Content uploaded by Jean R Clark
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Jean R Clark on Nov 02, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
49
NEUROSCIENCE IMPLICATIONS OF USING
HYPNOANALYSIS: TWO CASE STUDIES
Jeanne Clark*
Aurora University, Aurora, Illinois, USA
Abstract
Neuroscience is providing growing evidence that psychopathology is related to neural integration and
amygdala-based implicit memories. Understanding the brain provides psychotherapists a guide for
treatment, and neuroscience points to hypnosis as a potent treatment tool. Hypnosis affects functional
areas of the brain, including the amygdaloid memory system and neural integration. Hypnosis in itself is
not curative. Neuroscience, more recent psychotherapy models, and modern understandings of
hypnoanalysis appear to confirm Freud’s early observations of hypnosis effectiveness and, for his
objection that hypnosis works well for some clients but not for others, explain why. Neuroscience’s
understanding of the development of psychopathology and psychotherapy models give us tools and
models Freud did not have. Using age regression (abreaction) in hypnosis in the context of
psychotherapy appear to provide direct access to amygdala-based implicit memories during which the
content of implicit memories can be explored for the unconscious meaning and for the responses
conditioned during traumatic events. With the skilled use of psychotherapy treatment models while a
client is in hypnosis, new patterns can be created through neurologically vividly experiences. These
vivid experiences create new, reconditioned patterns that appear to reflect direct changes in the
amygdala-based implicit memories and neural integration, evidenced by clients’ new, automatic
responses in previously difficult circumstances. Two case studies illustrate the use of Ego Psychology,
Self Psychology, Attachment and Narrative psychotherapy models during hypnotic age regressions to
disconnect stimulus-response patterns in a phobia client and provide vivid experiences of attachment
and guidance for a borderline client.
Key words: Hypnosis; Hypnoanalysis; Age regression; Implicit memory; Phobia; Borderline personality disorder
1. INTRODUCTION
Psychopathology can be theoretically organized from an understanding of the brain in order
to devise an approach to treating the problem that is most likely to improve a client’s or
patient’s functioning (Moore & Tasso, 2012, p.717; Yapko, 2010, p.555). In the context of
neuropsychology of mental illness this article reviews some basic psychotherapeutic
implications of brain development and the effects of trauma on the brain, especially
neuroscience findings indicating that psychological problems may be understood as
imbalance in neural integration and conditioning in the amygdala mediated memory system
*Correspondence to: Jeanne Clark, e-mail: dreamswork45@yahoo.com
Received March 19, 2015; accepted May 11, 2015; Act Nerv Super 57(1), 49-58; ISSN-1802-9698
ANS: Journal for Neurocognitive Research
CASE STUDY
………………………...
Journal
Homepage:
www.activitas.org
………………………...
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
50
(Cozolino, 2010, p.34). The author locates the discussion historically addressing Freud’s early
understanding of hypnosis and refuting his reasons for abandoning the use of hypnosis, using
neuroscience to demonstrate why using hypnosis in psychotherapy appears to be effective.
Two case studies are presented to illustrate the catalyzing effect of hypnosis on the
psychotherapeutic concepts found in Self Psychology, Attachment, Narrative, and Ego
Psychology, using them in novel ways to directly edit the beliefs, processes, and stimulus
response in implicit memories and to affect functional responses with a phobia and a
borderline personality disorder client.
2. NEUROSCIENCE RELEVANT TO PSYCHOTHERAPY
Recent findings indicate the crucial role stressful and traumatic memories play in human
development. Those memories are mainly consolidated in the amygdaloid memory system,
and are the cause of many mental disorders (Cozolino, 2010, p.9). The amygdala memory
system represents the implicit memory system. The hippocampal system develops into the
explicit memory system. The amygdaloid and hippocampal systems serve different functions
as the brain continues to develop. The amygdala system is mainly subconscious, emotional
and focused on behavioral patterns, whereas the hippocampal system is organized by
language and focuses on visual images, narrative, and the conscious organization of
experience (Cozolino, 2010, p.78; LeDoux, 1996).
Recent research findings indicate that during highly traumatic experiences the amygdala’s
response overwhelms the hippocampus. The traumatic sensory information forms into highly
stable, slow-to-degrade implicit memory, dissociated from working memory or conscious
cognition (LeDoux, 2003, p.124) and from other ways the brain has for processing information
(Freyd, 1998, pp.99-101). These memories have the ability to overwhelm conscious thought
processing because the human brain has stronger neural connections from the emotional
systems up to the cortex than the rational systems have down to the amygdala (LeDoux, 1996,
p.19). Because of plasticity, implicit memories can be changed and the capacity to think more
clearly can be developed, all of which has the effect of changing the brain (Cozolino, 2010,
pp.336-338).
Recent neuroscience findings are showing increasingly that hypnosis, in many cases, may
be a highly effective tool. However the field of hypnosis has not yet capitalized on this
information (Vermetten & Bremner, 2004, p.281; Clark, 2014; Franklin, 1837; Gauld, 1992).
Freud was attracted to hypnosis by the work of Charcot (Gauld, 1992, p.306) and, with Breuer,
used hypnosis early in his practice for the treatment of hysteria (Nash & Barnier, 2012. p.208).
In Freud’s early work Studies on Hysteria, written in 1895, he recognized that pathogenic
memories were the cause of hysterical symptoms. While memories of normal events were
consciously available, these traumatic memories were not, or were available only in a
summary form without powerful affect. These memories, some decades old, would return
with “hallucinatory clarity” with all their “affective coloring” and with the “full force” of
recent memories when under the influence of hypnosis. Release of the “strangulated affect”
through speech and restoration to conscious memory or removal by suggestion was thought
to be curative (Freud, 2010).
Near the end of Freud’s writings we find this statement: “Hypnotic influence seemed to be
an excellent instrument for our purposes. . . . No substitute for hypnosis has yet been found”
(Freud, 2010). Also, recent findings indicate that in many cases hypnosis allows clients to heal
rapidly using their own innate resources, without the difficult side effects of medication and
other modalities (Barabasz & Watkins, 2005).
Following this historical context, neuroscience is offering us mounting proof that the effects
Freud achieved using hypnosis are real and related to measurable brain activities (Barnier &
McConkey, 2003; Faymonville et al., 2003; Kelliher, 2005). Research indicates hypnosis may
significantly influence brain structures that need quieting and those that need enhancing
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
51
(Halsband, Mueller, Hinterberger, & Strickner, 2009). Brain structures involved in implicit
memory, including the amygdala and thalamus and also other structures, such as
somatosensory, visual, and auditory cortexes, may be influenced by hypnosis (Barnier &
McConkey, 2003; Faymonville et al., 2003; Hoeft, Gabrieli, Whitfield-Gabrieli, et al., 2012;
Müller, Bacht, Prochnow, Schramm, & Seitz, 2013; Vanhaudenhuyse, Laureys, & Faymonville,
2014). For example, the amygdala is highly associated with the processing and conditioning of
fear, mating, eating and drinking and aggression (LeDoux, 2007). Some data also suggest that
hypnosis may influence cerebral functional connectivity (Faymonville et al., 2003).
Functions typically linked to neural connectivity on the level of implicit memory are
stimulus response and procedural information we automatically access and act on without
conscious thought or decision. These are conditioned responses about which Raz commented
in an interview discussing his research (Raz, Fan, & Posner, 2005). Using hypnosis, Raz said
we can “. . . 'un-ring' Pavlov's bell. This seems to be a top-down effect that may be able to
override a whole range of impulses" (Kelliher, 2005). In this context certain data may help
explain how traumatic stimulus-response memories can be processed and reconsolidated,
which may be closely linked to specific changes in brain complexity (Bob et al., 2006, 2009;
Bob, 2007) and in this process the new associations replace the fearful ones (Siegel, 2006).
3. USING HYPNOSIS IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
Hypnosis can affect somatosensory, visual and auditory cortices, the amygdala, left and right
hemispheres, and the prefrontal cortex (Barabasz & Barabasz, 2012; Bentall, 2000; Hoeft F et
al., 2012; Moore & Tasso, 2012; Müller et al., 2013), which does not mean that hypnosis by
itself is curative (Yapko, 2010). The effect is highly dependent on the nature of suggestions
given and the experiences that are created (Tasso & Perez, 2012). Various descriptions of
hypnotic and hypnoanalytic treatment methods are available (Brown & Fromm, 1986; Steven
J. Lynn, Rhue, & Kirsch, 2010; Nash & Barnier, 2012; Scott, Sr., 1996; Watkins, 1992). One of the
most important predictors of a psychotherapy outcome is a positive therapeutic relationship
(Cozolino, 2010, p.336). The experience of hypnosis promotes many of the most important
aspects of therapeutic relationships--warmth, trust, comfort, security--and lowers defenses
with the therapist (Barber, 2012), which indicates that hypnosis can be used in a variety of
ways in psychotherapy (Barabasz & Watkins, 2005; Bryant, 2012; Covino & Pinnell, 2010;
Elkins & Perfect, 2008; Steven Jay Lynn, Kirsch, Barabasz, Cardena, & Patterson, 2000; Nash,
2012; Peebles, 2012; Scott, Lagges, LaClave, Nash, & Barnier, 2012). The specific approach
taken within the hypnoanalytic framework is based on historic origins of symptoms that are
rooted in implicit memory of traumas that occurred before the symptom appeared (A. Freud,
1951, p, 487; Scott, Sr., 1996).
4. CASE STUDIES
4.1. Case study of phobia
M., a 34-year-old Caucasian male, came to therapy because of an intense phobia regarding
injury. He reported during intake that he was a construction worker; minor injuries were not
unusual in his job. He was avoiding a favorite hobby, camping, and was starting to avoid
work because his phobia was getting worse.
He had his first major panic attack while watching a first aid video in the Army Reserves.
He reported being cut with a pocketknife at Boy Scout camp when he was 8. He reported that
he received a cut on his head at birth. He was also born with an arm deformity that required
that he be in a brace for the first year of his life. He reported supportive, healthy relationships
with both parents. He was in a relationship of two years and planning to marry.
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
52
The first 45-minute hypnosis session was designed to create an experience of deep,
soothing relaxation, the purpose of which was to create a positive association with the office
and with me as the therapist, and to strengthen the therapeutic relationship begun during
intake. The session included standard imagery invoking peace, calm, and tranquil relaxation;
positive suggestions about his worth; protective suggestions to ensure he only used hypnosis
when it was safe to do so; and instruction in self-hypnosis. I used voice modulation to develop
his sense of safety and relaxation.
During the next two standard hypnoanalytic diagnostic sessions it became evident that M.
had a deep, unconscious fear of death and that even hearing about someone else’s banging his
thumb with a hammer triggered this deep fear. He claimed that while consciously that he was
not afraid of death, the joint understanding of this formulation of the problem increased his
hope for recovery.
The first age regression session began with guided relaxation and then suggestions for him
to go to the scene that first conditioned his mind to develop the problem. He spontaneously
went into an age regression to birth. He described the experience step by step and said
suddenly that his head hurt. He felt fear and said his mother was also afraid. I asked what he
was afraid of and he said he felt he was going to die. He also said he felt alone because his
mother seemed preoccupied with her own fear and problems with the delivery. He expressed
feeling acutely uncomfortable emotionally with the feeling of being alone. I then guided him
to go back even further in time in the pregnancy to when he felt safe. He went back further to
an earlier time in the pregnancy. Then I asked him for a memory of when he felt loved and
safe. He said he felt loved by a dog when he was a boy. I instructed him to give the love he felt
a color. I told him he was surrounded and filled with love and safety and intensified this
experience, telling him he was surrounded and filled with the color of love and safety. When
he reported that the new experience was vivid, I had him progress back to birth. I emphasized
that he was still surrounded and filled with love and safety. He went through the whole
experience and said, I feel fine. I’m safe. When he came out of hypnosis, he said, I’m so surprised!
I felt a sting on my head where I was cut when I was born. He lifted his hand and touched a place
on the top of his head.
At the next session, two weeks later, he reported being presented with the phobic stimulus
when a co-worker was cut. He said, I sat there and thought, What’s the matter with me? I’m fine!
The phobic response was completely eliminated. He reported that he still felt afraid of being
afraid, but the phobia was gone.
In subsequent sessions we did additional age regressions to other memories of injury. A
consistent factor was the feeling of being alone, even though he was with people who cared
about him. We did additional sessions of hypnotic desensitization. I instructed him to vividly
recall recent experiences in which he had the phobic response. We made this scene very vivid.
Then I instructed him to go to a time before the scene and after the scene when he felt safe.
Then, using an imaginary remote control, I had him fast forward and fast reverse through the
scene, repeatedly bringing the feeling of safety with him. We continued to do this until he felt
completely comfortable with the memory. By the end of fifteen sessions, he reported no
feelings of fear or anxiety associated with injury.
This was the first client with a phobia that I treated using hypnoanalysis. I was amazed by
this result, a personal reaction that continues after more than a decade of using this process,
and one I am not alone in experiencing (Barabasz & Watkins, 2005). This case illustrates the
power of hypnosis to instantly relieve the emotional power of implicit memories. In the age
regression described above, we used a Narrative process to discover the unfolding of events
and their deeper meaning to him. While his subconscious mind was afraid to die, the deeper
fear was of being alone. While he was reliving this preverbal event he experienced it as though
it were happening in the present because the amygdaloid memory system lacks a sense of
location in time. This is primarily a right-hemisphere experience, but through using hypnosis
he was also accessing his left hemisphere, so he could describe the event in words (Cozolino,
2010, pp.274-275). The experience was so vivid that he felt the sensation of a cut on his adult
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
53
head. We created a new meaning for the narrative by reliving the same memory, but with a
new sensory experience of feeling loved and safe; therefore he did not die and was, in fact,
alive with a new feeling of connection. This restructuring of an implicit memory using my
knowledge of narrative and the importance of putting experiences into a sequence of events
using words and recreating meaning (Saari, 1991, p.139) had the effect of disconnecting
stimulus and response and instantly curing the phobia.
4.2. Case study of Borderline Personality Disorder
D., a 40-year-old, low-income Guatemalan-Caucasian female, reported difficult friendships
with women toward whom she felt chronically jealous and angry. She reported feeling
depressed, lonely, irritable, tired, and wanting to sleep all the time. She said both she and her
close friend had put another friend on a pedestal, and they competed for her attention. She felt
worse when there was distance in the friendships. While the friendship was going well at the
time of intake, she felt sure there would be another crisis. She had strong feelings of being left
out, which caused anxiety and difficulty in relationships.
In her childhood history she recounted that when she was six years old, her paternal
grandmother, who was her primary caregiver, passed away. Starting around age eight she ran
away every weekend to stay with her maternal grandmother. Her father was so abusive when
punishing her sister that she had to call police several times to intervene when she was a
teenager. This increased her father’s anger because of fears of immigration authorities. That
fear seemed to prevent immediate new violence. She said her parents were a little careless and
put the children in harm’s way because of inadequate supervision. Her father was funny in
the presence of friends, but with the family he was a very strict Catholic. She constantly tried
to impress him. The parents fought and threatened divorce in front of the children; however,
financial realities kept them together. Her mother worked full-time at night and was
emotionally distant. She said that her mother still hurts her emotionally, promising to do
things but never following through. She described putting herself in danger with men and
engaging in self-injurious behaviors in her twenties. She was in love with her husband when
their relationship started. Now she feels her friendships with women are more important and
she is more emotionally involved with them than with her husband.
She has been married since age 19. She reports that this relationship is good. She described
herself as having difficulty organizing her schedule and her house. She has difficulty
concentrating on tasks and tends to go from one to another, rarely finishing anything. This is a
source of conflict with her husband.
Age regressions
Three hypnotic sessions preceded the first age regression, the same method as described in the
first case study. Before the hypnotic part of the first age regression session, I asked D. how she
was doing. She reported that she was worried because one friend had asked her to go out to
dinner, but it was someone her best friend didn’t like. She was worried about losing her best
friend if she proceeded. She felt she couldn’t tell either of them how she felt because she feels
she always has to seem happy so they will like her. This appears to be a situation of
disorganized attachment (Applegate & Shapiro, 2005).
After an induction and instruction to go to a scene from when the problem started, she
went to a scene in which she was a baby. She said, I couldn’t cry in front of my dad. If he saw a
tear, it angered him. I’d have to stop crying. He would just scream. I asked, Where does that leave
you? D. replied, By myself. Lonely. I don’t think I’m valuable. I’m not important. Emotionally I don’t
thrive. I need more attention. I want to stay with somebody who gives me attention. I asked, What do
you learn about your needs? D. said, They don’t get met. I can’t be myself. I’m molded to be what
Mother wants, to make it easier for her. I suggested that she imagine her adult self comes into the
scene knowing just what she needs. What does your adult self do? She picks me up. Holds me.
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
54
Feeds me, loves me. I asked, How does that feel? Warm. I asked her to switch and be her adult
self holding her baby self. What new messages do you want to give to your baby self? She
wasn’t sure. So I asked questions such as How do you feel toward your baby self? She
replied, You’re valuable. You’re important. I want you to be yourself. I’ll make sure your needs are
met. I love you very much. I’ll always be there to take care of you and help you. She began crying and
I paused the session to give her a tissue to reinforce the message that it’s OK to cry. She
continued talking to her younger self: I’ll always support you. You’re strong, beautiful, and
independent. I suggested some additional messages which she immediately said out loud to her
baby self. You can cry when you want to cry. I’ll talk to you and find out why you’re upset. I’ll give
the best advice I can. I’ll always connect to you. I told her to imagine that connection in some form,
like a ribbon of light. She nodded that she had done that. I told her, You are connected now.
Then she continued: I love you and you’re perfect. I told her to be her baby self again being held
by her adult self. I told her I was going to repeat these positive messages and I wanted her to
hear them as though they were coming from her adult self and that her adult self would
absorb them at the same time. I repeated the new messages. I then had her switch back to her
adult self. I told her to imagine her heart was full of the light of love and to bring her child self
into the light within her heart. I told her that her child self now grows up bringing these new
beliefs and feelings with her, integrating fully into her adult self. I asked her how she felt and
she replied, Emotionally amazing. Everything you always want to hear. Full. Whole. Complete.
Lighter.
After the hypnotic part of the session ended, I again asked how she felt. She replied,
Amazing. Then she added, I see myself trying to mold my daughter to make it easier for me. I need to
pay attention more to her. I instructed her to use self-hypnosis to imagine taking care of her baby
self. Then I asked her how she felt about going to dinner with her friend. She replied, I’m fine.
I’m not worried about my friendships.
This first age regression demonstrated an implicit memory in which D.’s parents
demonstrated a profound lack of attunement (Applegate & Shapiro, 2005) and in which she
was forced into a situation of reverse mirroring and having to mold herself on a basic
physiological level to her parents’ needs (Elson, 1988). This implicit memory informed how
she expected her friends to relate to her. It forced her to constantly orient herself outwardly,
longing for the love and attention she didn’t get as a child. Guided by the sound of my voice,
which she had already connected with soothing from the earlier preparatory sessions, she was
able to manage the anxiety and give a narrative account of what happened and what the
implicit meaning was to her. Then she was able to describe very precisely what she needed.
We created a vivid, new experience of her receiving exactly what she needed, an experience of
precise attunement. She was able to have this experience because the range of emotional
experiences available to her was greater while she was in hypnosis. Because hypnosis has the
ability to create vivid sensory experiences, she was able to have a vivid, rich sensory
experience of being cared for. This provided a powerful holding environment (Goldstein,
2002, p. 99) for her as she experienced this new scene as though it were actually happening
neurologically in the present. Hypnosis creates the experience in the somatosensory, visual
and auditory cortexes with hallucinatory vividness (Bentall, 2000). While she knew that she
was sitting in a chair in my office, her brain was having an experience as though it were real.
She had an experience that felt like an actual experience of love. The new, positive experience
was absorbed into implicit memory. I repeated those same messages to her so she heard them
in her own voice and in my voice. My repeating the positive messages to her added
reinforcement to the neural pathways already firing with the experience of love. Her verbal
responses indicated it was a very real experience for her. She also now has an image of a
loving presence that can always be with her in the image of her adult self. That adult self (ego)
has had the experience of being a loving and nurturing presence. On an implicit memory level,
this is a completely new, novel experience, so the dopamine systems in the brain trigger
rewarding feelings and strengthen the new memory (LeDoux, 1996, p.189). When she is in
self-hypnosis at home, D. can recreate this experience for herself and reinforce these new
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
55
neural pathways. As her feelings of neediness and expectation of loss recede, she will be able
to use her ego strength more effectively because the amygdaloid memory will be calmer and
will be less likely to overwhelm the prefrontal cortex processing system (LeDoux, 1996, p.303).
This will strengthen her ego capacities. By using self-hypnosis as a self-soothing tool and to
reenact this new experience, the subject uses self-hypnosis as an ego-strengthening tool. In
fact, in the process of using self-hypnosis, if D. gets distracted and brings her mind back
repeatedly, her prefrontal cortex and other areas associated with concentration will actually
grow and become stronger (McGonigal, 2012, p.25) and become less overwhelmed, increasing
her ego strength.
Short of physically reenacting such an event, which is considered unethical (NASW, 2008,
para.1.10), it is difficult to provide such a direct experience without hypnosis. Additionally, if
a psychotherapist were to provide such physical comfort, the therapist would become the
object of the client’s neediness and reduce the client’s self-determination (NASW, 2008, section
1.02). The source of support, care, and love would remain outside the client, and accessible
only during therapy sessions.
Second age regression
Before starting hypnosis, I asked D. how she was doing. She said, I’m PMS’ing like crazy. My
thoughts are racing. I’m disorganized. I feel like I’m going crazy. Referring to work, she said, I’m
going to make a ton of money selling jewelry. I’m supposed to have a mentor. I feel she’s not really
teaching me. She’s making me feel that I don't want to ask for help. She made a comment; she said,
You’re doing it but not learning it. After hypnotic induction I suggested she take us to the place
where the problem started. She said, I had a track meet. My coach said to get protein so I made
myself an egg. I used a dish rag to take it off the stove. It caught fire. It was scary. I asked, What do
you do? Get mom out of bed. She starts yelling. She called me stupid. How do you feel when she
does that? Insecure. I think, How could I be so stupid? I asked D. to imagine her adult self coming
into the scene. I would probably yell at her, too. It was clear to me that she did not have internal
models to help her in this situation, so I assisted her in developing a corrective scene. I asked,
Did anyone teach you how to use the stove? No. I commented, So young D. is on her own. Yes.
I did terrible in that track meet. I asked,What would be more helpful from adult D.? I don't know.
I gave her ideas about what might be more helpful, and she said, I explain what happened. I
should have taught you. You can’t put a dish rag by the burner. She went on to explain how to get
an egg of the stove safely. How does young D. feel?
There’s guidance. It wasn’t my fault. I suggested to her that her younger self ask her adult self
for help. Will you help me make my lunch? We then created an extensive scene where her adult
self was teaching her younger self and also working alongside her to help her do the tasks of
getting ready. She said, I’m learning something new. Then she imagined attending the track
meet and cheering for her younger self and her younger self’s experiencing that. Near the end
I asked, How does that feel? Complete. Taken care of. After the hypnosis ended she said, I yell at
my kids too much. Maybe they don’t know everything I want.
This second age regression demonstrates a lack of mirroring and the lack of a guiding adult
whom D. could admire and emulate, and who would admire her, leaving her vulnerable to
narcissistic injury (Elson, 1988, pp.49-51). In talking about her work, D. exhibited grandiosity
but not the requisite skill to do the work (Elson, 1988, p.51). At work she was unable to use the
input from an adult mentor because she had no inner model of how to learn a skill or how to
ask for help. She did not expect her current adult mentor at work to teach her or to admire her,
nor could she provide those experiences for her own children. The growth and organization of
the brain is deeply influenced by early relationships that stimulate the brain to develop an
interactive set of internalized objects just as object relations proposes (Cozolino, 2010, p.41).
Her reaction when she said, I would yell at her [her child self], too, demonstrates the power of the
implicit memory to determine her current behavior. In this situation, she needed my
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
56
knowledge of Self Psychology and Ego Psychology to help her develop a new scene that
created a new internal model.
Her response at the end of the session showed a greater sense of herself when she reported
feeling complete. In these age regressions there appears to be a combination of the client’s
becoming her own self-object, but also using me as the therapist to create a supportive
environment, hearing my safe, soothing voice, and responding with exacting imagery to what
she felt she needed. During this age regression we helped her develop ego functions. She was
demonstrating the capacity for self-reflection, observing her reactions to herself in the scene.
The age regression allowed her to develop new skills of thinking about her needs in a more
constructive way. It allowed her to have a vivid enactment of developing problem-solving
skills. After the hypnotic part of the session, she observed a correlation with her own
parenting and made decisions about new ways she could parent her daughter. All of these are
ego skills she practiced during the session, giving her a new template from which to work.
Additionally, asking her to switch between her child self and her adult self helped her develop
a distinction between her primitive needs and her ego capacity. She had an opportunity to
practice ego-based skills such as observation, planning, and executing a plan (Goldstein, 1995).
This is not a case that will be cured in a few short hypnosis sessions, because D. grew up
with deeply disturbed attachment. The repatterning likely needs to be done at every
developmental stage, and along all attachment/nurturance/love and guidance/
structure/discipline vectors. This is a long-term therapy case; however, I do expect it will take
much less time than using the same treatment models in talk therapy. Because we can create
vivid experiences with highly targeted interventions, it is likely that D. will begin to be able to
carry the new sense of self more quickly than if we were not able to directly access and modify
the patterns and the emotional arousal and valence in the implicit memories. She has had
vivid and immediate positive experiences that give her hope and make coming to therapy
highly rewarding for her. When she comes in feeling needy, we can create an experience for
her that is immediately emotionally gratifying so that she can begin to look to herself rather
than to look with haunting neediness to people who are unlikely to meet the needs she has for
mirroring, attunement, attachment, and guidance.
5. DISCUSSION
To address Freud’s question about why hypnosis works quickly for some clients and more
slowly for others, I propose the following: In the case of the phobia, a relatively simple
stimulus- response set of neural pathways were the cause of the problem, and so they were
easy to change. In someone with more complicated psychopathology, arousal has coordinated
learning over a larger set of neurological systems because of parallel plasticity (LeDoux, 2003,
pp.303-304). The range of emotional experiences D. had as a child was much narrower and
had a direct effect on her sense of self because the ongoing learning in the context of abuse and
neglect she developed set personality structures (LeDoux, 2003, p.322) that involve a wider set
of neurological systems. Healing will involve disconnecting certain stimulus-response
patterns but also building underdeveloped brain capacities through new and broader ranges
of affective and cognitive experiences.
Despite the belief expressed by Cozolino that the ability to “‘edit and re-edit’ emotional
memories . . . remains on a distant horizon” (2010, p.338), the ability is here today through the
theoretically informed use of hypnosis. Because the amygdaloid memory system creates an
emotional tone in particular situations, we can tell if the target memories have been resolved
by how a client feels in situations that are similar to those that triggered difficult affect,
distorted thinking, or problematic behavior in the past. I frequently have clients who come in
and say, I feel different, better, and I don’t know why. Or, I drive down that road where I used to get
anxious and it doesn’t bother me. One client had undergone twenty years of psychotherapy.
After twenty-two sessions using hypnoanalysis, she said, I don’t have anxiety any more. I lived
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
57
with anxiety all my life. She went on to describe very difficult work and family situations. She
said, Now, with all of this, I have no anxiety. She looked as though she had experienced a miracle.
These new experiences feel automatic, a feature of implicit memory. Nevertheless, further
research on the efficacy of using hypnosis in psychotherapy needs to be done to determine
these effects and to further uncover the innate abilities of the human brain (Nash, 2012, p.216).
REFERENCES
Applegate, J. S., & Shapiro, J. R. (2005). Neurobiology for Clinical Social Work: Theory and Practice (First).
New York, NY, US: W. W. Norton and Co.
Barabasz, A., & Barabasz, M. (2012). Hypnosis and the brain. In The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory,
Research, and Practice (Paperback, pp. 337–363). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Barabasz, A., & Watkins, J. (2005). Hypnotherapeutic Techniques 2E. New York, NY, US: Brunner-
Routledge.
Barber, J. (2012). Reclaiming the cognitive unconscious: integrating hypnotic methods and congitive-
behavioral therapy. In The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice (Paperback, pp.
457–465). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
Barnier, A. J., & McConkey, K. M. (2003). Hypnosis, human nature, and complexity: Integrating
neuroscience approaches into hypnosis research. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 51(3), 282–308. http://doi.org/10.1076/iceh.51.3.282.15524
Bentall, R. (2000). Hypnotic and psychotic hallucinations: Rich data capable of multiple interpretations.
Contemporary Hypnosis, 17(1), 21–25. http://doi.org/10.1002/ch.188
Bob, P. (2007). Hypnotic abreaction releases chaotic patterns of electrodermal activity during
dissociation. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 55(4), 435–456.
Bob, P., Kukleta, M., Riecansky, I., Susta, M., Kukumberg, P., & Jagla, F. (2006). Chaotic EEG Patterns
During Recall of Stressful Memory Related to Panic Attack. Psysiological Research, 55(1).
Bob, P., Siroka, I., & Susta, M. (2009). Chaotic Patterns of Autonomic Activity During Hypnotic Recall.
International Journal of Neuroscience, 119(2), 240–254. http://doi.org/10.1080/00207450802325744
Brown, D., & Fromm, E. (1986). Hypnotherapy and Hypnoanalysis. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers.
Bryant, R. A. (2012). Hypnosis and anxiety: early interventions. In The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis:
Theory, Research, and Practice (Paperback, pp. 536–547). New York, NY, US.
Clark, LCSW, J. R. (2014, December). A Critical History of Hypnosis" Why The Road Was Less Travelled.
Unpublished manuscript.
Covino, N., & Pinnell, C. (2010). Hypnosis and Medicine. In S. J. Lynn, J. W. Rhue, & I. Kirsch (Eds.),
Handbook of Clinical Hypnosis (2nd ed., pp. 551–573). Washington, CD: American Psychological
Association.
Cozolino, L. (2010). The Neuroscience of Psychotherapy: Healing the Social Brain (2nd ed.). New York, NY,
US: W. W. Norton & Company.
Elkins, G., & Perfect, M. (2008). Hypnosis for health-compromising behaviors. In The Oxford Handbook of
Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice (Paperback, pp. 569–592). New York, NY, US: Oxford
University Press.
Elson, M. (1988). Self Psychology in Clinical Social Work (1 edition). New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
Faymonville, M.-E., Roediger, L., Del Fiore, G., Delgueldre, C., Phillips, C., Lamy, M., Laureys, S. (2003).
Increased cerebral functional connectivity underlying the antinociceptice effects of hypnosis.
Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 225–262.
Franklin, B. (1837). Animal magnetism : report of Dr. Franklin and other commissioners, charged by the King of
France with the examination of the animal magnetism as practised at Paris : translated from the French, with
an historical outline of the “science”, an abstract of the report on magnetic experiments made by a committee of
the Royal Academy of Medicine in 1831, and remarks on Col. Stone’s pamphlet. Philadelphia : Perkins.
Retrieved from http://archive.org/details/56721030R.nlm.nih.gov
Freud, A. (1951). Symposium on genetic psychology: 2. The contribution of psychoanalysis to genetic
psychology. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 21(3), 476–497. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-
0025.1951.tb00006.x
Freud, S. (2010). Freud: Complete Works. (I. Smith, Ed.) (Digital). Ivan Smith.
Activitas Nervosa Superior 2015, 57, No. 2
58
Freyd, J. J. (1998). Betrayal Trauma: The Logic of Forgetting Childhood Abuse (Reprint edition). Cambridge,
Mass.; London: Harvard University Press.
Gauld, A. (1992). A history of hypnotism (Vol. xvii). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.
Goldstein, E. G. (1995). Ego psychology and social work practice (2nd ed.). New York : Free Press.
Goldstein, E. G. (2002). Object Relations Theory and Self Psychology in Social Work Practice. New York: The
Free Press.
Halsband, U., Mueller, S., Hinterberger, T., & Strickner, S. (2009). Plasticity changes in the brain in
hypnosis and meditation. Contemporary Hypnosis, 26(4), 194–215. http://doi.org/10.1002/ch.386
Hoeft F, Gabrieli JE, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, & et al. (2012). Functional brain basis of hypnotizability.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(10), 1064–1072. http://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2190
Kelliher, S. (2005). Hypnotic suggestion can reduce conflict in human brain, Weill Cornell medical college
researchers report (News Service). New York: Cornell University.
LeDoux, J. (1996). The Emotional brain (1st ed.). New York, NY, US: Touchstone.
LeDoux, J. (2003). Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. New York: Penguin Books.
LeDoux, J. (2007). The amygdala. Current Biology, 17(20), R868–R874.
Lynn, S. J., Kirsch, I., Barabasz, A., Cardena, E., & Patterson, D. (2000). Hypnosis as an empirically
supported clinical intervention: The state of the evidence and a look to the future. International Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 48(2), 239.
Lynn, S. J., Rhue, J. W., & Kirsch, I. (2010). Handbook of Clinical Hypnosis (2nd ed.). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
McGonigal, K. (2012). The Willpower Instinct. New York, NY, US: Penguin Group.
Moore, M., & Tasso, A. F. (2012). Clinical hypnosis; the empirical evidence. In The Oxford Handbook of
Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice (Paperback, pp. 697–725). New York, NY, US: Oxford
University Press.
Müller, K., Bacht, K., Prochnow, D., Schramm, S., & Seitz, R. J. (2013). Activation of thalamus in motor
imagery results from gating by hypnosis. NeuroImage, 66, 361–367.
Nash, M. R. (2012). A psychoanalytic theory of hypnosis: a clinically informed approach. In The Oxford
Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice (Paperback, pp. 201–222). New York, NY, US:
Oxford University Press.
Nash, M. R., & Barnier, A. J. (2012). The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice
(Paperback). Oxford University Press.
NASW. (2008). NASW Code of Ethics. NASW. Retrieved from Peebles, M. J. (2012). Trauma-related
disorders and dissociation. In The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice
(Paperback, pp. 647–679). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
Raz, A., Fan, J., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Hypnotic suggestion reduces conflict in the human brain.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(28), 9978–9983.
http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0503064102
Saari, C. (1991). The Creation of Meaning in Clinical Social Work. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Scott, E. L., Lagges, A., LaClave, L., Nash, M. R., & Barnier, A. J. (2012). Treating children using hypnosis.
In The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice (Paperback, pp. 592–609). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Scott, Sr., J. A. (1996). The Handbook of Brief Psychotherapy by Hypnoanalysis (2nd ed.). Bloomington, IN:
Author House.
Siegel, M. (2006). Can we cure fear? Scientific American Mind, 16(No. 4), 44–49.
Tasso, A. F., & Perez, N. A. (2012). Parsing everyday suggestibility: what does it tell us about hypnosis?
In The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice (Paperback, pp. 283–309). New York,
NY, US: Oxford University Press.
Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Laureys, S., & Faymonville, M.-E. (2014). Neurophysiology of hypnosis. Clinical
Neurophysiology, 44, 343–353.
Vermetten, E., & Douglas Bremner, J. (2004). Functional brain imaging and the induction of traumatic
recall: a cross-correlational review between neuroimaging and hypnosis. The International Journal Of
Clinical And Experimental Hypnosis, 52(3), 280–312.
Watkins, J. (1992). Hypnoanalytic Techniques (Vol. II). New York, NY, US: Irvington Publishers, Inc.
Yapko, M. D. (2010). Hypnosis and depression. In Handbook of Clinical Hypnosis (2nd ed., pp. 391–413).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.