ArticlePDF Available
1
A review of hypnosis in the treatment of parasomnias: Nightmare, sleepwalking and sleep
terror disorders.
Kennedy, G. A. (2002) A review of hypnosis in the treatment of parasomnias: Nightmares, sleepwalking and
sleep terror disorders. Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 30(2), 99-155
Gerard A. Kennedy
Running Head: Hypnosis for treating parasomnias
Department of Psychology, Victoria University, PO Box 14428 Melbourne City MC 8001
Australia
Gerard.Kennedy@vu.edu.au
2
Abstract
Hypnosis was used to treat patients with the primary parasomnias; nightmare, sleepwalking
and sleep terror disorders. The results for the patient with nightmare disorder suggested that
the effective element in decreasing the frequency of nightmares was the specific hypnotic
suggestion to alter the nightmare content. The generalised effects of increased relaxation and
improved sleep also contributed to therapeutic efficacy. Two other patients, with
sleepwalking and sleep terror disorders were also treated. In both the cases the effective
element in decreasing the frequency of these parasomnias appeared to be the generalised
effects of hypnosis. The data supports the observations of other authors who have suggested
the general lowering of tonic levels due to the anxiolytic effects of relaxation employed
during hypnosis is responsible for reducing the incidence of these disorders. In conclusion,
hypnosis is a relatively simple, non-invasive, inexpensive, and effective means of treating
nightmare, sleepwalking and sleep terror disorders.
3
INTRODUCTION
Was it a vision, or a waking dream? Fled is that music:- Do I wake or sleep?
John Keats 1795-1821
Parasomnias are disorders characterised by the occurrence of abnormal physiological and
behavioural events in association with specific stages of sleep and/or during transitional
stages of sleep (Mahowald & Rosen, 1990). In contrast to dyssomnias (disorders of initiation,
maintenance and/or timing of the sleep), parasomnias do not involve abnormalities of the
mechanisms generating or timing sleep. Parasomnias involve the inappropriate activation of
the autonomic nervous system or cognitive processes during various stages of sleep. Different
parasomnias occur at specific stages during sleep, during transitions from one sleep stage to
another, and/or at the sleep-wake or wake-sleep transitions. Most individuals suffering from
parasomnias present with complaints of unusual behaviour during sleep rather than
complaints of insomnia or excessive daytime sleepiness (Mahowald & Rosen, 1990).
However, in some cases excessive daytime sleepiness may result from awakenings caused by
parasomnia events during sleep.
Sleepwalking, nightmares and sleep terror are the most easily identifiable parasomnias.
Surveys suggest that nightmares, sleepwalking and sleep terror occur more frequently than
indicated by case study reports in the literature. A survey of 1006 households in the Los
Angeles area found that 11% of the subjects experienced nightmares and 2.5% sleepwalking
(Bixler, Kales, Soldatos, Kales & Healy, 1979). Belicki and Belicki (1982) found that 443
undergraduate students reported having more than five nightmares a year. Wood and Bootzin
(1990) found that 47% of 220 under graduates who kept dreams records over a two-week
period, reported at least one nightmare during the study. The estimated annual frequency of
nightmares from this study was 23.6%, which is 2.5 times the average suggested by other
retrospective studies.
It is important that parasomnias are correctly diagnosed and that effective treatment is given
if the behaviours associated with sleep are potentially injurious, violent or disruptive to the
patient or other people. Parasomnias can be organised into categories based on a number of
4
diagnostic criteria. These criteria are: (1) primary sleep phenomena and (2) symptoms of
organic systems that are manifested during sleep or associated transitional states (Mahowald
& Rosen, 1990). The classification system shown in Table 1 is based on clinical phenomena.
Mahowald and Rosen’s (1990) clinical approach to the classification of parasomnias differs
from that of the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
(DSM-IV) and that of the Association of Sleep Disorders Centers (Diagnostic Classification
of Sleep and Arousal Disorders). The advantage of Mahowald and Rosen’s (1990) system is
that it allows differential diagnosis of unusual sleep-related phenomenon and also points to
the therapeutic implications of diagnosis.
Insert Table 1. About Here
Primary sleep parasomnias are classified on the basis of whether events occur during non-
rapid-eye movement sleep (NREM), rapid-eye-movement sleep (REM) or occur during any
stage of sleep (miscellaneous). NREM and REM primary sleep parasomnias tend to occur
only during these sleep stages, while miscellaneous parasomnias may occur during any stage
of sleep (Mahowald & Rosen, 1990). Secondary sleep parasomnias are undesirable or
troublesome motor, behavioural or physiological events that occur during sleep. Secondary
sleep parasomnias result from the activity of bodily systems and can be classified according
to the organ system responsible for genesis.
Sleepwalking Disorder
During quiet sleepwalking the individual rises from bed and quietly walks about with their
eyes open. The person may walk towards a stimulus such as a light or noise or may walk
aimlessly from room to room. Children often walk into their parent’s bedroom. The person is
usually able to find their way around large obstacles. Occasionally, children and adults may
perform inappropriate behaviours during quiet sleepwalking (e.g. urinating on the floor).
Sleepwalking is very common in children with about 15% (mostly boys) having episodes (cf.
< 5% of adults) (Kales & Kales, 1974). The causes of sleepwalking vary according to the age
of the subject (Berlin & Qayyum, 1986). A study by Klackenberg (1982) of 212 children,
aged 6-16 in Sweden, found that the incidence of quiet sleepwalking was 40%. The study
also indicated an annual prevalence of 6-17% with only 2-3% having more than one episode
5
per month. In 33% of the children sleepwalking persisted for 5 years and in 12% it persisted
for more than 10 years.
In children, sleepwalking is usually benign and self-limiting, beginning at about age seven,
generally before age 10, and ceasing before age 15. Sleepwalking episodes usually last only a
few minutes and rarely extend beyond 10 minutes in duration. Sleepwalkers are difficult to
communicate with and if left alone often return to bed. When awakened, they have little
memory of anything that happened during the episode. Kales, Paulson, Jacobson, and Kales
(1966) found that few sleepwalking incidents were related to specific traumatic events.
Sleepwalking generally occurs about one or two hours after falling asleep. Sleepwalking
often begins with a burst of high voltage, slow frequency EEG activity and is related to
arousal from stage 3/4 NREM sleep (Kales et al, 1966; Broughton, 1968).
There is often a family history of sleepwalking (Vela-Bueno, Soldatos & Julius, 1987). Kales,
Soldatos, Bixler, Ladda, Charney, Weber & Schweitzer (1980) found that 96% of 52 patients
referred to a sleep research center reported one or more family member with a history of
sleepwalking. A Japanese study showed that sleepwalking was more common in 37 children
whose parents had also been sleepwalkers as children than in children with no family history
of this disorder (Abe, Amatoni & Oda, 1984).
Sleepwalking can be caused by the use of particular medications (Nadel, 1981). Barbiturates
and monamine oxidase inhibitor antidepressants, suppress all REM sleep, and tricyclic
antidepressants also reduce REM sleep. By altering the stages of sleep these drugs may
influence the frequency of sleepwalking (Kales, Tan, Preston & Allen, 1970; Kupfer &
Bowers, 1972). Flemenbaum (1976) administered a questionnaire to 30 medical and 106
psychiatric patients to determine the frequency of frightening dreams. In both groups roughly
equal numbers had dreams, but 28% of the psychiatric patients compared with 14% of the
medical patients had frightening dreams. The difference between the two groups was even
greater when patients who were taking psychotropic medication (antipsychotic or tricyclic
antidepressants) in a single dose at bedtime were compared with medical patients.
Sleepwalking, frightening dreams, confusion and physiological arousal were significantly
more frequent in psychiatric patients compared with hospitalised patients receiving no
medication. Huapaya (1979) reported seven cases of sleepwalking that were apparently the
result of the use of antipsychotic and antidepressant drugs in patients with long-standing
6
psychiatric problems. Sleepwalking and sleep terror have also been reported in children and
adolescents with febrile illnesses (Karacan, Wolff, Williams, Hursch & Webb, 1968; Kales,
Kales, Soldatos, Chamberlin & Martin, 1979).
Sleepwalkers tend to move in confused and clumsy manner, but gradually they manifest
better co-ordination, avoiding objects and often going to the bathroom (Kales & Kales, 1974).
In most cases sleepwalkers do not suffer any harm, but occasionally they may injure
themselves and have been know to injure others (Hartmann, 1983; Oswald & Evans, 1985).
Luchins, Sherwood, Gillin, Mendelson and Wyatt (1978) observed a 40-year old women in a
sleep laboratory, who had been acquitted of killing her daughter while sleepwalking. The
women had been diagnosed and hospitalised for schizophrenia prior to the filicide. Four years
after she was acquitted and released from hospital she was administered the same drug that
she had been taking when the filicide occurred. She was observed to sleepwalk on three of
the four nights she took the drug. Sleepwalking occurred during stage 4 sleep in the first half
of night.
Sleep Terror Disorder
Sleep terror disorder consists of nocturnal episodes of extreme terror and panic that occur
early in the sleep period in stage 3 or 4 sleep (Kales et al, 1982; Hartmann, 1984). Sleep
terror, has not received as much attention as nightmares because it is not as common.
Broughton (1968) found that sleep terror occurred during sudden and intense arousal from
slow-wave sleep with an EEG pattern similar to the awake pattern. Sleep terror episodes
usually only last a few minutes and there is little or no recall of the episode. Two thirds of
sleep terror episodes occurred during the first NREM sleep period in six subjects monitored
during 100 sessions (Fisher, Kahn, Edwards & Davis, 1973). Sleep terror usually begins
before the age of 10 and is usually outgrown during adolescence (Kales & Kales, 1974).
Sleep terror episodes may occur as often as three times a week (Kales, Soldatos, Caldwell,
Charney, Markel & Cadieux, 1980).
Sleep terrors are distinguishable from nightmares in terms of clinical features and sleep-
laboratory findings. Sleep terror includes more vocalisations, motility, and autonomic
discharge and usually more fear and anxiety than nightmares. Sleep terror occurs during sleep
stages 3 and 4, whereas nightmares occur during REM sleep (Kales et al, 1982; Hartmann,
7
1984; Vela-Bueno et al, 1987). Sleep terror and sleepwalking have similar clinical
characteristics (Hartmann, Greenwald & Brune, 1982; Kales, Cadieux, Soldatos & Kales,
1982; Oswald & Evans, 1985; Vela-Bueno et al, 1987). Kales, Soldatos, Caldwell, Charney
and Martin (1980) investigated sleep terror and sleepwalking in 40 adults and found that
sleep terror was often accompanied by sleepwalking and that the two sleep disorders seemed
related to genetic and developmental factors. Halstrom (1972) traced a family history of sleep
terror through three generations. When onset of these sleep disorders occurs after age 10, they
are likely to persist in adulthood, episodes are more frequent, and the time of onset is often
associated with major life stressors.
Mahowald and Rosen (1990) proposed a model for understanding the determinants and
manifestations of partial arousals from sleep. When a period of deep NREM sleep terminates
there is a transition that leads to one of three possible state changes: (1) beginning the next
sleep cycle by switching to different sleep stages; (2) continue arousal until full awakening;
or (3) become ‘trapped’ - unable to completely get out of deep sleep, unable to arouse fully,
and unable to move into the next sleep cycle.
There are three main factors that determine the occurrence of observable partial arousals.
These factors are: (1) tonic sleep factors; (2) phasic sleep factors; and (3) the behavioural
response to the arousal (Mahowald & Rosen, 1990). The tonic factors that occur throughout
the sleep period determine the underlying sleep pattern and arousal threshold. These include:
(1) genetic; (2) developmental; (3) sleep deprivation; (4) chaotic sleep/wake scheduling; (5)
drugs; and (6) psychological factors.
The most important tonic factors appear to be genetic and developmental. Partial arousals are
most common in young children because deep sleep is deeper and longer than it is in older
children. Phasic sleep factors (intermittent occurrence during the sleep cycle) can also cause
the disruption of sleep (Mahowald & Rosen, 1990). In some individuals who are predisposed
to partial arousals due to one of the tonic sleep factors listed above, a partial arousal can be
precipitated by any strategically timed phasic sleep factor. Phasic sleep factors include both
endogenous and exogenous factors. Endogenous factors may be both spontaneous and
pathologic in nature. Pathologic phasic factors are: (1) obstructive sleep apnoea; (2)
gastroesophageal reflux; (3) seizures; (4) fever; (5) periodic movements; and (6)
8
psychological. Exogenous factors are: (1) stimulation such as auditory, tactile or visual; and
(2) drugs.
In many individuals arousals appear to be spontaneous. This is evidenced by the fact that
sleepwalking can be induced in children by standing them up during slow wave sleep (Kales
et al, 1966; Broughton, 1968). Similarly, sleep terror can be triggered in susceptible
individuals by auditory stimulation during slow wave sleep. Both these observations suggest
that these behaviours are not the result of ongoing complex mental activity during sleep
(Fisher, Byrne, Edwards & Kahn, 1970; Fisher, Kahn Edwards, Davis & Fine, 1973). The
behavioural responses to the arousal is the clinically observed behaviour that occurs during
the partial arousal, and will be determined by developmental or psychological factors, as well
as by drugs. Tonic sleep factors, phasic sleep factors and the behavioural response to the
arousal interact to determine the frequency, duration and severity of the partial arousal.
Mahowald and Rosen’s (1990) model allows a framework for understanding the factors
contributing to partial arousals and in addition may point to the preferred treatment when the
diagnosis has been confirmed. Differential diagnosis of partial arousals excludes nocturnal
seizures, anxiety dreams (nightmares), behaviour disorders and dissociated states (Schenck,
Milner, Hurwitz, Bundlie & Mahowald 1989). Individuals with these conditions may show
unusual nocturnal behaviour that is indistinguishable from the disorders of partial arousal.
The important factor in the differential diagnosis of unusual nocturnal behaviour is a detailed
history that in most cases allows the correct diagnosis to be established. In obtaining a
detailed history the important facts include: (1) the timing of the event during the sleep cycle;
(2) a detailed description of the event; (3) the level of consciousness before during and after
the arousal; (4) daytime symptoms of sleepiness; (5) injury associated with the arousal; (6)
memory for the event; and (7) personal/family history of partial arousal disorder (Mahowald
& Rosen, 1990).
In the treatment of partial arousals it is important to confirm that a correct diagnosis has been
made. Patients, particularly children should be given reassurance that in most cases the
arousal is benign and self-limiting in nature. Patients should be given practical advice about
basic safety precautions that can be taken. For example, the bedroom should be cleared of
obstacles and any windows should be locked or otherwise secured. In some cases it may be
advisable to fit deadlocks or alarms to outside doors. Patients should be warned that sleep
9
deprivation should be avoided because it may precipitate partial arousals. They should also be
instructed to make the sleep/wake cycle as regular as possible. People living with the patient
should be advised not to intervene during arousals as this may prolong the event. In most
cases the sufferer is best left alone and any observer should just ensure that no injury occurs.
In cases where the arousals are dangerous or disruptive to the patient or to others, and no
obvious precipitant can be identified, symptomatic treatment is needed. Two successful
interventions for partial arousals have been described: pharmacological therapy
(benzodiazepines & tricyclic antidepressants) (Fisher, Kahn & Edwards, 1973; Kavey,
Whyte, Resor & Gidro-Frank, 1987; Guilleminault, 1989) and hypnosis with relaxation and
mental imagery (Pesikoff & Davis, 1971; Reid, Ahmed & Levie, 1981; Gardner & Olness,
1981). Valium, which tends to suppress slow-wave sleep, has been shown to reduce the
incidence of sleep terror episodes (Kales et al, 1982). The main problem with drug therapy
for partial arousals is the long-term nature of the disorder and the adverse effects chronic
drug usage may have on learning and behaviour (Weissbluth, 1984). Teaching patients
relaxation and mental imagery offers an alternative and allows the use of self-regulation
techniques to control previously uncontrolled nighttime behaviours. The mechanism(s)
resulting in reduced frequency of partial arousals after pharmacological and/or hypnotic
treatment are not well understood. It is possible that these treatments change underlying tonic
sleep factors resulting in a higher arousal threshold and fewer arousals. It is also possible that
the treatments (particularly hypnosis) cause a change in the behavioural response to the
arousal.
Nightmare Disorder (Dream Anxiety Attacks)
Dream anxiety attacks are frightening dreams (nightmares) that are accompanied by moderate
levels of autonomic activity (tachycardia, tachypnea, diaphoresis) and arousal (Association of
Sleep Disorders Centers, 1979). In comparison to sleep terror, the level of autonomic
activation is considerably less and recollection of the dream content is usually fairly
complete. Dream anxiety attacks do not occur at sleep onset and are not associated with
feelings of oppression or paralysis as may be experienced with sleep paralysis and
hypnogogic hallucinations.
10
Fisher Byrne, Edwards and Kahn (1970) recorded EEG, electroculogram (EOG), heart rate,
conversations with subjects, other vocalisations, and ratings of anxiety of 38 subjects with
nightmare disorder. Subjects were allowed to sleep undisturbed each night until
spontaneously awakening with anxiety. Subjects were interviewed on awakening to examine
the nature of the nightmare experience. Three types of nightmares were identified. Fifty
major stage 4 nightmares (sleep terror) were observed in seven subjects, 22 REM anxiety
dreams in 11 subjects, and seven subjects awoke spontaneously during stage 2 sleep.
Hartmann, Russ, Olfield, Sivan and Cooper (1987) found that 12 subjects remembered
having nightmares before they were 5 years old and that the frequency decreased between the
ages of seven to 11 years. Generally, nightmares do not become a matter of concern unless
they are recurrent, disrupt sleep and are anxiogenic (Erman, 1987).
Nightmares always occur during REM (dream sleep) and more often in the latter half of the
sleep period (Hartmann, 1984). Therefore, drugs that influence REM sleep may influence the
occurrence of nightmares. In addition to EEG activity during nightmares, the underlying
biological processes probably involve changes in the levels of brain neurotransmitters.
Parkinson’s patients frequently report nightmares and vivid dreams. L-Dopa, a form of the
neurotransmitter dopamine, counteracts the symptoms of Parkinson’s disease. Patients who
are more susceptible to nightmares report increased frequency of nightmares after small doses
of L-Dopa (Hartmann, Skoff, Russ & Oldfield, 1978; Hartmann et al, 1987).
During a nightmare there are signs of intense anxiety, evidenced by sweating, dilated pupils
and difficulty breathing. Dunn and Barrett (1988) found that scores on The Manifest Anxiety
Scale of 79 college students who experienced frequent nightmares, on were not significantly
different to the scores of students who did not experience nightmares. Individuals who
experience nightmares are more easily aroused and usually recall the content, which is more
lengthy and detailed than in sleep terror. Nightmares with frightening or unpleasant content
usually occur during REM sleep (Kales & Kales, 1974). Major life events have been
associated with the onset of nightmares and mental stress increases the frequency of attacks
(Celucci & Lawrence, 1978b; Kales Soldatos, Bixler, Ladda, Charney, Weber & Schweitzer,
1980; Garfield, 1987).
11
Earlier studies have shown that subjects who suffer frequent nightmares in REM sleep
showed many schizophrenic characteristics (Hartmann, 1965; Hartmann & Russ, 1979).
Psychiatric interviews revealed considerable psychopathology in them and in their family
histories. The mean scores of eight men were two standard deviations above normal on the
MMPI depression scale, and the scores of 12 women were two standard deviations above
normal on the psychopathic deviate and mania scales. In Hersen’s (1971) study, hospital
patients with psychotic disorders were divided into groups according to the frequency of
reported nightmares. Patients with more manifest anxiety and concern about death reported
more frequent nightmares. Jones (1951) reported that more frequent nightmare attacks tended
to accompany the development of hysteria and insanity, but his observations were based on
patients confined to psychiatric institutions.
Hartmann et al (1987) compared 12 lifelong nightmare sufferers with 12 subjects who had
vivid dreams, but no nightmares. Subjects who suffered nightmares scored significantly
higher on the psychosis scale of the MMPI. These subjects were also more likely to have
relatives who experienced nightmares and who required psychiatric admissions. In general,
nightmare sufferers were more sensitive and open people, tended to show some features of
the schizophrenic spectrum of disorders, and were more artistic with more manifest creative
tendencies and interests (Hartmann, Falke, Russ & Oldfield, 1981; Hartmann, Russ, Van Der
Kolk, Falke & Oldfield, 1981; Belicki & Belicki, 1986).
Studies have found that fear of death is often present in nightmares. In Feldman and Herson
(1967) study five percent of the subjects experienced nightmares frequently, and 86% had at
least one nightmare per year. Women scored higher than men on a 10-item Death Scale, but
subjects of both sexes with more frequent nightmares, expressed significantly more concern
about death. Feelings of helplessness and being threatened were common themes reported by
those experiencing frequent nightmares. Women reported these themes twice as often as men.
The final stage of women’s nightmare experiences was often escape from danger by
awakening (Feldman & Hyman, 1968).
Lester (1968) examined fear of death as a feature of nightmares by asking 304 college
students to estimate the frequency of dreams and nightmares and to complete a Fear of Death
Scale. Subjects with the poorest memory for dreams reported fear of death less often. Lester
(1968) suggested that type of question asked and subject’s memory of dreams overstated the
12
prominence of fear of death in the nightmares of frequent sufferers. In a later study of 170
students, the frequency of nightmares was found to be associated more with fears of others
dying rather than fear of one’s own death (Lester, 1969).
Surveys of war veterans indicate that combat personnel report nightmares more frequently
than non-combat personnel (Kramer, Schoen & Kinney, 1987; Kramer & Kinney, 1988). A
study of 529 World War II and Korean War combat veterans in a mixed medical and
psychiatric outpatient clinic showed that 32% had frequent nightmares, a higher rate than
expected from general population data (Van Der Kolk, Blitz, Burr, Sherry & Hartmann,
1984). The nightmares of veterans were found to occur earlier in sleep, usually reflected
actual events, and were more commonly associated with gross bodily movement. The MMPI
profiles were abnormally high and Rorschach protocols revealed evidence of thought disorder
in many veterans.
Some research has suggested that anxiety is associated with nightmares (Celucci &
Lawrence, 1978b). Haynes and Mooney (1975) explored the sleep patterns, anxiety levels,
and physiological arousal of college students experiencing nightmares in three questionnaire
studies. In the first study, 248 students who experienced nightmares did not differ in sleep
patterns from students who had no nightmares. The second study examined the relationship
between anxiety and the frequency of nightmares. It was found that scores on the Manifest
Anxiety Scale were positively related to the frequency of nightmares. This suggested that
anxiety influenced the frequency of nightmares. It was hypothesised that nightmares may
have an anxiety-reducing role, facilitating the extinction of anxiety and physical arousal. To
test this hypothesis, implosive therapy was used with four female students who experienced
frequent nightmares. Haynes and Mooney (1975) found that the therapy reduced the
frequency and intensity of nightmares experienced by the four subjects.
Psychological factors have been given a major role in the development and persistence of
nightmares (Kales, Soldatos, Caldwell, Charney, Kales, Markel & Cadieux, 1980).
Behavioural therapy has been shown to be beneficial for some adults who suffer recurrent
nightmares (Geer & Silverman, 1967; Shorkey & Himle, 1974; Schindler, 1980). Similarly,
systematic desensitisation has been shown to reduce the frequency and intensity of nightmare
experiences (Celucci & Lawrence, 1978a). Miller and DiPilato (1983) studied 32 nightmare
sufferers randomly assigned to a relaxation group, a systematic desensitisation group and a
13
waiting list group (control). In both treatment groups there was a significant decrease in the
frequency, but not the intensity of nightmares. Subsequent treatment of the waiting list group
also significantly reduced the frequency of nightmare attacks.
Pharmacotherapy has been shown to be effective in reducing the frequency and intensity of
some parasomnias. This effect probably operates via drug effects in altering sleep phases and
REM sleep (Hartmann, 1966, 1984). Nightmares occur during REM sleep, thus anti-anxiety
agents such as diazepam (Valium) and antidepressants that suppress REM sleep have helped
patients with frequent nightmares (Erman, 1987, Kales, Tan, Preston & Allen, 1970).
However, drug withdrawal may lead to REM-rebound and temporarily increase the
occurrence of nightmares (Kales, Soldatos & Kales, 1987). Beta-adrenergic blocker drugs
like propranol, prescribed for some heart patients, may increase the frequency of nightmares.
Reserpine a drug that prevents the intraneuronal storage of the neurotransmitters dopamine
and norepinephrine, also increases the frequency of nightmares (Hartmann, 1966, 1984).
Conclusions
In general, parasomnias such as nightmares, sleep terror and sleepwalking are better
understood than they were when Jones (1951) described them. Increased understanding of
parasomnias has largely been due to objective studies that have been performed using
polysomnography. Polysomnographic studies have allowed the behavioural disturbances
observed in parasomnias to be correlated with the underlying physiological states. However,
clinical understanding of sleep-associated phenomena remains relatively poor because often
those suffering from these conditions do not come to the attention of relevant medical
authorities.
Hypnosis and the Treatment of Nightmare Disorder
In the literature on sleep there are numerous references to theories about the function of
dreams (e.g. Hobson 1988; Bootzin, Kihlstrom & Schacter, 1990; Ellman & Antrobus, 1991).
However, there is very little literature on nightmares (Hartmann, 1984), and even less on
recurring nightmares. Repetitive nightmares may occur frequently with either the same or
differing themes. Recurrent nightmares have been reported and discussed as a definitive
feature of PTSD (Ross, Ball, Sullivan & Caroff, 1989). There are fewer reports of repetitive
14
nightmares featuring in non-traumatic conditions such as the anxiety and depressive
disorders. The only diagnosis other than PTSD for which nightmares are considered a
diagnostic feature is Nightmare Disorder (DSM-IV, 1994).
The lack of attention that recurrent nightmares have received may be due to the tendency for
clinicians the view nightmares as secondary symptoms of other underlying psychopathology.
Given that this is the case, the underlying disorder becomes the focus of treatment rather than
the nightmares (Eccles, Wilde & Marshall, 1988). Psychoanalytic practitioners may also view
nightmares as a useful source of information to aid in diagnosing and understanding the
patient’s psychopathology. In cases where nightmares cause severe sleep disturbance,
patients may be prescribed sedative medications. There are very few accounts of
psychotherapy being routinely used to directly address recurrent nightmares (Halliday, 1982;
Miller & DiPilato, 1983; Brylowski, 1990; Kellner, Neidhardt, Krakow & Pathak, 1992,).
The focus on nightmares as symptomatic of underlying psychopathology (which may be
correct in some cases) has probably led to needlessly prolonged distress in some patients.
Recurrent nightmares can cause fatigue from loss of sleep due to fear, and anxiety and
depression may increase as a results of the effect nightmares have on the patient’s waking
life. Patients may see the continuation of nightmares as evidence that psychotherapy is not
proving to be effective and this, in turn, can lead to further demoralisation (Frank & Frank,
1991).
Hypnosis is one direct method that has been effectively employed to allow patients to either
terminate or control recurrent nightmares (Kingsbury, 1993). There are a number of possible
mechanisms via which the hypnotic treatment of recurrent nightmares may have some impact
on other related problems in the cluster of “symptoms” that make up the diagnosed
psychopathology. For example, the successful treatment of any aspect of what patients
believe are related problems may lead to increased expectation that the overall therapy will be
helpful in addressing other problem areas. Furthermore, the successful treatment of a problem
may have an impact on other important areas of functioning. A treatment that leads to the
termination or control of recurrent nightmares may lead to significantly improved sleep and
hence higher daytime levels of energy and concentration. A further benefit of treating
nightmares directly is that this conforms to the patient’s view of what the problem really is.
Most patients are not able to understand that each symptom may be part of a set caused by
15
some central psychopathology or condition. The reflection of the patient’s understanding has
been held as an essential factor in demonstrating empathy and establishing a therapeutic
relationship (Rogers, 1957).
The proposed use of hypnosis as a treatment for recurrent nightmares may be questioned,
given that its differential effectiveness in altering dreams has not been clearly established.
However, there are a number of factors that suggest that hypnosis may be useful for treating
recurrent nightmares (Kingsbury, 1993). In the hypnotic state, the nightmare content is more
involving for the patient than under normal psychotherapy. Therefore, the patient has greater
access to the emotional state the nightmare produces and this appears to be therapeutically
useful (Gilligan, 1988). Furthermore, the hypnotic induction may mark a separate state from
normal consciousness and may heighten the expectation that this state has the power to effect
direct changes in what may otherwise be seemingly non-volitional behaviour (Combs &
Freedman, 1990; van der Hart, 1993). Research and theory support the link between hypnosis
and dreams via proposed dissociative mechanisms (Gabel, 1989, 1990). Finally, the available
literature indicates that hypnosis can be effectively used to either terminate or control the
content of recurrent nightmares (Tart, 1966; Moss, 1973; Marks, 1978; Seif, 1985;
Kingsbury, 1993).
A variety of interventions have been shown to alter dreams and their content (Arkin &
Antrobus, 1991; Walker & Johnson, 1974). However, there has been very little research on
the effect hypnosis has on subsequent dreaming. Research that has been carried out may
provide some ideas for therapeutic interventions, but in general its clinical significance has
been limited. Much of the earlier research examining the effects of hypnosis on dreams has
been carried out on populations of university students rather than on patients suffering
recurrent nightmares. The main aim of these studies has been to determine whether or not
specific dream content can be produced via hypnosis.
Tart and Dick (1970) studied the conscious control of dreaming in 13 highly hypnotisable
subjects during two nights in the laboratory. Before subjects went to sleep each night they
were hypnotised and instructed to dream about a stimulus narrative in every stage 1-REM
dream of the night. A second stimulus narrative was used for the other night in a
counterbalanced order. Dream reports were obtained from stage 1-REM awakenings. With
the exception of one subject, all subjects showed some effects, with two to four elements of
16
the stimulus narratives reported as appearing in dreams. Thematic analysis of subjects dream
reports showed that eight of the 13 subjects reported at least one dream in which the stimulus
narrative was a dominant organiser of content. The number of stimulus narrative elements
appearing in the dream reports was positively correlated with hypnotisability. The
correlations were positive for factors of hypnotisability characterising ability to function in an
altered state of consciousness and negative for compulsive, inhibitory aspects of
hypnotisability. Hypnosis was helpful, but not necessary to achieve deliberate control of
dream content. Such control has important therapeutic ramifications, particularly in assisting
those with recurrent nightmares to control or diminish the effects.
Barber, Walker and Hahn (1973) examined the effects of hypnotic induction and types of
suggestion on sleep mentation in 73 subjects. Just prior to sleep each night, half the subjects
were exposed to a hypnotic induction and half were not, and all subjects were given either
authoritative, permissive, or no suggestions to think and dream that night on a specific topic.
Subjects reported their thoughts and dreams when awakened at sleep onset and during REM
and NREM periods. The content of the dreams were affected by an interaction between
hypnotic induction and types of suggestions, and the hypnotic induction increased the number
of nocturnal thoughts that pertained to the specific topic. The findings of this study suggest,
as did the findings of the earlier study by Tart and Dick (1970) that hypnosis can be used to
effectively alter dreams and therefore might be useful for altering nightmare content.
A study by Belicki & Belicki (1986) examined the relationships between nightmare
frequency and hypnotic ability, vividness of visual imagery and tendency to become absorbed
in fantasy-like experiences. The subjects were 841 undergraduate university students who
participated in group tests of hypnotisability, after which they estimated the number of
nightmares that they had experienced in the previous year. In addition, 406 of the subjects
completed Mark’s Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire, and Rotenberg and Bower’s
Absorption Scale. A total of 76% of the subjects reported experiencing at least one nightmare
in the previous year and 8.3% reported experiencing one or more nightmares per month.
Individuals with frequent nightmares scored higher on hypnotisability, vividness of visual
imagery and absorption.
Some research has focused on the relationship between nightmare frequency and
psychopathology. Belicki and Belicki’s (1986) study examined the relationship of nightmares
17
with more general personality (or cognitive) styles. Hartmann et al (1981) observed increased
creativity in nightmare sufferers. Other studies have associated hypnotic ability with “fear-
proneness”. Positive correlations have been observed between hypnotic ability and phobias
(Frankel & Orne, 1976) and posttraumatic stress disorder (Stutman & Bliss, 1985). Increased
frequency of nightmares in highly hypnotisable subjects may be an indication of
predisposition towards fear-based syndromes. However, it in not clear how relevant these
observations are in relation to people in the general population who suffer nightmares.
Belicki, Altray and Hill (1985) observed that nightmares are not always anxiety dreams, but
can be primarily characterised by other dysphoric emotions, such as grief. Hartmann (1984)
argued that patients with PTSD are quite different from those that present with nightmares
that do not stem from a known traumatic incident.
The main source of information about the hypnotic treatment of recurrent nightmares are the
few available published case studies and anecdotal clinical notes. The problem with case
studies and clinical notes is that the amount of information given is often limited and may not
be very useful. In most of these reports only rarely is there any attempt to present a number of
similar cases in a coherent fashion and generalisations made are more often than not, poorly
transferable from one case to another. Nevertheless, case studies and clinical notes are
heuristically valuable in that they do suggest possible hypnotic treatment strategies.
Clinical Studies of the Hypnotic Treatment of Nightmare Disorder
The available clinical literature suggests there are a number of different strategies for treating
recurrent nightmares that replay the patient’s actual experiences (these strategies would be
equally applicable to nightmares based on real non-traumatic or imagined experiences).
These types of nightmares are can be indicative of traumatic experiences in recent adult life.
This type of recurrent nightmare has been more frequently reported in the literature, probably
due to the clear traumatic aetiology. One reported strategy involves transforming the
nightmare while the patient is reliving or replaying it under hypnosis (Eichelman, 1985;
Gilligan, 1988). In this technique new elements may introduced into the nightmare or
frightening elements in the nightmare may be altered to appear more benign. Regardless of
whether a new element is introduced separately, or made by changing something that occurs
within the nightmare, the goal of this strategy is to change the patient’s reality so that it can
be coped with more easily.
18
The reported case studies indicate that this technique can be used to achieve the desired goal
in a stable manner even though it alters a reality that the patient may know to be factual. In
such cases a helpful friend or parent may be introduced into a nightmare to provide support,
or a threatening weapon may be transformed into some harmless object. Some therapists may
be concerned about the fact the technique involves altering the patient’s recall of a factual
event rather than helping the patient to gain mastery over the nightmare content. However,
this strategy has been reported to be highly effective in practice. Erikson (1959), in work on
revivification of nightmares alluded to the fact that the technique was designed not to alter
the true memory occurring within the nightmare but to add fantasy elements that help the
patient cope with the frightening nightmare content.
Another strategy that has been reported in the literature involves having the patient re-expose
themselves to the content of the nightmare, but in a manner that allows them acquire mastery
of the nightmare content. The patient can be taught how to interrupt the nightmare, first in the
treatment setting under hypnosis and then later at home (Gorton, 1988). A further method is
based on desensitisation or flooding where there is repeated exposure to the nightmare while
the person is hypnotised. A variation of this technique allows the patient to have more felt-
control by having them place the nightmare on a television or movie screen. In this method
the hypnotised patient has control of the imagery via an imaginary remote-control. A further
variation re-exposes the patient to the nightmare in progressively less involving contexts
(Kingsbury, 1988). Bishay (1985), who used a non-hypnotic treatment, and Kingsbury (1988,
1992) a hypnotic treatment for nightmares both suggested that gaining felt self-efficacy was
an important therapeutic factor in eliminating recurrent nightmares.
Helping the patient to solve a nightmare in some manner is another way to eliminate
recurrent nightmares. In this context, the recurrent nightmare can be viewed as an unfinished
story that is interrupted at some point by the patient waking. Waking may be the only way the
patient can escape from extremely frightening nightmares. The patient can be guided past the
usual point of waking in the nightmare while in the hypnotic state and helped to discover the
benign or successful ending. Thus, the patient is assisted by the hypnotherapist to complete
the nightmare in a manner that is not fear inducing (Kingsbury, 1993). This method appears
to fit within the beliefs of many patients who view themselves as escaping their nightmares
before some horrible event befalls them. The completion of the otherwise unfinished
19
nightmare can be a method of utilising the patient’s beliefs and reframing them to allow a
sense of felt mastery (Watzlawick, 1978).
Some methods of treating recurrent nightmares are based on psychoanalysis but use hypnosis
as a means of helping the patients retrieve and process material from nightmare episodes. In
such treatments the patient may be asked to experience the nightmare under hypnosis and
then the material discovered is dealt with in regular therapy. Arluck (1964) successfully
treated a women with nightmare disorder using hypnoanalysis. Similarly, Moss (1973) used
hypnosymbolism analysis to facilitate a male inmate with nightmare disorder re-dreaming his
nightmares. The material uncovered during these psychotherapy sessions was then used to
make his nightmare more benign. Gorton (1988) treated a 57-year old women who presented
with obsessional worries, insomnia and lifelong nightmares. Gorton (1988) employed a
combination of psychodynamic psychotherapy, paradoxical re-experiencing of the
nightmares, and posthypnotic suggestion to treat the women over a period of 10 months.
According to Gorton (1988) the treatment allowed the patient to take control of her symptoms
by means of the paradoxical use of trance within a narcissistic transference that lead to the
emergence of central issues related to early childhood trauma and a series of losses of self-
objects. Finally, it was stated that an underlying ambivalent relationship with these lost self-
objects and a wish/fear dilemma with regard to death itself was then amenable to
psychotherapeutic intervention. The critical factor was having the patient practice self-
hypnosis at home to re-experience nightmares and understand the content.
The major criticism of methods that employ a combination of analytical or psychoanalytical
therapy with hypnosis to treat nightmare disorder is that they may take a long time to give the
patient any relief from the frightening nightmares. This is apparent from Gorton’s (1988)
report that stated the therapy was conducted over a 10-month period. Other hypnotic methods
of relieving nightmare sufferers reported above may be helpful even after one session.
Therefore, a case could be made for using hypnosis for the direct treatment of nightmare
disorder within any given psychological approach to therapy for other related and/or causal
issues.
Miller and DiPilato (1983) found that relaxation was just as effective as systematic
desensitisation and that both were superior to waiting list control condition in alleviating
nightmare sufferers. Their data suggests that therapies aimed at anxiety reduction in general
20
are effective in reducing the frequency of nightmares. This suggests that reduction of arousal
may be an important element in reducing the occurrence of nightmares, and that nightmares
are not necessarily enmeshed in a web of psychopathology. Therefore, hypnosis can be
employed in promoting a more relaxed sleep within the context of a given therapeutic
regimen. Korth (1964) used hypnosis with suggestions of restful, relaxed sleep and
confidence, to effectively treat a women experiencing nightmares about examinations.
Cooperman and Schafer (1983) reported treating a 56-year old female with nightmare
disorder. The patient reported the content of her nightmares centred on the loss of her family.
Hypnotic fantasy visits with her deceased husband and children were successfully used to
alleviate the nightmares. Eichelman (1985) used hypnosis with two Vietnam veterans who
had PTSD with recurring nightmares. In his work, hypnosis was used to modify the content
and outcome of the nightmares so that they took on a more benign character. The two
veterans reported a significant reduction in the intensity and frequency of their nightmares.
Jencks and Brazza (1986) used hypnosis with adult subjects to promote self-confrontation in
order to resolve nightmare content. Similarly, Kingsbury (1993) employed hypnosis with
three adults female patients. Two subjects (35 & 44 years old) had no psychopathology while
the third 26-year old patient was suffering from depression with suicidal ideation. In the
treatments, hypnosis was used to view the traumatic event in the nightmare as an observer
and thereby reinterpret the result in a more realistic manner. Marks (1978) treated a women
with nightmare disorder by employing hypnotic suggestions for the rehearsal of the
nightmare in the waking state. Seif (1985) used hypnosis to treat a man who was suffering
nightmare disorder. The man was constrained by his life situation and a high degree of
conformity to what others wanted him to do. The hypnotic treatment included suggestions
that the man was free to be himself and that he did not have to please anyone.
In summary, the use of hypnosis to treat nightmare disorder in reported studies appears to be
in most cases successful in that nightmares are either eliminated totally or at least altered to
some extent so that patients learn to cope better with the content. A variety of strategies have
been reported with some authors suggesting that the therapeutic element might centre around
reduced arousal. Other authors have suggested that cognitive elements may be important
because patients suffering from nightmares can reinterpret or directly alter nightmare content
so that it can be coped with. Finally, the use of hypnosis as an aid to uncovering material or
reinterpreting material within the context of more traditional psychotherapeutic approaches
21
also has its advocates. In examining the literature it is clear perhaps the best approach to
alleviating nightmare disorder in the short-term is an individualised and direct approach using
hypnosis. In the longer-term patients with nightmare disorder that is treated directly with
hypnosis may still require ongoing psychotherapy to assist with other symptomatology that
may or may not be causally related to nightmare disorder. The short-term alleviation of
frightening nightmares via hypnosis may help the patient to gain confidence and function at a
higher level and this, in turn, may help produce therapeutic gains.
Hypnotic Treatment of Sleepwalking & Sleep Terror Disorders
Broughton’s (1968) studies of parasomnias suggested that non-specific and chronic repressed
mental conflict associated with anxiety may result in physiological changes that alter the
individual’s arousal response during sleep and at the transitions to and from sleep. Broughton
proposed that sleepwalking and sleep terror are primarily disorders of arousal rather than
disorders of sleep per se and that they may be triggered by external stimuli. Fisher, Kahn,
Edwards and Davis (1973) have shown that this hypothesis was correct, because they were
able to demonstrate that night terror can be triggered in sleeping patients by sounding a
buzzer.
Clinical Studies of the Hypnotic Treatment of Sleepwalking and Sleep Terror Disorders
Koe (1989) noted that Broughton’s work on parasomnias suggested a possible alternative
application for using hypnosis in the treatment of sleep terror. Koe (1989) hypothesised that
limiting the perception of external stimulation at night through post-hypnotic suggestion
might reduce the occurrence of sleep terror episodes. In Koe’s (1989) case study of the
hypnotic treatment of sleep terror disorder, the patient was a 16-year old boy who had proven
resistant to other forms of treatment. Tranquillisers such as diazepam, behaviour modification
and psychotherapy all had little or no effect on the intensity and frequency of the patient’s
sleep terror episodes. The patient’s mother reported that there were only two nights when he
had not experienced sleep terror since the age of seven. The sleep terror episodes usually
began each night with repetitive, terrified, screaming and involved leaping out of bed and
running around the room. The patient was often observed to have more than one episode per
night and sometimes became violent, breaking his mothers nose on one occasion, smashing a
window while trying to run through it on another occasion, and frequently destroying objects
22
in his room. The patient’s diagnosis of sleep terror disorder was confirmed in a sleep research
laboratory. The patient was able to state that his night terror was a fear of dying in his sleep
but was not able to say how he came to have this fear.
The patient was highly embarrassed by his condition, and thus according to Koe (1989),
highly motivated to change. In the treating this patient, Koe (1989) used systematic relaxation
as a hypnotic induction and his patient achieved a depth of seven on the Long Stanford Scale
of Hypnotic Depth, indicating a deep state of hypnosis. While the patient was hypnotised, he
was given the suggestion that he was in a deep sleep, deep sleep - in the stage of sleep in
which he normally experienced sleep terror. He was then told that a sleep terror episode was
beginning. Koe observed that the patient’s respiration rate accelerated and he began to toss
and turn, but remained in a prone position, giving the appearance being asleep. After several
minutes of increased autonomic activity had passed, Koe tapped his pencil on his desk. The
patient responded to this stimulus by screaming and immediately jumping up and running
into the wall of the room. This episode of sleep terror induced under hypnosis lasted about 15
seconds.
The manner in which Koe’s patient responded to the pencil tap, suggested that external
sounds might be triggering his sleep terror episodes. When the patient returned on the
following week, Koe again hypnotised him and gave the post-hypnotic suggestion that he
would gradually become less and less aware of outside sounds and sensations while asleep.
During the subsequent week only three episodes of sleep terror where reported and only one
further episode in the following two weeks.
Koe (1989) wished to confirm that the post-hypnotic suggestions were in fact causal in the
reduction of the frequency of the sleep terror episodes. In order to achieve this aim he asked
the subject for permission to again hypnotise him and remove the suggestions. However, the
patient would not permit the removal of the post-hypnotic suggestions because he was so
pleased with the treatments effect. Therefore, Koe was unable to determine whether or not the
sleep terror episodes could be made to resume and thus was not able to establish the validity
of the hypnotic treatment. A follow-up three months later found that the patient was no longer
experiencing sleep terror and that he remained very pleased with the effect of the hypnotic
treatment.
23
According to Hurwitz, Mahowald, Schenck, Schutler and Bundlie (1991) hypnosis is an
underrated modality of therapy for individuals suffering automatised, embarrassing, and
occasionally dangerous behaviours associated with sleepwalking and sleep terror. Hurwitz
(1986) and Hurwitz and Mahowald (1988) reported the successful use of hypnosis for the
treatment of individuals with sleepwalking and sleep terror disorders. A fuller report of
subjects from these two studies and additional subjects is provided in Hurwitz et al’s, (1991)
paper. In this study hypnosis was introduced as a method of enhancing self-control. Hypnotic
trance was usually induced with the commonly used technique of suggesting eye closure
during upward gaze and subsequent relaxation and sensations of floating. Patient’s were then
asked to visualise themselves in a pleasant, comfortable scene where they would find a screen
on which they could watch a time-lapse film of themselves sleeping quietly and peacefully
through an entire night. The initial induction in the office was often recorded on
audiocassette, lasted about 20 minutes, and constituted instruction for self-hypnosis. Post-
hypnotic suggestions included suggestions for security and anxiety reduction (e.g. “your
unconscious mind even during sleep can be aware that you are safe and secure), restful sleep
with minimal movement, and the instruction that suggestions be reiterated during self-
hypnotic practice at home. The patients were instructed to practice self-hypnosis two times
each day, and that one of these sessions should be carried out just prior to retiring at night.
Follow-up sessions were scheduled for reinforcement of the hypnotic treatment and subjects
were encouraged to report their experiences by telephone. Patients were treated during one or
up to six sessions at varying intervals over a period of time.
There were a total of 27 patients (19 men and eight women) aged between 18-51 years old.
Eight had a primary diagnosis of sleepwalking disorder and four sleep terror disorder. The
other 15 patients were diagnosed with both sleepwalking and sleep terror disorders. Twenty-
two of the patients reported bruises and lacerations as a result of frequent nocturnal arousals.
The frequency of episodes ranged from one to 45 per month. Past psychiatric conditions were
noted in 45% of the patients. Three patients had history of depression, four had histories of
drug and alcohol abuse but were in long standing remission, one patient had a history of
generalised anxiety disorder, six phobic disorders, two adjustment disorder and two attention
deficit disorder. However, at the time of treatment only one patient had an active DSM-III
axis-I psychiatric disorder. This patient had panic disorder and she was later found to have a
prolapsed mitral valve. Thirteen of the patients had no psychiatric history and showed no
signs of psychiatric disorder at the time of the study. Eighteen patients had a family history of
24
parasomnia disorders. Polysomnographic studies on 17 of the patients revealed no evidence
of seizure, REM sleep behaviour disorder or other disorders that might present as sleep
terror/sleepwalking parasomnias. Eight of these patients showed a high number of arousals
from sleep during the sleep study. The number of arousals shown by these individuals was in
excess of what might have been expected as a first night effect in the sleep laboratory.
The hypnotically based treatment was helpful in 20 of the 27 patients with sleepwalking and
sleep terror disorders. The patients were followed up at substantial intervals to determine the
effectiveness of the treatment. Five patients reported complete remission, but most
emphasised the benefit derived from increased control of frenzied and ambulatory behaviours
rather than the complete elimination of the arousals. They were often aware of having
awakened, occasionally at the bedside, but perceived immediate awareness of the arousals
and returned to sleep. Six of the subjects did not continue to use self-hypnosis. The wives of
two of these patients reported that they knew that if their husbands did use self-hypnosis
when under stress it reduced their aberrant nocturnal behaviour markedly.
Dillahunt (1971) treated an adult male with sleepwalking disorder. The treatment consisted of
hypnosis with suggestions for calm sleep. Dillahunt (1973) also reported a successful single
session treatment of a middle-aged female sleepwalker. He used hypnotic suggestions that
somnambulism, like hypnotic trance is subject to self-regulation. Taboada (1975) treated a
seven-year old boy suffering acute onset of sleep terrors with one hypnotherapeutic session
that resulted in a complete remission that was maintained for 18 months. Eliseo (1975)
reported the amelioration of sleepwalking in a 19-year old man after seven sessions of
hypnotherapy. He used suggestions for sleeping calmly and immediate wakefulness if the
patent’s feet should touch the floor during sleep.
Reid (1975) described the successful treatment of four of six 17-21 year old military trainees
with serious sleepwalking using Dillahunts (1973) technique. Follow-up was limited to the
brief period of their military training, but the four trainees reported no further sleepwalking
during this period. Later, Reid, Ahmed and Levie (1981) conducted a blind crossover study
designed to evaluate specialised hypnotherapy for the treatment of severe sleepwalking
disorder. The subjects were free from significant psychiatric illness. It was hypothesised that
subjects suffering from otherwise uncomplicated sleepwalking disorder would show a decline
in sleepwalking episodes following a course of hypnotherapy designed to impart arousal cues
25
that were inconsistent with sleepwalking behaviour. Furthermore, it was also proposed that
improvement would be greater using the specialised hypnotic treatment than simple
hypnotherapy that involved suggestion alone.
Subjects were randomly assigned to either active (7 males & 2 females) or a suggestion only
group (3 males & 1 female). Groups were unequal because a lottery like assignment
procedure was employed. The design was a single-blind, rater-blind, modified cross-over
procedure. Every subject in the active group improved at the end of the three-week study
period. In the suggestion only group two subjects improved dramatically but the other two
showed no improvement. The two subjects that showed improvement elected to cross-over
into the active treatment, but showed no further improvement. One subject from the active
group elected to cross-over into the suggestion only treatment in order to make further gains,
but his symptoms remained the same. At a three month follow-up, only one subject from the
active group showed pre-treatment levels of sleepwalking. The two subjects from the
suggestion only group also remained stable with fewer sleepwalking episodes reported
compared with the pre-treatment period. The results indicated that a relatively simple, non-
invasive, inexpensive procedure could be used to alleviate sleepwalking disorder in adults. In
conclusion, a few subjects reported waking beside the bed early in the treatment phase.
However, it was more usual for the therapeutic result to generalise through some mechanism
that is not clear so that there was no sleepwalking and subsequent awakening.
Gutnik and Reid (1982) stated that 50% of the sleepwalking cases they treated responded to
hypnotic treatment in a positive manner. They used hypnosis to treat adults with sleepwalking
disorder and employed suggestions for calm sleep and alertness if the patient’s feet touched
the floor during sleep. Reid, Haffke and Chu (1984) treated 5 adults 28-56 years old with
intractable sleepwalking disorder. However, in this group of subjects, hypnosis and
psychotherapy were not effective, but some subjects responded to treatment with diazepam.
Pai (1946) reported his experiences of treating 1853 wartime neurotic male patients at the
Maudsley Hospital in London. Sleepwalking and other unusual nocturnal activities were
observed in 117 of these men. Most were differentially diagnosed with sleepwalking that was
elaborated to include anxiety, hysterical dissociation, post-infective, and hysteromalingering
states. Treatment for many included resolution of stressors and time-limited heavy sedation.
Hypnosis was used to aid understanding and treat those with hysterical-dissociative
26
underpinnings. Pai (1946) reported that hypnosis was helpful in resolving sleepwalking in
some of his patients.
Kramer (1988) used hypnosis to treat sleep terror disorder in a 10-year old boy. The boy
experienced his first night terror episode at the age of four. The onset of his sleep terror
episodes were usually about 20 minutes after falling asleep and their frequency was on
average four times per week. Six months prior to the onset of the sleep terror episodes the
boy had reportedly watched a horror movie about werewolves after which the content of his
sleep terror became feelings and vague images that he was turning into a werewolf. At the
time he presented for treatment the frequency of his sleep terror episodes had dropped to
about two each week. The content at this time was vaguely related to images of being
physically mutilated or hurt by someone. He was amnestic for each sleep terror episode.
The boy was treated using hypnosis and was able to enter a deep trance rapidly. The
induction consisted of a finger lowering technique, in which the middle two fingers were
raised and he was asked to watch his fingers as they “go to sleep”. On completion of the
induction, he was given an explanation of the nature of sleep, stage by stage. The regularity
and continual movement of cycles of sleep were emphasised. He was also given direct
suggestions for not dropping too quickly into an extremely deep stage of sleep. A follow up
session was planned and took place one week later. In the interim, he had one night terror
episode of comparatively low intensity to those experienced previously. The suggestions for
dropping off to sleep gradually and having rotating cycles of sleep were reinforced in the
second session and he had not had a recurrence at the two-year follow-up. Continued
psychotherapy was recommended for the patient, but the hypnotic treatment had allowed a
rapid resolution of the sleep terror episodes and the patient was able to get more restful sleep.
Taboada (1975) treated a seven-year old boy with persistent sleep terror disorder and found
that hypnosis was successful in alleviating the symptoms. Taboada used guided imagery to
desensitise the boy to the content of the sleep terror. Gardner and Olness (1981) treated an
11-year old boy for sleepwalking disorder using hypnotic suggestions for calm sleep. The
treatment was successful and sleepwalking episodes were largely eliminated. Gardner and
Olness (1981) treated sleepwalking and sleep terror disorders in children using hypnosis and
also taught self-hypnosis techniques to the children. Mason (1987) used hypnosis to promote
calm sleep in a five-year old girl with sleep terror. Guilleminault (1987) reported the
27
successful treatment of three adolescents, two whose parasomnias responded to the
adjunctive use of hypnosis. He suggested that children and adolescents with sleepwalking and
sleep terror disorders should be treated with a combination of psychotherapy, hypnosis and
relaxation techniques. Kohen, Mahowald and Rosen (1992) used imipramine briefly, in
conjunction with hypnosis to establish symptom control in four children aged 8-11 years old
with sleep terror disorder. A further seven children 21 months to 16 years old with sleep
terror were treated using hypnosis without concurrent medication. In general, hypnosis was
successful in decreasing the intensity and frequency of the sleep terror episodes.
In a large study Guilleminault, Moscovitch and Leger (1995) treated 28 adolescent and adult
patients with sleepwalking disorder with associated violence and a further 12 patients with
sleepwalking disorder without violence. All patients had been sleepwalking since childhood.
Patient’s age, timing of sleepwalking, sleep state preceding sleepwalking episodes and
associated sleep pathologies were similar in both groups. Increased levels of daytime stress
were associated with more frequent sleepwalking episodes. Temporal lobe lesions were found
only in the violent group. The treatment included pharmacological agents, psychotherapy,
hypnosis and treatment for sleep apnoea in some cases. It was concluded that
pharmacotherapy was the main factor responsible for reduced frequency of sleepwalking
episodes and also of reduced sleep associated violence. However, subjects that discontinued
pharmacotherapy, but maintained psychotherapy and hypnotic treatment also showed less
frequent episodes of sleepwalking and nocturnal violence.
Nugent, Carden and Montgomery (1984) reported that hypnosis was successfully used to
treat sleepwalking in adults subjects. Treatment included suggestions for calm sleep and
alertness should the patient’s feet touch the floor during the sleep state. Reid and Gutnik
(1980) employed hypnosis to suggest calm relaxed sleep. This technique proved beneficial to
the male patient who had otherwise intractable sleepwalking. Zach (1990) used hypnosis to
successfully treat adult subjects with sleepwalking disorder. Schenck and Mahowald (1995)
treated two females aged 17 and 46 years for pre-menstrual sleep terror and injurious
sleepwalking disorders using hypnosis. Both patients responded positively to calming
hypnosis applied just prior to bedtime although the 46-year old women was also given
clonazepam (.25 mg).
28
In summary, the available studies of hypnosis as a treatment for sleepwalking and night terror
episodes show that in most cases hypnosis can be used directly to either completely alleviate
or reduce the frequency and intensity of these problematic behaviours. Further studies are
required because it is not clear which aspect of hypnosis is important in successful treatments
of sleepwalking and sleep terror. Some authors have suggested that the general lowering of
tonic levels due to the anxiolytic effects of relaxation employed during hypnosis might
reduce the incidence of these disorders. However, others suggest that the individualised use
of hypnosis with suggested imagery and cognitive strategies may reinforce the acceptance of
the modality as well as its efficacy in altering behaviour. Although it is not clear how
hypnosis exerts an effect on sleepwalking and sleep terror episodes, it nevertheless represents
a relatively simple, non-invasive, inexpensive, and effective means of treating these
potentially dangerous disorders.
Aims of the Study
The aim of the case studies reported here was to apply hypnotic treatments based on ideas
derived from the literature to patients with accurately diagnosed primary parasomnias and to
follow-up the treatments to assess efficacy.
METHOD
Subjects
Subjects were selected on an opportunistic basis from among patients presenting at an
insomnia clinic at a major public hospital in Melbourne. During the period allocated for the
study three patients presented with clearly diagnosable primary parasomnias.
Procedure
Patients were interviewed and a detailed history of their past and present sleep difficulties
was obtained during the first session. In all cases, additional information was obtained from
the physicians’ referral letters. Each patient was requested to keep a sleep-diary and complete
a sleep-log over the following two-week period. The information obtained from the sleep-
diary and logs was subsequently used to clarify the diagnoses. Patients were also asked to
29
have other people living with them to record any unusual nocturnal behaviour that occurred
during the pre-treatment, treatment and post-treatment periods. However, two of the patients
were living alone at the time of the study. In two female patients the original presenting sleep
difficulty was confirmed. One of these two female patients had nightmare disorder and the
other patient sleepwalking disorder. The 30-year old patient with sleepwalking disorder was
also given an overnight diagnostic polysomnographic examination in the sleep laboratory.
However, on the particular night that the patient was examined she did not show any
behaviour consistent with sleepwalking behaviour. The 37-year old patient with nightmare
disorder was not examined in the sleep laboratory because her symptoms were consistent
with the diagnosis of nightmare disorder. The third patient was a 33- year old male who
reported symptoms consistent with sleep terror disorder. This diagnosis was confirmed by an
overnight diagnostic polysomnographic sleep study and visual observation in the laboratory
and was consistent with his reported history. The treatment protocols for each patient were
based on ideas derived from the available literature detailing the use of hypnosis for the
treatment of the primary parasomnias; nightmare, sleepwalking and sleep terror disorders.
RESULTS
Case Study 1: The Hypnotic Treatment of Nightmare Disorder
Patient
SB was a 37-year old female Keyboard Operator
Initial Interview & Patient History
During this initial interview the patient reported a history of recurring nightmares with the
theme of being chased by a masked man who intended killing her. The nightmares had begun
two years previously and coincided with a separation from her husband. The frequency of the
nightmares was three to four nights per week. The patient who now lived alone found it
difficult to return to sleep after experiencing the nightmares and as a result was suffering
from excessive daytime tiredness. The daytime tiredness was affecting her work performance
and also her social life. The patient had undertaken psychotherapy to determine the cause of
the nightmares, but this had not been helpful in alleviating the nightmare condition. The
30
patient was referred for treatment by her physician after requesting medication for insomnia.
The patient had no other medical conditions and was reported to be in good health by her
physician.
The nature of the nightmare the patients reported was as follows: She hears a noise and
realises that someone, a man, is breaking into her flat. She flees out the back door of the flat,
but the man who is large and dressed in all black clothing chases her. She can see that the
man is carrying a large knife and is terrified that he is going to kill her. She never sees the
man’s face because he is wearing a black stocking mask. The nightmare always begins the
same way and she runs but cannot to get away from the man. Eventually, she is exhausted
and cannot run any further her heart is pounding as the man approaches threateningly with
the knife raised. At this point she screams but no one is there to help her. She really feels that
she is going to be killed and awakens frightened and shaking. After several minutes she turns
on the light and checks all the doors and windows to make sure that they are locked. Her
hearing is hypersensitive and she finds it difficult to return to sleep after the nightmare
episode even though she is aware that it was just a dream and that her flat is secure.
During the interview the patient presented a pleasant women who was embarrassed by her
condition. She had no memory of having nightmares as a child and believed that her present
circumstances were the causal factor. These circumstances revolved mainly around the fact
that she was recently separated from her husband of 10 years and that for the first time in her
life she was living alone. She expressed concerns about security and recounted examples of
crimes committed against women who were living alone that had been reported in the local
newspapers. In following up on these concerns, she was able to rationalise that such events
were random, and that she was living in a group of flats that were very secure. The patient
had no history of psychopathology and there were no indications of psychopathology evident
during the initial interview. The presenting problem was recurring nightmares and insomnia
due to the sleep disturbance caused by the nightmares. The content of the nightmares did not
seem to be causing any daytime problems, apart from the fact that she was sensitive to issues
of personal security to a degree that could have been characterised as over concern. The
patient was requested to keep a sleep diary and complete a sleep log over the following two
weeks.
Treatment Program
31
Session 1 (2 weeks later)
The frequency of nightmare episodes was confirmed by the diary and sleep log that were
completed during the two weeks since the last session. Nightmares had occurred on three
nights during one week and four nights the following week. The theme of the nightmares was
almost identical on each occasion and was consistent with the nightmare the patient outlined
during the initial interview.
Rationale for the use of hypnosis: Given that she was suffering from insomnia due to inability
to sleep soundly after having been awakened by recurring nightmares and that her physician
had not recommended sedative medication, it was suggested that hypnosis might be a useful
treatment. It was proposed that hypnosis might allow the patient to have control over her own
dream content and that hypnosis could also be used to promote sleeping calmly and soundly
each night.
Explanation of hypnosis: Hypnosis was explained as a state of consciousness different to the
normal waking state. It was also explained that the role of the hypnotherapist was to assist the
patient to enter hypnosis, to guide the patient while under hypnosis and to ensure that the
patient was safely brought out of hypnosis.
Assessment of hypnotisability: There were no contraindications for hypnosis. The Creative
Imagination Scale (Wilson & Barber, 1973) was used to introduce the patient to hypnotic
phenomena and indicated that she was highly hypnotisable.
Session 2 (1 week later)
The patient reported experiencing only two nightmares during the intervening week. The
theme of these three nightmares was the same as that reported previously.
Hypnosis: The induction and deepening procedure were tape-recorded and the patient was
instructed to play the tape each night just prior to bedtime. The induction consisted of the
Speigel eye-roll technique followed by suggestions for progressive relaxation of all the
muscles in the body and sensations of floating. When the patient achieved a deep level of
32
trance, it was suggested to her that since it was her nightmare she could control the outcome
in anyway she wished. It was suggested that she could bring other people into the nightmare
to help her or that she could use her intelligence to either escape or trap the man who was
chasing her. For example, it was suggested that if she ran in a different direction to the one in
which she usually ran that she would come to deserted carnival (like Luna Park) with a hall
of mirrors (cf.. Kingsbury, 1993). If she lured the man into the hall of mirrors he would be
confused by all the images of her and she could escape locking the door behind her.
It was then suggested that she replay her nightmare as if it was on a TV screen and make a
change to the ending that allowed her to have complete control. At the end of the session the
patient was asked to report what changes she had made to the nightmare ending. She reported
that she enjoyed changing the ending of the dream and that she had lured the pursuer to a hall
of mirrors at Luna Park and had locked him inside. She then reported that she had called the
police and that he had been taken away and locked up.
The patient was instructed to use the techniques on the tape recording as a guide for
practising self-hypnosis.
Session 3 (1 week later)
The patient reported that for the first few nights after the hypnosis session she had no
nightmares and felt a lot more relaxed during both the day and at night. However, as the week
went by she experienced the nightmare on one night. The nightmare was not as intense as it
had been before and she noted that she had a feeling of control and that she was able to
change the nightmare so that the man who chased her was not able to catch her. The patient
listened to the taped hypnosis session each night and reported no further nightmares during
that week.
Hypnosis: A further session of hypnosis was tape-recorded. The induction and deepening
procedure used was essentially the same as that used in the previous session. However,
suggestions for coping with any difficulties and setbacks in the progress of treatment
implementation were included towards the end of the tape.
Session 4 (1 week later)
33
The patient reported that she had experienced no further nightmares with the theme that had
troubled her for the past two years. However, she reported having some dreams that made her
anxious but that she was able to cope with these by employing similar techniques to the ones
she had used to make the original nightmare harmless.
Hypnosis: A similar session of hypnosis was carried out to the last one except that there were
suggestions for general well-being and coping with difficulties made towards the end of the
session. This session was also tape-recorded. The patient was encouraged to practice self-
hypnosis each day and to use the tapes of the hypnosis sessions. This was the last scheduled
treatment session and it was mutually agreed that there was no need for any further sessions
at this stage. It was agreed that the patient would contact the clinic again if she found that her
nightmares returned.
Treatment Follow-up
During the three months after treatment ended follow-up telephone calls were made every
two weeks. The patient reported that she had not experienced any nightmares with the
original theme during this period. Nevertheless, she did have some dreams that with
frightening themes but the intensity of these was much less than the original nightmare. The
patient reported that she had continued to use the hypnosis tapes and practice self-hypnosis
and that this was also helping her to feel more relaxed and cope better with her life situation
and work-related stress.
Case study 2: The Hypnotic Treatment of Sleepwalking Disorder
Patient
CH was a 30-year old female Solicitor.
Initial Interview & Patient History
The patient sought advice from her physician regarding her sleepwalking behaviour. She was
given a medical examination and was found to be in excellent physical health. She was
34
subsequently referred to the insomnia clinic for further investigation. The physician’s referral
letter gave no indications of any other medical or psychiatric conditions and concluded that
the patients might have parasomnia. Sedative medication was suggested, but the patient was
unwilling to take medication at that stage.
During this interview the patient presented as a well groomed, pleasant and intelligent young
women. There were no indications of psychopathology and no psychological concerns or
issues which might have been contributory to the patient’s episodes of nocturnal arousal. The
patient was referred for an overnight diagnostic test in the sleep laboratory. The subsequent
report from the overnight diagnostic test indicated that on the test night the patient had a
normal nights sleep and no sleepwalking behaviour was observed. Further nights of testing
could have been performed but this was unwarranted at this stage because the diagnosis was
consistent with sleepwalking disorder. An examination of the patient and her overnight sleep
record by a respiratory medicine specialist revealed no respiratory conditions that might have
been responsible for triggering the sleepwalking episodes.
The patient reported having sleepwalking episodes for as long as she could remember. Her
parents also confirmed that she had usually had at least one or two episodes of sleepwalking
each week throughout her childhood. The patient was not disturbed by her sleepwalking
because she rarely ventured far from her bed and usually had no memory of the episodes in
the morning. In most instances she returned to bed and went quietly back to sleep. Sometimes
she thrashed about while in bed rather than leaving the bed and wandering around. The
patient noted that she tended to experience more episodes of sleepwalking when she had been
mentally or physically activated or aroused just prior to going to bed.
The patient was involved in basketball and karate and often trained or played in the evening.
When she returned on these nights, she retired to bed soon after arriving home. On these
nights she often had episodes of sleepwalking. Similarly, when she worked in the evening
preparing material for the next day she often experienced sleepwalking episodes on these
nights. The episodes were brief (1-5 minutes) and usually occurred only once per night and
on average three times per week. The patient had never previously been concerned by her
sleepwalking because she had no associated nightmares or sleep terror, no memory of the
events and had never harmed herself or anyone else during the episodes. The reason the
patient gave for presenting at this stage of her life was that her partner’s sleep was being
35
disturbed by her sitting up in bed and moving around or getting out of bed a walking around
the room. A recent event where she thrashed about in bed had resulted in her partner
sustaining a black-eye. Her partner was understanding of her condition and was used to her
nocturnal wandering, but the patient felt that she should try to do something about the
behaviour because of the injury she had caused her partner.
The patient was informed that hypnosis might be a helpful in minimising her episodes of
sleepwalking. The patient agreed that she would be willing to try this in preference to taking
sedative medication. The patient was asked, with the assistance of her partner, to keep a sleep
diary and sleep log during the two weeks that followed.
Treatment Program
Session 1 (2 weeks later)
The data from the sleep dairy and sleep logs confirmed that during the 14 day period the
patient was observed to sleepwalk or thrash about in bed on six nights. On the nights when
sleepwalking episodes occurred there was a positive correlation with the level of physical
and/or mental activity the patient engaged in prior to retiring in the evening. The patient had
previously noted that her sleepwalking was worse on nights that she played sport or worked
in the evenings.
Rationale for the use of hypnosis: Given that the patient showed no other symptomatology,
that she was seeking treatment of her sleepwalking disorder for the sake of her partner’s sleep
and that she was unwilling to consider sedative medication, hypnosis was suggested as a
treatment for her condition.
Explanation of hypnosis: Hypnosis was explained as a state of consciousness different to that
of the normal waking state. It was further explained that the role of the hypnotherapist was to
assist the patient to enter hypnosis, to guide the patient whilst under hypnosis and to ensure
that the patient was safely brought out of hypnosis.
36
Assessment of hypnotisability: There were no contraindications for hypnosis and The
Creative Imagination Scale (Wilson & Barber, 1973) indicated that the patient was
moderately to highly hypnotisable.
Session 2 (1 week later)
The patient reported that she had one sleepwalking episode during the week. This episode
was noted by her partner and she was amnestic for the event.
Hypnosis: The induction and deepening procedure were tape-recorded and the patient was
instructed to use the tape each night just prior to bedtime. The induction consisted of
progressive muscular relaxation with suggestions for slipping more and more deeply into a
hypnotic trance. When a deep level of trance was achieved, suggestions were given for
sleeping very calmly and that if she should get out of bed as soon as her feet touched the floor
she would wake up immediately and be fully alert but return to bed and quickly return to
sleep (cf.. Eliseo, 1975; Reid, 1975; Hurwitz et al, 1991). The patient was also encouraged to
use self-hypnosis in the evening to assist her to be more relaxed prior to retiring.
The patient was instructed, if possible, to avoid vigorous sporting activity (karate &
basketball) late in the evenings as it was noted previously that there was a positive correlation
between the level of activity in the evening and sleepwalking episodes. If the patient could
not avoid playing sport or working late in the evening she was instructed to allow herself
sufficient time to relax before going to bed. During this time it was suggested that she might
use the recorded tape from the hypnosis session, practice self-hypnosis or sit quietly and
listen to some relaxing music.
Session 3 (1 week later)
The patient reported that she had not experienced any episodes of sleepwalking during the
intervening week. This was also confirmed by her partner who was not disturbed by her
activity on any night.
Hypnosis: The induction and deepening procedure used in this session were similar to that
used in the previous session.
37
Session 4 (1 week later)
The patient reported no nocturnal arousal during the week and again this was confirmed by
her partner.
Hypnosis: The induction and deepening procedure was identical to that used in the previous
sessions except that additional suggestions for calm, deeply relaxed sleep were added towards
the end of the tape recorded session. This was the last treatment session scheduled and as the
patient had experienced very few sleepwalking episodes since the treatment began it was
agreed that no further session were required at this stage.
Treatment Follow-up
The patient was followed-up over a five-month period by telephone calls every three weeks.
During this period there was a marked decrease in the frequency of sleepwalking episodes
from three per week to about one episode every three weeks. The patient also reported that
when she did not allow herself sufficient time to relax in the evening after sport or work
activities before retiring she was more likely to sleepwalk or move about in bed. The patient
reported that if she used the hypnosis tapes, self-hypnosis or relaxed for some time after
playing sport or working in the evening she generally did not have a sleepwalking episode.
Case Study 3: The Hypnotic Treatment of Sleep Terror Disorder
Patient
GS was 33-year old male Secondary School Teacher
Initial Interview & Patient History
The patient was referred by his physician for episodes of nocturnal arousal. The physician’s
referral letter stated that the patient complained of waking in terror on one or two nights per
week and that a physical examination had not revealed any medical condition that might
account for these episodes of arousal. The physician noted that the patient appeared to be
38
highly distressed by the recent breakdown of his marriage and the subsequent separation from
his wife and children.
The patient presented as an agitated individual with little insight into the problems that were
troubling him with regards to his marital separation, issues related to his parents, his present
career and earlier professional sporting career. In addition, he was overly focused on various
somatic complaints, taht he believed stemmed from his sleep disturbance. The patient was
referred on for counselling for his relationship difficulties because it was not possible to
address these issues fully within the context of the insomnia clinic.
The patient reported a history of sleep terror episodes that had begun at about the age of 12.
His parents were able to confirm that the sleep terror episodes had begun when he was 12
years old and that on average he experienced one to two episodes each week, with some
weeks where there were none. At the time he presented for treatment he was experiencing
one to two sleep terror episodes per week. These episodes were described as occasions were
he screamed out in a terrified manner and often jumped up from his bed and ran into another
room. Sometimes if he woke shortly after an episode he had a vague feeling that someone
was coming into his room to harm him but otherwise he had no recall of the episodes. When
he was 16 years old during one of these sleep terror episodes he had jumped through his
bedroom window, shattering the glass and landing on the front lawn. During this incident he
sustained only minor cuts and abrasions but was fearful of hurting himself more severely in
the future. Attempts to calm him during episodes of nocturnal arousal, either by his parents or
his wife while they were living together, were met with resistance and if he woke on these
occasions he was confused and disoriented. In the more recent months since he separated
from his wife he had experienced one or two episodes of the arousals each week. After most
episodes of sleep terror the patient reported waking usually confused and disoriented and then
having difficulty returning to sleep. What had prompted him to seek help was the fact that he
had smashed an internal glass door at his parent’s house while experiencing a sleep terror
episode.
The patient was referred for overnight diagnostic testing and observation in the sleep
laboratory. The results of the tests and observation of the patient confirmed the diagnosis of
sleep terror disorder. During the diagnostic test the patient aroused with terrified screaming
early in the night from stage 3/4 sleep. Attempts by the sleep laboratory technicians to calm
39
the patient were met with resistance and when he finally woke he was confused and
distressed. At this point, which was early in the night, the patient wanted to leave, but was
eventually convinced to stay for the rest of the night. Sleep during the rest of the night was
characterised by frequent awakenings but no further sleep terror episodes. Subsequent
examination of the patient’s sleep record and a physical examination by a respiratory
specialist revealed no respiratory anomalies that might have triggered the sleep terror
episodes.
The patient was requested to complete a sleep diary and sleep log during the two weeks until
the next session.
Treatment Program
Session 1 (2 weeks later)
The sleep diary and log showed that the patient had experienced four sleep terror episodes
that he was aware of during the two-week period since the initial interview.
Rationale for the use of hypnosis: The patient’s diagnosis of night terror was confirmed by
the diagnostic tests and observations conducted in the sleep laboratory. Medical examination
by the attending respiratory medicine specialist revealed no respiratory events that might be
acting as triggers for the sleep terror episodes. The patient was unwilling to take medication
that had been suggested by the respiratory physician and therefore hypnosis was suggested as
treatment that might allow him to sleep more calmly and soundly each night.
Explanation of hypnosis: Hypnosis was explained as a state of consciousness different to the
normal waking state. It was also explained that role of the hypnotherapist was to assist the
patient to enter hypnosis, to guide the patient whilst under hypnosis and to ensure that the
patient was brought safely out of hypnosis.
Assessment of hypnotisability: There were no contraindications for hypnosis and The
Creative Imagination Scale (Wilson & Barber, 1973) indicated that the patient was low to
moderately hypnotisable.
40
Session 2 (1 week later)
The patient reported being aware of experiencing two sleep terror episodes during the week.
After each event the patient awoke in a confused state and had considerable difficulty
returning to sleep.
Hypnosis: The induction and deepening procedure were tape-recorded and the patient was
instructed to play the tape each night just prior to bedtime. The induction procedure consisted
of progressive muscular relaxation with suggestions for sinking deeper and deeper into a
hypnotic trance. Despite showing only a low to moderate degree of hypnotisability on The
Creative Imagination Scale the patent achieved a deep level of trance. Suggestions were
given for sleeping very calmly and not dropping into deep sleep too quickly. It was further
suggested that patient would cycle through all the stages of sleep very calmly. It was also
suggested that if he should find his feet on the floor he would wake immediately, be fully
alert and return to bed and fall asleep rapidly (cf.. Kramer, 1988). The patient was also
instructed to use the techniques worked through in the tape recording to practice self-
hypnosis each day.
Session 3 (2 weeks later)
The patient reported that he was aware of experiencing one episode of sleep terror after
which he awakened and was not able to return to sleep.
Hypnosis: A further session of hypnosis was recorded which reiterated the same suggestions
as were given in session two. The induction and deepening procedure used was essentially
the same as that used in the previous session. Suggestions for coping with any difficulties and
setbacks in the progress of treatment implementation were included towards the end of the
tape.
Session 4 (1 weeks later)
The patient reported no experiences of sleep terror that he was aware of during the last week.
41
Hypnosis: This session followed the same format as the previous session with reinforcement
of the suggestions for calm sleep, not dropping into deep sleep too quickly and for alertness if
his feet touched the floor.
Session 5 (1 week later)
The patient reported no episodes of sleep terror that he was aware of during the week.
Hypnosis: The hypnosis session followed essentially the same format as that of the last
session.
Treatment Follow-up
The patient was followed up at intervals of three weeks over a six-month period. The follow-
up indicated that the intensity and the frequency of sleep terror episode dropped dramatically.
The pre-treatment level had been one to two episodes per week. During the follow-up period
frequency of sleep terror episodes was approximately one every three weeks. The patient
reported that he tended to experience episodes when he was agitated about his marital
situation or work, particularly if he did not use the hypnosis tape or self-hypnosis techniques
that he had been taught.
DISCUSSION
Hypnotic Treatment of Nightmare Disorder
Nightmares are frightening dreams accompanied by moderate levels of autonomic activity
and arousal and in contrast with sleep terror, the level of autonomic activation observed in
nightmares, is less and usually results in a complete awakening. Recall of nightmare content
is usually reasonably complete. Studies indicate that many people remember having
nightmares at an early age, but that the frequency of nightmares declines between the ages of
seven to 11 years old (Hartmann et al, 1987). Nightmares usually do not become a matter of
concern unless they are recurrent, disrupt sleep, or are anxiogenic (Erman, 1987).
In the present study, the case of SB presented a classic example of an individual suffering
from nightmare disorder. This patient was troubled by recurrent nightmares over a period of
42
two years. Nightmares are often caused by traumatic events in the life of the individual.
These events can be real traumatic events, as in the case of war veterans where nightmares
have been discussed as a definitive feature of PTSD (Ross et al, 1989). There are fewer
reports of repetitive nightmares featuring in non-traumatic conditions such as the anxiety and
depressive disorders. However, many people experience transient nightmares in association
with life events that cause high levels of stress (Celucci & Lawrence, 1978b; Kales et al,
1980; Garfield, 1987). Individuals under conditions of acute stress (e.g. examinations) may
experience nightmares , but these usually abate when the acute stress is resolved. However, in
some individuals nightmares may continue over a more extended period of time and come to
constitute nightmare disorder. Whether the threatening situation that caused the nightmares to
begin was real or was simply perceived as real makes little difference. For the patient SB in
this study the anxiety resulting from having to live alone after the separation from her
husband, coupled with her tendency towards over concern about issues of personal security
lead to the development of recurrent nightmares. Psychological factors are believed to play a
major role in the development and persistence of nightmares (Kales et al, 1980) and research
has suggested that anxiety is associated with nightmares (Celucci & Lawrence, 1978b).
The fear that resulted from the recurrent nightmares in SB’s case lead to secondary symptoms
that were centred around sleep deprivation. SB was unable to return to sleep on three to four
nights per week and experienced chronic insomnia. The insomnia caused by SB’s persistent
fear following the nightmare episodes resulted in considerable daytime somnambulance.
Daytime tiredness was interfering with SB’s work performance, social life and probably
reduced her ability to cope with her general life situation. It was the inability to sleep
following nightmare episodes rather than the nightmares themselves that lead SB to seek
medical treatment.
Nightmares can be treated with sedative hypnotic drugs derived from the benzodiapines and
brand names include medications such as Normison and Valium. However, drug treatment is
only warranted if the nightmares cause very severe sleep disturbance. In the case of SB her
sleep disturbance was judged by her physician not to be severe enough warrant treatment
with sedative hypnotic drugs. In cases such as that of SB, where nightmares are chronic, but
do not severely disrupt the patient’s daytime performance the prescription of sedative
hypnotic cannot be justified. Although sedative drugs can relieve the suffering caused by
recurrent nightmares they can exacerbate daytime somnambulance due to the hangover
43
effects. In addition, short-term usage is recommended and longer-term use often results in
chronic dependency.
There is a tendency for clinicians to focus on nightmares as symptomatic of underlying
psychopathology, which in many cases is a correct assumption to make. There are a variety
of reasons for why clinicians focus on treating other symptomology rather than directly
addressing nightmares. One reason is that historically, dreams have been viewed as a
valuable source of information in psychoanalytic approaches to the treatment of
psychopathology. Prior to seeking medical treatment, SB underwent lengthy a course of
psychotherapy in which the content of her nightmares was explored in relation to her
childhood and her marital separation. However, psychotherapy did not alleviate the suffering
that the nightmares were causing SB. The focus on nightmares as symptomatic of underlying
psychopathology can in fact needlessly prolong patients’ distress. Distress can be worsened
by fatigue due sleep loss, and anxiety and depression can increase as a result of nightmare
themes intruding into patient’s waking cognitions. Patients may interpret the persistence of
nightmares as evidence that psychotherapy is ineffective and be further demoralised (Frank &
Frank, 1991). In SB’s case these factors, particularly sleep deprivation, were affecting her
ability to cope with daily tasks at work and home.
Behavioural therapies, systematic desensitisation and relaxation training have been shown to
be effective in reducing the intensity and frequency of recurrent nightmares (Geer &
Silverman, 1967; Shorkey & Himle, 1974; Celucci & Lawrence, 1978a; Schindler, 1980;
Miller and DiPilato, 1983). Hypnosis has also been used to directly treat nightmare disorder
in children and adults (Marks, 1978; Moss, 1973; Seif, 1985; Tart, 1966; Kingsbury, 1993). A
number of mechanism through which hypnotic treatment may exert an effect have been
outlined. Under hypnosis nightmare content is more involving for the patient than under
normal psychotherapy giving the patient greater access to the emotional state associated with
the nightmare which is therapeutically useful (Gilligan, 1988). The hypnotic induction marks
a separate state from normal consciousness and heightens the expectation that it has the
power to change what seems to be non-volitional behaviour (Combs & Freedman, 1990; van
der Hart, 1993). Research and theory support a link between hypnosis and dreams via
proposed dissociative mechanisms (Gabel, 1989, 1990). Finally, the available clinical
literature indicates that hypnosis can be effectively used to either terminate or control the
44
content of recurrent nightmares (Kingsbury, 1993; Marks, 1978; Moss, 1973; Seif, 1985;
Tart, 1966).
With respect to the hypnotic treatment used in the case of SB there are several elements
mentioned above which were central to treatment. The technique used to treat SB was based
on one reported by Kingsbury (1993). In this technique the patient (SB) was hypnotised and
instructed that if she wished, she could change any detail of her nightmare. For example, she
could include other people who would help her in the nightmare in some way, or she could
use her intelligence to fool the person chasing her and therefore gain the advantage. After
giving SB examples of how she could make changes to the nightmare, she was asked to
replay the nightmare on a movie or TV screen which she could control. This method was
employed so that SB would be somewhat removed from the nightmare and thus feel safe and
in control of the situation. The patient was told that she could make changes to the nightmare
content as she viewed it on the screen. She was given the following example to illustrate the
kind of changes she could make to have control over the outcome of the nightmare. “You run
in a different direction to the one in which you usually run when being chased and eventually
end up in a place like Luna Park with a hall of mirrors. Inside the hall of mirrors the man that
is chasing you is confused by all the images of you and this allows you to escape. When you
escape you lock the man inside the hall of mirrors and know you are safe from harm.” Later
SB reported that she had enjoyed making changes based on the above story which resulted in
a safe ending for her nightmare. Subsequently, in her own home she was asked use the tape
recordings of the hypnosis session and to practice altering the nightmare content. She was
also encouraged to do the same thing under self-hypnosis. The technique adapted from
Kingsbury (1993) engages the patient to make cognitive changes to the nightmare by editing
it in way that makes it less threatening. The technique uses the projection of the nightmare on
some kind screen as a way of safely distancing the patient from it so it is viewed from an
observers perspective and in a controlled manner. In replaying the dream, there is also the
element of looking at the dream from an observers view point rather than as a participant.
This may afford the patient more insight into the fact they can actually have control over
some or all the elements in the nightmare. Having felt-control has been reported to be an
important factor in having mastery over nightmares (Watzlawick, 1978; Gorton, 1988).
In the present study SB was able to make changes to her nightmare under hypnosis so that it
was transformed in manner that allowed her to feel safe. The generalisation of the changes
45
from the hypnotic session to her home setting was relatively rapid with few nightmares
experienced at the level of intensity she had previously reported. It appeared that once the
patient gained insight into the fact she had the power to change the nightmare outcome, the
apparent hold of the nightmare was broken.
There were also other non-specific benefits of hypnosis and the acquisition of self-hypnosis
which are difficult to quantify given that this was a case study. These benefits seemed to flow
from the fact that associated with the hypnosis and self-hypnosis, were effects which were
best described as generalised relaxation effects. That is, the patient felt more relaxed both
during the day and at night. These effects could also have been due the fact that, concomitant
with the decline in nightmare frequency, there was also an improvement in sleep and
consequently energy levels. Increased levels of energy and concentration probably allowed
SB to cope more effectively with the challenges of the altered circumstances of her life and
work-related pressures. The effects of relaxation should not be under estimated in the
hypnotic treatment as relaxation processes without trance have been show to be effective in
decreasing the intensity and frequency of nightmares (Miller & DiPilato, 1983). However,
the advantage of hypnotic treatment over simple relaxation techniques is that additional
cognitive processes designed to alter nightmare content and outcome can also be
simultaneously introduced to allow the patient to have some degree of control almost
immediately. The effect of giving a person who has suffered very frightening nightmare even
a small degree of control should not be underestimated. In SB’s case it was apparent after the
first therapeutic session of hypnosis that she had gained a sense that she could control
nightmare content and outcome. This small gain, for the long suffering patient like SB, can be
a major boost for their moral and can increase self-confidence, which may, in turn, lead to
further therapeutic gain.
In summary, hypnotic treatments designed to treat recurrent nightmares are best
conceptualised in terms of the specific and the general effects hypnosis has on the patient.
Specific effects are those that result from direct hypnotic suggestion/instruction to the patent
involving transforming the nightmare (Eichelman, 1985; Gilligan, 1988), changing the
ending of the nightmare, interrupting the nightmare (Gorton, 1988), acquiring mastery of the
nightmare, exposure in less involving contexts (desensitisation) (Kingsbury, 1993), using
controlled imagery (Kingsbury, 1993), solving the nightmare (Kingsbury, 1993), and
reframeing the nightmare and incorporating it in some way (Watzlawick, 1978). General
46
effects of hypnosis which may not necessarily stem from direct instruction include; increased
feelings of relaxation, unconscious processing of material, improved cognitive function,
improved sleep and the secondary effects which might flow from this.
In conclusion, involving the patient in directly editing or making cognitive changes to
projected imagery of recurrent nightmare scripts can play an important role in hypnotic
techniques designed to treat nightmare disorder. The inclusion of cognitive processing is not
necessary because non-specific hypnotic-relaxation techniques are also effective. However,
generalised techniques without specific focus may take longer to be effective. Techniques
designed to focus specifically on the content and make changes seem to allow very rapid
treatment of the nightmare disorder. In addition, once a patient has these skills they can be
deployed to deal with other anxiety provoking dreams or nightmares which may arise at some
future time. In the case of SB, she found that she was able to use the general technique of
editing the nightmare to change other anxiety provoking dreams. Therefore, based on the
literature and the results of the case study of SB, direct hypnotic treatment employing
techniques designed to quickly alter nightmare content is a highly appropriate approach to the
treatment of nightmare disorder. A further factor which appeared to be critical was having the
patient practice hypnosis/self-hypnosis at home to re-experience nightmares and learn to
make the content more benign.
Hypnotic Treatment of Sleepwalking & Sleep Terror Disorders
It is important that parasomnias like sleepwalking and sleep terror disorders are correctly
diagnosed and that effective treatment is given if the associated behaviours are potentially
injurious, violent or disruptive to either the patient or other individuals. Sleepwalkers are
usually difficult to communicate with and if left alone often return to bed. When awakened,
they have little memory of anything that happened during the episode. Kales et al, (1966)
found that few sleepwalking incidents were related to specific traumatic events. Sleepwalking
generally occurs about an hour or two after falling asleep. In most cases, sleepwalking begins
with a burst of high voltage, slow frequency EEG activity, and is related to arousal from stage
3/4 NREM sleep (Kales et al, 1966; Broughton, 1968).
In the case of CH the primary diagnosis was sleepwalking disorder. This patient had a long
history of sleepwalking behaviour which began in childhood. The patient sought medical
47
treatment for her sleepwalking disorder because it was disrupting her partners sleep. The
patient herself was not aware of sleepwalking on most occasion and this is entirely consistent
with the diagnosis of sleepwalking disorder. The patient CH was offered medical treatment
for her sleepwalking in the form of sedative medication, but she was not willing to commence
drug therapy. The main problem with drug therapy for sleepwalking is the long-term nature
of the disorder and the adverse effects chronic drug usage may have on behaviour and
learning (Weissbluth, 1984).
Studies have shown that patients with sleepwalking disorder can learn to self-regulate
previously uncontrolled nocturnal behaviours by teaching them techniques such as relaxation
and mental imagery and self-hypnosis. Reid et al, (1981) compared hypnosis designed to
impart arousal cues inconsistent with sleepwalking behaviour to non-specific hypnotic
treatment. They found that both treatments were effective, but that the specialised technique
was superior. Reid et al’s study showed that a relatively simple, non-invasive, inexpensive
procedure could be used to alleviate sleepwalking disorder in adults. They found that a few
subjects reported waking beside the bed early in the treatment phase which was consistent
with what was expected in accordance with the post-hypnotic suggestions. Nevertheless, it
was more usual for the therapeutic result to generalise through some mechanism which was
not clear so that there was no sleepwalking and subsequent awakening.
The hypnotic treatment employed with the patient CH incorporated three main ideas which
been reported in the literature, relaxation, posthypnotic suggestions for calm sound sleep and
for alertness if the feet touched the floor during the night. The hypnotic treatment was
designed to promote relaxation in CH in the evenings before she retired to bed. CH had
previously reported that there was a positive correlation of her arousal level in the evening
and with the frequency of her sleepwalking episodes. When CH played sport or worked late
in the evening the likelihood of her having a sleepwalking episode was increased. Therefore,
it was also suggested that she avoid playing sport or working too late in the evenings or if she
could not avoid these activities to take time to relax before retiring to bed. Methods of
relaxing included using taped hypnosis sessions, practising self-hypnosis or simply listening
to some relaxing music. The second part of the hypnotic treatment incorporated post-hypnotic
suggestions for calm, sound sleep and alertness if the feet touched the floor during the night.
48
In line with what other researcher (e.g. Reid et al, 1981) have reported in the case of CH the
most effective element in the hypnotic treatment was not the post-hypnotic suggestions for
alertness if the feet touched the floor during sleep, but the reduced levels of tonic arousal
produced via the relaxation elements included in hypnosis sessions, the taped hypnosis
sessions and in self-hypnosis/relaxation practised in vivo. This conclusion was partly based
on the feedback CH gave about the correlation between high levels of mental and/or physical
activity and episodes of sleepwalking. CH who was heavily into sport found that on evenings
when she played sport late there was an increased likelihood of sleepwalking. Similarly,
when CH worked late at her office or at home on demanding mental tasks there was also an
increased chance of her sleepwalking. Mahowald and Rosen’s (1990) model of factors which
are likely to influence the frequency and intensity of parasomnias like sleepwalking includes
tonic arousal. Therefore, treatments which are likely to alter the level of tonic arousal
(i.e.lower it) are more likely to be helpful in stopping sleepwalking. The was probably no
specific factor which was central to the decrease in the frequency of sleepwalking episodes.
The factors most likely to have decreased the level of sleepwalking are twofold. Firstly, the
instruction to reduce activity in the evening and make sure that CH was sufficiently relaxed
before going to bed (behavioural change) and secondly the use of hypnosis tapes and self-
hypnosis in the evening prior to bedtime to lower tonic arousal levels. Thus, the main effect
from the hypnotic treatment was probably increased relaxation.
Sleep terror disorder is best described as nocturnal episodes of extreme terror and panic
which occur early in sleep when the person is in stage 3/4 deep NREM sleep (Hartmann,
1984; Kales et al, 1982). Sleep terror disorder is not as common as nightmare disorder.
Episodes of sleep terror usually last only a few minutes and the subject has little or no recall
of the event. Sleep terror disorder can be distinguished from nightmare disorder by clinical
features and sleep-laboratory findings. Sleep terror and sleepwalking have similar clinical
characteristics and many individuals experience both (Hartmann et al, 1982; Kales et al,
1982; Oswald & Evans, 1985; Vela-Bueno et al, 1987). Sleep terror disorder frequently
begins before the age of 10 and usually abates during adolescence (Kales & Kales, 1974).
In the present study the patient GS showed a history of symtomatology which was consistent
with a diagnosis of sleep terror. This diagnosis was subsequently confirmed by an overnight
polysomnographic sleep study. During the early part of the night when the patient was in
deep NREM sleep he aroused in a state of extreme terror. The attempts made by the sleep
49
laboratory technicians to comfort GS were met with resistance which is a typical reaction of
patients experiencing a sleep terror episode. Eventually, when GS awoke he was confused
disorientated and had to be convinced to remain in the sleep laboratory for the rest of the
night. The patient had no detailed recall of the mental processes that had lead to this episode
of sleep terror. However, he was able to state that he had a vague feeling that someone was
entering the room to get him.
Sleep terror can be triggered in susceptible individuals by auditory stimulation during slow
wave sleep (Fisher et al, 1970; Fisher et al, 1973). In many individuals and in the majority of
children arousals appear to be spontaneous. This is evidenced by the fact that sleepwalking
can be induced readily in children by standing them up during slow wave sleep (Kales et al,
1966; Broughton, 1968). Similarly, sleep terror can be triggered in susceptible individuals by
auditory stimulation during slow wave sleep. Both these observations suggest that these
behaviours are not the result of ongoing complex mental activity during sleep (Fisher, Byrne,
Edwards & Kahn, 1970; Fisher, Kahn Edwards, Davis & Fine, 1973).
The induction and deepening procedure used to treat GS consisted of progressive muscular
relaxation with suggestions for sinking deeper and deeper into a hypnotic trance. When the
patient was in trance he was given suggestions for sleeping calmly and not dropping too
quickly into deep sleep. It was also suggested that he would cycle through all the stages of
sleep very calmly. Finally it was suggested that he would wake and be alert should his feet
touch the floor during the night and that after this he would return to bed and fall asleep
rapidly. These suggestions were modelled on suggestions used by Kramer (1988) in his case
study report of hypnotic treatment of a 10 year old boy with persistent sleep terror disorder.
The patient GS was assessed as low to moderately hypnotisable using The Creative
Imagination Scale. However, GS was able to achieve a deep level of trance during the formal
induction for treatment of his sleep terror disorder. During the subsequent weeks GS reported
that the frequency of his sleep terror episodes declined so that during the follow-up period he
reported very few episodes of night terror. There were no occasions where GS reported being
awakened during a night terror episode. This observation is further supported by the fact that
on the few occasion where he did experience sleep terror after the commencement of the
hypnotic treatment program, when he awakened during the episode he was confused and
disorientated. Therefore, post-hypnotic suggestions for alertness if his feet touched the floor
during a sleep terror episode were not the effective element in the hypnotic treatment
50
program. Over the period of treatment sleep terror episodes declined markedly in frequency
and also to some extent in intensity. Patients in Hurwitz et al’s (1991) study were given
similar post-hypnotic suggestions for calm sleep and wakefulness if their feet touched the
floor during a sleep terror episode. However, these authors reported that there was a
generalised effect with less intense and frequent sleep terror episodes reported. Few patients
actually reported being alerted during an episode. Some patients reported complete remission,
but most emphasised the benefit derived from increased control of frenzied and ambulatory
behaviours rather than the complete elimination of the arousals. Six of the subjects did not
continue to use self-hypnosis. The wives of two of these patients reported that if their
husbands used self-hypnosis when under stress it reduced their aberrant nocturnal behaviours
markedly. The patient GS reported that he was more likely to experience sleep terror episodes
when he did not use the taped hypnosis sessions or self-hypnosis.
When onset of these sleep disorders occurs after age 10, they are likely to persist in
adulthood, episodes are more frequent, the time of their onset is often associated with major
life stress events, and they seem to be related to psychological factors. The patient GS was
distressed by psychological factors which seemed to mostly stem from his background and
current marital problems. Since the break down of his marriage he had reported more
frequent episodes of night terror. The patients psychological problems in combination with
his poor sleep habits and insomnia were probably interacting to cause increased arousal
during the night. Mahowald and Rosen (1990) proposed a model for understanding the
determinants and manifestations of sleep terror. Their model suggests that at the end of a
period of deep NREM sleep the individual suffering sleep terror may become ‘caught’ -
unable to completely get out of deep sleep, unable to arouse fully, and unable to move into
the next sleep cycle. Factors which determine the occurrence of partial arousals are: (1) tonic
sleep factors; (2) phasic sleep factors; and (3) the behavioural response to the arousal
(Mahowald & Rosen, 1990). The tonic factors which occur throughout the sleep period
determine the individual’s underlying sleep pattern and arousal threshold. In the case of GS
three factors which may have increased his level of tonic arousal were; sleep deprivation,
chaotic sleep/wake scheduling, and psychological problems.
The mechanism underlying the decrease in the intensity and frequency of sleep terror
episodes after hypnotic treatment are not well understood. However, it is possible that the
main factor contributing to the success of hypnotic treatments is a change in underlying tonic
51
sleep factors resulting in a higher arousal threshold and hence fewer arousals. Therefore, the
non-specific general factors in hypnosis which lead to the patient being more relaxed may be
important. In other studies (e.g. Hurwitz et al, 1991) the hypnotic treatment effect was
reported to generalise so that patients had few and/or less intense sleep terror episodes. In the
present study there was no evidence that specific post-hypnotic suggestions had any effect on
sleep terror as the patient did not report being alerted when experiencing a sleep terror
episode. In fact GS noted that when he did not use the hypnosis tapes or practice self-
hypnosis he was more likely to have sleep terror episodes. Research has shown that
relaxation training can decrease the occurrence sleep terror episodes. Therefore, the elements
of hypnosis that lower tonic arousal during sleep may be important therapeutic element in the
treatment.
In conclusion, studies of sleepwalking and sleep terror episodes employing hypnotically
based treatments indicate that in many cases this form of treatment may be used to directly to
either eliminate or reduce the intensity and frequency of these problematic behaviours.
Further studies of hypnotic treatments for these disorders are required because it is not clear
which aspect of hypnosis is important in successful treatments of sleepwalking and sleep
terror. The present cases studies of CH and GS support the observations of other authors (e.g.
Hurwitz, 1991) that suggest the general lowering of tonic levels due to the anxiolytic effects
of relaxation employed during hypnosis might reduce the incidence of these disorders.
However, others authors (e.g. Koe, 1989; Kingsbury, 1993) have suggested that the
individualised use of hypnosis with suggested imagery and a cognitive strategies may
reinforce the acceptance of the modality as well as its efficacy in altering sleepwalking and
sleep terror behaviours. Although it is not really clear how hypnosis exerts an effect on
sleepwalking and sleep terror episodes, it is nevertheless is a relatively simple, non-invasive,
inexpensive, and effective means of treating these potentially dangerous disorders.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I wish to thank Susan Hook, for her optimistic encouragement and expert advice during the
course of this project. I would also like to thank my fellow ASH members; John Redman, Dr.
John White and Dr. Allison Weber who also provided support and valuable comment on this
project.
52
REFERENCES
Abe K, Amatoni M & Oda N (1984) Sleepwalking and recurrent sleeptalking in the children
of childhood sleepwalkers. American Journal of Psychiatry 141: 800-801
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
(1994) Edition 4. Washington, American Psychiatric Association
Arkin AM & Antrobus JS (1991) The effects of external stimuli applied prior to and during
sleep on sleep experience. In Ellman SJ & Antrobus JS (Eds.) The mind in sleep: Psychology
and psychopharmacology. Wiley, New York
Arluck E (1964) Hypnoanalysis: A case study. New York: Random House
Association of Sleep Disorders Centers: Diagnostic Classification of Sleep and Arousal
Disorders. (1979) Edition 1. Prepared by the Sleep Disorders Classification Committee, HP
Roffwarg, Chairman. Sleep 2: 99-121
Barber TX, Walker PC & Hahn KW (1973) Effects of hypnotic induction and suggestions on
nocturnal dreaming and thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 82: 414-427
Belicki D & Belicki K (1982) Nightmares in a university population. Sleep Research 11: 116
Belicki K & Belicki D (1986) Predispositions for nightmares: A study of hypnotic ability,
vividness of imagery, and absorption. Journal of Clinical Psychology 42: 714-718
Belicki K, Altray H & Hill C (1985) Varieties of nightmare experience. Association for the
Study of Dreams Newsletter 2: 1-3
Berlin RM & Qayyum U (1986) Sleepwalking: Diagnosis and treatment through the life
cycle. Psychosomatics 27: 755-781
Bishay N (1985) Therapeutic manipulation of nightmares and the management of neuroses.
British Journal of Psychiatry 147: 67-70
Bixler EO, Kales A, Soldatos CR, & Healy S (1979) Prevalence of sleep disorders in the Los
Angeles metropolitan area. American Journal of Psychiatry 136: 1257-1262
Bootzin RR, Kihlstrom JF & Schacter DL (Eds.) (1990) Sleep and cognition. American
Psychological Association, Washington DC
Broughton RJ (1968) Sleep disorders: Disorders of arousal. Science 159: 1070-1078
Brylowski A (1990) Nightmares in crisis: Applications of lucid dreaming techniques.
Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa 15: 79-84
Celucci AJ & Lawrence PS (1978a) The efficacy of systematic desensitization in reducing
nightmares. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 9: 109-114
53
Celucci AJ & Lawrence PS (1978b) Individual differences in self-reported sleep variable
correlations among night-mare sufferers. Journal of Clinical Psychology 34: 721-725
Cooperman S & Schafer D (1983) Hypnotherapy over the telephone. American Journal of
Hypnosis 25: 277-279
Combs G & Freedman J (1990) Symbol, story & ceremony: Using metaphor in individual
and family therapy. Norton, New York
Dillahunt D (1971) In A Handbook of therapeutic suggestions. American Society of Clinical
Hypnosis - Education and Research Foundation
Dillahunt D (1973) Sleepwalking. In American Society of Clinical Hypnosis Education and
Research Foundation (Ed.) A syllabus on hypnosis and a handbook of therapeutic
suggestions. Des Plaines, IL, American Society of Clinical Hypnosis, pp 95
Dunn KK & Barrett D (1988) Characteristics of nightmare subjects and their nightmares.
Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa 13: 91-193
Eccles A, Wilde A & Marshall WL (1988) In vivo desensitization in the treatment of
recurrent nightmares. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 19: 285-288
Eichelman B (1985) Hypnotic change in combat dreams of two veterans with posttraumatic
stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 142: 112-114
Eliseo T (1975) The hypnotic treatment of sleepwalking in an adult. American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis 17: 272-276
Ellman SJ & Antrobus JS (Eds.) (1991) The mind in sleep: Psychology and
psychopharmacology. Wiley, New York
Erikson MH (1959) Further clinical techniques of hypnosis: Utilization techniques. American
Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 2: 3-21
Erman MK (1987) Dream anxiety attacks (nightmares). Psychiatric Clinics of North America
10: 667-674
Feldman MJ & Herson M (1967) Attitudes toward death in nightmare subjects. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology 72: 421-425
Feldman MJ & Hyman E (1968) Content analysis of nightmare reports. Psychophysiology 5:
221
Fisher C, Byrne J, Edwards A & Kahn E (1970) A psychophysiological study of nightmares.
Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association 18: 747-782
Fisher C, Kahn E & Edwards A (1973) The psychophysiological study of nightmares and
night terrors. Archives of General Psychiatry 28: 252-259
54
Fisher C, Kahn E, Edwards E & Davis DM (1973) A psychophysiological study of
nightmares and night terrors: The suppression of stage 4 night terrors with diazepam.
Archives of General Psychiatry 28: 252-259
Fisher C, Kahn E, Edwards E, Davis DM & Fine J (1973) A psychophysiological study of
nightmares and night terrors: III. Mental content and recall of stage 4 night terrors. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease 157: 75-98
Flemenbaum A (1976) Pavor nocturnus: A combination of a single daily tricyclic or
neuroleptic dosage. American Journal of Psychiatry 133: 570-572
Frank JD & Frank JB (1991) Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of psychotherapy.
Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore
Frankel F & Orne M (1976) Hypnotizability and phobic behavior. Archives of General
Psychiatry 33: 1259-1261
Gabel S (1989) Dreams as a possible reflection of a dissociated self-monitoring system.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 177: 560-568
Gabel S (1990) Dreams and dissociation theory: Speculations on beneficial aspects of their
linkage. Dissociation 3: 38-47
Gardner G & Olness K (1981) Hypnosis and hypnotherapy with children. Grune & Stratton,
New York
Garfield P (1987) Nightmares in the sexually abused teenager. Psychiatric Journal of the
University of Ottawa 12: 93-97
Geer JH & Silverman I (1967) Treatment of a recurrent nightmare by behaviour-modification
procedures: A case study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 72: 188-190
Gilligan SG (1988) Symptom phenomena as trance phenomena. In Zeig JK & Lankton SR
(Eds.) Developing Ericksonian therapy: State of the art. Brunner/Mazel, New York, pp. 327-
352
Gorton GE (1988) Lifelong nightmares: An eclectic treatment approach. American Journal of
Psychotherapy XLII: 610-618
Guilleminault C (1987) Obstructive sleep apnoea in children. In Guilleminault C (Ed.) Sleep
and its disorders in children. Raven Press, New York, pp 213-224
Guilleminault C (1989) Sleepwalking and night terrors. In Kryger MH, Roth T & Dement
WC (Eds.) Principles and practice of sleep medicine. Saunders, Philadelphia, pp 379-384
Guilleminault C, Moscovitch A & Leger D (1995) Injury, violence and nocturnal wanderings.
American Journal of Forensic Psychiatry 16: 33-46
Gutnik B & Reid W (1982) Adult somnambulism: Two treatment approaches. Nebraska
Medical Journal 67: 309-312
55
Halliday G (1982) Direct alteration of a traumatic nightmare. Perceptual and Motor Skills 54:
413-414
Halstrom T (1972) Night terror in adults through three generations. Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavica 48: 350-352
Hartman E, Falke R, Russ D, & Oldfield (1981) Who has nightmares? Persons with lifelong
nightmares compared with vivid dreamers and non-vivid dreamers. Sleep Research 10: 171
Hartmann E & Russ D (1979) Frequent nightmares and the vulnerability to schizophrenia:
The personality of the nightmare sufferer. Psychopharmacology Bulletin 15: 10-12
Hartmann E (1965) The D-state; A review and discussion of studies on the psychological
state concomitant with dreaming. New England Journal of Medicine 273: 30-35, 87-92
Hartmann E (1966) Reserpine: Its effects on the sleep-dream cycle in man.
Psychopharmcologia 9: 242-247
Hartmann E (1983) Two case reports: Night terrors with sleepwalking. A potentially lethal
disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 171: 503-505
Hartmann E (1984) The nightmare: The psychology and biology of terrifying dreams. Basic
Books, New York
Hartmann E, Greenwald D & Brune P (1982) Night terrors-sleepwalking: Personality
characteristics. Sleep Research 11: 121
Hartmann E, Russ D, Oldfield M, Sivan I & Cooper S (1987) Who has nightmares? The
personality of the lifelong nightmare sufferers. Archives of General Psychiatry 44: 49-56
Hartmann E, Russ D, Van Der Kolk B, Falke R & Oldfield M (1981) A preliminary study of
the personality of the nightmare sufferer: Relationship to schizophrenia and creativity.
American Journal of Psychiatry 138: 794-797
Hartmann E, Skoff B, Russ D & Oldfield M (1978) The biochemistry of the nightmare:
Possible involvement of dopamine. Sleep Research 7: 186
Haynes SN & Mooney DK (1975) Nightmares: Etiological, theoretical and behavioral
treatment considerations. Psychological Record 25: 225-236
Hernsen M (1971) Personality characteristics of nightmare sufferers. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Diseases 153: 27-31
Hobson JA (1988) The dreaming brain. Basic Books, New York
Huapaya LVM (1979) Seven cases of somnambulism induced by drugs. American Journal of
Psychiatry 136: 985-986
56
Hurwitz T & Mahowald M (1988) Further experience with hypnosis in the treatment of
somnambulism/pavor nocturnus in adults. Sleep Research 17: 190
Hurwitz T (1986) Treatment of somnambulism and pavor nocturnus in adults with hypnosis.
Sleep Research 15: 131
Hurwitz TD, Mahowald MW, Schenck CH, Schutler JL & Bundlie SR (1991) A retrospective
outcome study and review of hypnosis as treatment of adults with sleepwalking and sleep
terror. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 179: 228-233
Jencks B & Brazza G (1986) Hypnotic self-confrontation to resolve unpleasant dreams.
Paper presented at the 28th Annual Scientific meeting of the American Society of Clinical
Hypnosis, Seattle, WA
Jones E (1951) On the nightmare. Liveright, New York
Kales A & Kales JD (1974) Sleep disorders: Recent findings in diagnosis and treatment of
disturbed sleep. New England Journal of Medicine 290: 487-499
Kales A, Paulson MJ, Jacobson A & Kales JD (1966) Somnambulism: Psychophysiological
correlates. Archives of General Psychiatry 14: 586-594
Kales A, Soldatos CR & Kales JD (1987) Sleep disorders: Insomnia, sleepwalking, night
terrors, nightmares and enuresis. Annals of Internal Medicine 106: 582-592
Kales A, Soldatos CR, Bixler EO, Ladda RL, Charney DS, Weber G & Schweitzer PK (1980)
Hereditary factors in sleepwalking and night terrors. British Journal of Psychiatry 137: 111-
118
Kales A, Soldatos CR, Caldwell AB, Charney DS, Kales JD, Markel D & Cadieux R (1980)
Nightmares: Clinical characteristics and personality patterns. American Journal of Psychiatry
139: 1197-1201
Kales A, Soldatos CR, Caldwell AB, Kales JD, Humphrey FJ, Charney DS & Schweitzer PK
(1980) Somnambulism: Characteristics and personality patterns. Archives of General
Psychiatry 37: 1406-1410
Kales A, Tan TL, Preston TA & Allen C (1970) Stage 4 sleep: Studies of hypnotic,
tranquillizing and antidepressant drugs. Psychophysiology 7: 342-343
Kales JD, Cadieux RJ, Soldatos CR & Kales A (1982) Psychotherapy with night-terror
patients. American Journal of Psychotherapy 36: 399-407
Kales JD, Kales A, Soldatos CR, Chamberlin K & Martin ED (1979) Sleepwalking and night
terrors related to febrile illness. American Journal of Psychiatry 136: 1214-1215
Kales JD, Soldatos CR, Caldwell AB, Charney DS & Martin ED (1980) Nightmares: Clinical
characteristics and personality patterns. Archives of General Psychiatry 37: 1413-1417
57
Karacan I, Wolff SM, Williams RL, Hursch CJ & Webb WB (1968) The effects of fever on
sleep and dream patterns. Psychosomatics 9: 331-339
Kavey NB, Whyte J, Resor S & Gidro-Frank S (1987) Classification and treatment of
somnambulism. Sleep Research 16:368
Kellner R, Neidhardt J, Krakow B & Pathak D (1992) Changes in chronic nightmares after
one session of desensitization or rehearsal instructions. American Journal of Psychiatry 149:
659-663
Kingsbury SJ (1988) Hypnosis in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder: An
isomorphic intervention. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 31: 81-90
Kingsbury SJ (1992) Strategic psychotherapy for trauma: Hypnosis and trauma in context.
Journal of Traumatic Stress 51: 85-96
Kingsbury SJ (1993) Brief hypnotic treatment of repetitive nightmares. American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis 35: 161-169
Klackenberg G (1982) Somnambulism in childhood - prevalence, course and behavioral
correlations. Acta Paediatrica Scandanavica 71: 495-499
Koe GG (1989) Hypnotic treatment of sleep terror disorder: a case report. American Journal
of Clinical Hypnosis 32: 36-40
Kohen DP, Mahowald MW & Rosen GM (1992) Sleep terror disorder in children: The role of
self-hypnosis in management. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 34: 233-244
Korth L (1964) The healing sleep. Heath Science Press, Rustington, England
Kramer M & Kinney L (1988) Sleep patterns in trauma victims with disturbed dreaming.
Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa 13: 12-16
Kramer M, Schoen LS & Kinney D (1987) Nightmares in Vietnam veterans. Journal of
American Academy of Psychoanalysis 15: 67-81
Kramer RL (1988) The treatment of childhood night terrors through the use of hypnosis: A
case study: Brief communication. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis 37: 283-284
Kupfer DJ & Bowers MB Jr. (1972) REM sleep and central monoamine oxidase inhibition.
Psychopharmacologia 27: 183-190
Lester D (1968) The fear of death of those who have nightmares. Journal of Psychology 69:
245-247
Lester D (1969) Fear of death and nightmare experiences. Psychological Reports 25: 437-438
58
Luchins DJ, Sherwood PM, Gillin JC, Mendelson WB & Wyatt RJ (1978) Filicide during
psychotropic-induced somnambulism: A case report. American Journal of Psychiatry 135:
1404-1406
Mahowald MW & Rosen GM (1990) Parasomnias in children. Pediatrician 17: 21-31
Marks I (1978) Rehearsal relief of a nightmare. British Journal of Psychiatry 133: 461-465
Marshall JR (1975) The treatment of night terrors associated with the posttraumatic stress
syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry 132: 293-295
Mason RO (1987) Educational uses of hypnotism: A reply to Prof. Lightner Witmer’s
editorial in Pediatrics for January 1, 1987. Pediatrics 3: 97-105
Miller WR & DiPilato M (1983) Treatment of nightmares via relaxation and desensitization:
A controlled evaluation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 51: 870-877
Moss C (1973) Treatment of a recurrent nightmare by hypnosymbolism. American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis 16: 23-30
Nadel C (1981) Somnambulism, bed-time medication and overeating. British Journal of
Psychiatry 139: 79
Nugent WR, Carden NA & Montgomery DJ (1984) Utilizing the creative unconscious in the
treatment of hypodermic phobias and sleep disturbance. American Journal of Clinical
Hypnosis 26: 201-205
Oswald I & Evans J (1985) On serious violence during sleep-walking. British Journal of
Psychiatry 147: 688-691
Pai M (1946) Sleep-walking and sleep activities. British Journal of Psychiatry 92: 756-765
Pesikoff RB & Davis PC (1971) Treatment of pavor nocturnus and somnambulism in
children. American Journal of Psychiatry 128: 778-781
Reid W & Gutnik B (1980) Case report: Treatment of intractable sleepwalking. Psychiatric
Journal of the University of Ottawa 5: 86-88
Reid W (1975) Treatment of somnambulism in military trainees. American Journal of
Psychotherapy 29: 101-106
Reid W, Ahmed I & Levie C (1981) Treatment of sleepwalking: A controlled study.
American Journal of Psychotherapy 35: 27-37
Reid WH, Haffke EA & Chu CC (1984) Diazepam in intractable sleepwalking: A pilot study.
Hillside Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 6: 49-55
Rogers CR (1957) The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change.
Journal of Consulting Psychology 21: 95-103
59
Ross RJ, Ball WA, Sullivan KA & Caroff SN (1989) Sleep disturbance as the hallmark of
posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 146: 697-707
Schenck CH, Milner D, Hurwitz TD, Bundlie SR & Mahowald MW (1989) A
polysomnographic and clinical report on sleep related injury in 100 adult patients. American
Journal of Psychiatry 146: 1166-1173
Schenck CH & Mahowald MW (1995) Two cases of premenstrual sleep terrors and injurious
sleepwalking. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetric Gynaecology 16: 79-84
Schindler FE (1980) Treatment by systematic desensitization of a recurring nightmare of a
real life trauma. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 11:53-54
Seif B (1985) Clinical hypnosis and recurring nightmares: A case report. American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis 27: 166-168
Shorkey C & Himle DP (1974) Systematic desensitization treatment of a recurring nightmare
and related insomnia. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 5: 97-98
Stutman R & Bliss E (1985) Posttraumatic stress disorder, hypnotizability, and imagery.
American Journal of psychiatry 142: 741-743
Taboada EL (1975) Night terrors in a child treated with hypnosis. American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis 17: 270-271
Tart CT & Dick L (1970) Conscious control of dreaming: I. The posthypnotic dream. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology 76: 304-315
Tart CT (1966) Some effects of post-hypnotic suggestion on the process of dreaming.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 14: 30-46
van der Hart O (1993) Rituals in psychotherapy: Transition and continuity. Irvington, New
York
Van Der Kolk B, Blitz R, Burr W, Sherry S & Hartmann E (1984) Nightmares and trauma: A
comparison of nightmares after combat with lifelong nightmare veterans. American Journal
of Psychiatry 141: 187-190
Vela-Bueno, A., Soldatos, C. R., & Julius, D. A. (1987) Parasomnias: Sleepwalking, night
terrors and nightmares. Psychiatric Annals 17, 460-465.
Walker, P. C., & Johnson, R. F. Q. (1974) The dream influence of presleep suggestions on
dream content: Evidence and methodological problems. Psychological Bulletin 81, 362-370
Watzlawick, P. (1978) The language of change: Elements of therapeutic communication.
New York, Norton.
Weissbluth M (1984) Is drug treatment of night terrors warranted? American Journal of
Diseases in Children 138: 1086
60
Wilson, S. C., & Barber, T. X. (1973) The creative imagination scale as a measure of
hypnotic responsiveness: Applications to experimental and clinical hypnosis. The American
Journal of Clinical Hypnosis. 20, 235-243.
Wood, J. M., & Bootzin, R. R. (1990) The prevalence of nightmares and their independence
from anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 99, 64-68.
Zach, G. A. (1990) Hypnosis, Part II: theories and structure. Compendium 11, 360-364.
61
Table 1. Mahowald & Rosen’s (1990) Classification of Parasomnias
Primary sleep parasomnias
A. NREM parasomnias
1. Sleep starts
2. Disorders of arousal (Sleep Terror & Sleepwalking Disorders (DSM-IV))
3. Sleep drunkenness
B. REM parasomnias
1. Dream anxiety attacks (Nightmare Disorder (DSM-IV))
2. Hypnogogic hallucinations and/or sleep paralysis
3. REM sleep behaviour disorder
C. Non-sleep stage specific parasomnias
1. Bruxism
2. Enuresis
3. Rhythmic movement disorder
4. Periodic movements of sleep
5. Posttraumatic stress disorder
6. Somniloquy (sleeptalking)
Secondary sleep parasomnias
A. Central nervous system
1. Seizures
2. Headaches
B. Cardiopulmonary
1. Sleep-related arrhythmias
2. Nocturnal asthma
3. Miscellaneous
4. Sleep apnoea
C. Gastrointestinal
1. Gastroesophageal reflux
2. Diffuse oesophageal spasm
D. Miscellaneous
1. Panic attacks
2. Nocturnal muscle cramps
3. Psychogenic dissociative states
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
An adult male with chronic destructive, virtually intractable sleepwalking symptoms was treated with a hypnotic conditioning paradigm as part of a larger study. He did not respond adequately, and some months later low h.s. doses of diazepam were instituted. The response was dramatic, positive, and has lasted almost one year. There has been no indication of relapse or of tolerance to the diazepam. The authors hypothesize that the medication helps to stabilize and normalize sleep patterns. There is no evidence to indicate that the antianxiety potential of the drug is addressing the sleepwalking symptom.
Article
The effectiveness of systematic desensitization in ameliorating nightmares was evaluated in a controlled study. Twenty-nine subjects were assigned to either systematic desensitization, a nightmare discussion placebo, or continuous self-recording. The placebo treatment did not differ from desensitization in terms of rated logicalness and potential effectiveness. However, the desensitization group showed a significantly greater reduction in the frequency of nightmares as well as a decrease in rated intensity. Moreover the desensitization subjects also had a significantly greater decrease in reported fear and state anxiety. Related research on nightmares is noted and future research needs are discussed.
Article
Since the authors were inspired by the life and work of Milton H. Erickson and Gregory Bateson, they begin, in Section I on "Fundamentals," with overviews of Ericksonian psychotherapy and the Batesonian perspective. Then they show how to use metaphor in six basic therapeutic strategies: developing a relationship, gathering information, suggesting ideas, accessing and utilizing resources, reframing, and facilitating new patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behavior. In Section II, "How to Construct Metaphors," readers will find a compendium of practical exercises designed to develop flexibility, creativity, and confidence in thinking metaphorically. Specific guidelines are offered to help therapists develop strategies for creating symbols, stories, and ceremonies. In Section III, "The Therapeutic Interaction," the authors give their ideas on how to set up a context in which to work metaphorically and how to deliver metaphors effectively. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Reviews psychological attempts to understand dreams from a dissociative perspective. Characteristics of several dream theories are discussed, and it is argued that dreams as now understood in some psychoanalytic and nonpsychoanalytic orientations may also be understood within the framework of dissociation theory. The study of dreams may be enriched by or contribute to greater development of dissociation theory in many areas, including developmental psychology and psychobiology, brain–behavior relationships, and learning theory. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Discusses the evaluation and treatment of parasomnias. Characteristics of parasomnias are described, along with causative factors and differential diagnosis. Sleepwalking and night terrors are considered to be disorders involving impaired arousal, occur during slow-wave sleep, and are more common in children than adults. Nightmares occur during REM sleep, and recall is usually more vivid than sleepwalking or night terrors. Suggested treatment for sleepwalking or night terrors in children involves protection from self-injury. Treatment for nightmares involves reassurances that nightmares are a normal part of childhood and will be outgrown. It is noted that parasomnias in adults usually reflect underlying psychopathology. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)