ArticlePDF Available

Effects of Intermittent Fasting, Caloric Restriction, and Ramadan Intermittent Fasting on Cognitive Performance at Rest and During Exercise in Adults

Authors:

Abstract

The aim of this review was to highlight the potent effects of intermittent fasting on the cognitive performance of athletes at rest and during exercise. Exercise interacts with dietary factors and has a positive effect on brain functioning. Furthermore, physical activity and exercise can favorably influence brain plasticity. Mounting evidence indicates that exercise, in combination with diet, affects the management of energy metabolism and synaptic plasticity by affecting molecular mechanisms through brain-derived neurotrophic factor, an essential neurotrophin that acts at the interface of metabolism and plasticity. The literature has also shown that certain aspects of physical performance and mental health, such as coping and decision-making strategies, can be negatively affected by daylight fasting. However, there are several types of intermittent fasting. These include caloric restriction, which is distinct from fasting and allows subjects to drink water ad libitum while consuming a very low-calorie food intake. Another type is Ramadan intermittent fasting, which is a religious practice of Islam, where healthy adult Muslims do not eat or drink during daylight hours for 1 month. Other religious practices in Islam (Sunna) also encourage Muslims to practice intermittent fasting outside the month of Ramadan. Several cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have shown that intermittent fasting has crucial effects on physical and intellectual performance by affecting various aspects of bodily physiology and biochemistry that could be important for athletic success. Moreover, recent findings revealed that immunological variables are also involved in cognitive functioning and that intermittent fasting might impact the relationship between cytokine expression in the brain and cognitive deficits, including memory deficits.
1 23
Sports Medicine
ISSN 0112-1642
Sports Med
DOI 10.1007/s40279-015-0408-6
Effects of Intermittent Fasting, Caloric
Restriction, and Ramadan Intermittent
Fasting on Cognitive Performance at Rest
and During Exercise in Adults
Anissa Cherif, Bart Roelands, Romain
Meeusen & Karim Chamari
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer
International Publishing Switzerland. This e-
offprint is for personal use only and shall not
be self-archived in electronic repositories. If
you wish to self-archive your article, please
use the accepted manuscript version for
posting on your own website. You may
further deposit the accepted manuscript
version in any repository, provided it is only
made publicly available 12 months after
official publication or later and provided
acknowledgement is given to the original
source of publication and a link is inserted
to the published article on Springer's
website. The link must be accompanied by
the following text: "The final publication is
available at link.springer.com”.
REVIEW ARTICLE
Effects of Intermittent Fasting, Caloric Restriction, and Ramadan
Intermittent Fasting on Cognitive Performance at Rest
and During Exercise in Adults
Anissa Cherif
1
Bart Roelands
2,3
Romain Meeusen
2,4
Karim Chamari
1
Ó Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015
Abstract The aim of this review was to highlight the potent
effects of intermittent fasting on the cognitive performance
of athletes at rest and during exercise. Exercise interacts with
dietary factors and has a positive effect on brain functioning.
Furthermore, physical activity and exercise can favorably
influence brain plasticity. Mounting evidence indicates that
exercise, in combination with diet, affects the management
of energy metabolism and synapt ic plasticity by affecting
molecular mechanisms through brain-derived neurotrophic
factor, an essential neurotrophin that acts at the interface of
metabolism and plasticity. The literature has also show n that
certain aspects of physical performance and mental health,
such as coping and decision-making strategies, can be neg-
atively affected by daylight fasting. However, there are
several types of intermittent fasting. These include caloric
restriction, which is distinct from fasting and allows subjects
to drink water ad libitum while consuming a very low-calorie
food intake. Another type is Ramadan intermittent fasting,
which is a religious practice of Islam, where healthy adult
Muslims do not eat or drink during daylight hours for 1
month. Other religious practices in Islam (Sunna) also
encourage Muslims to practice intermittent fasting outside
the month of Ramadan. Several cross-sectional and longi-
tudinal studies have shown that intermittent fasting has
crucial effects on physical and intellectual performance by
affecting various aspects of bodily physiology and bio-
chemistry that could be important for athletic success.
Moreover, recent findings revealed that immunological
variables are also involved in cognitive functioning and that
intermittent fasting might impact the relationship between
cytokine expression in the brain and cognitive deficits,
including memory deficits.
Key Points
Both exercise and nutrition influence brain structure
and function. Exercise while fasting can be a genuine
challenge for athletes’ cognitive function.
Physical activity exerts neuroplasticity-enhancing
effects that are potentially mediated by
neuroimmune mechanisms and brain-derived
neurotrophic factor.
Systemic inflammation can also occur in intermittent
fasting through the maintenance of neurotrophic
support for hippocampal neurons, which are part of a
major brain structure involved in spatial learning.
1 Introduction
Physical activity and exercise promote health, assist with
weight management, and avoid and reduce health prob-
lems, vascular disease and inflammatory disease [1].
& Anissa Cherif
anissa.cherif@aspetar.com
1
Athlete Health and Performance Research Center, Aspetar-
Qatar Orthopaedic and Sports Medicine Hospital,
PO Box 29222, Doha, Qatar
2
Department Human Physiology, Faculty of Physical
Education and Physiotherapy, Vrije Universiteit Brussel
(VUB), Brussels, Belgium
3
Fund for Scientific Research Flanders (FWO), Brussels,
Belgium
4
School of Public Health, Tropical Medicine and
Rehabilitation Sciences, James Cook University, Townsville,
QLD, Australia
123
Sports Med
DOI 10.1007/s40279-015-0408-6
Author's personal copy
Moreover, they have both been associated with the reduced
prevalence of a number of physical and mental disorders
[2]. In their review, Dishman et al. [3] stated that physical
activity and exercise can favorably influence brain plas-
ticity by facilitating neurogenerative, neuroadaptive, and
neuroprotective processes [3]. Moreover, Chytrova et al.
[4] showed that exercise acts on specific molecular systems
that control axonal growth and synaptic plasticity, which
are also modulated by diet [4]. Exercise, coupled with a
balanced diet, works as a means to prevent or even reverse
negative health effects. For instance, obesity, which is
induced by nutritional imbalance, may cause a variety of
diseases such as cardiovascular diseases, type 2 diabetes,
and depression [3]. Obesity is also an important factor that
can be responsible for a decline in cognitive function [3].
Therefore, exercise and nutrition are both powerful means
that influence brain structure and function [5]. Diet and
regular exercise are two non-invasive approaches that can
be used to enhance neural signaling by influencing synaptic
transmission, brain plasticity, and cogni tive function [4, 6].
Regular exercise enhances certain types of learning,
including the executive functions of cognition, learning and
memory span, and it also stimulates neurogenesis [3, 7].
Diet can also provoke significant change s in energy
expenditure, oxidative damage, insulin sensitivity, and
inflammation, including functional changes in both the
neuroendocrine and sympathetic nervous systems [8].
Gomez-Pinilla [1] showed that exercise, in combination
with dietary factors, affects the management of energy
metabolism and synaptic plasticity by affecting molecular
mechanisms through brain-derived neurotrophic factor
(BDNF), which acts at the interface of metabolism and
plasticity [1]. Interestingly, the results of recent research
indicate that insufficient levels of exercise and poor dietary
practices are considered risk factors for various neurode-
generative diseases such as Alzheimer’s [9, 10].
Elite sporting efforts require optimal physical and
cognitive capabilities for maximal performance. The lit-
erature indicates that certain physical performance aspects
can be negatively affected by daylight fasting [11, 12].
Furthermore, there are many types of intermittent fasting
(IF) that are practiced differently from one religion to
another. Caloric restriction (CR) is distinct from fasting
and allows subjects to drink water ad libitum while
consuming a very low-calorie food intake. Ramadan
intermittent fasting (RIF) is a religious practice of Islam
during which healthy adult Muslims neither eat nor drink
during daylight hours. Moreover, some religious practices
in Islam (Sunna) advise all adul t Muslims to also practice
IF outside of the month of Ramadan. The current litera-
ture regarding the effects of different types of dietary
restrictions on cognitive performance is summa rized in
Sect. 2.
Interestingly, the majority of the experiments investi-
gating the effects of fasting on cognitive performance in
athletes have assessed athletes while in resting conditions,
i.e., not during exercise sessions. This represents a signif-
icant limit to the conclusions drawn by these studies.
Indeed, it is not possible to conclude that fasting does not
impact the cognitive performance of athletes during exer-
cise when the cognitive tests are performed outside of an
exercise session. Furthermore, regarding the assessment of
cognitive performance during exercise, the available
information is scarce [1315], and currently, it indicates
that depending on exercise intensity, cognitive function
could be improved or negatively impacted [16]. Moreover ,
these studies focused on continuous endurance exercise.
This aspect of studying cognitive function while exercising
has to be combined with the effects of fasting to verify if
the responses of fasted subjects differ from those of fed
subjects. Moreover, depending on the sport or activity,
exercise patterns during competition are not always con-
tinuous and of constant intensity. Indeed, during team
sports (e.g., soccer, handball, rugby, etc.), performance is
composed of repeated bouts of intense exercise and athletes
must make important decisions while exercising and
between or even within intensive actions (e.g., decision of
movement and reaction to a corner kick in soccer).
Therefore, the repeated-sprint ability (RSA) tests were
designed to replicate the demands of an intense period of
play during multiple-sprint sports and involve the repeti-
tion of explosive actions with relatively short periods of
rest in between [17].
Food and fluid intake before, during, and after both
training and competition has important implications for
performance [17]. The absence of fluid intake in the day
may have an even greater impact on performanc e than the
absence of food [18]. Even mild dehydration may have
adverse effects on a number of physiological and cognitive
functions that are important components of performance.
Collectively, these changes may cause perturb ations that
alter the physiological responses to exercise, which may
have detrimental effects on sports performance [19].
Therefore, the present review article will elucidate the
fascinating and potent effects of IF on the cognitive per-
formance of athletes at rest and during exercise (Table 1).
The relevant literature was reviewed, including studies
on healthy subjects with control groups/conditions. Studies
testing subjects with pathologies were excluded. Addi-
tionally, experiments on the effects of any type of fasting in
extreme conditions such as altitude or hyperthermia were
also excluded from the current review. A literature search
was conducted from 1992 to 2015 through the PubMed and
Web of Knowledge databases, using specific key words:
‘exercise,’ ‘brain,’ ‘cognitive performance,’ ‘physical
performance,’ ‘nutrition,’ ‘collaborative effects of diet and
A. Cherif et al.
123
Author's personal copy
Table 1 Effects of different dietary interventions on cognitive performance of athletes at rest and during exercise
Caloric restriction Intermittent fasting Ramadan intermittent fasting
Authors Effect Authors Effect Authors Effect
Mattson [30] : Lifespan
; Many age-associated diseases
: The general health of overweight humans
Izumida et al. [50] ; Weight
Delayed aging
Optimized health
Chaouachi et al. [11],
Thomson and
Sutherland [72]
; Afternoon performance in
many sports
: Fatigue at the end of
Ramadan intermittent fasting
Mattson and Wan
[31]
; Several risk factors for coronary artery
disease
; Blood pressure
: Insulin sensitivity
Greenberg et al. [51] : Brain function and peripheral
energy metabolism
Mansur et al. [86],
Dantzer et al. [87]
Protected neurons against
dysfunction and
degeneration
: Levels of antioxidant
defenses and anti-
inflammatory IL-10
: Protein chaperones (heat
shock proteins)
Green et al. [28] : Lethargy, depression, irritability
; Heart rate
; Intellectual functioning
; Concentration and memory
; Ability to remember
; Simple reaction times
Mattson [48], Cahill [49],
Izumida et al. [50]
: Parasympathetic activity
: Gut motility
; Heart rate
; Blood pressure
: Lipolysis
Egan et al. [133] :
Dehydration
; Cognitive function
; Performance on a sustained attention task
Mansur et al. [86], Dantzer
et al. [87]
Protected neurons against
dysfunction and degeneration
: Levels of antioxidant
defenses and anti-
inflammatory IL-10
: Expression of BDNF and
protein chaperones such as
HSP-70
; Levels of circulating pro-
inflammatory IL-1b, IL-6, and
TNF-a
Bruss et al. [111] ; Inflammation
Improved glucose
Green et al. [32] ; Cognitive function Kim and Diamond [74] ; Lipopolysaccharide (LPS) Boden et al. [116] Improved fat metabolism
Singh et al. [25],
Singh Kalra and
Fults [26]
: Performance on behavioral tests of
sensory and motor function and learning
and memory
Benton, [117], Boden et al.
[116]
; LDL, TC, TG, and HDL
levels
Improved fat metabolism
Kim and Diamond [74] : Expression of BDNF
Lee et al. [27] : Brain function in adult rats
: Synaptic plasticity
: Production of new neurons from neural
stem cells
Benefer et al. [118], Boden
et al. [116], Bruss et al.
[111]
; Insulin levels
Improved glucose
: Glucose tolerance
Mansur et al. [86],
Dantzer et al. [87]
; Levels of circulating pro-
inflammatory IL-1b
, IL-6,
and TNF-a
Fasting, Cognitive Performance and Exercise
123
Author's personal copy
exercise on cognitive performance,’ ‘impact of intermittent
fasting dietary restriction on physical performance,’ and
‘BDNF.’
2 Effects of Caloric Restriction, Intermittent
Fasting, and Ramadan Intermittent Fasting
There has been a recent increasing awareness of the par-
ticular effects of different dietary interventions, and their
specific mechanisms are beginning to be understood. In the
present review, we will highlight the three most published
protocols in the literature: CR, IF, and RIF.
2.1 Caloric Restriction
Recently, CR has gained considerable popularity, as some
physicians find these diets easy to follow. CR is distinct
from fasting and typically refers to a 20–40 % daily
reduction in calorie intake. CR is used in clinics by
physicians and allows patients to drink water ad libitum
while consuming a very low-calorie food intake for a week
or more. This diet is used for weight management and/or
disease treatment [2022]. More recently, the effects of CR
have been studied in different model organisms to dete r-
mine the molecular mechanisms by which dietary inter-
ventions may modulate lifespan in both human and non-
humans [23, 24]. These studies indicated that reduced
nutrient intake without falling into malnutrition could
increase the mean and the maximum lifespan of rats [24].
According to Singh et al. [25], moderate CR in rats
enhances performance on behavioral tests for sensory and
motor functions, as well as lea rning and memory [26]. CR
can also increase brain function in adult rats, where it has
been associated with increased synaptic plasticity and
increased production of new neurons from neural stem
cells [27]. In contrast, other studies revealed that severe
food deprivation might contribute to dieting-related deficits
in cognitive function [28, 29].
Ongoing studies of CR in humans now allow the anal-
ysis of changes in the brain and explain some of the
mechanisms related to associated alterations in cognitive
performance. CR has been demonstrated to prolong lifes-
pan, preventing many age-associated diseases, and it
improves the general health of overweight humans [30].
Mattson and Wan [31] also revealed that CR has an
important role in regulating both cardiovascular and brain
functions. More over, CR can effectively prolong lifespan,
preventing most age-associated diseases by ameliorating
several risk factors for coronary artery disease, such as
increased blood pressure and decreased insulin sensitivity
[31]. However, long-term food restriction was associated
with lethargy, depression, irritability, and reduced heart
Table 1 continued
Caloric restriction Intermittent fasting Ramadan intermittent fasting
Authors Effect Authors Effect Authors Effect
Mattson [23] : Brain function
Might forestall age-related cognitive
decline in humans
Cahill [49] : Maintenance of muscle mass
Witte et al. [34] : Performance on memory tests after 30 %
caloric reduction for 3 months
: Performance on memory tests
Warren and Frier [101] : Brain function by enhancing
Neurogenesis and synaptic
plasticity
: BDNF
Lee et al. [27],
Suzuki et al. [98]
: Hippocampal neurogenesis
: BDNF, IGF-1, and VEGF
: increases, ; decreases, BDNF brain-derived neurotrophic factor, HDL high-density lipoprotein, HSP-70 heat shock protein, IGF-1 insulin-like growth factor 1, IL interleukin, LDL low-density
lipoprotein, TC total cholesterol, RIF ramadan intermittent fasting, TG triglycerides, TNF-a tumor necrosis factor, VEGF vascular endothelial cell growth factor
A. Cherif et al.
123
Author's personal copy
rate, as well as a range of self-reported impairments in
intellectual functioning, including the inability to concen-
trate and poor memory [32]. Dieting to lose weight has also
been associated with impairments in cognitive function,
including poorer performance on a sustained attention task
[33]. This result was simulated in another study that
demonstrated that in addition to a deficit in sustained
attention, self-reported dieting was associated with a
reduced ability to remember and slower simple reaction
times (RTs) [32 ].
Therefore, a combination of moderate CR and exercise
could potentially enhance brain function and may forestall
age-related cognitive decline in humans. Witte et al. [34]
demonstrated that when the caloric intake of 50 normally
fed elderly subjects was reduced by 30 % for 3 months,
their performance on memory tests significantly improved
compared with two different control diet groups [34].
Nutrition can also substantially influence the development
of brain structure and function. Indeed, nutrition provides
the proper building blocks for the brain to create and
maintain neural connections, which are critical for
improved cognition [5]. Dietary factors have broad and
positive actions on neuronal function and plastici ty [5].
Brain function is undoubtedly dependent on adequate
nutrition, and short-term variations in the amount and
composition of nutrient intake in healthy individuals may
influence measures of cognitive function [5].
In summary, diet manipulation can generate positive or
negative cognitive outcomes, depending on the dieting
pattern, which is predominantly influenced by the extent of
CR. Recent studies on animal models and human subjects
revealed strong beneficial effects of regular exercise or CR
on cognitive function [23].
2.2 Intermittent Fasting
IF generally involves a ‘feast day’ on which food is con-
sumed ad libitum, which alternates with a ‘fast day,’ during
which food is withheld or strongly reduced. Another form
of IF involves eating only one meal per day. For instance,
during the month of Ramadan, healthy adult Muslims are
required to abstain from ingesting both food and fluids
during daylight hours (from dawn to sunset). Other types of
fasting are commonly found in other religious practices
such as Christianity [22, 35], Judaism [36, 37], and Bud-
dhism [38]. For instance, Greek Orthodox Christians fast
for a total of 180–200 days each year, and their principal
fasting periods are the Nativity Fast (40 days prior to
Christmas), Lent (48 days prior to Easter), and the
Assumption (15 days in August). These fasting periods can
be described as a variant of vegetarianism [22]. During the
Nativity fast, fasters abstain from dairy products, eggs, and
meat during the 40 consecutive days. They also abstain
from fish and olive oil consumption on Wednesdays and
Fridays during this period. During Lent, fasters also abstain
from dairy products, eggs, and meat every day; they abstain
from consuming olive oil on weekdays and from fish every
day except March 25th and Palm Sunday. During the
Assumption, fasters abstain from dairy products, eggs, and
meat; they abstain from olive oil on weekdays and from
fish every day except August 6th. In addition to these
principal fasts, every Wednesday and Friday that fall out-
side of a principal fasting period call for the proscription of
cheese, eggs, fis h, meat, milk, and olive oil. Exceptions to
these proscriptions occur on the week following Christmas,
Easter, and the Pentecost [22, 39]. IF can also include
alternate-day fasting or 3-days-a-week fasting, every 2 or
more weeks. Some religious practices in Islam (Sunna) also
advise adult Muslims to fast the Islamic way every Monday
and Thursday and for 3 consecut ive days in the middle
(days 13, 14, and 15) of lunar months in the Islamic Cal-
endar [40]. IF has shown potential to impact the physical
and cognitive performance of fasters, even if this is chal-
lenged by several studies [4143]. However, this impact
appears to be dependent on the task performed, the dura-
tion of the fast, and the time of day of the assessments [44].
In rats, the combination of IF (alternate-day fasting) and
treadmill exercise resulted in greater maintenance of
muscle mass than IF or exercise alone [45]. Findings from
animal experiments, as well as emerging human stud ies,
indicate that fasting may be an effective strategy to reduce
weight, delay aging, and optimize health [46 ]. According to
Longo and Mattson [47], IF modifies brain neurochemistry
and neuronal network activity in ways that optimize brain
function and peripheral energy metabolism [47]. Four brain
regions that are particularly important in adaptive respon-
ses to IF include the hippocampus (cognitive processing),
striatum (control of body movements), hypothalamus
(control of food intake and body temperature), and brain-
stem (control of cardiovascular and digestive systems) [3].
The brain communicates with all peripheral organs
involved in energy metabolism [48]. IF enhances
parasympathetic activity (mediated by the neurotransmitter
acetylcholine) in the autonomic neurons that innervate the
gut, heart, and arteries, resulting in improved gut motility
and reduced heart rate and blood pressure. By depleting
glycogen from liver cells, fasting also results in lipolysis
[49, 50]. In addition, when food is scarce, the liver
becomes the storage site for triacylglycerols (TAGs), thus
acting as a reservoir of energetic substrates that can be
liberated [51]. When the stored TAGs are depleted, the free
fatty aci ds (FFAs) and glycerol released are directly oxi-
dized as an energy source by certain tissues (liver and
muscle) [50 ]. Moreover, adenosine triphosphate can be
produced through the oxidation of FFAs, and glycerol can
be used as a substrate in gluconeogenesis or lipogenesis
Fasting, Cognitive Performance and Exercise
123
Author's personal copy
[50]. Therefore, adipose tissue can directly and indirectly
modulate the availability of other metabolic substrates
[52].
Certain beneficial effects of IF on both the cardiovas-
cular system and the brain are mediated by BDNF sign al-
ing in the brain [53]. Interestingly, cellular and molecular
effects of IF and CR on the cardiovascular system and the
brain are similar to those of regular exercise, suggesting
shared mechanisms [31]. Indeed, the Dishman et al. [3]
review suggested that chronic physical activity increases
the expression of genes that encode several brain neu-
rotrophins such as BDNF and nerve growth factor [3].
Widenfalk et al. [ 54] studied the potential effects of
physical training on the expression of neurotrophic factors
and their receptors in the rodent brain. They found that
chronic exercise increased the expression of genes that
encode several brain neurotrophins such as BDNF [54]. In
addition, chronic physical activity may als o have neuro-
generative and neuroprotective influences on the brain by
stimulating the growth and development of new cells and
protecting against ischemic neuronal damage in the hip-
pocampal formation [55]. Both the action and function of
BDNF are presented later in this review.
2.3 Ramadan Intermittent Fasting
RIF is one of the five pillars of Islam. It occurs during the
ninth month of the Islamic lunar calendar, during which
healthy adult Muslims abstain from food and fluid intake
from dawn (el fajr) to sunset for approximately 30 days
[56]. The duration of the daytime fast varies and is sig-
nificantly impacted by the location and season. The typical
fasting duration is generally 10 h, but can exceed 18 h [57
62]. A longer daylight period is found at extreme latitudes,
and for these extreme fasting durations, some religious
exceptions may apply [63]. Several studies examined the
effects of RIF, and their findings indicated that it may
induce either positive or negative effects on mental health
[64], coping strategies, and decision-making strategies
[65]. RIF causes major changes in sleep patterns, eating
habits, and physical activity, which may cause changes in
metabolism. Many aspects of these changes have been
studied. For example, Alabed et al. [66] developed a
questionnaire to assess subjective estimates of physical,
mental, and social activities, as well as fatigue. They dis-
covered that there were several changes that occurred
during daytime fasting, as the daytime mental, physical,
and social activities all decreased below control values
[66]. There is now ample evidence that RIF negatively
affects afternoon performance in many sports and elicits
higher feelings of fatigue at the end of RIF [19, 67].
However, morning and evening (after breaking the fast)
performances are generally not affected by RIF [19, 68].
Several cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have
demonstrated that RIF has a crucial effect on the physical
and intellectual performance of Muslims by affecting var-
ious aspects of bodily physiology and biochemistry that are
important to athleti c success. Thus, both the physical and
cognitive aspects of exercise can be a true challenge for
fasting Muslim athletes during the daytime throughout RIF.
3 Fasting/Caloric Restriction and Cognitive
Function
3.1 Effects on the Immune System, Syste mic
Inflammation, and Cytokines
According to the results of recent studies, system ic
inflammation/sepsis can be considered a risk factor for
cognitive impairment [69, 70]. There is now ample evi-
dence that pro-inflammatory mediators may disturb hip-
pocampal neuronal functions through the relationship
between neuronal plasticity and working memory, a fun-
damental process for specific brain functions [7173].
Cytokines such as interleukin-1b (IL-1b), IL-6, and tumor
necrosis factor-a (TNF-a) are important in hippocampal
neural signal transmission (long-term potentiation) and
dendritic convergence, which are processes involved in
memory formation and memory preservation [69, 74].
Several studies showed that there is considerable evidence
for a relationship between cytokine expression in the brain
and cognitive deficits, including memory deficits [75, 76].
Indeed, neuroinflammation can negatively affect neuroge-
nesis and lead to impaired survival and proliferation of new
neurons. This negative effect is a result of immunoresponse
to different exogenous and endogenous stimuli through
cells within the brain such as microglia and the action of
signaling molecules such as pro- or anti-inflammatory
cytokines [73]. Moreover, TNF-a and IL-6 are critical for
neuroinflammation-induced memory impairment and are
known to disrupt cognition through pathways related to
neural plasticity, neurogenesis, and long-term potentiation
[69, 7779]. Certain neurons in the preoptic nucleus have
receptors for IL-1b, IL-6, and TNF-a, to which the
cytokines bind and thus pass from circulation into the brain
[80]. Consequently, hippocampal IL-1 b overexpression
impairs contextual and spatial long-term memory [69].
Therefore, previous animal studies have shown that CR and
IF protect neurons against dysfunction and degeneration by
increasing the levels of antioxidant defenses and anti-in-
flammatory IL-10, increasing the expression of BDNF and
protein chaperones such as heat shock protein (HSP-70),
and reducing the levels of circulating pro-inflammatory IL-
1b, IL-6, and TNF-a [81, 82]. In addition, IF can also
inhibit lipopolysaccharide (LPS)-induced increases in
A. Cherif et al.
123
Author's personal copy
serum interferon-c (IFN-c), sugges ting that the anti-in-
flammatory effect of IF is not restricted to the central
nervous system [69]. Vasconcelos et al. [69] showed that
systemic inflammation can also be established by IF
through the maintenance of the neurotrophic support for
hippocampal neurons [69], which are part of a major brain
structure involved in spatial learning [83].
Recently, peripheral infection has also been shown to
activate the immune system thr ough a message to the brain
that leads to the production of cytokines [84]. Conse-
quently, excessive expression of pro-inflammatory cytoki-
nes in the brain has been indicated to be a central factor in
the development of cognitive impairment [8588]. There-
fore, regulating the inflammatory response in the brain
following a peripheral infection may be important in pro-
tection against behavioral disorders [89].
Furthermore, one possible mechanism by which the
peripheral inflammatory response may affect cognitive
function could be through interference with the expression
of BDNF [73, 90, 91]. Lapchak et al. [92] showed a sig-
nificant reduction of BDNF gene expression within the
brain by injecting pro-inflammatory IL-1b into the rat
hippocampus [92]. Moreover, physical activity may also
exert neuroplasticity-enhancing effects potentially medi-
ated by neuroimmune mechanisms. In fact, Lee et al. [27]
demonstrated that exercise and CR might enhance hip-
pocampal neurogenesis by similar mechanism s involving
the upregulation of the expression of trophic factors
including BDNF, insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF-1), and
vascular endothelial cell growth factor (VEGF) [27, 93]. A
study by Nascimento et al. [94] on the effects of physical
exercise on peripheral BDNF levels and on TNF-a and IL-
6 as pro-inflammatory markers in cognitive, healthy indi-
viduals showed that exercise is effective in reducing pro-
inflammatory cytokines and increasing peripheral BDNF
levels with positive effects on cognition [94]. Moreover,
Whiteman et al. [95] showed an increase in plasticity
mechanisms in the hippocampus [95], and Van Praag et al.
[96] demonstrated that exercise and IF could optimize
brain function by enhancing neurogenesis and synaptic
plasticity [96]. The latter is indeed a critical aspect of the
brain responsible for learning and memory through BDNF,
IGF-1, and other growth factors and hormones [95]. These
data confirm that immunological variables are involved in
cognitive functioning and that fasting or CR might impact
this relationship.
3.2 Effects on Biochemical Parameters: Insulin,
Glucose, Free Fatty Acids, Cholesterol,
and Triglycerides
Nutrition provides the building materials for the brain to
produce and preserve connections between brain structures
and function, which is important for enhanced cognition
[5]. Because of the important role of glucose for the brain
as the source of energy [97
, 98], acute hypoglycem ia
results in a rapid deterioration of some aspects of cerebral
function [97]. Glucose passes through the blood–brain
barrier for entry into both neurons and astrocytes. Glyco-
gen is stored in astrocytes [99] and is rapidly converted into
pyruvate/lactate and metabolized in the tricarboxylic acid
cycle or used for the biosynthesis of glutamate [100]. Both
glucose and lactate can then be exported to neurons as fuel
[99]. Draelos et al. [97] examined neuropsychological
function during experimentally induced periods of hyper-
glycemia and hypoglycemia to determine the impact of
glycemic control on cognitive function. The outcomes of
their study showed that severe hypoglycemia (\2.2 mmol/
L) causes impairments in all measured aspects of cognitive
function including RT (simple and choice) [97]. Further-
more, even modest hypoglycemia (a blood glucose level of
2.6–3.0 mmol/L) can cause a measurable performance
decrease, with complex tasks being more sensitive to
hypoglycemia than simple tasks [101]. Dalsgaard et al.
[102] indicated that glucose and lactate uptake by the brain
increased out of proportion with O
2
when the brain was
activated by exhaustive exercise [102]. Therefore, these
metabolic changes seem to be influenced by the will to
exercise and the exercise load [102]. Moreover, mental
stimulation was considered by Madsen et al. [103]tobea
physiological activation during which the ratio O
2
/glucose
becomes reduced [103]. Findings by Williamson et al.
[104] indicated that intense exercise increases physiologi-
cal activation of the brain [104], and in that case, energy
demand surpasses energy production [105]. This imbalance
in the energy produced and the energy demanded may
cause glycogen depletion in the brain regions activated
during intense exercise and has been proposed as a possible
cause of the end of exercise [102]. In contrast, a review by
Amigo and Kowaltowski [106] reported that reduced food
intake over short periods of IF consistently improves glu-
cose tolerance. They also note, however, that in vertebrate
organisms, the brain is responsible for *25 % of total
body glucose consumption [106]. This energy consumption
is required to maintain ionic balance in neurons, produce
action potentials, generate post-synaptic currents, and
recycle neurotransmitters [107, 108]. All of these charac-
teristics show the reliance of the brain on glucose and that
it is an organ highly sensitive to energy deficits [106 ]. In
addition, during fasting, Brow n [109] demonstrated that
astrocytes, but not neurons, can accumulate glucose in the
form of glycogen, which acts as a short-term energetic
reservoir in the brain [109]. Indeed, this argument has been
tested in vivo by Suzuki et al. [98] by injecting inhibitory
avoidance compounds into rat hippocampus. Their out-
comes revealed that astrocyte–neuron metabolic coupling
Fasting, Cognitive Performance and Exercise
123
Author's personal copy
mechanisms play a critical role in memory formation. They
also demonstrated that learning leads to a significant
increase in extracellular lactate levels, which are derived
from glycogen, an energy reserve selectively localized in
astrocytes [98]. Therefore, Suzuki et al. [98] revealed that
astrocytic glycogen could be directly related to learning
and that the glycolytic end product lactate appears to play a
role in long-term memory formation; however, the exact
mechanism has not yet been established [98]. Amnesia can
be caused by disturbing the expression of astrocytic and
neuronal lactate transporters, suggesting that lactate import
into neurons is necessary for long-term memory. To
demonstrate that astrocytic glycogen metabolism is
required in the hippocampus for long-term memory for-
mation, Suzuki et al. [98] injected an inhibitor of glycogen
phosphorylation, 1,4-dideoxy-1,4-imino-
D-arabinitol
(DAB), into the rat hippocampus before or immediately
after training. The obtained results indicated that DAB
injection did not affect short- and long-term memories.
However, to prove that astrocyte–neuron lactate transport
is required for long-term memory formation, Suzuki et al.
[98] injected a combination of DAB and
L-lactate into the
hippocampus of rats before and after training using in vivo
microdialysis [110]. Their results confirmed that DAB-in-
duced amnesia could be rescued by the administration of
exogenous lactate and that astrocyte–neuron lactate trans-
port is required for long-term memory formation [98].
Accumulating evidence suggests that in healthy adults,
most syst emic changes induced by IF, CR, or RIF, such as
decreased inflammation and improved glucose- [106] and
fat-metabolism [111], protect against brain damage [106].
Many studies have confirmed that IF decreases serum low-
density lipoprotein (LDL), a major protein component of
LDL cholesterol, and also decreases total cholesterol,
triglycerides, and high-density lipoprotein cholesterol levels
[112]. Moreover, certain evidence suggests that after 3
weeks of alternate-day fasting, body fat and insulin levels
decrease [113]. Elevated concentrations of FFAs in plasma
cause insulin resistance (IR) in humans throu gh the inhibi-
tion of glucose transport activity [114]. Furthermore, both
local and circulating FFAs could be considered important
factors in the induction of IR, hyperlipidemia, and inflam-
matory processes [114, 115]. Indee d, Boden et al. [116]
found, through studying mechanisms by which FFAs cause
hepatic IR in male rats, that FFA-caused hepatic IR is
associated with the increased expression of inflammatory
cytokines such as TNF-a and IL-1b and also with hyper-
glycemia and the overproduc tion of glucose [116].
3.3 Effects of Dehydration
Water is a major constituent of cells, and approximately
60 % of the body is composed of water, which is necessary
in numerous basic functions [117]. To study the effect of
hydration on cognition, Benefer et al. [118] examined the
relationship between fluid intake and cognitive function
following endurance exercise in a group of recreational
runners and walkers. Based on the water intake factor
scores, their outcomes showed a significant improvement
in short-term memory [118]. Other studies have used
extreme conditions to show the impact of hydration on the
brain, such as a study by Lieberman et al. [119] in which
subjects who had exercised for 53 h in hot conditions
experienced alterations in mood and performance for a
range of cognitive functions, including vigilance, RTs,
attention, memory, and reasoning [119]. In contrast, the
effect of hydration in cold conditions was tested and
exhibited no effect on cognitive, psychomotor, or self-re-
ported parameters [120]. Food and fluid intake before,
during, and after both training and competition have an
important impact on sport performance [121], and early
symptoms of dehydration are headaches and feeling tired
and lightheaded. Additionally, dehydration deteriorates
mental performance by *10 %, and drinking more water
improves the ability to learn [117].
It has been shown that certain components of football
performance (physiological and cognitive functions) can be
indirectly affected by dehydration [122]. These change s
can indirectly disturb the physiological responses to exer-
cise, which may negatively affect sports performance [19].
There is now ample evidence that attention is reduced in
conditions of thirst [
123, 124]. In fact, dehydration
exceeding a 1 % loss of body weight resulted in poorer
performance on a visual vigilance task and slower RTs on a
working memory task [125]. Several cross-sectional and
longitudinal studies have established the assoc iation
between the aforementioned negative effects of dehydra-
tion and cognitive performance [123, 125]. Prolonged
states of reduced water intake can also result in an
increased perceived effort during exercise. Kempton et al.
[126] used magnetic resonance imaging and showed that
when dehydrated, subjects used a higher level of neuronal
activity to complete the same performance level [126].
However, few studies have reported on the effect of
dehydration due to IF without fluid consumption. One such
study by NasrAllah and Osman [127] explored the effect of
fasting among 52 patients with chronic kidney disease
during RIF. Their results suggested that dehydration occurs
during daytime fasting and is reversed at sunset but that its
effects are difficult to assess because of these daily fluc-
tuations. Despite variations in kidney function, they did not
notice any changes in blood pressure, which, however,
does not necessarily exclude the possi bility of dehydration
[127]. Further studies should explore the possible impact of
fasting and dehydration on cognitive functioning in greater
depth and detail.
A. Cherif et al.
123
Author's personal copy
4 Effect of Exercise and Nutrients on the Brain:
Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor
Exercise and nutrition are both powerful factors that influ-
ence the brain [128]. Studies on both animals and humans
describing the important effects of exercise and diet on the
health and plasticity of the nervous system are increasingly
emerging [5]. This combination undeniably affects funda-
mental and molecular processes related to the management
of energy metabolism and broad aspects of synaptic plas-
ticity; the latter is believed to be essential for learning and
memory. Therefore, learning capacity is affected by the
action of BDNF, an essential neurotrophin [5, 129] that is a
central contributor to the effects of exercise on synaptic and
cognitive plasticity [ 130]. BDNF regulates not only synaptic
plasticity but also neurogenesis and neuronal survival in the
adult brain [131]. The induced mechanisms include neuro nal
protection and survival, neurite expression, axonal and
dendritic growth and remodeling, neuronal differentiation,
synaptic plasticity (such as synaptogenesis in arborizing
axon terminals), and synaptic transmission efficacy [132].
The principal BDNF function is essential to learning and to
maintaining memory capacity in humans [133, 134]. There is
a direct link between the pathways associated with metabo-
lism and synaptic plasticity, and this association can deter-
mine important aspects of behavioral plasticity such as
learning and memory [130, 135]. Specifically, exercise
modulates molecular systems in the brain associated with
energy balance and energy transduction. Thus, exercise has
the capacity to affect learning and memory by (1) activating
the secretion of IGF-I, which plays a role in synapt ic plas-
ticity [93, 136]; (2) stimulating neurotransmitter synthesis
and release [137]; and (3) supporting cognitive function
[138, 139]. Therefore, the general conclusion is that exercise
has a positive influence on cognition, and it simultaneously
increases BDNF levels [5]. Recently, an exer cise regimen
known for its capacity to enhance learning and memory was
reported to mediate its effects through a BDNF-related
mechanism [140]. Of all the neurotrophins, BDNF is con-
sidered most susceptible to regulation by exercise and
physical activity, and it acts at the interface of metabolism
and brain plasticity [1, 132]. Consequently, exercise and
BDNF have been associated with reduced symptoms of
depression and the promotion of cognitive enhancement
[141]. Therefore, exercise can influence the epigenome to
reduce depression and enhance cognitive abilities [1, 7, 142].
The BDNF levels in blood samples from subjects who
underwent endurance training for 3 months versus more
sedentary control subjects showed evidence that endurance
training increases BDNF release from the human brain [143].
Moreover, a review by Dishman et al. [3] suggested that
chronic physical activity increases the expression of genes
that encode several brain neurotrophins including BDNF and
nerve growth factor [3]. Widenfalk et al. [54] studied the
effects of physical training on the expression of neurotrophic
factors and their receptors in the brain using animal models
and also discovered that chronic exercise can increase the
expression of genes that encode several brain neurotrophins,
such as BDNF [54]. Recently, IF has also been prove n to
stimulate BDNF production in the hippocampus, cerebral
cortex, and striatum. Vasconcelos et al. [69] showed that the
reduction of hippocampal BDNF levels in an LPS-treated rat
model of systemic inflamm ation was prevented by IF [69].
Additionally, IF reduces cognitive deficits in rats by sup-
pressing the expression of pro-inflammat ory cytokines, such
as IL-1 b, and enhancing neurotrophic support. Because
BDNF plays critical roles in hippocampal synaptic plasticity
and cogni tive function, the maintenance of BDNF signaling
most likely contributes to the beneficial effects of IF on
cognitive function in LPS-treated rats [92]. There is now
ample evidence that both acute aerobic and anaerobic exer-
cise elevate serum BDNF in athletes and sedentary partici-
pants when compared with participants at rest. A wealth of
literature confirms that an acute bout of intensive aerobic or
anaerobic physical activity is capable of elevating serum
BDNF levels in both sedentary subjects and athletes [132,
144, 145]. Vaughan et al. [146] showed that a multimodal
exercise program resulted in neurocognitive and physical
performance improvements and increased levels of plasma
BDNF in older women when compared with controls.
Therefore, this study shows that both acute and chronic
exercise could increase BDNF concentrations in humans,
most likely explaining the positive effects of physical
activity on cognitive performance [146]. BDNF can also
reduce food intake, increasing glucose oxidation, which
consequently results in a decrease in blood glucose levels
and an increase in insulin sensitivity [147]. In animals, a
high-fat diet reduces the hippocampal concentration of
BDNF [148], but exercise is able to reverse this dietary-
related decrease [149].
In summary, studies on animals have shown that exer-
cise and IF increase BDN F expression in several regions of
the brain and that BDNF partially mediates the exercise-
and IF-induced enhancement of synaptic plasticity. BDNF
signaling in the brain may also affect behavioral and
metabolic responses to fasting and exercise, including the
regulation of appetite, activity levels, and peripheral glu-
cose metabolism.
5 Conclusion
Exercise impacts brain functioning, but information on the
effects of the combination of exercise and IF (CR, RIF and
IF) on cognitive function is scarce. Research has revealed
Fasting, Cognitive Performance and Exercise
123
Author's personal copy
that regular exercise enhances certain types of learning,
including the executive functions of cognition, learning,
and memory span, and it also stimulates neurogenesis.
Moreover, depending on the type of IF, cognitive function
and physical performance could be either improved or
negatively impacted. Several studies have demonstrated
that long-term food restriction was associated with
impairments in cognitive function, including poor perfor-
mance on a sustained attention task. However, other studies
have shown that memory performance significantly
improved during fasting. In the investigation of the
mechanisms by which dietary restriction acts on cognitive
function and to determine how these diets work, further
detailed and unified research studies are necessary.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Funding No sources of funding were used to assist in the prepa-
ration of this article.
Conflicts of interest Anissa Cherif, Bart Roelands, Romain
Meeusen, and Karim Chamari declare that they have no conflicts of
interest relevant to the content of this review.
References
1. Gomez-Pinilla F. The combined effects of exercise and foods in
preventing neurological and cognitive disorders. Prev Med.
2011;52(Suppl 1):S75–80.
2. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your
heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci.
2008;9(1):58–65.
3. Dishman RK, Berthoud HR, Booth FW, et al. Neurobiology of
exercise. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2006;14(3):345–56.
4. Chytrova G, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F. Exercise contributes to the
effects of DHA dietary supplementation by acting on membrane-
related synaptic systems. Brain Res. 2010;23(1341):32–40.
5. Meeusen R. Exercise, nutrition and the brain. Sports Med.
2014;44(Suppl 1):S47–56.
6. Gomez-Pinilla F. Collaborative effects of diet and exercise on
cognitive enhancement. Nutr Health. 2011;20(3–4):165–9.
7. Fabre C, Chamari K, Mucci P, et al. Improvement of cognitive
function by mental and/or individualized aerobic training in
healthy elderly subjects. Int J Sports Med. 2002;23(6):415–21.
8. Redman LM, Ravussin E. Caloric restriction in humans: impact
on physiological, psychological, and behavioral outcomes.
Antioxid Redox Signal. 2011;14(2):275–87.
9. Barnard ND, Bush AI, Ceccarelli A, et al. Dietary and lifestyle
guidelines for the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol
Aging. 2014;35(s2):S74–8.
10. Paoli A, Bianco A, Damiani E, et al. Ketogenic diet in neuro-
muscular and neurodegenerative diseases. Biomed Res Int.
2014;2014:10.
11. Chaouachi A, Coutts AJ, Chamari K, et al. Effect of Ramadan
intermittent fasting on aerobic and anaerobic performance and
perception of fatigue in male elite judo athletes. J Strength Cond
Res. 2009;23(9):2702–9.
12. Zerguini Y, Kirkendall D, Junge A, et al. Impact of Ramadan on
physical performance in professional soccer players. Br J Sports
Med. 2007;41(6):398–400.
13. Mouelhi Guizani S, Tenenbaum G, Bouzaouach I, et al. Infor-
mation-processing under incremental levels of physical loads:
comparing racquet to combat sports. J Sports Med Phys Fit.
2006;46(2):335–43.
14. Mouelhi Guizani S, Bouzaouach I, Tenenbaum G, et al. Simple
and choice reaction times under varying levels of physical load
in high skilled fencers. J Sports Med Phys Fit. 2006;46(2):
344–51.
15. Jarraya M, Chtourou H, Megdich K, et al. Effect of a moderate-
intensity aerobic exercise on estimates of egocentric distance.
Percept Mot Skills. 2013;116(2):658–70.
16. Ploughman M, Granter-Button S, Chernenko G, et al. Exercise
intensity influences the temporal profile of growth factors
involved in neuronal plasticity following focal ischemia. Brain
Res. 2007;30(1150):207–16.
17. Aloui A, Chaouachi A, Chtourou H, et al. Effects of Ramadan
on the diurnal variations of repeated-sprint performances. Int J
Sports Physiol Perform. 2013;8(3):254–62.
18. Shirreffs SM, Maughan RJ. Water and salt balance in young
male football player s in training during the holy month of
Ramadan. J Sports Sci. 2008;26(Suppl 3):S47–54.
19. Chaouachi A, Leiper JB, Souissi N, et al. Effects of Ramadan
intermittent fasting on sports performance and training: a
review. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2009;4(4):419–34.
20. Varady KA, Bhutani S, Klempel MC, et al. Alternate day fasting
for weight loss in normal weight and overweight subjects: a
randomized controlled trial. Nutr J. 2013;12:146.
21. Varady KA, Hellerstein MK. Alternate-day fasting and chronic
disease prevention: a review of human and animal trials. Am J
Clin Nutr. 2007;86(1):7–13.
22. Trepanowski JF, Bloomer RJ. The impact of religious fasting on
human health. Nutr J. 2010;9:57.
23. Mattson MP. Lifelong brain health is a lifelong challenge: From
evolutionary principles to empirical evidence. Ageing Res Rev.
2015;20:37–45.
24. Taormina G, Mirisola MG. Calorie restriction in mammals and
simple model organisms. Biomed Res Int. 2014;2014:1–10.
25. Singh R, Lakhanpal D, Kumar S, et al. Late-onset intermittent
fasting dietary restriction as a potential intervention to retard
age-associated brain function impairments in male rats. Age
(Dordr). 2012;34(4):917–33.
26. Singh Kalra RR, Fults DW. Preuss award 121 leptomeningeal
dissemination cascade in medulloblastoma. Neurosurgery.
2014;61(Suppl 1):198–9.
27. Lee J, Duan W, Mattson MP. Evidence that brain-derived neu-
rotrophic factor is required for basal neurogenesis and mediates,
in part, the enhancement of neurogenesis by dietary restriction in
the hippocampus of adult mice. J Neurochem. 2002;82(6):
1367–75.
28. Green MW, Elliman NA, Rogers PJ. Lack of effect of short-
term fasting on cognitive function. J Psychiatr Res. 1995;29(3):
245–53.
29. Yanai S, Okaichi Y, Okaichi H. Long-term dietary restriction
causes negative effects on cognitive functions in rats. Neurobiol
Aging. 2004;25(3):325–32.
30. Mattson MP. Challenging oneself intermittently to improve
health. Dose Response. 2014;12(4):600–18.
31. Mattson MP, Wan R. Beneficial effects of intermittent fasting
and caloric restriction on the cardiovascular and cerebrovascular
systems. J Nutr Biochem. 2005;16(3):129–37.
32. Green MW, Rogers PJ, Elliman NA, et al. Impairment of cog-
nitive performance associated with dieting and high levels of
dietary restraint. Physiol Behav. 1994;55(3):447–52.
33. Rogers PJ, Green MW. Dieting, dietary restraint and cognitive
performance. Br J Clin Psychol. 1993;32(Pt 1):113–6.
A. Cherif et al.
123
Author's personal copy
34. Witte AV, Fobker M, Gellner R, et al. Caloric restriction
improves memory in elderly humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.
2009;106(4):1255–60.
35. Develioglu ON, Sirazi S, Topak M, et al. Differences in mucociliary
activity of volunteers undergoing Ramadan versus Nineveh fasting.
Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2013;270(5):1655–9.
36. Gesundheit B. Medicine and Judaism—a patient is forbidden to
endanger his life in order to fast on Yom Kippur. Harefuah.
2009;148(9):583–5, 659.
37. Katz Y, Zangen D, Leibowitz G, et al. Diabetic patients in the
Yom Kippur fast—who can fast and how to treat the fasting
patients. Harefuah. 2009;148(9):586–91, 659, 8.
38. Chiu TH, Huang HY, Chiu YF, et al. Taiwanese vegetarians and
omnivores: dietary composition, prevalence of diabetes and IFG.
PLoS One. 2014;9(2):e88547.
39. Sarri KO, Tzanakis NE, Linardakis MK, et al. Effects of Greek
orthodox christian church fasting on serum lipids and obesity.
BMC Public Health. 2003;3:16.
40. Kadri N, Tilane A, El Batal M, et al. Irritability during the
month of Ramadan. Psychosom Med. 2000;62(2):280–5.
41. Chaouachi A, Leiper JB, Chtourou H, et al. The effects of
Ramadan intermittent fasting on athletic performance: recom-
mendations for the maintenance of physical fitness. J Sports Sci.
2012;30(Suppl 1):S53–73.
42. Tian HH, Aziz AR, Png W, et al. Effects of fasting during
Ramadan month on cognitive function in muslim athletes. Asian
J Sports Med. 2011;2(3):145–53.
43. Reilly T, Waterhouse J. Altered sleep-wake cycles and food
intake: the Ramadan model. Physiol Behav. 2007;90(2–3):
219–28.
44. Trabelsi K, Rebai H, El-Abed K, et al. Effect of Ramadan
fasting on body water status markers after a rugby sevens match.
Asian J Sports Med. 2011;2(3):186–94.
45. Sakamoto K, Grunewald KK. Beneficial effects of exercise on
growth of rats during intermittent fasting. J Nutr. 1987;117(2):
390–5.
46. Jongbloed F, de Bruin RW, Pennings JL, et al. Preoperative
fasting protects against renal ischemia-reperfusion injury in aged
and overweight mice. PLoS One. 2014;9(6):1–9.
47. Longo VD, Mattson MP. Fasting: molecular mechanisms and
clinical applications. Cell Metab. 2014;19(2):181–92.
48. Mattson MP. Energy intake and exercise as determinants of
brain health and vulnerability to injury and disease. Cell Metab.
2012;16(6):706–22.
49. Cahill GF Jr. Fuel metabolism in starvation. Annu Rev Nutr.
2006;26:1–22.
50. Izumida Y, Yahagi N, Takeuchi Y, et al. Glycogen shortage
during fasting triggers liver-brain-adipose neurocircuitry to
facilitate fat utilization. Nat Commun. 2013;4:8.
51. Greenberg AS, Coleman RA, Kraemer FB, et al. The role of
lipid droplets in metabolic disease in rodents and humans. J Clin
Invest. 2011;121(6):2102–10.
52. Viscarra JA, Ortiz RM. Cellular mechanisms regulating fuel
metabolism in mammals: role of adipose tissue and lipids during
prolonged food deprivation. Metabolism. 2013;62(7):889–97.
53. Mattson MP. Energy intake, meal frequency, and health: a
neurobiological perspective. Annu Rev Nutr. 2005;25:237–60.
54. Widenfalk J, Olson L, Thoren P. Deprived of habitual running,
rats downregulate BDNF and TrkB messages in the brain.
Neurosci Res. 1999;34(3):125–32.
55. Tong L, Shen H, Perreau VM, et al. Effects of exercise on gene-
expression profile in the rat hippocampus. Neurobiol Dis.
2001;8(6):1046–56.
56. Fenneni MA, Latiri I, Aloui A, et al. Effects of Ramadan on
physical capacities of North African boys fasting for the first
time. Libyan J Med. 2014;24(9):25391.
57. Damit NF, Lim VTW, Muhamed AMC, et al. Exercise
responses and training during daytime fasting in the month of
Ramadan and its impact on training-induced adaptations. Effects
of Ramadan Fasting on Health and Athletic Performance. 2015.
http://esciencecentral.org/ebooks/effects-of-ramadan-fasting/pdf/
exercise-responses-and-training-during-daytime-fasting-in-the-
month-of-ramadan-and-its-impact-on-traininginduced-adaptations.
pdf. Accessed 4 Jan 2015.
58. Trabelsi K, El Abed K, Trepanowski JF, et al. Effects of
Ramadan fasting on biochemical and anthropometric parameters
in physically active men. Asian J Sports Med. 2011;2(3):
134–44.
59. Trabelsi K, Stannard SR, Ghlissi Z, et al. Effect of fed- versus
fasted state resistance training during Ramadan on body com-
position and selected metabolic parameters in bodybuilders. J Int
Soc Sports Nutr. 2013;10(1):23.
60. Burke LM, King C. Ramadan fasting and the goals of sports
nutrition around exercise. J Sports Sci. 2012;30(Suppl 1):
S21–31.
61. Latifynia A, Vojgani M, Gharagozlou MJ, et al. Effect of
Ramadan on neutrophil’s respiratory burst (innate immunity)
and circulating immune complex. J Ayub Med Coll Abbottabad.
2008;20(3):128–31.
62. Chtourou H, Hammouda O, Souissi H, et al. The effect of
Ramadan fasting on physical performances, mood state and
perceived exertion in young footballers. Asian J Sports Med.
2011;2(3):177–85.
63. Intekhab A. Ramadan fasting in extreme latitudes. J Soc Health
Diabetes. 2014;2(1):2.
64. Amirfakhraei A, Alinaghizadeh A. The impact of praying and
fasting on the mental health of studentsattending the Bandar
Abbas Branch of Islamic Azad University in Iran in 2012. Life
Sci J. 2012;2012(9):6.
65. Briki W. Involvement in religion and self-regulation: explana-
tions of Muslims’ affects and behaviors during Ramadan effects
of Ramadan fasting on health and athletic performance. 2015.
http://esciencecentral.org/ebooks/effects-of-ramadan-fasting/pdf/
exercise-responses-and-training-during-daytime-fasting-in-the-
month-of-ramadan-and-its-impact-on-traininginduced-adaptations.
pdf. Accessed 6 Jan 2015.
66. Alabed H, Abuzayan K, Fgie KZ. Effects of length of time of
fasting upon subjective and objective variables when controlling
sleep, food and fluid intakes International Journal of Medical,
Health. Pharm Biomed Eng. 2014;8(5):9.
67. Guvenc A. Effects of Ramadan fasting on body composition,
aerobic performance and lactate, heart rate and perceptual
responses in young soccer players. J Hum Kinet. 2011;29:79–91.
68. Chtourou H, Hammouda M, Aloui A, et al. The optimal time of
day for training during Ramadan: a review study. J Fasting
Health. 2014;2:7.
69. Vasconcelos AR, Yshii LM, Viel TA, et al. Intermittent fasting
attenuates lipopolysaccharide-induced neuroinflammation and
memory impairment. J Neuroinflammation. 2014;11:85.
70. Fito M, Guxens M, Corella D, et al. Effect of a traditional
Mediterranean diet on lipoprotein oxidation: a randomized
controlled trial. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167(11):1195–203.
71. Liu X, Wu Z, Hayashi Y, et al. Age-dependent neuroinflam-
matory responses and deficits in long-term potentiation in the
hippocampus during systemic inflammation. Neuroscience.
2012;2(216):133–42.
72. Thomson LM, Sutherland RJ. Systemic administration of
lipopolysaccharide and interleukin-1beta have different effects
on memory consolidation. Brain Res Bull. 2005;67(1–2):24–9.
73. Calabrese F, Rossetti AC, Racagni G, et al. Brain-derived
neurotrophic factor: a bridge between inflammation and neuro-
plasticity. Front Cell Neurosci. 2014;8:430.
Fasting, Cognitive Performance and Exercise
123
Author's personal copy
74. Kim JJ, Diamond DM. The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plas-
ticity and lost memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(6):453–62.
75. Ben Menachem-Zidon O, Goshen I, Kreisel T, et al. Intrahip-
pocampal transplantation of transgenic neural precursor cells
overexpressing interleukin-1 receptor antagonist blocks chronic
isolation-induced impairment in memory and neurogenesis.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(9):2251–62.
76. Hein AM, Stasko MR, Matousek SB, et al. Sustained hip-
pocampal IL-1beta overexpression impairs contextual and spa-
tial memory in transgenic mice. Brain Behav Immun.
2010;24(2):243–53.
77. Shaftel SS, Kyrkanides S, Olschowka JA, et al. Sustained hip-
pocampal IL-1 beta overexpression mediates chronic neuroin-
flammation and ameliorates Alzheimer plaque pathology. J Clin
Invest. 2007;117(6):1595–604.
78. Heyser CJ, Masliah E, Samimi A, et al. Progressive decline in
avoidance learning paralleled by inflammatory neurodegenera-
tion in transgenic mice expressing interleukin 6 in the brain.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1997;94(4):1500–5.
79. Eyre H, Baune BT. Neuroplastic changes in depression: a role
for the immune system. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37(9):
1397–416.
80. Navalta JW, McFarlin BK, Lyons S, et al. Cognitive awareness
of carbohydrate intake does not alter exercise-induced lym-
phocyte apoptosis. Clinics (Sao Paulo). 2011;66(2):197–202.
81. Arumugam TV, Phillips TM, Cheng A, et al. Age and energy
intake interact to modify cell stress pathways and stroke out-
come. Ann Neurol. 2010;67(1):41–52.
82. Ugochukwu NH, Figgers CL. Caloric restriction inhibits up-
regulation of inflammatory cytokines and TNF-alpha, and acti-
vates IL-10 and haptoglobin in the plasma of streptozotocin-
induced diabetic rats. J Nutr Biochem. 2007;18(2):120–6.
83. Nadel L, Hardt O. Update on memory systems and processes.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(1):251–73.
84. Zhu B, Wang ZG, Ding J, et al. Chronic lipopolysaccharide
exposure induces cognitive dysfunction without affecting BDNF
expression in the rat hippocampus. Exp Ther Med. 2014;7(3):
750–4.
85. Krzyszton CP, Sparkman NL, Grant RW, et al. Exacerbated
fatigue and motor deficits in interleukin-10-deficient mice after
peripheral immune stimulation. Am J Physiol Regul Integr
Comp Physiol. 2008;295(4):R1109–14.
86. Mansur RB, Zugman A, Asevedo EM, et al. Cytokines in
schizophrenia: possible role of anti-inflammatory medications in
clinical and preclinical stages. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci.
2012;66(4):247–60.
87. Dantzer R, O’Connor JC, Freund GG, et al. From inflammation
to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates
the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(1):46–56.
88. Smith CJ, Emsley HC, Udeh CT, et al. Interleukin-1 receptor
antagonist reverses stroke-associated peripheral immune sup-
pression. Cytokine. 2012;58(3):384–9.
89. Richwine AF, Sparkman NL, Dilger RN, et al. Cognitive deficits
in interleukin-10-deficient mice after peripheral injection of
lipopolysaccharide. Brain Behav Immun. 2009;23(6):794–802.
90. Zhang XY, Liang J, da Chen C, et al. Low BDNF is associated
with cognitive impairment in chronic patients with schizophre-
nia. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2012;222(2):277–84.
91. Oral E, Canpolat S, Yildirim S, et al. Cognitive functions and
serum levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in patients
with major depressive disorder. Brain Res Bull. 2012;88(5):
454–9.
92. Lapchak PA, Araujo DM, Hefti F. Systemic interleukin-1 beta
decreases brain-derived neurotrophic factor messenger RNA
expression in the rat hippocampal formation. Neuroscience.
1993;53(2):297–301.
93. Trejo JL, Llorens-Martin MV, Torres-Aleman I. The effects of
exercise on spatial learning and anxiety-like behavior are
mediated by an IGF-I-dependent mechanism related to hip-
pocampal neurogenesis. Mol Cell Neurosci. 2008;37(2):402–11.
94. Nascimento CM, Pereira JR, de Andrade LP, et al. Physical
exercise in MCI elderly promotes reduction of pro-inflammatory
cytokines and improvements on cognition and BDNF peripheral
levels. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2014;11(8):799–805.
95. Whiteman AS, Young DE, He X, et al. Interaction between
serum BDNF and aerobic fitness predicts recognition memory in
healthy young adults. Behav Brain Res. 2014;1(259):302–12.
96. van Praag H, Fleshner M, Schwartz MW, et al. Exercise, energy
intake, glucose homeostasis, and the brain. J Neurosci.
2014;34(46):15139–49.
97. Draelos MT, Jacobson AM, Weinger K, et al. Cognitive function
in patients with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus during
hyperglycemia and hypoglycemia. Am J Med. 1995;98(2):
135–44.
98. Suzuki A, Stern SA, Bozdagi O, et al. Astrocyte-neuron lactate
transport is required for long-term memory formation. Cell.
2011;144(5):810–23.
99. Brown AM, Baltan Tekkok S, Ransom BR. Energy transfer from
astrocytes to axons: the role of CNS glycogen. Neurochem Int.
2004;45(4):529–36.
100. Hamprecht B, Verleysdonk S, Wiesinger H. Enzymes of car-
bohydrate and energy metabolism. In: Kettenmann H, Ransom
BR, editors. Neuroglia. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University
Press; 2005. p. 202–215.
101. Warren RE, Frier BM. Hypoglycaemia and cognitive function.
Diabetes Obes Metab. 2005;7(5):493–503.
102. Dalsgaard MK, Ide K, Cai Y, et al. The intent to exercise
influences the cerebral O(2)/carbohydrate uptake ratio in
humans. J Physiol. 2002;540(Pt 2):681–9.
103. Madsen PL, Cruz NF, Sokoloff L, et al. Cerebral oxygen/glu-
cose ratio is low during sensory stimulation and rises above
normal during recovery: excess glucose consumption during
stimulation is not accounted for by lactate efflux from or
accumulation in brain tissue. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab.
1999;19(4):393–400.
104. Williamson JW, McColl R, Mathews D, et al. Activation of the
insular cortex is affected by the intensity of exercise. J Appl
Physiol (1985). 1999;87(3):1213–9.
105. Sappey-Marinier D, Calabrese G, Fein G, et al. Effect of photic
stimulation on human visual cortex lactate and phosphates using
1H and 31P magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J Cereb Blood
Flow Metab. 1992;12(4):584–92.
106. Amigo I, Kowaltowski AJ. Dietary restriction in cerebral
bioenergetics and redox state. Redox Biol. 2014;2:296–304.
107. Attwell D, Laughlin SB. An energy budget for signaling in the
grey matter of the brain. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab.
2001;21(10):1133–45.
108. Mergenthaler P, Lindauer U, Dienel GA, et al. Sugar for the
brain: the role of glucose in physiological and pathological brain
function. Trends Neurosci. 2013;36(10):587–97.
109. Brown AM. Brain glycogen re-awakened. J Neurochem.
2004;89(3):537–52.
110. Rex A, Bert B, Fink H, et al. Stimulus-dependent changes of
extracellular glucose in the rat hippocampus determined by
in vivo microdialysis. Physiol Behav. 2009;98(4):467–73.
111. Bruss MD, Khambatta CF, Ruby MA, et al. Calorie restriction
increases fatty acid synthesis and whole body fat oxidation rates.
Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2010;298(1):E108–16.
112. Hammouda O, Chtourou H, Aloui A, et al. Concomitant effects
of Ramadan fasting and time-of-day on apolipoprotein AI, B,
Lp-a and homocysteine responses during aerobic exercise in
Tunisian soccer players. PLoS One. 2013;8(11):e79873.
A. Cherif et al.
123
Author's personal copy
113. Heilbronn LK, Smith SR, Martin CK, et al. Alternate-day fasting
in nonobese subjects: effects on body weight, body composition,
and energy metabolism. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;81(1):69–73.
114. Dresner A, Laurent D, Marcucci M, et al. Effects of free fatty
acids on glucose transport and IRS-1-associated phosphatidyli-
nositol 3-kinase activity. J Clin Invest. 1999;103(2):253–9.
115. Koutsari C, Jensen MD. Thematic review series: patient-ori-
ented research. Free fatty acid metabolism in human obesity.
J Lipid Res. 2006;47:8.
116. Boden G, She P, Mozzoli M, et al. Free fatty acids produce
insulin resistance and activate the proinflammatory nuclear
factor-kappaB pathway in rat liver. Diabetes. 2005;54(12):
3458–65.
117. Benton D. Dehydration influences mood and cognition: a plau-
sible hypothesis? Nutrients. 2011;3(5):555–73.
118. Benefer MD, Corfe BM, Russell JM, et al. Water intake and
post-exercise cognitive performance: an observational study of
long-distance walkers and runners. Eur J Nutr. 2013;52(2):
617–24.
119. Lieberman HR, Bathalon GP, Falco CM, et al. Severe decre-
ments in cognition function and mood induced by sleep loss,
heat, dehydration, and undernutrition during simulated combat.
Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(4):422–9.
120. Adam GE, Carter R 3rd, Cheuvront SN, et al. Hydration effects
on cognitive performance during military tasks in temperate and
cold environments. Physiol Behav. 2008;93(4–5):748–56.
121. Burke L. Practical issues in nutrition for athletes. J Sports Sci.
1995;13:Spec No:S83–90.
122. Edwards AM, Mann ME, Marfell-Jones MJ, et al. Influence of
moderate dehydration on soccer performance: physiological
responses to 45 min of outdoor match-play and the immediate
subsequent performance of sport-specific and mental concen-
tration tests. Br J Sports Med. 2007;41(6):385–91.
123. Edmonds CJ, Crombie R, Gardner MR. Subjective thirst mod-
erates changes in speed of responding associated with water
consumption. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:363.
124. Riebl SK, Davy BM. The hydration equation: update on water
balance and cognitive performance. ACSMs Health Fit J.
2013;17(6):21–8.
125. Ganio MS, Armstrong LE, Casa DJ, et al. Mild dehydration
impairs cognitive performance and mood of men. Br J Nutr.
2011;106(10):1535–43.
126. Kempton MJ, Ettinger U, Foster R, et al. Dehydration affects
brain structure and function in healthy adolescents. Hum Brain
Mapp. 2011;32(1):71–9.
127. NasrAllah MM, Osman NA. Fasting during the month of
Ramadan among patients with chronic kidney disease: renal and
cardiovascular outcomes. Clin Kidney J. 2014;7(4):348–53.
128. Vaynman S, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F. Interplay between brain-
derived neurotrophic factor and signal transduction modulators
in the regulation of the effects of exercise on synaptic-plasticity.
Neuroscience. 2003;122(3):647–57.
129. Ninan I. Synaptic regulation of affective behaviors; role of
BDNF. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76(Part C):684–95.
130. Vaynman S, Ying Z, Wu A, et al. Coupling energy metabolism
with a mechanism to support brain-derived neurotrophic factor-
mediated synaptic plasticity. Neuroscience. 2006;139(4):1221–34.
131. Mattson MP. Glutamate and neurotrophic factors in neuronal
plasticity and disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1144:97–112.
132. Knaepen K, Goekint M, Heyman EM, et al. Neuroplasticity—
exercise-induced response of peripheral brain-derived neu-
rotrophic factor: a systematic review of experimental studies in
human subjects. Sports Med. 2010;40(9):765–801.
133. Egan MF, Kojima M, Callicott JH, et al. The BDNF val66met
polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF
and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell. 2003;
112(2):257–69.
134. Hariri AR, Goldberg TE, Mattay VS, et al. Brain-derived neu-
rotrophic factor val66met polymorphism affects human mem-
ory-related hippocampal activity and predicts memory
performance. J Neurosci. 2003;23(17):6690–4.
135. Kim SJ, Linden DJ. Ubiquitous plasticity and memory storage.
Neuron. 2007;56(4):582–92.
136. Ramsey MM, Adams MM, Ariwodola OJ, et al. Functional
characterization of des-IGF-1 action at excitatory synapses in
the CA1 region of rat hippocampus. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94(1):
247–54.
137. Anlar B, Sullivan KA, Feldman EL. Insulin-like growth factor-I
and central nervous system development. Horm Metab Res.
1999;31(2–3):120–5.
138. Saatman KE, Contreras PC, Smith DH, et al. Insulin-like growth
factor-1 (IGF-1) improves both neurological motor and cogni-
tive outcome following experimental brain injury. Exp Neurol.
1997;147(2):418–27.
139. Gomez-Pinilla F, Vaynman S, Ying Z. Brain-derived neu-
rotrophic factor functions as a metabotrophin to mediate the
effects of exercise on cognition. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(11):
2278–87.
140. Gomez-Pinilla F, Zhuang Y, Feng J, et al. Exercise impacts
brain-derived neurotrophic factor plasticity by engaging mech-
anisms of epigenetic regulation. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33(3):
383–90.
141. Lee IH, Seo EJ, Lim IS. Effects of aquatic exercise and CES
treatment on the changes of cognitive function, BDNF, IGF-1,
and VEGF of persons with intellectual disabilities. J Exerc Nutr
Biochem. 2014;18(1):19–24.
142. Fabre C, Masse-Biron J, Chamari K, et al. Evaluation of quality
of life in elderly healthy subjects after aerobic and/or mental
training. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 1999;28(1):9–22.
143. Seifert T, Brassard P, Wissenberg M, et al. Endurance training
enhances BDNF release from the human brain. Am J Physiol
Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2010;298(2):R372–7.
144. Babaei P, Damirchi A, Mehdipoor M, et al. Long term habitual
exercise is associated with lower resting level of serum BDNF.
Neurosci Lett. 2014;30(566):304–8.
145. Mang CS, Campbell KL, Ross CJ, et al. Promoting neuroplas-
ticity for motor rehabilitation after stroke: considering the
effects of aerobic exercise and genetic variation on brain-
derived neurotrophic factor. Phys Ther. 2013;93(12):1707–16.
146. Vaughan S, Wallis M, Polit D, et al. The effects of multimodal
exercise on cognitive and physical functioning and brain-derived
neurotrophic factor in older women: a randomised controlled
trial. Age Ageing. 2014;43(5):623–9.
147. Stranahan AM, Norman ED, Lee K, et al. Diet-induced insulin
resistance impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity and cognition
in middle-aged rats. Hippocampus. 2008;18(11):1085–8.
148. Chung JY, Yoo DY, Im W, et al. Electroacupuncture at the
Zusanli and Baihui acupoints ameliorates type-2 diabetes-in-
duced reductions in proliferating cells and differentiated neu-
roblast in the hippocampal dentate gyrus with increasing brain-
derived neurotrophic factor levels. J Vet Med Sci. 2015;77(2):
167–73.
149. Molteni R, Wu A, Vaynman S, et al. Exercise reverses the
harmful effects of consumption of a high-fat diet on synaptic
and behavioral plasticity associated to the action of brain-
derived neurotrophic factor. Neuroscience. 2004;123(2):429–40.
Fasting, Cognitive Performance and Exercise
123
Author's personal copy
... Among them, the activation of anti-inflammatory response is one of the mechanisms by which CR is associated with cognitive function. Inflammatory-triggered neuroinflammation may result in deficits in learning and memory, exemplified by interleukin-6 (IL-6) and tumor necrosis factor (TNFα), potentially disrupting cognitive processes via neurogenesis and neuroplasticity pathways, certain animal studies indicate that CR elevates IL-10, an anti-inflammatory agent, while reducing IL-6 and TNFα levels to safeguard neuron survival and growth [9]. CR is considered to be one of the most effective ways to improve cognitive performance in rodents, especially protective against cognitive deficits caused by aging or chronic diseases. ...
... Appropriate CR not only improves the performance of rats in behavioral tests of learning and memory, but also enhances their brain function and synaptic plasticity, and increases the generation of new neurons. As an illustration, CR could elevate the hippocampus's brain-derived trophic factor (BDNF) levels by activating the gene expression of heat shock proteins, BDNF is widely present in the brain, plays a pivotal role in neuronal survival, growth, and operation, and is essential for cognitive functions like memory acquisition and emotional control [9,11,12]. Based on the optimization of nutrients, feeding strategy and other considerations, the researchers have designed the appropriate CR protocol under these conditions and proved after six months of experiments that CR could enhance the cognitive function of KM mice [13]. ...
... In human research, CR can not only analyze changes in the brain and explain the mechanisms associated with cognitive performance, but also prevent the onset of a variety of diseases associated with aging [9]. Oxidative stress is another potential mechanism through which CR is associated with cognitive function. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of Review The impact of dietary habits on cognitive function is increasingly gaining attention. The review is to discuss how caloric restriction (CR) and intermittent fasting (IF) can enhance cognitive function in healthy states through multiple pathways that interact with one another. Secondly, to explore the effects of CR and IF on cognitive function in conditions of neurodegenerative diseases, obesity diabetes and aging, as well as potential synergistic effects in combination with exercise to prevent cognitively related neurodegenerative diseases. Recent Findings With age, the human brain ages and develops corresponding neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and epilepsy, which in turn trigger cognitive impairment. Recent research indicates that the impact of diet and exercise on cognitive function is increasingly gaining attention. Summary The benefits of exercise for cognitive function and brain plasticity are numerous, and future research can examine the efficacy of particular dietary regimens during physical activity when combined with diet which can prevent cognitive decline.
... Future research should focus on developing personalized exercise prescriptions that consider these factors, as well as investigating the potential synergistic effects of combining exercise with other interventions, such as cognitive training or pharmacological treatments (Bherer et al., 2021;Castellote-Caballero et al., 2024). Other promising approaches could be considered as performing mixed non-pharmacological interventions such as exercise and fasting (Cherif et al., 2016). Indeed, the separated effects of these interventions on neuroplasticity could be potentiated, but this has to be investigated from an outcome and safety perspective. ...
Article
Full-text available
This review aimed to elucidate the mechanisms through which (i) physical activity (PA) enhances neuroplasticity and cognitive function in neurodegenerative disorders, and (ii) identify specific PA interventions for improving cognitive rehabilitation programs. We conducted a literature search in PubMed, Medline, Scopus, Web of Science, and PsycINFO, covering publications from January 1990 to August 2024. The search strategy employed key terms related to neuroplasticity, physical exercise, cognitive function, neurodegenerative disorders, and personalized physical activity. Inclusion criteria included original research on the relationship between PA and neuroplasticity in neurodegenerative disorders, while exclusion criteria eliminated studies focusing solely on pharmacological interventions. The review identified multiple pathways through which PA may enhance neuroplasticity, including releasing neurotrophic factors, modulation of neuroinflammation, reduction of oxidative stress, and enhancement of synaptic connectivity and neurogenesis. Aerobic exercise was found to increase hippocampal volume by 1–2% and improve executive function scores by 5–10% in older adults. Resistance training enhanced cognitive control and memory performance by 12–18% in elderly individuals. Mind–body exercises, such as yoga and tai-chi, improved gray matter density in memory-related brain regions by 3–5% and enhanced emotional regulation scores by 15–20%. Dual-task training improved attention and processing speed by 8–14% in individuals with neurodegenerative disorders. We also discuss the potential role of AI-based exercise and AI cognitive training in preventing and rehabilitating neurodegenerative illnesses, highlighting innovative approaches to personalized interventions and improved patient outcomes. PA significantly enhances neuroplasticity and cognitive function in neurodegenerative disorders through various mechanisms. Aerobic exercise, resistance training, mind–body practices, and dual-task exercises each offer unique cognitive benefits. Implementing these activities in clinical settings can improve patient outcomes. Future research should focus on creating personalized interventions tailored to specific conditions, incorporating personalized physical exercise programs to optimize cognitive rehabilitation.
... IMPACT ON COGNITIVE FUNCTION IF appears to offer various benefits for brain health and cognitive performance. When combined with regular physical activity and exercise, IF has been associated with improved physical and mental performance, particularly in athletes [44]. Potential advantages have been identified for brain-related conditions, including epilepsy, Alzheimer's disease, and multiple sclerosis. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction:Obesity rates have risen sharply in recent decades, with one in eight people worldwide living with obesity in 2022. This has driven interest in the search for effective and sustainable dietary strategies. Intermittent fasting (IF), which alternates periods of eating and fasting, has gained considerable attention as a tool for weight management and overall health improvement. Unlike traditional calorie-restricted diets, IF’s simplicity and flexibility make it more accessible, potentially increasing adherence and long-term sustainability. Purpose:This study reviews current knowledge on IF, evaluating its physiological mechanisms, health benefits, risks, and long-term implications. It focuses on weight management, glucose metabolism, cardiovascular health, cognitive function, circadian rhythm, and sports performance, while addressing challenges related to safety and sustainability. Materials and Methods:This study reviews existing literature, including clinical trials, meta-analyses, and observational studies. It compares IF protocols such as alternate-day fasting (ADF), the 5:2 diet, and time-restricted eating (TRE), emphasizing physiological effects and practical implications. Conclusion:Intermittent fasting represents a promising dietary intervention with wide-ranging health benefits, from weight management to improved metabolic and cognitive health. However, challenges such as nutritional deficiencies, risks of hypoglycemia, and limited long-term data highlight the need for personalized approaches and further investigation. Future research should prioritize large-scale, long-term studies to refine fasting protocols and ensure safety and effectiveness across diverse populations. By addressing these gaps, IF could evolve into a sustainable, evidence-based strategy for improving global health outcomes.
... Despite the benefits of CR on physical performance, fatigue, inflammation, and immunity we recognized key aspects of implementing CR. Moderate and severe CR could decline cognitive behaviour and memory due to decreased blood glucose (Cherif et al., 2016;Zouhal et al., 2020). In players undergoing CR, short intervals to stop intervention or "diet breaks" are advisable. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: In athletes, caloric restriction (CR) improves physiological mechanisms, although its effects on professional soccer players are unclear. This study aims to evaluate the effects of CR on physical performance, fatigue, and inflammation in male professional soccer players compared with a no-restriction diet. Methods: This was a controlled, randomized, parallel-group study with 28 participants. The experimental group received a CR diet (−25% of recommended energy intake; mean caloric intake: 2650 kcal/d). Controls received a normal caloric (NC) diet (mean caloric intake: 3500 kcal/d). Both groups received a protein supplement. Six weeks of intervention were followed by 6 weeks without intervention. Thereafter, the participants were allowed to eat ad libitum. The study evaluated leukocytes, lymphocytes, creatine phosphokinase (CPK), urea, testosterone, lactate dehydrogenase (LDH), rate of perceived exertion (RPE), countermovement-jump (CMJ), and squat jump (SJ). Results: Average age was 27.6 ± 4.4 years. After 6 and 12 weeks, differences between the two groups were insignificant in terms of the immune response, fatigue (CPK, urea, testosterone, and cortisol), and inflammation (LDH) (p > .05). The CR group had lower RPE levels at 12 weeks (0.01 vs. 0.62 points; p = .001) than the NC group. Conclusion: CR is an effective intervention for male professional soccer players, because it decreased RPE while preserving biochemical parameters.
... In this specific scenario, an important role is also observed in the energy provided by protein metabolism, where normally its relevance is much less [50,51]. In contrast, there are many studies in the literature concerning the adaptations of various types of fasting in chronic [51][52][53][54]. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Nowadays, the focus on health and nutrition has developed greatly. Literature data suggest that a healthy lifestyle is positively correlated with a reduction in numerous risk factors. In addition to the classic nutritional schemes, such as the Mediterranean diet or the Okinawa or vegan diet, fasting is becoming a choice that many people follow. But how many types of fasting are there? Are they all optimal? Are they advisable for all individuals? What is the impact of fasting on physical activity? Literature data show that glycaemic control, pre-workout meal and post-workout meal, are crucial for improved performance. But what are our body’s adaptations to fasted training? As happens, for example, during the period of RAMADAN. Is there a sport in which fasting can be most useful for performance purposes?
... Studies have demonstrated that learning ability is influenced by the activity of BDNF. As a crucial neurotrophin, BDNF is a major factor in the consequences of cognitive plasticity [26]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aqueous extract of Lycium ruthenicum Murray (LRE) could attenuate neuroinflammation in mice induced by a high-fat and high-fructose diet (HFFD). Moreover, LRE could adjust bile acid (BA) metabolism and the gut microbiota. Behavioral tests revealed that LRE prevented HFFD-induced cognitive deficits. The treatment of LRE resulted in a decreased expression of inflammation-related mRNA of TNF-α, IL-6, and IL-1b in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus. Furthermore, LRE ameliorated gut microbiota disorder caused by HFFD by markedly elevating the relative abundances of Streptococcus and probiotics such as Lactococcus. Concurrently, it reduced the relative abundances of Helicobacter and Clostridium_XIVa. The levels of tauroursodeoxycholic acid, known for its neuroprotective property, and taurocholic acid, recognized as an anti-inflammatory agent, were significantly enhanced in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex due to the treatment with LRE. In a word, LRE might have the potential to alleviate HFFD-induced cognitive dysfunction by modulating intestinal microbiota and promoting the synthesis of neuroprotective BAs.
... Still, there is limited academic evidence concerning whether hunger induced by fasting impacts short-term cognitive ability. Given the limited and inconclusive results of studies on the relationship between intermittent fasting and cogntitive ability, Cherif, et al. [30] concluded that "several studies have demonstrated that long-term food restriction was associated with impairments in cognitive function, including poor performance on a sustained attention task. However, other studies have shown that memory performance significantly improved during fasting. ...
Article
Full-text available
Growing evidence suggests that resource scarcity can severely impede individuals’ cognitive capacity, resulting in sub-optimal decision making. Few experimental studies investigate whether food deprivation as a form of resource scarcity influences decisions in other non-hunger related domains. We examine the effect of short term fasting on cognitive capacity by exogenously manipulating individuals’ fasting time in a laboratory experiment. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three treatments: 1) 3-hour fast; 2) 12-hour fast; and 3) control, in which participants were not required to fast and consumed a protein shake upon arriving to the lab. Following the manipulation, participants completed the Raven’s Progressive Matrices test which measures cognitive function. Although we find null treatment effects on cognitive ability, our results provide evidence that short term fasting does not directly inhibit cognition.
... Reducing sedentary behavior and increasing physical activity have been shown to significantly improve metabolic and physical function, particularly in older adults engaged in exercise training programs [145]. Exercise interacts with dietary factors and has neurocognitive benefits on brain functioning [146][147][148]. Valuable insights were gained into how caloric restriction, combined with diet and physical activity, can improve cognitive function and overall health [149][150][151][152]. ...
Article
Full-text available
With the aging of the global population, neurodegenerative diseases are emerging as a major public health issue. The adoption of a less sedentary lifestyle has been shown to have a beneficial effect on cognitive decline, but the molecular mechanisms responsible are less clear. Here we provide a detailed analysis of the complex molecular, cellular, and systemic mechanisms underlying age-related cognitive decline and how lifestyle choices influence these processes. A review of the evidence from animal models, human studies, and postmortem analyses emphasizes the importance of integrating physical exercise with cognitive, multisensory, and motor stimulation as part of a multifaceted approach to mitigating cognitive decline. We highlight the potential of these non-pharmacological interventions to address key aging hallmarks, such as genomic instability, telomere attrition, and neuroinflammation, and underscore the need for comprehensive and personalized strategies to promote cognitive resilience and healthy aging. Citation: Diniz, D.G.; Bento-Torres, J.; da Costa, V.O.; Carvalho, J.P.R.; Tomás, A.M.; Galdino de Oliveira, T.C.; Soares, F.C.; de Macedo, L.D.e.D.; Jardim, N.Y.V.; Bento-Torres, N.V.O.; et al. The Hidden Dangers of Sedentary Living: Insights into Molecular, Cellular, and Systemic Mechanisms.
Article
Full-text available
Fasting is a voluntary action carried out by restraining the appetite for eating, drinking and anything that could break it for a certain period of time. One example of fasting is Ramadan fasting. Ramadan fasting is a fast that is carried out during the month of Ramadan for 29 days or 30 days. The time for fasting during Ramadan begins when the sun rises at dawn until sunset. Fasting is a voluntary action carried out by restraining the appetite for eating, drinking and anything that could break it for a certain period of time. The time for fasting during Ramadan begins when the sun rises at dawn until sunset. Fasting is a deep practice in Islam and has the potential to impact human health, especially in the context of obesity and diabetes. Aim: To determine the effect of fasting according to Islamic and scientific perspectives in reducing weight in obese and diabetic patients. Methods: This research involved a careful literature review and analysis of data from a variety of relevant sources. We evaluated scientific research on fasting in the context of weight loss and diabetes management. We also combine it with the Islamic religious view of fasting, especially during the month of Ramadan. Results: Fasting, both from an Islamic and scientific perspective, can have a positive impact on obese and diabetic patients. Ramadan fasting, which involves fasting from dawn to sunset, has been linked to improved blood sugar control and weight loss in diabetes patients. Additionally, intermittent fasting, such as the 16/8 method, has been shown to be effective in improving insulin sensitivity and reducing body weight in obese individuals. Studies also show that fasting can reduce the risk of heart disease, which is often associated with diabetes. Discussion: This article discusses health implications from two points of view: religion and science. Combining Islamic views on fasting with scientific research provides a strong basis for applying fasting methods in the management of obesity and diabetes. Conclusion: Fasting, from both an Islamic and scientific perspective, has great potential in reducing weight and managing diabetes in obese patients. This approach may be an effective adjunct in the treatment of this condition. Therefore, it is important to understand and utilize the benefits of fasting wisely, taking into account religious views and medical recommendations..
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Ramadan requires individuals to abstain from food and fluid intake between sunrise and sunset; physiological considerations predict that poorer mood, physical performance and mental performance will result. In addition, any difficulties will be worsened because preparations for fasting and recovery from it often mean that nocturnal sleep is decreased in length, and this independently affects mood and performance. A difficulty of interpretation in many studies is that the observed changes could be due to fasting but also to the decreased length of sleep and altered food and fluid intakes before and after the daytime fasting. These factors were separated in this study, which took place over three separate days and compared the effects of different durations of fasting (4, 8 or 16h) upon a wide variety of measures (including subjective and objective assessments of performance, body composition, dehydration and responses to a short bout of exercise)-but with an unchanged amount of nocturnal sleep, controlled supper the previous evening, controlled intakes at breakfast and daytime naps not being allowed. Many of the negative effects of fasting observed in previous studies were present in this experiment also. These findings indicate that fasting was responsible for many of the changes previously observed, though some effect of sleep loss, particularly if occurring on successive days (as would occur in Ramadan) cannot be excluded.
Article
Full-text available
Literature concerning the effects of Ramadan fasting on sports performance presents conflicting results. In this context, some studies reported a significant impairment of sports performance during the month of Ramadan. However, other studies suggested that Ramadan fasting has no significant effect on physical performance. The discrepancies between the studies could be explained by time-of-day variations in testing. In this regard, recent studies reported that Ramadan negatively affects the afternoon sports performance; however, the morning and the evening (after breaking the fast) performances were not affected by fasting. This suggests that the optimal time of day for training during Ramadan is the morning or the evening. Therefore, coaches should schedule the training sessions in the morning or evening during the month of Ramadan. However, further studies should investigate the effect of training at a specific time of day on sports performance during Ramadan.
Article
Full-text available
Cytokines are key regulatory mediators involved in the host response to immunological challenges, but also play a critical role in the communication between the immune and the central nervous system. For this, their expression in both systems is under a tight regulatory control. However, pathological conditions may lead to an overproduction of pro-inflammatory cytokines that may have a detrimental impact on central nervous system. In particular, they may damage neuronal structure and function leading to deficits of neuroplasticity, the ability of nervous system to perceive, respond and adapt to external or internal stimuli. In search of the mechanisms by which pro-inflammatory cytokines may affect this crucial brain capability, we will discuss one of the most interesting hypotheses: the involvement of the neurotrophin brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which represents one of the major mediators of neuroplasticity.
Article
BDNF plays a critical role in activity-dependent neuroplasticity underlying learning and memory in the hippocampus. A frequent single nucleotide polymorphism in the targeting region of the human BDNF gene (val ⁶⁶ met) has been associated with abnormal intracellular trafficking and regulated secretion of BDNF in cultured hippocampal neurons transfected with the met allele. In addition, the met allele has been associated with abnormal hippocampal neuronal function as well as impaired episodic memory in human subjects, but a direct effect of BDNF alleles on hippocampal processing of memory has not been demonstrated. We studied the relationship of the BDNF val ⁶⁶ met genotype and hippocampal activity during episodic memory processing using blood oxygenation level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging and a declarative memory task in healthy individuals. Met carriers exhibited relatively diminished hippocampal engagement in comparison with val homozygotes during both encoding and retrieval processes. Remarkably, the interaction between the BDNF val ⁶⁶ met genotype and the hippocampal response during encoding accounted for 25% of the total variation in recognition memory performance. These data implicate a specific genetic mechanism for substantial normal variation in human declarative memory and suggest that the basic effects of BDNF signaling on hippocampal function in experimental animals are important in humans.
Article
This chapter discusses enzymes of carbohydrate and energy metabolism in glial cells. These include enzymes of glucose metabolism, enzymes of glycogen metabolism, carbonic anhydrases, and enzymes of cellular energization.
Article
Introduction: Mental health issues are common in Iran. This study examined the impact of praying and fasting on the mental health of students attending the Bandar Abbas Branch of Islamic Azad University in Iran in 2012. Methods: A total of 200 undergraduate students (85 girls and 115 boys) at the Bandar Abbas Branch of Islamic Azad University were selected as the sample using a multi-stage cluster random sampling process. The GHQ-28 mental health questionnaire was administered to them 2 weeks before Ramadan as a pre-test and 2 weeks after Ramadan as a post-test. Results: After analyzing data using a one-way ANOVA test and t-test, it was revealed that people who fasted the whole month of Ramadan or most of it, even they did so just for amusement, received more favorable scores on all the mental health subscales; meanwhile, reduced scores were evident after Ramadan among those who did not fast at all or, due to religious or medical reasons, could not fast. In addition, people who always or usually prayed also received higher scores on the mental health subscales. Conclusion: The results of this study indicate that fasting, even for amusement purposes, enhances individuals' mental health. In addition, people who always or usually pray have higher mental health scores than those who never or rarely pray.