Content uploaded by Marianne Schmid Mast
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Marianne Schmid Mast on Oct 15, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 1
The Role of Nonverbal Behavior in Leadership: An Integrative Review
Annick Darioly
Claremont McKenna College
Marianne Schmid Mast
University of Neuchatel, Switzerland
Author Note
Annick Darioly, Kravis Leadership Institute, Claremont McKenna College,
Claremont, California, United States; Marianne Schmid Mast, Department of Work and
Organizational Psychology, University of Neuchatel, Neuchatel, Switzerland. Annick
Darioly is currently at the Department of Work and Organizational Psychology, University
of Neuchatel, Neuchatel, Switzerland.
We thank Claudia Raigoza and Petra Schmid for comments on previous drafts of the
manuscript.
Correspondence concerning this chapter should be addressed to Annick Darioly,
Department of Work and Organizational Psychology, University of Neuchatel, Rue Emile-
Argand 11, CH-2000 Neuchatel, phone: +41 32 718 1390, fax: + 41 32 718 1391
Email: annick.darioly@unine.ch
Chapter proposed for Riggio, R. E., & Tan, S.. Understanding and Assessing Soft Leader
Skills.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2
The Role of Nonverbal Behavior for Leadership: An Integrative Review
One of the main activities of leaders is interacting with others (e.g., Yukl, 2010).
Their interactions with followers, colleagues, or business partners happen through verbal
and nonverbal behavior. In this chapter, we focus on leader nonverbal behavior (NVB).
NVB plays an important role in interpersonal communication in general and accounts for a
majority (about 65 to 90%) of the meaning conveyed in social interactions (e.g., Crane &
Crane, 2010).
NVB refers to any behavior other than speech content. However, the distinction
between verbal and nonverbal behavior is not always clear. For example, “emblems,” such
as nonverbal gestures like the “okay” made with the thumb and forefinger, or the “thumbs
up” gesture, have a distinct verbal meaning. But most nonverbal cues are subject to
interpretation. A distinction between speech-related NVB and speech-unrelated NVB can
be helpful (Knapp & Hall, 2010). Speech-related NVB encompasses, for instance, tone of
voice, speech modulation, and speech duration. Examples of speech-unrelated NVB include
eye gaze, facial expressions, body movements, posture, touch, smell, mode of dress, and
walking style (Knapp & Hall, 2010). Whether verbal or nonverbal behavior matters more as
a source of information depends on the situation. In an equivocal situation, NVB is often
referred to as a source of information. The more a situation is equivocal, the more
important NVB is. People often turn to NVB for information when the NVB contradicts the
verbal communication or when individuals doubt the honesty of a verbal communication
(e.g., Mehrabian, 1972).
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 3
NVB is important for successful social interactions. Its functions include revealing
personality characteristics, signaling interpersonal orientations (dominance, friendliness), or
expressing emotions (Knapp & Hall, 2010). When strangers meet for the first time, the
impression they form about each other is mostly based on verbal and nonverbal cues (e.g.,
Ambady, Hallahan, & Rosenthal, 1995; Costanzo & Archer, 1989; Hyde, 2005). Regardless
of whether the formed impressions are correct or not, they affect what one thinks about the
social interaction partners and how one behaves toward them. In sum, interaction partners
express their states and traits – not only but also – through NVB, consciously or
unconsciously, and they use NVB to form impressions about others (Mehrabian & Wiener,
1967). This process can be illustrated with the Brunswikian lens model (Brunswik, 1956)
(Figure 1). Two perspectives are present in the model. On the one hand, the model depicts
the perceiver who observes the target’s NVB and interprets it. The perceiver forms an
impression about the target, for example, regarding the target’s personality, based on the
target’s NVB. On the other hand, the model depicts the target and how he or she expresses
him or herself in NVB. The Brunswikian lens model (Brunswik, 1956) has been used
extensively to explain accuracy in social perception in different situations, including the
leadership context. To illustrate, in a business meeting, on the one hand a new employee
typically observes the NVB of the individuals present in the meeting, and infers who might
be the leader through their NVB. This refers to the relationship between the perception of
leadership and the observed NVB. On the other hand, the actual leader might speak more
and approach more closely than the followers. This describes the relation between an
individual’s actual leadership and his or her NVB. If the perception of leadership and the
actual leadership correspond with each other, this is accuracy. Accuracy as we described it
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 4
here is one aspect of a person’s nonverbal communication abilities (Riggio, 2006).
Nonverbal communication abilities are understood as individual differences in people’s
skills to convey nonverbal messages to others, to read others’ NVB, and to regulate and
control their nonverbal displays (Riggio, 2006). They are part of the domain of
interpersonal skills, which are the skills used by a person to properly interact with others
(Riggio, Riggio, Salinas, & Cole, 2003).
***Figure 1***
Nonverbal communication abilities and NVB play an important role in leadership
(Stein, 1975). Leadership is the process of influencing or controlling the behavior of others
in order to reach a shared goal (Northouse, 2007; Stogdill, 1950). It has even been
suggested that in the leadership context, nonverbal communication is more important than
verbal communication. When the leader’s verbal and nonverbal cues are in contradiction,
the followers are more likely to trust the leader’s nonverbal cues (Remland, 1981).
Individuals in leadership positions express their power and authority not only verbally but
also nonverbally to get followers’ attention and exert influence over them, for example, by
being nonverbally persuasive (using greater facial expressiveness and greater fluency and
pitch variety; Burgoon, Birk, & Pfau, 1990). A number of studies have documented the
effects of leader NVB on leadership effectiveness (i.e., the evaluations of leader's
competence, supportiveness, or success and the leader effects on followers satisfaction,
motivation, and performance; Kaiser, Hogan, & Craig, 2008). For example, leaders
establish a high level of mutual trust, cohesion, and sensitivity to the follower’s needs by
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 5
demonstrating abilities to communicate nonverbally (Yukl, 2010). According to Riggio and
colleagues, leaders who are able to correctly read and interpret nonverbal cues and act upon
this understanding are more likely to exhibit behaviors that meet the needs of their
followers (Reichard & Riggio, 2008; Riggio, 1986, 2006; Riggio & Carney, 2003),
ultimately resulting in more positive perceptions of the leader’s effectiveness (Riggio et al.,
2003). Uhl-Bien (2004) suggests that the leader’s nonverbal interpersonal skills are part of
the key features needed to build effective leader-follower relationships. Thus NVB is a
crucial means through which interpersonal skills lead to effective leadership.
In this chapter, we present an integrative review regarding the role of NVB in
leadership. We organize the chapter around the following central questions: Based on
which NVB do individuals perceive or infer leadership in emergent hierarchies? Based on
which leader NVB do followers perceive effective leadership in actual hierarchies? Which
NVB do leaders exhibit? How does leader NVB impact leadership effectiveness?
Consequently, the goals of this chapter are (1) to provide an overview of the empirical
findings pertaining to NVB in a leadership context; (2) to show how individual differences
affect the relation between NVB and leadership; (3) to discuss implications of the reported
findings for leaders; and (4) to draw conclusions and make suggestions on how to advance
research in this field.
Nonverbal Behavior and the Perception of Leadership
Nonverbal behavior plays an important part in the perception of leadership.
Research on NVB and perceived leadership has focused on two distinct aspects: the role of
NVB for emergent leadership, and the perception of leadership based on NVB. In this
section, both aspects are presented and discussed.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 6
NVB and Emergent Leadership
An emergent leader is defined as the person who is not assigned a leadership
position but arises as a leader within a group (Guastello, 2002; Stein & Heller, 1979). The
emergent leader is typically the one who has the most influence in the group (Stein &
Heller, 1979). An individual emerges as a leader based on other individuals’ perceptions of
him or her (e.g., Gray & Densten, 2007; Schyns, Felfe, & Blank, 2007). This mechanism is
explained by the Expectation States Theory (EST; Berger, Conner, & Fisek, 1974; Berger,
Fisek, Norman, & Zelditch, 1977; Ridgeway & Berger, 1986). According to EST, group
members form performance expectations about each other. A performance expectation is a
“generalized anticipation of one’s own or another’s capacity to make useful contributions to
the task” (Ridgeway & Berger, 1986, p. 604). To the extent that all group members share
these expectations, they become self-fulfilling prophecies. Expectations are affected,
especially in relatively homogenous peer groups, by the NVB exhibited by the group
members (Ridgeway & Berger, 1986). To illustrate, an individual who talks a lot in a group
discussion might be perceived as an expert on the discussion topic, so the performance
expectations for this individual are high. As a consequence, this individual is provided with
more opportunities to contribute, thus gaining more influence in the group and emerging as
the group’s leader.
The typical research design to assess emergent leadership is to videotape group
interactions, to code the NVB of each group member, and then to compare it with the group
members’ ratings of each other in terms of leadership (e.g., Baird, 1977; Riggio et al.,
2003). Some studies have, however, used external (non-group members) for assessing the
leadership of each group member. To illustrate, in some studies (Moore & Porter, 1988;
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 7
Schmidt Mast, Hall, Murphy, & Colvin, 2003; Stang, 1973), external observers watched
different targets and then rated their leadership or dominance (i.e., any behavior aiming at
gaining influence over others; Schmid Mast, 2010). The observations yield information
about which of the targets’ NVB is used by observers to infer leadership, thus emergent
leadership. There are a number of NVB that people use to infer leadership. For example,
gazing more, especially at the end of a statement (Kalma & van Rooij, 1982) in order to
invite others to speak up, is a behavior that emergent leaders exhibit. Also, body
movements such as more or less arm and shoulder movements contribute to perceptions of
emergent leadership (Baird, 1977). The choice of seating place can also affect emergence
of a leader (e.g., Heckel, 1973; Porter & Geis, 1981; Ward, 1968). It seems to be normal in
developed countries at least, that leaders are expected to sit at the head of the table.
Speaking time has also been shown to relate to emergent leadership as demonstrated in a
meta-analysis by Schmid Mast (2002). Visual dominance, defined as the ratio of the
percentage of looking while speaking divided by the percentage of looking while listening
(Exline, Ellyson, & Long, 1975) also shows a positive link to emergent leadership (Dovidio
& Ellyson, 1982).
The most comprehensive meta-analysis on the link between NVB and emergent
leadership or perceived dominance stems from Hall, Coast, and Smith LeBeau (2005)1.
Results suggest that many different cues are assumed to be markers of emergent leadership.
Individuals are perceived as emergent leaders when they show more gazing, more nodding,
and lowered eyebrows. They are perceived as emergent leaders when they demonstrate less
self-touching but more touching others. They are perceived as emergent leaders when they
have a more variable tone of voice, a faster speech rate, and a lower voice pitch as well as
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 8
when they show more vocal relaxation. Additionally, they are perceived as emergent
leaders when they show more erect or tense postures, have more hand and arm gestures,
more body or leg shifts, as well as more body openness. Also, they are perceived as
emergent leaders when they interrupt others more often.
NVB and Perceptions of Leadership
The perception of leadership by group members is also important in established
hierarchies. The power leaders have depends on how they are perceived by followers
(Hollander & Julian, 1969; Maurer & Lord, 1991; Pfeffer, 1977).
The perception of leadership in an established hierarchy can be understood by using
implicit leadership theory (ILT; e.g., Lord, de Vader, & Alliger, 1986; Lord, Foti, &
Phillips, 1982). This theory holds that individuals develop a set of beliefs about the
characteristics and behaviors of effective and ineffective leaders (e.g., strength, charisma,
sensitivity, tyranny) based on previous experiences (Schyns & Schilling, 2010). These
beliefs are outside of conscious awareness − they are implicit. Thus, followers use their
beliefs to explain and evaluate their leader’s behaviors. Research suggests that the degree of
matching that occurs between followers’ beliefs and their leaders’ behavior partially
determines whether followers categorize their leaders as effective or ineffective leaders
(Nye, 2002; Nye & Forsyth, 1991; Schyns & Schilling, 2010).
NVB plays a role in leadership perception. For example, Savvas and Schyns (2012)
used ILT and pictures of facial expression to investigate how leadership was perceived.
Participants reported their beliefs about the characteristics and behaviors of leaders. Then
each participant examined a photo of a man in which the facial expression differed (neutral
vs. raising/lowering and pulling together the eyebrows ). Raising and pulling together the
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 9
eyebrows typically expresses sadness or fear, whereas lowering and pulling together the
eyebrows typically expresses anger (Ekman, Friesen, & Hager, 2002). The participants then
were asked to evaluate the man in the picture with respect to leadership perception using
the same questionnaire in which they reported their beliefs about the characteristics and
behaviors of leaders. Results showed that when the participants’ beliefs matched how they
perceived the man based on his facial expression, the depicted man was evaluated to be
more leader-like.
In sum, the implicit theories about leader characteristics that followers harbor
influence how a leader is perceived. For all of these judgments, perceptions and evaluations
of leadership are based on the leader’s NVB. In order to complete this overview, it is
important not only to understand how leadership is perceived but also how it is expressed
through NVB.
Nonverbal Behavior and the Expression of Leadership
A leader’s role is to provide information, to instruct, direct, coordinate, and to give
feedback (Mintzberg, 1973). Obviously, encoding or sending of nonverbal messages to
followers, co-workers, or business partners is part of the leader role. The leader’s NVB
differs according to the leadership style adopted by the leader (constructive vs. destructive).
In this section, we review studies in which NVB of actual leaders was studied in order to
identify the NVB relevant to constructive and destructive leadership theories.
One of the most effective or constructive leadership styles is the charismatic or
transformational leadership style2 (Bass & Bass, 2008). It results in increased follower
satisfaction and more organizational effectiveness (Lowe, Kroeck, & Sivasubramaniam,
1996). This style typically includes NVB such as animated facial expressions, faster rate of
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 10
speech, and erect posture or expansive body movements (Friedman, Prince, Riggio, &
DiMatteo, 1980; Friedman & Riggio, 1981). Charismatic leaders use these nonverbal cues
“to move, inspire, or captivate others” (Friedman et al., p. 133), to express a strong and
confident presence, and to stimulate desired responses from followers (Gardner & Avolio,
1998). Even in an experimental setting it has been shown that the expression of certain
NVB makes people judge somebody as charismatic (e.g., Awamleh, 1997; Awamleh &
Gardner, 1999; Howell & Frost, 1989; Shea & Howell, 1999). For example, Awamleh and
his colleague (Awamleh, 1997; Awamleh & Gardner, 1999) presented videotaped
charismatic speeches to participants. The actor was trained to use animated facial
expressions, dynamic hand and body gestures, to show vocal fluency, and to maintain eye
contact. Results demonstrated that leaders were perceived as charismatic when they
exhibited the above-mentioned NVB more so than when they did not. In a laboratory
experiment, Shea and Howell (1999) trained actors to be charismatic or non-charismatic
leaders. Charismatic leaders were trained to maintain direct eye contact, to have an
animated facial expression, to use a captivating voice tone, to lean forward toward the
participants, and to alternate between pacing and sitting on the edge of the desk.
Contrastingly, non-charismatic leaders were trained to maintain sporadic eye contact, a
neutral tone of voice, and a neutral facial expression. The study showed that charismatic
leaders interacting with the participants were perceived as charismatic when they exhibited
the corresponding NVB. Consistent with the experimental studies, Groves (2006) examined
actual organizational leaders and found that leader nonverbal expressivity was positively
related to follower ratings of leader charisma.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 11
In contrast, one of the less effective leadership styles is labeled “destructive
leadership” (Schyns & Schilling, 2012). Destructive leaders intentionally or unintentionally
affect the activities and relationships within the team or the organization (e.g., attempting to
reach higher performance or to bully a follower into leaving) (Schyns & Schilling, 2012). It
results in undermining the follower’s satisfaction and the organization’s effectiveness
(Einarsen, Aasland, & Skogstad, 2007). Many concepts have been used to describe
destructive leadership, such as “toxic leadership” (Lipman-Blumen, 2005) or “abusive
supervision” (Tepper, 2000). Contrary to charismatic leadership research, NVB related to
this style has almost never been investigated. In his definition of abusive supervision,
Tepper (2000) included the display of NVB excluding physical contact, however, he did
not mention specific NVB related to destructive leadership.
Although to date no empirical research has identified the specific NVB relevant to
the expressions of destructive leadership, it is of great importance to expand this research
area. For instance, researchers might want to clarify which NVB refers to destructive
leadership and then how destructive NVB impacts leadership effectiveness. At this point a
table of the results regarding the role of NVB in leadership is provided (Table 1).
***Table 1***
Nonverbal Behavior and Effective Leadership
Obviously, the expression of leadership through NVB can be beneficial for leaders
in order to be effective. In this section we review some of the research that demonstrates
the role of NVB in leadership effectiveness, integrating both aspects of leadership
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 12
effectiveness: (1) the evaluations of leaders’ competence, supportiveness, or success, and,
(2) followers’ outcomes such as satisfaction, motivation, or follower/team performance
(Kaiser et al., 2008).
With respect to NVB and leader evaluation, research shows that leader NVB can
convey supportiveness (Remland, Jacobson, & Jones, 1983) and professional success
(DePaulo & Friedman, 1998; Shamir, House, & Arthur, 1993). In an experimental study
(Remland et al., 1983), undergraduate students were asked to read a scenario in which
different aspects of leader NVB were described, and then the students were asked to
evaluate the leaders’ supportiveness. Results showed that participants perceived leaders as
supportive when they touched their followers, were oriented toward their followers, spoke
with a soft voice, smiled with compassion, gazed, and nodded. In contrast, leaders who kept
their distance, were leaning back, spoke in a firm voice, interrupted, did not look or smile,
and turned away from their followers were perceived as non-supportive. Moreover,
DePaulo and Friedman’s review (1998) demonstrated that the display of more eye contact,
more gesturing, more smiling, animated facial expressions, and more pitch variation were
related to professional success. Research on charismatic leadership shows that more
expressive NVB is linked to more leader success (Bass, 1990; Riggio, 1998).
Leader NVB not only impacts the evaluations of leaders, but also follower
outcomes. In a work context, according to the Pygmalion theory (Eden, 1990), leaders
might adapt their behavior toward their followers in accordance with the leaders’
expectations about followers’ performance. This behavior, in turn, influences the followers’
self-efficacy (Bandura, 1977) and performance (Sutton & Woodman, 1989). This influence
can be beneficial as well as detrimental. For instance, if the leader expects increased
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 13
performance from his or her followers, then the followers indeed show that increase; and if
the leader expects decreased performance from them, then the followers show that
decrease. Research demonstrated that leaders’ NVB is different when interacting with
followers of whom they have higher performance expectations than those of whom they
have lower performance expectations. However, the difference is undetectable by followers
(King, 1971). King (1971) demonstrated the Pygmalion effect in a training program for
disadvantaged people using an experimental approach. He randomly selected different
individuals as high aptitude personnel (HAPs), leading the leaders to expect higher
performance from these followers. Results showed that the HAPs showed significantly
higher performance than the other followers (control group). Post-experimental interviews
were conducted with the followers in order to better understand the effect. Two pictures of
their leader were shown to the followers: they were identical except that one was modified
to make the pupil-size of the leader’s eyes larger than the other. Enlarged pupil size is
indicative of favorable attitudes toward others (Janisse, 1973). Both HAPs and control
group were asked to choose the picture that was closest to the way in which their leader
looked at them. The HAPs picked pictures with enlarged pupils significantly more often
than the control group. However, they did not notice the pupil size difference between
pictures. Thus, the way a leader looks at and to his or her followers subconsciously
influences the followers’ performance.
Also, leaders’ NVB can affect followers’ satisfaction, motivation, and performance
(Tjosvold, 1984). In a laboratory study (Tjosvold, 1984), participants interacted with a
leader in order to complete a task. The leader was either directive or non-directive and
behaved in a nonverbally cold or warm manner. Cold NVB consisted of a tough voice,
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 14
smiling avoidance, stiff facial expression, greater interpersonal distance, and eye contact
avoidance, whereas warm NVB included soft and audible tone of voice, smiling, friendly
facial expression, closer interpersonal distance, and direct eye contact. Results showed that
participants who interacted with a warm leader were satisfied with the leader, perceived the
leader as helpful, wanted to work again with the leader, and wanted to meet the leader
socially. Moreover, leaders’ warm NVB coupled with directive instructions increased
followers’ productivity, whereas leaders’ warm NVB coupled with non-directive
instructions decreased followers’ productivity. In the same vein, Gaddis, Connelly, and
Mumford (2004) demonstrated that after a failure feedback situation in which leaders
delivered the feedback in a positive and supportive way (i.e., calm voice and smile), teams
performed better on the task than did teams whose leaders displayed negative affect (i.e.,
tense voice, negative tone of voice). In a recent experimental study, Talley (2012)
demonstrated that attraction or repulsion toward a leader can be determined by the leader’s
hand gestures displayed during a speech. Participants watched a video of a leader using
different hand gestures: positive (humility, community, and steepling hands), defensive
(hands behind back or in pocket, or crossed arms), and no hand gestures. Results showed
that participants perceived positive and defensive hand gestures as more immediate than no
hand gestures, which were perceived as distancing. Moreover, leaders with positive hand
gestures were perceived as more attractive than leaders with defensive and no hand
gestures.
The way leaders use NVB to influence and guide their followers can be explained
by the emotional contagion process (see Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994). This process
refers to the follower’s tendency to automatically imitate and synchronize with the facial
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 15
expression, postures, tone of voice, or body movements of the leader, mostly
unconsciously. This results in emotional convergence between the leader and the follower,
whereby the follower actually feels the mimicked expressions. Empirical evidence supports
this. Sullivan and Masters (1988) showed videotaped excerpts of political candidates to
participants. The candidates displayed either happy/reassuring (e.g., raised eyebrows,
smiles) or neutral facial expressions. Results indicated that changes in participants’
attitudes of political support (i.e., measure of warmth toward the candidate) were more
likely to be influenced by the emotional responses to happy displays than by party
identification or assessment of leadership skills. More recently, Cherulnik and colleagues
(Cherulnik, Donley, Wiewel, & Miller, 2001) found that followers imitated the nonverbal
cues (e.g., smiles) emanated by charismatic leaders during their talks, whereas followers
did not imitate the cues of non-charismatic leaders.
We can conclude that leader NVB affects leadership effectiveness most likely
through an interactive process between leader expressive NVB and followers’ perception of
and imitation thereof. Although there is no simple and easy recipe for leadership
effectiveness (Eden et al., 2000; White & Locke, 2000), we suggest in the next section that
leaders be trained in nonverbal communication in order to maximize their impact on
followers.
Importance of Leader NVB for Leadership Outcomes
As mentioned in the introduction, effective leaders need specific interpersonal
skills, and NVB is an important part of the interpersonal skills that lead to effective
leadership. In this section, we provide some tips on how leaders can be trained to improve
their nonverbal encoding and decoding skills in order to be effective.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 16
There is evidence that leaders can improve their nonverbal expression of leadership
through training (e.g., Frese, Beimel, & Schoenborn, 2003; Taylor, 2002; Towler, 2003;
Vrij & Mann, 2005). Training of charismatic nonverbal communication (e.g., facial
expressions, body gestures, eye contact, and animated voice tone) and of visionary or
inspirational verbal content communication (e.g., articulating a vision, using metaphors)
both showed an increase in leadership effectiveness. For example, in a study by Towler
(2003), participants who received charismatic leadership training exhibited more
charismatic behaviors and influenced followers to perform better on a task. In the same
vein, such training successfully developed a range of NVB — using gestures, variation of
speech, increased speech speed and loudness — that lead to charismatic leadership
behavior (Frese et al., 2003). Using a similar approach to the two aforementioned studies,
Antonakis, Fenley, and Liechti (2011) also demonstrated in two studies that charismatic
leadership training influenced evaluations of leader charisma positively. The results from
these studies suggest that charismatic NVB is an acquirable skill.
The skill to accurately decode subtle nonverbal cues is also important for leaders to
possess, not only to understand the messages sent by the followers, but also for building
rapport and for being responsive to the needs of followers. There is evidence showing that
leaders might be more skilled in correctly assessing others’ states and traits based on
observing others’ NVB than are followers (Schmid Mast, Jonas, & Hall, 2009). Moreover,
accurate assessment of others by leaders is related to positive leadership outcomes, such as
increases in follower satisfaction (Byron, 2007; Schmid Mast, Jonas, Klöckner Cronauer, &
Darioly, 2012). Although not much is known about the possibility of training leaders’
nonverbal decoding skills, Costanzo’s (1992) findings suggest that leaders’ NVB decoding
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 17
skills can be improved. The author conducted a study in which participants received either
an informational lecture on verbal and nonverbal cues, or training in detecting relevant cues
in filmed interactions. In the latter condition, participants watched videotaped excerpts of
social interactions and were asked to judge, for instance, the type of relationship among the
social interaction partners. Then, the correct answer and the specific nonverbal cues
indicative of the correct answer for each scene were pointed out to the participants. Results
indicated that only participants who received the detection training significantly improved
their skills to correctly interpret NVB.
It seems that it is possible to train leaders and NVB training for leaders is beneficial
for leadership effectiveness. Riggio and colleagues (Riggio, 1989; Riggio & Carney, 2003;
Riggio & Reichard, 2008; Riggio et al., 2003) highlight that feedback is important in order
to improve skills in nonverbal decoding and encoding. Leaders can become more aware of
their own NVB as well as that of their followers.
Leadership, Nonverbal Behavior, and Individual Differences
Considering leadership as an interactive dynamic between a leader and a follower, it
is relevant to take into account the individual characteristics that might have an impact on
this dynamic. We will discuss gender, cultural background, and other individual differences
that can affect the leadership-NVB link.
Gender and Leadership
Research shows that the gender of the leader plays a significant role in the
leadership context. The perception of leadership through NVB might vary according to the
gender of the leader. On the one hand, leadership is inferred from different NVB for female
and male leaders. Perceivers rely more on downward head tilt and lowered eyebrows when
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 18
assessing the leadership position of women than when assessing leadership in men (Schmid
Mast & Hall, 2004b). On the other hand, the same behavior exhibited by a female or male
leader results in different perceptions. Women using more eye contact, gesturing, smiling,
animated facial expressions, and variations in pitch are seen as more charismatic than men
showing the same NVB (Bass & Avolio, 1989).
On the other hand, women and men exhibit different NVB in leadership positions.
In a leadership position, men use more expansive body positions, speak more, use a louder
voice, and interrupt others more frequently than do women (Hall, 2006). However, female
leaders have more expressive faces and maintain closer interpersonal distance than do male
leaders (Hall, 2006).
Finally, the same NVB exhibited by female leaders and by male leaders affects
followers differently. Assertive and directive behaviors (e.g., speaking first or responding
quickly in conversation) are perceived more favorably in male than in female leaders
(Eagly & Karau, 2002). This suggests that gender-congruent NVB affects the leader
perception positively and gender-incongruent NVB hurts the leader.
Although we focus on leader gender, follower gender can also be a moderator of
how male or female leaders behave nonverbally and how they are perceived based on their
exhibited NVB. Research in leadership emergence suggests that the interaction between the
perceiver’s gender and the target’s gender influences how people infer leadership. For
example, the cue of sitting at the end of the table held for leadership emergence (e.g.,
Heckel, 1973; Porter & Geis, 1981; Ward, 1968), but when individuals have the choice
between a man and woman seated at each end of the table, they tended to choose a person
of their own sex as leader (Jackson, Engstrom, & Emmers-Sommer, 2007). In regard to
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 19
leadership position, when female leaders exhibit upright posture, high speech rate,
moderate eye contact while speaking, few vocal hesitations, and calm restrained hand
gestures, they influence male followers less than a male leader exhibiting similar NVB
(Carli, LaFleur, & Loeber, 1995). Female leaders who exhibit the above-mentioned NVB
were also perceived as less likable by male followers in comparison to men exhibiting
similar NVB. However, female leaders who exhibited the above-mentioned NVB did not
have a differential effect on female followers. For women followers, the woman leader’s
nonverbal cues did not affect how much followers were influenced or their liking for the
leader (Carli et al., 1995).
To conclude, the relationship between leadership, NVB, and gender is complex and
multifaceted. Depending on the interaction between gender and leadership position, men or
women express different kinds of NVB, which affect the way others perceive the NVB
expressed by them.
Cultural Background and Leadership
The relationship between NVB and the cultural background of the followers or
leaders is also relevant to leadership. Cultural background affects how leadership is
perceived and expressed. NVB takes on shared meaning in a specific cultural setting
(Knapp & Hall, 2010). For example, a Japanese leader may interact at a more pronounced
interpersonal distance compared to an American leader, and so, cultural differences in NVB
between leaders and followers might result in misunderstandings. Some authors
(Matsumoto, 1990, 1991) suggest that persons from individualistic cultures (e.g., the
United States) express feelings more openly and tend to be more nonverbally demonstrative
than individuals of collectivistic cultures (e.g., China). Moreover, people of individualistic
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 20
cultures tend to be more accurate in decoding subtle nonverbal cues (e.g., Beck, Bröske,
Koster, Menzel, & Mohr, 2003; Hofstede, 2001; Matsumoto et al., 2002) and there is a
cultural in-group advantage at correctly assessing others’ emotions (Elfenbein, Beaupré,
Lévesque, & Hess, 2007), despite emotion recognition being universal (Ekman, 1994). The
Global Leadership and Organizational Behavior Effectiveness study (GLOBE; House,
Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman, & Gupta, 2004), a project that included 62 cultures,
demonstrated that there are both universal characteristics and significant cultural
differences concerning leadership. While charismatic leadership is preferred in many
cultures, the overall behaviors associated with leadership and the expected behaviors from
leaders may be dissimilar. For example, in her study, Gaal (2007) examined the relationship
between charismatic/transformational leadership, NVB, and culture. Two cultures were
observed: The United States (low on power distance) and Hungary (high on power
distance). A male actor was asked to recite a charismatic speech in three different ways:
reserved, orchestrated, and aggressive. In the reserved scenario, the actor had a monotone
voice, did not look at the camera or move his arms. In the orchestrated scenario, the actor
was dynamic, with an animated voice, a natural eye contact with the camera, and with his
palms open. In the aggressive scenario, the actor yelled and showed intense emotions
during his speech, maintained direct eye contact with the camera and used his arms or
hands to point or knock on the podium. Participants randomly watched one of the three
scenarios. The NVB displayed by the leader was perceived differently by observers in the
United States than by observers in Hungary. For both countries, there was a positive
relationship between the orchestrated NVB and charismatic leadership characteristics (i.e.,
vision, inspiration, and trustworthiness) and a negative relationship between the reserved
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 21
NVB and charismatic leadership characteristics. However, the aggressive scenario was
perceived as more detrimental for the perception of charismatic leadership characteristics in
the United States than in Hungary compared to the two other scenarios. In the same vein,
Matsumoto (1990) studied displayed emotions in Americans and Japanese people.
Participants saw faces portraying emotions and assessed the suitability of each in different
social situations such as in interactions between a leader and a follower. Results showed
that on the one hand, the Japanese found it suitable to express negative emotions (e.g.,
anger) toward followers because the expression of such behavior serves to maintain the
existing, culturally grounded power distance. On the other hand, the Americans
discouraged leader displays of negative emotions to followers because these emphasize
status differences, which is contradictory to the American culture of equality.
Although the impact of cultural difference in NVB expressed by leaders has not
been covered in great detail, the research demonstrates that leaders may be perceived
differently in one culture than another and what is “acceptable” leader NVB may be
culturally dependent.
Other Characteristics and Leadership
There is an almost endless list of other characteristics that do or potentially could
affect the NVB-leadership relationship. For example, expressions and perceptions of
leadership may differ in important ways depending on the individual’s social motives (e.g.,
goals, desires) or on his or her emotional state (e.g., happiness, anger). A leader who argues
with a follower about respecting a deadline might behave differently than a leader who
wants to fire an ineffective follower. Moreover, smiling in a situation of crisis may be
regarded as sarcastic rather than supportive. This idea is supported by Bucy (2000), who
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 22
showed that leaders were assessed more favorably when the NVB they demonstrated was
considered compatible with the message they conveyed.
The nature of the relationship the individual has with others (e.g., new or well-
known followers, colleagues, leader, or clients) might also affect the NVB-leadership
relationship. Cashdan (1998) demonstrated that, in discussions, female and male leaders
showed differences in NVB depending on whether they were with acquaintances or
strangers. Female and male leaders spoke more in discussions with strangers than in
discussions with acquaintances. Female leaders had more open body postures; in particular,
their legs were more open in discussions with strangers than in discussions with
acquaintances. Male leaders smiled less in discussions with strangers than in discussions
with acquaintances.
Personality is certainly another important factor. For example, extraversion and
dominance affect emergent leadership. Extraversion refers to a predisposition to be
outgoing, active, or assertive (Judge & Bono, 2000) and the personality trait of dominance
refers to a predisposition to try to influence others (Ellyson & Dovidio, 1985). There is
evidence indicating that personality influences sitting positions. Extraverts tend to choose
seating positions that put them in the focus of the others (Cook, 1970) which then, as we
have discussed earlier, increases the chances for those people to emerge as leaders. In the
same vein, Hare and Bales (1963) noted that people at the head, foot, or center of the table
were likely to have dominant personalities. Kalma, Visser, and Peeters (1993) demonstrated
that in an emergent leadership situation, individuals who scored higher on sociable
dominance (i.e., high self-esteem, positive attitudes toward others, a central position in
groups, a strong need to influence others, and an independent and active attitude) or
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 23
aggressive dominance (negative attitudes toward others and a strong motivation to realize
one’s goals, even at the detriment of personal relationships) emerge as leaders with sociably
dominant individuals being chosen more frequently as group leaders than aggressively
dominant individuals. Moreover, sociably dominant individuals behaved differently from
aggressive dominant individuals in that they looked at others more while speaking, had
more eye contact, and used more gestures. Aggressively dominant individuals looked at
others less while listening and interrupted more.
Some of the discussed characteristics can interact with each other and affect the
NVB-leadership relationship. Not much research has looked at such complex patterns. One
example is a study showing that leader gender interacted with dominance and leadership
position in predicting NVB. In non-leadership positions, women who were high in
dominance smiled less than women who were low in dominance, while no such effect
emerged for men (Schmid Mast & Hall, 2004a). There is clearly more research needed to
address such complex interplays.
Conclusion and Outlook
The aim of this chapter is to better understand the role of NVB in leadership by
showing that leader NVB is an important means for framing the relationship between
leaders and followers, and for effective leadership. Differences in NVB among group
members are part of the basis on which leaders emerge in groups. Moreover, followers use
different leader NVB to judge and evaluate their leaders. The NVB that leaders exhibit is
linked to their leadership styles, and leader NVB impacts (most of the time unconsciously)
leadership effectiveness. Knowing which leader NVB is related to better or worse
leadership outcomes is beneficial because it allows for the training of leaders. Leader
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 24
interpersonal skill training can make leaders aware of their own NVB and provide them the
tools to adapt to others’ NVB. This awareness and adaptability is necessary in order to be
effective.
Organizations that want to improve should be interested in NVB training for their
leaders because it potentially increases leadership effectiveness. Moreover, knowing that
NVB plays a primary role when dissonance occurs between verbal and nonverbal behavior
may help in understanding the demands of leadership in organizations. Leaders are often
required to show different emotions than those they actually feel. For example, during
times of crisis, leaders might more easily find the right words rather than the right NVB to
support their followers. However, they need to display NVB indicative of confidence and
optimism even if they are as worried and anxious as their followers. Thus it is important
that leaders be trained in the context of “emotional labor” (i.e., leaders are expected to
display certain emotions as part of their leadership position; Humphrey, Pollack, &
Hawver, 2008).
Research in nonverbal communication and leadership is still scarce. It might be
relevant to know which NVB are more or less related to interpersonal skills in order to
achieve a better focus in leader NVB training. For example, is touching more related to
emotional or social skills according to Riggio’s Social Skills Inventory (Riggio, this
volume; Riggio & Carney, 2003)? Moreover, much of the research focuses on the NVB of
effective leaders, but we need to identify the NVB related to destructive and “toxic” leaders
in order to know their effects and avoid them.
Regarding the methods used, research has tested a range of diverse NVB in relation
to constructive leadership, but these typically remain on a correlational and descriptive
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 25
level. We need to better understand the effects of the interactions and combinations of
different NVB (e.g., touching and smiling) in order to know their effects and see whether
they are perceived as effective (i.e., similar effects as incongruent verbal and nonverbal
behavior). Additionally, analyzing mediators of the expression or perception of leadership
(e.g., perceived competence or perceived self-confidence) are needed to better understand
why NVB is used to convey or to infer leadership. Finally, methodological innovations in
the study of NVB and leadership are needed. For example, computer-mediated automatic
coding of NVB related to emergent leadership is being developed and might facilitate the
work of researchers (Sanchez-Cortes, Aran, Schmid Mast, & Gatica-Perez, 2011).
Another important area that deserves the attention of researchers is how leaders and
followers cope with the absence of some NVB in virtual teams. These specific teams use
computer-mediated communication (CMC). CMC includes a variety of electronic message
systems that can be supplemented by audio and video links. Examples of CMC are email,
chat, or video-conference. It is well-established that there is little or no NVB in most of
CMC (e.g., Kiesler, Siegel, & McGuire, 1984; Walther, 1996), except for video-conference,
but even in this situation NVB is limited to some extent. Because one of the functions of
NVB is to reduce the ambiguity of a message, there is a high probability for
misinterpretation in CMC (Sanderson, 1993). Interestingly, individuals create a number of
strategies to compensate for the lack of NVB in CMC. Most notable is the use of
“emoticons” – smiley-faced characters used to express emotions (Walther & D'Addario,
2001). Additionally, it has been suggested that individuals may become more precise in
their use of words to more clearly communicate emotions in CMC (Newlands, Anderson, &
Mullin, 2003). Research on emergent leadership in virtual teams demonstrated that
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 26
emergent leaders sent more and longer email messages than their team members did (Yoo
& Alavi, 2004), suggesting that they act similarly to emergent leaders in face-to-face teams
who speak more (Schmid Mast, 2002).
In conclusion, additional research is needed and leader NVB training is important to
reach individual, leadership, and organizational effectiveness. Thus, the future for NVB
research in the leadership context and for leader development seems encouraging. This
integrative review on the role of NVB in leadership provides organizations with evidence
that NVB greatly influences the attribution of leadership characteristics and may be trained
in order to improve interpersonal skills.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 27
References
Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Rosenthal, R. (1995). On judging and being judged
accurately in zero-acquaintance situations. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 69(3), 518-529.
Antonakis, J., Fenley, M., & Liechti, S. (2011). Can charisma be taught? Tests of two
interventions. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 10(3), 374-396.
Awamleh, R. A. (1997). Charismatic leadership: The effects of vision content, vision
delivery, organizational performance, and the romance of leadership on
perceptions of charisma and effectiveness. Dissertation, University of Mississippi,
USA.
Awamleh, R. A., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Perceptions of leader charisma and
effectiveness: The effects of vision content, delivery, and organizational
performance. The Leadership Quarterly, 10(3), 345-373.
Baird, J. E. (1977). Some non-verbal elements of leadership emergence. Southern Speech
Communication Journal, 42(4), 352-361.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New York: Free
Press.
Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass and Stogdill’s handbook of leadership: Theory, research, and
managerial applications. New York: Free Press.
Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1989). Potential biases in leadership measures: How
prototypes, leniency, and general satisfaction relate to ratings and rankings of
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 28
transformational and transactional leadership constructs. Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 49(3), 509-527. doi: 10.1177/001316448904900302
Bass, B. M., & Bass, R. B. (2008). The Bass handbook of leadership: Theory, research,
and managerial applications (4th ed.). New York: The Free Press.
Beck, J., Bröske, A., Koster, I., Menzel, A., & Mohr, G. (2003). Die Bedeutung kultureller
Orientierung und Persönlichkeit für die Dekodierungsfähigkeit im Rahmen
interpersonaler Wahrnehmung [The importance of cultural orientation and
personality for decoding abilities in the context of interpersonal perception].
Unpublished Working Paper. Department of Work and Organizational Psychology.
University of Leipzig.
Berger, J., Conner, T. L., & Fisek, H. (1974). Expectation States Theory: A theoretical
research program. New York: Elsevier.
Berger, J., Fisek, M. H., Norman, R. Z., & Zelditch, M., Jr. (1977). Status characteristics in
social interaction: An expectation states approach. New York: Elsevier.
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative: Design of psychological
experiments. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Bucy, E. P. (2000). Emotional and evaluative consequences of inappropriate leader
displays. Communication Research, 27, 194-229.
Burgoon, J. K., Birk, T., & Pfau, M. (1990). Nonverbal behaviors, persuasion, and
credibility. Human Communication Research, 17(1), 140-169. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-
2958.1990.tb00229.x
Byron, K. (2007). Male and female managers' ability to read emotions: Relationships with
supervisor's performance ratings and subordinates' satisfaction ratings. Journal of
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 29
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 80(4), 713-733. doi:
10.1348/096317907x174349
Carli, L. L., LaFleur, S. J., & Loeber, C. C. (1995). Nonverbal behavior, gender, and
influence. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 68(6), 1030-1041.
Cashdan, E. (1998). Smiles, speech, and body posture: How women and men display
sociometric status and power. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22(4), 209-228. doi:
10.1023/a:1022967721884
Cherulnik, P. D., Donley, K. A., Wiewel, T. S. R., & Miller, S. R. (2001). Charisma is
contagious: The effect of leaders’ charisma on observers’ affect. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 31(10), 2149-2159. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb00167.x
Cook, M. (1970). Experiments on orientation and proxemics. Human Relations, 23(1), 61-
76. doi: 10.1177/001872677002300107
Costanzo, M. (1992). Training students to decode verbal and nonverbal cues: Effects on
confidence and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(3), 308-313.
Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interpreting the expressive behavior of others: The
Interpersonal Perception Task. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13(4), 225-245.
Crane, J., & Crane, F. G. (2010). Optimal nonverbal communications strategies physicians
should engage in to promote positive clinical outcomes. Health Marketing
Quarterly, 27(3), 262-274. doi: 10.1080/07359683.2010.495300
DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Nonverbal communication. In D. Gilbert, S.
Fiske & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 3-40).
Boston, NY: McGraw Hill.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 30
Dovidio, J. F., & Ellyson, S. L. (1982). Decoding visual dominance: Attributions of power
based on relative percentages of looking while speaking and looking while listening.
Social Psychology Quartely, 45(2), 106-113.
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female
leaders. Psychological Review, 109(3), 573-598.
Eden, D. (1990). Pygmalion in management. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Eden, D., Geller, D., Gewirtz, A., Gordon-Terner, R., Inbar, I., Liberman, M., . . . Shalit,
M. (2000). Implanting Pygmalion leadership style through workshop training:
Seven field experiments. The Leadership Quarterly, 11(2), 171-210.
Einarsen, S., Aasland, M. S., & Skogstad, A. ( 2007). Destructive leadership behaviour: A
definition and conceptual model. The Leadership Quarterly, 18, 207-216.
Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply to Russell’s
mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 268-287.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C. (2002). Facial action coding system: The
manual. USA: Research Nexus division of Network Information Research
Corporation.
Elfenbein, H. A., Beaupré, M., Lévesque, M., & Hess, U. (2007). Toward a dialect theory:
Cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions.
Emotion, 7(1), 131-146.
Ellyson, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1985). Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior: Basic
concepts and issues. In S. L. Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance, and
nonverbal behavior (pp. 1-27). New York: Springer.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 31
Exline, R. V., Ellyson, S. L., & Long, B. D. (1975). Visual behavior as an aspect of power
role relationships. In P. Pliner, L. Krames & T. Alloway (Eds.), Advances in the
Study of Communication and Affect (pp. 21-52). New York: Plenum.
Frese, M., Beimel, S., & Schoenborn, S. (2003). Action training for charismatic leadership:
Two evaluations of studies of a commercial training module on inspirational
communication of a vision. Personnel Psychology, 56(3), 671-698.
Friedman, H. S., Prince, L. M., Riggio, R. E., & DiMatteo, M. R. (1980). Understanding
and assessing nonverbal expressiveness: The Affective Communication Test.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(2), 333-351. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.39.2.333
Friedman, H. S., & Riggio, R. E. (1981). Effect of individual differences in nonverbal
expressiveness on transmission of emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6(2),
96-104.
Gaal, M. A. (2007). Cross-cultural comparison of nonverbal communication, culture, and
the attribution of charismatic leadership among Hungarian and American
university students. PhD, Walden University, ProQuest Dissertations and Theses.
Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/304768522?accountid=10141
(304768522)
Gaddis, B., Connelly, S., & Mumford, M. D. (2004). Failure feedback as an affective event:
Influences of leader affect on subordinate attitudes and performance. The
Leadership Quarterly, 15(5), 663-686. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2004.05.011
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 32
Gardner, W. L., & Avolio, B. J. (1998). The charismatic relationship: A dramaturgical
perspective. Academy of Management Review, 23(1), 32-58.
Gray, J. H., & Densten, I. L. (2007). How leaders woo followers in the romance of
leadership. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 56(4), 558-581.
Groves, K. S. (2006). Leader emotional expressivity, visionary leadership, and
organizational change. Leadership and Organization Development Journal, 27(7),
566 - 583. doi: 10.1108/01437730610692425
Guastello, S. J. (2002). Managing Emergent Phenomena. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hall, J. A. (2006). Nonverbal behavior, status, and gender: How do we understand their
relations? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30(4), 384-391. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-
6402.2006.00313.x
Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & Smith LeBeau, L. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical
dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 898-
924. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.898
Hare, A. P., & Bales, R. F. (1963). Seating position and small group interaction.
Sociometry, 26, 480-486.
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Heckel, R. V. (1973). Leadership and voluntary seating choice. Psychological Reports, 32,
141-142.
Hofstede, G. (Ed.). (2001). Culture’s consequences. Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hollander, E. P., & Julian, J. W. (1969). Contemporary trends in the analysis of leadership
processes. Psychological Bulletin, 71(5), 387-397.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 33
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. (2004). Culture,
leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies. London, UK:
Sage.
Howell, J. M., & Frost, P. J. (1989). A laboratory study of charismatic leadership.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 43(2), 243 - 269. doi:
10.1016/0749-5978(89)90052-6
Humphrey, R., Pollack, J. M., & Hawver, T. H. (2008). Leading with emotional labor.
Journal of Managerial Psychology, 23, 151-168.
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581-
592. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581
Jackson, D., Engstrom, E., & Emmers-Sommer, T. (2007). Think leader, think male and
female: Sex vs. seating arrangement as leadership cues. Sex Roles, 57(9), 713-723.
doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9289-y
Janisse, M. P. (1973). Pupil size and affect: A critical review of the literature since 1960.
Canadian Psychologist/Psychologie canadienne, 14(4), 311-329. doi:
10.1037/h0082230
Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2000). Five-factor model of personality and transformational
leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(5), 751-765. doi: 10.1037/0021-
9010.85.5.751
Kaiser, R. B., Hogan, R., & Craig, S. B. (2008). Leadership and the fate of organizations.
American Psychologist, 63(2), 96-110.
Kalma, A. P., & van Rooij, J. (1982). Dominantie en interactieregulering: het kijkgedrag
van invloedrijke personen [Dominance and regulation of interaction: Looking
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 34
behaviour of influential persons]. Nederlandse Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie en
haar Grensgebieden, 37, 431-443.
Kalma, A. P., Visser, L., & Peeters, A. (1993). Sociable and aggressive dominance :
Personality differences in leadership style ? The Leadership Quarterly, 4(1), 45-64.
doi: 10.1016/1048-9843(93)90003-C
Kiesler, S., Siegel, J., & McGuire, T. W. (1984). Social psychological aspects of computer-
mediated communication. American Psychologist, 39(10), 1123-1134.
King, A. S. (1971). Self-fulfilling prophecies in training the hard-core: Supervisor’s
expectations and the underprivileged worker’s performance. Social Science
Quarterly, 52, 369-378.
Knapp, M. L., & Hall, J. A. (Eds.). (2010). Nonverbal communication in human interaction
(7th ed.). Boston, MA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.
Lipman-Blumen, J. (2005). The allure of toxic leaders: Why we follow destructive bosses
and corrupt politicians and how we can survive them. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Lord, R. G., de Vader, C. L., & Alliger, G. M. (1986). A meta-analysis of the relation
between personality traits and leadership perceptions: An application of validity
generalization procedures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71(3), 402-410. doi:
10.1037/0021-9010.71.3.402
Lord, R. G., Foti, R. J., & Phillips, J. S. (1982). A theory of leadership categorization. In J.
G. Hunt, U. Sekaran & C. A. Schriesheim (Eds.), Leadership: Beyond establishment
views. Carbondale: Southern Illinois: University Press.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 35
Lowe, K. B., Kroeck, K. G., & Sivasubramaniam, N. (1996). Effectiveness correlates of
transformational and transactional leadership: A meta analytic review. The
Leadership Quarterly, 7(3), 385.
Matsumoto, D. (1990). Cultural similarities and differences in display rules. Motivation and
Emotion, 14, 195-214.
Matsumoto, D. (1991). Cultural influences on facial expression of emotion. Southern
Communication Journal, 56, 128-137.
Matsumoto, D., Consolacion, T., Yamada, H., Suzuki, R., Franklin, B., Paul, S., . . .
Uchida, H. (2002). American-Japanese cultural differences in judgement of
emotional expressions of different intensities. Cognition and Emotion, 16(6), 721-
747.
Maurer, T. J., & Lord, R. G. (1991). An exploration of cognitive demands in group
interaction as a moderator of information processing variables in perceptions of
leadership. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21(10), 821-839. doi:
10.1111/j.1559-1816.1991.tb00445.x
Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal communication. Chicago, IL: Aldine-Atherton.
Mehrabian, A., & Wiener, M. (1967). Decoding of inconsistent communications. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 6(1), 109-114. doi: 10.1037/h0024532
Mintzberg, H. (1973). The nature of managerial work. New York: Harper & Row.
Moore, H. A., & Porter, N. K. (1988). Leadership and nonverbal behaviors of Hispanic
females across school equity environments. [Article]. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 12(2), 147.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 36
Newlands, A., Anderson, A. H., & Mullin, J. (2003). Adapting communicative strategies to
computer-mediated communication: An analysis of task performance and dialogue
structure. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(3), 325-348.
Northouse, G. (2007). Leadership theory and practice (4th ed). Thousand Oak, CA: Sage
Publications.
Nye, J. L. (2002). The eye of the follower-information processing effects on attribution
regarding leaders of small groups. Small Group Research, 33(3), 337-360.
Nye, J. L., & Forsyth, D. R. (1991). The effects of prototype-based biases on leadership
appraisals. Small Group Research, 22(3), 360-379. doi:
10.1177/1046496491223005
Pfeffer, J. (1977). The ambiguity of leadership. Academy of Management Review, 2(1),
104-112.
Porter, N., & Geis, F. L. (1981). Women and nonverbal leadership cues: When seeing is not
believing. In C. Mayo & N. Henley (Eds.), Gender and nonverbal behavior (pp. 39-
61). New York: Springer Verlag.
Reichard, R. J., & Riggio, R. E. (2008). An interactive process model of emotions and
leadership. In C. L. Cooper & N. Ashkanasy (Eds.), Research companion to
emotions in organizations. Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Publishers.
Remland, M. (1981). Developing leadership skills in nonverbal communication: A
situational perspective. Journal of Business Communication, 18(3), 17-29.
Remland, M., Jacobson, C., & Jones, T. (1983). Effects of psychological gender and sex-
incongruent behavior on evaluations of leadership. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
57(3), 783-789.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 37
Ridgeway, C. L., & Berger, J. (1986). Expectations, legitimation, and dominance behavior
in task groups. American Sociological Review, 51(5), 603- 617.
Riggio, R. E. (1986). Assessment of basic social skills. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 51(3), 649-660.
Riggio, R. E. (1989). Manual of the Social Skills Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Riggio, R. E. (1998). Charisma. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), Encyclopedia of mental health
(pp. 387-396). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Riggio, R. E. (2006). Nonverbal skills and abilities. In V. Manusov & M. L. Patterson
(Eds.), The SAGE handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 79-95). Thousand
Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Riggio, R. E. (this volume). A social skills model for understanding the foundations of
leader communication.
Riggio, R. E., & Carney, D. C. (2003). Manual for the Social Skills Inventory (2nd ed.).
Montain View, CA: Mind Garden.
Riggio, R. E., & Reichard, R. J. (2008). The emotional and social intelligences of effective
leadership: An emotional and social skill approach. Journal of Managerial
Psychology, 23(2), 169-185.
Riggio, R. E., Riggio, H. R., Salinas, C., & Cole, E. J. (2003). The role of social and
emotional communication skills in leader emergence and effectiveness. Group
Dynamics: Theory, Research, & Practice, 7(2), 83-103.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 38
Sanchez-Cortes, D., Aran, O., Schmid Mast, M., & Gatica-Perez, D. (2011). A nonverbal
behavior approach to identify emergent leaders in small groups. IEEE transactions
on multimedia. doi: 10.1109/TMM.2011.2181941
Sanderson, D. W. (1993). Smileys. Sebastopol, CA: O’Reilly.
Savvas, T., & Schyns, B. (2012). The face of leadership: Perceiving leaders from facial
expression. The Leadership Quarterly, 23(3), 545-566. doi:
10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.12.007
Schmid Mast, M. (2002). Dominance as expressed and inferred through speaking time: A
meta-analysis. Human Communication Research, 28(3), 420-450.
Schmid Mast, M. (2010). Interpersonal behavior and social perception in a hierarchy: The
Interpersonal Power and Behavior Model. European Review of Social Psychology,
21(1), 1-33.
Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2004a). When is dominance related to smiling? Assigned
dominance, dominance preference, trait dominance, and gender as moderators. Sex
Roles: A journal of research, 50(5/6), 387-399.
Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2004b). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of
judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 145-165. doi:
10.1023/b:jonb.0000039647.94190.21
Schmid Mast, M., Jonas, K., & Hall, J. A. (2009). Give a person power and he or she will
show interpersonal sensitivity: The phenomenon and its why and when. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5), 851-865. doi: 10.1037/a0016234
Schmid Mast, M., Jonas, K., Klöckner Cronauer, C., & Darioly, A. (2012). On the
importance of the superior's interpersonal sensitivity for good leadership. Journal of
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 39
Applied Social Psychology, 42(5), 1043-1068. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-
1816.2011.00852.x
Schmidt Mast, M., Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., & Colvin, C. R. (2003). Judging
assertiveness. Facta Universitatis, 2(10), 731- 744.
Schyns, B. (2001). The relationship between employees’ self-monitoring and occupational
self-efficacy and transformational leadership. Current Research in Social
Psychology, 7(3), 30-42.
Schyns, B., Felfe, J., & Blank, H. (2007). Is charisma hyper-romanticism? Empirical
evidence from new data and a meta-analysis. Applied Psychology: An International
Review, 56(4), 505-527.
Schyns, B., & Schilling, J. (2010). Implicit Leadership Theories: Think leader, think
effective? Journal of Management, 20, 141-150. doi: 10.1177/1056492610375989
Schyns, B., & Schilling, J. (2012). How bad are the effects of bad leaders? A meta-analysis
of destructive leadership and its outcomes. The Leadership Quarterly. Retrieved
from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1048984312000872
doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.09.001
Shamir, B., House, R. J., & Arthur, M. B. (1993). The motivational effects of charismatic
leadership: A self-concept based theory. Organization Science, 4(4), 577-594.
Shea, C. M., & Howell, J. M. (1999). Charismatic leadership and task feedback: A
laboratory study of their effects on self-efficacy and performance. The Leadership
Quarterly, 10(3), 375-396.
Stang, D. J. (1973). Effect of interaction rate on ratings of leadership and liking. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 27(3), 405-408. doi: 10.1037/h0034940
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 40
Stein, R. T. (1975). Identifying emergent leaders from verbal and nonverbal
communications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(1), 125-135.
doi: 10.1037/h0076842
Stein, R. T., & Heller, T. (1979). An empirical analysis of the correlations between
leadership status and participation rates reported in the literature. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 37(11), 1993-2002. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.37.11.1993
Stogdill, R. M. (1950). Leadership, membership and organization. Psychological Bulletin,
47(1), 1-14. doi: 10.1037/h0053857
Sullivan, D. G., & Masters, R. D. (1988). "Happy Warriors": Leaders' facial displays,
viewers' emotions, and political support. American Journal of Political Science,
32(2), 345-368.
Sutton, C. D., & Woodman, R. W. (1989). Pygmalion goes to work: The effects of
supervisor expectations in a retail setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(6),
943-950. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.74.6.943
Talley, L., L. (2012). Influence of leader hand gestures on the perception of nonverbal
immediacy. PhD Doctoral dissertation, Walden University. (3527702)
Taylor, S. J. (2002). Effects of a nonverbal skill training program on perceptions of
personal charisma. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California,
Riverside, CA.
Tepper, B. J. (2000). Consequences of abusive supervision. Academy of Management
Journal, 43(2), 178-190.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 41
Tjosvold, D. (1984). Effects of leader warmth and directiveness on subordinate
performance on a subsequent task. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69(3), 422-427.
doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.69.3.422
Towler, A. (2003). Effects of charismatic influence training on attitudes, behavior, and
performance. Personnel Psychology, 56(2), 363-381. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-
6570.2003.tb00154.x
Uhl-Bien, M. (2004). Relationship development as a key ingredient for leadership
development. In S. E. Murphy & R. E. Riggio (Eds.), The future of leadership
development (pp. 129-147). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2005). Police use of nonverbal behavior as indicators of deception. In
R. E. Riggio & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), Applications of nonverbal communication.
Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and
hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23(1), 3-43.
Walther, J. B., & D'Addario, K. P. (2001). The impact of emoticons on message
interpretation in computer-mediated communication. Social Science Computer
Review, 19, 324-347.
Ward, C. D. (1968). Seating arrangement and leadership emergence in small discussion
groups. Journal of Social Psychology, 74(1), 83-90.
Weber, M. (1980, original 1921). Wirtschaft und Gesellschaft [Economy and Society].
Tuebingen, Germany: Mohr.
White, S. S., & Locke, E. A. (2000). Problems with the Pygmalion effect and some
proposed solutions. The Leadership Quarterly, 11(3), 389-415.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 42
Yoo, Y., & Alavi, M. (2004). Emergent leadership in virtual teams: what do emergent
leaders do? Information and Organization, 14(1), 27-58. doi:
10.1016/j.infoandorg.2003.11.001
Yukl, G. A. (2010). Leadership in organizations (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson
Education, Inc.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 43
Footnotes
1. Hall and colleagues’ meta-analysis also includes studies with personality dominance
(single target). In this chapter, we only took the results on emergent leadership and
perceived dominance (group interaction).
2. Whereas originally, charisma referred to attributes of leaders (Weber, 1980, original
1921), modern research focuses on the behavioral side of charisma, which is
represented in the notion of transformational leadership (Bass, 1985). Charismatic
and transformational leadership both refer to the same phenomenon (cf., Schyns,
2001) and can be used interchangeably.
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 44
Table 1
Overview of Results Regarding the Role of NVB in Leadership
NVB Categories
Emergent
leadership/
Perception of
leadership
Expression
of
leadership
Effective leadership
Leader’s
supportive-
ness
Leader’s
professional
success
Attraction
toward
the
leader
Follower's
perfor-
mance
Satisfac-
tion with
leader
Motivation
to work
with
leader
Meeting
the
leader
socially
Smiling
+
+
+
+
+
Gazing/ Eye contact
+
+
+
+
+ a
+
+
+
Visual dominance c
+
Lowered eyebrows
+
Facial expressiveness
/intensity
+
+
+
+
+
+
Nodding
+
+
Self-touch
-
Other touch
+
+
Hand/arm gestures
+
+
+
Postural openness
+
+
Postural relaxation
Erect posture
+
+
Forward lean
+
Body/leg shifting
+
Interpersonal distance
-
-
-
-
Facing orientation
+
Vocal variability
+
+
+b
+b
+b
+b
Interruptions
+
-
NON VERBAL BEHAVIOR 45
Speaking time c
+
Speech errors
-
Faster rate of speech
+
+
Lower voice pitch
+
+
Vocal relaxation
+
+
Seating position c
Head of table
+
Edge of the desk
+
+
+
Note: The categories related to leadership are based on Hall et al.'s meta-analysis (2005, p. 903)
+ = positive and significant relationship (e.g., more vocal variation, more gesture); - = negative and significant
relationship (e.g., less speech errors, less interruption); a = Enlarged pupil size; b = Soft voice; c = three
additional NVB categories besides Hall et al.’s meta-analysis; blank cells = relations were not tested.