ArticlePDF Available

Gender and homosexuality attitudes across religious groups from the 1970s to 2014: Similarity, distinction, and adaptation

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study uses General Social Survey data to compare gender and homosexuality across American religious groups from the 1970s to 2014, examining three possible patterns for how evangelical attitudes relate to those of other groups: (1) they are similar; (2) they are different, but move together over time; (3) they are different and converge or diverge over time. Evangelical gender attitudes regarding work and family issues are more conservative than those of all other groups, but are adaptive to broad trends, changing at a rate similar to those of other groups. Evangelical attitudes toward the morality of homosexuality and same-sex marriage are more conservative than those of all other religious groups, and their rate of change is slower over time. Separate trends on the two issues suggest that gender and sexuality attitude change is decoupled, especially among evangelicals who are adapting more on gender while increasingly distinguishing themselves on same-sex relationships. A three-stage process of religious tension appears to characterize evangelical identity-building: (1) similarity, (2) distinction, and (3) adaptation.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Gender and homosexuality attitudes across religious groups
from the 1970s to 2014: Similarity, distinction, and adaptation
Landon Schnabel
*
,
1
Indiana University, Bloomington, United States
article info
Article history:
Received 7 January 2015
Received in revised form 5 September 2015
Accepted 29 September 2015
Available online 9 October 2015
Keywords:
Public opinion
Attitudes
Religion
Gender roles
Same-sex marriage
Morality of homosexuality
Symbolic boundaries
Culture
Identity
abstract
This study uses General Social Survey data to compare gender and homosexuality across
American religious groups from the 1970s to 2014, examining three possible patterns for
how evangelical attitudes relate to those of other groups: (1) they are similar; (2) they are
different, but move together over time; (3) they are different and converge or diverge over
time. Evangelical gender attitudes regarding work and family issues are more conservative
than those of all other groups, but are adaptive to broad trends, changing at a rate similar
to those of other groups. Evangelical attitudes toward the morality of homosexuality and
same-sex marriage are more conservative than those of all other religious groups, and
their rate of change is slower over time. Separate trends on the two issues suggest that
gender and sexuality attitude change is decoupled, especially among evangelicals who are
adapting more on gender while increasingly distinguishing themselves on same-sex re-
lationships. A three-stage process of religious tension appears to characterize evangelical
identity-building: (1) similarity, (2) distinction, and (3) adaptation.
©2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Should women be supported in the workplace and hold civil leadership positions? Should gay men and lesbians have the
right to get married? Gender and sexuality are contentious topics in both private and public debate, sparking conict in
American families, religious communities, and national politics. Examining beliefs about gender and sexuality across religious
groups provides an opportunity to consider not only attitude change, but also how social groups distinguish themselves from
one another (Edgell, 2012; Lamont and Moln
ar, 2002). Gender and sexuality are both central for boundary drawing among
religious groups and often seem to operate in tandem (Tranby and Zulkowski, 2012), but do American religious groups reveal
similar patterns for attitude change on both of these issues? Is it possible that those in conservative Protestant denominations
are adapting more on one issue while distinguishing themselves more on the other?
To investigate the questions at hand, I use General Social Survey (GSS) data and a work and family focused gender attitude
scale (Cotter et al., 2011)torst compare overall religious group differences and then compare rates of attitude change over
time. Following the examination of gender attitudes, I compare them to attitudes toward the morality of homosexuality and
same-sex marriage. Though I examine and discuss all major American religious groups, I focus on evangelicals because
American evangelicalism exemplies the drawing of symbolic boundariesdthey use moral stances to distinguish the people
*Corresponding author. Department of Sociology, Indiana University, 744 Ballantine Hall, 1020 E. Kirkwood Ave., Bloomington, IN 47405, United States.
E-mail address: lpschnab@indiana.edu.
1
The author owes special thanks to Clem Brooks, Youngjoo Cha, and especially Brian Powell for frequent support on this project. He would also like to
thank Cate Taylor, Lisa Miller, Emily Wurgler, Ariel Sincoff-Yedid, fellow participants in the summer cohort writing group, fellow participants in the S700
writing workshop, and the anonymous reviewers for insightful comments on earlier drafts. This paper was presented at the 2014 annual meetings of
Sociologists for Women in Society and the Society for the Scientic Study of Religion.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Social Science Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ssresearch
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2015.09.012
0049-089X/©2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e47
in their group from those outside their group (Smith et al., 1998; Tranby and Zulkowski, 2012). These examinations will reveal
whether conservative religion slows movement toward equality in the realms of gender and sexuality, and whether the
liberalization of gender attitudes and sexuality attitudes follow similar or different patterns. The ndings demonstrate that
evangelical Protestant gender attitudes are more conservative relative to other groups, but have changed at rates similar to
other groupsdin fact, there has been no clear differentiation in the rate of gender attitude change across religious groups
since 1994. Likewise, evangelical Protestant attitudes toward same-sex relationships are more conservative relative to other
groups, but they have diverged to become even more distinct over time.
1. Background
1.1. Symbolic boundaries and American evangelicalism
In the United States, religion is a particularly strong cultural power for shaping beliefs about gender and sexuality (Tranby
and Zulkowski, 2012). Culture inuences attitudes and action through ideas, symbols, and metaphors that function as tools in
acultural toolkit(Swidler, 1986). People draw upon these cultural tools to construct boundaries between themselves and
others that demarcate their membership in a worthy and moral group (Edgell et al., 200 6; Swidler, 1986). In this construction
of boundaries, they draw distinctions between the worthy in-group and the out-group of others(Edgell et al., 2006; Edgell,
2012; Tranby and Zulkowski, 2012). These boundaries are moral and symbolic rather than ethnic or material, and thus are
labeled symbolic boundaries (Edgell et al., 2006; Lamont and Moln
ar, 2002; Tranby and Zulkowski, 2012). Lamont and Molnar
(2002) dened symbolic boundaries as conceptual distinctions made to categorize objects, practices, peopledand even time
and spacedby which individuals and groups grapple with and eventually agree upon shared denitions of reality. Symbolic
boundaries come to take on a realcharacter, generating collective identities and separating people into distinct groups.
Sociological research on evangelicals highlights how they seek to be both in the world (adaptation), but not of the world
(distinction) (Bartkowski and Read, 2003; Gallagher and Smith, 1999; Heath, 2003; Tranby and Zulkowski, 2012). Symbolic
boundary drawing is central to evangelicals' strength because they thrive with a sense of embattlement with secular society
even as they adapt to it (Smith et al., 1998). This strength is the type that arises in strict churches that are in tension with the
outside world and thus reduce free-riding and foster commitment and vitality (Iannaccone, 1994; Stark and Finke, 2000).
Gender and sexuality attitudes are particularly important areas for the drawing of symbolic boundaries, especially for
evangelicals (Bartkowski, 2001; Edgell, 2006; Gallagher, 2003 2004; Heath, 2003; Hoffman and Bartkowski, 2008; Tranby and
Zulkowski, 2012).
Though evangelicals have constructed marked symbolic boundaries between themselves and other groupsddrawing on
gender and sexuality as important sites to emphasize cultural distinctions to show that they are not like the rest of the
worlddevangelical attitudes do seem to be adaptive to social change and broad cultural trends (Edgell, 2006; Wilcox, 2004).
Attitude change hints at potential shifts in the drawing of symbolic boundaries. The present study's comparative examination of
rates of change in gender and sexuality attitudes across religious groups will help us to better understand not just attitude
change on these relevant social issues, but whether and howevangelical symbolic boundaries are adapting to a changing world.
1.2. Gender attitudes
American gender attitudes surrounding issues of work and family changed rapidly through the early-1990s, but appeared
to stall in the mid-1990s, and were no more egalitarian in 2008 than they were in the early 1990s (Cotter et al., 2011). In light
of this slowing of gender attitude liberalization and lack of change in other measures of gender equalitydsuch as occupational
segregation (Charles and Grusky 2005)dsociologists have suggested that movement toward gender equality is uneven, has
slowed, and may even have stalled (Cotter et al., 2011; England, 2010; Gerson, 2010).
Cotter et al. (2011) argued that a neo-traditional frame of egalitarian essentialismhad arisen. Such a frame is new,
according to Cotter et al. (2011), because it combines some aspects of feminist equalitydwhich distinguishes it from the older
separate spheresframedwith traditional familism. Egalitarian essentialism asserts that women and men have equal but
essentially different natures that make them more or less skilled at different roles. In this framedwhich is highly consistent
with ideas surrounding intensive mothering and the opting outnarrative
2
(Stone 2007)dwomen are viewed as natural
caretakers who are inherently skilled at nurturing. Correspondingly, men are viewed as particularly able to produce in the
workforce and provide for their families. In sum, this frame's justication of gender roles moves away from explicit subor-
dination to assumptions about different propensities and skills, and mothering is spoken about as having value equal to, or
even greater than, paid labor. Cotter et al. (2011) assert that this framedby combining conicting elements of feminism and
traditional familismdfacilitates a return to traditional gender roles while denying implications of a lower valuing of women.
Such frames could be most prominent among religious groups that promote complementarian theology. Com-
plementarian theology argues that, having been created to complement one another, women and men are essentially
different and suited to different roles. Evangelical religious leaders developed the current version of
2
The opting out narrative suggests that women should and do leave professional careers when they have children because they feel naturally driven to
be full-time caregivers.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e4732
complementarianismdGod assigned women and men different, but equal, roles
3
that complement one anotherdin the 1970s
in direct response to second-wave feminism (Schnabel, 2015). By combining some elements of feminismdwomen and men
are of equal worthdwith complementary (i.e., traditional) gender roles, this religious response to feminism sought to
maintain men's leadership role and women's submissive role while denying claims that this means that women and men are
unequal. If evangelical religious leaders were successful in imparting this perspective to everyday evangelicals, there should
be different patterns of attitude change among evangelicals than among other religious groups. Research on gender and
American religion, however, shows that the rhetoric of religious leaders and the everyday beliefs and practices of religious
adherents are often only loosely coupled (Denton, 2004; Edgell, 2006; Gallagher, 2003). It is possible, therefore, that everyday
evangelicals have not followed the traditional elite discourse, and have liberalized along with other Americans.
1.2.1. Previous research on religion and American gender attitudes
Survey research on the relationship between religion and gender attitudes in America nds that conservative religion is
linked with conservative gender attitudes and that nonreligion is associated with particularly egalitarian attitudes
(Brinkerhoff and MacKie, 1984; 1985; Burn and Busso, 2005; Moore and Vanneman, 2003; Peek et al., 1991). Not only are
conservative Protestants less gender egalitarian than the nonreligious, but qualitative research on a Catholic, Black Protestant,
and LGBTQ inclusive mainline Protestant congregations has found surprisingly high levels of afrmation for traditional
gender roles in these more progressive congregations (Edgell and Docka, 2007). Based on previous research, we know that
religion inuences gender attitudes, but we do not know how that inuence may change over time (but see Petersen and
Donnenwerth, 1998 for an older study on attitude change).
1.3. Attitudes toward same-sex relationships
Previous research shows that sexuality, like gender, is an important area in which religious people draw symbolic
boundaries (Olson et al., 2006; Tranby and Zulkowski, 2012). Rather than examine all available sexuality attitudes, this
paper examines particularly relevant attitudesdbeliefs about homosexuality and same-sex marriage. In America, religion is
central to public debate surrounding same-sex marriage and is particularly salient in the shaping of attitudes toward ho-
mosexuality (Adamczyk and Pitt, 2009). Though attitudes toward the morality of homosexuality and same-sex marriage did
not start changing as early as did gender attitudes, they now appear to be consistently liberalizing to the apparent dismay of
many conservative religious leaders and pundits (Baunach, 2011; Loftus, 2001; Powell et al., 2010; Sherkat et al., 2011).
Petersen and Donnenwerth (1998) compared gender attitudes and support for gay rights from the early 1970s to the early
1990s, which is just prior to the stall in gender attitude change noted by Cotter et al. (2011). Using the General Social Survey,
they found that all Christian groups liberalized on gay rights and gender attitudes from the 1970s to 1993 (Petersen and
Donnenwerth, 1998). During this same perioddin which all Christian groups liberalized on limited gay rightsdAmerican
beliefs on the morality of homosexuality did not liberalize (Loftus, 2001). Analyzing the General Social Survey through 2002,
Linneman (2004) found that conservative Christians' views on the morality of homosexuality changed more slowly than
people who were not conservative Christians, but conservative Christians' attitudes toward basic gay rights were changing at
a similar rate to other Americans. He argued that conservative Christians perceived tension and hostility from lesbians, gay
men, and American society in general over LBGTQ issues, believed that their conservative Christian values were marginalized,
and therefore changed their views toward the morality of homosexuality more slowly. In a study focused on California, Lewis
and Gossett (2008) used news polls to examine attitude change on same-sex marriage. They found that Protestant's attitudes
changed more slowly than those of other religious groups.
In more recent years, the General Social Survey has included a measure of support for same-sex marriage. Not only is
religion an important factor for understanding American attitudes toward same-sex marriage, but Olson et al. (2006) found
that it has a stronger inuence than sociodemographic characteristics and Perry (Forthcoming) found that conservative
religious identity predicts stronger opposition to same-sex marriage than belief in inerrancy, belief in creationism, and
frequency of bible reading. Sherkat and colleagues (Sherkat et al., 2010; Sherkat et al., 2011) have used the same-sex marriage
measure to show that religion is a strong predictor for negative views of homosexuality and opposition to same-sex marriage,
and that the inuence of religion may be increasing over time. Similarly, Baunach (2011, 2012) found that opposition to same-
sex marriage was higher among all groups in 1988, and that groupsdsuch as evangelicals, Republicans, Southerners, and
Blacksdbegan to become more distinct from other Americans over time.
Other research, however, indicates that evangelical attitudes are changing more quickly than suggested by the afore-
mentioned literature. Conservative religious groups are now more willing to extend limited civil rights (e.g., freedom of
speech) to gay men and lesbians than they were in the past (Kenneavy, 2012). Similarly, Bean and Martinez (2014) found that
while many active evangelicals still view gay men and lesbians as immoral, some of them are open to the idea of civil unions.
Nevertheless, conservative religion is still one of the most important predictors for opposition to same-sex marriage in
America (Powell et al., 2010).
3
The theological literature promoting this perspective uses the term roleto make this point. Furthermore, much of the gender attitude literature also
uses the term (Cotter et al., 2011). Therefore, I use it at times for convenience and comparability while recognizing that it has been problematized as
presenting an overly static view of gender.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e47 33
1.4. The relationship between gender and same-sex relationship attitudes
Much of the literature tends to suggest that beliefs and attitudes about gender roles and same-sex relationships map closely
onto one another (McVeigh and Diaz, 2009; Tranby and Zulkowski, 2012), are inseparable parts of polarized worldviews (Luker,
2006), and operate in tandem (Kenneavy, 2012). The relationship, however, may not be as straightforward as it seems.
In some cases, gender and sexuality attitudes appear to shift differently over time. Whereas a large part of gender attitude
change can be explained by generational change (Brooks and Bolzendahl, 2004; Pampel, 2011; Schnittker et al., 2003), Baunach
(2011) showed that much of the recent rapid rise in support for same-sex marriage is due to intracohort changedor people
changing their views over timedrather than younger generations replacing older generations.
4
Other studies have discussed
how gender and sexuality attitudes are heterogeneous, suggesting that they are related, but in complex ways (Bolzendahl and
Myers, 2004; Schnittker et al., 2003). One recent study explicitly argued that value issues are not always aligned, using abortion
and gay rights attitudes to highlight attitude asynchrony (Dillon, 2014). Finally, some research has suggested that future studies
might be able to uncover a decoupling of rates of change in gender and sexuality attitudes across religious groups (Powell et al.,
2010). In other words, one set of beliefs could change more quickly than the other even in the presence of a continued cor-
relation between the attitudes, and the relative rates of change could differ across religious groups.
1.5. Expectations
This study uses the General Social Survey to examine change in gender and sexuality attitudes over time across religious
groups to assess whether they reveal similar or divergent trends. Three competing patterns of attitude change are possible
and will be considered, rst for gender attitudes and then for sexuality attitudes:
(1) Evangelical attitudes could be similar to those of other groups. In this case, evangelical attitudes would be just as
egalitarian as those of other groups, and their attitudes would move along with other groups to maintain this similarity.
(2) Evangelicals could have more conservative attitudes overall, but move along with everyone else. In this case, their attitudes
would be distinctive at individual points in time, but adaptive to broad social forces over time, changing just as rapidly
as those of other religious groups.
(3) Evangelical attitudes could be distinctive and change at a different rate than that of other groups. If evangelical attitudes
change more slowly than other groups, they would become increasingly different over time.
Gender and sexuality attitudes could t the same pattern, or they could follow different patterns. If evangelicals have
adapted their attitudes more in one area and come to distinguish themselves more in the other, it would not only suggest a
decoupling of change in gender and sexuality attitudes, but also a remapping of symbolic boundariesdor how evangelicals
are distinguishing themselves from the broader culture.
2. Data, measures, and methods
2.1. Data
I use General Social Survey (GSS) data to analyze gender attitude and sexuality attitude change across religious categories.
The NORC General Social Survey is a probability-based face-to-face interview sampling of non-institutionalized U.S. adults 18
and older, elded from 1972 through 2014. Following recent work on gender attitude change (Cotter et al., 2011), I examine
four key gender attitude measures
5
that have been consistently asked from 1977 to 2014 (N ¼21,435).
6
Separate samples are
used for corresponding analyses of attitudes toward the morality of homosexuality (asked from 1973 to 2014; N ¼29,983) and
support for same-sex marriage measures (asked in 1988 and then again from 2004 to 2014; N ¼8785).
2.2. Dependent variables
2.2.1. Gender attitudes scale
This study uses the same standardized gender attitudes scaledconstructed by standardizing and then summing scores for
each of four gender attitude measures (Cronbach's
a
¼0.70)dused by Cotter et al. (2011). Using the same scale allows for
comparison with, and extension of, Cotterand colleagues' ndings. Three items ask the respondent whether and how strongly
s/he agrees (strongly disagree ¼1 to strongly agree ¼4) with the following statements:
4
Generational change does still account for a signicant portion of the change in same-sex marriage attitudes (Sherkat et al., 2011).
5
There are other repeated questions in the GSS, but the survey stopped asking them in 1998.
6
I do not include the 1982 and 1987 Black oversamples, and restrict my analyses to respondents for whom complete data are available, losing 10.2% of
the total 23,874 for missing controls, primarily due to the income measure. As a sensitivity test, I used a multiple imputation methoddGaussian normal
regression imputationdto replace missing values and arrived at substantively equivalent results across models.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e4734
A working mother can establish just as warm and secure a relationship with her children as a mother who does not work.
A preschool child is likely to suffer if his or her mother works.
It is much better for everyone involved if the man is the achieveroutside the home and the woman takes care of the home
and family.
These are coded so that higher values are more egalitarian. The other measure is dichotomous, asking the respondents
whether they agree (disagree ¼1) with the following statement:
Most men are better suited emotionally for politics than are most women.
Taken together, these four variables provide a good measuredthe best available for comparing attitude change from the
1970s to the presentdof a respondent's level of gender essentialism and her/his attitudes about gender roles, especially in
regard to work and family issues. See Table 1 for descriptive statistics for this measure and the others included in this study.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics.
Measures Metric Mean SD Range
Dependent Variables
Gender Attitudes Scale Standardized and Summated 0.013 0.726 1.922 to 1.297
A working mother can establish just
as warm and secure a relationship
with her children as a mother
who does not work.
Strongly Disagree ¼1 to Strongly Agree ¼4 2.840 0.880 1e4
A preschool child is likely to suffer
if his or her mother works.
Strongly Agree ¼1 to Strongly Disagree ¼4 2.559 0.808 1e4
It is much better for everyone
involved if the man is the achiever
outside the home and the woman
takes care of the home and family.
Strongly Agree ¼1 to Strongly Disagree ¼4 2.720 0.857 1e4
Most Men are better suited
emotionally for politics
than are most women.
Disagree ¼1 0.741
Morality of Homosexuality
a
Always Wrong ¼1, Almost Always
Wrong ¼2, Sometimes Wrong ¼3,
Not Wrong at All ¼4
1.920 1.294 1e4
Support for Same-Sex Marriage
b
Strongly Disagree ¼1 to Strongly Agree ¼5 3.190 1.519 1e5
Religious Category
Evangelical Evangelical ¼1 0.263
Black Protestant Black Protestant ¼1 0.080
Mainline Mainline ¼1 0.207
Catholic Catholic ¼1 0.248
No Afliation No Afliation ¼1 0.128
Jewish Jewish ¼1 0.020
Other Faith Other Faith ¼1 0.054
Survey Year
Year Spline (1977e1994) 1977 ¼0 to 1994 ¼17; All Following Years ¼17 14.261 4.642 0e17
Year Spline (1994e2000) 1977e1994 ¼0 to 2000 ¼6; All Following Years ¼6 2.922 2.808 0e6
Year Spline (2000e2014) 1977e2000 ¼0 to 2014 ¼14 3.050 4.710 0e14
Controls
Female Female ¼1 0.562
Race
White White ¼1 0.811
Black Black ¼1 0.130
Other Other ¼1 0.059
Age in Decades In Decades 4.535 1.684 1.8e8.9
Parental Status Parent ¼1 0.725
Marital Status Married ¼1 0.513
Female Work Status If Female, Working Full Time ¼1; If Male,
Spouse Working Full Time ¼1
0.347
South South ¼1 0.356
Highest Degree Earned
Less Than High School Less than High School Diploma ¼1 0.170
High School High School Diploma ¼1 0.521
Some College Some College ¼1 0.066
Bachelor Bachelor's Degree ¼1 0.163
Graduate Graduate Degree ¼1 0.080
Family Income Ination-Adjusted (2000) Tens-of-Thousands of Dollars 4.640 3.790 0.037e18.039
N¼21,435.
a
N for Morality of Homosexuality is 29,983.
b
N for Support for Same-Sex Marriage is 8785.
Source: General Social Survey 1977e2014
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e47 35
2.2.2. Morality of homosexuality and support for same-sex marriage
To compare gender attitudes with attitude change toward gay and lesbian Americans, I use a measure of attitudes toward
the morality of homosexuality and a measure of support forsame-sex marriage.
7
Fielded consistently since 1973, the morality
of homosexuality question asks respondents: What about sexual relations between two adults of the same sexddo you think
it is (1) always wrong, (2) almost always wrong, (3) wrong only sometimes, or (4) not wrong at all?Fielded in 1988 and then
again from 2004 to 2014, the support for same-sex marriage question asks whether respondents (1) strongly disagree to (5)
strongly agree with the following statement: Homosexual couples should have the right to marry one another.
2.3. Key independent variables
2.3.1. Religious afliation
Religious afliation is measured using the Steensland et al. (2000) religious categorization scheme (RELTRAD), which
allows for meaningful comparisons between evangelicals and other religious groups to explore trends in distinction and
adaptation. The scheme uses self-reported denominational afliation to place respondents into seven categories based on the
historical development of religious traditions in the United States: evangelical Protestant, Black Protestant, mainline Prot-
estant, Catholic, no afliation, Jewish, and other faith.
2.3.2. Year
To compare gender attitude change over time, I use a best-tting spline function with knots(or hinges) at the same
locationsd1994 and 2000dused by Cotter et al. (2011). A spline function splices linesdspecically, it joins together two or
more lines with different slopes. The slopes, hingedat statistically driven locations, are then estimated by transforming year
so that each period between the hinges can be interpreted as a linear term for that time range.
8
As Cotter et al. (2011) showed,
American gender attitudes hingeat 1994 and 2000. As presented in the models, the period coefcients measure the linear
change during the indicated interval of timedYear Spline (1977e1994) measures attitude change from 1977 to 1994. Reli-
gious groups are interacted with period terms to compare rates of change within intervals of time.
The same-sex relationship attitudes do not follow the same rise, decline, and subsequent rise pattern of the gender at-
titudes. For these attitudes, Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) scores prefer the simpler model with a linear interaction
term for year and religious groups rather than interaction terms for spline intervals and religious groups (Raftery, 1995).
Whereas the pattern of gender attitude change requireda spline function, a straightforward linear term for year is appropriate
for sexuality attitude change, already suggesting that the general population follows a different attitude change pattern on
these attitudes.
9
Religious groups are interacted with year to compare change from 1973 to 2014 for morality of homosex-
uality and from 1988 to 2014 for same-sex marriage.
2.4. Demographic controls
This study takes into account key demographic controls that may affect gender and sexuality attitudes. These items were
selected based on theory and what is standard in the literature (Bartkowski and Hempel, 2009; Cotter et al., 2011). These
measures were then tested using BIC scores to determine whether they should be included (Raftery, 1995). Controls include
sex (female ¼1), race (white ¼1, Black ¼2, other ¼3),
10
age (in years), parental status (parent ¼1), marital status
(married ¼1), female work status (coding explained below), region (South ¼1), education (dummies for highest degree
7
There are three other questions in the GSSdas part of a sequence of questions on speech rights for different groups, such as atheists, Muslims, and
communistsdthat ask about whether a gay man should be allowed to speak in a community, whether he should be able to hold a position at a college, and
whether a gay-friendly book should be allowed to stay at the public library. These questions, while interesting and important, are less about sexuality (and
more about free speech) than the two questions this paper examinesdwhich are particularly relevant to current political debate. Furthermore, the rights
questions have limited variability, especially among more liberal religious groups, such as Jews and the unafliated, who already revealed very high support
for these minimal rights in the 1970s.
8
I used the default mkspline command in Stata 13 for the spline function with hinges at 1994 and 2000. The rst interval in the spline function,
1977e1994, is coded as 0 for 1977 and increases by one for each year until 1994, which is 17.17 is then the value for 1994 and all following years. The second
interval, 1994e2000, is 0 until 1994 and then increases by one for each year until 2000, which is 6, and 6 is then the value for 20 00 and all following years.
The third interval, 2000e2014, is coded as 0 for all years until 2000 and then increases by one for each year until the nal year in the sample 2014, where it
has a value of 14. Cotter et al. (2011) coded their spline function variables so that the year term does not stop increasing by one for every year once the next
knot is reached (in Stata, this is done by using the margin option with the mkspline command). Rather than providing the linear term for year within a
period of time, their approach tested the rate of change against the immediately previous interval, limiting their ability to compare the rate of change af ter
2000 to the rate of change before 1994.
9
I conducted a sensitivity analysis with a spline function for the morality measure with a knot at 1994 to parallel the gender attitudes analyses and
another analysis with a knot at 1990 when attitudes toward the morality of homosexuality began to steadily rise. The patterns for group change were
substantively similar to the linear model.
10
Although the Black Protestant religious afliation category and Black race category are correlated at .7, I control for race to ensure that I measure the
impact of Black Protestantism rather than just racial attitude differences. Scholars have been concerned about collinearity when including both Black
Protestantism and race in the same model, but a follow-up to the Steensland et al. (200 0) religious categorization scheme argues that collinearity should
not be a problem when using large surveys such as the GSS (Woodberry et al., 2012). Sensitivity analyses not controlling for race reveal substantively
equivalent ndings for rates of attitude change over time across religious categories.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e4736
attained), and family income (ination-adjusted to 2000 and expressed in tens-of-thousands of dollars). Female work status
is coded 1 if a female respondent works full time and if a married male respondent indicates that his spouse works full time.
11
2.5. Analytic strategy
Following Cotter et al. (2011), this study uses ordinary least squares (OLS) regression for the gender attitudes scale.
12
Ordinal logistic regression is used for the morality of homosexuality and support for same-sex marriage measures. All out-
comes are estimated using Huber/White sandwich robust standard errors. All analyses are unweighted.
13
Sensitivity analyses
including weights yielded substantively equivalent results.
Irst compare religious group main effects on gender attitudes. Second, I present gures that show gender attitude change
across groups based on separate models for eachyear with centered covariates. I then include all years in the same model (the
primary method as described above) and interact religious groups with best-tting spline function interval terms to compare
rates of change during three periods of time. Subsequently, I present the parallel analyses of attitudes toward the morality of
homosexuality and support for same-sex marriage. Finally, I present a linear analysis of gender attitudes for the sake of easier
comparison with the sexuality attitude analysis, and to examine whether there has been a consistent gender attitude
liberalization trend since the start of the stallnoted by Cotter et al. (2011) (Table 1).
3. Results
3.1. Gender attitudes
3.1.1. Gender attitudes across religious groups
Table 2 presents religious group afliation effects with just controls for survey year
14
(Model 1) and then afliation effects
with demographic controls added (Model 2). Evangelical is the reference category. In Model 1, all religious categories are more
egalitarian than evangelicals (p<.001). For example, the unafliated (b¼.352, p<.001) are half a standard deviation of the
gender attitude scale more egalitarian than evangelicals. Evangelical attitudes are clearly distinctive, being more conservative
than those of all other groups.
In the spline function, the 1977 to 1994 interval measures attitude change during this time period. Similarly,1994 to 2000
and 2000 to 2014 measure change during these time periods. Gender attitudes have become more egalitarian since the 1970s
except for a period from 1994 to 2000 when they became less egalitarian (b¼.025, p<.001).
Model 2, which includes demographic controls, shows that, with one exception, the religious afliation differences are not
attributable to sociodemographic differences. All but the other faithcategory are still more egalitarian (p <.001) than
evangelicals. Consistent with the literature, women aremore egalitarian than men (b¼.200, p<.001). The gender attitudes of
Black Americans do not differ signicantly from whites.
15
Those in the other racecategory are less egalitarian than whites
(b¼.186, p<.001). People who are older, married, or from the South are less egalitarian (p<.001), whereas parents,
working women or men married to working women, people with more education, and those with a higher income are more
egalitarian (p<.001).
16
The 2000e2014 year interval shows that attitudes are currentlyliberalizing and that this change is not
attributable to sociodemographic shifts (b¼.020, p<.001).
Afliation effects decrease with the inclusion of demographic controls, but they persist and can be compared to de-
mographic characteristics. Being an evangelical, when compared to those with no religious afliation (b¼.249, p<.001) or
Jews (b¼.313, p<.001), has a larger effect than gender (b¼.200, p<.001). In fact, being evangelical, in comparison to the
most liberal groups, has a larger effect than all demographics except having a bachelor's degree (b¼.272, p<.001)dabout the
same magnitude as being evangelical compared to the unafliated and Jewsdor having a graduate degree (b¼.413, p<.001).
In sum, evangelicals are the least egalitarian religious group and religion is a stronger predictor of gender attitudes than all
measures but education, suggesting that gender attitudes are an important eld for drawing symbolic boundaries and
expressing evangelical collective identity. This still leaves open the question of whether the rst or third possible expectation
ts the data: are evangelical attitudes changing more slowly than other groups (expectation 1), or are they adapting to broad
social change at the same rate as other groups (expectation 3)?
11
I use the same female work status coding method used by Bartkowski and Hempel (2009). Though this control is not as standard to the literature as the
others, its inclusion is supported by BIC tests. Sensitivity analyses without this measure provided substantively equivalent results.
12
I conducted sensitivity analyses using ordinal logistic regression with the same models and the results were substantively equivalent.
13
Winship and Radbill (1994) showed that unweighted regression estimates tend to be unbiased and consistent when using large surveys with weights
that are solely a function of independentdrather than dependentdvariables.
14
Sensitivity analyses using BIC tests show that dummy variables for year are very strongly preferred for t over just a linear term for year. However,
including the best-t spline with knots at 1994 and 2000, the method presented, is very strongly preferred by BIC tests over year dummy variables.
15
A model (not shown) including the demographics, but not the independent variable (religious afliation), shows that Blacks are signicantly more
egalitarian than whites. This effect is no longer signicant with the inclusion of religious categories.
16
In sensitivity analyses, I included measures of religious belief (biblical literalism, which is not available for all years) and practice (attendance). More
literal views of the Bible and more frequent religious service attendance are both associated with less egalitarian gender attitudes. Including these measures
in the models decreases overall group difference, but leads to substantively equivalent ndings for rates of change over time.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e47 37
3.1.2. Gender attitudes across religious groups over time
Fig. 1 shows gender attitudes from 1977 to 2014 for all Americans and then for Protestant religious groups, controlling for
demographics with centered covariates. These plotted values are based on separate OLS regression models (not shown) for
each year with centered controls (the same sociodemographic measures included in Tables 2e4).
17
We see in Fig. 1a that the
overall gender attitude trend was rapid change in the 1970s and 1980s with decline in the 1990s. From 2000 onward, attitudes
have become increasingly egalitarian. As noted by Cotter et al. (2011), the attitudes of all Americans in 2008 had not risen
much above the level in the early 1990s. Challenging the idea that American gender attitudes have stagnated, however, in
2010, in 2012, and again in 2014 Americans' attitudes became more egalitarian than ever before.
Fig. 1b shows that evangelical attitudes have followed the same general trend of all Americans, liberalizing until the mid-
1990s, declining through 2000, and then rising again since 2000. Evangelicals started out closer to the average in the late
1970s, diverged in the late 1980s, and have generally paralleled average American attitude change since then. Mainline
Protestants have also followed the same general pattern, as revealed in Fig. 1c. Mainline Protestants, however, have remained
much closer to the average, diverging from evangelicals in the late 1980s. Black Protestants also followed a similar pattern,
though with more year-to-year volatility because they make up a smaller portion of the sample.
18
All Protestant groups
followed the same trend, paralleling the average for all Americans. They liberalized quickly from the late 1970s to the mid-
1990s, when they became more traditional. Since 2000, their attitudes have become more egalitarian.
Fig. 2 shows the gender attitudes by year for Catholics, the unafliated, Jews, and those in the other faithcategory.
Catholic attitudes, like those of mainline Protestants, are similar to the average and move in conjunction with the general
population. The unafliated are generally more egalitarian than the American average, but again follow the same trend. The
Table 2
Regression of group membership effects on gender attitudes scale.
Model 1 Model 2
Religious Categories
Evangelical ee
Black Protestant 0.219
***
(0.019) 0.149
***
(0.023)
Mainline 0.182
***
(0.014) 0.161
***
(0.013)
Catholic 0.170
***
(0.014) 0.126
***
(0.013)
No Afliation 0.352
***
(0.016) 0.249
***
(0.015)
Jewish 0.437
***
(0.035) 0.313
***
(0.032)
Other Faith 0.080
***
(0.024) 0.012 (0.023)
Survey Year
Year Spline (1977e1994) 0.034
***
(0.001) 0.030
***
(0.001)
Year Spline (1994e2000) 0.025
***
(0.003) 0.026
***
(0.003)
Year Spline (2000e2014) 0.017
***
(0.001) 0.020
***
(0.001)
Controls
Female 0.200
***
(0.009)
White e
Black 0.030 (0.018)
Other 0.186
***
(0.019)
Age in Decades 0.098
***
(0.003)
Parental Status 0.044
***
(0.011)
Marital Status 0.124
***
(0.010)
Female Work Status 0.203
***
(0.010)
South 0.078
***
(0.010)
Less Than High School e
High School 0.158
***
(0.013)
Some College 0.228
***
(0.021)
Bachelor 0.272
***
(0.017)
Graduate 0.413
***
(0.020)
Family Income 0.009
***
(0.001)
Constant 0.609 0.407
N21,435 21,435
R
2
0.075 0.224
BIC 45508.477 41880.434
Note: Standard errors in parentheses; Models estimated with robust standard errors.
*
p<.05,
**
p<.01,
***
p<.001 (two-tailed).
Source: General Social Survey 1977e2014
17
When using OLS regression to compare groups with centered covariates, the intercept is the value of the excluded group and the coefcients for other
groups can be added to the intercept for their predicted values. I used separate models for each year with religious groups to get the values for each group
and then a model without religious groups and binary variables for each year to ascertain the yearly values for all Americans.
18
Sensitivity analyses of the gures without controls reveal substantively equivalent patterns of change over time across groups. Without controls,
including the one for race, Black Protestants appear slightly more egalitarian than the average, but follow the same pattern over time.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e4738
Jewish and other faithcategories are very small, and thus unstable and not particularly meaningful.
19
That said, they do not
seem follow the same gender attitude change trend as other Americans. Jews start out quite egalitarian and are typically more
egalitarian than other groups, without a clear pattern of change over time. The other faithcategory contains disparate
groups and thus their trends cannot be meaningfully interpreted. In sum, Figs. 1 and 2 show that most religious groups
followed a similar upward, downward, and then upward trend.
By including all years in the same models presented in Table 2 and interacting religious groups with time periods, we can
more explicitly test rates of change during the movetoward egalitarianism from 1977 to 1994, during the decline from 1994 to
2000, and during the resurgence of gender attitude change from 2000 to 2014. Table 3 presents coefcients for the effect of
year and period on gender attitudes with evangelical as the reference category. These values represent the overall rate of
group change during the move toward egalitarianism from1977 to1994, the decline from 1994 to 2000, and the resurgence of
attitude change from 2000 onward.
Model 1 (without controls) and Model 2 (with controls) of Table 3 demonstrate similar rates of attitude change over time
after 1994. Interaction effects for religious groups and time period in Model 2 show that from 1977 to 1994 mainline Prot-
estant (b¼.009, p<.05) and Catholic (b¼.010, p<.01) attitudes liberalized more rapidly than those of evangelicals. The
interaction of religious group and the 1994 to 2000 time period demonstrates that evangelical decline in egalitarianism in this
period did not differ from that of other Christian groups or the unafliated. The other faith(b¼.042, p<.01) category
operated differently, but that is because, as shown in Fig. 2, they did not follow the same pattern as other groups. The
interaction effects for the interval from 2000 to 2014 show that evangelicals have liberalized just as quickly as other Christian
groups and the unafliated since 2000. In fact, before sociodemographic controls are taken into account, evangelical attitudes
-0.6
-0.4
-0.2
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015
Egalitarian Attitudes
a: All Americans
1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015
b: Evangelicals vs. All
1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015
d: Black Protesant vs. All
-0.6
-0.4
-0.2
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015
Egalitarian Attitudes
c: Mainline vs. All
Fig. 1. Gender Attitude Scale over Time: All Americans and Protestants.
Source: General Social Survey, 1977e2012; Source: General Social Survey, 1977e2014
19
Despite their year-to-year volatility, I present these gures for Jewish and other faithattitudes to provide context for the interaction effects between
year and these groups in Table 3.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e47 39
actually changed more quickly than Catholic attitudes (b¼.009, p<.05). When controls are included in the model, just the
disparate other faithcategory is different from evangelicals (b¼.021, p<.05).
Although one might expect evangelical attitude change to be slower than that of other groups based on the rhetoric of
evangelical public gures, Table 3 showed that evangelical attitudes changed just as quicklydupward from 1977 to 1994,
downward from 1994 to 2000, and upward again from 2000 onwarddas those of other Christians and the unafliated. The
only noteworthy difference was that mainline Protestants and Catholics liberalized more quickly from 1977 to 1994. Since
then, evangelical attitudes have paralleled those of other Christian groups and the unafliated. In sum, gender attitude change
has not stagnated and evangelical attitudes are changing at a rate similar to that of other groups. Gender attitudes follow the
second possible pattern set forth in the expectations: similar rates of change but continued difference; or, in other words,
adaptation over time with distinction at any given point in time.
3.2. Attitudes toward same-sex relationships
The gender attitudes of all Christian groups and the unafliated moved in parallel to one another. But did attitudes toward
gay men and lesbians follow a similar pattern? Figs. 3 and 4 present the yearly means for all Americans, the unafliated, and
evangelicals on attitudes toward the morality of homosexuality and same-sex marriage. On belief in morality, we see that
attitudes are relatively stagnant from the early 1970s to the early 1990s. In the mid-1990s, a continuing upward trend toward
accepting same-sex relationships as moral starts among all Americans, the same period in which gender attitudes declined.
Table 3
Interaction effects of membership and year on gender attitudes scale.
a
Model 1 Model 2
Main Effects
Evangelical ee
Black Protestant 0.154
*
(0.072) 0.121 (0.065)
Mainline 0.105
*
(0.048) 0.041 (0.043)
Catholic 0.045 (0.050) 0.005 (0.045)
No Afliation 0.485
***
(0.071) 0.351
***
(0.064)
Jewish 0.546
***
(0.117) 0.443
***
(0.099)
Other Faith 0.012 (0.106) 0.055 (0.101)
Year Spline (1977e1994) 0.030
***
(0.003) 0.026
***
(0.003)
Year Spline (1994e2000) 0.030
***
(0.006) 0.030
***
(0.005)
Year Spline (2000e2014) 0.023
***
(0.003) 0.024
***
(0.003)
Year Spline (1977 to 1994) Interaction Effects
Evangelical*Year Spline (1977e1994) ee
Black Protestant*Year Spline (1977e1994) 0.003 (0.006) 0.002 (0.005)
Mainline*Year Spline (1977e1994) 0.005 (0.004) 0.009
*
(0.003)
Catholic*Year Spline (1977e1994) 0.011
**
(0.004) 0.010
**
(0.004)
No Afliation*Year Spline (1977e1994) 0.008 (0.005) 0.006 (0.005)
Jewish*Year Spline (1977e1994) 0.011 (0.009) 0.013 (0.008)
Other Faith*Year Spline (1977e1994) 0.001 (0.008) 0.001 (0.008)
Year Spline (1994 to 2000) Interaction Effects
Evangelical*Year Spline (1994e2000) ee
Black Protestant*Year Spline (1994e2000) 0.009 (0.011) 0.001 (0.010)
Mainline*Year Spline (1994e2000) 0.004 (0.009) 0.001 (0.008)
Catholic*Year Spline (1994e2000) 0.002 (0.008) 0.003 (0.007)
No Afliation*Year Spline (1994e2000) 0.007 (0.010) 0.005 (0.009)
Jewish*Year Spline (1994e2000) 0.036 (0.020) 0.034 (0.018)
Other Faith*Year Spline (1994e2000) 0.044
**
(0.014) 0.042
**
(0.013)
Year Spline (2000 to 2014) Interaction Effects
Evangelical*Year Spline (2000e2014) ee
Black Protestant*Year Spline (2000e2014) 0.003 (0.006) 0.001 (0.005)
Mainline*Year Spline (2000e2014) 0.002 (0.005) 0.001 (0.004)
Catholic*Year Spline (2000e2014) 0.009
*
(0.004) 0.006 (0.004)
No Afliation*Year Spline (2000e2014) 0.008 (0.004) 0.007 (0.004)
Jewish*Year Spline (2000e2014) 0.023
*
(0.011) 0.015 (0.010)
Other Faith*Year Spline (2000e2014) 0.026
***
(0.007) 0.021
**
(0.007)
Controls
a
No Yes
Constant 0.558 0.344
N21,435 21,435
R
2
0.078 0.226
BIC 45635.982 42004.611
Note: Standard errors in parentheses; Models estimated with robust standard errors.
*
p<.05,
**
p<.01,
***
p<.001.
a
Controls are female, race, age, parental status, marital status, female work status, region, education, and family income. Control values are
almost identical to those in Table 2.
Source: General Social Survey 1977e2014
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e4740
Evangelicals and the unafliated follow a similar trend: stable attitudes through the mid-1990s and then an upward trend.
The evangelical shift, however, appears to be slower than that of the religiously unafliated and the general population.
Evangelicals start out closer to the American average when most people were opposed to same-sex relationships, but as
society liberalized evangelical attitudes diverge. Mainline Protestants and Catholics (not shown) closely follow the American
average.
20
Although fewer years are available, we see a similar pattern on support for same-sex marriage: slower change early on and
more rapid change in recent years. Evangelical attitudes again start out closer to the average and then appear to be changing
more slowly than those of others. This trend already suggests both that gender and sexuality followed clearly different
patterns overall, and that attitudes toward same-sex relationships may be becoming more important for expressing a
distinctive evangelical collective identity in opposition to secular society. The uptick for evangelical support for same-sex
marriage in 2014, however, may indicate that what has happened in the past could shift in the future.
Table 4 shows overall religious group differences and then differences in rates of change over time on attitudes toward
homosexuality and same-sex marriage. Evangelical is the reference category. With only a control for year in Model 1,
evangelicals are more conservative (p <.001) than all but Black Protestants on the morality of homosexuality. When all
controls are included in Model 2, evangelicals are more conservative than all other religious groups (p <.001), including Black
Protestants. On support for same-sex marriage, evangelicals are more conservative than all religious groups (p <.001), both
without (Model 4) and with controls (Model 5). Similar patterns show up across both outcome measures, with the unafliated
and Jewish categories revealing the largest difference from evangelicals. Year is signicant (p <.001) for all models, revealing
1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015
b: No Affiliation vs. All
-0.6
-0.4
-0.2
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015
Egalitarian Attitudes
a: Catholic vs. All
1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015
d: Jewish vs. All
-0.6
-0.4
-0.2
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2015
Egalitarian Attitudes
c: Other Faith vs. All
Fig. 2. Gender Attitude Scale over Time: Catholic, Unafliated, Other Faith, and Jewish.
Source: General Social Survey, 1977e2012; Source: General Social Survey, 1977e2014
20
Groups not shown for clarity and parsimony generally parallel the attitudes of one of these three groups on both the morality of homosexuality and
support for same-sex marriage. Jewish attitudes are similar to those of the unafliated, mainline Protestant and Catholic attitudes are similar to the average,
and Black Protestant attitudes are similar to those of evangelicals.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e47 41
a liberalizing trend over time. Evangelical attitudes toward same-sex relationshipsdlike gender attitudesdare distinctive
from those of other groups, but are they as adaptive as gender attitudes to the overall liberalizing trend?
Interaction terms for provide an effective way to test group rates of attitude change. Models 3 and 6 on Table 4 show that
both evangelicals and Black Protestants demonstrate slower change than other groups on the morality of homosexuality and
support for same-sex marriage. On the morality of homosexuality, mainline Protestants (b¼.012, p <.001), Catholics
(b¼.019, p <.001), the unafliated (b¼.013, p <.001), and those in the other faithcategory (b¼.022, p <.001) liberalized
more quickly than evangelicals. Catholics (b¼.021, p <.01) and the unafliated (b¼.040, p <.001) have moved to accept
1
1.5
2
2.5
3
3.5
4
1972
1974
1976
1978
1980
1982
1984
1986
1988
1990
1992
1994
1996
1998
2000
2002
2004
2006
2008
2010
2012
2014
Always Wrong=1 to Not Wrong at All=4
All Americans No Affiliation Evangelical
Fig. 3. Belief in morality of homosexuality over time.
Source: General Social Survey, 1973e2014.
1
1.5
2
2.5
3
3.5
4
4.5
5
1972
1974
1976
1978
1980
1982
1984
1986
1988
1990
1992
1994
1996
1998
2000
2002
2004
2006
2008
2010
2012
2014
Strongly Oppose=1 to Strongly Support=5
All Americans No Affiliation Evangelical
Fig. 4. Support for same-sex marriage over time.
Source: General Social Survey, 1988e2014.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e4742
same-sex marriage more quickly than have evangelicals. In sensitivity analyses examining just 2004 to 2014, the coefcients
for the religious group and year are similar on views of the morality of homosexuality, but smaller on same-sex marriage. In
other words, evangelicals' views are continuing to lag on the morality of homosexuality at a similar rate as in the past, but in
recent years their attitudes toward same-sex marriage are trending toward changing at the same rate as other groups.
Overall, these sexuality attitudes followed the rst possible pattern discussed in the expectations: distinctive differences
and slower attitude change among evangelicals. Gender attitudes and sexuality attitudes follow different patterns of change
across American religious groups, suggesting that change in these attitudes is decoupled for some groups. Evangelical atti-
tudes toward same-sex relationships are becoming increasingly distinctive, whereas their genderattitudes are more adaptive,
paralleling the national trend. Therefore, attitudes toward same-sex relationships seem to be becoming more important for
drawing symbolic boundaries and the expression of collective evangelical identity. The 2014 evangelical uptick in support for
same-sex marriage, however, may indicate that this trend could shift in the future.
3.3. Further analysis conrming different change patterns
Fig. 1e4showed that the gender and sexuality attitudes of the general population have followed different change patterns.
Illustratively, gender attitudes changed more quickly than sexuality attitudes until 1994. From 1994 to 2000, sexuality atti-
tudes liberalized while gender attitudes became more conservative. Since 2000, both types of attitudes have been changing
quickly.
Further regression analyses conrm that gender and sexuality attitudes have followed different patterns across religious
groups. Table 5 shows the effect of religious afliation on gender attitudes over time with a linear term for yeardinstead of
the spline function shown in Table 3dto make it more easily comparable to the same-sex relationship attitudes analyses.
Observations from 1994 to 2014, the period of time that evangelical attitudes changed at the same rate as other groups in the
spline function, are included. The table shows evangelical attitudes changed just as quickly as those of other groups from 1994
to 2014, conrming that gender and sexuality attitudes follow a different pattern of change across religious groups.
21
The
Table 4
Ordinal regression of religious groups and over-time interactions on morality of homosexuality and support for same-sex marriage.
Morality Marriage
123456
Main Effects
Evangelical eeeeee
Black Protestant 0.036 (0.060) 0.518
***
(0.082) 0.547
***
(0.156) 0.374
***
(0.076) 0.680
***
(0.101) 0.732
***
(0.192)
Mainline 0.786
***
(0.038) 0.675
***
(0.041) 0.405
***
(0.081) 0.741
***
(0.062) 0.718
***
(0.065) 0.450
**
(0.152)
Catholic 1.004
***
(0.037) 0.811
***
(0.040) 0.399
***
(0.082) 0.997
***
(0.055) 0.890
***
(0.057) 0.547
***
(0.144)
No Afliation 2.061
***
(0.045) 1.756
***
(0.047) 1.456
***
(0.098) 1.759
***
(0.064) 1.519
***
(0.066) 0.734
***
(0.204)
Jewish 2.535
***
(0.088) 2.136
***
(0.094) 1.825
***
(0.164) 1.958
***
(0.155) 1.711
***
(0.160) 1.296
***
(0.368)
Other Faith 1.074
***
(0.060) 0.803
***
(0.062) 0.322
*
(0.135) 1.027
***
(0.100) 0.869
***
(0.104) 0.823
***
(0.220)
Year 0.039
***
(0.001) 0.040
***
(0.001) 0.028
***
(0.003) 0.061
***
(0.002) 0.068
***
(0.003) 0.055
***
(0.006)
Interaction Effects
Evangelical*Year ee
Black Protestant*Year 0.002 (0.006) 0.004 (0.010)
Mainline*Year 0.012
***
(0.004) 0.014 (0.008)
Catholic*Year 0.019
***
(0.003) 0.019
*
(0.007)
No Afliation*Year 0.013
***
(0.004) 0.040
***
(0.010)
Jewish*Year 0.015 (0.008) 0.023 (0.020)
Other Faith*Year 0.022
***
(0.006) 0.002 (0.011)
Controls
a
No Yes Yes No Yes Yes
Constant (Cut 1) 2.146 1.432 1.173 0.918 0.083 0.160
Constant (Cut 2) 2.398 1.715 1.456 1.740 0.968 0.725
Constant (Cut 3) 2.812 2.173 1.915 2.352 1.632 1.391
Constant (Cut 4) 3.532 2.907 2.672
N29,983 29,983 29,983 8,785 8,785 8,785
BIC 53250.456 50061.611 50076.383 26089.061 25236.232 25265.590
Note: Standard errors in parentheses; Models estimated with robust standard errors.
*
p<.05,
**
p<.01,
***
p<.001.
a
Analyses use the same controls as Table 2: female, race, age, parental status, marital status, female work status, region, education, and family income.
Source: General Social Survey (Morality 1973e2014; Marriage 1988, 2004e2014)
21
When including all years, the faster liberalization of mainline Protestants and Catholics from 1977 to 1994 shows up as a signicant effect. In a
sensitivity analysis, I examined the morality of homosexuality measure including only those cases from 1994 to 2014 to ensure that the linear term for
gender attitudes presented in 5 is comparable to what is presented in Table 4. In this analysis, evangelical attitudes on the morality of homosexuality still
changed more slowly than those of other groups.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e47 43
simple year term also allows us to see that American gender attitudes have not stagnated. In spite of the decline in attitudes
from 1994 to 2000 shown in the spline function, American attitudes have liberalized signicantly since 1994.
22
4. Discussion and conclusion
This study used General Social Survey data to compare attitudes toward gender roles and same-sex relationships across
American religious groups from the 1970s to 2014, considering three possible patterns for how evangelical attitudes relate to
those of other groups: (1) they are similar; (2) they are different, but move together over time; (3) they are different and
converge or diverge over time. The results show that gender attitudes t the second pattern, and same-sex relationship
attitudes the third. Evangelical gender attitudes regarding work and family issues are more conservative than those of all
other groups, but are adaptive to broad trends, changing at a rate similar to those of other groups since 1994. Evangelical
attitudes toward the morality of homosexuality and same-sex marriage are also more conservative than those of all other
religious groups. Their rate of change, however, is slower than other religious groups, meaning that they are diverging over
time. Separate patterns on the two issues suggest that gender and sexuality attitude change is decoupled, especially among
evangelicals who are adapting more on gender while increasingly distinguishing themselves on same-sex relationships.
Gender attitudes differ across religious groups at any point in time, but all Christian groups and the unafliated have
followed the same change pattern: liberalization in the late-1970s to mid-1990s, a shift toward traditionalism from the mid-
1990s to early 2000s, and then an upward trend until 2014. Mainline Protestant and Catholic gender attitudes did liberalize
more quickly than evangelicals from 1977 to 1994, which suggests that gender roles used to be a more important site for
distinction. But evangelicals did not differ from other Christian groups or the unafliated in their rate of attitude decline from
1994 to 2000 or in their rate of attitude resurgence from 2000 to 2014. Religious afliation does not seem to affect gender
attitude change in more recent yearsdthough evangelicals are still less gender egalitarian than other groups. Therefore, on
gender attitudes, evangelicals followed the second possible pattern with continued distinction, but rates of change similar to
those of other groups.
The examination of attitudes toward same-sex relationships revealed different change patterns. Attitudes on the morality
of sexuality began liberalizing quickly at the same time that gender attitudes moved back toward traditionalism (based on the
trends across available years, support for same-sex marriage likely followed a similar pattern). Whereas evangelical gender
attitudes moved in parallel to those of other Christian groups and the unafliated, their attitudes on the morality of
Table 5
Interaction effects of membership and linear year term on gender attitudes scale (N ¼13,509)
a
.
Model 1 Model 2
Main Effects
Evangelical ee
Black Protestant 0.225
***
(0.042) 0.157
***
(0.042)
Mainline 0.184
***
(0.032) 0.171
***
(0.029)
Catholic 0.228
***
(0.030) 0.179
***
(0.028)
No Afliation 0.367
***
(0.035) 0.287
***
(0.033)
Jewish 0.486
***
(0.070) 0.336
***
(0.064)
Other Faith 0.176
***
(0.051) 0.097
*
(0.048)
Year 0.009
***
(0.002) 0.010
***
(0.002)
Year Interaction Effects
Evangelical*Year ee
Black Protestant*Year 0.001 (0.004) 0.000 (0.003)
Mainline*Year 0.001 (0.003) 0.001 (0.003)
Catholic*Year 0.006
*
(0.002) 0.003 (0.002)
No Afliation*Year 0.003 (0.003) 0.003 (0.003)
Jewish*Year 0.007 (0.007) 0.004 (0.006)
Other Faith*Year 0.008 (0.004) 0.004 (0.004)
Controls
a
No Yes
Constant 0.301 0.167
N13,509 13,509
R
2
0.035 0.167
BIC 28069.815 26210.206
Note: Standard errors in parentheses; Models estimated with robust standard errors.
*
p<.05,
**
p<.01,
***
p<.001.
a
Controls are female, race, age, parental status, marital status, female work status, region, education, and family
income. Control values are similar to those in Table 2.
Source: General Social Survey 1994e2014
22
A sensitivity analysis including both attitudes in the same model further conrms the differing change patterns. Though only about half the people
were asked both questions due to the years the questions were asked and subsampling withinyears, an analysis of the sexuality attitudes examined in Table
4with controls for gender attitudesdgender attitudes, year binaries, and interaction terms for theseddoes not alter the change patterns across religious
groupsdevangelical sexuality attitudes still change more slowly than those of the other groups.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e4744
homosexuality and support for same-sex marriagedalong with those of Black Protestantsdchanged more slowly. Therefore,
on sexuality attitudes, evangelicals followed the third possible pattern of more conservative attitudes and a distinctly slower
rate of change.
Gender and sexuality attitude change t different patterns. Attitudes toward same-sex relationships began to liberalize in
the mid-1990s, the same time that gender attitudes declined, indicating a decoupling of changedthis is not to say that they
are not still related, just that they are changing differently.
23
Beyond the decoupling of change, groups differ in their location
on the conservative-liberal spectrum on gender and sexuality attitudes. For example, Black Protestants were just as gender
egalitarian as mainline Protestants and Catholics, but their attitudes toward same-sex relationships were more conservative.
The distinctive patterns in rates of attitude change suggest an increasing importance of sexuality attitudes for the drawing of
symbolic boundaries. Evangelicals now seem to be dening and distinguishing themselves more on their views toward same-
sex relationships than gender roles.
This study shows that religious afliation cannot be used to explain the gender attitude decline in the mid-1990s noted by
Cotter et al. (2011) and others. Nevertheless, it is possible that complementarian religious ideas diffused across other groups.
Fig. 1 showed that evangelical and mainline Protestants diverged from one another in the late 1980s, which hints at the
importance of the rise of the Christian Right and especially the prominence of the Moral Majority at that time (Schnabel,
2013). In the mid-1990s through 2000, both the non-denominational Promise Keeper's Christian men's movement and the
Christian Coalition were at their peak. 2000 was a particularly important turning point in the decline of both movements for a
variety of sociopolitical reasons (Schnabel, 2013,Bartkowski, 2004). Although these groups were most popular among
evangelicals, they appealed to social conservatives across religious divides. The popularity of these groups at the same time
that gender attitudes became more traditional suggests a connection; this popularity could also be indicative of a general
essentialist backlash in conjunction with the rise of intensive mothering, which combines a rhetoric of choice and equality
with an idealization of stay-at-home mothering (Cotter et al., 2011; Faludi, 1991; Stone, 2007). Indicative of the rising
popularity of these ideas, one of the foundational books for attachment parenting and intensive mothering came out just
before the attitude decline (Sears and Sears, 1993).
The results also demonstrate that gender attitudes have not stalledas proposed by Cotter et al. (2011), whose data went
through 2008.
24
Rather than stagnating, American gender attitudes have liberalized consistently both before and after a short
period of decline. Since 2008, we have seen the highest levels of egalitarianism yet. Current liberalization, similar to past
change, is happening at similar rates across religious groups. Rather than trying to understand why gender attitude change
has slowed or stalled, the question future research may want to address is what caused gender attitudes to become more
traditional while attitudes toward same-sex relationships continued to liberalize.
Why are evangelical attitudes toward same-sex relationships changing more slowly, and what might happen in the
future? There are a few possibilities, but one likelyexplanation is that theology, history, and politics have linked conservative
Christian identity and opposition to same-sex marriage. Evangelical traditional valuesare currently in more tension with
the larger society on same-sex relationships than on work and family roles for women and men. There is a clear and widely
shared evangelical view on same-sex relationships rooted in evangelical politics and literal interpretations of the Bible, but it
would now be harder to determine and dene any one shared evangelical position on gender, work, and family. When women
had started joining the workforce but most Americans were still opposed to same-sex relationships, evangelicals distin-
guished themselves more on conservative gender roles and less on views toward same-sex relationships. Today, however,
evangelical leaders can support working women and women politicians without facing much boundary policing from
evangelicals in the pews (consider evangelical support for Sarah Palin), but would face backlash from some segments of
evangelicalism for coming out in favor of same-sex marriage. Research suggests that evangelical afliation and evangelical
politics, of which same-sex marriage is a key issue, are so closely tied that people will shift their religious identity because of
their political identity (Hout and Fischer, 2002; 2014). Slower change on views of same-sex relationships, therefore, could
come both from evangelicals changing their views more slowly and the disafliation of some evangelicals with more liberal
views on same-sex relationships.
Things may be changing, however. Following the nationwide legalization of same-sex marriage, the tension between
traditional evangelical views on same-sex relationships and cultural norms of equality, especiallyamong younger people, may
become too strong for evangelical identity to continue to be dened by opposition to same-sex relationships. The uptick in
support for same-sex marriage among evangelicals in 2014 and recent news about key evangelical leaders, such as Tony
Campolo and David Neff, coming out in favor of same-sex marriage suggest that such a transition may happen sooner rather
than later. Just as gender roles became a less important factor for expressing and policing evangelical identity, opposition to
same-sex marriage could also become a less important factor and eventually be replaced in evangelical political rhetoric by
other issues. For now, however, sexuality remains a key dening issue for American religions as demonstrated by public
responses to same-sex marriage legalization and related debates about religious liberty.
23
Polychoric correlations show that people with conservative gender attitudes continue to be more likely to have conservative attitudes toward same-sex
relationships than people with more liberal gender attitudes.
24
This is not to say that Cotter et al. (2011) were wrong. They made it clear that the question mark at the end of their titledThe End of the Gender
Revolution?dwas important even though they did talk about gender attitudes as stalled. Furthermore, they were careful to only speculate tentatively
about attitude change following 2008 (the last year in their sample).
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e47 45
The results of this study suggest a three-stage process of religious tension and evangelical identity building that should be
further tested in future research: (1) similarity, (2) distinction, and (3) adaptation. When all of society agrees that something is
wrong, evangelicals are less likely to use that issue to distinguish themselvesdfor example, evangelical attitudes toward
gender egalitarianism in 1977 and their attitudes toward same-sex relationships through the early 1990s were similar to the
rest of American society because most Americans agreed that these were unacceptable. When an issue becomes contested,
however, evangelicals tend to hold the traditionalposition and use it to distinguish themselves. When the vast majority of
society subsequently shifts and the issue is no longer widely contesteddas happened with slavery, racial segregation,
interracial marriage, women joining the workforce, and now same-sex marriage (Schnabel, 2015)dthe tension may become
too great and American evangelicals then shift along with the broader society and distinguish themselves on newly contested
issues.
In light of the decoupling of rates of change in gender and sexuality attitudes among evangelicals and Black Protestants,
future work could consider the relationship between afliation and attitudes on other issues of sexuality (i.e., sexual edu-
cation, pornography, pre- and extra-marital sex, cohabitation, and abortion) and personhood (i.e., birth control, abortion,
physician-assisted suicide, and stem cell researchd(Schnabel, 2014; Schnabel and Breitwieser, 2015)) that are commonly
debated on religious grounds in the public sphere. Finally, future research could compare the effect of religious afliation on
other changing attitudes to examine whether afliation has a stronger or weaker effect on issues less commonly debated in
religious termsdsuch as marijuana and vaccination.
Religious afliation remains important for understanding Americans' gender and sexuality attitudes, but does not seem
particularly important for understanding rates of gender attitude change. Religion, however, continues to be one of the most
important factors keeping some Americans from supporting the recent nationwide legalization of same-sex marriage.
Through social change, evangelicalism continues its pursuit of distinctiondto be in but not of the world (Smith et al., 1998;
Stark and Finke 2000)dby recreating a changing but still distinctive collective identity. Just as there was a shift from gender
roles to same-sex relationships as key sites for symbolic boundary drawing, the future could reveal a shift from same-sex
relationships to other sites of religious tension and identity building.
References
Adamczyk, Amy, Pitt, Cassady, 2009. Shaping attitudes about homosexuality: the role of religion and cultural context. Soc. Sci. Res. 38 (2), 338e351.
Bartkowski, John, 2001. Remaking the Godly Marriage: Gender Negotiation in Evangelical Families. Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, NJ.
Bartkowski, John, Read, Jen'nan Ghazal, 2003. Veiled submission: gender, power, and identity among evangelical and muslim women in the United States.
Qual. Sociol. 26 (1), 71e92.
Bartkowski, John, 2004. The Promise Keepers: Servants, Soldiers, and Godly Men. Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, NJ.
Bartkowski, John, Hempel, Lynn M., 2009. Sex and gender traditionalism among conservative protestants: does the difference make a difference? J. Sci.
Study Relig. 48 (4), 805e816.
Baunach, Dawn Michelle, 2011. Decomposing trends in attitudes toward gay marriage, 1988-2006. Soc. Sci. Q. 92 (2), 346e363.
Baunach, Dawn Michelle, 2012. Changing same-sex marriage attitudes in American from 1988 through 2010. Public Opin. Q. 76 (2), 364e378.
Bean, Lydia, Martinez, Brandon C., 2014. Evangelical ambivalence toward gays and lesbians. Sociol. Relig. 75 (3), 395e417.
Bolzendahl, Catherine, Myers, Daniel J., 2004. Feminist attitudes and support for gender equality: opinion change in women and men, 1974-1998. Soc.
Forces 83 (2), 759e790.
Brinkerhoff, Merlin B., MacKie, Marlene, 1984. Religious denominations' impact upon gender attitudes: some methodological implications. Rev. Relig. Res.
25 (4), 365e378.
Brinkerhoff, Merlin B., MacKie, Marlene, 1985. Religion and gender: a comparison of canadian and American student attitudes. J. Marriage Fam. 47 (2),
415e429.
Brooks, Clem, Bolzendahl, Catherine, 2004. The transformation of U.S. gender role attitudes: cohort replacement, social-structural change, and ideological
learning. Soc. Sci. Res. 33 (1), 106e133 .
Burn, Shawn Meghan, Busso, Julia, 2005. Ambivalent sexism, scriptural literalism, and religiosity. Psychol. Women Q. 29 (4), 412e418.
Charles, Maria, Grusky, David B., 2005. Occupational Ghettos: the Worldwide Segregation of Women and Men. Standford University Press, Standford, CA.
Cotter, David, Hermsen, Joan M., Vanneman, Reeve, 2011. The end of the gender revolution? Gender role attitudes from 1977 to 2008. Am. J. Sociol. 117 (1),
259e289.
Denton, Melinda Lundquist, 2004. Gender and marital decision making: negotiating religious ideology and practice. Soc. Forces 82 (3), 1151e118 0.
Dillon, Michele, 2014. Asynchrony in attitudes toward abortion and gay rights: the challenge to values alignment. J. Sci. Study Relig. 53 (1), 1e16.
Edgell, Penny, 2006. Religion and Family in a Changing Society. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Edgell, Penny, Gerteis, Joseph, Hartmann, Douglas, 2006. Atheists as Other: moral boundaries and cultural membership in American Society. Am. Sociol.
Rev. 71 (2), 211e234.
Edgell, Penny, Docka, Danielle, 2007. Beyond the nuclear family? Familism and gender ideology in diverse religious communities. Sociol. Forum 22 (1),
25e50.
Edgell, Penny, 2012. A cultural sociology of religion: new directions. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 38 (1), 247e265.
England, Paula, 2010. The gender revolution: uneven and stalled. Gend. Soc. 24 (2), 149e166 .
Faludi, Susan, 1991. Backlash: the Undeclared War against American Women. Crown, New York.
Gallagher, Sally, Smith, Christian, 1999. Symbolic traditionalism and pragmatic egalitarianism. Gend. Soc. 13 (2), 211e233.
Gallagher, Sally K., 2003. Evangelical Identity &Gendered Family Life. Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, NJ.
Gallagher, Sally K., 2004. Where are the antifeminist evangelicals? Evangelical identity, subcultural location, and attitudes toward feminism. Gend. Soc. 18
(4), 451e472.
Gerson, Kathleen, 2010. The Unnished Revolution: How a New Generation Is Reshaping Family, Work, and Gender in America. Oxford University Press,
New York.
Heath, Melanie, 2003. Soft-boiled masculinity: renegotiating gender and racial ideologies in the promise keepers movement. Gend. Soc. 17 (3), 423e444.
Hoffman, John P., Bartkowski, John, 2008. Gender, religious tradition and biblical literalism. Soc. Forces 86 (3), 1245e1272.
Hout, Michael, Fischer, Claude S., 2002. Why more americans have no religious preference: politics and generations. Am. Sociol. Rev. 67 (2), 165e190.
Hout, Michael, Fischer, Claude S., October 2014. Explaining why more Americans have no religious preference: political backlash and generational suc-
cession, 1987e2012. Sociol. Sci. 1, 423e447.
Iannaccone, Laurence R., 1994. Why strict churches are strong. Am. J. Sociol. 99 (5), 1180e1211.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e4746
Kenneavy, Kristin, 2012. Support for homosexuals' civil liberties: the inuence of familial gender role attitudes across religious denominations. Soc. Forces
90 (4), 1347e1375.
Lamont, Mich
ele, Moln
ar, Vir
ag, 2002. The study of boundaries in the social sciences. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 28, 167e195.
Lewis, Gregory B., Gossett, Charles W., 2008. Changing public opinion on same-sex marriage: the case of California. Polit. Policy 36 (1), 4e30.
Linneman, Thomas J., 2004. Homophobia and hostility: christian conservative reactions to the political and cultural progress of lesbians and gay men. Sex.
Res. Soc. Policy 1 (2), 56e76.
Loftus, Jeni, 2001. America's liberalization in attitudes toward homosexuality, 1973-1998. Am. Sociol. Rev. 66 (5), 762e782.
Luker, Kristin, 2006. When Sex Goes to School. Norton, New York.
McVeigh, Rory, Diaz, Maria-Elena D., 2009. Voting to ban same-sex marriage: interests, values, and communities. Am. Sociol. Rev. 74 (6), 891e915.
Moore, Laura M., Vanneman, Reeve, 2003. Context matters: effects of the proportion of fundamentalists on gender attitudes. Soc. Forces 82 (1), 115e139.
Olson, Laura R., Cadge, Wendy, Harrison, James T., 2006. Religion and public opinion about same-sex marriage. Soc. Sci. Q. 87 (2), 340e360.
Pampel, Fred, 2011. Cohort changes in the socio-demographic determinants of gender egalitarianism. Soc. Forces 89 (3), 961e982.
Peek, Charles W., George, D. Lowe, Williams, L. Susan, 1991. Gender and god's word: another look at religious fundamentalism and sexism. Soc. Forces 69,
1205e1221.
Perry, Samuel L., 2015. Bible beliefs, conservative religious identity, and same-sex marriage support: examining main and moderating effects. J. Sci. Study
Relig. Forthcoming.
Petersen, Larry R., Donnenwerth, Gregory V., 1998. Religion and declining support for traditional beliefs about gender roles and homosexual rights. Sociol.
Relig. 59 (4), 353e371.
Powell, Brian, Bolzendahl, Catherine, Geist, Claudia, Steelman, Lala Carr, 2010. Counted Out: Same-Sex Relations and Americans' Denitions of Family.
Russell Sage Foundation, New York.
Raftery, Adrian E., 1995. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociol. Methodol. 25 (1), 111e163.
Schnabel, Landon, 2013. When fringe goes mainstream: a sociohistorical content analysis of the christian coalition's contract with the American family and
the republican party platform. Politics Relig. Ideol 14 (1), 94e113.
Schnabel, Landon, 2014. The question of subjectivity in three emerging feminist science studies frameworks: feminist postcolonial science studies, new
feminist materialisms, and queer ecologies. Womens Stud. Int. Forum 4 4 (3), 10e16.
Schnabel, Landon, 2015. Toward a standpoint hermeneutic: the case of the evangelical gender subordination debate. Claremont J. Relig 4 (1), 86e112 .
Schnabel, Landon, Breitwieser, Lindsey, 2015. Recent advances in feminist science and technology studies: reconceptualizing subjectivity and knowledge.
Adv. Gender Res 20 (1), 43e63.
Schnittker, Jason, Freese, Jeremy, Powell, Brian, 2003. Who are feminists and what do they believe? The role of generations. Am. Sociol. Rev. 68 (4),
607e622.
Sears, William, Sears, Martha, 1993. The Baby Book: Everything You Need to Know about Your Babydfrom Birth to Age Two. Little, Brown, and Company,
Boston, MA.
Sherkat, Darren E., Mattias de Vries, Kylan, Creek, Stacia, 2010. Race, religion, and opposition to same-sex marriage. Soc. Sci. Q. 91 (1), 80e98.
Sherkat, Darren E., Powell-Williams, Melissa, Maddox, Gregory, de Vries, Kylan Mattias, 2011. Religion, politics, and support for same-sex marriage in the
United States, 1988e2008. Soc. Sci. Res. 40 (1), 167e180.
Smith, Christian, Emerson, Michael, Gallagher, Sally, Kennedy, Paul, Sikkink, David, 1998. American Evangelicalism: Embattled and Thriving. University of
Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.
Stark, Rodney, Finke, Roger, 2000. Acts of Faith: Explaining the Human Side of Religion. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA.
Steensland, Brian, Park, Jerry Z., Regnerus, Mark D., Robinson, Lynn D., Wilcox, W. Bradford, Woodbury, Robert D., 2000. The measure of American religion:
toward improving the state of the art. Soc. Forces 79 (1), 291e318.
Stone, Pamela, 2007. Opting Out? Why Women Really Quit Careers and Head Home. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA.
Swidler, Ann, 1986. Culture in action: symbols and strategies. Am. Sociol. Rev. 51 (2), 273e286.
Tranby, Eric, Zulkowski, Samanth E., 2012. Religion as cultural power: the role of religion in inuencing Americans' symbolic boundaries around gender and
sexuality. Sociol. Compass 6 (11), 870e882.
Wilcox, W. Bradford, 2004. Soft Patriarchs, New Men: How Christianity Shapes Fathers and Husbands. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Winship, Christopher, Radbill, Larry, 1994. Sampling weights and regression analysis. Sociol. Methods &Res. 23 (2), 230e257.
Woodberry, Robert D., Park, Jerry Z., Kellstedt, Lyman A., Regnerus, Mark D., Steensland, Brian, 2012. The measure of American religious traditions:
theoretical and measurement considerations. Soc. Forces 91 (1), 65e73.
L. Schnabel / Social Science Research 55 (2016) 31e47 47
... Stigmatization of LGB+ identities in the United States is primarily attributed to conservative social, political, and religious beliefs that prescribe "traditional values" (Bean and Martinez, 2014;Schnabel, 2016;Costa et al., 2019). Linked religious doctrine and political beliefs have furthered sexual minority stigmatization (Schnabel, 2016) and historically, theological beliefs frequently precluded acceptance of people who identified as LGB+, constructing them as immoral, criminal, and mentally ill (Boswell, 1980;Herek et al., 2007;Rosati et al., 2020). ...
... Stigmatization of LGB+ identities in the United States is primarily attributed to conservative social, political, and religious beliefs that prescribe "traditional values" (Bean and Martinez, 2014;Schnabel, 2016;Costa et al., 2019). Linked religious doctrine and political beliefs have furthered sexual minority stigmatization (Schnabel, 2016) and historically, theological beliefs frequently precluded acceptance of people who identified as LGB+, constructing them as immoral, criminal, and mentally ill (Boswell, 1980;Herek et al., 2007;Rosati et al., 2020). ...
... Since Stonewall, social acceptance of people who identify as lesbian and gay has increased in the United States. Examination of nationally representative General Social Survey data revealed that Americans' attitudes about same-sex sexual relations stagnated from 1973 through the early 1990s, with a favorable trend beginning at that time (Loftus, 2001;Schnabel, 2016). Although views on same-sex relations remain polarized, the number of Americans who indicated same-sex sexual relations was not wrong at all improved from 12% in 1987 to 49% in 2014 (Associated Press-NORC Center for Public Affairs Research, Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org ...
Article
Full-text available
Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and pansexual (LGB+) individuals have disproportionate rates of mental illness. Minority stress and sexual identity stigma are posited as the primary social determinants of LGB+ mental health disparities. Discussions in the literature have questioned the impact of sexual identity stigma in a world increasingly accepting of sexual minorities. Additionally, the LGB+ population in the United States South is often overlooked in American research. This article details a qualitative study exploring experiences related to sexual identity stigma among adults who identify as LGB+ in the United States South. Semi-structured interviews with 16 individuals were analyzed using content analysis. Six thematic categories of stigma emerged from participants’ experiences: (a) navigating an LGB+ identity, (b) social acceptability of an LGB+ identity, (c) expectation of LGB+ stigma, (d) interpersonal discrimination and harassment, (e) structural stigma, and (f) relationship with the lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and queer (LGBTQ) community. Findings suggest that sexual identity stigma remains a common experience among these Southern United States participants. Further, thematic categories and subcategories primarily aligned with extant theory with one exception: Intracommunity stigma, a form of stigma emanating from the LGBTQ community, emerged as a stigma type not currently accounted for in theoretical foundations underpinning mental health disparities in this population.
Chapter
Social views of sexual interest in same-sex partners continue to vary widely across historical era and cultural context. Sexual minorities continue to experience social stigma and marginalization, despite increased acceptance in many, mostly Western counties. This reflects the prevailing expectations of heterosexuality and gender conformity. Social stigma compromises the social identity of LGB people and adversely affects their social inclusion and mental and physical health. This social inequality in health and wellbeing is a result of the additional, minority stressors that LGB people uniquely face, including experiences of discrimination, rejection, internalized stigma, and concerns regarding disclosure. The solidarity of LGB communities and their heterosexual allies is critical to providing social support and facilitating the collective activism required to achieve social justice for LGB people.
Article
Between one-fifth and a third of people who are transgender have been refused treatment by a medical provider due to their gender identity. Yet, we know little about the factors that shape public opinion on this issue. We present results from a nationally representative survey experiment ( N = 4,876) that examines how common justifications issued by providers for the denial of healthcare, and the race and gender identity of the person being denied care, intersect to shape public opinion concerning the acceptability of treatment refusal. We find that religious objections are viewed as less acceptable compared to a medical justification, in this case, inadequate training. However, the difference between religious objections and inadequate training is larger when the person being denied healthcare is White or Asian than when the person is Black or Latinx. Analysis of open-ended responses indicates the modest effect of doctor’s rationale on attitudes toward treatment refusal with respect to Black and Latinx patients is partially attributable to a racialized, free-market logic. Respondents were more likely to advocate for a doctor’s fundamental right to refuse service when evaluating Black and Latinx patients compared to White patients. We discuss the implications of these findings for intersectional approaches to trans studies and future public opinion research.
Article
Within an open systems theorization, the degree to which religious identity and attendance at religious services influence attitudes toward same-sex relations and civil liberties for gays and lesbians will be shaped by how religious groups respond to societal inputs over time. In recent decades, while some Christian denominations in the United States have remained resolute in their condemnatory stance on these issues, the religious outputs of others have become more tolerant. A cross-cohort examination of the influence of religious identity and attendance at religious services on such attitudes can help uncover effects of this interplay over time. The present study tests the significance of religious identity and attendance at religious services on attitudes toward same-sex relations and civil liberties for gays and lesbians, comparing young adults across the Baby Boomer, Generation X, and Millennial cohorts. The uniqueness of the study is twofold: (1) Cross-cohort analyses are used to compare young adults within a single study rather than examining each cohort in isolation, and (2) the analyses are contextualized within an open systems framework. Data from the General Social Survey are used to examine the significance of religious identity and attendance at religious services on the attitudes of young adults (ages 20–37) toward moral acceptance of same-sex relations and civil liberties of gays and lesbians. Multiple linear regression analyses are used to test for significant differences across three birth cohorts, controlling for relevant sociodemographics. The analyses demonstrate cross-cohort change in the significance of religious identity on attitudes about the morality of same-sex relations and the civil liberties of gays and lesbians, while attendance at services is consistently a strong predictor across the generations. Of particular interest, among Millennial young adults, unlike previous generations, the evangelical Protestant identity does not affect attitudes about civil liberties for gays and lesbians. The findings suggest the importance of examining the interplay of religious outputs and societal inputs and how these dynamics influence public opinion over time. This study reveals a need for increased research into how societal inputs have shifted the output of religious organizations, not just toward gays and lesbians, but also other sexual and gender minorities.
Article
This study considers the role of religious habitus and self-concept in educational stratification. We follow 3,238 adolescents for 13 years by linking the National Study of Youth and Religion to the National Student Clearinghouse. Survey data reveal that girls with a Jewish upbringing have two distinct postsecondary patterns compared to girls with a non-Jewish upbringing, even after controlling for social origins: (1) they are 23 percentage points more likely to graduate college, and (2) they graduate from much more selective colleges. We then analyze 107 interviews with 33 girls from comparable social origins interviewed repeatedly between adolescence and emerging adulthood. Girls raised by Jewish parents articulate a self-concept marked by ambitious career goals and an eagerness to have new experiences. For these girls, elite higher education and graduate school are central to attaining self-concept congruence. In contrast, girls raised by non-Jewish parents tend to prioritize motherhood and have humbler employment aims. For them, graduating from college, regardless of its prestige, is sufficient for self-concept congruence. We conclude that religious subculture is a key factor in educational stratification, and divergent paths to self-concept congruence can help explain why educational outcomes vary by religion in gendered ways.
Article
This article sets forth a critical integrative review of the study of gender, sexuality, and religion. Treating religion as a cause, an effect, and an intermediary factor in relation to gender and sexuality, it draws on and synthesizes multiple theoretical approaches, including gender and queer lenses on religion, cultural analysis, and intersectionality. The article is structured around 10 big‐picture questions about gender, sexuality, and religion and argues that gender and sexuality are a key symbolic boundary and cultural divide in religious and political life in the United States and around the world. It concludes with an agenda for future research.
Article
Full-text available
There is little research on how resident perceptions of neighborhood unsafety develop over time and how changes in these perceptions relate to decreasing crime rates. The present study analyzes and explains trends in perceived neighborhood unsafety within the Dutch city of Rotterdam, based on survey and register data collected in the years 2003–2017 (N = 148.344, 62 neighborhoods). In addition to crime, we also assess to what extent (changes in) the economic status, level of ethnic heterogeneity, degree of residential mobility, and amount of disorder in the neighborhood play a role in how safe or unsafe inhabitants have felt in a 15-year period. We find that unsafety levels steadily declined in the years up to 2007. This decrease was best explained by changes regarding the economic status, victimization rates and disorder level of neighborhoods. After a sudden increase in feelings of unsafety between 2007 and 2008, explained by the shift towards using more self-administrated questionnaires, fear levels stabilized during the remaining years (2008–2017) although recorded crime levels continued to decrease in this period.
Preprint
Full-text available
Americans are increasingly polarized by a variety of metrics. The dimensions, extent, causes, and consequences of that polarization have been the subject of much debate. Yet despite the centrality of religion to early discussions, the analytical focus on America’s divides has largely shifted toward partisan identity, political ideology, race, and class interests. I show that religion remains powerfully implicated in all dimensions of American polarization, and sociologists must once again make religion more central to their analyses. After outlining research on American polarization, focusing on the role of religion, I survey findings within the burgeoning literatures on cultural transformation processes, (white) Christian nationalism, complex religion, and Americans’ attitudes toward science in order to underscore the centrality of ethno-religious identities, religious demography, and religious institutions for both shaping and exacerbating various forms of polarization. Lastly, I propose an agenda for elucidating religion’s ongoing role in understanding polarization beyond public opinion research at the macro-, meso-, and micro-levels. Though polarization research has been dominated by political scientists, leveraging religion in our analyses—not merely as a sui generis “variable,” but as a “site” of complex social behavior—facilitates novel sociological contributions to these literatures via our relative attention to multiple levels of analysis, theoretical eclecticism, and methodological fluidity.
Article
Intersectionality scholars have long identified dynamic configurations of race and gender ideologies. Yet, survey research on racial and gender attitudes tends to treat these components as independent. We apply latent class analysis to a set of racial and gender attitude items from the General Social Survey (1977 to 2018) to identify four configurations of individuals’ simultaneous views on race and gender. Two of these configurations hold unified progressive or regressive racial and gender attitudes. The other two formations have discordant racial and gender attitudes, where progressive views on one aspect combine with regressive views on the other. In the majority of survey years, the most commonly held configuration endorsed gender equality but espoused new racialist views that attributed racial disparities to cultural deficiencies. This perspective has become increasingly common since 1977 and is most prevalent among White women and White men, likely due to racial-group interest. Black women and Black men, in contrast, are more likely to embrace progressive racial and gender attitudes. We argue that White men’s gender egalitarianism may be rooted in self-interest, aimed at acquiring resources through intimate relationships. In contrast, Black men adopt progressive racial and gender attitudes to form a necessary coalition with Black women to challenge racism.
Chapter
Full-text available
Purpose The purpose of this chapter is to bring three recent and innovative feminist science and technology studies paradigms into dialogue on the topics of subjectivity and knowledge. Findings Each of the three frameworks – feminist postcolonial science and technology studies, queer ecologies, and new feminist materialisms – reconceptualizes and expands our understanding of subjectivity and knowledge. As projects invested in identifying and challenging the strategic conferral of subjectivity, they move from subjectivity located in all human life, to subjectivity as indivisible from nature, to a broader notion of subjectivity as both material and discursive. Despite some methodological differences, the three frameworks all broaden feminist conceptions of knowledge production and validation, advocating for increased consideration of scientific practices and material conditions in feminist scholarship. Originality This chapter examines three feminist science and technology studies paradigms by comparing and contrasting how each addresses notions of subjectivity and knowledge in ways that push us to rethink key epistemological issues. Research Implications This chapter identifies similarities and differences in the three frameworks’ discussions of subjectivity and knowledge production. By putting these frameworks into conversation, we identify methodological crossover, capture the coevolution of subjectivity and knowledge production in feminist theory, and emphasize the importance of matter in sociocultural explorations.
Article
Full-text available
This paper has two goals, one descriptive and the other prescriptive. The descriptive goal is to introduce and describe standpoint theory—an epistemological framework from sociology—and describe how it relates to the current evangelical gender debates as well as historical slavery debates. The prescriptive goal is a call for theologians to consider adopting a standpoint hermeneutic, which would involve being more self-reflexive about power and privilege. Theologians using a standpoint hermeneutic would (1) work from the standpoint of the disadvantaged, (2) ground interpretations in personal interests and experience, (3) maintain a strategically diverse discourse, (4) create knowledge that empowers the disadvantaged, (5) and include voices from as many social locations as possible in the project of religious knowledge generation.
Article
Full-text available
Twenty percent of American adults claimed no religious preference in 2012, compared to 7 percent twenty-five years earlier. Previous research identified a political backlash against the religious right and generational change as major factors in explaining the trend. That research found that religious beliefs had not changed, ruling out secularization as a cause. In this paper we employ new data and more powerful analytical tools to: (1) update the time series, (2) present further evidence of correlations between political backlash, generational succession, and religious identification, (3) show how valuing personal autonomy generally and autonomy in the sphere of sex and drugs specifically explain generational differences, and (4) use GSS panel data to show that the causal direction in the rise of the “Nones” likely runs from political identity as a liberal or conservative to religious identity, reversing a long-standing convention in social science research. Our new analysis joins the threads of earlier explanations into a general account of how political conflict over cultural issues spurred an increase in non-affiliation.
Book
The 1950s religious boom was organized around the male-breadwinner lifestyle in the burgeoning postwar suburbs. But since the 1950s, family life has been fundamentally reconfigured in the United States. How do religion and family fit together today? This book examines how religious congregations in America have responded to changes in family structure, and how families participate in local religious life. Based on a study of congregations and community residents in upstate New York, sociologist Penny Edgell argues that while some religious groups may be nostalgic for the Ozzie and Harriet days, others are changing, knowing that fewer and fewer families fit this traditional pattern. In order to keep members with nontraditional family arrangements within the congregation, these innovators have sought to emphasize individual freedom and personal spirituality and actively to welcome single adults and those from nontraditional families. Edgell shows that mothers and fathers seek involvement in congregations for different reasons. Men tend to think of congregations as social support structures, and to get involved as a means of participating in the lives of their children. Women, by contrast, are more often motivated by the quest for religious experience, and can adapt more readily to pluralist ideas about family structure. This, Edgell concludes, may explain the attraction of men to more conservative congregations, and women to nontraditional religious groups.
Book
Noting a phenomenon that might seem to recall a previous era, The New York Times Magazine recently portrayed women who leave their careers in order to become full-time mothers as "opting out." But, are high-achieving professional women really choosing to abandon their careers in order to return home? This provocative study is the first to tackle this issue from the perspective of the women themselves. Based on a series of candid, in-depth interviews with women who returned home after working as doctors, lawyers, bankers, scientists, and other professions, Pamela Stone explores the role that their husbands, children, and coworkers play in their decision; how womens efforts to construct new lives and new identities unfold once they are home; and where their aspirations and plans for the future lie. What we learncontrary to many media perceptionsis that these high-flying women are not opting out but are instead being pushed out of the workplace. Drawing on their experiences, Stone outlines concrete ideas for redesigning workplaces to make it easier for womenand mento attain their goal of living rewarding lives that combine both families and careers.
Article
When state voters passed the California Marriage Protection Act (Proposition 8) in 2008, it restricted the definition of marriage to a legal union between a man and a woman. The act's passage further agitated an already roiling national debate about whether American notions of family could or should expand to include, for example, same-sex marriage, unmarried cohabitation, and gay adoption. But how do Americans really define family? The first study to explore this largely overlooked question, Counted Out examines currents in public opinion to assess their policy implications and predict how Americans' definitions of family may change in the future. Counted Out broadens the scope of previous studies by moving beyond efforts to understand how Americans view their own families to examine the way Americans characterize the concept of family in general. The book reports on and analyzes the results of the authors' Constructing the Family Surveys (2003 and 2006), which asked more than 1,500 people to explain their stances on a broad range of issues, including gay marriage and adoption, single parenthood, the influence of biological and social factors in child development, religious ideology, and the legal rights of unmarried partners. Not surprisingly, the authors find that the standard bearer for public conceptions of family continues to be a married, heterosexual couple with children. More than half of Americans also consider same-sex couples with children as family, and from 2003 to 2006 the percentages of those who believe so increased significantly-up 6 percent for lesbian couples and 5 percent for gay couples. The presence of children in any living arrangement meets with a notable degree of public approval. Less than 30 percent of Americans view heterosexual cohabitating couples without children as family, while similar couples with children count as family for nearly 80 percent. Counted Out shows that for most Americans, however, the boundaries around what they define as family are becoming more malleable with time. Counted Out demonstrates that American definitions of family are becoming more expansive. Who counts as family has far-reaching implications for policy, including health insurance coverage, end-of-life decisions, estate rights, and child custody. Public opinion matters. As lawmakers consider the future of family policy, they will want to consider the evolution in American opinion represented in this groundbreaking book. © 2010 by the American Sociological Association. All rights reserved.
Article
Research finds that Americans who espouse theologically conservative beliefs about the Bible generally oppose same-sex marriage. Studies exploring this link, however, have been limited in that their operationalization of fundamentalist belief has been problematically conceptualized and they have potentially confounded the effect of conservative religious identity. The current study asks: (1) How do distinct beliefs about the nature and authority of the Bible influence same-sex marriage support? (2) Do these beliefs influence same-sex marriage support independently of conservative religious identity? (3) To what extent do Bible beliefs and conservative religious identity moderate one another's effects? And (4) to what extent are these factors moderated by religious tradition and frequency of Bible reading? Analyses of 2006 Portraits of American Life Study data reveal that while identifying as religiously conservative is the strongest predictor of opposition to same-sex marriage, believing in inerrancy and creationism remain strong predictors in full models. I also find moderating effects between belief in creationism, inerrancy, inspiration; religious-conservative identity; and religious tradition. Findings clarify how theological beliefs and religious identity shape support for same-sex marriage across religious traditions.
Article
Evangelical Protestants are known as vocal opponents of equal rights for gays and lesbians. Yet there is growing ambivalence among evangelicals who oppose homosexuality but support equal rights. The authors extend the concept of 'structured ambivalence' to explain why tolerance toward gays and lesbians continues to grow, even within subcultures that promote traditional views of human sexuality. The Evangelical subculture has institutionalized competing scripts and expectations about how to "do" religion with regard to gays and lesbians, which creates structured ambivalence at the overlap of social positions and institutions. Using national survey data, the authors find that 35% of Evangelicals have consistently progressive attitudes toward homosexuality, but are less religiously observant. Conversely, 24% of Evangelicals support gay civil unions, even though they are morally opposed to homosexuality. Yet these Ambivalent Evangelicals exhibit the same levels of religiosity as Gay Rights Opponents. Ambivalent support for gay rights has taken root at the core of Evangelical subculture, not just at the margins.
Article
The hypothesis that religion is conservatively linked with gender attitudes was examined by means of questionnaire data provided by 938 American and Canadian postsecondary students. Multiple indicators were used for both religion and gender attitudes. In general, the higher the religiosity, the more traditional the gender attitudes. Current religious identification proved to be a stronger correlate of gender attitudes than childhood affiliation. The "Religious Nones" were the most egalitarian, followed by the Catholics, Mainline Protestants, and Fundamentalists, with the Mormons being the most traditional of all. The religion variables were considerably more powerful predictors of gender attitudes than were the demographic variables.