ArticlePDF Available

The Resilience Portfolio Model: Understanding Healthy Adaptation in Victims of Violence

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Objective: Exposure to diverse forms of interpersonal violence is associated with a wide range of psychological problems in children and adults. However, many people who experience violence do not develop symptoms of psychopathology. Studies of resilience in victims of violence have identified protective factors associated with healthier outcomes but have a number of limitations for understanding how individuals exposed to violence adapt and even thrive. The present article addresses these limitations by introducing a conceptual framework that integrates insights from theory and research on resilience, positive psychology, posttraumatic growth, and stress and coping. Approach: The Resilience Portfolio Model is a strengths-based framework designed to provide a holistic understanding of the protective factors and processes that promote resilience in children and adults exposed to violence. It proposes that the density and diversity of resources and assets available to individuals (their resilience portfolio) shapes their responses to violence, and identifies 3 higher-order functional categories of strengths that are proposed to be particularly salient for resilience: regulatory, interpersonal, and meaning-making strengths. Conclusion: The Resilience Portfolio Model offers new directions for studying resilience in victims of violence and identifies a wider range of strengths and protective factors to address in prevention and intervention efforts.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Resilience Portfolio Model: Understanding Healthy Adaptation in
Victims of Violence
John Grych
Marquette University
Sherry Hamby
Sewanee, University of the South
Victoria Banyard
University of New Hampshire
Objective: Exposure to diverse forms of interpersonal violence is associated with a wide range of
psychological problems in children and adults. However, many people who experience violence do not
develop symptoms of psychopathology. Studies of resilience in victims of violence have identified
protective factors associated with healthier outcomes but have a number of limitations for understanding
how individuals exposed to violence adapt and even thrive. The present article addresses these limitations
by introducing a conceptual framework that integrates insights from theory and research on resilience,
positive psychology, posttraumatic growth, and stress and coping. Approach: The Resilience Portfolio
Model is a strengths-based framework designed to provide a holistic understanding of the protective
factors and processes that promote resilience in children and adults exposed to violence. It proposes that
the density and diversity of resources and assets available to individuals (their resilience portfolio) shapes
their responses to violence, and identifies 3 higher-order functional categories of strengths that are
proposed to be particularly salient for resilience: regulatory, interpersonal, and meaning-making
strengths. Conclusion: The Resilience Portfolio Model offers new directions for studying resilience in
victims of violence and identifies a wider range of strengths and protective factors to address in
prevention and intervention efforts.
Keywords: adaptation, adversity, positive psychology, protective factors, resilience
Research on the effects of violence on human functioning has
focused primarily on the negative outcomes that follow from these
experiences. This work has provided an important foundation for
describing the adverse impact of diverse forms of violence and
spurred the need for action at the levels of policy, prevention, and
intervention. However, many people exposed to violence exhibit
healthy functioning (e.g., Bonanno, 2004;Masten, 2001), and if
we are to develop a more complete understanding of the effects of
violence and identify more effective ways for helping those who
experience it, we will need to investigate the processes that ac-
count for positive as well as negative outcomes. Studies of resil-
ience in victims of violence have identified a number of protective
factors associated with better adjustment, but this work has limi-
tations for explaining the mechanisms that foster healthy adapta-
tion. The goal of this article is to introduce a framework, the
Resilience Portfolio Model, that is rooted in theory and research on
resilience but integrates insights from the fields of positive psy-
chology, posttraumatic growth, and coping to provide a more
comprehensive understanding of how individuals build fulfilling
lives despite their exposure to violence.
In the sections to follow, we highlight key contributions from
several lines of research that have examined how people adapt to
adversity and identify limitations of each for understanding
healthy functioning in victims of violence. We then introduce the
Resilience Portfolio Model, describing the strengths and protective
factors proposed to contribute to well-being and the specific path-
ways through which they are proposed to foster resilience. Finally,
we outline the implications of the model for research, prevention,
and intervention.
Research on Adaptation to Violence
Several fields of study have investigated healthy functioning in
individuals exposed to adversity, but they have developed rela-
tively independently and rarely have been integrated in the same
explanatory models. Research on different forms of violence sim-
ilarly is siloed, with most studies focusing on a single type of
violence (see Hamby & Grych, 2013). Recent work demonstrates
the interconnected and overlapping nature of different forms of
This article was published Online First August 24, 2015.
John Grych, Department of Psychology, Marquette University; Sherry
Hamby, Department of Psychology, Sewanee, University of the South;
Victoria Banyard, Department of Psychology, University of New Hamp-
shire.
The opinions expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not
necessarily reflect the views of the John Templeton Foundation. This
project was made possible through the support of a grant from the John
Templeton Foundation. We thank Al Bardi for his input on terminology,
Ed de St. Aubin, Christina Caiozzo, and Jessica Houston for their helpful
comments on an earlier draft of this article, and Liz Taylor for her
assistance with the references.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John
Grych, Department of Psychology, Marquette University, 604 North 16th
Street, Milwaukee, WI 53233. E-mail: John.grych@marquette.edu
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Psychology of Violence © 2015 American Psychological Association
2015, Vol. 5, No. 4, 343–354 2152-0828/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039671
343
violence (Hamby & Grych, 2013), and suggests that exposure to
different types of violence has similar outcomes and may operate
through similar mechanisms. The processes that promote resilience
in victims of diverse forms of violence also may be similar, and if
so, this argues for the value of integrating findings from related
areas of study to develop overarching models that describe path-
ways from violence to healthy outcomes.
Contributions From Research on Resilience
The term “resilience” has been used to refer both to healthy
functioning after exposure to significant adversity and to the
capacities needed to adapt successfully to significant adversity
(e.g., Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000;Masten, 2007,2011).
Resilience is best understood as a dynamic process rather than a
stable quality of a person because it depends on the constellation
of stressors, risk, and protective factors that characterize the per-
son’s life at a particular time (e.g., Masten, 2011;Rutter, 2012).
Research on resilience in different populations and following
different types of adverse events has identified a set of individual,
relationship, and community factors that are consistently associ-
ated with healthy functioning, including self-regulation, secure
attachment, and neighborhood collective efficacy (for a review,
see Masten, 2007), and described general mechanisms by which
protective factors give rise to adaptive outcomes (e.g., Luthar et
al., 2000).
The identification of these protective factors has made an important
contribution to clinical psychology, and several dozen studies have
reported associations between many of them and positive outcomes in
individuals exposed to different types of violence (for a review, see
Houston & Grych, 2015). However, this work also has limitations for
understanding the processes that promote resilience in victims of
violence. First, many of the protective factors studied simply are the
inverse of risk factors (e.g., Masten & Tellegen, 2012). High levels of
constructs such as emotion regulation and parental warmth are reli-
ably related to healthy functioning, but low levels of these same
constructs are associated with greater symptomatology; labeling the
opposite pole of a continuous variable documented to be risk factor as
a “protective factor” does not reveal anything new about the nature of
the association between that construct and health. Second, most stud-
ies of resilience in victims of violence have defined good outcomes in
terms of the absence of pathology rather than the presence of health;
a recent review found that 2/3 of studies examining resilience in
children exposed to violence used a measure of symptomatology as
the sole measure of adjustment, with low levels of symptoms repre-
senting adaptive functioning (Houston & Grych, 2015). Although low
levels of pathology clearly are desirable, the emphasis on pathology
reflects a narrow view of human functioning and has limited what we
know about how people overcome adversity to lead fulfilling lives.
Few people characterize their hopes for a happy life simply in terms
of avoiding pain; human beings— even those who have experienced
tremendous adversity—are motivated to seek joy, love, and meaning,
not nondepression and nonanxiety (e.g., Bonanno, 2004;Linley &
Joseph, 2004). The emphasis on suffering also neglects the fact that
suffering and happiness are not incompatible; many people report
high levels of well-being despite experiencing violence and trauma
(e.g., Albrecht & Devlieger, 1999;Lecci, Okun, & Karoly, 1994).
Contributions From Research on Positive Psychology
The field of positive psychology was founded as a response to
the emphasis of most psychological research on risk and pathology
(e.g., Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). It has advanced our
understanding of the conceptualization and measurement of
healthy functioning and identified a range of characteristics that
foster well-being, which are typically referred to as character or
personal strengths (Peterson, Park, Pole, D’Andrea, & Seligman,
2008;Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005). Unlike many
protective factors, the strengths emphasized in the positive psy-
chology literature (e.g., gratitude, compassion, grit) generally do
not represent the inverse of a risk factor; for example, ingratitude
is not considered to be a risk factor for psychopathology. Thus,
strengths may function as unique protective factors that could
enhance psychological health following exposure to adversity.
However, positive psychology research also has limitations for
understanding resilience in victims of violence. First, much of the
work on strengths is correlational, and correlations between
strengths and well-being do not show that they have a causal effect
on psychological functioning. Second, the processes by which
these characteristics promote well-being have not been studied
systematically. To contribute to a model of adaptation following
adversity and serve as a means for improving psychological health,
it is critical to understand how these strengths function. Finally,
positive psychology research rarely has focused on victims of
violence. There are exceptions, such as studies of victims of the
9/11 terrorist attacks that found that character strengths such as
gratitude were associated with healthy functioning (Peterson &
Seligman, 2003), but we know little about whether character
strengths may help individuals exposed to violence.
Contributions From Research on
Posttraumatic Growth
One line of research that has focused explicitly on positive
aspects of functioning after exposure to adversity is the study of
posttraumatic growth (PTG). Posttraumatic growth refers to deriv-
ing meaning from highly stressful experiences that lead to positive
changes in views of self, the world, and/or relationships (Tedeschi
& Calhoun, 2004). Whereas resilience is conceptualized as main-
taining psychological health despite exposure to violence, post-
traumatic growth is a healthy outcome that occurs because the
individual experienced a stressful event. Research on PTG has
focused attention on the idea that the process of coping with
trauma can have beneficial effects on health, but this research has
some limitations for understanding its role in responding to
trauma. For example, PTG has been conceptualized both as a
process for coping with trauma and as an outcome of the coping
process (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004;Zoellner & Maercker, 2006),
and there is inconsistency in the extent to which growth is viewed
as a cognitive, emotional, or behavioral phenomenon (e.g., Hob-
foll, 2002;Janoff-Bulman, 2004;Wortman, 2004). Further, em-
pirical findings on the links between PTG and mental health are
mixed, with some studies showing positive associations between
PTG and health indicators and others showing negative associa-
tions (Helgeson, Reynolds, & Tomich, 2006). These data raise the
question of when the process of seeking to make meaning of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
344 GRYCH, HAMBY, AND BANYARD
traumatic experiences is growth-producing rather than an indicator
of a maladaptive process such as rumination.
Contributions From Research on Coping
Finally, research on stress and coping focuses on understanding
behavioral, cognitive, and emotional processes that are more or
less helpful for responding to stress, including coping strategies
used by victims of violence (e.g., Goodman, Smyth, Borges, &
Singer, 2009;Hamby, 2014). Many coping models have been
developed, but one of the most influential was created by Lazarus
and Folkman (1984). It proposes that behavioral responses to
stressful events are guided by individuals’ appraisals of the event,
which involve perceptions of how threatening the event is and
beliefs about their ability to cope effectively with the event. This
model suggests pathways through which strengths and protective
factors can shape both appraisals and coping behavior and thus
offers potential mechanisms through which intrapersonal charac-
teristics lead to adaptive outcomes. The coping literature also has
limitations for understanding resilience in victims of violence,
however. The focus of most coping research has been on attenu-
ating the impact of negative events in the short term, and less
attention has been paid to identifying behavior that promotes
growth and thriving over time. In addition, coping models have
often sought to classify particular strategies as effective or inef-
fective without taking the context into account (e.g., Hamby,
2014). For example, avoidant behavior generally is considered a
maladaptive strategy (Billings & Moos, 1981), but in some violent
situations, avoidance may be the safest response (Hamby, 2014).
In the Resilience Portfolio Model, we propose that particular
strengths and protective factors foster resilience by shaping ap-
praisals and coping behavior, but do not adopt any existing coping
frameworks to categorize these behaviors.
The Resilience Portfolio Model
The Resilience Portfolio Model draws on theory and research on
resilience, positive psychology, posttraumatic growth, and coping to
better understand how people build fulfilling lives despite experienc-
ing violent and traumatic events. Consistent with Bronfenbrenner’s
(1977) social-ecological framework, it includes protective factors at
the individual, family, peer, and community level, and proposes
processes through which they foster resilience in victims of violence.
The model differs from prior efforts to understand healthy functioning
after exposure to violence in several ways.
First, our primary goal is to inform prevention and intervention
efforts and so the Resilience Portfolio Model emphasizes what
people do in the face of stress that promotes health and well-being,
rather than simply describing qualities or resources that they have.
Although the coping options available to individuals depend in
part on the resources available to them, focusing on malleable
behavior offers greater potential for prevention and intervention
than addressing static personal characteristics or aspects of the
environment. Second, it integrates character strengths studied in
the positive psychology literature with protective factors identified
in work on resilience to provide a more comprehensive accounting
of the qualities that lead to better functioning in response to
adversity. Third, it proposes processes through which these
strengths and protective factors lead to positive outcomes. Fourth,
the model describes a wider array of possible outcomes than are
usually investigated in the violence literature. Finally, it has a life
span focus that considers continuities and discontinuities in how
protective processes may operate in childhood and adulthood.
There is a developmental disconnect in both resilience and positive
psychology research, with nearly all of the studies in these fields
focusing either on children or on adults (for exceptions, see Burt &
Paysnick, 2012); consequently, there is a need for theoretical
models that link the processes studied with children with those
from work on adults. See Table 1 for a summary of the unique
aspects of the model.
As illustrated in Figure 1, the Resilience Portfolio Model pro-
poses that individuals’ psychological health after exposure to
violence is a product of the characteristics of the adversity, the
assets and resources available to them, and their behavior or
responses. The relationships among these constructs are proposed
to be transactional: people who have the assets and resources to
deal effectively with adversity will tend to function better over
time, whereas those who do not will be increasingly vulnerable to
adversity; in turn, health and well-being tend to enhance resources
and assets (Bronfenbrenner, 1977). Although the largest influence
on children’s adaptation to adversity lies in their environment,
especially their relationships with caregivers (e.g., Biglan, Flay,
Embry, & Sandler, 2012), over time the sources of resilience
become increasingly internalized. Because both resources and
sources of stress tend to be at least somewhat stable, there often is
continuity in individuals’ ability to adapt to adversity (see Burt &
Paysnick, 2012); however, changes in either can lead to changes in
functioning.
Exposure to Violence
The processes in the model are set in motion by exposure to
violence. As noted above, different forms of violence tend to have
similar effects on adjustment and many people who experience one
Table 1
Key Elements of the Portfolio Resilience Model
1. Builds systematically and integratively on research in resilience, positive psychology, posttraumatic growth, and coping.
2. Introduces new potential protective factors that are not simply the inverse of risk factors.
3. Proposes that the density and diversity of resources and assets are more important than particular characteristics for understanding resilience.
4. Identifies 3 higher-order functional categories of strengths that are particularly salient for resilience: regulatory, interpersonal, and meaning-making
strengths.
5. Incorporates key resilience mechanisms (additive, buffering, inoculating, and insulating) in a single model.
6. Adopts a multi-dimensional approach to defining well-being, including psychological, physical, and spiritual aspects.
7. Adopts a developmental, lifespan focus.
8. Identifies specific paths to targets for prevention or intervention.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
345
THE RESILIENCE PORTFOLIO MODEL
type of violence experience others as well (see Hamby & Grych,
2013); consequently, we propose that the model applies to diverse
forms of violence. At the same time, certain aspects of the partic-
ular violent event(s) are likely to shape the impact of the event on
the individual and the efficacy of particular protective factors and
coping behaviors. First, the identity of the perpetrator is proposed
to be important. Violence perpetrated by caregivers or intimate
partners is likely to have a more negative impact that violence
perpetrated by strangers, in part because it also tends to undermine
critical sources of support (e.g., Martin, Cromer, DePrince, &
Freyd, 2013). Whether it is acute or chronic and occurs in the
context of other significant stressors also is proposed to have
different implications for the sufficiency of individuals’ ability to
cope effectively (e.g., Molnar, Buka, & Kessler, 2001).
Assets and Resources: Portfolio of Protective Factors
Consistent with prior work (e.g., Fergus & Zimmerman, 2005),
we use the term assets to describe characteristics of the person that
promote healthy functioning and resources to refer to sources of
support outside of the person. Together, they represent what we
refer to as each person’s unique “portfolio” of strengths and
protective factors. This portfolio thus includes variables from
across the social ecology (Bronfenbrenner, 1977) and, also con-
sistent the ecological model, resources and assets are proposed to
have a reciprocal relationship: greater resources promote the de-
velopment of assets, and assets in turn can build resources. For
example, sensitive and responsive caregiving leads to greater so-
cial competence in children, and more socially competent children
in turn are able to draw others toward them and develop mutually
rewarding relationships that may help situate them in communities
with greater social capital and collective efficacy.
Assets. Of the wide array of individual characteristics that
have been identified as potential protective factors, resilience
researchers have described a “short list” of characteristics that are
most consistently associated with healthy adaptation (see Masten,
2007). Many of the strengths described in research on positive
psychology are conceptually similar to these protective factors but
expand on or add unique elements to them. In the model, we
organize these individual characteristics into categories represent-
ing functions proposed to be particularly critical for healthy adap-
tation: regulating emotions and behavior; building interpersonal
relationships; and fostering meaning-making. This grouping also
reflects an insight from the positive psychology literature that it is
useful to think not only about specific characteristics but also
about classes of constructs that share similar features. We propose
that it is the number and variety—what Schnell (2011) has called
the “density and diversity”— of these characteristics that is most
critical for resilience. That is, what matters most for adapting to
adversity is the total constellation of strengths and protective
factors represented in one’s “portfolio” rather than the presence of
any particular strength. We refer to this as a “poly-strengths”
approach that parallels the construct of “poly-victimization” that
has arisen in research on the cumulative burden of violence (Fin-
kelhor, Ormrod, & Turner, 2007). This concept is particularly
important for understanding adaptation to violence because expo-
sure to diverse forms of violence can adversely affect many of the
protective factors described below. For example, physical and
sexual abuse have been shown to undermine children’s capacity to
regulate affect (e.g., Kim & Cicchetti, 2010;Shipman, Zeman,
Penza, & Champion, 2000). Having more types of assets and
resources (diversity) and more strengths within each category
(density) is proposed to increase individuals’ capability to meet
self-regulatory, interpersonal, and meaning-making needs despite
their exposure to violence.
Regulatory strengths. Self-regulation is a multifaceted pro-
cess that involves emotional, cognitive, behavioral, and physiolog-
ical components (e.g., Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004;Thompson,
1994). It involves sustaining and supporting goal-driven behavior
both in the immediate situation (e.g., coping with a stressor) and
over longer periods of time (e.g., graduating from college). Suc-
cessfully achieving academic, occupational, and even relational
goals requires individuals to maintain focus and effort over months
or years, plan and organize their time, and continue to strive
despite difficulties and diversions. Aspects of self-regulation such
as executive functioning and planfulness have been identified as
protective in longitudinal research (e.g., Masten et al., 2004;Mof-
fitt et al., 2011), and this category encompasses a number of other
frequently identified protective factors, including cognitive abili-
ties, self-efficacy, achievement motivation, and self-direction (e.g.,
Masten, 2007). Several character strengths studied in the positive
psychology literature also can foster regulation and goal attain-
ment over time. Most notably, perseverance and grit (Duckworth,
Steen, & Seligman, 2005) reflect the ability to sustain motivation
and overcome obstacles while striving toward a goal, and charac-
teristics such as optimism and future-mindedness support contin-
ued effort by symbolizing desired outcomes and enhancing beliefs
in their attainability.
Emotion regulation is an aspect of self-regulation that plays a
critical role in responding effectively to stress. It has been studied
primarily in the context of managing negative emotions (e.g.,
Greenberg, Kusche, & Speltz, 1991;Kim & Cicchetti, 2010), and
has been shown to predict better outcomes in children exposed to
family (e.g., Cicchetti, Rogosch, Lynch, & Holt, 1993) and com-
munity violence (e.g., Kliewer et al., 2004). Although studied
much less, the capacity to generate and sustain positive emotions
may be just as important for resilience (e.g., Fredrickson, 2001;
Zautra, Affleck, Tennen, Reich, & Davis, 2005). Functionalist
models of emotion state that positive affect leads people to engage
Figure 1. The Resilience Portfolio Model. See the online article for the
color version of this figure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
346 GRYCH, HAMBY, AND BANYARD
the environment (Fredrickson, 2001;Oatley & Jenkins, 1996),
which can further build their resources and promote effective
coping. Therefore, the capacity to experience, maintain, or gener-
ate positive affect, which is at least somewhat independent of the
capacity to manage negative affect, can support coping in difficult
times and build essential assets and resources in good times (e.g.,
Garland et al., 2010;Layous, Chancellor, & Lyubomirsky, 2014;
e.g., Ong, Bergeman, Bisconti, & Wallace, 2006).
Interpersonal strengths. Interpersonal strengths are charac-
teristics within the individual that foster the development and
maintenance of close relationships, which are a primary source of
happiness and meaning for many people as well as an important
source of support when adversity occurs. Social support is one of
the most commonly studied protective factors (for a review, see
Thoits, 2011) and constitutes a key resource for victims of vio-
lence, but resilience research has paid far less attention to people’s
ability to establish social bonds and what they do to strengthen
interpersonal connections. In any social milieu, including schools,
communities, and even families, the degree of social support
individuals have ranges widely. The positive psychology literature
offers the potential to identify personal qualities that are valuable
for developing and maintaining good relationships and to explore
how this process occurs rather than simply describing the level of
support that exists.
The strengths included in this category have both intra- and
interpersonal aspects, but we include them here to emphasize their
potential for building and sustaining supportive relationships.
Qualities such as gratitude, compassion, generosity, and forgive-
ness all can be conceptualized as internal states, but when ex-
pressed behaviorally they enhance social bonds and strengthen
interpersonal connections. For example, gratitude has been defined
as a disposition that involves noticing and being thankful for past
and present experiences (Wood, Froh, & Geraghty, 2010), and it
has been shown to predict well-being in longitudinal research
(Bono, McCullough, & Root, 2008;Wood, Maltby, Gillett, Linley,
& Joseph, 2008). Feeling grateful may enhance subjective well-
being, but acting grateful, for example, by expressing appreciation
to a friend, extends its internal benefits by enhancing interpersonal
relationships. Numerous studies have documented links between
gratitude and indicators of positive social relationships (for a
review see Wood et al., 2010). For example, Lambert and Fincham
(2011) showed that expressing gratitude promotes relationship
maintenance behaviors, and close relationships are particularly
important protective factors when adversity occurs (Fredrickson,
2004).
Meaning-making strengths. This category incorporates ideas
from research on resilience, positive psychology, and posttrau-
matic growth, all of which propose that the capacity to find
meaning in difficult and even traumatic life events promotes
mental health (e.g., Lyubomirsky, 2001;Masten, 2007). The desire
to explain and understand one’s experiences is a powerful human
characteristic. Even young children spontaneously seek explana-
tions for events that occur in their lives, and this tendency is
particularly strong when these events are aversive or distressing
(e.g., Fabes, Eisenberg, Nyman, & Michealieu, 1991;Garmezy,
1983;Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Individuals’ capacity to make
sense of the events that occur in their lives and to maintain
coherence between events and their broader beliefs and values is
instrumental in coping with adversity in the short term and also can
foster positive affect and optimism and support sustained effort
toward achieving long-term goals (Lyubomirsky, 2001;C. L. Park,
2010). The importance of attempting to make meaning of adverse
events has been the central focus of research on posttraumatic
growth (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004). Studies in this area have
described different aspects of meaning-making that individuals
engage in following trauma and examined their links with psycho-
logical health. However, there is disagreement about the number of
dimensions captured by current measures, and most assess self-
perceptions of growth that often show little relationship to behav-
ioral measures of changes (Frazier et al., 2009;Gunty et al., 2011;
Powell, Rosner, Butollo, Tedeschi, & Calhoun, 2003). Conse-
quently, the aspects of meaning-making that are most critical for
growth following traumatic events are not yet known.
Efforts to make meaning from stressful events do not invariably
improve well-being. They can result in rumination and distress if
the events cannot be integrated into individuals’ broader beliefs
and values, or if the meaning made reinforces maladaptive beliefs
(e.g., the self is bad and deserving of punishment) (e.g., Park,
2010). The Resilience Portfolio Model proposes that making
meaning of difficult experiences is facilitated when individuals
have a clear set of beliefs, values, and goals and the sense that life
has meaning and purpose. Their origin may be secular, but for
many people, meaning is rooted in spiritual and religious systems
of belief (e.g., King, Hicks, Krull, & Del Gaiso, 2006;C. L. Park,
2010). Although spirituality historically has been neglected in
mainstream psychological research (V. Banyard & Graham-
Bermann, 1993;Hamby, 2014) and religious activities like prayer
at times have been characterized as ineffective forms of coping,
empirical studies consistently show that spirituality is associated
with life satisfaction and posttraumatic growth (e.g., Shaw, Joseph,
& Linley, 2005), and of the strengths studied, spirituality has some
of the highest associations with adjustment following adversity (N.
Park, Peterson, & Seligman, 2004;Peterson & Seligman, 2003).
Regardless of their source, the ability to make sense of traumatic
events and integrate them into broader beliefs about the self and
the world is proposed to lead to more adaptive appraisals and
behavioral responses.
Resources
Resources include people who provide emotional, instrumental,
and/or financial support, characteristics of the social ecology such
as positive school climate, neighborhood cohesion and collective
efficacy, and socioeconomic status.
Supportive relationships. Across the life span, supportive
and caring relationships with others are reliably related to more
adaptive functioning, although which relationships are most salient
evolves over time (e.g., Biglan et al., 2012;Masten, 2007). For
children, caregivers are the most critical resource for fostering
resilience; they provide protection and nurturance and foster the
development of qualities such as self-regulation (Biglan et al.,
2012;Conger & Conger, 1982;Fergusson & Lynskey, 1997).
Although parents remain important influences, peers become in-
creasingly important sources of support as children transition into
adolescence, and romantic partners take on more significant roles
in late adolescence and adulthood. These relationships provide a
wealth of benefits for mental health, including the enhancement of
self-worth and efficacy (e.g., Collishaw et al., 2007). Parental
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
347
THE RESILIENCE PORTFOLIO MODEL
support (represented by constructs such as parental warmth and
secure attachment) is the most commonly studied protective factor
in studies of children exposed to violence, and has been associated
with resilient outcomes in the majority of these studies (Houston &
Grych, 2015). Unfortunately, for children exposed to violence in
the home and adult victims of intimate partner violence, the people
in the best position to provide support are also the source of the
violence. In these cases, relationships with individuals outside of
the home—friends, extended family, teachers, and so forth—
become increasingly important resources. Indeed, the documented
success of prevention programs like Big Brothers, Big Sisters rests
on cultivating such relationships (Tierney & Grossman, 1995).
Environmental factors. Environmental characteristics play a
key role in theories of healthy development (Bronfenbrenner,
1977), though they have been the subject of relatively little em-
pirical research on resilience in victims of violence. Certain envi-
ronmental factors have effects on both the resources that promote
resilience and the likelihood of being exposed to certain kinds of
adversity (Swisher, 2008) including violence (Rothman et al.,
2011). For example, the socioeconomic status of individuals and
their families often is used as a marker for the ecological context
in which individuals live, what resources are available to them, and
so forth, and also is consistently related to better mental health
outcomes (e.g., Wickrama & Noh, 2010). Nurturing schools and
community organizations characterized by supportive staff who
model, teach, and reinforce pro-social behavior provide opportu-
nities for children to develop important competencies and promote
well-being (e.g., Biglan et al., 2012). In addition to providing
resources that can promote healthy functioning, environmental
forces also may shape how resilience is expressed or what coping
options are utilized. For example, Fleisher (2009) discusses “cul-
tural resilience” or processes of adaptation and response to adver-
sity by groups of people, often via social networks and relation-
ships. Investigating the possibility that resilience is expressed
differently in different environmental contexts, as a result of cul-
tural norms about relationships, role flexibility, and the acceptance
of particular coping strategies, is an important direction for future
study.
Coping responses. This component refers to what people do
specifically in response to adversity and how these actions may
promote well-being. A number of classification schemes for cop-
ing behaviors have been developed (for a review, see Compas,
Connor-Smith, Saltzman, Thomsen, & Wadsworth, 2001), but
because the context in which violence occurs has a powerful effect
on what behaviors may be adaptive or maladaptive (Hamby,
2014), the Resilience Portfolio Model does not label particular
strategies as inherently positive. Rather, it identifies how particular
strengths and protective factors may influence individuals’ ap-
praisals and behavioral responses to violence.
The focus on appraisals underscores the importance of the
meaning of an adverse event to an individual; when a threatening
or stressful event occurs, people try to make sense of why it
occurred and what it may mean for and about their lives (Lazarus
& Folkman, 1984;C. L. Park, 2010). Meaning-making strengths
thus are proposed to shape the kinds of appraisals individuals make
for specific events. People who have a coherent sense of meaning
may find it easier to fit stressful events into their world view and
experience them as less negative and more controllable. For ex-
ample, Skaggs and Barron (2006) reported that individuals who
view an adverse event as something that helped them to develop as
a person show better functioning than those who cannot find
meaning in the experience. Meaning-making strengths such as a
sense of purpose also can sustain goal-driven behavior and rein-
force regulatory and interpersonal strengths (Hamby, 2014). Reg-
ulatory and interpersonal strengths are proposed to guide coping as
well. Emotion regulation serves to modulate affect and physiolog-
ical arousal in the face of stressful events and to help individuals
to sustain positive affect and motivation to engage in constructive
behavior (e.g., Cole et al., 2004). Individuals high in self-efficacy
tend to experience a stronger sense of control in the face of
stressful events and are more likely to engage in adaptive forms of
coping (e.g., Masten et al., 2004). Interpersonal strengths tend to
engender support from others; for example, Wood and colleagues
(2007) found that more grateful people were more likely to seek
and utilize social support and to use coping strategies that involved
engaging with the problem.
Psychological Health and Well-Being
The final component of the model, psychological health, is
defined broadly to include indicators of well-being and positive
affect in addition to symptoms of psychopathology. Well-being is
conceptualized both in terms of subjective evaluations of satisfac-
tion with different aspects of life and the attainment of develop-
mental tasks or competence in multiple areas of functioning (e.g.,
Bonanno, 2004;Masten, Burt, & Coatsworth, 2006). Positive
emotions are included not only because they are phenomenologi-
cally pleasant, but because they have functional benefits: positive
affect promotes creativity, engagement with others, and participa-
tion in productive and enjoyable activities, and enhances a sense of
meaning in life (e.g., Fredrickson, 2001;King et al., 2006). Pos-
itive affect also can reduce negative affect in the moment and thus
may play a role in reducing the negative impact of adverse events
(e.g., Garland et al., 2010;Ong et al., 2006;Zautra et al., 2005).
Research shows that individuals’ experience of positive affect is
largely independent of their experience of negative affect (e.g.,
Zautra et al., 2005), and thus the factors that give rise to positive
emotions may be somewhat distinct from those that lead to neg-
ative affect and could be enhanced even if circumstances causing
negative affect do not change.
Proposed Mechanisms of the Path to Resilience
The resilience literature has described a number of mechanisms
by which protective factors may operate; these schemes tend to
describe similar processes, but often use different terms for the
same mechanisms or use the same terms to refer to different
processes. Fergus and Zimmerman (2005) attempted to add clarity
to this situation by organizing resilience processes into three
primary categories, which they label compensatory, protective, and
challenge. This classification effectively captures the essential
differences among the processes, but in the interest of further
increasing clarity and improving communication across subdisci-
plines of research and practice, we use terms for the mechanisms
that we believe best convey how each is proposed to work and
avoid using terms that have been used inconsistently, ambigu-
ously, or for more than one concept in studies of resilience. In the
model, assets and resources are proposed to promote healthy
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
348 GRYCH, HAMBY, AND BANYARD
functioning in four ways. First, they reduce the likelihood that
individuals will experience certain kinds of adversity (Path A).
This can be designated an “insulating” effect. For example, indi-
viduals with greater economic resources tend to live in neighbor-
hoods that experience less community violence. Types of adversity
that are more random (e.g., natural disasters) or unavoidable (e.g.,
death of a loved one) will not be prevented by individuals’ port-
folio of assets and resources, but those with more “reliable re-
source reservoirs” (Hobfoll, 2002) are likely to have lower cumu-
lative lifetime exposure to stress. Although reducing exposure to
adversity might not be considered a process that promotes resil-
ience because resilience is defined by the experience of significant
adversity, decreasing exposure to stressful events is one way that
a robust portfolio of protective factors fosters healthy develop-
ment. Second, assets and resources can have a “main effect” on
health and well-being (Path D). This type of mechanism involves
protective factors directly promoting healthy functioning indepen-
dent of their exposure to violence. For example, people who
experience higher levels of gratitude report greater positive affect
in their daily life (e.g., Wood et al., 2008) and secure and support-
ive interpersonal relationships enhance psychological and physical
health across the life span(e.g., Fergusson & Lynskey, 1997;
Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005). From this perspective,
individuals’ psychological functioning can be viewed in terms of
the balance of protective factors and stressors. A scale therefore
makes an apt metaphor: as the number or potency of protective
factors available to individuals increase, they tend to exhibit
healthier functioning, whereas increasing exposure to adversity
tends to reduce psychological health. Fergus and Zimmerman
(2005) use the term “compensatory” for this mechanism, but
because it could also apply to the next mechanism, we believe that
“additive” offers more distinctive and immediately apparent mean-
ing.
Third, resources and assets can have a buffering (moderating)
effect on health and well-being (Path B). This type of model
represents the protective process originally described by Garmezy
in his pioneering work on resilience (e.g., Garmezy, Masten, &
Tellegen, 1984). A buffering effect occurs when a protective factor
reduces the impact of a stressful event on adjustment but does not
lead to healthier functioning in the absence of the stressor. A
metaphor for this mechanism is an umbrella: they are valuable for
staying dry when it is raining (when stress is experienced), but do
not make someone more dry when it is not raining (in the absence
of stress). Statistically, this represents a moderating or interaction
effect because the association between stress and adjustment dif-
fers at different levels of the protective factor. For example, Skopp,
McDonald, Jouriles, and Rosenfield (2007) found that exposure to
intimate partner violence was positively related to children’s ex-
ternalizing behaviors when their mothers were low in warmth, but
not at high levels of warmth. In the model, this effect occurs
because individuals’ unique constellation of protective factors—
their portfolio—shapes what they do when faced with adversity
(Path C); effective coping then mitigates the impact of the stressor
on mental health and well-being (Path F). Fergus and Zimmerman
(2005) use the term “protective” for this mechanism, but because
that term is used to label the factors that benefit people in any of
the three ways described here, using it as the label for one of the
mechanisms can create confusion. Finally, PathsE&Grepresent
inoculation effects. Inoculation models propose that exposure to
manageable amounts of stress promotes the development of coping
skills that in turn equip the individual to adapt to more markedly
negative events that occur later in life. This also has been referred
to as a “steeling” effect and is labeled the “challenge” model by
Fergus and Zimmerman (2005). Vaccinations provide a metaphor
for this type of effect: giving a small dose of a virus creates
antibodies that help individuals defend against a more serious
infection later. This model proposes that there is a nonlinear
relationship between stress and both protective factors and health
more broadly in which low levels of stress promote the develop-
ment of protective factors that maintain healthy functioning
whereas high levels of stress are more likely to overwhelm indi-
viduals, undermining effective coping and leading to clinical
symptoms. Almost all research on adversity assumes that the best
score on any adversity measure is zero, but the inoculation model
questions this assumption. Given that some form of adversity will
befall virtually every person who survives to adulthood, perhaps
the experience of successful coping with minor adversities leaves
people better equipped to deal with more significant losses. For
example, children who experience the death of a pet often are quite
distressed but may develop better emotion regulation skills by
talking about the loss with a supportive parent. Most research
investigating processes by which protective factors predict resil-
ience in victims of violence have tested either additive or buffering
models. Additive effects tend to be more commonly documented
than buffering effects (Houston & Grych, 2015), with the excep-
tion of the social support literature (V. Banyard & Williams,
2007), but it is not clear whether those findings indicate that the
additive model better explains how protective factors work or the
greater difficulty in detecting statistical interactions in correla-
tional research. The few studies examining inoculation effects are
equivocal. For example, the National Survey of Children’s Expo-
sure to Violence shows a linear association between the degree of
adversity faced by individuals and their health (Turner, Finkelhor,
& Ormrod, 2010). It is possible that protective factors may func-
tion differently in different contexts or in relation to different types
of stress, and so it may not be the case that a particular factor
always operates through the same mechanism. However, explicat-
ing the specific ways that protective factors foster resilience is
important for developing more effective prevention and interven-
tion strategies.
Developmental Considerations
The resources, assets, and adversities described in the model all
are expected to change over the course of development. A full
treatment of the developmental trajectories of each element is
beyond the scope of this article, but we highlight a few points for
illustration. Assets such as self-regulation skills develop over the
course of childhood, though different strengths develop at different
rates. For example, basic abilities to self-soothe and inhibit ag-
gression normally become established in childhood, whereas as-
pects of executive functioning (e.g., inhibitory control) continue to
develop into early adulthood (Burt & Masten, 2009). Similarly,
meaning-making strengths depend in part on cognitive develop-
ment and evolve over the course of adolescence and early adult-
hood, and may become more salient as individuals get older. The
relative influence of internal assets versus external resources also
is likely to change over the life span. Most of the protective factors
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
349
THE RESILIENCE PORTFOLIO MODEL
available to children are external resources, most centrally their
caregivers, whereas internal assets become increasingly important
as autonomy increases in adolescence and adulthood. External
resources remain influential for adults, but they are in a position to
exert more control over the resources available to them. It also is
important to note the transactional nature of the relations among
the model’s components. The double-sided arrows in Figure 1
denote reciprocal associations among the constructs. Hobfoll
(2002) used the term “resource caravans” to describe how indi-
viduals who have greater resources are able to continue to build on
them over time, and the model similarly proposes that individuals
who exhibit resilience after adversity are more likely to further
develop intra- and interpersonal resources that can protect them in
the future. For example, those who characteristically experience
and express greater positive affect are likely to develop more
supportive relationships (Path D), and those who have greater
competencies are likely to feel more efficacious to cope with new
difficulties that arise (Path F). At the same time, adversity expe-
rienced over the course of childhood and adolescence can under-
mine the development of resources (Path A), and so children who
are exposed to high levels of adversity are likely to have fewer of
the environmental, relational, and individual resources that foster
well-being. This transactional process explains why considerable
continuity in both maladjustment and resilience has been docu-
mented by studies that follow participants from childhood into
adolescence (e.g., Masten et al., 2004;Werner & Smith, 2001).
However, age also presents opportunities to change. One of the
most important factors explaining why youths exhibiting a mal-
adaptive trajectory switched to the resilient group in adulthood in
these longitudinal studies was their capacity to take advantage of
“second chance” opportunities in adolescence. These opportunities
most often involved developing a satisfying intimate relationship
with a well-functioning partner, a stable job, or entry into second-
ary education (e.g., Werner & Smith, 2001). Of course, their
capacity to take advantage of these opportunities was influenced
by the assets and resources that they had developed earlier in life,
but these findings speak to the importance of considering context
in understanding how resilience develops.
Implications for Future Research
The value of a conceptual framework lies in large part in its
capacity to organize existing data and its potential to generate new
research questions. The Resilience Portfolio Model identifies sev-
eral directions for research investigating how children and adults
cope with and overcome adversity, many of which arise from the
effort to bridge research in different domains (e.g., positive psy-
chology and resilience). In the next section we describe some of
the most pressing issues highlighted by the model.
Specifying Relations Among Resources, Assets,
Behavior, and Health
A key premise of the model is that personal strengths can foster
resilience in victims of violence, and this idea leads to a number of
testable hypotheses regarding the potential for strengths identified
in the positive psychology literature to uniquely predict adjustment
beyond established protective factors. The model generates ques-
tions about the specific relations among strengths, protective fac-
tors and coping; that is, how resources and assets influence what
people do in response to adversity. For example, how does the
process of meaning-making affect how individuals appraise and
respond to violence in the moment? Or how does a strength like
perseverance relate to individuals’ success in managing their affect
and behavior at the time a stressor occurs? Conceptualizing health
more broadly also leads to questions about whether particular
strengths and protective factors have different associations with
different outcomes, such as whether strengths are more closely
related to (greater) well-being and protective factors are more
closely associated with (lower) clinical symptoms. As noted
above, happiness and clinical symptoms are somewhat indepen-
dent, and it is possible that specific assets and resources may have
different associations with different aspects of psychological
health. Examining multiple aspects of well-being will offer a more
comprehensive understanding of how resilience is experienced.
Investigating protective factors and personal strengths together
creates opportunities to test hypotheses that reflect the full con-
stellation of assets and resources available to people. It might be
possible to develop profiles of strengths and protective factors that
characterize individuals and test whether these profiles better pre-
dict adjustment than specific strengths, or whether there are
strengths that appear to be more critical or foundational for par-
ticular outcomes than others. Adopting a polystrengths perspective
similar to the recent focus on polyvictimization in violence re-
search (Finkelhor et al., 2007) also raises the question of whether
the total number of strengths in individuals’ portfolio is a more
potent predictor of their functioning than the nature of those
strengths.
Investigating the Mechanisms by Which Strengths
Promote Resilience
Research on resilience and positive psychology has identified a
number of correlates of better mental health, but still has far to go
in identifying which have a causal effect on functioning and how
they promote resilience (e.g., Vagi et al., 2013). The Resilience
Portfolio Model offers a guide for specifying and testing the
processes by which particular strengths influence psychological
health directly (additive effect; Path D) and indirectly via apprais-
als and behavioral responses to stressors (buffering effect, Path C).
Kleiman and colleagues (2013) provide an illustrative example.
They found that the interaction of gratitude and grit predicted
increased meaning in life, which in turn predicted lower levels of
suicidal ideation over 4 weeks in a sample of college students.
Investigating how specific self-regulatory, interpersonal, and
meaning-making strengths promote resilience following adversity
has important implications for improving prevention and interven-
tion. To test questions about the processes that foster healthy
functioning, longitudinal, and where feasible, experimental de-
signs are needed to investigate whether resources and assets pro-
mote mental health or simply act as indicators of resilience. More-
over, these processes may operate differently at different ages, and
so it will be important to consider developmental changes in when
particular strengths develop and whether their associations with
adjustment differ at different ages.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
350 GRYCH, HAMBY, AND BANYARD
Exploring Situational and Cultural Factors
One limitation of the research on positive psychology and
resilience that is reflected in the Resilience Portfolio model is a
lack of attention to situational and cultural contexts. Most of the
work in these areas implicitly adopts a culture-neutral, resource-
neutral view that emphasizes individual characteristics as determi-
nants of functioning (V. Banyard & Graham-Bermann, 1993;
Hamby, 2014). McMillen (2004) also notes the lack of attention to
situational and cultural contexts in research on posttraumatic
growth and outlines a number of ways that such contexts may
influence growth after stress. To fully understand how individuals
adapt to violence, it is critical to move beyond a focus on the
individual to explore how communities can serve as sources of
resilience. Which community or communities are most relevant is
likely to vary across sample, as are the most important aspects of
the community. In some places, physical location (e.g., neighbor-
hood, county) may be most salient, whereas in others, family ties
or shared culture may be more meaningful to individuals. These
distinctions may not be obvious to people outside of the commu-
nity, and so researchers interested in studying violence in a par-
ticular area will need to be familiar with the community and open
to learning from its members.
Implications for Prevention and Intervention
We believe there is a real hunger in the field for work that can
help people achieve true thriving and not simply the avoidance of
symptoms. By describing how regulatory, relational, and meaning-
making strengths can foster well-being in victims of violence, the
Resilience Portfolio model offers a number of directions for en-
hancing prevention and intervention efforts.
Enhancing Prevention
Prevention programs addressing violence typically focus on
identifying risk factors and warning signs rather than building a
foundation of skills for healthy relationships, self-efficacy, and
decision-making (for an exception, see Social Emotional Learning;
Durlak & Weissberg, 2011;Hamby & Grych, 2013). Research
stemming from the Resilience Portfolio Model could be useful for
identifying character strengths and protective factors that are most
relevant for fostering resilience in the face of diverse forms of
violence. For example, early in life programs could focus on
building resources and assets, such as parenting competencies, to
create more nurturing environments (Biglan et al., 2012). Such
efforts could have both inoculating and additive effects by reduc-
ing children’s exposure to violence, fostering strengths, and di-
rectly enhancing psychological functioning. Protective factors that
have buffering effects may be particularly important targets for
more focused prevention programs designed for individuals al-
ready experiencing adversity. For example, Wolfe’s “4th R” pro-
gram demonstrated success in preventing interpersonal violence
among youth who had experienced child maltreatment (Wolfe,
Scott, Wekerle, & Pittman, 2001). Many bystander programs work
to help friends be better supports when they receive a disclosure
about interpersonal violence, which may have positive effects on
community as well as interpersonal resources (V. L. Banyard,
Plante, & Moynihan, 2004).
The emphasis of the model on what people do to respond to
adversity also highlights the value of building key skills across the life
span and developing constructive ways to appraise and cope with
difficult events. This can include timing programs to match particular
developmental moments. For example, Testa and colleagues (2010)
evaluated an intervention for incoming college students that involved
teaching parents how to have directed conversations about alcohol
abuse and sexual assault with their children prior to beginning their
first semester. They found that participants receiving the intervention
reported increased mother– daughter communication, which in turn
predicted less heavy drinking and lower rates of sexual victimization
during the first year of college.
Improving Intervention
Enhancing well-being rather than simply reducing symptoms is
central for some models of psychotherapy (e.g., ACT; Hayes, Stro-
sahl, & Wilson, 2012) but traditionally has been emphasized much
less than reduction of symptoms. Research in positive psychology has
given rise to a wide range of activities designed to build strengths and
improve people’s quality of life, and they may add value to existing
prevention and intervention programs focused more on clinical prob-
lems (for reviews, see Seligman et al., 2005;Sin & Lyubomirsky,
2009). Interventions that foster gratitude are among the most fre-
quently studied (see Wood et al., 2010). They involve activities such
as regularly making lists of things for which one is grateful and
writing and delivering letters to people for whom one is grateful (e.g.,
Emmons & McCullough, 2003;Froh, Kashdan, Ozimkowski, &
Miller, 2009). Empirical evaluations of many of these activities doc-
ument positive effects on mood and well-being, but few studies have
employed longer term follow-ups to determine whether the effects are
lasting (for an exception, see Cohn & Fredrickson, 2010), and their
efficacy for victims of violence is unknown. Concerns also have been
raised about the quality of data used to support some interventions,
including the appropriateness of the comparison groups utilized (e.g.,
Sin & Lyubomirsky, 2009;Wood et al., 2010). Although much needs
to be done to establish their efficacy and generalizability, existing data
suggest that focusing on strengths may provide new avenues for
increasing the impact of prevention and intervention efforts (e.g.,
Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013;Seligman et al., 2005).
As described above, the Resilience Portfolio Model’s focus on
building strengths describes how individuals can respond to victim-
ization in ways that not only alleviate symptoms and enhance well-
being but perhaps also create buffers against future adversity. The
proposed effects of character strengths on appraisals and coping, for
example, suggest specific behavioral strategies that could be inte-
grated into psychotherapy. The model also supports the value of
systematically assessing multiple dimensions of well-being. Some
dimensions, particularly spiritual well-being, have been relatively
neglected in mainstream psychology, and the model’s emphasis on
meaning-making would be helpful in many direct care settings. Like-
wise, a multifaceted conceptualization of psychological health has
implications for the development of better assessment tools to capture
the full range of well-being as well as intervention goals that go
beyond symptom reduction.
Conclusions
The Resilience Portfolio Model draws on key contributions from
research on resilience, positive psychology, posttraumatic growth, and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
351
THE RESILIENCE PORTFOLIO MODEL
coping in an effort to develop a more comprehensive understanding of
how individuals exposed to violence overcome these experiences and
go on to live happy and successful lives. The model expands the range
of protective factors that have been studied in violence research by
incorporating personal strengths identified in the positive psychology
literature, and describes processes by which they guide appraisals and
coping behaviors and promote healthy functioning following expo-
sure to violence. It also identifies new questions for investigating
resilience in victims of violence and offers new ideas for prevention
and intervention. Integrating insights from these complementary
fields of study offers the potential to provide a deeper understanding
of how people adapt to adversity and to improve the strategies that we
develop to assist them in that process.
References
Albrecht, G. L., & Devlieger, P. J. (1999). The disability paradox: High
quality of life against all odds. Social Science & Medicine, 48, 977–988.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(98)00411-0
Banyard, V., & Graham-Bermann, S. A. (1993). A gender analysis of
theories of coping with stress. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17,
303–318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1993.tb00489.x
Banyard, V. L., Plante, E. G., & Moynihan, M. M. (2004). Bystander
education: Bringing a broader community perspective to sexual violence
prevention. Journal of Community Psychology, 32, 61–79. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1002/jcop.10078
Banyard, V. L., & Williams, L. M. (2007). Women’s voices on recovery:
A multi-method study of the complexity of recovery from child sexual
abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31, 275–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.chiabu.2006.02.016
Biglan, A., Flay, B. R., Embry, D. D., & Sandler, I. N. (2012). The critical
role of nurturing environments for promoting human well-being. Amer-
ican Psychologist, 67, 257–271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026796
Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of coping responses and
social resources in attenuating the stress of life events. Journal of
Behavioral Medicine, 4, 139 –157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
BF00844267
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we
underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive
events? American Psychologist, 59, 20 –28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0003-066X.59.1.20
Bono, G., McCullough, M. E., & Root, L. M. (2008). Forgiveness, feeling
connected to others, and well-being: Two longitudinal studies. Person-
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 182–195. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0146167207310025
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human
development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0003-066X.32.7.513
Burt, K. B., & Masten, A. S. (2009). Development in the transition to
adulthood: Vulnerabilities and opportunities. In D. Cicchetti & G. I.
Roisman (Eds.), The origins and organization of adaptation and mal-
adaptation (pp. 5–18). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Burt, K. B., & Paysnick, A. A. (2012). Resilience in the transition to
adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 493–505. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000119
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Lynch, M., & Holt, K. D. (1993). Resilience
in maltreated children: Processes leading to adaptive outcome. Devel-
opment and Psychopathology, 5, 629 647. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
S0954579400006209
Cohn, M. A., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). In search of durable positive
psychology interventions: Predictors and consequences of long-term
positive behavior change. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 355–
366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2010.508883
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as
a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child
development research. Child Development, 75, 317–333. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.x
Collishaw, S., Pickles, A., Messer, J., Rutter, M., Shearer, C., & Maughan,
B. (2007). Resilience to adult psychopathology following childhood
maltreatment: Evidence from a community sample. Child Abuse &
Neglect, 31, 211–229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.02.004
Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., Thomsen, A. H., &
Wadsworth, M. E. (2001). Coping with stress during childhood and
adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research.
Psychological Bulletin, 127, 87–127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-
2909.127.1.87
Conger, J. C., & Conger, A. J. (1982). Components of hetersocial compe-
tence. In J. P. Curran & P. M. Monti (Eds.), Social skill training: A
practical handbook for assessment and treatment (pp. 313–347). New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Duckworth, A. L., Steen, T. A., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Positive
psychology in clinical practice. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,
1, 629 651. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803
.144154
Durlak, J. A., & Weissberg, R. P. (2011). Promoting social and emotional
development is an essential part of students’ education. Human Devel-
opment, 54, 1–3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000324337
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus
burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-
being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
377–389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Nyman, M., & Michealieu, Q. (1991). Young
children’s appraisals of others’ spontaneous emotional reactions. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 27, 858 866. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-
1649.27.5.858
Fergus, S., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2005). Adolescent resilience: A frame-
work for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annual
Review of Public Health, 26, 399 419. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/
annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144357
Fergusson, D. M., & Lynskey, M. T. (1997). Physical punishment/
maltreatment during childhood and adjustment in young adulthood.
Child Abuse & Neglect, 21, 617– 630. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0145-
2134(97)00021-5
Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R. K., & Turner, H. A. (2007). Polyvictimization
and trauma in a national longitudinal cohort. Development and Psychol-
ogy, 19, 149 –166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407070083
Fleisher, M. (2009). Coping with macro-structural adversity: Chronic pov-
erty, female youth gangs, and cultural resilience in a US African-
American urban community. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Man-
agement, 17, 274 –284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5973.2009
.00589.x
Frazier, P., Tennen, H., Gavian, M., Park, C., Tomich, P., & Tashiro, T.
(2009). Does self-reported posttraumatic growth reflect genuine positive
change? Psychological Science, 20, 912–919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
j.1467-9280.2009.02381.x
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive
psychology. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Amer-
ican Psychologist, 56, 218 –226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X
.56.3.218
Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Gratitude, like other positive emotions, broadens
and builds. In R. A. Emmons & M. E. McCullough (Eds.), The Psy-
chology of Gratitude (pp. 144 –166). New York, NY: Oxford University
Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195150100.003.0008
Froh, J. J., Kashdan, T. B., Ozimkowski, K. M., & Miller, N. (2009). Who
benefits the most from a gratitude intervention in children and adolescents?
Examining positive affect as a moderator. The Journal of Positive Psychol-
ogy, 4, 408 422. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760902992464
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
352 GRYCH, HAMBY, AND BANYARD
Garland, E. L., Fredrickson, B., Kring, A. M., Johnson, D. P., Meyer, P. S.,
& Penn, D. L. (2010). Upward spirals of positive emotions counter
downward spirals of negativity: Insights from the broaden-and-build
theory and affective neuroscience on the treatment of emotion dysfunc-
tions and deficits in psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 30,
849 864. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.002
Garmezy, N. (1983). Stressors of childhood. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter
(Eds.), Stress, coping, and development in children (pp. 43– 84). New
York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The study of stress and
competence in children: A building block for developmental psychopa-
thology. Child Development, 55, 97–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
1129837
Goodman, L. A., Smyth, K. F., Borges, A. M., & Singer, R. (2009). When
crises collide: How intimate partner violence and poverty intersect to
shape women’s mental health and coping? Trauma, Violence, & Abuse,
10, 306 –329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1524838009339754
Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Speltz, M. (1991). Emotion regulation,
self-control, and psychopathology: The role of relationships in early
childhood. In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on
Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 2.Internalizing and externalizing
expressions of dysfunction (pp. 21–55). Rochester, NY: University of
Rochester Press.
Gunty, A. L., Frazier, P. A., Tennen, H., Tomich, P., Tashiro, T., & Park,
C. (2011). Moderators of the relation between perceived and actual
posttraumatic growth. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Prac-
tice, and Policy, 3, 61– 66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020485
Hamby, S. (2014). Battered women’s protective strategies: Stronger than
you know. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Hamby, S., & Grych, J. (2013). The web of violence: Exploring connec-
tions among different forms of interpersonal violence and abuse. New
York, NY: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-5596-3
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2012). Acceptance and
commitment therapy: The process and practice of mindful change (2nd
ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Helgeson, V. S., Reynolds, K. A., & Tomich, P. L. (2006). A meta-analytic
review of benefit finding and growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 74, 797– 816. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.5
.797
Hobfoll, S. E. (2002). Social and psychological resources and adaptation.
Review of General Psychology, 6, 307–324.
Houston, J., & Grych, J. (2015). Resilience in youth exposed to violence:
Protective factors and underlying mechanisms. Milwaukee, WI: Mar-
quette University.
Janoff-Bulman, R. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: Three explanatory mod-
els. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 30 –34.
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child
maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 706 –716. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02202.x
King, L. A., Hicks, J. A., Krull, J. L., & Del Gaiso, A. K. (2006). Positive
affect and the experience of meaning in life. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 90, 179 –196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
.90.1.179
Kleiman, E. M., Adams, L. M., Kashdan, T. B., & Riskind, J. H. (2013).
Gratitude and grit indirectly reduce risk of suicidal ideations by enhanc-
ing meaning in life: Evidence for a mediated moderation model. Journal
of Research in Personality, 47, 539 –546. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp
.2013.04.007
Kliewer, W., Cunningham, J. N., Diehl, R., Parrish, K. A., Walker, J. M.,
Atiyeh, C.,...Mejia, R. (2004). Violence exposure and adjustment in
inner-city youth: Child and caregiver emotion regulation skill, caregiver-
child relationship quality, and neighborhood cohesion as protective
factor. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 477–
487. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3303_5
Lambert, N. M., & Fincham, F. D. (2011). Expressing gratitude to a partner
leads to more relationship maintenance behavior. Emotion, 11, 52– 60.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021557
Layous, K., Chancellor, J., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2014). Positive activities as
protective factors against mental health conditions. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 123, 3–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034709
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New
York, NY: Springer, Inc.
Lecci, L., Okun, M. A., & Karoly, P. (1994). Life regrets and current goals
as predictors of psychological adjustment. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 66, 731–741. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
.66.4.731
Linley, P. A., & Joseph, S. (2004). Positive change following trauma and
adversity: A review. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 11–21. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014671.27856.7e
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resil-
ience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child De-
velopment, 71, 543–562. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00164
Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than others? The
role of cognitive and motivational processes in well-being. American
Psychologist, 56, 239 –249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.3
.239
Lyubomirsky, S., & Layous, K. (2013). How do simple positive activities
increase well-being? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22,
57– 62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721412469809
Martin, C. G., Cromer, L. D., Deprince, A. P., & Freyd, J. J. (2013). The
role of cumulative trauma, betrayal, and appraisals in understanding
trauma symptomatology. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research,
Practice, and Policy, 52, 110 –118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025686
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic. Resilience processes in develop-
ment. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0003-066X.56.3.227
Masten, A. S. (2007). Resilience in developing systems: Progress and
promise as the fourth wave rises. Development and Psychopathology,
19, 921–930. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407000442
Masten, A. S. (2011). Resilience in children threatened by extreme adver-
sity: Frameworks for research, practice, and translational synergy. De-
velopment and Psychopathology, 23, 493–506. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1017/S0954579411000198
Masten, A. S., Burt, K. B., & Coatsworth, J. D. (Eds.). (2006). Competence
and psychopathology in development (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
Masten, A. S., Burt, K. B., Roisman, G. I., Obradovi´
c, J., Long, J. D., &
Tellegen, A. (2004). Resources and resilience in the transition to adult-
hood: Continuity and change. Development and Psychopathology, 16,
1071–1094. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579404040143
Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (2012). Resilience in developmental psy-
chopathology: Contributions of the Project Competence Longitudinal
Study. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 345–361. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1017/S095457941200003X
McMillen, J. C. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: What’s it all about? Psy-
chological Inquiry, 15, 48 –52.
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J.,
Harrington, H.,...Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-
control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. PNAS Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108,
2693–2698. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108
Molnar, B. E., Buka, S. L., & Kessler, R. C. (2001). Child sexual abuse and
subsequent psychopathology: Results from the National Comorbidity
Survey. American Journal of Public Health, 91, 753–760. http://dx.doi
.org/10.2105/AJPH.91.5.753
Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotions: In psychol-
ogy, psychiatry, and social science. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
353
THE RESILIENCE PORTFOLIO MODEL
Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., Bisconti, T. L., & Wallace, K. A. (2006).
Psychological resilience, positive emotions, and successful adaptation to
stress in later life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91,
730 –749. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.730
Park, C. L. (2010). Making sense of the meaning literature: An integrative
review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life
events. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 257–301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0018301
Park, N., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Strengths of character
and well-being. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 603–
619. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.23.5.603.50748
Peterson, C., Park, N., Pole, N., D’Andrea, W., & Seligman, M. E. (2008).
Strengths of character and posttraumatic growth. Journal of Traumatic
Stress, 21, 214 –217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.20332
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2003). Character strengths before and
after 9/11. Psychological Science, 14(381–384). http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/1467-9280.24482
Powell, S., Rosner, R., Butollo, W., Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G.
(2003). Posttraumatic growth after war: A study with former refugees
and displaced people in Sarajevo. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 59,
71– 83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.10117
Rothman, E. F., Johnson, R. M., Young, R., Weinberg, J., Azrael, D., &
Molnar, B. E. (2011). Neighborhood-level factors associated with phys-
ical dating violence perpetration: Results of a representative survey
conducted in Boston, MA. Journal of Urban Health-Bulletin of the New
York Academy of Medicine, 88, 201–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s11524-011-9543-z
Rutter, M. (2012). Resilience as a dynamic concept. Development and
Psychopathology, 24, 335–344. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
S0954579412000028
Schnell, T. (2011). Individual differences in meaning-making: Considering
the variety of sources of meaning, their density and diversity. Person-
ality and Individual Differences, 51, 667– 673. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.paid.2011.06.006
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology.
An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5–14.
Seligman, M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positive
psychology progress: Empirical validation of interventions. American
Psychologist, 60, 410 421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.5
.410
Shaw, A., Joseph, S., & Linley, P. A. (2005). Religion, spirituality, and
posttraumatic growth: A systematic review. Mental Health, Religion &
Culture, 8, 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1367467032000157981
Shipman, K., Zeman, J., Penza, S., & Champion, K. (2000). Emotion
management skills in sexually maltreated and nonmaltreated girls: A
developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psycho-
pathology, 12, 47– 62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400001036
Sin, N. L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing well-being and allevi-
ating depressive symptoms with positive psychology interventions: A
practice-friendly meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65,
467– 487. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20593
Skaggs, B. G., & Barron, C. R. (2006). Searching for meaning in negative
events: Concept analysis. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 53, 559 –570.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2006.03761.x
Skopp, N. A., McDonald, R., Jouriles, E. N., & Rosenfield, D. (2007).
Partner aggression and children’s externalizing problems: Maternal and
partner warmth as protective factors. Journal of Family Psychology, 21,
459 467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.459
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W. A. (2005). Placing early
attachment experiences in developmental context. In K. E. Grossman, K.
Grossman, & E. Waters (Eds.), Attachment from infancy to adulthood:
The major longitudinal studies. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Swisher, R. (2008). Neighborhoods and youth: How neighborhood demo-
graphics and social processes affect youth outcomes. Prevention Re-
searcher, 15, 7–11.
Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: Concep-
tual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychological Inquiry, 15,
1–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1501_01
Testa, M., Hoffman, J. H., Livingston, J. A., & Turrisi, R. (2010). Pre-
venting college women’s sexual victimization through parent based
intervention: A randomized controlled trial. Prevention Science, 11,
308 –318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11121-010-0168-3
Thoits, P. A. (2011). Mechanisms linking social ties and support to phys-
ical and mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 52,
145–161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022146510395592
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of
definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
ment, 59, 25–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x
Tierney, J. P., & Grossman, J. B. (1995). Making a difference: An impact
study of Big Brothers, Big Sisters. Philadelphia, PA: Public/Private
Ventures.
Turner, H. A., Finkelhor, D., & Ormrod, R. (2010). Poly-victimization in
a national sample of children and youth. American Journal of Preventive
Medicine, 38, 323–330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2009.11.012
Vagi, K. J., Rothman, E. F., Latzman, N. E., Tharp, A. T., Hall, D. M., &
Breiding, M. J. (2013). Beyond correlates: A review of risk and protective
factors for adolescent dating violence perpetration. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 42, 633– 649. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-9907-7
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (2001). Journeys from childhood to midlife:
Risk, resilience and recovery. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Wickrama, K. A. S., & Noh, S. (2010). The long arm of community: The
influence of childhood community contexts across the early life course.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 894 –910. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/s10964-009-9411-2
Wolfe, D. A., Scott, K., Wekerle, C., & Pittman, A. L. (2001). Child
maltreatment: Risk of adjustment problems and dating violence in ado-
lescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 40, 282–289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-
200103000-00007
Wood, A. M., Froh, J. J., & Geraghty, A. W. A. (2010). Gratitude and
well-being: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology
Review, 30, 890 –905. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005
Wood, A. M., Joseph, S., & Linley, P. A. (2007). Coping style as a psycho-
logical resource of grateful people. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 26, 1076 –1093. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2007.26.9.1076
Wood, A. M., Maltby, J., Gillett, R., Linley, P. A., & Joseph, S. (2008).
The role of gratitude in the development of social support, stress, and
depression: Two longitudinal studies. Journal of Research in Personal-
ity, 42, 854 871. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.11.003
Wortman, C. B. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: Progress and problems.
Psychological Inquiry, 15, 81–90.
Zautra, A. J., Affleck, G. G., Tennen, H., Reich, J. W., & Davis, M. C.
(2005). Dynamic approaches to emotions and stress in everyday life:
Bolger and Zuckerman reloaded with positive as well as negative affects.
Journal of Personality, 73, 1511–1538. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
.0022-3506.2005.00357.x
Zoellner, T., & Maercker, A. (2006). Posttraumatic growth in clinical
psychology-a critical review and introduction of a two component mod-
el. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 626 653. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.cpr.2006.01.008
Received October 19, 2014
Revision received July 13, 2015
Accepted July 13, 2015
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
354 GRYCH, HAMBY, AND BANYARD
... Next, we discuss the findings of the extant literature in relation to the resilience portfolio model (Grych et al., 2015) and dignity literatures, which we assert hold great promise in preventing family violence among Indigenous populations. It is important to note that compared to the number of studies presented in this review more programs (and therefore research) have been developed for Indigenous communities to prevent other negative outcomes that share similar risk and protective factors, such as alcohol/substance use, suicidality, and mental health problems all of which are often tied to trauma (Gameon & Skewes, 2020;Leske et al., 2016;Snijder et al., 2020). ...
... The Resilience Portfolio Model describes assets and resources across individuals' and community's social ecologies including regulatory, interpersonal, and meaning making factors (Grych et al., 2015). This strengths-based model emphases the importance of assets ("characteristics of the person that promote healthy functioning" (Grych et al., 2015, p. 345) which includes skills that many of the programs reviewed include and has been applied in research with Indigenous communities Schultz et al., 2023). ...
... Beyond assets, the Resilience Portfolio Model discusses meaning making, which includes the desire and capacity to make sense of the events that occur in one's life and to foster a link between life events and an individual's broader beliefs and values (Grych et al., 2015;Panter-Brick & Eggerman, 2012). Meaning making is relevant to healing from past traumas, which is likely an important mechanism in family violence prevention among Indigenous populations (Edwards et al., 2023a(Edwards et al., , 2023b(Edwards et al., , 2023cRay et al., 2019;Shea et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The purpose of this paper is to conduct a systematic literature review focused on prevention initiatives for family violence among Indigenous populations in the U.S. and Canada. Methods The authors of this systematic review identified, critically reviewed, and synthesized findings from nine studies through searches of research databases on family violence prevention among Indigenous populations. Results Findings from the review highlight that skills-based, strengths-focused, culturally grounded programs targeting families, caregivers, and children have utility in the prevention of family violence among Indigenous populations. There is also some evidence that programs even with minimal cultural adaptations but that include staff cultural competency training may be beneficial in reducing family violence among Indigenous populations. Further, the resilience portfolio model and dignity literatures have great promise for the future of preventing family violence among Indigenous populations. These literatures point to the importance of a focus on strengths, culture, and intrinsic value of all people in family violence prevention work in addition to highlighting the importance of early intervention strategies to interrupt intergenerational processes contributing to high rates of family violence. Conclusions Future research on family violence prevention among Indigenous populations should utilize large samples, randomized trials (as long as they are acceptable by tribal/community partners), methods of data collection beyond self-report only, methods to ensure high rates of enrollment and retention, and intersectional approaches. Macro level initiatives are needed to reduce interlocking systems of oppression that drive disparities in family violence experienced by Indigenous populations.
... Psychological resilience is best understood as a collection of dynamic strengths-based processes that protect individuals from the psychological effects of trauma and support their ability to thrive (Hamby et al., 2018;Masten, 2015). The resilience portfolio model (RPM; Grych et al., 2015;Hamby et al., 2018) is a theoretical framework integrating research from positive and trauma psychology to explain how wellbeing occurs after adverse events. According to the RPM, resilience to trauma involves (a) prevention of/ decreased mental health problems and (b) promotion of/ increased wellbeing. ...
... Other strengths are external resources, that is, relationship and environmental PFs across ecological levels (family, community, and system-level). The RPM posits that everyone has a unique portfolio of assets and resources and that drawing upon them flexibly and reliably promotes wellbeing (Grych et al., 2015;Hamby et al., 2018). ...
... The RPM grew from research on civilian interpersonal and community violence (Grych et al., 2015), but the model also applies to other forms of adversity (Gonzalez-Mendez et al., 2021;Hamby et al., 2018Hamby et al., , 2020 and has been applied to civilians in postwar regions (Kelmendi & Hamby, 2023;Hamiti et al., 2024). To our knowledge, and based on the findings of our review, ours is the first study to apply the RPM explicitly to SMs or veterans. ...
Article
Military service members experience higher levels of cumulative trauma than the general population, increasing their risk of mental health problems. This scoping review synthesizes evidence on protective factors that contribute to posttraumatic wellbeing among military service members and veterans. PubMed and PsycINFO databases were searched using keywords for military/veterans, traumatic event exposure, posttraumatic stress, and wellbeing outcomes (e.g., quality of life [QoL]). Article abstracts and full texts were screened by two reviewers, with a third reviewer resolving conflicts. Inclusion criteria consisted of the following: (a) empirical study, (b) military/veteran sample, (c) exposed to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) criterion A event, (d) =1 protective factor examined, (e) =1 wellbeing outcome examined. After data extraction, Bibliometric Network Analysis was used to visualize the topics covered. Literature searches yielded 1,341 articles. Of these, 104 articles were retained after screening. Of the wellbeing outcomes studied, QoL, functioning, and posttraumatic growth were well-researched. Across intervention types (CBT-based, third wave, and complementary), some interventions were efficacious for wellbeing outcomes (mainly QoL), but many had negligible or nonsignificant effects. Other than social support, external resources, and systemic supports were understudied. Intensive interventions and those involving daily practice most effectively promoted wellbeing. Protective factors such as social support, executive functioning, optimism, and system-level resources should be better incorporated into PTSD care for service members/veterans.
... Numerous conceptual models of resilience emphasize that it is a multidimensional construct (Hamby, Grych, et al., 2018;Masten et al., 2021;Panzeri et al., 2021;Ungar, 2013) with a range of strengths and protective factors contributing to positive outcomes. Yet the study of resilience has often been fragmented by type of adversity or by the study of one strength at a time (Grych et al., 2015). At least within fields like psychology and social work, analyses often focus on an individual's resilience or perhaps a family's well-being in relation to psychosocial outcomes . ...
... The RPM also seeks to move beyond the limitations of past research by comparing many different strengths and including a composite measure, polystrengths, that considers the breadth of one's resilience portfolios. Early RPM work focused on three domains (regulatory, meaning making, and interpersonal) and the density and diversity of a person's strengths portfolio (Grych et al., 2015). In the original model, these domains of assets (that mostly reside within the individual including emotion regulation, interpersonal skills, and sense of purpose) are put together with resources outside the individual (relationships, social contexts) to try to fully capture strengths that may be part of resilience processes and lead to aspects of well-being across the social ecological model. ...
... Interpersonal strengths, another domain of the RPM, encompass both positive interpersonal relationships and individual characteristics that support the formation and maintenance of interpersonal connections (Grych et al., 2015;Hamby, Grych, et al., 2018). In previous work, aspects of the social environment such as school climate or social norms have also been included in this domain as social ecological resources that contribute to the process of resilience and that are made up of relationships between people either dyadically or within broader networks or the social environment (e.g., . ...
Article
Full-text available
This special issue offers a series of papers that use the resilience portfolio model (RPM) to explore multidimensional, strength-based approaches to resilience in a wide variety of communities. The field is urgently in need of research that helps identify the factors that help people thrive despite exposure to violence and other trauma. We need to know how people typically overcome victimization and other adverse experiences if we are going to improve intervention and minimize the global burden of trauma. The series of papers in this issue consider resilience portfolios across geographic locations, forms of violence and trauma, communities, and age groups. In this introduction, we synthesize key themes in four RPM domains: meaning making, regulatory, interpersonal, and environmental. We use these themes to describe a revised RPM and implications for future research and practices.
... Such findings underscore the universal importance of studying and promoting protective factors across diverse populations. The resilience portfolio model, proposed by Grych et al. (2015), offers a framework for categorizing and understanding these protective factors, proposing that the individual's response to adversity, particularly violence, is influenced by density and diversity of available resources and assets (both internal and external). This evolving understanding should gradually lead toward a more personalized approach in intervention design, with the potential to tailor programs to specific developmental periods and individual trajectories (Malti, 2020a). ...
Article
Objectives: Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) pose a significant public health concern, negatively impacting children’s physical and mental health. This study examines the association between ACEs and the co-occurrence of somatization and post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSSs) among Chinese college students. Additionally, it explores the roles of both internal (psychological resilience) and external (social support) protective factors in this relationship. Methods: A sample of 701 students were analyzed using the Adverse Childhood Experiences International Questionnaire, the Somatization subscale from the Symptom Checklist-90, the PTSD Checklist from the DSM-5, the Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale, and the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. A four-level outcome variable was created based on measures of somatization and PTSSs: no symptoms, somatization-only, post-traumatic stress symptoms-only (PTSSs-only), and co-occurring symptoms. Data analysis was conducted using multiple logistic regression. Results: Among Chinese college students, the prevalence of ACEs was 62.9%, while the prevalence of co-occurring somatization and PTSSs was 13.7%. The results of the multiple logistic regression analysis indicated a positive association between ACEs and the co-occurrence of somatization and PTSSs compared to no symptoms (OR = 2.28, p < 0.001). Furthermore, social support (OR = 0.26, p < 0.001) and psychological resilience (OR = 0.48, p = 0.049) were negatively associated with the co-occurrence. Conclusions: ACEs are risk factors for the co-occurrence of somatization and PTSSs among college students, while social support and psychological resilience serve as effective protective factors against this risk.
Article
Children from minoritized racial and ethnic backgrounds are at a higher risk for exposure to trauma and violence because of longstanding structural inequities; yet, these children can experience resilience by drawing on assets and resources across multiple levels of the social and physical ecology. Guided by the Resilience Portfolio Model, this scoping review aimed to synthesize evidence from the quantitative and qualitative literature on strengths among minoritized racial and ethnic groups of children in the United States exposed to trauma, violence, and maltreatment. The review also explored similarities and differences in strengths across racial and ethnic groups. The inclusion criteria were peer-reviewed empirical articles published in the past decade (2013–2023), written in English, focused on U.S. children (ages 0–17 years) from minoritized racial and ethnic groups, and examined strengths and resilience in the context of trauma, violence, or maltreatment. Comprehensive literature searches were conducted using electronic databases. A total of 57 articles were included in the review. The review identified various regulatory, meaning-making, and interpersonal strengths, as well as combined strengths (i.e., a mixture of regulatory, meaning-making, or interpersonal strengths) among minoritized children. These findings illustrate the utility of the Resilience Portfolio Model in understanding both distinct and overlapping strengths across minoritized racial and ethnic groups. Our findings highlight the need for a more nuanced and expanded investigation of resilience, including the identification of culturally specific strengths, among minoritized racial and ethnic groups and subgroups of children.
Article
Internationally, there has been a decline in the use of juvenile legal facilities (Puzzanchera et al., 2022). Yet, it is well documented that the process of reentering the community following placement in these settings presents numerous challenges for young people. Resilience-focused research offers a critical framework for prevention and intervention efforts to promote positive youth development; however, limited attention is given to the resilience of young people reentering communities from carceral and residential facilities. Addressing this gap in empirical knowledge requires an understanding of existing research on youth reentry and resilience. This scoping review identified peer-reviewed research from the past 20 years that focuses on the resilience of young people reentering from juvenile legal facilities, with a special interest in the subpopulations and identity groups represented and underrepresented in this research area. We identified, synthesized, and appraised 75 international and multidisciplinary studies on resilience experienced by youth who reenter from juvenile legal facility settings. Utilizing the Resilience Portfolio Model (Grych et al., 2015), we classified resilience processes relevant to youth reentry, including: (a) regulatory processes such as coping strategies and motivation; (b) meaning-making strengths such as sense of self, cultural identity, and future orientation; and (c) interpersonal relationships and social ecology, such as educational and vocational support. The utility of this scoping review includes identifying opportunities for future study on the role of resilience for system-involved young people, as well as a translational discussion of implications for existing and future practice, policy, and research in juvenile and criminal legal systems.
Article
This scoping review aimed to synthesize current literature on strengths associated with resilience and well-being among youth in Spanish-speaking Latin American countries exposed to macro-level violence and by type of exposure (i.e., political, community, and anti-LGBTQ+ violence and child soldiers)—guided by the Resilience Portfolio Model (RPM). Following the Preferred Reporting Items of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis-Scoping Review (PRISMA-R) guidelines, 42 studies were reviewed including 12 gray literature studies. Empirical studies were eligible for inclusion if they were (1) conducted in Spanish-speaking Latin American countries with (2) youth aged 15 to 24 who were (3) exposed to macro-level violence and (4) displayed positive outcomes or did not display negative outcomes. Multiple strengths in the domains of meaning-making (50% of studies), regulatory (52%), and interpersonal strengths (79%) were identified, outlining rich—although not comprehensive—resilience portfolios for youth exposed to macro-level violence in Spanish-speaking Latin America. These were supplemented by external social resources (43%) and cultural collective factors (33%). These factors focus on a person’s cultural background, collective worldview and responsibility, solidarity, civic participation, and activism for the benefit of the group or community. Findings confirmed and expanded the RPM by exploring cultural and collective strengths separately, informing policy and practice around the development of programs to enhance youth’s connections, supports, collectivism, purpose, and future orientation. Further research is needed across countries, cultures, and forms of violence.
Article
Resilience in the global south, especially in Kenya, is understudied. Despite limited research, it’s acknowledged that strengths stem from diverse cultural backgrounds. Global studies of resilience may help the field identify new areas of strength that can be a part of prevention and intervention with trauma survivors. This scoping review aimed to identify the range of strengths that exist among communities in Kenya. Additionally, this study will look at how components of the resilience portfolio model (regulatory, meaning-making, interpersonal, and resources) help predict indicators of well-being in Kenyan communities. This scoping review included empirical research on resilience in Kenya. The search in the electronic databases PubMed and PsycINFO produced a final selection of 40 articles for a full-text review. The results illustrate a range of strengths inherent within Kenyan communities that map onto the domains of the resilience portfolio model. The results underscore the significance of recognizing cultural strengths amid trauma and adversity. These include using art as a cultural anchor, utilizing food, meaningful caregiving, and adhering to specific tribal values such as respect. Research lenses used to study strengths in Kenyan communities are limited, and qualitative data suggests key areas for further inquiry.
Article
The effect of a grateful outlook on psychological and physical well-being was examined. In Studies 1 and 2, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 experimental conditions (hassles, gratitude listing, and either neutral life events or social comparison); they then kept weekly (Study 1) or daily (Study 2) records of their moods, coping behaviors, health behaviors, physical symptoms, and overall life appraisals. In a 3rd study, persons with neuromuscular disease were randomly assigned to either the gratitude condition or to a control condition. The gratitude-outlook groups exhibited heightened well-being across several, though not all, of the outcome measures across the 3 studies, relative to the comparison groups. The effect on positive affect appeared to be the most robust finding. Results suggest that a conscious focus on blessings may have emotional and interpersonal benefits.
Book
Since the original publication of this seminal work, acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) has come into its own as a widely practiced approach to helping people change. This book provides the definitive statement of ACT—from conceptual and empirical foundations to clinical techniques—written by its originators. ACT is based on the idea that psychological rigidity is a root cause of a wide range of clinical problems. The authors describe effective, innovative ways to cultivate psychological flexibility by detecting and targeting six key processes: defusion, acceptance, attention to the present moment, self-awareness, values, and committed action. Sample therapeutic exercises and patient–therapist dialogues are integrated throughout. New to This Edition *Reflects tremendous advances in ACT clinical applications, theory building, and research. *Psychological flexibility is now the central organizing focus. *Expanded coverage of mindfulness, the therapeutic relationship, relational learning, and case formulation. *Restructured to be more clinician friendly and accessible; focuses on the moment-by-moment process of therapy.
Article
Psychology has increasingly turned to the study of psychosocial resources in the examination of well-being. How resources are being studied and resource models that have been proffered are considered, and an attempt is made to examine elements that bridge across models. As resource models span health, community, cognitive, and clinical psychology, the question is raised of whether there is overuse of the resource metaphor or whether there exists some underlying principles that can be gleaned and incorporated to advance research. The contribution of resources for understanding multicultural and pan-historical adaptation in the face of challenge is considered.
Article
The study of resilience in development has overturned many negative assumptions and deficit-focused models about children growing up under the threat of disadvantage and adversity. The most surprising conclusion emerging from studies of these children is the ordinariness of resilience. An examination of converging findings from variable-focused and person-focused investigations of these phenomena suggests that resilience is common and that it usually arises from the normative functions of human adaptational systems, with the greatest threats to human development being those that compromise these protective systems. The conclusion that resilience is made of ordinary rather than extraordinary processes offers a more positive outlook on human development and adaptation, as well as direction for policy and practice aimed at enhancing the development of children at risk for problems and psychopathology. The study of resilience in development has overturned many negative assumptions and deficit-focused models about children growing up under the threat of disadvantage and adversity.
Article
This article describes the concept of posttraumatic growth, its conceptual foundations, and supporting empirical evidence. Posttraumatic growth is the experience of positive change that occurs as a result of the struggle with highly challenging life crises. It is manifested in a variety of ways, including an increased appreciation for life in general, more meaningful interpersonal relationships, an increased sense of personal strength, changed priorities, and a richer existential and spiritual life. Although the term is new, the idea that great good can come from great suffering is ancient. We propose a model for understanding the process of posttraumatic growth in which individual characteristics, support and disclosure, and more centrally, significant cognitive processing involving cognitive structures threatened or nullified by the traumatic events, play an important role. It is also suggested that posttraumatic growth mutually interacts with life wisdom and the development of the life narrative, and that it is an on-going process, not a static outcome.