ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The present study assesses the possible disruption effect of posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) with regard to the protective role of attachment on pain, among ex-POWs. While secure attachment seems to serve as a buffer, decreasing the perception of pain, this function may be disrupted by PTSS. The study sample included 104 subjects who were combat veterans of the 1973 Yom Kippur War comprising of 60 male ex-prisoners of war (ex-POWs) and 44 comparable male combat veterans. Both attachment and pain were investigated experimentally in the laboratory and via questionnaires. We found that ex-POWs showed higher levels of clinical pain and attachment insecurities compared to controls. Moreover, attachment avoidance and soothing effect of attachment (SEA) were both associated with lower levels of clinical pain. Most importantly, PTSS moderated the associations between attachment and pain, as well as the mediation role of attachment between captivity and pain. The results imply that although attachment can be an important resource for coping with pain, it can be severely disrupted by PTSS among trauma survivors.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Attachment security and pain The disrupting effect of captivity
and PTSS
Tonny Elmose Andersen
a,
,YaelLahav
b
, Ruth Defrin
c
, Mario Mikulincer
d
, Zahava Solomon
b
a
Department of Psychology, University of Southern Denmark, Denmark
b
Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel, Postdoc, University of Southern Denmark, Denmark
c
School of Health Professions, Tel Aviv University, Israel
d
New School of Psychology, Interdisciplinary Center (IDC), Herzliya, Israel
abstractarticle info
Article history:
Received 14 January 2015
Received in revised form 1 September 2015
Accepted 24 October 2015
Available online xxxx
Keywords:
Pain
Attachment
Posttraumatic stress
Captivity
POW
Trauma
The present study assesses the possible disruption effect of posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) with regard to
the protective role of attachment on pain, among ex-POWs. While secure attachment seems to serve as a buffer,
decreasing the perception of pain, this function may be disrupted by PTSS. The study sample included 104 sub-
jects who were combat veterans of the 1973 Yom Kippur War comprising of 60 male ex-prisoners of war (ex-
POWs) and 44 comparable male combat veterans. Both attachment and pain were investigated experimentally
in the laboratory and via questionnaires. We found that ex-POWs showed higher levels of clinical pain and at-
tachment insecurities compared to controls. Moreover, attachment avoidance and soothing effect of attachment
(SEA) were both associated with lower levels of clinical pain. Most importantly, PTSS moderated the associations
between attachment and pain, as well as the mediation role of attachment between captivity and pain. The re-
sults imply that although attachment can be an important resource for coping with pain, it can be severely
disrupted by PTSS among trauma survivors.
© 2015 Published by Elsevier Inc.
War captivity is an extreme traumatic experience that entails harsh
physical and psychological abuse, such as torture, deprivation of basic
needs, humiliation, threats and mock executions [1]. Even decades
after repatriation, captivity often has long-term psychiatric (i.e., PTSD),
somatic and interpersonal ramications [e.g.,2,3]. The present study in-
vestigates the relationship between attachment, pain, and posttraumat-
ic stress symptoms (PTSS) among ex-POWs and control veterans.
Pain, attachment and war captivity
Physical abuse during captivity might negatively affect pain [4,5].
Ex-POWs have been found to experience high levels of persistent clini-
cal pain and altered pain perception [4,5]. Furthermore, research indi-
cated that ex-POWs had dysf unctional pain modulation and higher
levels of pain reporting when their pain threshold was reached [4,5].
The emotional abuse asso ciated with captivity might impact the
ability to trust others, manifested in attachment [e.g.,1]. Attachment ori-
entations are cognitive-emotional schemas of the self and others [6].
Adult attachment can be conceptualized along two dimensions: attach-
ment anxiety (worry over the availability and positive regard of others)
and attachment avoidance (discomfort with closeness and dependence
on others) [7]. Low levels on both dimensions indicate attachment
security.
Attachment may change as a result of meaningful interpersonal life
events [8]. While positive life events are associated with attachment se-
curity [9], negative life events are implicated with attachment insecurity
[10]. As such, exposure to prolonged and deliberate interpersonal trau-
ma, such as captivity, may undermine attachment [1]. Indeed, studies
[11,12] revealed higher attachment insecurities, which increased over
time among ex-POWs, compared to decreasing or unchanging attach-
ment insecurities among controls.
Pain, attachment and PTSS
The mutual maintenance model [13] suggests that PTSD and pain are
mutually maintained by a variety of psychological mechanisms, includ-
ing elevated arousal, attention bias, reasoning bias, reminders of the
trauma, and avoidant behaviours. Indeed, research indicates that PTSS
are associated with higher clinical pain in both civilian and military
samples [1517]. Furthermore, traumatiz ed indivi duals experienced
more intense and widespread clinical pain, compared to healthy con-
trols and patients with anxiety [18]. A more compl ex pattern was
found with regard to experimental pain [14-16]. For example, a previ-
ous study indicated that traumatized individuals had higher pain
thresholds (i.e., decreased sensitivity to pain). However, at the same
Journal of Psychosomatic Research 79 (2015) 471476
Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Southern Denmark,
Campusvej 55, DK-5230 Odense M, Denmark.
E-mail address: tandersen@health.sdu.dk (T.E. Andersen).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.10.008
0022-3999/© 2015 Published by Elsevier Inc.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Psychosomatic Research
time they reported elevated pain when the thresholds were exceeded
[17].
Literature regarding PTSS and attachment implies a reciprocal rela-
tionship. The rst direction implies that attachment might predict
PTSS. Ex posure to trauma activates attachment by recalling gures
that provide a sense of protection [18]. This mobilization might act as
a resilience factor, helping the indivi dual co pe and decrease the risk
for PTSS. Moreover, even after the trauma, activation of the attachment
system might ease the negative effects of PTSS, creating a soothing effect
[18]. In line with this contention, a prospective study among Israeli un-
dergraduates indicated a relationship between attachment before the
Gulf War and the subsequent development of PTSS. Furthermore, re-
search indicated a soothing effect of attachment (SEA), i.e., positive ef-
fect of secure representations on cognitive manifestation of PTSD [18].
Participants with PTSD had longer response times in id entifying the
color of trauma-r elated words, compared to negative or neutral
words, indicating greater mental accessibility of the traumatic content
[19]. However, priming with a security-related word reduced this
heightened accessibility [18].
However, in the second direction of association, PTSS might detri-
mentally affect attachment. Symptoms such as avoidance or outbursts
of anger, may lead to negative interpersonal events, which may height-
en attachment insecurities [10]. Furthermore, it is suggested that while
a healthy attachment system seems to serve as a buffer against emo-
tional and physical stressors, it may be disrupted by traumatic reaction
[2].Specically, Herman observed that prolonged captivity harms
human relationships and leads to dysregulation of interpersonal func-
tioning [1]. Likewise, Mikuli ncer et al. [2] noted that the reactivation
of trauma, re ected in PTSS, might erode the sense of security and
lead to disorganization of the attachment system over time, so that its
functioning might be damaged.
Research supports both possibilities. Studies among ex-POWs found
that PTSS predicted increased attachment insecurities over time [11,12].
Furthermore, ex-POWs with chronic PTSD exhibited disruptions in the
soothing and healing functions of attachment [2]. Although ex-POWs
searched for representations of a security provider, this activation of at-
tachment failed to soothe them [2].
Attachment and pain
According to Bowlby [8], attachment motivates seeking proximity to
signicant others as a way of attaining security and comfort when in
pain. The relationship between attachment and pain might also rely
on affect regulation. It has been proposed that attachment insecurities
might be related to persistent pain due to dysfunctional affect regula-
tion and problematic adjustment to acute pain [20]. Research indicates
associations between attachment insecurities and persistent clinical
pain [20-23]. A number of experimental pain studies found attachment
insecurities to be associated with both lower [23-26] and higher pain
thresholds [22].
Although research on the effect of secure representations on pain is
quite limited, studies suggest that attachment may have a buffering ef-
fect on the experience of pain. Pictures of attachment gures, compared
to pictures of strangers, have found to lead to reductions in pain and
pain related neural activity [27]. Likewise, priming mental representa-
tions of loved ones had a positive effect, leading to higher pain
thresholds [28]
.
PT
SS, captivity, attachment and pain
The documented associations between attachment and pain [e.g.,23,
27], as well as PTSS and pain [e.g.,17] raise two questions. The rst
regards the mediation role of attac hment within the association be-
tween captivity and pain. Specically, whether attachment acts as a
mechanism, thereby explaining increased pain among ex-POWs. Expo-
sure to captivity might damage attachment, leading to high attachment
insecurities and low SEA. These negative effects on attachment may, in
turn, predict amplied pain.
The second question regards the moderation role of PTSS between
captivity, attachment and pain. While attachment as a buff er against
pain is well documented, it is still unclear whether this function of at-
tachment may be disrupted by traumatic response, i.e., PTSS. Specical-
ly, PTSS may not only lead to increased attachment insecurities, it may
also harm the protective function of attachment on pain. Hence, while
attachment security and SEA might be associated with lower pain and
act as a mechanism to help relieve the negative effects of captivity on
pain, this process might be cancelled or overshadowed by the negative
impact of high PTSS.
To the best of our knowledge both the mediation role of attachment
between captivity and pain, as well as the moderation role of PTSS be-
tween captivity, attachment and pain, have not been investigated. Ex-
amination of these signicant issues has both important theoretical
and clinical contributions. The present study aimed to ll these knowl-
edge gaps by exploring these issues among ex-POWs and controls.
Hypotheses
We hypothesized: A) ex-POWs, compared to controls, will experi-
ence higher pain threshold, levels of clinical pain and attachment inse-
curities and lo wer levels of SEA; B) PTSS will be associated with
higher pain threshold, levels of clinical pain and attachment insecurities
and lower levels of SEA; C) high attachment insecurities and low SEA
will be associated with higher levels of clinical pain and lower pain
threshold; D) PTSS will moderate between attachment insecurities
and SEA, and the clinical level of pain and pain threshold; and E) PTSS
will moderate the mediating role of attachment insecurities and SEA be-
tween captivity, clinical pain and pain threshold.
Method
Participants
The present study uses data from a longitudinal study on the psycho-
logical implications of war [29]. The sample included 104 Israeli combat
veterans of the 1973 Yom Kippur War: 60 ex-POWs (mean age 57.5 ±
3 years) and 44 control combat veterans (58.8 ± 4 years).
According to Israel's Ministry of Defense, 240 combat soldiers were
POWs during the Yom Kippur War. The ex-POW groups consisted of
veterans who were captured and imprisoned in Egypt or Syria. The du-
ration of captivity in Egypt was 6 weeks (44/60 of the present sample)
and 9 months in Syria (16/60) from 1973 to 1974. In previous measures
the ex-POW group included 164 participants in 1991, 144 in 2003, and
183 in 2008.
The control group included combat veterans from the same war,
who were never held captive. Co ntrols were selected on the basis of
similarity to the ex-POWs in regard to military and personal variables
such as age, combat exposure, and rank. In previous measures the con-
trol group included 185 participants in 1991, 143 in 2003, and 118 in
2008.
Overall 117.5% data were missing across the study variables. No dif-
ferences were found b etween the 2 groups with regard to religion
(χ
2
(2) = 1.82, n.s.), income (t(92) = 1.58, n.s.), age (t(100) = .28,
n.s.) and health before the war (χ
2
(1) = .28, n.s.). Twenty-six percent
of participants were married during the war and 70% had completed
high school.
Participants were obtained through the records of the Israeli Minis-
try of Defense. We sent letters of invitation to participate in the present
study, followed by a phone call two weeks later. Those who agreed to
participate in the study were scheduled for a single testing session.
Each participant signed an informed consent after receiving a detailed
explanation on th e study's aims and procedures. The stu dy was ap-
proved by the institutional review board of Tel-Aviv University.
472 T.E. Andersen et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 79 (2015) 471476
Materials and procedure
Heatpain threshold
Heatpain threshold was used to evaluate the sensitivity of the pain
system. The threshold was measured using a Peltier-based computer-
ized thermal stimulator (TSA II, Medoc Ltd., Ramat Yishai, Israel), with
a 3 × 3 cm contact probe. The probe of the stimulator was attached to
the forearm with a Velcro strap. Subjects rec eived four successive
ramps of gradually increasing temperature, starting from a baseline of
35 °C, at a rate of 2 °C/s with an inter-stimulus interval of 30 s. During
each ramp, subjects were asked to press a switch when a pain sensation
was rst perceived, thus dening heatpain threshold. Pressing the
switch resulted in an automatic recording of the threshold temperature
and reset the probe temperature to baseline. Heatpain threshold was
computed by averaging the readings of four successive trials [30].
The McGill pain questionnaire
The McGill pain questionnaire (MPQ; [31]) provides a quantitative
evaluation of the patient's clinical pain experience with a separate mea-
sure of sensory, affective and cognitive dimensions. We used two quan-
titative parameters from the MPQ: pain rating index (PRI) the total
sum of the values of words chosen out of a list of pain descriptors and
number of words chosen (NWC) the number of words chosen from
the list.
SEA
SEA was investigated experimentally in the laboratory. First, partic-
ipants were asked to provide the rst names of secure gures using
the 6-item WHOTO scale [32], i.e., people from whom they sought prox-
imity (2 items), provided them with a safe haven (2 items), and provid-
ed them with a secure foundation for engaging in other activities (2
items). Second, they provided the rst names of their parents, siblings,
grandparents, close friends, and current and previous romantic part-
ners. Third, we read them a list of 100 rst names and asked them to in-
dicate the names of people they knew (acquaintances). This built a list
of names for each participant of security providers, other close persons
not includ ed in the WHOTO scale, and acquaintan ces. These names
were used in the computerized tasks. No participant had difculty nam-
ing security providers.
We used Stroop color-naming procedure [18] to examine the effect
of secure representations in reducing the accessibility of trauma-
related mental representations. This procedure has been used in previ-
ous studies [6]. The target words were either war-related (e.g., captivity,
prison), negatively valenced (e.g., povert y, failure), or neutral
(e.g. , circle, dress). Words were printed in H ebrew and matched for
the number of letters, syllables, and frequency of use. Different blocks
of ex perimental trials were preceded by subliminal presentations of
the name of a participant's attachment gure [32] or the name of an ac-
quaintance. Following 30 practice trials, participants performed 144 ex-
perimental trials. Each experimental trial began with an X in the middle
of the screen, which was replaced by a rapid (32-millisecond) sublimi-
nal presentation of one of 4 prime names (i.e., attachment gure, close
friend, acquaintance, unknown) followed immediately by a masking
stimulus (an XXX pa ttern) for 500 ms. Immediately following the
prime, one of six possible words (2 captivity-related words, 2 negatively
valenced words, 2 neutral words) was presented for 1000 ms in 1 of 4
colors. Each combination of name and color was presented three
times with each of the two primes (12 trials for each word in each prim-
ing condition = 24 total trials for each word), for a total of 144 trials.
None of the participants perceived the subliminal presentation of the
names.
SEA
was measured as the response time for color-identifying war-
related words after attachment gure name-priming compared to the
response time for the same words after other-priming (i.e. , name of
close frien d, acquaintance or unknown). The lower the gap (i.e., the
lower the response time after attachment gure name-priming while
controlling for the response time after other-priming), the stronger
the soothing effect.
Attachment anxiety and avoidance
Attachment anxiety and avoidance were assessed using the 36-
items Experiences in Close Relationships questionnaire (ECR; [33]). Par-
ticipants were asked to think about their close relationships, without fo-
cusing on a specic pers on, and rate the exte nt to which each item
accurately described their feelings in such relationships using a
7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much). Eighteen
items measured attachment-related anxiety (e.g., I worry about being
abandoned) and 1 8 items measured avoida nce (e.g., I prefer not to
show a partner how I feel deep down). The reliability and validity of
the two subscales have been repeatedly demonstrated [e.g.,6]. In the
present study, the inventory was found to have high internal consisten-
cy (Cronbach's α = 0.89, 0.91 for attachment avoidance and attachment
anxiety, respectively).
The PTSD Inventory
The PTSD Inventory [34] was used to measure combat-related PTSS.
This instrument is based on the clusters of PTSS according to DSM-IV
[35]. The questionnaire consists of 17 statements describing different
expressions of the disorder following war experience. Respondents
rate each statement according to the frequency of the experience during
the last month, on a 4-point scale from never to very often.Thetotal
score for the scale was based on the total number of symptoms en-
dorsed. In the present study, the inventory was found to have high in-
ternal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.96). The scale was also found to
have high convergent validity when compared to diagnoses based on
structured clinical interviews [36].
Results
Inter-correlations between the main study measures are presented
in Table 1.
Hypothesis A. Captivity, attachment and pain.
To examine differences in pain and PTSS as a function of type of trau-
ma (ex-POWs versus combat veterans), we conducted a series of t-tests
for in depen dent samples with type of trauma as factor, and pain
Table 1
Inter-correlations between the main study measures.
Measure 1 2 3 4567
1. Pain
threshold
1 .09 .11 .06 .16 .07 .00
2. NWC 1 .95
⁎⁎⁎
.42
⁎⁎⁎
.18 .24
.26
3. PRI 1 .41
⁎⁎⁎
.15 .23
.25
4. PTSS 1 .36
⁎⁎⁎
.45
⁎⁎⁎
.15
5. Attachment
anxiety
1 .33
⁎⁎
.01
6. Attachment
avoidance
1 .02
7. SEA 1
M (SD) 45.10
(3.60)
8.24
(4.50)
18.09
(11.23)
6.92
(5.55)
3.02
(1.13)
4.10
(1.09)
999.91
(281.39)
Notes. 1. PTSS = posttraumatic stress symptoms; SEA = soothing effect of attachment.
SEA was measured in milliseconds. Pearson correlations are computed as case-wise rela-
tionships. Coefcients for SEA and other variables represent partial correlations.
= p b .05.
⁎⁎
= p b .01.
⁎⁎⁎
= p b .001.
473T.E. Andersen et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 79 (2015) 471476
measures (PRI, NWC and pain threshold) as well as PTSS as dependent
variables. To examine differences in attachment as a function of type
of trauma we conducted multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)
with type of trauma as factor and the two dimensions of attachment in-
security measures (attachment avoidance, attachment anxiety) as the
dependent variables. Lastly, we conducted analysis of covariance
(ANCO VA) with type of trauma as factor, response time for war-
related words after attachment gure name-priming as dependent var-
iable and response time for war-related words after other-priming as
covar iates, in order to examine SEA (see Table 2). We controlled for
Type I error with Bonferroni corrections.
Analysis of PRI and PTSS revealed signicant effect for type of trau-
ma. Ex-POWs reported higher pain ratings and more PTSS compared
to combat veterans. Analysis of NWC and pain threshold did not reveal
asignicant effect for type of trauma. Analysis on trauma-SEA did not
reveal signicant effect for type of trauma.
MANOVA on attac hment avoidance and attac hment anxiety re -
vealed signicant effect for type of trauma, Pillai's Trace F(4180) =
5.44, p b .001, Partial Eta
2
= .11. Ex-POWs reported higher levels of at-
tachment avoidance and attachment anxiety compar ed to combat
veterans.
Hypothesis B. PTSS, attachment and pain.
In order to examine the relationship between PTSS on the one hand,
and pain measures and attachment insecurities, on the other, we con-
ducted two-tailed Pearson correlations. In order to examine the rela-
tionship between PTSS and SEA, we conducted partial correlations
with PTSS as the independent variable, response time for war-related
words after attachment gure name-priming as the dependent variable,
and response time for war-related words after other-priming as covar-
iates (see Table 1).
The analyses revealed signicant positive correlations between PTSS
and PRI as well as NWC. Meaning, the more the PTSS, the more pain de-
scriptor words chosen and the higher the pain rating. Analyses revealed
signicant positive correlations between PTSS and attachment insecu-
rities. The more the PTSS, the higher the attachment insecurities. The re-
lationship between PTSS and pain threshold as well as SEA was non-
signicant.
Hypothesis C. Attachment and pain.
In order to examine the relationship between attachment insecu-
rities and pain measures we conducted series of linear regressions
with attachment insecurities as the independent variabl es and pain
measures as dependent variables. In order to examine the relationship
between SEA and pain measures we conducted a series of linear regres-
sions with response time for war-related words after attachment gure
name-priming as the independent variable, and pain measures as the
dependent variables. Response time for war-related words after other-
priming was used as covariate.
The relationships between attachment avoidance and attachment
anxiety and PRI (β = .20, p = .07; β = .09, p = .39, respectively), as
well as pain threshold (β =.15,p =.14;β =.02,p = .83, respectively)
were non-signicant. However, while the relationship between attach-
ment anxiety and NWC was non-signicant (β = .12, p =.26),there-
lationship between atta chment avoidance and NWC was signicant
(β = .21, p = .05); higher attachment avoidance implied more pain de-
scriptor words and a higher pain rating.
The relationship between SEA and pain thre shold was non-
signicant (β =.00,p = .99). However, th e rela tionships between
SEA and PRI as well as NWC were signicant (β =.66,p =.02;β =
.70, p = .02, respectively). The higher the response time for war-
related words after attachment gu
re n ame-priming (i.e., the lower
the SEA), the more pain descriptor words chosen and a higher pain
rating.
Hypothesis D. PTSS as moderator between attachment and pain.
To examine Hypothesis D, we performed a series of multiple regres-
sion analyses for attachment insecurities, SEA and the interactions with
PTSS, predicting pain measures, and controlling for 2 study groups (ex-
POWs vs. controls). All of our predictors were mean-centered prior to
entering them into the regression a nalyses. Signicant interactions
were probed using the Hayes's [37] PROCESS (model 1) computational
macro. Table 3 presents the regression coefcients.
The interaction between SEA and PTSS contributed signicantly to
the explained variance of pa in threshold. Simple e ffect analyse s re-
vealed that among participants with low PTSS there was a signicant ef-
fect of attachment soothing on the pain threshold (β = 1.03, p =.02)
the longer the response time for war-related words after attachment
gure name-priming (i.e., lower SEA), the lower the pain threshold.
However, among participants with high PTSS, the soothing effect on
pain threshold was not signicant (β =.29,p = .38). Other interactions
between SEA and PTSS in predicting pain measures were non-
signicant.
The interaction between attachment avoidance and PTSS contribut-
ed signicantly to the explained variance of NWC. Simple effect analyses
Table 2
Means, SDs, t tests/analyses of variance F results of pain and attachment measures among
ex-POWs and controls.
Ex-POWs Controls t(df)/F (df1, df2) Cohen's
d/η
p
2
n = 60 n = 44
M (SD) M (SD)
Pain measures
PRI 20.22 (10.66) 15.19 (11.49) 2.08
(83) .45
NWC 8.98 (4.07) 7.22 (4.91) 1.80 (83) .39
Pain threshold 45.30 (3.19) 44.83 (4.11) .65
⁎⁎
(78.66) .13
Attachment measures
Attachment
avoidance
4.47 (1.08) 3.60 (.87) 19.42
⁎⁎⁎
(1101) .16
Attachment
anxiety
3.37 (1.08) 2.56 (1.03) 14.56
⁎⁎⁎
(2101) .13
SEA 942.43(292.90) 1000.50(264.47) 2.50 (1, 97) .03
PTSS 9.74 (4.37) 2.61 (4.27) 7.90
⁎⁎⁎
(94) 1.65
Note. Ex-POWs = ex-prisoners of war; PRI = pain rating index; NWC = the number of
words chosen to describe pain; PTSS = posttraumatic stress symptoms; SEA = soothing
effect of attachment. SEA was measured in milliseconds.
= p b .05.
⁎⁎
= p b .01.
⁎⁎⁎
= p b .001.
Table 3
Hierarchical multiple regression analyses predicting pain measures with group, attach-
ment, PTSS, and the interaction between PTSS and attachment.
Predicting variables
Pain threshold NWC PRI
βββ
Model 1 Group .09 .12 .04
Attachment anxiety .17 .05 .00
Attachment avoidance .00 .13 .12
PTSS .19 .45
⁎⁎
.45
⁎⁎
Attachment anxiety × PTSS .05 .00 .05
Attachment avoidance × PTSS .06 .27
.27
Model 3 Group 11 .04 .03
PTSS .45 1.44
⁎⁎
1.48
⁎⁎
SEA .40 .71
.65
SEA × PTSS 2.654
1.29
⁎⁎⁎
1.30
Note: Ns ranged from 79 to 103. The table depicts a series of regression analyses. PRI =
pain rating index; NWC = the number of words chosen to descri be pain; PTSS =
posttraumatic stress symptoms. SEA = soothing effect of attachment. SEA was measured
in milliseconds.
= p b .05
⁎⁎
= p b .01
⁎⁎⁎
= p b .001.
474 T.E. Andersen et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 79 (2015) 471476
revealed that among participants with low PTSS there was a signicant
effect of attachment avoidance on NWC (β = 2.30, p = .02) the lower
the attachment avoidance, the fewer pain descriptor words. However,
among participants with high PTSS, the effect of attachment avoidance
on NWC was non-signicant (β =1.07,p =.22).
The interaction between attachment avoidance and PTSS contribut-
ed signicantly to the explained variance of PRI. Simple effect analyses
revealed that among participants with low PTSS there was a signicant
effect of attachment avoidance on PRI (β =.43,p = .04) the lower the
attachment avoidance, the lower the pain rating. However, among par-
ticipants with high PTSS, the effect of attachment avoidance on PRI was
non-signicant (β = .21, p = .2 1). Other interactions were non-
signicant.
Hypothesis E. The mediating role of attachment in the effect of captiv-
ity on pain PTSS as a moderator.
We conducte d a moderated mediation model using PROCESS
(model 14) to investigate whether the level of PTSS moderated the me-
diation role of attachment within the relationship between type of trau-
ma and pain measures. The correlation between type of trauma and SEA
was non-signicant (r = .16, n.s.). Hence, we did not include SEA in the
analyses as a mediator. In addition, the correlation s between type of
trauma and pain threshold (r = .06, n.s.) as well as NWC were non-
signicant (r = .19, n.s.). Hence, we did not include pain threshold
and NWC in the analyses as outcome measures.
The a nalysis included type of trauma as the predictor, attachment
anxiety and attachment avoidance as the mediators, PRI as the out-
come measure, and PTSS as moderator. The model revealed t hat
only attachment avoidance mediated the relationship between
type of trauma and PRI. Type of trauma had a signicant main effect
on attachment avoidance (the a path in mediation analysis), b =
.26, p b .05. In addition, PTS S signicantly moderated the link be-
tween at tachment avoidance and PRI, b = .31, p b .05. In other
words, the path (the
b path
in mediation analysis) going from at-
tachment avoidance (i.e., the mediator) to PRI (i.e., t he outcome)
was qualied by PTSS. As expected, a higher degr ee of attachment
avoidance increased PRI among participants with low PTSS, but did
not predict PRI among participants with high PTSS. In other words,
being an ex-POW was associated with hi gher attachment avoidance,
which in turn increased PRI among participants with low PTSS (Boot-
strap condence i nterval (CI) [.02, .30]). This indirect effect was non-
signicant among participants with high PTSS (Bootstrap condence
interval (CI) [ .18, .02]).
Discussion
The present study aimed to investigate the role of PTSS in the asso-
ciations between war captivity, attachment and pain. As expected, ex-
POWs reported higher levels of pain and attachment insecurities com-
pared to controls. The present ndings are consistent with previous
studies [4,12], indicating the widespread and long-term ramications
of war captivity. It is suggested that physical torture and psychological
abuse during captivity alter pain modulation and contribute to elevated
pain [38]. At the same time, the abusive relationship with the captor
may permeate the captive's personality, thereby harming basic personal
resources, such as attachment [1].
Consistent with previous studies [e.g.,14] we found that PTSS were
associated with higher levels of clinical pain. According to the mutual
maintenance model, PTSS and pain are mutually maintained by psycho-
logical mechanisms, such as reminders of trauma and avoidant behav-
iours [13]. However, another possibility is that physical harm resulting
from torture led to both PTSS and elevated pain. The present study did
not control for physical harm and hence cannot rule out this alternative.
Future studies should account for this while assessing the link between
PTSS and pain.
Results regarding the associations between PTSS and attachment in-
securities correspond with previous research [e.g.,18]. It might be that
elevated insecure attachment increases the risk for PTSS. However, it
is also possible that the negative interpersonal implications of PTSS pre-
dicted negative changes in attachment [10]. Due to the nature of the
present study, the precise nature of the association between attachment
and PTSS cannot be determined. However, it is suggested that the rela-
tionship between the two variables is mutual.
As expected, signicant correlations between attachment avoidance
and SEA, on the one hand, and clinical pain, on the other hand, were
found. These results are consistent with previous studies linking attach-
ment insecurities to pain [e.g.,20]. According to the attachment theory,
attac hment acts as a resource [8]. Hence, low levels of attachment
avoidance and high levels of SEA might impact coping with pain, leading
to decreased pain perceptions [20]. An alternative explanation, which
arises from social pain theory [39], suggests that physical pain and social
distress are based on a shared neurological system. Hence, high attach-
ment avoidance or low SEA might lead to elevated physical pain [39].
Contrary to our hypotheses, we did not nd associations between at-
tachment anxiety and pain. This discrepancy between our ndings
and existing theory and empirical ndings [20,21] is unexpected.
The main contribution of the present research relates to the moder-
at
ing role of PTSS between attachmen t and pain. While attachment
avoidance and SEA were related with pain among participants with
low PTSS, these associations became non-signicant among participants
with high PTSS. Moreover, the relationship between captivity and pain
ratings was mediated by attachment avoidance only among individuals
with low PTSS. Our results imply that although attachment is an impor-
tant personal resource, its positive impact on pain may not be strong
enough among trauma survivors who suffer from elevated PTSS. One
explanation for the present ndings offers that attachment does have
positive impact on pain among individuals with high PTSS. However,
this effect is obscured by the pervasive negative implications of higher
levels of PTSS on pain. An alternative explanation suggests that PTSS dis-
rupt the attachment system to such an extent [2],thatitbecomesinef-
fective in reducing pain.
One may suggest that re-experiencing of the traumatic event, as
manifested by high PTSS, exposes the traumatized individual to the lim-
itations of close relationships as a source of safety. At these moments,
the protective ro le of attachment gures (real or internalized) is
undermined as the mental apparatus is ooded. The traumatized victim
feels helpless and faces the failure of the attachment system to provide a
sense of security. Over time, this might erode the attachment system's
protective role against distress such as pain.
Several limitations may have affected our ndings. First, the present
study uses cross-sectional data, hence we cannot infer the direction of
causality between attachment, pain and PTSS. Second, the present
study uses a highly selective sample of ex-POWs and combat veterans.
This emphasizes the necessity of caution in generalizing our results.
The present ndings have important theoretical and clinical implica-
tions for the treatment of trauma survivors. Our results call attention to
the possible role of PTSS in the association between attachment and
pain. Ex-POWs who suffer from high levels of PTSS may be particularly
vulnerable to pain, partially due to damaged attachment. The present
results imply that although attachment can be an important resource
for coping with pain, it can be severely disrupted by PTSS. It is important
that clinical interventions will focus on the relief of PTSS before enhanc-
ing secure attachment.
The authors have no competing interests to report.
References
[1] J.L. Herman, Trauma and Recovery: The Aftermath of Violence, Basic Books, New
York, 1992.
[2] M. Mikulincer, Z. Solomon, P. Shaver, T. Ein-Dor, Attachment-related consequences
of war captivity and trajectories of posttraumatic stress disorder: a 17-year longitu-
dinal study, J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 33 (2014) 207228.
475T.E. Andersen et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 79 (2015) 471476
[3] Y. Lahav, R. Rodin, Z. Solomon, Somatic complaints and attachment in former pris-
oners of war: a longitudinal study, Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Process-
es, 2015 (in press).
[4] R. Defrin, K. Ginzburg, M. Mikulincer, Z. Solomon, The long term impact of tissue in-
jury on pain proce ssing and modulation: a study on ex-prisoners of war who
underwent torture, Eur. J. Pain 18 (2013) 548558.
[5] K. Prip, A.L. Persson, B.H. Sjölund, Pain when walking: individual sensory proles in
the foot soles of torture victims a controlled study using quantitative sensory test-
ing, BMC Int. Health Hum. Rights 12 (2012) 40.
[6] M. Mikulincer, P.R. Shaver, Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and
Change, Guilford, New York, 2007.
[7] K. Bartholomew, L.M. Horowitz, Attachment styles among young adults: a test of a
four-category model, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61 (1991) 226244.
[8] J. Bowlby, Attachment and Loss: Volume 3. Loss, Sadness and Depression, Basic
Books, New York, 1980.
[9] J.A. Crowell, D. Treboux, E. Waters, Stability of attachment representations: the tran-
sition to marriage, Dev. Psychol. 38 (4) (2002) 467 (2002).
[10] J. Davila, R. Cobb, Predicting change in self-reported and interviewer assessed adult
attachment: tests of the individual difference and life stress models of attachment
change, Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29 (2003) 859870.
[11] M. Mikulincer, T. Ein-Dor, Z. Solomon, P.R. Shaver, Trajectories of attachment insecu-
rities over a 17-year period: a latent growth curve analysis of the impact of war cap-
tivity and posttraumatic stress disorder, J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 30 (2011) 960984.
[12] Z. Solomon, R. Dekel, M. Mikulincer, Complex trauma of war captivity: a prospective
study of attachment and posttraumatic stress disorder, Psychol. Med. 38 (2008)
14271434.
[13] T.J. Sharp, A.G. Harvey, Chronic pain and posttraumatic stress disorder: mu tual
maintenance? Clin. Psychol. Rev. 21 (2001) 857877.
[14] A. Kraus, E. Geuze, C. Schmal, W. Greffrath, R. Treede, M. Bohus, E. Vermetten, Differ-
entiation of pain ratings in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder, Pain 143
(2009) 179185.
[15] J.D. Otis, T.M. Keane, R.D. Kerns, An examination of the relationship between chronic
pain and post-traumatic stress disorder, J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 40 (2003) 397406.
[16] R. Stam, PTSD and stress sensitisation: a tale of brain and body part 1: human stud-
ies, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31 (2007) 530557.
[17] R. Defrin, K. Ginzburg, Z. Solomon, E. Polad, M. Bloch, M. Govezensky, S. Schreiber,
Quantitative testing of pain perception in subjects with PTSDimplications for the
mechanism of the coexistence between PTSD and chronic pain, Pain 138 (2)
(2008) 450459.
[18] M. Mikulincer, P.R. Shaver, N. Horesh, Attachment bases of emotion regulation and
posttraumatic adjustment, in: D.K. Snyder, J.A. Simpson, J.N. Hughes (Eds.), Emotion
Regulation in Couples and Families: Pathways to Dysfunction and Health, American
Psychological Association, Washington 2006, pp. 7799.
[19] G. Emilien, C. Penasse, G . Charles, D. Martin, L. Lasseaux, A. Waltregny, P ost-
traumatic stress disorder: hypotheses from clinical neuropsychology and psycho-
pharmacology research, Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 4 (2000) 318.
[20] P.J. Meredith, T. Ownsworth, J. Strong, A review of the evidence linking adult attach-
ment theory and chronic pain: presenting a conceptual model, Clin. Psychol. Rev. 28
(2008) 407429.
[21] T.E. Andersen, O. Brink, A. Elklit, PTSD symptoms mediate the effect of attachment
on pain and somatisation after whiplash injury, Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health
9(2013)75
83.
[2
2] N.E. Andrews, P.J. Meredith, J. Strong, Adult attachment and reports of pain in
experimentally-induced pain, Eur. J. Pain 15 (2011) 523530.
[23] P.J. Meredith, J. Strong, A. Feeney, The relationship of adult attachment to emotion,
catastrophizing, control, threshold and tolerance, in experimentally-induced pain,
Pain 120 (2006) 4452.
[24] J.M. Meredith, A review of the evidence regarding associations between attachment
theory and experimentally induced pain, Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 17 (2013) 326.
[25] C.F. Sambo, M. Howard, M. Kopelman, S. Williams, A. Fotopoulou, Knowing you care:
effects of perceived empathy and attachment style on pain perception, Pain 151
(2010) 687693.
[26] C.L. Wilson, M.A. Ruben, A pain in her arm: attachment, social support, and the tour-
niquet task, Pers. Relat. 18 (2011) 241265.
[27] N.I. Eisenberger, S.L. Master, T.K. Inagaki, S.E. Taylor, D. Shirinyan, M.D. Lieberman,
B.D. Naliboff, Attachment gures activate safety signal-related neural region and re-
duce pain experience, PNAS 108 (28) (2011) 1172111726.
[28] A.C. Rowe, K.B. Carnelley, J. Harwood, D. Micklerwright, L. Russouw, C.L. Rennie, C.
Liossi, The effect of attachment orientation priming on pain sensitivity in pain-free
individuals, J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 29 (2012) 488507.
[29] Z. Solomon, D. Horesh, T. Ein-Dor, A. Ohry, Predictors of PTSD trajectories following
captivity: a 35-year longitudinal study, Psychiatry Res. 199 (3) (2012) 188194.
[30] G. Zeilig, S. Enosh, D. Rubin-Asher, B. Lehr, R. Defrin, The nature and course of sen-
sory changes following spinal cord injury: predictive properties and implications
on the mechanism of central pain, Brain 135 (2012) 418430.
[31] R. Melzack, The McGill pain questionnaire: major properties and scoring methods,
Pain 1 (1975) 12771299.
[32] R.C. Fraley, K.E. Davis, Attachment formation and transfer in young adults' close
friendships and romantic relationships, Pers. Relat. 4 (1997) 131144.
[33] K.A. Brennan, C.L. Clark, P.R. Shaver, Self-report measurement of adult romantic at-
tachment: an integrative overview, in: J.A. Simpson, W.S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment
Theory and Close Relationships), Guilford Press, New York 1998, pp. 4676.
[34] Z. Solomon, R. Benbenishty, Y. Neria, M. Abramowitz, K. Ginzburg, A. Ohry, Assess-
ment of PTSD: validation of the revised PTSD Inventory, Isr. J. Psychiatry Relat. Sci.
30 (1993) 110115.
[35] American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor-
ders, 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association, Washington DC, 1994.
[36] Z. Solomon, D. Horesh, Changes in diagnostic criteria of PTSD: implications from two
prospective longitudinal studies, Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 77 (2007) 182188.
[37] A.F. Hayes, Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analy-
sis: A Regression-based Approach, Guilford Press, New York, 2013.
[38] A.J. Carinci, P. Mehta, P.J. Christo, Chronic pain in torture victims, Curr. Pain Head-
ache Rep. 14 (2010) 7379.
[39] G. MacDonald, M.R. Leary, Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship be-
tween social and physical pain, Psychol. Bull. 131 (2) (2005) 202223.
476 T.E. Andersen et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 79 (2015) 471476
... Previous studies have shown links between PTSS and subsequent increases in attachment insecurity (Andersen et al., 2015;Benoit et al., 2010;Besser & Neria, 2012;Halpern et al., 2012;Mikulincer et al., 2011, Renaud, 2008Sciarrino et al., 2020;Solomon et al., 1998Solomon et al., , 2008. For example, one study found a positive association between PTSS and attachment insecurity among Israeli veterans who were involved in the 1973 Yom Kippur War (Mikulincer et al., 2011). ...
... For example, one study found a positive association between PTSS and attachment insecurity among Israeli veterans who were involved in the 1973 Yom Kippur War (Mikulincer et al., 2011). From the same cohort, Andersen et al. (2015) demonstrated that ex-prisoners of war (POWs) who experienced greater PTSS reported greater attachment insecurity. Other studies have shown that people with greater attachment insecurity prior to experiencing a traumatic event have more severe reactions to traumatic events than people with less attachment insecurity (Benoit et al., 2010;Besser & Neria, 2012;Halpern et al., 2012;Renaud, 2008;Sciarrino et al., 2020;Solomon et al., 1998Solomon et al., , 2008. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is an established association between posttrauma symptom severity (PTSS) and attachment insecurity (anxiety and avoidance). However, few studies have examined this association among community samples of trauma-exposed individuals and identified factors that might moderate these associations. We sought to (a) replicate existing associations between PTSS and attachment insecurity in a community sample of trauma survivors and (b) determine the potential moderating role of support-seeking and coping behaviors. Our sample included 824 trauma-exposed individuals (MPCL-5= 31.6, SD = 20.0; Mage = 37.4, SD = 13.2; 69.3% female; 79.1% White; 77.2% heterosexual). Participants completed an online survey via Amazon Mechanical Turk. Linear regression and moderation analyses tested the association between PTSS and attachment insecurity and whether support-seeking and coping behaviors moderated these associations. PTSS was positively associated with global attachment anxiety (β = .48, 95% confidence interval [.42, .54]) and global attachment avoidance (β = .06, [.15, .29]). The association between PTSS and attachment avoidance was weaker for people who reported greater instrumental and emotional support-seeking or greater active coping. This study provides evidence for the association between PTSS and attachment insecurity. Findings underscore the need to understand causal mechanisms underlying this association and critically evaluate how existing and future interventions can buffer attachment insecurity in trauma-exposed individuals.
... Three observations are worth noting. Firstly, the fact that fearful attachment was a key moderator contradicts literature mainly emphasizing the impact of insecure, dismissive or anxious attachment styles on victims exposed to war, the World Trade Center attack or rocket and mortar fire (Andersen et al., 2015;Neria, 2010, 2012;Besser et al., 2009;Dieperink et al., 2001;Fraley et al., 2006;Mikulincer et al., 2011;Mikulincer et al., 2014;Woodward et al., 2013). Neither did the results reveal secure attachment as a buffer against PTSD (Wisco et al., 2014). ...
Article
This study examined the impact of bombing on posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and psychiatric co-morbidity over time, whether this relationship was mediated by shattered world assumptions and altered self-capacities, and whether the mediational effects were moderated by attachment style and crisis support among Iraqi civilians. One hundred and fifty-nine Iraqi civilians completed questionnaires measuring the aforementioned psychological constructs. Bombing exposure was associated with PTSD and psychiatric co-morbidity over time; 87% and 77% met the criteria for PTSD at baseline and five months respectively. Shattered world assumptions and altered self-capacities mediated the impact of bombing exposure on PTSD and psychiatric co-morbidity over time. The mediational effect for shattered world assumptions was not moderated by attachment style and crisis support. However, the mediational effect for altered self-capacities was moderated by fearful attachment and a medium level of crisis support. To conclude, following bombing, Iraqi civilians’ assumptions about the world and others can change. These changes can have long term effects on psychological distress but are not influenced by childhood attachment experiences or the amount of crisis support received. Bombing can also change civilians’ perceptions of internal capacities like emotional regulation, particularly those with fearful attachment who rely on crisis support to some extent.
... In a future study, an active manipulation of attachment primes and the social context should be included. This could be done by the social exclusion paradigm [38] or with active priming of attachment security and insecurity [23,39]. The results should be interpreted with care, since attachment insecurity in general was very low in the sample and no control for perceived empathy was added. ...
Article
Objectives: Pain perception and pain behaviors are distinct phenomena with different functions. Pain behaviors are protective in their functions, which include eliciting empathy or caring behaviors from others. Moreover, pain behaviors are intertwined with interpersonal relationships with significant others, which is why attachment orientations have been suggested as interpersonal schemas moderating the association between pain and pain behaviors. The aim of the current study was to assess the impact of insecure attachment dimensions on pain behaviors in laboratory-induced pain. Methods: This experimental study included a sample of 60 patients with low back pain recruited from a large spine center in a hospital in Region of Southern Denmark. Patients were recorded on video during a cold pressor procedure and asked to rate their level of pain. Prior to the procedure, attachment orientations were assessed by the Revised Adult Attachment Scale. Two assessors independently coded the recorded video material for protective and communicative pain behaviors. Results: A positive correlation of moderate size was found between pain intensity and pain communication. As hypothesized, attachment anxiety moderated the association between pain and pain behaviors. A high level of attachment anxiety was associated with at weaker association between pain and pain behaviors. None of the attachment dimensions correlated with pain intensity or pain behaviors. Conclusion: The results indicate that patients with high levels of attachment anxiety may downplay pain and communication thereof. This finding is of potential clinical importance, since pain communication, among others, serves the function of eliciting caring behavior from healthcare personnel.
Article
Objectives: Psychological trauma often co-occurs with pain. This relationship has been explored using laboratory pain measures; however, findings have been mixed. Previous studies have limited operationalization of trauma (eg, posttraumatic stress disorder) or pain (eg, pain thresholds), which may contribute to conflicting results. Further, prior reviews likely underrepresent trauma experiences among people who are not receiving clinical care, limiting generalizability. Materials and methods: We systematically reviewed the existing literature on the relationship between psychological trauma (eg, car accidents, sexual assault, childhood abuse, neglect) and laboratory pain (ie, quantitative sensory testing measures of pain threshold, intensity, summation, modulation), using inclusive criteria. The direction of the relationship between psychological trauma and pain sensitivity was evaluated, and moderation by purported pain mechanism (ie, pain detection, suprathreshold pain, central sensitization, inhibition) was explored. Results: Analyses were conducted using 48 studies that provided 147 effect sizes. A multivariate random-effects model with robust variance estimation resulted in a small but statistically significant overall effect size of g=0.24 (P=0.0002), reflecting a positive association between psychological trauma and enhanced laboratory pain sensitivity. Upon examination of mechanistic moderators, this relationship appears driven by effects on pain detection (g=0.28, P=0.002) and central sensitization (g=0.22, P=0.04). While effect sizes were similar across all moderators, effects on suprathreshold pain and inhibition were not statistically significant. Discussion: Findings demonstrate an overall pattern of trauma-related pain enhancement and point to central sensitization as a key underlying mechanism.
Article
It is well-documented that insecure attachment poses substantial risks to post-trauma recovery. Although attachment anxiety reliably predicts and is associated with elevated post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSS), the attachment avoidance–PTSS link has been far less consistent. This suggests both attenuating and enhancing connections between attachment difficulties and post-trauma outcomes. The goal of this study is 2-fold: (a) to review and summarize extant evidence concerning the relation between insecure attachment and PTSS and (b) to review mechanism(s) underlying attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance that may explain the development of PTSS. Following a systematic review of the empirical literature investigating attachment, trauma, and post-trauma reactions, a total of 138 studies were identified and summarized. The review further identifies explanatory mechanisms underlying the association between insecure attachment and PTSS, including stress appraisals, cognitive factors, self-esteem, emotion regulation strategies, social factors, and trauma-specific factors (i.e., type of trauma, extreme stress). Implications for practice, policy, and research are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Chronic pain and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are frequently observed within the Department of Veterans Affairs healthcare system and are often associated with a significant level of affective distress and physical disability. Clinical practice and research suggest that these two conditions co-occur at a high rate and may interact in such a way as to negatively impact the Course of either disorder; however, relatively little research has been conducted in this area. This review summarizes the current literature pertaining to the prevalence and development of chronic pain and PTSD. Research describing the comorbidity of both conditions is reviewed, and several theoretical models are presented to explain the mechanisms by which these two disorders may be maintained. Future directions for research and clinical implications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
TSaChi eiN-Dor Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya attachment security contributes to resilience in times of stress, but it can be disrupted by traumatic events that shatter positive views of self and others. We followed israeli ex-PoWs of the Yom kippur War over 17 years and examined associations between trajectories of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and dis-ruptions in the regulatory functions of the attachment system. Fifty-nine ex-PoWs and 39 control veterans from the same war who had reported on PTSD 18, 30, and 35 years after the war performed laboratory cognitive tasks assessing activa-tion of mental representations of security in response to threat and the ability of these representations to color neutral stimuli with positive affect and reduce ac-cess to trauma-related thoughts. as compared to controls, ex-PoWs with persis-tent PTSD over the 17-year period exhibited dramatic disruptions in the soothing and healing functions of attachment security representations. These disruptions
Article
Objective: War captivity includes a unique constellation of simultaneous somatic and interpersonal assaults. This raises questions about the link between attachment and somatic complaints among ex-prisoners of war (ex-POWs). Although the attachment literature assumes that attachment affects somatic complaints and not vice versa, to date no empirical studies assess the association between the two variables over time. In this article we prospectively examine the association between attachment and somatic complaints over time among ex-POWs and comparable veterans. Method: The current study included two groups of male Israeli veterans of the 1973 Yom Kippur War: ex-POWs and comparable veterans who were not taken captive. Both groups were assessed via self-report measures at three times: T1 (1991), T2 (2003), and T3 (2008)-18, 30, and 35 years after the war, respectively. Results: Ex-POWs reported higher levels of somatic complaints and attachment insecurities. These levels increased over time compared to combatant veterans. Moreover, while there was a unidirectional influence of somatic complaints on attachment security over time among combatant veterans, this relationship was bidirectional among ex-POWs. Conclusions: The present study suggests that the combined physical and interpersonal assaults experienced during captivity have adverse effects on combatants and on attachment security, even three decades later. More important, in ex-POWs the relationship between these domains appears to be interactive and mutual, with one reinforcing the other, and vice versa.
Article
This study examined the stability of adult attachment representations across the transition to marriage. One hundred fifty-seven couples were assessed using the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; C. George, N. Kaplan, & M. Main, 1985), the Current Relationship Interview (J. A. Crowell & G. Owens, 1996), and measures describing relationship functioning and life events 3 months prior to their weddings and 18 months into their marriages. The authors tested the hypotheses that attachment classifications are stable and that change is related to experiences in the relationship and/or life events; 78% of the sample received the same primary AAI classification (secure, preoccupied, and dismissing) at both times. Change was toward increased security and was associated with feelings and cognitions about the relationship. Only 46% of participants initially classified as unresolved retained the classification, Stability of the unresolved classification was associated with stressful life events and relationship aggression.
Article
Characteristic features of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) include intrusive memories, avoidance, memory and concentration difficulties, and hyperalertness. Neuropsychological investigations of individuals with PTSD have suggested global and specific impairments of performance on standardized tests of memory. The use of the Emotional Stroop test has shown that trauma- related words are a sensitive measure of clinical state in PTSD patients. The Stroop paradigm has also shown that patients with PTSD appear to be characterized by implicit, explicit and autobiographical memory impairment. Available treatments for chronic post-traumatic stress disorder include cognitive-behaviour therapy, psychodynamic therapy and pharmacotherapy. Whereas drug treatment alone can rarely alleviate the suffering in PTSD, it appears to be most useful as an adjunct to psychotherapy. Tricyclic antidepressants are generally thought to be effective in alleviating symptoms, including nightmares, depression, sleep disorders and startle reactions, but are less able to relieve numbing. On the other hand, selective re-uptake blockers may be effective in decreasing numbing. However, rigorous clinical trials with double-blind placebo-controlled designs need to be performed to confirm these results. With new scientific discoveries in the understanding of PTSD, a new generation of pharmacological treatment is likely to emerge.