ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The main goal of the present study was to investigate how the degree of orthographic overlap between translation equivalents influences bilingual word recognition processes at different stages of reading development. Spanish–Basque bilingual children with ages ranging from 8 to 15 years were tested in an explicit translation recognition task with a large set of items. Critically, the degree of cross-language similarity (i.e. the cognate status) between the references and the correct targets was manipulated along a continuum in order to investigate how the reliance on cross-language orthographic overlap varies as a function of reading experience. Results showed that younger children were significantly more sensitive to the cognate status of words than older children while recognising translation equivalents, and that this difference did not depend on the speed of response of the participants. These results demonstrate that the influence of cross-language similarity progressively diminishes as a function of increased exposure to print together with the maturation of the mechanisms responsible for language interference suppression, as suggested by developmental models of bilingual lexical access.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Developmental changes associated with cross-language similarity in bilingual children
Jon Andoni Duñabeitia
Lela Ivaz
Aina Casaponsa
BCBL. Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language; Donostia, Spain
Contact information:
Jon Andoni Duñabeitia
Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language (BCBL)
Paseo Mikeletegi 69, 2
20009 Donostia, SPAIN
j.dunabeitia@bcbl.eu
+34 943 309 300 (ext. 208)
Acknowledgements: This research has been partially funded by grants PSI2012-32123 from the
Spanish Government, ERC-AdG-295362 grant from the European Research Council, and by the
AThEME project funded by the European Union (grant number 613465).
!
!
!
1
Abstract: The main goal of the present study was to investigate how the degree of orthographic
overlap between translation equivalents influences bilingual word recognition processes at
different stages of reading development. Spanish-Basque bilingual children with ages ranging
from 8 to 15 years were tested in an explicit translation recognition task with a large set of items.
Critically, the degree of cross-language similarity (i.e., the cognate status) between the references
and the correct targets was manipulated along a continuum in order to investigate how the
reliance on cross-language orthographic overlap varies as a function of reading experience.
Results showed that younger children were significantly more sensitive to the cognate status of
words than older children while recognizing translation equivalents, and that this difference did
not depend on the speed of response of the participants. These results demonstrate that the
influence of cross-language similarity progressively diminishes as a function of increased
exposure to print together with the maturation of the mechanisms responsible for language
interference suppression, as suggested by developmental models of bilingual lexical access.
Short title: Cognate effects during childhood.
Keywords: Translation recognition; Cognate words; Reading development; Cross-language
interactions; Orthographic processing.
!
!
!
2
1. Introduction
Numerous recent studies have explored how bilingual individuals process words in their
languages at different stages of language consolidation and proficiency, and several factors have
been found to modulate bilingual word recognition. Bilingual lexical access largely depends on
second language (L2) proficiency and on the age of L2 acquisition (see van Hell & Tanner, 2012,
for a review), the combination of languages at hand and their orthographic distance (e.g., same
script vs. different script; see Chen, & Ng, 1989; Dimitropoulou, Duñabeitia, & Carreiras, 2011a;
Gollan, Forster & Frost, 1997; Wu & Thierry, 2010), or the orthotactic rules that govern each of
the languages and the language-dependent orthographic regularities (e.g., Casaponsa, Carreiras
& Duñabeitia, 2014; Vaid & Frenck-Mestre, 2002; Van Kesteren, Dijkstra, & Smedt, 2012). At
this regard, research on the processing of translation equivalents has become essential for our
understanding of how bilinguals process print in different languages. Translation equivalents are
words that belong to different languages but that denote the same concept, and they can vary in
the degree of orthographic and/or phonological overlap, going from non-overlapping
representations (e.g., the Spanish-English translation equivalents playa-beach), to completely
overlapping word forms (e.g., the Spanish and English words detective).
One of the most interesting findings from studies on bilingual word processing is the
degree of automaticity with which bilinguals activate translation equivalents when reading words
in one of their languages (e.g., Basnight-Brown & Altarriba, 2007; Dimitropoulou, Duñabeitia,
& Carreiras, 2011a, 2011b; Duñabeitia, Perea, & Carreiras, 2010; Duyck & Warlop, 2009;
Grainger & Frenck-Mestre, 1998; Schoonbaert, Duyck, Brysbaert, & Hartsuiker, 2009;
Schoonbaert, Holcomb, Grainger, & Hartsuiker, 2010; Thierry & Wu, 2007). In this vein, recent
behavioral studies have shown that it is easier to recognize native (L1) and non-native (L2)
!
!
!
3
words when they are preceded by their translation equivalents in the form of briefly presented
primes (e.g., masked priming paradigm). Interestingly, this facilitation effect for translation
equivalents has been shown to depend on several factors such as L2 proficiency, translation
direction and sub- and supra-lexical properties of translation equivalents. It has generally been
found that the influence of the L1 over L2 word processing is significantly greater than vice
versa when it comes to translation recognition in unbalanced bilinguals, giving rise to a
translation direction asymmetry which only disappears at L2 native-like levels of proficiency
(see Dimitropoulou et al., 2011a, 2011b; Duñabeitia, Perea, & Carreiras, 2010; Schoonbaert et
al., 2009). While balanced simultaneous bilinguals automatically activate translation equivalents
to the same extent when reading words in any of their languages independently of the translation
direction, at lower levels of L2 proficiency the translation facilitation effects are mainly found in
the L1-L2 direction (e.g., De Groot & Nas, 1991; Dimitropoulou et al., 2011a; Gollan, et al.,
1997; Jiang & Forster, 2001; Kim & Davis, 2003; Williams, 1994) and only a few studies have
found a translation facilitation effect in the L2-L1 direction (Dimitropoulou et al., 2011b; Duyck
& Warlop, 2009; Schoonbaert et al., 2009; Schoonbaert et al., 2010).
Moreover, it has been shown that the translation facilitation effects are also modulated by
the degree of word-form overlap between languages. Greater translation effects have been found
for cognates (i.e., words that share semantic and orthographic and/or phonological
representations across languages) than for non-cognates (i.e., words that only share semantic
representations). The confluence of overlapping representational levels in cognates provides
them with a processing advantage over non-cognate words and gives rise to the cognate
facilitation effect: bilinguals recognize, read, produce and translate cognates faster and more
accurately than non-cognates (e.g., Caramazza & Brones, 1979; Cristoffanini, Kirsner, & Milech,
!
!
!
4
1986; De Groot, Borgwaldt, Bos, & Van den Eijnden, 2002; Dijkstra, Van Jaarsveld, & Ten
Brinke, 1998; Duñabeitia, Perea, & Carreiras, 2010; Lemhöfer & Dijkstra, 2004; Lemhöfer,
Dijkstra, & Michel, 2004; van Heuven, Dijkstra, & Grainger, 1998; see Boada, Sánchez-Casas,
Gavilán, García-Albea, & Tokowicz, 2013, for a review). While the precise mechanisms driving
the cognate effects are still being debated, they are typically thought to arise from the convergent
triggering of language-shared orthographic, phonological and semantic activation for cognate
words. Following the interpretation of connectionist models (e.g., Dijkstra & Van Heuven, 1998,
2002), the cognate effects may result from the combination of bottom-up and top-down
activation, so that “as a consequence of orthographic and semantic overlap, more semantic
activation will arise for cognates than for non-cognates”, and “the two cognate readings together
may induce more global activation in the lexicon than other similar words (such as neighbors)
do” (Dijkstra, Miwa, Brummelhuis, Sappelli, & Baayen, 2010, p. 299). This way, a Spanish-
English bilingual would access an English word like detective significantly faster than a word
like beach due to the greater overlap at different levels (sub-lexical and lexico-semantic) of the
former word with its corresponding Spanish translation equivalent (detective-detective vs. playa-
beach).
Interestingly, the degree of orthographic overlap that cognates share across languages
largely determines the size of the cognate facilitation effect. It has been found that
orthographically identical cognates (e.g., the Spanish-English example detective) are processed
faster than near-identical cognates (e.g., the Spanish-English cognate pairs evidencia-evidence;
see Dijkstra, van Heuven, & Grainger, 1999; Dijkstra et al., 2010; Lemhöfer & Dijkstra, 2004;
Lemhöfer et al., 2004; Van Assche, Duyck, Hartsuiker, & Diependaele, 2009). According to
localist connectionist models of bilingual lexical access (e.g., BIA / BIA+ models; see Dijkstra &
!
!
!
5
Van Heuven, 2002), an increase in cross-linguistic similarity of translation equivalents yields
higher levels of activation for the two words in the lexicon, which leads to a modulation of
cognate effects as a function of the degree of orthographic overlap. Furthermore, and critically
for the purposes of the current study, Bultena, Dijkstra, & Van Hell (2014) showed that the
magnitude of the cognate facilitation effect when reading in a second language decreases as a
function of increased L2 proficiency. Nonetheless, and despite the wide range of factors shown
to modulate the cognate facilitation effect, what still remains to be explored is the sole influence
of reading experience (and not proficiency in a given language) on the cognate facilitation
effects, and the current cross-sectional study endeavors to do so by investigating how sensitive
bilingual developing readers of different ages are to cognates and non-cognates. To date, the
majority of studies exploring cross-language similarity have predominantly tested young adult
participants (e.g., all the aforementioned studies) and little is known about the way in which
reading expertise modulates bilingual visual word recognition and about the developmental
changes that underlie bilingual word comprehension.
Literacy acquisition and consolidation during elementary school has been shown to
generate critical changes in the organization of the orthographic lexicon of novice readers. At the
beginning stages of reading development, lexical competition between neighboring orthographic
representations is enhanced as compared to later stages in which reading skills are consolidated
and the structure of the mental lexicon is better established (e.g., Castles, Davis, & Forster, 2003;
Duñabeitia & Vidal-Abarca, 2008; Laxon, Coltheart, & Keating, 1988; Perea & Estévez, 2008).
It has been suggested that lexical competition among orthographically similar word forms
decreases as an inverse function of reading expertise (see Acha & Perea, 2008, for review), and
as a function of the development of top-down regulatory connections from the language nodes
!
!
!
6
and of lateral inhibitory connections (see Grainger et al., 2010, for review of theoretical
models)1. Considering that orthographic similarity is one of the key factors responsible for the
cognate effects, and that younger children are more sensitive than older individuals to
orthographic overlap among competing lexical forms, our guiding hypothesis in the current study
is that the processing of translation equivalents with different degrees of orthographic overlap in
the early stages of life (e.g., during childhood and early adolescence) will markedly change over
the course of reading development. While most of the studies testing this hypothesis have
focused on between-word competition effects in monolingual children, the present study
explored highly proficient bilingual children tested with words from their two lexicons as a
window to uncovering developmental changes in the organization of the bilingual lexicon.
Bilingual lexical processing necessarily requires language control mechanisms (e.g.,
Costa, Santesteban, & Caño, 2005; Green, 1998), and these mechanisms are largely based on
skills related to interference suppression and inhibitory control that develop during childhood
and early adolescence (e.g., Antón et al., 2014; Rueda et al., 2004; see Diamond, 2013, for
review). While it is unclear whether or not bilingual and monolingual children differ in executive
functioning and domain-general inhibitory capacities (cf. Duñabeitia et al., 2014), the necessity
of bilingual readers to efficiently organize the words from two languages in lexico-semantic
memory is uncontroversial. However, little is known with regard to the specific manner in which
the bilingual (orthographic) lexicon is created and even less is known about the developmental
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
1!Language nodes were assumed to play a key role at the word identification (lexical) level in the BIA
model, limiting cross-language interference via top-down inhibitory control (Dijkstra & Van Heuven, 1998). In
contrast, in its latest version (BIA+) they were assumed to have a representational status, providing information
regarding language membership as a function of ortho-phonological information, and regulating the interference
caused by the non-target language at a post-lexical stage (Dijkstra & Van Heuven, 2002). Even though the role of
language nodes was not fully determined in the BIA-d model (Grainger et al., 2010), they were conceptualized in
line with the original proposal, exerting top-down inhibition from the L2 word forms to the L1 translation
equivalents, and this inhibitory control was assumed to develop as a function of increased proficiency in the L2.
!
!
!
7
changes that arise during the course of bilingual reading consolidation. As we will review below,
to date, the number of studies testing this issue is very limited, and in most cases restricted to
children who are L2 learners.
A promising way to explore bilingual lexical organization, development and competition
effects is to investigate the changes in the processing of words with varying degrees of
orthographic similarity across languages in children (e.g., cognate words such as the Spanish-
English word pair guitarra-guitar). In their seminal study on this topic, Brenders, van Hell and
Dijkstra (2011) conducted a series of lexical decision experiments testing different groups of
Dutch children who were in the process of acquiring English as a second language. Specifically,
in two cross-sectional experiments they tested 5th graders (mean age of 10.5 years), 7th graders
(mean age of 12.6 years) and 9th graders (mean age of 14.3 years) in a series of lexical decision
tasks including cognates and control words. Their results showed that the cognate effects (shorter
RTs and lower error rates for cognates than for non-cognates) were present in all groups when
responding to L2 words (Experiment 1), whereas it vanished when responding to L1 words
(Experiment 2). Interestingly, there were no statistical differences between the magnitudes of the
cognate effects across groups in the L2 lexical decision task, in spite of a numerical trend
suggesting somewhat larger cognate effects for the younger group (52ms, 10ms and 25ms,
respectively). In a related study, Poarch and van Hell (2012) explored the cognate facilitation
effects in a series of language production tasks (picture naming) with multilingual children and
adults, following preceding research (cf. Hoshino & Kroll, 2008). In a first experiment, they
tested German children (ages ranging between 5 and 8 years) who were immersed in an English-
speaking academic environment and who were relatively proficient in English, and demonstrated
the presence of robust cognate effects in an English picture naming task (but not in their L1,
!
!
!
8
German). In a subsequent experiment exclusively testing more proficient yet unbalanced
bilinguals from the same cohort, they showed that the presence of cognate effects also extended
to the L1, even though the magnitude of the effect was still larger in the L2-naming condition.
When these results were compared to those obtained from a bilingual adult sample, Poarch and
van Hell observed that the magnitude of the cognate effects was two times larger in children than
in adults, both in their L1 and in their L2 (66ms vs. 38ms, and 197ms vs. 102ms, respectively).
This result was accompanied by a general difference in response latencies between children and
adults, showing that children took more time to respond than adults.
Based on the aforementioned results suggesting a developmental change in the
processing of cognates as a function of reading expertise in bilinguals (e.g., the numerical
differences observed by Brenders et al., 2011, with L2 learners in their lexical decision
experiments and those reported by Poarch & van Hell, 2012, with highly proficient bilinguals in
their picture naming tasks), in the current study we tested Spanish-Basque balanced bilingual
children of different ages in two explicit translation recognition tasks including words with
different degrees of cross-language similarity. We tested a large group (n=100) of bilingual
participants of different ages using a large set of translation equivalents, while measuring the
influence of the cognate rate of the translations on their performance. We used a modified
version of the traditional translation recognition task (see Prior, Kroll, & MacWhinney, 2012, for
review), in which a target word in one language was followed by two words in the other
language, out of which only one was the target’s translation equivalent that the participants were
asked to identify.
Following preceding evidence, some predictions could be made with regard to how
participants would perform in these translation recognition tasks. First, it was expected that older
!
!
!
9
participants would perform better (namely, exhibit shorter response latencies) than younger
participants due to their greater exposure to print. Second, considering preceding studies on
bilingual word recognition and translation processing, a general effect of cognate status (namely,
a modulation of the translation recognition processes as a function of the orthographic overlap
between word forms across languages) was also expected in both language directions and for all
participants. Finally, bearing in mind the amount of orthographic information cognates carry (to
which novice readers are predicted to be especially responsive), larger cognate effects were
expected for younger than older participants.
2. Experiment
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Participants. One hundred Spanish-Basque bilingual children and adolescents (68
females) were recruited from a bilingual school in the Basque Country. These children had an
age range between 8 and 15 years (mean age=11.51 years, SD=2.21). The distribution of
participants according to their age (in years) is presented in Figure 1. None of the participants
had previously been diagnosed with any neurocognitive disorder or learning disability. In order
to preserve the homogeneity across the different age groups, we made sure that all the
participants were born and raised in the Basque Country, and that their parents were also
originally from the Basque Country. All participants were native speakers of Spanish and
Basque, and they were exposed to the two languages from birth at home. Besides, all participants
had been attending the same bilingual school from the first grade of elementary school onwards.
!
!
!
10
All participants had daily exposure to Basque and Spanish as vehicular languages (linguae
francae) during tuition, and all were perfectly fluent in both languages. As already shown in
several scientific reports, pupils attending Spanish-Basque bilingual schools in the Basque
Country, where both languages are used as the languages of instruction, are highly proficient in
both languages (see Antón et al., 2014; Duñabeitia et al., 2014). According to the legal
requirements, schools in the Basque Country following a bilingual instruction method (called the
B model) require that teachers switch from one language to the other as they switch academic
subjects, ensuring a similar distribution of the two languages across subjects and school time
(roughly 50% in each language). This way, Basque children attending bilingual schools are
actively exposed to the two languages on a daily basis during schooling. All the participants’
parents or legal guardians signed informed consent forms before the experiment and were
appropriately informed regarding the basic procedure of the experiment, according to the ethical
commitments established by the BCBL Scientific Committee and by the BCBL Ethics
Committee that approved the experiment.
2.1.2. Materials. For the Spanish-to-Basque translation direction, 700 Spanish words were
selected as references, taken from B-Pal (Davis & Perea, 2005). These words were part of a
translation database containing 2100 Spanish and Basque word pairs rated for the quality of
translation, age of acquisition in each of the languages and concreteness by a group of native
Basque-Spanish bilingual adults (Duñabeitia, Casaponsa, Dimitropoulou, Martí, Larraza, &
Carreiras, in preparation). The mean length of these Spanish words was 7.67 characters
(SD=2.14), the mean word frequency was 45.26 appearances per million words (SD=79.92), the
mean AoA was 3.2 (SD=0.58) on a scale from 1 to 5 where higher values corresponded to words
!
!
!
11
acquired later in life, the mean number of orthographic neighbors was 1.43 (SD=2.65) and the
mean concreteness was 4 (SD=0.86) on a scale from 1 to 7 where higher values corresponded to
very concrete items. Each of these 700 Spanish reference words was then paired with two
different Basque words. One of the Basque words corresponded to the effective translation of the
Spanish word (e.g., the Spanish word salud (health) was paired with its corresponding translation
equivalent in Basque, osasun). The mean length of these Basque words was 7.59 characters
(SD=2.10), the mean word frequency taken from E-Hitz; (Perea, Urkia, Davis, Agirre, Laseka, &
Carreiras, 2006) was 57.32 appearances per million words (SD=218.93), the mean AoA was 3.26
(SD=0.51), the mean number of orthographic neighbors was 1.26 (SD=2.16) and the mean
concreteness was 4.03 (SD=0.84). These translation equivalents were rated as correctly matching
translations according to the 1-to-7 scale used in the translation database where 7 corresponded
to a perfect translation (mean=6.65, SD=0.19). Critically for the purposes of the present study,
the cognate status of these translation equivalents was calculated following the length-corrected
version of the orthographic Levenshtein distance between Spanish and Basque, ranging from 0
(non-cognates; e.g., febrero and otsail, the Spanish and Basque words for February,
respectively) to 1 (full cognates; e.g., maleta, the Spanish and Basque word for suitcase; see
Casaponsa, Antón, Pérez, & Duñabeitia, 2015; Duñabeitia, Dimitropoulou, Morris, &
Diependaele, 2013; Schepens, Dijkstra, & Grootjen, 2011). Given the extremely high
correspondence between the Spanish and Basque phonemes and considering that all the Basque
letters are also present in the Spanish alphabet, we focused exclusively on the orthographic
Levenshtein distance. The cognate status ranged from 0 to 1, and the mean cognate rate was 0.46
(SD=0.36). The distribution of items according to the length-corrected Levenshtein distance is
presented in Figure 1. In order to associate each Spanish reference with an unrelated Basque
!
!
!
12
word, the real Basque translations were rotated across trials and paired with mismatching
Spanish references. Care was taken so that the unrelated Spanish-Basque pairs did not overlap in
either orthography or semantics (e.g., the Spanish word salud (health) was paired with the
Basque word autobus (bus)). For the Basque-to-Spanish translation direction task, the same 700
Basque words that were previously selected as targets were now used as references (e.g., osasun
(health)). These Basque references were paired with their corresponding Spanish translations
(e.g., salud), or with an orthographically and semantically unrelated Spanish word created by
rotating the related items (e.g., autobús (bus)). In order to maintain homogeneity across the two
translation directions, the Spanish and Basque unrelated words used in each trial were also
translation equivalents across directions (e.g., autobus and autobús).
< Insert Figure 1 here >
2.1.3. Procedure. All the participants completed both tasks (i.e., translation matching from
Spanish to Basque and translation matching from Basque to Spanish) in two different
experimental sessions that took place in two different days with at least a three-day lag between
them and in a counterbalanced order across participants. We explicitly counterbalanced the order
of the tasks across participants and set a delay between sessions in order to avoid the presence of
cross-language repetition priming effects, which have been previously reported in tasks that
require conceptual processing when the study and test phase take place in the same session (see
Zeelenberg & Pecher, 2003). The items were presented using the Experiment Builder software
on a CRT screen (positioned approximately 70 cm in front of the participants) that was linked to
a PC, and data collection associated with button presses was collected with a response box
(Empirisoft DirectIN High Speed Button-Box). Data collection was always carried out
individually on the school premises during the teaching hours. In each experimental session,
!
!
!
13
participants were informed that they were going to be presented with a series of words
corresponding to the reference language (Spanish or Basque, depending on the translation
direction) presented in the center of the screen, followed by two words in a different language
presented on the left and right sides of the screen. They were instructed to press, as quickly and
accurately as possible, one out of two buttons on the response box to indicate which of the two
target words corresponded to the exact translation of the reference word (the leftmost or the
rightmost button, depending on the location of the correct translation on the screen). Across
trials, half of the correct translations for each reference appeared on the right of the screen, and
the other half appeared on the left side. Each trial started with the presentation of a mask (e.g.,
######) for 500ms located in the exact place were the reference would appear. Then the
corresponding word was presented in uppercase Courier New font for 1000ms. Finally, the two
target words were presented in lowercase letters until a participant responded or for a maximum
of 5000ms (see Figure 2). After each trial, feedback about the accuracy was provided for 500ms
in the center of the screen (! for correct responses and " for incorrect responses). The order of
trial presentation was randomized across participants, and there were 6 small breaks during the
experiment. Prior to the experimental trials, each participant was presented with a short practice
consisting of 20 items (10 requiring a left response and 10 requiring a right response). All
interactions with the participants and the experimental instructions were in the language
corresponding to the references. Each experimental session lasted for around 50 minutes.
< Insert Figure 2 here >
2.2. Results
!
!
!
14
Correct RTs for target words were analyzed with linear mixed effects (lme) models as
implemented in the lme4 package (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2014) in R (R CoreTeam,
2013). Significance p-values and Type III F-statistics for main effects and interactions were
calculated using Satterthwaite approximations to degrees of freedom as implemented in the
lmerTest package (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, 2014). Timeouts (Basque targets:
.59%; Spanish targets: .52%), errors (Basque targets: 7.72%; Spanish targets: 9.10%), and RTs
shorter than 300 ms (Basque targets: .24%; Spanish targets: .41%) were excluded from the
analysis. For each translation direction, latencies shorter than Q1-2.5*IQR and longer than
Q3+2.5*IQR for each participant (Basque targets: 1.78%; Spanish targets: 1.94%) and item
(Basque targets: 1.80%; Spanish target: 1.86%) were also discarded. This resulted in a dataset of
123,076 data points. Transformed RTs, either by the inverse-transform (-1000/RT) or
logarithmic scale attenuated the skew in their distribution just for the oldest participants.
Therefore, in order to avoid any age-dependent bias in the distribution of the RTs in the analysis,
RTs were entered into the model without any transformation.
We first investigated the presence of a significant interaction between the continuous
predictor Cognate Rate and Age, and the categorical factor Translation Direction (i.e., Spanish-
to-Basque and Basque-to-Spanish) with subjects and items as crossed random factors (e.g.,
Baayen, 2008; Baayen, Davidson, & Bates, 2008). Apart from the experimental conditions, we
also considered three other control variables that might affect decision latencies along the
experimental session, as shown in previous studies (e.g., Diependaele, Duñabeitia, Morris, &
Keuleers, 2011; Mulder, Dijkstra, Schreuder, & Baayen, 2014, among others). Thus, reaction
times to the previous trial (Previous Response), Trial Order, and Session Order were considered
as predictors. The maximal-order interaction of the within-unit predictors of interest were
!
!
!
15
included as random slopes (see Barr et al., 2013). The random-effect structure of the final model
consisted of random intercepts for Item (SD=145), and Subject (SD=206), by-subject random
slopes for the interaction of Cognate Rate with Translation Direction (Basque targets: SD=160;
Spanish targets: SD= 162), and by-item random slopes for Age (SD=21). Due to the differences
in scale of our predictors resulting in a very large eigen value in the model, the predictor Age was
centered for its inclusion in the final model. The standard deviation of the residual was 339.
< Insert Tables 1 and 2 here >
The F-statistics associated with the main effects and interactions resulting from the model
are presented in Table 1, and Table 2 summarizes the coefficients along with standard errors and
t-values. Results from the model showed a significant inverse relation between reaction times
and Age, showing that response latencies decreased as age increased. The main effect of Cognate
Rate was significant, showing that response latencies also decreased as the cognate status of
words increased (i.e., translation equivalents with large orthographic overlap across languages
yielded shorter response latencies). The main effect of Translation Direction was significant,
showing that participants responded faster when targets were presented in Spanish than when
they were presented in Basque. This factor significantly interacted with Age, showing that the
decrease in reaction times as Age increased was milder for Spanish targets (b= -78, SD= 10, t=-
8.22, p<.001) than for Basque targets (b= -87, SD=10, t=-9.14, p<.001; see Figure 3).
Interestingly for the purposes of the current study, the interaction between Cognate Rate and Age
resulted significant in the model and did not significantly differ between both translation
directions (see Table 1). As can be seen in Figure 3 (b and c), there was a marked decrease in the
reaction times as the orthographic overlap between languages increased and, critically, the
magnitude of these cognate effects diminished with age. In other words, results showed that the
!
!
!
16
magnitude of the cognate effects significantly decreased as an inverse function of age in both
translation directions (Basque targets: b= 41, SD= 8, t= 5.30, p<.001; Spanish targets: b= 36,
SD= 8, t=4.51, p<.001), being maximal at initial stages of reading development and
progressively diminishing with age.
< Insert Figure 3 here >
Recent research has suggested that the classic cognate effects may depend on the
presence of identical (fully overlapping) cognates within the list of stimuli (see Comesaña et al.,
2015; see also Comesaña et al., 2012). Given that the current experiment also included identical
cognates, we decided to further explore whether the presence of such translation equivalents may
have modulated the results by reanalyzing the data while excluding fully overlapping cognates
from the list. Tentatively, it could be argued that, in the context of the current experiment,
translation recognition of full cognate words (translation equivalents with cognate rates of 1)
could be guided by a mere repetition-match strategy, thus boosting the presence of a cognate
effect. To ensure that the results of the model were not biased by this factor, the same model was
refitted by excluding fully overlapping cognate translations from the set (86 items). Importantly,
the analysis yielded a similar pattern of results. The three main effects remained significant
(Cognate Rate: F=145.25, p<.001; Age: F=43.83, p<.001, Translation Direction: F=18.93,
p<.001). As in the analysis with the whole set of items, in this new analysis the interaction
between Age and Translation Direction was also found (F=6.79, p<.01) and, critically, the
interaction between Cognate Rate and Age was also replicated (F=14.95, p<.001), showing that
response latencies diminished as the orthographic overlap between translations increased, and
that these cognate effects diminished with Age in both translation directions (Basque targets: b=
40, SD= 9, t=4.72, p<.001; Spanish targets: b= 34, SD= 9, t= 3.84, p<.001). Furthermore, the
!
!
!
17
magnitude of these effects did not statistically differ between translation directions (F=.65,
p=.52).
Previous literature has shown that overall speed differences across participants can lead
to spurious over-additive interactions (see Faust, Balota, Spieler, & Ferraro, 1999; Ziegler,
Bertrand, Lété, & Grainger, 2014, among others). Not surprisingly, the participants tested in the
current study showed an overall effect of Age (i.e., shorter reaction times for the older
participants), and this could be thought to contribute to the greater Cognate Rate effects (i.e., the
more marked slopes) for the younger participants. To address this critical issue, we standardized
the data for each participant using z-scores. Thus, data were transformed so that each participant
had a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 in each translation direction. We then reanalyzed
these data following the same structure used in the main analysis in order to explore whether the
interaction between Age and Cognate Rate remained significant with the standardized scores.
Results are summarized in Tables 3 and 4. Results confirmed the significant inverse relationship
between Cognate Rate and Age, as shown in the analysis on the non-standardized RTs. The
interaction between these two continuous predictors of interest was significant, showing that the
slope of Cognate Rate was more pronounced for older than for younger participants (i.e., greater
cognate effects for younger children; see Figure 4). Hence, the results from the analysis on the
transformed data replicated the initial analysis, showing that as the age increased, the magnitude
of the cognate effects decreased.
< Insert Tables 3 and 4 here >
< Insert Figure 4 here >
!
!
!
18
4. General Discussion
The main goal of the present study was to investigate how the degree of orthographic
overlap between translation equivalents influenced bilingual word recognition processes at
different stages of reading development, in order to better understand how bilingual lexical
organization changes over the course of bilingual reading consolidation. We tested 100 Spanish-
Basque bilinguals of different ages in two translation recognition tasks (Spanish-to-Basque
translation direction and Basque-to-Spanish translation direction). Results showed a general
modulation of reading performance as a function of reading development. The younger the
participants were, the slower the translation recognition process. In addition, and more
importantly for the focus of this study, the results showed that the degree of orthographic overlap
between translation equivalents helped bilingual translation recognition and, critically, that this
cognate effect progressively diminished with age.
Results showed that the response latencies to translation equivalents decreased with age.
Hence, we observed a general modulation of bilinguals’ ability to recognize translation
equivalents that is related to their general reading skills and to their stage of reading
development. These general results from the translation recognition task are in line with evidence
from monolingual samples showing that general reading skills get progressively tuned and
consolidated during the transition from childhood to adolescence (see Grainger et al., 2012). It is
well known that at the early stages of reading development several factors play an important role
and affect the variability observed in children’s proficiency while reading (e.g., general cognitive
development, working memory capacity, general intelligence, exposure to print at home, etc.)
and that this variability tends to diminish by the end of elementary education (Coombe, 2010;
Ellis, 1996; Folse, 2004; Nation, 1990, 2001, 2006; Papagno & Vallar, 1995; Service, 1992).
!
!
!
19
Critically, response latencies were modulated by the degree of orthographic overlap
between translation equivalents, showing that along the continuum of cognate status, cognate
words were recognized faster than non-cognate words (see Figure 2). The degree of orthographic
overlap between translation equivalents is a critical aiding factor during the bilingual word
recognition process, as shown by multiple studies demonstrating facilitation effects for cognates
over non-cognates using different tasks and paradigms (e.g., Boada et al., 2013; De Groot &
Comijs, 1995; De Groot, Dannenburg, & Van Hell, 1994; Duñabeitia, Perea, & Carreiras, 2010).
In the current study we replicated this cognate advantage in the translation recognition task using
a continuous measure of the degree of cross-linguistic orthographic overlap (based on the
length002Dcorrected Levenshtein distance; see Casaponsa et al., 2015; Duñabeitia et al., 2013;
Schepens et al., 2011; see also Prior, Kroll, & MacWhinney, 2013, for an alternative approach).
The present experiment adds to that evidence by showing a general cognate facilitation effect
that increases as a direct function of the degree of orthographic overlap between translation
equivalents. Participants’ response latencies decreased as the orthographic similarity between the
translation equivalents increased.
Even though all participants had already acquired basic reading skills and were proficient
in both of their languages, there was a clear-cut gradation of the degree of automation and
consolidation of the bilingual visual word recognition processes as a function of age. In this line,
results showed a significantly better performance in the translation recognition task for older
than for younger participants (i.e., shorter response latencies). Moreover, this pattern was
replicated in both language translation directions (Spanish-to-Basque and Basque-to-Spanish).
While the effect of age was significantly stronger in the Spanish-to-Basque translation direction
than in the Basque-to-Spanish translation direction, the magnitude of the effect of cognate rate
!
!
!
20
was not modulated by the translation direction. In other words, the direction of the translation
recognition process had little impact on the observed decreasing pattern of cognate effects across
age. This is not such a surprising finding, considering that in simultaneous balanced bilinguals a
straightforward L1-L2 division cannot be made at the level of proficiency, given that they are
equally proficient in both languages. Their languages are in that sense better characterized as
multiple L1s (see Duñabeitia, Dimitropoulou, Uribe-Etxebarria, Laka, & Carreiras, 2010;
Duñabeitia, Perea, & Carreiras, 2010; Perea, Duñabeitia, & Carreiras, 2008).
More importantly, the effect of orthographic overlap between translation equivalents was
modulated by the age of the bilingual participants. The younger the children were, the greater
facilitation effects along the cognate status continuum were found, and these results were
replicated in both translation directions. Furthermore, an analysis of the data using z-scores in
order to control for the fact that younger children were much slower on the task than older
participants showed that these results did not depend on the general speed of response. These
results suggest that less proficient readers are more responsive to and reliant on cross-language
orthographic overlapping features than more experienced readers, and that they benefit more
from these types of cues during bilingual word recognition. Hence, these results demonstrate that
cognate effects are not only maximal at lower levels of L2 proficiency (see Bultena et al., 2014),
but also at lower levels of reading skills, even when the participants tested are highly proficient
in the two languages at test. (Note in this regard that all bilinguals tested in the current study
attended a bilingual school and had acquired reading in both languages simultaneously). This is
in line with the evidence suggesting that the orthographic characteristics of words play an
important role during word processing in young children (e.g., Davis, Castles, & Iakovidis, 1998;
!
!
!
21
Grainger & Ziegler, 2011; Grainger et al., 2012; Ziegler et al., 2014), and extends this finding to
bilingual word processing.
The present results complement a recent observation from a word production study by
Poarch and van Hell (2012) testing bilingual children’s sensitivity to the cognate status of words
by extending those results to the visual word recognition domain suggesting that the cognate
effects diminish as an inverse function of age in translation recognition tasks. The highly
proficient non-balanced bilingual samples tested by Poarch and van Hell in their picture naming
study displayed significant cognate effects in both their L1 and L2, and critically, the magnitude
of the cognate effects was found to be much larger (specifically, two times larger) for children
with a mean age of 7.28 years than for young adults with a mean age of 24.4 years. (Note,
however, that Poarch and van Hell did not directly statistically compare the magnitude of the
cognate effects across the ages). Hence, the present study extends those findings by
demonstrating that sensitivity to cross-language orthographic overlap diminishes with age in
highly proficient bilinguals in a language comprehension task. In a related vein, it is worth
noting that in a bilingual language comprehension study using the lexical decision task with
young Dutch learners of English, Brenders et al. (2011) also showed significant cognate effects
in the L2 (but not in the L1) for three groups of relatively proficient non-balanced bilingual
children (mean ages of 10.5, 12.6 and 14.3 years). However, and in partial contrast to the present
results, the interaction between cognate status and group did not result significant in the study by
Brenders et al., thus suggesting that the cognate effects remained relatively constant across ages.
Nonetheless, there are sufficient reasons to believe that the present results and those reported by
Brenders et al. are not at odds. Leaving aside the differences in the tasks at stake (lexical
decision vs. translation recognition), in the present study we tested highly proficient bilingual
!
!
!
22
children from different ages immersed in a bilingual community and who were attending a
bilingual school where the two active languages (Spanish and Basque) are used a similar amount
of time, while Brenders and colleagues tested a completely different group of bilinguals with
much more limited exposure to their L2 (i.e., they were native Dutch children living in
monolingual Dutch families, and they had been exposed to English as a second language only in
the academic context). Besides, the amount of exposure to English as a second language of the
children tested in the Brenders et al. study largely varied across groups (i.e., 5 months for the
younger group, and 3 and 5 years for the two older groups, respectively), thus making a direct
comparison between the two cohorts difficult.
Which are the mechanisms responsible for the developmental changes in the cognate
effects observed in the current study? The first approach interpreting the finding of the cognate
effects’ diminution as a function of age relies on the different reading skills (i.e., reading
expertise) of the participants tested. Our bilingual children’s two languages are essentially
transparent, in that there are relatively unambiguous grapheme-phoneme correspondences that
are shared across Basque and Spanish. Relatedly, developmental studies on reading acquisition
and consolidation in multiple languages suggest that inexperienced readers of orthographically
consistent languages strongly rely on recoding strategies based on small ortho-phonological units
(i.e., the psycholinguistic grain size theory; see Ziegler & Goswami, 2005, 2006), and that larger
size units are used once reading has been consolidated. Hence, according to these premises, we
tentatively interpret the larger cognate effects observed during translation recognition for
younger rather than for older participants as a consequence of the greater reliance of the former
on sub-lexical units, which in the case of cognate translations are shared across languages, thus
speeding up the recognition process. The shared (language-independent) nature of sub-lexical
!
!
!
23
orthographic and phonological units that we assume here is in line with preceding studies on
biscriptal readers suggesting that graphemes and phonemes that are identical across languages or
scripts (with identical spelling-sound correspondences) are integrated in a single orthographic
lexicon (see Dimitropoulou, Duñabeitia, & Carreiras, 2011c; Havelka & Rastle, 2005). In
contrast, and as suggested by Ziegler and Goswami (2006), consolidation of reading skills
involves a lesser degree of reliance on sub-lexical cues during visual word identification, and this
could partially explain why older bilinguals exhibit reduced cognate effects (see also Acha &
Perea, 2008, and Kohnert, 2010). In a related vein, studies exploring similarities and differences
in the magnitude of cognate effects between young and older bilingual adults have also
suggested a critical modulation of cross-language interactions as a function of age. It has been
proposed that “lexical access in older bilinguals may be detrimentally affected by the form
similarity of cognate translations” (Siyambalapitiya, Chenery, & Copland, 2009, p. 548), in line
with previous research demonstrating that the reliance on form similarity between competing
lexical forms dramatically changes as a function of age (see Logan & Balota, 2003), so that the
cognate effects can vanish (and even go in the opposite direction) in the elderly.
Developmental models of bilingual lexical access have proposed that lateral inhibition
processes that are responsible for the management of cross-language interactions are
strengthened as a function of experience (see BIA-d model, Grainger et al., 2010). Following the
rationale of the Bilingual Interactive-Activation model (BIA; Grainger & Dijkstra, 1992; van
Heuven, Dijkstra, & Grainger, 1998), the BIA-d model assumes that at the initial stages of visual
word recognition, lexical access is non-selective, and language-specific representations are
accessed “due to the conjoint operation of top-down and lateral inhibitory mechanisms
(Grainger et al., 2010, p. 271). According to this model, the excitatory connections between
!
!
!
24
translation equivalents are initially enhanced in the presence of orthographic overlap (thus
explaining the cognate facilitation effect), but it is predicted that these excitatory connections
decay rapidly as proficiency increases, as an inverse function of the development of top-down
regulatory mechanisms from the corresponding language nodes and of the development of lateral
inhibitory connections between formally overlapping words in the two languages. Hence, the
BIA-d model suggests that the strength of the connections between translation equivalents, and
especially between orthographically similar translations, is reduced due to the development of
lateral and top-down inhibitory connections (Grainger et al., 2010; see also Casaponsa et al.,
2015). Hence, one could interpret the current findings based on the different interference
suppression skills of younger and older bilingual children, as suggested by the BIA-d model.
While it is true that bilingual children master interference suppression in order to deal with the
words from one of their two languages with minimal intrusions from the other language (see
Martin-Rhee & Bialystok, 2008, for review), the attentional mechanisms supposedly responsible
for such language control are not rigidly set during early childhood as they are in continuous
development during infancy (e.g., Antón et al., 2014; Rueda et al., 2004). It could be
hypothesized that younger children show greater cross-language similarity effects than older
children as a consequence of their still immature language control system (yielding impoverished
top-down regulatory activity from the language nodes and lateral inhibitory regulation at the
lexical level), which makes them more sensitive to cross-language interference effects (here
measured by the cognate status of words). While the current results fit well along the lines
sketched in the BIA-d model, we acknowledge that this model was initially intended to account
for bilingual lexical access in late learners of a second language. Hence, the future revised
versions of this developmental model should also incorporate predictions regarding the way in
!
!
!
25
which access to the bilingual lexicon is differently mediated by bottom-up and top-down
regulatory factors as a function of age in relatively balanced and simultaneous bilinguals.
Together, these results demonstrate that the consolidation of the bilingual orthographic
lexicon follows a developmental trajectory during childhood that is initially governed by a
greater reliance on cross-language similarity possibly due to the still immature bilingual
language control system of novice readers. Once bilingual reading becomes automatic as a
function of increased exposure to print and once the attentional mechanisms responsible for
cross-language interference suppression are fully set and developed, bilinguals’ sensitivity to
competing neighboring orthographic representations decays, leading to the progressive
diminishing of the cognate effects observed in the current study.
!
!
!
26
References
Acha, J., & Perea, M. (2008). The effects of length and transposed-letter similarity in
lexical decision: Evidence with beginning, intermediate, and adult readers. British Journal of
Psychology, 99, 245-264.
Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J.,
Davidson, D. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task?
Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5:398.
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing Linguistic Data: A Practical Introduction to Statistics
Using R. Cambridge University Press. New York.
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with
crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390-
412.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random-effects structure for
confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255-
278.
Basnight-Brown, D. M., & Altarriba, J. (2007). Differences in semantic and translation
priming across languages: the role of language direction and language dominance. Memory and
Cognition, 35, 953–965.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects
models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-7, [Computer software manual]. Available
from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
!
!
!
27
Boada, R., Sánchez-Casas, R., Gavilán, J. M., García-Albea, J. E., & Tokowicz, N.
(2013). Effect of multiple translations and cognate status on translation recognition performance
of balanced bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 183-197.
Brenders, P., Van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T. (2011). Word recognition in child second
language learners: Evidence from cognates and false friends. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 109, 383-396.
Bultena, S., Dijkstra, T., & Van Hell, J. G. (2014). Cognate effects in sentence context
depend on word class, L2 proficiency, and task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 67(6), 1214-1241.
Caramazza, A., & Brones, I. (1979). Lexical access in bilinguals. Bulletin of the
Psychonomic Society, 13, 212-214.
Casaponsa, A., Antón, E., Pérez, A., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2015). Foreign language
comprehension achievement: insights from the cognate facilitation effect. Frontiers in
Psychology, 6:588.
Casaponsa, A., Carreiras, M., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2014). Discriminating languages in
bilingual contexts: The impact of orthographic markedness. Frontiers in Psychology, 5:424.
Castles, A., Davis, C., & Forster, K.I. (2003). Word recognition development in children:
Insights from masked-priming. In S. Kinoshita & S. Lupker (Eds.), Masked priming: State of the
art (pp. 345-360). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Chen, H. C., & Ng, M. L. (1989). Semantic facilitation and translation priming effects in
Chinese-English bilinguals. Memory & Cognition, 17(4), 454-462.
!
!
!
28
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). The DRC model:
A model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204 - 258.
Comesaña, M., Ferré, P., Romero, J., Guasch, M., Soares, A. P., & García-Chico, T.
(2015). Facilitative effect of cognate words vanishes when reducing the orthographic overlap:
The role of stimuli list composition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 41(3), 614-635.
Comesaña, M., Sánchez-Casas, R., Soares, A. P., Pinheiro, A. P., Rauber, A., Frade, S.,
& Fraga, I. (2012). The interplay of phonology and orthography in visual cognate word
recognition: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 529, 75-79.
Coombe, C. (2010). Assessing vocabulary in the language classroom. Kuala Lumpur,
Malaysia: the Malaysian English Language Teaching Association.
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Caño, A. (2005). On the facilitatory effects of cognate
words in bilingual speech production. Brain and Language, 94, 94-103.
Cristoffanini, P., Kirsner, K., & Milech, D. (1986). Bilingual lexical representation: The
status of Spanish-English cognates. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 367-
393.
Davis, C. J., & Perea, M. (2005). BuscaPalabras: A program for deriving orthographic
and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. Behavior
Research Methods, 37, 665-671.
!
!
!
29
Davis, C. J., Castles, A., & Iakovidis, E. (1998). Masked homophone and
pseudohomophone priming in children and adults. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 625-
651.
De Groot, A. M. B., & Comijs, H. (1995). Translation recognition and translation
production: Comparing a new and an old tool in the study of bilingualism. Language Learning,
45, 467-509.
De Groot, A. M. B., & Nas, G. L. J. (1991). Lexical representation of cognates and
noncognates in compound bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 90-132.
De Groot, A. M. B., Borgwaldt, S., Bos, M., & Van den Eijnden, E. (2002). Lexical
decision and word naming in bilinguals: Language effects and task effects. Journal of Memory
and Language, 47, 91-124.
De Groot, A. M. B., Dannenburg, L., & Van Hell, J. G. (1994). Forward and backward
word translation by bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 600-629.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168.
Diependaele, K., Duñabeitia, J. A., Morris, J., & Keuleers, E. (2011). Fast morphological
effects in first and second language word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 64,
344-358.
Dijkstra, T., Miwa, K., Brummelhuis, B., Sappeli, M., & Baayen, R. H. (2010). How
cross-language similarity and task demands affect cognate recognition. Journal of Memory and
Language, 62, 284-301.
!
!
!
30
Dijkstra, A., & Van Heuven, W. J. B. (1998). The BIA model and bilingual word
recognition. In J. Grainger & A. Jacobs (Eds.), Localist connectionist approaches to human
cognition (pp. 189–225). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dijkstra, A., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word
recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5,
175–197.
Dijkstra, T., van Heuven, W. J. B., & Grainger, J. (1999). Recognition of cognates and
interlingual homographs: The neglected role of phonology. Journal of Memory and Language,
41, 496-518.
Dijkstra, T., Van Jaarsveld, H., & Ten Brinke, S. (1998). Interlingual homograph
recognition: Effects of task demands and language intermixing. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition, 1, 51-66.
Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2011a). Masked translation
priming effects with low proficient bilinguals. Memory and Cognition, 39(2), 260-275.
Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2011b). Two words, one meaning:
evidence of automatic co-activation of translation equivalents. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(188),
1-20.
Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J.A., & Carreiras, M. (2011c). Transliteration and
transcription effects in bi-scriptal readers: The case of Greeklish. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 18(4), 729-735.
!
!
!
31
Duñabeitia, J. A., Casaponsa, A., Dimitropoulou, M., Martí, A., Larraza, S., & Carreiras,
M. (In preparation). BaSp: A Basque-Spanish database of translation equivalents.
Duñabeitia, J. A., Dimitropoulou, M., Morris, J., & Diependaele, K. (2013). The role of
form in morphological priming: Evidence from bilinguals. Language and Cognitive Processes,
28, 967-987.
Duñabeitia, J. A., Dimitropoulou, M., Uribe-Etxebarria, O., Laka, I., & Carreiras, M.
(2010). Electrophysiological correlates of the masked translation priming effect with highly
proficient simultaneous bilinguals. Brain Research, 1359, 142–154.
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., &
Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality?
Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 234-251.
Duñabeitia, J. A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Masked translation priming effects
with highly proficient simultaneous bilinguals. Experimental Psychology, 57(2), 98-107.
Duñabeitia, J. A., & Vidal-Abarca, E. (2008). Children like dense neighborhoods:
Orthographic neighborhood density effects in novel readers. The Spanish Journal of Psychology,
11, 26-35.
Duyck, W., & Warlop, N. (2009). Translation priming between the native language and a
second language: new evidence from Dutch-French bilinguals. Experimental Psychology, 56(3),
173–179.
!
!
!
32
Ellis, N. C. (1996). Working memory in the acquisition of vocabulary and syntax: Putting
language in good order. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A 49: 234-
250.
Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H., & Ferraro, F. R. (1999). Individual differences
in information-processing rate and amount: Implications for group differences in response
latency. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 777–799.
Folse, K. S. (2004). Vocabulary myths: Applying second language research to classroom
teaching. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Gollan, T. H., Forster, K. I., & Frost, R. (1997). Translation priming with different
scripts: Masked priming with cognates and noncognates in Hebrew-English bilinguals. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 23(5), 1122-1139.
Grainger, J. Midgley, K. J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2010). Re-thinking the bilingual
interactive-activation model from a developmental perspective (BIA-d). In M. Kail and M.
Hickman (Eds.), Language Acquisition across linguistic and cognitive systems (pp. 267-284).
Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Grainger, J., & Dijkstra, T. (1992). On the representation and use of language
information in bilinguals. In R. Harris (Ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 207–220).
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Grainger, J., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (1998). Masked priming by translation equivalents in
bilinguals. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 601-623.
Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing.
Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 54.
!
!
!
33
Grainger, J., Lété, B., Bertand, D., Dufau, S., & Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Evidence for
multiple routes in learning to read. Cognition, 123, 280-292.
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81.
Havelka, J. & Rastle, K. (2005). The assembly of phonology from print is serial and
subject to strategic control: Evidence from Serbian. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 31, 148-158.
Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F. (2008). Cognate effects in picture naming: Does cross
language activation survive a change of script? Cognition, 106, 501-511.
Jiang, N., & Forster, K. I. (2001). Cross-language priming asymmetries in lexical
decision and episodic recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(1), 32-51.
Kim, J., & Davis, C. (2003). Task effects in masked cross-script translation and
phonological priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 484–499.
Kohnert, K. (2010). Bilingual children with primary language impairment: issues,
evidence and implications for clinical actions. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43, 456–
473.
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2014). lmerTest: Tests in
Linear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 2.0-20. Available from http://CRAN.R-
project.org/package=lmerTest
!
!
!
34
Laxon, V., Coltheart, V., & Keating, C. (1988). Children find friendly words friendly too:
Words with many orthographic neighbours are easier to read and spell. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 58, 103-119.
Lemhöfer, K., & Dijkstra, T. (2004). Recognizing cognates and interlexical homographs:
Effects of code similarity in language specific and generalized lexical decision. Memory and
Cognition, 32(4), 533-550.
Lemhöfer, K., Dijkstra, T., & Michel, M. C. (2004). Three languages, one ECHO:
Cognate effects in trilingual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(5), 585-
611.
Logan, J. M., & Balota, D. A. (2003). Conscious and unconscious lexical retrieval
blocking in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 18, 537–550.
Martin-Rhee M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory
control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 81–
93.
Mulder, K., Dijkstra, T., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2014). Effects of primary and
secondary morphological family size in monolingual and bilingual word processing. Journal of
Memory and Language, 72, 59-84.
Nation, I. S. P. (1990). Teaching and learning vocabulary. New York: Heinle and Heinle.
Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
!
!
!
35
Nation, I. S. P. (2006). How large a vocabulary is needed for reading and listening? The
Canadian Modern Language Review, 63, 59–82.
Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1995). Short-term memory and vocabulary learning in
polyglots. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 98-107.
Perea, M., & Estévez, A. (2008). Transposed-letter similarity effects in naming
pseudowords: Evidence from children and adults. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,
20, 33-46.
Perea, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2008). Masked associative/semantic
priming effects across languages with highly proficient bilinguals. Journal of Memory and
Language, 58, 916-930.
Perea, M., Urkia, M., Davis, C. J., Agirre, A., Laseka, E., & Carreiras, M. (2006). E-Hitz:
A word-frequency list and a program for deriving psycholinguistic statistics in an agglutinative
language (Basque). Behavior Research Methods, 38, 610-615.
Poarch, G. J. & Van Hell, J. G. (2012). Cross-language activation in children’s speech
production: Evidence from second language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 111 (3), 419-438.
Prior, A., Kroll, J. F., & MacWhinney, B. (2013). Translation ambiguity but not word
class predicts translation performance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(2), 458-474.
R CoreTeam. (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing
[Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from: http://www.R-project.org/
(Version 3.0.0).
!
!
!
36
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., &
Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia,
42(8), 1029–1040.
Schepens, J., Dijkstra, T., & Grootjen, F. (2011). Distributions of cognates in Europe as
based on Levenshtein distance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 157-166.
Schoonbaert, S., Duyck, W., Brysbaert, M., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2009). Semantic and
translation priming from a first language to a second and back: Making sense of the findings.
Memory and Cognition, 37, 569-586.
Schoonbaert, S., Holcomb, P. J., Grainger, J., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2010). Testing
asymmetries in noncognate translation priming: Evidence from RTs and ERPs.
Psychophysiology, 1-8.
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, & foreign-language learning. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 21–50.
Siyambalapitiya, S., Chenery, H. J., & Copland, D. A. (2009). Bilingualism and aging:
Reversal of the cognate advantage in older bilingual adults. Applied Psycholinguistics 30(3),
531-554.
Thierry, G. & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during
foreign language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
104(30), 12530-12535.
Vaid, J., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2002). Do orthographic cues aid language identification?
A laterality study with French-English bilinguals. Brain and Language, 82, 47-53.
!
!
!
37
Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Diependaele, K. (2009). Does
bilingualism change native-language reading? Cognate effects in sentence context. Psychological
Science, 20, 923-927.
Van Hell, J. G., & Tanner, D. (2012). Second language proficiency and cross-language
lexical activation. Language Learning, 62,148-171.
van Heuven, W. J. B., Dijkstra T., & Grainger J. (1998). Orthographic neighborhood
effects in bilingual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 458–483.
van Kesteren, R., Dijkstra, T., & de Smedt, K. (2012). Markedness effects in Norwegian-
English bilinguals: Task-dependent use of language-specific letters and bigrams. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11), 2129-2154.
Williams, J. N. (1994). The relationship between word meanings in the first and second
language: Evidence for a common, but restricted, semantic code. European Journal of Cognitive
Psychology, 6, 195–220.
Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2010). Chinese–English bilinguals reading English hear
Chinese. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(22), 7646-7651.
Zeelenberg, R., & Pecher, D. (2003). Evidence for long-term cross-language repetition
priming in conceptual implicit memory tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 80-94.
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and
skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological
Bulletin, 131, 3–29.
!
!
!
38
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2006). Becoming literate in different languages: Similar
problems, different solutions. Developmental Science, 9(5), 429–436.
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Lété, B., & Grainger, J. (2014). Orthographic and
Phonological Contributions to Reading Development: Tracking Developmental Trajectories
Using Masked Priming. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1026-1036.
!
!
!
39
Tables
Table 1. Main effects and interactions of the predictors of interest of the full model (Type-3 F-
tests using Satterthwaite denominator degrees of freedom approximations)
F-value
p-value
136.5
<.001
29.0
<.001
19.5
<.001
6.3
.01
1.0
.39
7.2
.001
.4
.51
!
!
!
40
Table 2. Coefficients of the fixed effects together with standard errors and t-values.
Estimate
Std. Error
t-value
(Intercept)
1145
25
45.92
***
Cognate rate
-418
22
-18.62
***
Age
-87
10
-9.14
***
Direction
-87
13
-6.70
***
Trial order
-.05
-.01
-11.24
***
Session order
7
20
.39
PreviousResponse
.14
.01
-56.58
***
Cognate rate||Age
41
7
5.30
***
Cognate rate||Direction
36
28
1.29
Age||Direction
9
2
3.71
***
Cognate rate||Age||Direction
-6
9
-.66
Note. All flagged t-values are significant at the p<.001 level.
The reference value for translation direction is Spanish-to-Basque.
!
!
!
41
Table 3. Main effects and interactions of the predictors of interest of the full model for the
transformed data using z-scores (Type-III F-tests using Satterthwaite denominator degrees of
freedom approximations).
F-value
p-value
1245.3
<.001
42.2
<.001
.1
.72
13.7
<.001
!
!
!
42
Table 4. Coefficients of the fixed effects together with standard errors and t-values for the model
for the transformed data using z-scores.
Estimate
Std. Error
t-value
(Intercept)
.56
.02
45.92
***
Cognate rate
-1.03
.03
-35.30
***
Age
-.02
.003
-6.50
***
Direction
.003
.02
.36
Trial order
-.001
.001
-10.50
***
Session order
.01
.006
1.53
PreviousResponse
.12
.002
48.45
***
Cognate rate||Age
.02
.005
3.70
***
Note. All flagged t-values are significant at the p<.001 level.
The reference value for translation direction is Spanish-to-Basque.
!
!
!
43
Figures
Figure 1. a) Distribution of participants per age. b) Distribution of items along the cognate
continuum.
!
!
!
44
Figure 2. Schematic representation of a trial.
!
!
!
45
Figure 3. a) Estimated RTs for the continuous predictor Age for each translation direction (the
predictor Age is represented mean-centered). b) Estimated RTs in the Spanish-to-Basque
translation direction as a function of Age and Cognate Rate. c) Estimated RTs in the Basque-to-
Spanish translation direction as a function of Age and Cognate Rate. Cognate status refers to the
orthographic overlap between translation equivalents as measured by the length-corrected
Levenshtein distance (continuous predictor where 1 corresponds to fully-overlapping words).
The continuous predictor Age is plotted (for illustration purposes only) so that each colored line
represents a unique set of age (from 8 to 15).
!
!
!
46
Figure 4. a) Estimated z-scores in the Spanish-to-Basque translation direction as a function of
Age and Cognate Rate. b) Estimated z-scores in the Basque-to-Spanish translation direction as a
function of Age and Cognate Rate.
... This cognate facilitation effect, like other forms of lexical cross-linguistic influence, is considered evidence that bilinguals have one integrated lexicon containing words from both languages (e.g., Dijkstra & van Heuven, 2018). This view of the bilingual lexicon is commonly accepted for adults, and emerging evidence suggests that the same processes occur in simultaneous bilingual children (e.g., Bosma & Nota, 2020;Duñabeitia et al., 2016;Koutamanis et al., 2023b;Poarch & van Hell, 2012;Schröter & Schroeder, 2016). ...
... In children, the organization of and processing in the bilingual lexicon has historically been studied less. More recently, however, several studies have found cognate facilitation effects in simultaneous bilingual children (Bosma et al., 2019;Bosma & Nota, 2020;Duñabeitia et al., 2016;Koutamanis et al., 2023b;Poarch & van Hell, 2012;Schröter & Schroeder, 2016). For example, Poarch and van Hell (2012) conducted picture naming tasks with German-English bilingual children and found that pictures depicting cognates were named more quickly and accurately than noncognates, in both languages. ...
... For example, Poarch and van Hell (2012) conducted picture naming tasks with German-English bilingual children and found that pictures depicting cognates were named more quickly and accurately than noncognates, in both languages. Other studies found similar cognate facilitation effects in bilingual children's comprehension, namely word recognition (Duñabeitia et al., 2016;Schröter & Schroeder, 2016), receptive vocabulary (Bosma et al., 2019), and sentence reading (Bosma & Nota, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
We examined the extent to which cognate facilitation effects occurred in simultaneous bilingual children’s production and comprehension and how these were modulated by language dominance and language context. Bilingual Dutch-German children, ranging from Dutch-dominant to German-dominant, performed picture naming and auditory lexical decision tasks in single-language and dual-language contexts. Language context was manipulated with respect to the language of communication (with the experimenter and in instructional videos) and by means of proficiency tasks. Cognate facilitation effects emerged in both production and comprehension and interacted with both dominance and context. In a single-language context, stronger cognate facilitation effects were found for picture naming in children’s less dominant language, in line with previous studies on individual differences in lexical activation. In the dual-language context, this pattern was reversed, suggesting inhibition of the dominant language at the decision level. Similar effects were observed in lexical decision. These findings provide evidence for an integrated bilingual lexicon in simultaneous bilingual children and shed more light on the complex interplay between lexicon-internal and lexicon-external factors modulating the extent of lexical cross-linguistic influence more generally.
... In other studies, the degree of orthographic overlap between translation equivalents was manipulated on a continuous scale. More similar (i.e., cognate-like) translation equivalents were processed more quickly in Frisian (but not Dutch, as discussed below) sentence reading by Dutch-Frisian bilingual children between 9 and 12 years old (Bosma & Nota, 2020) and in Spanish-to-Basque and Basque-to-Spanish translation recognition by Spanish-Basque bilingual children between 8 and 15 years old (Duñabeitia et al., 2016). In a receptive Frisian vocabulary test, the form-similarity of translation equivalents affected the performance of Dutch-Frisian bilingual children between 5 and 8 years old (Bosma et al., 2019). ...
... We assumed that models of the bilingual lexicon based on adult studies are also applicable to the lexicon of bilingual children-specifically, we assumed that activation spreading between shared sub-lexical and semantic representations in the lexicon leads to cognate facilitation in both comprehension and production. We predicted cognate facilitation effects in both lexical decision and picture naming, in accuracy as well as in reaction times, in line with earlier studies with children (Bosma et al., 2019;Bosma & Nota, 2020;Duñabeitia et al., 2016;Poarch & van Hell, 2012;Schröter & Schroeder, 2016) and adults (e.g., Costa et al., 2000;Dijkstra et al., 2010;Hoshino & Kroll, 2008;Lemhöfer et al., 2004), although the way cognate effects manifest in different tasks may differ (de Groot et al., 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
The cognate facilitation effect, a classic example of cross-language interaction in the bilingual lexicon, has mostly been studied in adults. We examined the extent to which such effects occurred in simultaneous bilingual children’s word processing, to what extent these were modulated by language dominance, and to what extent this differed between comprehension and production tasks. Simultaneous bilingual Dutch-Greek children, ranging from Dutch-dominant to Greek-dominant, performed auditory lexical decision and picture-naming tasks in an online experiment. Cognate facilitation effects emerged in both tasks but manifested themselves differently. In lexical decision, there was an interaction effect with language dominance in accuracy, while in picture naming there was a main effect in reaction times. These findings suggest that, similar to what has been found for adults, simultaneous bilingual children have an integrated lexicon, in which both languages are interactively connected. Effects may differ as a combined result of factors such as comprehension versus production and individual differences in language dominance. Importantly, despite such differences, our results show that cognate effects emerge across tasks and across a range of individual children’s language dominance, indicating that shared representations within the bilingual lexicon are accessed during both word comprehension and production.
... Once we have just checked that it doesn't, the data can be pooled so that we have just one experimental group. A good example of a study that follows a within-groups design is Duñabeitia et al. (2016). The study investigated vocabulary knowledge in bilingual children and how the overlap between languages might help them during early stages of reading development. ...
... Repeated measures design.Source: As used inDuñabeitia et al. (2016). ...
... Cognates are usually more easily processed in language production, comprehension, and translation tasks than non-cognate words, irrespective of whether they are identical or nearly identical cognates (de Groot, 1993;Dunabeitia et al., 2008, Dunabeitia et al., 2016. ...
Book
This chapter introduces the ACTFL World-Readiness Standards for Learning Languages as an organizing framework for teaching and learning EIL. These span a total of 11 standards that correspond to the 5 Cs goal areas of Communication, Cultures, Connections, Comparisons, and Communities. The chapter highlights the historical milestones of cooperation in education, explicates differences between collaborative and cooperative learning, and compares the characteristics of cooperative, competitive, and individualistic instruction. Additionally, the chapter describes the types of CL groups and clarifies disparities between these types and unstructured group work. The chapter concludes with a survey of the various formal CL approaches and structures that have been in vogue and proved effective. These include the Structural Approach, Student Team Learning, Learning Together, Complex Instruction, and Group Investigation.
... Although no prior response-based studies have investigated orthographic neighborhood effects in bilingual children, there is evidence that their visual word recognition is differentially mediated by cross-language orthographic overlap, especially when they are younger in age (for a review, see van Hell, 2020). For instance, Duñabeitia et al. (2016) tested a large sample (N = 100) of balanced Spanish-Basque bilingual children (aged 8-15) on L1 and L2 translation recognition tasks and found that the younger children's performance was more sensitive to the target words' cognate status, with greater orthographic overlap facilitating word recognition (see also Schröter and Schroeder, 2016and Duñabeitia et al., 2020, for similar findings involving similar and other aspects of cross-language orthographic processing in children during L1 and L2 lexical decision tasks). These effects have been attributed to younger children's reduced print exposure and developing language control systems, which render their lexical representations more susceptible to the effects of cross-language activation. ...
Article
Full-text available
We used eye movement measures of first-language (L1) and second-language (L2) paragraph reading to investigate how the activation of multiple lexical candidates, both within and across languages, influences visual word recognition in four different age and language groups: (1) monolingual children; (2) monolingual young adults; (3) bilingual children; and (4) bilingual young adults. More specifically, we focused on within-language and cross-language orthographic neighborhood density effects, while controlling for the potentially confounding effects of orthographic neighborhood frequency. We found facilitatory within-language orthographic neighborhood density effects (i.e., words were easier to process when they had many vs. few orthographic neighbors, evidenced by shorter fixation durations) across the L1 and L2, with larger effects in children vs. adults (especially the bilingual ones) during L1 reading. Similarly, we found facilitatory cross-language neighborhood density effects across the L1 and L2, with no modulatory influence of age or language group. Taken together, our findings suggest that word recognition benefits from the simultaneous activation of visually similar word forms during naturalistic reading, with some evidence of larger effects in children and particularly those whose words may have differentially lower baseline activation levels and/or weaker links between word-related information due to divided language exposure: bilinguals.
... When calculating the LD, the number of insertions, deletions, and alterations needed to get from the produced word-the recalled string-to the original wordthe studied string-is taken into account to calculate a standardized value. Then, these values are divided by the word length in order to normalize the value [55][56][57]. We also ran a model on the LD between the produced word and the correct response. ...
Article
Full-text available
Learning new content and vocabulary in a foreign language can be particularly difficult. Yet, there are educational programs that require people to study in a language they are not native speakers of. For this reason, it is important to understand how these learning processes work and possibly differ from native language learning, as well as to develop strategies to ease this process. The current study takes advantage of emotionality—operationally defined as positive valence and high arousal—to improve memory. In two experiments, the present paper addresses whether participants have more difficulty learning the names of objects they have never seen before in their foreign language and whether embedding them in a positive semantic context can help make learning easier. With this in mind, we had participants (with a minimum of a B2 level of English) in two experiments (43 participants in Experiment 1 and 54 in Experiment 2) read descriptions of made-up objects—either positive or neutral and either in their native or a foreign language. The effects of language varied with the difficulty of the task and measure used. In both cases, learning the words in a positive context improved learning. Importantly, the effect of emotionality was not modulated by language, suggesting that the effects of emotionality are independent of language and could potentially be a useful tool for improving foreign language vocabulary learning.
Article
Full-text available
Cognates, words that are similar in form and meaning across two languages, form compelling test cases for bilingual access and representation. Overwhelmingly, cognate pairs are subjectively selected in a categorical either- or manner, often with criteria and modality unspecified. Yet the few studies that take a more nuanced approach, selecting cognate pairs along a continuum of overlap, show interesting, albeit somewhat divergent results. This study compares three measures that quantify cognateness continuously to obtain modality-specific cognate scores for the same set of Norwegian-English word-translation pairs: (1) Researcher Intuitions – bilingual researchers rate the degree of overlap between the paired words, (2) Levenshtein Distance – an algorithm that computes overlap between word pairs, and (3) Translation Elicitation – English-speaking monolinguals guess what Norwegian words mean. Results demonstrate that cognateness can be ranked on a continuum and reveal measure and modality-specific effects. Orthographic presentation yields higher cognateness status than auditory presentation overall. Though all three measures intercorrelated moderately to highly, Researcher Intuitions demonstrated a bimodal distribution, yielding scores on the high and low end of the spectrum, consistent with the common categorical approach in the field. Levenshtein Distance would be preferred for fine-grained distinctions along the continuum of form overlap.
Chapter
A great majority of people around the world know more than one language. So, how does knowing one language affect the learning and use of additional languages? The question of cross-language influences is the focus of this book. Do bilinguals hear, understand, and produce language and meaning differently because of the languages they speak? How well can theoretical and computational models of language processing and acquisition explain and predict bilingual use patterns and acquisition trajectories? What learner, language, and context characteristics influence bilingual comprehension and production? This book provides a state-of-the-art review and critique of research into cross-language influences in phonology, lexicon, and morphosyntax, and suggests directions for future research. The interdisciplinary nature of the book bridges the gap between research on bilingualism and second language acquisition. The book will be of interest to graduate students, teachers, and researchers in linguistics and second language acquisition, cognitive psychology, and language education.
Chapter
This chapter discusses the importance of language and culture comparisons in the context of learning English as an International Language (EIL). The chapter is premised on the principle that language comparisons support learners’ understanding of their own language and culture and deepen their knowledge of the nature of language and how it works in authentic and real-world communicative situations. The chapter highlights the role of cognates and contrastive rhetoric in foreign language acquisition and endorses a comparative approach for teaching culture so as to enable learners to reflect on cultural similarities and differences. The chapter concludes by presenting sample Cooperative Learning activities for comparing native languages and cultures to the target language (TL) and its culture.KeywordsCognatesContrastive rhetoricCulture comparisonsLanguage comparison
Article
Purpose The purpose of this systematic review was to determine evidence of a cognate effect for young multilingual children (ages 3;0–8;11 [years;months], preschool to second grade) in terms of task-level and child-level factors that may influence cognate performance. Cognates are pairs of vocabulary words that share meaning with similar phonology and/or orthography in more than one language, such as rose – rosa (English–Spanish) or carrot – carotte (English–French). Despite the cognate advantage noted with older bilingual children and bilingual adults, there has been no systematic examination of the cognate research in young multilingual children. Method We conducted searches of multiple electronic databases and hand-searched article bibliographies for studies that examined young multilingual children's performance with cognates based on study inclusion criteria aligned to the research questions. Results The review yielded 16 articles. The majority of the studies (12/16, 75%) demonstrated a positive cognate effect for young multilingual children (measured in higher accuracy, faster reaction times, and doublet translation equivalents on cognates as compared to noncognates). However, not all bilingual children demonstrated a cognate effect. Both task-level factors (cognate definition, type of cognate task, word characteristics) and child-level factors (level of bilingualism, age) appear to influence young bilingual children's performance on cognates. Conclusions Contrary to early 1990s research, current researchers suggest that even young multilingual children may demonstrate sensitivity to cognate vocabulary words. Given the limits in study quality, more high-quality research is needed, particularly to address test validity in cognate assessments, to develop appropriate cognate definitions for children, and to refine word-level features. Only one study included a brief instruction prior to assessment, warranting cognate treatment studies as an area of future need. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.12753179
Technical Report
Full-text available
Description Fit linear and generalized linear mixed-effects models. The models and their components are represented using S4 classes and methods. The core computational algorithms are implemented using the 'Eigen' C++ library for numerical linear algebra and 'RcppEigen' ``glue''.
Book
Cambridge Core - ELT Applied Linguistics - Learning Vocabulary in Another Language - by I. S. P. Nation
Article
Linear mixed-effects models (LMEMs) have become increasingly prominent in psycholin-guistics and related areas. However, many researchers do not seem to appreciate how random effects structures affect the generalizability of an analysis. Here, we argue that researchers using LMEMs for confirmatory hypothesis testing should minimally adhere to the standards that have been in place for many decades. Through theoretical arguments and Monte Carlo simulation, we show that LMEMs generalize best when they include the maximal random effects structure justified by the design. The generalization performance of LMEMs including data-driven random effects structures strongly depends upon modeling criteria and sample size, yielding reasonable results on moderately-sized samples when conservative criteria are used, but with little or no power advantage over maximal models. Finally, random-intercepts-only LMEMs used on within-subjects and/or within-items data from populations where subjects and/or items vary in their sensitivity to experimental manipulations always generalize worse than separate F 1 and F 2 tests, and in many cases, even worse than F 1 alone. Maximal LMEMs should be the 'gold standard' for confirmatory hypothesis testing in psycholinguistics and beyond.
Article
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Article
This paper argues that working memory is heavily involved in language acquisition as (a) a major part of language learning is the learning of sequences, (b) working memory allows short-term maintenance of sequence information, and (c) short-term rehearsal of sequences promotes the consolidation of long-term memories of language sequences. It first reviews evidence supporting this position. Next it presents an experiment that demonstrates that subjects encouraged to rehearse foreign language (FL) utterances are better than both silent controls and subjects who are prevented from rehearsal by articulatory suppression at (a) learning to comprehend and translate FL words and phrases, (b) explicit metalinguistic knowledge of the detailed content of grammatical regularities, (c) acquisition of the FL forms of words and phrases, (d) accuracy in FL pronunciation, and (e) some aspects of productive (but not receptive) grammatical fluency and accuracy. Finally, it describes possible mechanisms underlying these effects.