional-anatomic heterogeneity within the
DMN and on trait subjective experiences commonly found following meditation allow us to propose
that the first-person perspective and the sense of agency (the witnessing observer) are most likely
represented by the frontal DMN OM, while the posterior OMs are generally responsible for the
experience of the continuity of ‘I’ as embodied and localized within bodily space. Specifically,
increased integrity of the frontal and decreased integrity of posterior DMN modules may give a
neural explanation to the well-known subjective experiences of meditation training as ‘the unbroken
experience of existence attained by the still mind’ (Nash and Newberg, 2013), avoidance of intruding
unintended thoughts with simultaneous unconditional feeling of loving-kindness and compassion
(Ricard et al., 2014) and decreased disturbing interoceptive and exteroceptive bodily sensations
(Newberg et al., 2001; Newberg and Iversen, 2003).
Our analysis of the putative functions served by separate DMN modules suggests that such a net
comprises multiple but interacting subsystems, each contributing specific functions or qualities
characterizing self-referential processing. These findings have several implications.
In the theoretical domain, our findings add more evidence that the DMN consists of functionally
differentiable but interacting subdivisions or subnets and that analyzing each subnet (or module)
individually will lead to a richer understanding of the functions of the DMN and the phenomenon of
self. In the clinical domain, characterization of each of the subnets of the DMN in various clinical
populations may provide greater insights into which region of the default mode is compromised the
most in clinically impaired individuals, and thus provide further information about the underlying
neuropathology of a concrete pathological condition.
The main limitation of the present study is the lack of subjective reports reflecting the
phenomenological experience of the participants. The interpretations of the current findings were
done based on the published data about the trait subjective experiences commonly found following
meditation. Even though such approach is valid, future studies should adopt a
neurophenomenological study design (Lutz and Thompson, 2003), incorporating both EEG
recordings and first-person descriptions of the same subjects. Another limitation is the small sample
of participants; thus, the results of the present study warrant replication in a larger group.
Additionally it would be interesting to perform the same analysis of separate DMN subnets as in the
present study but separately for different meditative traditions.
Yet another potential limitation could be the absence of a control group. The design of the study
makes such a group unnecessary. Our study was longitudinal – subjects were required to meditate 20
15
min daily for a period of 4-month training course. Considering such a long period and completely
different life-styles of participants, as well as the multitude of inter-subject independent events
happening during this period, the only common factor was a daily meditation. Since all possible other
(not related to meditation) factors are random (in relation to the aims of the present study, as well as
among the subjects) it is very unlikely that they may produce any common effect. On the contrary,
they should cancel effect of each other at the group level. The fact that we have got statistically
significant result (in fact 9 from 10 subjects show decreases/increases in the same direction as for the
group level for each studied OM), which confirms the initial hypothesis, signifies that the effect is
related to a mediation (the only common factor present during very long period in all subjects) and
not to a random interference of other possible factors.
Acknowledgments
The authors did not receive funding for this study. The authors would like to thank Dmitry Skarin
for language editing and all participants of the meditation training course. Special thanks go to Tapio
Saarinen with whom the authors discussed DMN-related aspects, which then motivated the authors to
conduct this study.
References
Andrews-Hanna, J.R., 2012. The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation.
Neuroscientist 18, 251–270.
Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Smallwood, J., Spreng, R.N., 2014. The default network and self-generated thought:
component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1316, 29–52.
Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Reidler, J.S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., Buckner, R.L., 2010. Functional-anatomic
fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65, 550–562.
Arnow, B.A., Desmond, J.E., Banner, L.L., Glover, G.H., Solomon, A., Polan, M.L., Lue, T.F., Atlas, S.W.,
2002. Brain activation and sexual arousal in healthy, heterosexual males. Brain 125, 1014–1023.
Aspell, J.E., Lenggenhager, B., Blanke, O., 2009. Keeping in touch with one’s self: multisensory
mechanisms of self-consciousness. PLoS One 4:e6488, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006488
Baars, B.J., Ramsøy, T.Z., Laureys, S., 2003. Brain, conscious experience and the observing self. Trends
Neurosci. 26, 671-675.
Barbas, H., 2000. Connections underlying the synthesis of cognition, memory, and emotion in primate
prefrontal cortices. Brain Res. Bull. 52, 319-330.
16
Basar, E., Guntekin, B., 2009. Darwin's evolution theory, brain oscillations, and complex brain function in a
new “Cartesian view”. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 71, 2–8.
Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., Goldstein, A., 2012. Mindfulness-induced changes in gamma band
activity - implications for the default mode network, self-reference and attention. Clin. Neurophysiol.
123, 700-710.
Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., Goldstein, A., 2014. Studying the default mode and its mindfulness-
induced changes using EEG functional connectivity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1616-1624.
Berlucchi, G., Aglioti, S., 1997. The body in the brain: neural bases of corporeal awareness. Trends
Neurosci. 20, 560–564.
Binder, J.R., Desai, R.H., 2011. The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 527–536.
Blanke, O., Metzinger, T., 2009. Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends Cogn.
Sci. 13, 7–13.
Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., Seeck, M., 2002. Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature
419, 269–270.
Blood, A.J., Zatorre, R.J., 2001. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain
regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 11818–11823.
Boly, M., Tshibanda, L., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Noirhomme, Q., Schnakers, C., Ledoux, D., Boveroux, P.,
Garweg, C., Lambermont, B., Phillips, C., Luxen, A., Moonen, G., Bassetti, C., Maquet, P., Laureys S.,
2009. Functional connectivity in the default network during resting state is preserved in a vegetative
but not in a brain dead patient. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2393-2400.
Brewer, J.A., Worhunsky, P.D., Gray, J.R., Tang, Y.Y., Weber, J., Kober, H., 2011. Meditation experience
is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U.S.A. 108, 20254–20259.
Buckner, R.L., Carroll, D.C., 2007. Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 49-57.
Cahn, B.R., Polich, J., 2006. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol.
Bull. 132, 180–211.
Chen, A.C., Oathes, D.J., Chang, C., Bradley, T., Zhou, Z.W., Williams, L.M., Glover, G.H., Deisseroth, K.,
Etkin, A., 2013. Causal interactions between fronto-parietal central executive and default-mode
networks in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 19944-19949.
Christoff, K., Ream, J.M., Geddes, L.P.T., Gabrieli, J.D.E., 2003. Evaluating self-generated information:
anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. Behav. Neurosci. 117, 1161-1168.
Craik, F.I.M., Moroz, T.M., Moscovich, M., Stuss, D.T., Winocur, G., Tulving, E., Kapur, S., 1999. In
search of the self: a positron emission tomography study. Psychol. Sci. 10, 26-34.
Critchley, H.D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., Dolan, R.J., 2004. Neural systems supporting
interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 189–95.
Damasio, A.R., 1999. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness.
Harcourt Trade Publishers, USA.
17
Davidson, R., 2010. Empirical explorations of mindfulness: conceptual and methodological conundrums.
Emotion 10, 8–11.
de Vignemont, F., Fourneret, P., 2004. The sense of agency: A philosophical and empirical review of the
‘‘Who’’ system. Conscious. Cogn. 13, 1–19.
Dor-Ziderman, Y., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., Goldstein, A., 2013. Mindfulness-induced
selflessness: a MEG neurophenomenological study. Frontiers Human Neurosci. 7, 582,
doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00582.
Doucet, G., Naveau, M., Petit, L., Delcroix, N., Zago, L., Crivello, F., Jobard, G., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N.,
Mazoyer, B., Mellet, E., Joliot, M., 2011. Brain activity at rest: a multiscale hierarchical functional
organization. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 2753-2763.
Eslinger, P.J., 1998. Neurological and neuropsychological bases of empathy. Eur. Neurol. 39, 193-199.
Faber, P.L., Lehmann, D., Gianotti, L.R.R., Kaelin, M., Pascual-Marqui, R.D., 2004. Scalp and intracerebral
(LORETA) theta and gamma EEG coherence in meditation, in: Meeting of the International Society for
Neuronal Regulation. Winterthur, Switzerland.
Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Dosenbach, N.U., Church, J.A., Miezin, F.M., Barch, D.M., Raichle, M.E.,
Petersen, S.E., Schlaggar, B.L., 2008. The maturing architecture of the brain’s default network. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 4028–4032.
Faulkner, E.A., 1969. Introduction to the Theory of Linear Systems. Chapman and Hall, London.
Fell, J., 2012. I think, therefore I am (unhappy). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 132, doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2012.00132
Fingelkurts, An.A., Fingelkurts, Al.A., 2008. Brain-mind operational architectonics imaging: Technical and
methodological aspects. Open Neuroimag. J. 2, 73-93.
Fingelkurts, Al.A., Fingelkurts, An.A., 2010. Short-term EEG spectral pattern as a single event in EEG
phenomenology. Open Neuroimag. J. 4, 130-156.
Fingelkurts, An.A., Fingelkurts, Al.A., 2011. Persistent operational synchrony within brain default-mode
network and self-processing operations in healthy subjects. Brain Cogn. 75, 79-90.
Fingelkurts, Al.A., Fingelkurts, An.A., 2014. EEG oscillatory states: Universality, uniqueness and
specificity across healthy-normal, altered and pathological brain conditions. PLoS ONE 9(2), e87507,
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0087507
Fingelkurts, An.A,, Fingelkurts, Al.A., 2015. Operational architectonics methodology for EEG analysis:
Theory and results, in: Sakkalis, V. (Ed.), Modern Electroencephalographic Assessment Techniques:
Theory and Applications. Neuromethods 91, Springer, pp.1–59.
Fingelkurts, An.A., Fingelkurts, Al.A., Neves, C.F.H., 2010 Natural world physical, brain operational, and
mind phenomenal space-time. Phys. Life Rev. 7, 195–249.
Fingelkurts, An.A., Fingelkurts, Al.A., Kallio-Tamminen T., 2015. EEG-guided meditation: A personalized
approach. J. Physiol. Paris, Epub ahead of print, doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2015.03.001.
18
Fingelkurts, An.A., Fingelkurts, Al.A., Bagnato, S., Boccagni, C., Galardi, G., 2012. DMN operational
synchrony relates to self-consciousness: Evidence from patients in vegetative and minimally conscious
states. Open Neuroimag. J. 6, 55-68.
Fingelkurts, An.A., Fingelkurts, Al.A., Bagnato, S., Boccagni, C., Galardi, G. The chief role of frontal
operational module of the brain default mode network in the potential recovery of consciousness from
the vegetative state: A preliminary comparison of three case reports. Open Neuroimag. J. in press.
Fischer, R., 1971. A cartography of the ecstatic and meditative states. Science 174, 897–904.
Fletcher, P.C., Frith, C.D., Baker, S.C., Shallice, T., Frackowiak, R.S., Dolan, R.J., 1995. The mindʼs eye—
precuneus activation in memory-related imagery. Neuroimage 2, 195–200.
Fransson, P., Skiöld, B., Horsch, S., Nordell, A., Blennow, M., Lagercrantz, H., Aden, U., 2007. Resting-
state networks in the infant brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104 15531–15536.
Fransson, P., Skiöld, B., Engström, M., Hallberg, B., Mosskin, M., Aden, U., Lagercrantz, H., Blennow, M.,
2009. Spontaneous brain activity in the newborn brain during natural sleep—An fMRI study in infants
born at full term. Pediatr Res. 66, 301–305.
Fuster, J.M., 1993. Frontal lobes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 3, 160-165.
Gallagher, S., 2000. Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn
Sci. 4, 14-21.
Gao, W., Zhu, H., Giovanello, K.S., Smith, J.K., Shen, D., Gilmore, J.H., Lin, W., 2009. Evidence on the
emergence of the brain’s default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 6790–6795.
Goleman, D.J., 1996. The Meditative Mind: Varieties of Meditative Experience. Penguin Putnam, New
York.
Greicius, M.D., Kiviniemi, V., Tervonen, O., Vainionpaa, V., Alahuhta, S., Reiss, A.L., Menon V., 2008.
Persistent default-mode network connectivity during light sedation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 839–847.
Gusnard, D.A., 2005. Being a self: considerations from functional imaging. Conscious. Cogn. 14, 679-697.
Gusnard, D.A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G.L., Raichle, M.E., 2001. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-
referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
98, 4259-4264.
Hagerty, M.R., Isaacs, J., Brasington, L., Shupe, L., Fetz, E.E., Cramer, S.C., 2013. Case study of ecstatic
meditation: fMRI and EEG evidence of self-stimulating a reward system. Neural Plast. 2013, 653572,
doi: 10.1155/2013/653572.
Haggard, P., Taylor-Clarke, M., Kennett, S., 2003. Tactile perception, cortical representation and the bodily
self. Curr. Biol. 13, R170–R173.
Hinterberger, T., Kamei, T.,Walach, H., 2011. Psychophysiological classification and staging of mental
states during meditative practice. Biomed. Tech. (Berl.) 56, 341–350.
Hohwy, J., 2007. The sense of self in the phenomenology of agency and perception. Psyche (Stuttg) 13, 1–
20.
19
Ionta, S., Gassert, R., Blanke, O., 2011. Multi-sensory and sensorimotor foundation of bodily self-
consciousness - an interdisciplinary approach. Front. Psychol. 2, 383, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00383
Jann, K., Dierks, T., Boesch, C., Kottlowa, M., Strik, W., Koenig, T., 2009. BOLD correlates of EEG alpha
phase-locking and the fMRI default mode network. Neuroimage 45, 903–916.
Jeannerod, M., 2007. Being oneself. J. Physiol. Paris 101, 161–168.
Johnson, S.C., Baxter, L.C., Wilder, L.S., Pipe, J.G., Heiserman, J.E., Prigatano, G.P., 2002. Neural
correlates of self-reflection. Brain 125, 1808–1814.
Kaiser, D.A., 2000. QEEG. State of the art, or state of confusion. J. Neurother. 1530-017X, 57-75.
Killingsworth, M.A., Gilbert, D.T., 2010. A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330, 932.
Kircher, T.T.J., Senior, C., Phillips, M.L., Benson, P.J., Bullmore, E.T., Brammer, M., Simmons, A.,
Williams, S.C., Bartels, M., David, A.S., 2000. Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self processing:
effects of faces and words. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 10, 133-144.
Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., Hanslmayr, S., 2007. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition–timing hypothesis.
Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88.
Knyazev, G.G., Slobodskoj-Plusnin, J.Y., Bocharov, A.V., Pylkova, L.V., 2011. The default mode network
and EEG alpha oscillations: An independent component analysis. Brain Res. 1402, 67–79.
Koenig, T., Prichep, L., Lehmann, D., Sosa, P.V., Braeker, E., Kleinlogel, H., Isenhart, R., John, E.R., 2002.
Millisecond by millisecond, year by year: Normative EEG microstates and developmental stages.
Neuroimage 16, 41–48.
Koessler, L., Maillard, L., Benhadid, A., Vignal, J.P., Felblinger, J., Vespignani, H., Braun, M., 2009.
Automated cortical projection of EEG sensors: Anatomical correlation via the international 10-10
system. Neuroimage 46, 64-72.
Laufs, H., Kleinschmidt, A., Beyerle, A., Eger, E., Salek-Haddadi, A., Preibisch, C., Krakow, K., 2003.
EEG-correlated fMRI of human alpha activity. Neuroimage 19, 1463-1476.
Laureys, S., 2005. The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the vegetative state. Trends. Cogn.
Sci. 9, 556-559.
Leech, R., Kamourieh, S., Beckmann, C.F., Sharp, D.J., 2011. Fractionating the default mode network:
distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. J.
Neurosci. 31, 3217–3224.
Lehmann, D., Faber, P.L., Tei, S., Pascual-Marqui, R.D., Milz, P., Kochi, K., 2012. Reduced functional
connectivity between cortical sources in five meditation traditions detected with lagged coherence
using EEG tomography. Neuroimage, 60, 1574–86.
Li, W., Mai, X., Liu, C., 2014. The default mode network and social understanding of others: what do brain
connectivity studies tell us. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 74, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00074
Lou, H.C., Luber, B., Stanford, A., Lisanby, S.H., 2010. Self-specific processing in the default network: a
single-pulse TMS study. Exp. Brain. Res. 207, 27-38.
Ludwig, D.S., Kabat-Zinn, J., 2008. Mindfulness in medicine. JAMA 300, 1350–1352.
20
Lutz, A., Thompson, E., 2003. Neurophenomenology. J. Conscious. Stud. 10, 31-52.
Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., Davidson, R.J., 2008. Regulation of the neural circuitry of
emotion by compassion meditation: effects of meditative expertise. PLoS ONE 3, e1897, doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0001897
Mantini, D., Perrucci, M.G., Del Gratta, C., Romani, G.L., Corbetta, M., 2007. Electrophysiological
signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 13170-
13175.
Mascaro, J.S., Rilling, J.K., Tenzin Negi, L., Raison, C.L., 2013. Compassion meditation enhances empathic
accuracy and related neural activity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 48–55.
Metzinger, T., 2004. Being no one: The self-model theory of subjectivity. MIT Press, Cambridge.
Múnera, C.P., Lomlomdjian, C., Gori, B., Terpiluk, V., Medel, N., Solís, P., Kochen, S., 2014. Episodic and
semantic autobiographical memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res. Treat. 2014, 157452,
doi:10.1155/2014/157452
Moran, J.M., Kelley, W.M., Heatherton, T.F., 2013. What can the organization of the brain’s default mode
network tell us about self-knowledge? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 391.
Musholt, K., 2013. A philosophical perspective on the relation between cortical midline structures and the
self. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 536, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00536
Nash, J.D., Newberg, A., 2013. Toward a unifying taxonomy and definition for meditation. Front. Psychol.
4, 806, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00806
Neuner, I., Arrubla, J., Werner, C.J., Hitz, K., Boers, F., Kawohl, W., Shah, N.J., 2014- The Default Mode
Network and EEG Regional Spectral Power: A Simultaneous fMRI-EEG Study. PLoS ONE 9(2):
e88214, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0088214
Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., d'Aquili, E.G., 2001. The measurement
of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT
study. Psychiatry Res. 106, 113–122.
Newberg, A., Iversen, J., 2003. The neural basis of the complex mental task of meditation: neurotransmitter
and neurochemical considerations. Med. Hypothesis 61, 282–291.
Newen, A., Vogeley, K., 2003. Self-representation: searching for a neural signature of self-consciousness.
Conscious. Cogn. 12, 529-543.
Nitschke, J.B., Nelson, E.E., Rusch, B.D., Fox, A.S., Oakes, T.R., Davidson, R.J., 2004. Orbitofrontal
cortex tracks positive mood in mothers viewing pictures of their newborn infants. Neuroimage 21, 583–
592.
Noack, R.A., 2012. Solving the “human problem”: The frontal feedback model. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 1043-
1067.
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., Panksepp, J., 2006. Self-referential
processing in our brain. A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31, 440-457.
21
Ott, U., Walter, B., Gebhardt, H., Stark, R., Vaitl, D., 2010. Inhibition of default mode network activity
during mindfulness meditation, in: Abstracts of the 16th Annual Meeting of the Organization for
Human Brain Mapping (June 6–10) 2010, Barcelona, Spain, p. 8.
Pagnoni, G., Cekic, M. Guo, Y., 2008. “Thinking about Not-Thinking”: neural correlates of conceptual
processing during Zen meditation. PLoS ONE 3(9), e3083, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003083
Palva, S., Palva, J.M., 2007. New vistas for alpha-frequency band oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 30, 150–
158.
Patterson, K., Nestor, P.J., Rogers, T.T., 2007. Where do you know what you know? The representation of
semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8: 976–987.
Phan, K.L., Wager, T.D., Taylor, S.F., Liberzon, I., 2004. Functional neuroimaging studies of human
emotions. CNS Spectr. 9, 258-266.
Qin, P., Northoff, G., 2011. How is our self related to midline regions and the default-mode network?
Neuroimage 57, 1221-1233.
Raffone, A., Srinivasan, N., Barendregt, H.P., 2014. Attention, Consciousness and Mindfulness in
Meditation, in: Singh N.N. (Ed.), Psychology of Meditation. Nova Science Publishers, pp. 147-166.
Raichle, M.E., 2006. Neuroscience. The brain’s dark energy. Science 314, 1249–1250.
Revonsuo, A., 2006. Inner presence: Consciousness as a biological phenomenon. MIT Press, Cambridge.
Ricard, M., Lutz, A,, Davidson, R.J., 2014. Mind of the meditator. Sci. Am. 311, 38-45.
Salomon, R., Levy, D.R., Malach, R., 2014. Deconstructing the default: cortical subdivision of the default
mode/intrinsic system during self-related processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 1491-1502.
Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S.B., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Fink, G.R., Vogeley, K., 2008. Minds at rest? Social
cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the
brain. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 457-467.
Shaw, J.C., 2003. The Brain’s alpha rhythms and the mind. Elsevier Science BV, Amsterdam.
Shonin, E., Gordon, W.V., Griffiths, M.D., 2014. Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and meditation
awareness training (MAT) for the treatment of co-occurring schizophrenia and pathological gambling:
A case study. Int. J. Ment. Health. Addict. 12, 181-196.
Shulman, G.L., Fiez, J.A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R.L., Miezin, F.M., Raichle, M.E., Petersen, S.E., 1997.
Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9,
648–663.
Sims, A.C.P., 2003. Symptoms in the Mind: An Introduction to Descriptive Psychopathology. Elsevier
Health Sciences.
Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L.K., Phillips, L.H., 2009. Shifting moods, wandering minds: negative
moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion 9, 271–276.
Spreng, R.N., Grady, C.L., 2010. Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory,
prospection, and theory-of-mind and their relationship to the default mode network. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
22, 1112-1123.
22
Taylor, V.A., Daneault, V., Grant, J., Scavone, G., Breton, E., Roffe-Vidal, S., Courtemanche, J.,
Lavarenne, A.S., Marrelec, G., Benali, H., Beauregard, M., 2013. Impact of meditation training on the
default mode network during a restful state. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 4-14.
Thomason, M.E., Chang, C.E., Glover, G.H., Gabrieli, J.D., Greicius, M.D., Gotlib, I.H., 2008. Default-
mode function and task-induced deactivation have overlapping brain substrates in children.
Neuroimage 41, 1493–1503.
Travis, F., Shear, J., 2010. Focused attention, open monitoring and automatic self-transcending: categories
to organize meditations from Vedic, Buddhist and Chinese traditions. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 1110–
1118.
Trehub, A., 2007. Space, self, and the theater of consciousness. Conscious. Cogn. 16, 310–330.
Uddin, L.Q., Kelly, A.M.C., Biswal, B.B., Castellanos, X., Milham, M.P., 2009. Functional connectivity of
default mode network components: Correlation, anticorrelation, and causality. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30,
625–637.
Uhtomskiy, A.A., 1966. Dominanta. Nauka, Moscow-Leningrad.
Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Noirhomme, Q., Tshibanda, L.J., Bruno, M.A., Boveroux, P., Schnakers, C., Soddu,
A., Perlbarg, V., Ledoux, D., Brichant, J.F., Moonen, G., Maquet, P., Greicius, M.D., Laureys, S.,
Boly, M., 2010. Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative
brain-damaged patients. Brain 133, 161-171.
Von Stein, A., Sarnthein, J., 2000. Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: From
local gamma to long-range alpha/theta synchronization. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 38, 301–313.
Walker, J.E., Kozlowski, G.P., Lawson, R., 2007. A modular activation/coherence approach to evaluating
clinical/QEEG correlations and for guiding neurofeedback training: modular insufficiencies, modual
excesses, disconnections, and hyperconnections. J. Neurother. 11, 25-44.
Weiss, S., Rappelsberger, P., 2000. Long-range EEG synchronization during word encoding correlates with
successful memory performance. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 9, 299-312.
Wicker, B., Ruby, P., Royet, J.P., Fonlupt, P., 2003. A relation between rest and the self in the brain? Brain
Res. Rev. 43, 224-230.
Yeo, B.T., Krienen, F.M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M.R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., Roffman, J.L.,
Smoller, J.W., Zöllei, L., Polimeni, J.R., Fischl, B., Liu, H., Buckner, R.L., 2011. The organization of
the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125-
1165.
Zahavi, D., 2005. Subjectivity and selfhood. Investigating the first-person perspective. MIT Press,
Cambridge.
Chapter
The third chapter is dedicated to the exploration of the neurophysiological mechanisms orchestrating the steps from nociception to pain behaviors. We delve the intricate understandings of the initial step of pain known as transduction, from the triggering event to the first receptors bound in the skin. Subsequently, the pain signal navigates through the various fibers involved in pain response and in visceral receptors. These last visceral receptors play a crucial role in elucidating radiating pain phenomena supporting significant diagnosis of pain-related pathologies. The second step of pain progression involved the transmission of the nociceptive signal according the chemical substances release from the peripheral nervous system to entry into the central nervous system. Once integrated into the central nervous system, the journey of pain involves spinal cord areas to the supraspinal structures like the thalamus and the cortex leading to pain perception. Within the supra-spinal areas, the selection of accurate balance between two pivotal components of pain will evoke the final pain behavior: the sensory-discriminative and motivational-affective aspects. Profound understandings of pain mechanisms are essential scaffold for effective of pain treatment.
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, several theoretical models have been suggested as complementary to the adaptative information processing model of eye movement desensitization and reprocessing therapy. A narrative review of such models was conducted to assess the contributions of each, as well as their convergences, contradictions, and potential complementarity. Seven theoretical models were identified. All focus on the effects of EMDR therapy as a comprehensive psychotherapy approach with its principles, procedures, and protocols. Several refer to concepts related to propositional or predictive processing theories. Overall, the contribution of these proposals does appear to bolster Shapiro’s original AIP model, potentially offering additional depth and breadth to case conceptualization and treatment planning in clinical practice, as well as a more precise theoretical understanding. The current exploratory comparative analysis may serve as a preliminary baseline to guide research into the relative merit of suggested theoretical proposals to enhance current standards for the clinical practice and teaching of EMDR therapy.
Article
Full-text available
As reliable facilitators of characteristically unique altered states of consciousness that are notoriously difficult to comprehend, psychedelics naturally attract a multidisciplinary scope for their research, with direct relevance to neuroscience, pharmacology, psychology, as well as philosophy of mind, metaphysics, and epistemology — disciplines united by the common inquiry into the nature of conscious experience. Accordingly, the central focus of this paper concerns the metaphysical debate between internalism and externalism about whether psychedelic phenomenology — in particular, its ‘breakthrough’ level — could be said to directly ontologically depend on external factors, as well as the interpretive implications arising from each of those positions. The main contributions of the author of this paper include the provision of a coherent account of the essential phenomenal characteristics of psychedelic experiences, the integration of relatively fragmented philosophical debates on psychedelic phenomenology into the traditional internalist-externalist framework in metaphysics, as well as the exploration of the extent to which the externalist position on psychedelic phenomenology — traditionally associated with non-naturalism — is compatible with naturalistic explanatory frameworks. In Part 1, psychedelic phenomenology will be outlined first, after which the central inquiries of this paper will be contextualised and qualified. In Part 2, the internalist position on psychedelic phenomenology — both its strong and weak versions — will be discussed. In Part 3, the externalist position will be explored, which will be followed by a final evalutation between the three positions presented in this paper. The paper will conclude in favour of the weak internalist position, and that whilst the externalist position remains significantly more speculative than both the strong and the weak internalist positions, the externalist position cannot be rejected altogether until further progress in consciousness research is made.
Article
Full-text available
The primary determinant of the self (S) is the conscious experience (CE) we have of it. Therefore, it does not come as a surprise that empirical research on S mainly resorts to the CE (or lack of CE) that subjects have of their S. What comes as a surprise is that empirical research on S does not tackle the problem of how CE contributes to building S. Empirical research investigates how S either biases the cognitive processing of stimuli or is altered through a wide range of means (meditation, hypnosis, etc.). In either case, even for different reasons, considerations of how CE contributes to building S are left unspecified in empirical research. This article analyzes these reasons and proposes a theoretical model of how CE contributes to building S. According to the proposed model, the phenomenal aspect of consciousness is produced by the modulation—engendered by attentional activity—of the energy level of the neural substrate (that is, the organ of attention) that underpins attentional activity. The phenomenal aspect of consciousness supplies the agent with a sense of S and informs the agent on how its S is affected by the agent’s own operations. The phenomenal aspect of consciousness performs its functions through its five main dimensions: qualitative, quantitative, hedonic, temporal, and spatial. Each dimension of the phenomenal aspect of consciousness can be explained by a specific aspect of the modulation of the energy level of the organ of attention. Among other advantages, the model explains the various forms of S as outcomes resulting from the operations of a single mechanism and provides a unifying framework for empirical research on the neural underpinnings of S.
Article
Full-text available
Mindfulness can produce neuroplastic changes that support adaptive cognitive and emotional functioning. Recently interest in single-exercise mindfulness instruction has grown considerably because of the advent of mobile health technology. Accordingly, the current study sought to extend neural models of mindfulness by investigating transient states of mindfulness during single-dose exposure to focused attention meditation. Specifically, we examined the ability of a brief mindfulness induction to attenuate intimate partner aggression via adaptive changes to intrinsic functional brain networks. We employed a dual-regression approach to examine a large-scale functional network organization in 50 intimate partner dyads (total n = 100) while they received either mindfulness (n = 50) or relaxation (n = 50) instruction. Mindfulness instruction reduced coherence within the Default Mode Network and increased functional connectivity within the Frontoparietal Control and Salience Networks. Additionally, mindfulness decoupled primary visual and attention-linked networks. Yet, this induction was unable to elicit changes in subsequent intimate partner aggression, and such aggression was broadly unassociated with any of our network indices. These findings suggest that minimal doses of focused attention-based mindfulness can promote transient changes in large-scale brain networks that have uncertain implications for aggressive behavior.
Article
Full-text available
Alterations of the sense of self induced by meditation include an increased sense of boundarylessness. In this study, we investigated behavioural and functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of trait self-boundarylessness during resting state and the performance of two experimental tasks. We found that boundarylessness correlated with greater self-endorsement of words related to fluidity and with longer response times in a math task. Boundarylessness also correlated negatively with brain activity in the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus during mind-wandering compared to a task targeting a minimal sense of self. Interestingly, boundarylessness showed quadratic relations to several measures. Participants reporting low or high boundarylessness, as compared to those in between, showed higher functional connectivity within the default mode network during rest, less brain activity in the medial prefrontal cortex during self-referential word processing, and less self-endorsement of words related to constancy. We relate these results to our previous findings of a quadratic relation between boundarylessness and the sense of perspectival ownership of experience. Additionally, an instruction to direct attention to the centre of experience elicited brain activation similar to that of meditation onset, including increases in anterior precentral gyrus and anterior insula and decreases in default mode network areas, for both non-meditators and experienced meditators.
Article
Full-text available
It has been argued that complex subjective sense of self is linked to the brain default-mode network (DMN). Recent discovery of heterogeneity between distinct subnets (or operational modules - OMs) of the DMN leads to a reconceptualization of its role for the experiential sense of self. Considering the recent proposition that the frontal DMN OM is responsible for the first-person perspective and the sense of agency, while the posterior DMN OMs are linked to the continuity of 'I' experience (including autobiographical memories) through embodiment and localization within bodily space, we have tested in this study the hypothesis that heterogeneity in the operational synchrony strength within the frontal DMN OM among patients who are in a vegetative state (VS) could inform about a stable self-consciousness recovery later in the course of disease (up to six years post-injury). Using EEG operational synchrony analysis we have demonstrated that among the three OMs of the DMN only the frontal OM showed important heterogeneity in VS patients as a function of later stable clinical outcome. We also found that the frontal DMN OM was characterized by the process of active uncoupling (stronger in persistent VS) of operations performed by the involved neuronal assemblies.
Article
Full-text available
Several recently developed philosophical approaches to the self promise to enhance the exchange of ideas between the philosophy of the mind and the other cognitive sciences. This review examines two important concepts of self: the ‘minimal self’, a self devoid of temporal extension, and the ‘narrative self’, which involves personal identity and continuity across time. The notion of a minimal self is first clarified by drawing a distinction between the sense of self-agency and the sense of self-ownership for actions. This distinction is then explored within the neurological domain with specific reference to schizophrenia, in which the sense of self-agency may be disrupted. The convergence between the philosophical debate and empirical study is extended in a discussion of more primitive aspects of self and how these relate to neonatal experience and robotics. The second concept of self, the narrative self, is discussed in the light of Gazzaniga’s left-hemisphere ‘interpreter’ and episodic memory. Extensions of the idea of a narrative self that are consistent with neurological models are then considered. The review illustrates how the philosophical approach can inform cognitive science and suggests that a two-way collaboration may lead to a more fully developed account of the self.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter discusses various aspects of operational architectonics methodology for EEG analysis that have been developed over the course of last 17 years in relation to nonstationarity of brain functioning. At first we detail the peculiarities and evidence for a spatial and temporal nonstationarity in the EEG signal, then we review a theoretical framework that could integrate the existing data with a focus on theoretical advantages provided by an operational architectonics framework, and finally we describe the experimental results related to methodology. In the last part of the chapter we outline the application of OA methodology to clinical, pharmacological, cognitive, and neurophilosophical studies.
Article
Full-text available
The therapeutic potential of meditation for physical and mental well-being is well documented, however the possibility of adverse effects warrants further discussion of the suitability of any particular meditation practice for every given participant. This concern highlights the need for a personalized approach in the meditation practice adjusted for a concrete individual. This can be done by using an objective screening procedure that detects the weak and strong cognitive skills in brain function, thus helping design a tailored meditation training protocol. Quantitative electroencephalogram (qEEG) is a suitable tool that allows identification of individual neurophysiological types. Using qEEG screening can aid developing a meditation training program that maximizes results and minimizes risk of potential negative effects. This brief theoretical-conceptual review provides a discussion of the problem and presents some illustrative results on the usage of qEEG screening for the guidance of mediation personalization. Copyright © 2015. Published by Elsevier Ltd.
Book
What is a self? Does it exist in reality or is it a mere social construct—or is it perhaps a neurologically induced illusion? The legitimacy of the concept of the self has been questioned by both neuroscientists and philosophers in recent years. Countering this, in Subjectivity and Selfhood, Dan Zahavi argues that the notion of self is crucial for a proper understanding of consciousness. He investigates the interrelationships of experience, self-awareness, and selfhood, proposing that none of these three notions can be understood in isolation. Any investigation of the self, Zahavi argues, must take the first-person perspective seriously and focus on the experiential givenness of the self. Subjectivity and Selfhood explores a number of phenomenological analyses pertaining to the nature of consciousness, self, and self-experience in light of contemporary discussions in consciousness research. Philosophical phenomenology—as developed by Husserl, Heidegger, Sartre, Merleau-Ponty, and others—not only addresses crucial issues often absent from current debates over consciousness but also provides a conceptual framework for understanding subjectivity. Zahavi fills the need—given the recent upsurge in theoretical and empirical interest in subjectivity—for an account of the subjective or phenomenal dimension of consciousness that is accessible to researchers and students from a variety of disciplines. His aim is to use phenomenological analyses to clarify issues of central importance to philosophy of mind, cognitive science, developmental psychology, and psychiatry. By engaging in a dialogue with other philosophical and empirical positions, says Zahavi, phenomenology can demonstrate its vitality and contemporary relevance. Bradford Books imprint
Article
Cortical activity and perception are not driven by the external stimulus alone; rather sensory information has to be integrated with various other internal constraints such as expectations, recent memories, planned actions, etc. The question is how large scale integration over many remote and size-varying processes might be performed by the brain. We have conducted a series of EEG recordings during processes thought to involve neuronal assemblies of varying complexity. While local synchronization during visual processing evolved in the gamma frequency range, synchronization between neighboring temporal and parietal cortex during multimodal semantic processing evolved in a lower, the beta1 (12-18 Hz) frequency range, and long range fronto-parietal interactions during working memory retention and mental imagery evolved in the theta and alpha (4-8 Hz, 8-12 Hz) frequency range. Thus, a relationship seems to exist between the extent of functional integration and the synchronization-frequency. In particular, long-range interactions in the alpha and theta ranges seem specifically involved in processing of internal mental context, i.e. for top-down processing. We propose that large scale integration is performed by synchronization among neurons and neuronal assemblies evolving in different frequency ranges.
Article
The capacity to reflect on one’s sense of self is an important component of self‐awareness. In this paper, we investigate some of the neurocognitive processes underlying reflection on the self using functional MRI. Eleven healthy volunteers were scanned with echoplanar imaging using the blood oxygen level‐dependent contrast method. The task consisted of aurally delivered statements requiring a yes–no decision. In the experimental condition, participants responded to a variety of statements requiring knowledge of and reflection on their own abilities, traits and attitudes (e.g. ‘I forget important things’, ‘I’m a good friend’, ‘I have a quick temper’). In the control condition, participants responded to statements requiring a basic level of semantic knowledge (e.g. ‘Ten seconds is more than a minute’, ‘You need water to live’). The latter condition was intended to control for auditory comprehension, attentional demands, decision‐making, the motoric response, and any common retrieval processes. Individual analyses revealed consistent anterior medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate activation for all participants. The overall activity for the group, using a random‐effects model, occurred in anterior medial prefrontal cortex ( t = 13.0, corrected P = 0.05; x , y , z , 0, 54, 8, respectively) and the posterior cingulate ( t = 14.7, P = 0.02; x , y , z , –2, –62, 32, respectively; 967 voxel extent). These data are consistent with lesion studies of impaired awareness, and suggest that the medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortex are part of a neural system subserving self‐reflective thought.
Article
Due to its potential to concurrently improve work-related wellbeing (WRW) and job performance, occupational stakeholders are becoming increasingly interested in the applications of meditation. The present study conducted the first randomized controlled trial to assess the effects of meditation on outcomes relating to both WRW and job performance. Office-based middle-hierarchy managers (n = 152) received an eight-week meditation intervention (Meditation Awareness Training; MAT) or an active control intervention. MAT participants demonstrated significant and sustainable improvements (with strong effect sizes) over control-group participants in levels of work-related stress, job satisfaction, psychological distress, and employer-rated job performance. There are a number of novel implications: (i) meditation can effectuate a perceptual shift in how employees experience their work and psychological environment and may thus constitute a cost-effective WRW intervention, (ii) meditation-based (i.e., present-moment-focussed) working styles may be more effective than goal-based (i.e., future-orientated) working styles, and (iii) meditation may reduce the separation made by employees between their own interests and those of the organizations they work for.