ArticlePDF Available

Mind Full of Ideas: A Meta-Analysis of the Mindfulness–Creativity Link

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Mindfulness improves people’s functioning in many areas, but its relationship with creativity is equivocal. To assess the link between mindfulness and creativity, this paper presents a multilevel meta-analysis of 89 correlations obtained from 20 samples published between 1977 and 2015 and demonstrates a statistically significant correlation of medium size (r = .22) between these two constructs. This effect was moderated by the type of mindfulness, being significantly lower in case of the awareness aspect, but not in open-monitoring aspect of mindfulness . We discuss theoretical and practical consequences of these findings.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Mind full of ideas: A meta-analysis of the mindfulnesscreativity link
Izabela Lebuda
a,
, Darya L. Zabelina
b
,MaciejKarwowski
a
a
Department of Educational Sciences, The Maria Grzegorzewska University, 40 Szczesliwicka St., 02-353 Warsaw, Poland
b
Medical Social Science, Northwestern University, 633 N St Clair Street, Chicago, IL 60611, United States
abstractarticle info
Article history:
Received 29 March 2015
Received in revised form 22 September 2015
Accepted 23 September 2015
Available online xxxx
Keywords:
Creativity
Meditation
Meta-analysis
Mindfulness
Mindfulness improves people's functioning in many areas, but its relationship with creativity is equivocal. To
assess the link between mindfulness and creativity, we present a multilevel meta-analysis of 89 correlations ob-
tained from 20samples in studies published between 1977 and 2015and demonstrate a statistically signicant,
but relatively weak correlation (r= .22) between these two constructs. This effect was moderated by thetype of
mindfulness, being signicantly lower in case of the awareness aspect of mindfulness, than in the case of the
open-monitoring aspect. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications of these ndings.
© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Mindfulness is a state of nonjudgmental, sustained, and alert aware-
ness resulting from living in the moment (Brown & Ryan, 2003), which
improves people's cognitive, emotional, and interpersonal functioning
(Carson & Langer, 2006; Sedlmeier et al., 2012) and positively affects
the efcacy of stress regulation. In previous studies authors have
found that mindfulness also improves the ability to concentrate
(Sedlmeier et al., 2012), decreases the fear of being judged,as well as re-
duces aversive self-conscious experience (Brown, Ryan, & Creswell,
2007), and helps to deal with thoughts and feelings (Shapiro, Carlson,
Astin, & Freedman, 2006). The enhancement of mindfulness through
practicing meditation (Lutz, Dunne, & Davidson, 2007)aswellashigh
level of self-reported mindfulness have previously been linked to pro-
cesses important to creativity (Ball, 1980; Colzato, Ozturk, & Hommel,
2012). Creativity, understood as the ability to produce ideas that are
both novel and appropriate (Amabile, 1996; Sternberg & Lubart,
1996), is typically measured by the divergent thinking tests, during
which participantsare asked to name as manyuses for a common object
(e.g., brick) as possible within a limited amount of time (Guilford,
1967). Responses are scored in terms of uency (number of ideas),
exibility (number of categories), originality (statistical novelty of
responses), and elaboration (level of details). Other measures of creativity
include self-report scales concerning creative behavior, personality, and
activities (Baas, De Dreu, & Nijstad, 2008; Simonton, 2012), or creative
achievement (Carson, Peterson, & Higgins, 2005). Less commonly, crea-
tivity researchers use tasks with a single correct answer, such as insight
problems for example, remote association tests (RAT; Mednick &
Mednick, 1967).
A number of abilities which are associated with trait mindfulness, or
facilitated by mindfulness training are also linked with creativity (De
Dreu, Nijstad, Baas, Wolsink, & Roskes, 2012). For instance, mindfulness
is associated with the increased ability to switch perspectives (Carson &
Langer, 2006; Feldman, Hayes, Kumar, Greeson, & Laurenceau, 2007),
while mindfulness training leads to the improvement of working mem-
ory (Chiesa, Calati, & Serretti, 2011), as well as increases the ability to re-
spond in a non-habitualfashion (Moore & Malinowski, 2009). Practicing
mindfulness also reduces the fear of judgment (Carson & Langer, 2006),
which is conducive to creativity (Baas et al., 2008; Nijstad, De Dreu,
Rietzschel, & Baas, 2010). Consequently, mindfulness may be both
directly and indirectly related to creative thinking (Davis, 2009; De
Dreu, Baas, & Nijstad, 2008), and to creative achievement (Langer,
2014).
A wide body of research has indeed shown that meditation training
enhances creative thinking and creative performance as well as im-
proves the ability to solve insight problems (Colzato et al., 2012; Ding,
Tang, Deng, Tang, & Posner, 2015; Ding, Tang, Tang, & Posner, 2014;
Ostan & Kassman, 2012; Ren et al., 2011) and facilitates creative elab-
oration (Zabelina, Robinson, Ostan, & Council, 2011). Experienced
meditators also outperform others in verbal uency and are better at
nding novel solutions to a given problem (Grenberg, Reiner, &
Meiran, 2012). Importantly, meditation has a positive effect on creativ-
ity regardless of the length of practice (Jedrczak, Beresford, & Clements,
1985), which means that even short meditation can effectively stimu-
late creative abilities (Ding et al., 2014).
However, although the ndings of several studies support the posi-
tive link between mindfulness and creativity, some inconsistencies
exist. For example, while meditation was clearly demonstrated to
improve verbal uency, exibility, and originality (Justo, 2009), longitu-
dinal examination of groups practicing transcendental meditation for
Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2015) xxxxxx
Corresponding author.
E-mail address: izalebuda@gmail.com (I. Lebuda).
PAID-07063; No of Pages 5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.040
0191-8869/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Differences
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
Please cite this article as: Lebuda, I., et al., Mind full of ideas: A meta-analysis of the mindfulnesscreativity link, Personality and Individual
Differences (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.040
ve months did not show any signicant gains in verbal creativity, but
did reveal a signicant improvement in gural exibility and originality
(Travis, 1979). Such inconsistencies may be attributed to a number of
moderators, among them the type of meditation (Colzato, Szapora,
Lippelt, & Hommel, 2014), and the multidimensional character of mind-
fulness (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006; Baas,
Nevicka, & Ten Velden, 2014). Mindfulness is a complex phenomenon,
composed of a set of different skills: the ability to pay attention to
various stimuli (observation), the ability to focus with full awareness
(acting with awareness), the ability to give a nonevaluative verbal de-
scription of the observed phenomena (description), and the ability to
avoid immediate evaluation (Baer et al., 2006). These skills may be dif-
ferentially related to creativity for example, while open-monitoring
meditation (so-called targeting observation) may tend to increase crea-
tive thinking, focused-attention meditation (aimed at acting with
awareness) may be either unrelated to creativity (Colzato et al., 2012),
or may even impede performance on creativity tasks (Baas et al.,
2014; Zedelius & Schooler, 2015). Additionally, phenomena contrary
to mindfulness, such as disinhibition and mind-wandering, predict cre-
ative thinking and creative achievement (Baird et al., 2012; Carson,
Peterson, & Higgins, 2003; Eysenck, 1995; Schooler, Reichle, &
Halpern, 2004; Zabelina, O'Leary, Pornpattananangkul, Nusslock, &
Beeman, 2015; Zedelius & Schooler, 2015). Thus it is possible that
the facets of mindfulness may moderate the mindfulnesscreativity
association.
1
Despite inconsistencies, both the general pattern of empirical results
as well as theoretical arguments (Langer, 2014) provide a rationale to
hypothesize a positive association between mindfulness and creativity.
Although empirical studies do not always conrm this link
(e.g., Domino, 1977; O'Haire & Marcia, 1980), the higher statistical
power of meta-analysis enables a more robust estimation of this rela-
tionship. It also allows us to explore the role of potential moderators.
The scarcity of published studies makes it impossible to investigate
all of the theoretically relevant moderators. However, it is possible to
examine the role of study design (correlational versus experimental
studies showing the inuence of meditation on creativity), the
creativity aspects measured (insight problem solving versus divergent
thinking), as well as theaspects of mindfulness measured. Both existing
theories (Fink, Slamar-Halbedl, Unterrainer, & Weiss, 2012)andprevi-
ous research (Zedelius & Schooler, 2015) lead to the expectation
that the attention aspect of the mindfulness measured, for example,
by the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan,
2003)will be signicantly less strongly (or even negatively see
Baas et al., 2014) related to creativity than other aspects of mindfulness.
1. Method
1.1. The selection of studies
We performed a literature search in the Google Scholar, PsycInfo,
Ebsco, and Scopus databases as well as at ResearchGate.net and
Academia.edu. The rst stage involved a search for articles by means
of the following keywords: creativity and mindfulness, creativity and
meditation, creative problem solving and mindfulness, and creative
problem solving and meditation. In the second stage, we scanned the
databases for all the authors of the publications found. In the last
stage, the query involved an analysis of the references from each of
the papers. The rst author found and analyzed 33 articles. The third au-
thor conducted an independent review of all the identied articles. This
meta-analysis includes papers published in peer-reviewed journals and
based on quantitative research; we excluded theoretical or review
papers (e.g., Horan, 2009; Mooneyham & Schooler, 2013), and those
in which only one of the main variables (creativity or mindfulness)
was directly measured (e.g., Langer, Russell, & Eisenkraft, 2009). We
also excluded publications that concerned constructs closely related
to, but not identical with creativity, such as openness to experience or
cognitive exibility (e.g., Moore & Malinowski, 2009).
We included articles devoted to both trait and state mindfulness
(Bishop et al., 2004). In the case of experimental studies, we did not
exclude any of the types of meditation (e.g., focused attention or
open-monitoring). One study that lacked a control group or baseline
level of creativity was excluded from the analysis (Colzato et al.,
2014). This procedure resulted in 20 independent samples and 89
correlations obtained in a total sample of 1549 participants.
1.2. Data analysis
We applied three-level meta-analysis (Cheung, 2014a; Cheung,
2014b) in the metaSEM package (Cheung, 2014a) for the Renvironment
(RDevelopment Core Team, 2013). Level 1 describes the participants in
studies, Level 2 describes effects within studies, and Level 3 describes
the studies themselves. Three-level meta-analysis allows us to give un-
biased estimates of standard errors, Level 2 (within-study) variance,
and Level 3 (between-study) variance. Three-level meta-analysis has
an advantage over traditional random-effect meta-analysis (which
should be considered a two-level model) because averaging the effects,
which is necessary in random-effects models, reduces the statistical
power of the analysis.
We converted all the obtained effects (i.e., mean differences
between experimental and control groups in experimental designs) to
Pearson's r, applying widely used formulas (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). All
correlations were corrected for unreliability: they were divided by the
square root of the reliabilities of the variables (Hunter & Schmidt,
1990). When reliability estimates were not provided, we used average re-
liabilities. For comparison purposes, Table 1 contains both reliability-
corrected and uncorrected correlations. All studies and correlations are
included in the online supplementary material.
2. Results
We processed data in three-steps. First, we estimated overall effect
sizes for the relationship between mindfulness and creativity. Second,
we tted three three-level models to assess the role of potential moder-
ators. Finally, we performed an analysis of publication bias to examine
whether selective reporting may have inuenced the results.
2.1. The overall relationship
The effect sizes obtained using three-level meta-analysis are
presented in Table 1.
The correlation between mindfulness and creativity was estimated
at r=.22(r= .18 without correction for attenuation). This correlation
is signicant but heterogeneous. We found more between-study than
within-study variability, which means moderators are more likely to
exist between than within studies. Despite this heterogeneity, our
main hypothesis nds support creativity does correlate with mindful-
ness signicantly, with a small-to-mediumeffect size (Cohen, 1992;
Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
2.2. Moderator analysis
In the rst model testing the role of moderators, we included:
(1) study design, coded: 0 = correlational,1=experimental; (2) creativity
measurement, coded: 0 = self-reported,1=test; (3) the aspect of
creativity, coded: 0 = achievement,1=potential, and (4) gender (the
percentage of females). This model was not characterized by a signi-
cantly improved t compared to the baseline model, 2LL(df =6)=
5.93, Δ-2LL(Δdf =3) = 6.57, p= .09, and none of the moderators
were signicant, pN.05. Consequently, the effect was stable across
1
We are grateful to the anonymous reviewer for bringing this to our attention.
2I. Lebuda et al. / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2015) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: Lebuda, I., et al., Mind full of ideas: A meta-analysis of the mindfulnesscreativity link, Personality and Individual
Differences (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.040
correlational and experimental studies as well as studies with creativity
assessed via test or self-reported measures.
In the second step, we tested the role of the measure of mindfulness.
The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS), which was used
most often across the studies (35 times) served as a reference category,
while the MAAS, Integrative Mind-Body Training (IBMT), the Five Facet
Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ), OM Meditation, Transcendental
Meditation, and othertypes of meditation were introduced as
dummy variables. This model was better tted than the baseline,
2LL(df =9)=1.125, Δ-2LL(Δdf = 6) = 13.63, p= .03, and, consis-
tently with our predictions, MAAS scores resulted in signicantly weak-
er relationships with creativity than KIMS scores (B=0.31,SE =0.14,
p= .03).
In the third step, we reduced all the analyzed effects to those focus-
ing on creative abilities (68 correlations from 18 studies, with the aver-
age effect size of r=.20,SE =0.07,p= .004) and examined whether
the type of creative abilities moderated the obtained effects. Insight
problem solving skills served as a reference category, while other as-
pects of creativity were introduced as dummies. The model was tted
signicantly better than the baseline model, 2LL(df =8)=0.29,
Δ-2LL(Δdf =5)=25.01,pb.001, with all predictors except composite
divergent thinking being statistically signicant (Table 2).
We applied our nal model to simultaneously test the role of crea-
tive thinking skills (0 = divergent thinking,1=insight), the type of
mindfulness (0 = other,1=MAAS), and the interaction of the two.
This model was tted better than the baseline, 2LL(df =6
)=15.26,
Δ-2LL(Δdf = 3) = 9.46, p=.02(Table 3). MAAS remained the only
predictor of the mindfulnesscreativity relationship; neither insight
nor the Insight ×MAAS interaction was signicant.
2.3. Publication bias
We used two methods to assess the risk of publication bias. The rst
one was the analysis of the funnel plot (Duval & Tweedie, 2000). The
second one involved the application of p-curve analysis (Simonsohn,
Nelson & Simmons, 2014). The funnel plotwas symmetric (Fig. 1), with-
out a pattern showing that smaller studies yielded higher effect sizes.
The trim-and-ll method (Duval & Tweedie, 2000) suggests adding
four more studies, which would reduce the observed effect to r=.135
(95% CI: .03, .24). However, in the case of high heterogeneity, this
method is considered too restrictive (Peters, Sutton, Jones, Abrams, &
Rushton, 2007).
To examine the publication bias using a more recent technique, we
performed a p-curve analysis (Simonsohn, Nelson & Simmons, 2014;
http://www.p-curve.com/). The p-curve analysis focuses only on
statistically signicant effects and serves to check whether just signi-
cant effects(i.e., slightly lower than p= .05, or between p= .04 and
p= .05) are not overrepresented in the analyzed studies. Such overrep-
resentation may stem from publication bias, but also from cherry-
picking,”“p-hacking,or other questionable research practices
(Simonsohn, Simmons & Nelson, 2014). The p-curve analysis did not
provide any evidence of the le-drawer effect a majority of studies
provided signicant results, and there was no overrepresentation of
just-signicantones (Fig. 2).
The continuous test for a right-skewed curve showing that
studies contain evidential value was statistically signicant
(z=5.31, pb.001), while testing for the left-skewed studies
(i.e., those that exhibit evidence of p-hacking) did not bring signicant
results (pN.999).
3. Discussion
Although famous creators are sometimes absentminded, creativity
seems to require mindfulness. Indeed, this meta-analysis showed that
creativity and mindfulness are signicantly related, with a small-to-
mediumeffect size (Cohen, 1992). Although this effect was not moder-
ated by the design of the studies, it tended to be stronger whencreativ-
ity measurement had the form of insight tasks rather than divergent
thinking tasks. When the aspects of creative thinking skills were
regressed on the general effect together with the mindfulness type,
the only statistically signicant relationship was the one with the
awareness aspect of the mindfulness (measured by the MAAS scale),
generating lower effect size. We found no serious evidence of publica-
tion bias or p-hacking, which allows us to conclude that this estimation
is both accurate and robust.
From the theoretical standpoint, the relationship we obtained ts
well into the postulated role of mindful mind in creative thinking and
behavior (Langer, 2014). However, the moderators that we were able
to include in our analyses also shed light on the theoretically important
questionsabout the nature of this relationship. First, we were unable to
nd any differences between correlational and experimental studies
in both types of studies the effect size of the association was the same.
We perceive this null nding as important, as it shows not only that cre-
ativity and mindfulness correlate with each other, but also, more impor-
tantly, that developing mindfulness during meditation increases
Table 1
Overall effect size obtained using three-level meta-analysis.
Model No. of studies No. of effects NEffect size (r) 95% CI p
Unreliability-corrected 20 89 1549 .220 .095, .344 b.001
Unreliability-uncorrected 20 89 1549 .183 .078, .289 b.001
Model summary
Level-2 s
2
= .029 (SE = .007), pb.001, I
2
= .30
Level-3 s
2
= .061 (SE = .026), p= .02, I
2
= .63
Q(df = 88) = 1027.37, pb.001, 2LL(df = 3) = 12.50
Table 2
Moderator analysis multilevel model est imating the effects of different aspects of
creative abilities (insight= reference category).
Predictor Estimate SE 95% CI p
Intercept 0.45 0.11 0.24, 0.66 b.001
Fluency 0.52 0.12 0.76, 0.29 b.001
Flexibility 0.34 0.13 0.59, 0.09 .007
Originality 0.47 0.12 0.70, 0.24 b.001
Elaboration 0.57 0.16 0.88, 0.26 .001
Composite divergent thinking 0.22 0.16 0.54, 0.09 .17
Within-study variance 0.02 0.006 0.005, 0.03 .005
Between-study variance 0.13 0.06 0.02, 0.25 .02
Table 3
Moderator analysis multilevel model estimating the effects of the type of creative
abilities and the type of mindfulness.
Predictor Estimate SE 95% CI p
Intercept 0.31 0.07 0.18, 0.44 b.001
Insight (other =0) 0.005 0.16 0.31, 0.32 .98
MAAS (other =0)0.73 0.23 1.17, 0.28 .002
Insight ×MAAS 0.47 0.29 0.11, 1.04 .11
Within-study variance 0.04 0.01 0.02, 0.06 b.001
Between-study variance 0.03 0.02 0.004, 0.07 .08
Note. Thismodel is based on 68 correlations from 18 studiesdealing with creativeabilities.
3I. Lebuda et al. / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2015) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: Lebuda, I., et al., Mind full of ideas: A meta-analysis of the mindfulnesscreativity link, Personality and Individual
Differences (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.040
creativity as well. Therefore, there are good reasons to believe that there
is not only correlation, but also causation in the mindfulnesscreativity
link. Putting these ndings together i.e., showing a correlational as
well as causal link between mindfulness and creativity understood as
potential (comprising cognitive and self-concept aspects) may have
important consequences for the educational psychology of creativity
and for the practice of creative education. It was demonstrated
previously that the kind of creativity training that promotes not only
awareness and imagination but also mindfulness-related skills is
effective (Karwowski & Soszyński, 2008). It is very likely that such
mindfulness-based interventions, especially ones based on open-
monitoring meditation, may be benecial for creative abilities as well
as for creative self-concept.
The relationship between mindfulness and creativity was signi-
cantly lower when research concerned the awareness aspects of mind-
fulness. The awareness aspect of mindfulness can be contrasted with
disinhibition and mind-wandering, which have been previously report-
ed to be linked with creativity (Schooler et al., 2004). Previous studies
reported that the awareness of irrelevant environmental clues, as well
as shifting attention from one object to another can lead to insight and
play an important role in the creative process (Baird et al., 2012;
Carson et al., 2003). Similarly, the inability to effectively lter irrelevant
sensory information may lead to creativity in real world settings
(Zabelina et al., 2015). At least two previous studies (included in this
meta-analysis) showed that high awareness is negatively associated
with creativity (Baas et al., 2014; Zedelius & Schooler, 2015). Indeed,
the effect of the relationship between attention-based mindset and
creativity was signicantly weaker than in the case of other aspects of
mindfulness. It is likely that different aspects of mindfulness, such as
open-monitoring abilities and awareness, play a role at different stages
of the creative process. It is worth to explore this issue in future
research.
3.1. Limitations and future research
It is necessary to consider a number of limitations while interpreting
the results of this meta-analysis. First, due to the small number of stud-
ies, it was impossible to analyze all potential moderators of this relation-
ship. Consequently, future research on the creativitymindfulness link
should differentiate between various aspects, levels, and forms of
creativity as related to mindfulness. It is especially important to
examine whether the observed relationship between mindfulness and
self-reported creativity stems to a greater extent from creative self-
concept variables (Karwowski & Lebuda, in press)orfrompastcreative
activity or achievement. Although this meta-analysis demonstrates the
link between mindfulness and creativity mainly at the little-c creativity
level (Kaufman & Beghetto, 2009), there are arguments to believe that
mindfulness may also play a role in the case of professional creators in
different domains (Langer et al., 2009). Similarly, it is important to
explain how mindfulness works in general and how its different types
work at different stages of the creative process, when problems are
dened and when solutions are generated, elaborated, and assessed.
Acknowledgements
This article was written thanks to the funding obtained in the
Mobility Plus program (1152/1/MOB/2014/0) from the Ministry of
Science and Higher Education, Poland.
Maciej Karwowski was supported by the Iuventus Plus program of
the Ministry of Science and Higher Education.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at http://dx.
doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.040.
References
2
Amabile, T.M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to the social psychology of creativity.
Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Baas, M., De Dreu, C.K.W., & Nijstad, B.A. (2008). A meta-an alysis of 25 years of research
on mood and creativity: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological
Bulletin,134,739756.
*Baas, M., Nevicka, B., & Ten Velden, F.S. (2014). Specic mindfulness skills differentially
predict creative performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,9,10921106.
Baer, R.A., Smith, G.T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report as-
sessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment,13,2745.
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M.D., Kam, J.W.Y., Franklin, M.S., & Schooler, J.W. (2012).
Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological
Science,23, 11171122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612446024.
*Ball, O.E. (1980). The effect of TMand the TM-Sidhi program onverbal and gural creat ivity
(TTCT), auditory creativity (S and I), and hemispheric dominance (SOLAT). Atlanta, GA:
University of Georgia.
Bishop, M.A., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N.D., Carmody, J., ... Devins, G.
(2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational denition. Clinical Psychology: Science
and Practice,11,230241.
Brown, K.W., & Ryan, R.M. (2003). The benets of being present: Mindfulness and its role
in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,84,822848.
Brown, K.W., Ryan, R.M., & Creswell, J.D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations
and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry,18,211237.
Carson, S.H., & Langer, E.J. (2006). Mindfulness and self-acceptance. Journal of Rational-
Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy,24,2943.
Carson, S.H., Peterson,J.B., & Higgins, D.M. (2005). Reliability, validity, andfactor structure
of the Creative Achie vement Questi onnaire. Creativity Research Journal,17,3
750.
Carson, S., Peterson, J.B., & Higgins, D. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated
with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individua ls. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,85,499506.
Fig. 1. Publication bias analysis afunnelplot.
Fig. 2. P-curve analysis results.
2
Asterisk indicates studies included in the meta-analysis.
4I. Lebuda et al. / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2015) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: Lebuda, I., et al., Mind full of ideas: A meta-analysis of the mindfulnesscreativity link, Personality and Individual
Differences (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.040
Cheung, M.W. -L. (201 4a). Modeling dependent effect sizes with three-level meta-
analyses: A structural equation mode ling approach. Psychological Methods,2,
211229.
Cheung, M.W. -L. (2014b). MetaSEM: An R package for meta-analysis using structural
equation modeling. Frontiers in Psychology,5,1521.
Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training improve cognitive
abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological ndings. Clinical Psychology
Review,31,449464.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin,112,155159.
*Colzato, L.S., Ozturk, A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Meditate to create: The impact of focused
attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking.
Frontiers in Psychology,3, 116.
Colzato, L.S., Szapora, A., Lippelt, D., & Hommel, B. (2014). Prior meditation practice mod-
ulates performance and strategy use in convergent- and divergent-thinking problem.
Mindfulness. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12671-014-0352-9 (online publication ahead
of print).
Davis, M.A. (2009). Understanding the relationship between mood and creati vity: A
meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,108,2538.
De Dreu, C.K.W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B.A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation in the
moodcreativity link: Towards a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,94,739756.
De Dreu, C.K.W., Nijstad, B.A., Baas,M., Wolsink, I., & Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory
benets creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through main-
tained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,38,656669.
Development Core Team, R. (2013). R: A language and environm ent for statistical
computing. Vienna, Austria: The R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
*Ding, X., Tang, Y., Deng, Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M.I. (2015). Mood and personality predict
improvement in creativity due to medita tion training. Learning and Individual
Differences,37,217221.
*Ding, X., Tang, Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M.I. (2014). Improving creativity performance by
short-term meditation. Behavioral and Brain Functions,10. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1186/1744.9081.10.9 online publication ahead of print.
*Domino, G. (1977). Transcendental meditation and creativity: An empirical investiga-
tion. Journal of Applied Psychology,62,358362.
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and ll: A simple funnel-plot-based method of test-
ing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics,56,455463.
Eysenck, H.J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J.P. (2007). Mindfulness and
emotion regulation: The development and initial validation of the Cognitive and
Affective Mindfulness ScaleRevised (CA MSR). Journal of Psychopath ology and
Behavioral Assessment,29,177190.
Fink, A., Slamar-Halbedl, M., Unterrainer, H.F., & Weiss, E.M. (2012). Creativity: Genius,
madness, or a combination of both? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts,
6,1118.
*Grenberg, J., Reiner, K., & Meiran, N. (2012). Mind the trap: Mindfulness practice re-
duces cognitive rigidity. PLoS ONE,7. http://dx. doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
0036206 (e36206).
Guilford, J.P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Horan, R. (2009). The neuropsychological connection between creativity and meditation.
Creativity Research Journal,21,199222.
Hunter, J.E., & Schmidt, F.L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in
research nding (1st ed.). Newbury: Sage.
*Jedrczak, E., Beresford, M., & Clements, G. (1985). The TM-Sidhi program, pure
consciousness, creativity and intelligence. The Journal of Creative Behavior,19,
270275.
*Justo, F.C. (2009). Efectos de un programa de meditación sobre los niveles de creatividad
verbal sobre un grupo de alumnos/as de bachillerato [Effects of a meditation program
on verbal creative levels in a group of students in late secondary education]. Suma
Psicológica,16,113120.
Karwowski, M., & Lebuda, I.(2015). The big ve, the huge two, and creative self-beliefs: A
meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts (in press).
Karwowski, M., & Soszyński, M. (2008). How to develop creative imagination? Thinking
Skills and Creativity,3,163171.
Kaufman, J.C., & Be ghetto, R.A. (2009). Be yond big and little: The fou r c model of
creativity. Review of General Psychology,13,112.
Langer, E.J. (2014). Mindfulness. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo Press.
Langer, E.J., Russell, T., & Eisenkraft, N. (2009). Orchestral performance and the footprint
of mindfulness. Psychology of Music,37,125136.
Lipsey, M.W., & Wilson, D.B. (2001). Pract ical meta-analysis (Applied Social Research
Methods Series. Vol. 49, . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lutz, A., Dunne, J.D., & Davidson, R.J. (2007). Me ditation and the neuros cience of
consciousness: An introduction. In P. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.),
The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 499551). New York, US: Cambridge
University Press.
Mednick, S.A., & Mednick, M.T. (1967). Examiner's manual: Remote Associates Test. Boston:
Houghton Mifin.
Mooneyham, B.W., & Schooler, J.W. (2013). The costs and benets of mind-wandering: A
review. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,7,1118.
Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness, and cognitive exibility.
Consciousness and Cognition,18,176186.
Nijstad, B.A., De Dreu, C.K.W., Rietzschel, E.F., & Baas, M. (2010). Towards a dual pathway
to creativity model: Crea tive ideation as a function of exibility and persistence.
European Review of Social Psychology,21,3477.
*O'Haire, T.D., & Marcia, J.E. (1980). Some personality character istics associated with
Ananda Marga meditators: A pilot study. Perceptual and Motor Skills,51,447452.
*Ostan, B.D., & Kassman, K.T. (2012). Stepping out of history: Mindfulness improves
insight problem solving. Consciousness and Cognition,21,10311036.
Peters, J.L., Sutton, A.J., Jones, D.R., Abrams, K.R., & Rushton, L. (2007). Performance of the
trim and ll method in the presenceof publication bias and between-study heteroge-
neity. Statistics in Medicine,26, 45444562.
*Ren, J., Huang, Z.H., Luo, J., Wei, G.X.,Ying, X.P., Ding, Z., ... Luo, F. (2011). Meditation pro-
motes insightful problem-solving by keeping people in a mindful and alert conscious
state. Science China. Life Sciences,54,961965.
Schooler, J.W., Reichle, E.D., & Halpern, D.V. (2004). Zoning out while reading: Evidence
for dissociations between experience and metaconsciousness. In D.T. Levin (Ed.),
Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adult s and children (pp. 203226).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., & Kunze, S.
(2012). The psychological effects of med itation: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin,138,11391171.
Shapiro, S.L., Carlson, L.E., Astin, J.A., & Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness.
Journal of Clinical Psychology,62,373386.
Simonsohn, U., Nelson, L.D., & Simmons, J.P. (2014). P-curve: A key to the le drawer.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,143,534547.
Simonsohn, U., Simmons, J.P., & Nelson, L.D.(2014). P-curve and effect size: Correcting for
publication bias using only signicant results. Perspectives on Psychological Science,9,
666681.
Simonton, D.K. (2012). Quantifying creativity: Can measures span the spectrum?
Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience,14,100104.
Sternberg, R.J., & Lubart, T.I. (1996) . Investing in creativity. American Psychologist,51,
677688.
*Travis, F. (1979). The transcendental meditation technique and creativity: A longitudinal
study of Cornell University undergraduates. The Journal of Creative Behavior,13,
169180.
Zabelina,D.L., O'Leary, D.,Pornpattananangkul, N., Nusslock, R., & Beeman, M. (2015). Cre-
ativity and sensory gating indexed by the P50: Selective versus leaky sensory gating
in divergent thinkers and creative achievers. Neuropsychologia,69,7784.
*Zabelina, D.L., Robinson, M.D., Ostan,B.O., & Council, J.C. (2011). Manipulating mindful-
ness benets creative elaboration at high levels of neuroticism. Empirical Studies of
the Arts,29,243255.
*Zedelius, C.M ., & Schooler, J.W. (2015). Mind wandering Ahasversus mindful
reasoning: Alternative routes to creative solutions. Frontiers in Psychology,6, 834.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00834.
5I. Lebuda et al. / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2015) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: Lebuda, I., et al., Mind full of ideas: A meta-analysis of the mindfulnesscreativity link, Personality and Individual
Differences (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.040
... Mindfulness is typically defined in the literature as focusing in a non-judgmental (i.e., neutral) way on what is happening in the present moment and has been seen both as a unidimensional (Brown & Ryan, 2003) and as a multi-dimensional construct (Baer et al., 2004). Because of the known effects of the "observation" sub-component of mindfulness on performance and creativity (Lebuda et al., 2016;Leung et al., 2016), our study operationalizes mindfulness as "observing, noticing, or attending to a variety of stimuli, including internal phenomena, such as bodily sensations, cognitions, and emotions, and external phenomena, such as sounds and smells" (Baer et al., 2004;p. 193). ...
... Instead, mindful individuals live on the "here and now", attending to what there is, rather than what will be. Even though proactivity and mindfulness represent quite different ways in which people regulate their actions, they both have been found to benefit work behavior (Dane, 2011;Joo & Bennett, 2018;Lebuda et al., 2016), such as task performance (i.e., meeting expectations and requirements of one's work role; Griffin et al., 2007) or creativity (i.e., generating and implementing novel and useful work-related ideas; Miron et al., 2004). ...
... For example, the observation facet of mindfulness (e.g., "deliberately noticing sensations" or "noticing visual elements"; Baer et al., 2004;p. 196) has been found to be a better predictor of both task performance (Leung et al., 2016) and creativity (Lebuda et al., 2016). Unlike other employee outcomes, such as well-being, task performance clearly requires employees to "do something" (e.g., complete a certain task) and the same holds for creativity (e.g., think of solutions to a certain problem). ...
Article
Full-text available
In line with control theory and related self-regulation perspectives as well as the dual pathway to creativity model, proactivity and mindfulness are both expected to enhance employee task performance and creativity. However, whether proactivity and mindfulness strengthen or undermine each other’s effects remains an open question. To address this question, we conducted a cross-sectional (Study 1; N = 255 employees) and a 4-week diary study (Study 2; N = 143 employees), and we adopted a latent profile analysis (LPA) approach with task performance and creativity as focal outcomes. Even though several employee profiles emerged from the datasets, two of them were replicated across both studies namely, the “proactive employees” (reporting high proactivity and low mindfulness) and the “proactive mindful employees” (reporting high proactivity and high mindfulness). Findings of both studies showed that the “proactive mindful employees” report the highest levels of creativity. The analyses of Study 2 furthermore revealed that both profiles display superior task performance compared to other profiles. The findings are discussed in light of the current debate in the literature, and recommendations for future research as well as for individual employees are provided (e.g., formulating mindful goals in a proactive way or proactive goals in a mindful way).
... According to Illeris' (2007) full-perspective learning theory, learning includes an internal psychological acquisition process and an external interaction process with the social environment. On the one hand, depending on spiritual reflection and meditation, individual concentration and cognitive flexibility are effectively improved, thus promoting the generation of new ideas or new products (Lebuda, Zabelina, and Karwowski 2016;Lusnig, Radach, and Hofmann 2022). On the other hand, teachers can communicate with authority figures to gain inspiration, and then produce novel teaching design concepts (Aggarwal, Hwang, and Tan 2021;Groenewoudt, Rooks, and van Gool 2019). ...
... Furthermore, static IM is beneficial for both individuals and groups to organize their thoughts and improve their creativity (Lusnig, Radach, and Hofmann 2022). During the IM process, an individual's observation and concentration are significantly enhanced (Baas, Nevicka, and Ten Velden 2020;Lebuda, Zabelina, and Karwowski 2016), which is helpful for teachers to obtain effective information and produce appropriate STEM instructional design ideas. That research is consistent with the results of this study, revealing that IM was positively related to IGID (H3 was supported). ...
Article
Amidst the unprecedented challenges brought on by the COVID-19 pandemic, the landscape of education has undergone profound transformations. One of the most notable shifts has been the rapid transition to online teaching methodologies. This transition has highlighted the critical role of teachers in self-regulating their instructional approaches while actively engaging in the realm of online learning. In this study, we delve into this dynamic environment, closely examining the practices of different age groups of educators: 177 teachers aged 20-30, 217 teachers aged 31-40, 68 teachers aged 41-50, and 15 teachers aged 51 and above. Our research takes a comprehensive approach, organizing the investigation into three distinct phases, each comprising six essential elements. Through this structured analysis, intriguing patterns emerge, particularly within the Performance and Appraisal Phase, where notable disparities among age groups within the Self-regulated Teaching Model become evident. In response to the multifaceted challenges posed by online teaching, our study proposes a compelling solution. We advocate for a strategic alignment of instructors from diverse age groups to collaboratively guide online courses. By harnessing the collective strengths and perspectives of educators across generations, this collaborative pedagogical approach has the potential to significantly enhance the efficacy of online teaching endeavors.
... Research linking mindfulness 18 and creativity has found a significant relationship between the two (Henriksen et al., 2020;Lebuda et al., 2016); however, this relationship was stronger when creativity was measured with insight tasks rather than divergent thinking tasks (Lebuda et al., 2016). Regardless, it is likely that the awareness and open-monitoring (OM) aspects of mindfulness (especially if used in a therapeutic context) enhance creativity. ...
... Research linking mindfulness 18 and creativity has found a significant relationship between the two (Henriksen et al., 2020;Lebuda et al., 2016); however, this relationship was stronger when creativity was measured with insight tasks rather than divergent thinking tasks (Lebuda et al., 2016). Regardless, it is likely that the awareness and open-monitoring (OM) aspects of mindfulness (especially if used in a therapeutic context) enhance creativity. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Anxiety-driven counterfactual thinking can be a slippery slope. Building on the premise that both positive and negative emotions can impact creativity, the present study examines how trait anxiety, optimism, and other mental health factors like therapy experience shape outputs in divergent thinking (DT) tasks. Using an online sample (N = 647), the study introduces counterfactual DT tasks, where participants are prompted to respond to how a situation can either go in their favor (positive) or against it (negative). Participants generated more original responses in the positive counterfactual DT task versus the negative one; however, they generated a higher number of ideas in the negative task compared to the positive one. Both counterfactual tasks led to higher originality and fluency as compared to a real-world DT task. Trait anxiety, especially when combined with past therapy experience or a previous mental health diagnosis, increased the number of negative counterfactual ideas. In contrast, optimism acted as a protective factor, enabling fewer ideas in the negative counterfactual task when combined with past therapy experience or a past mental health diagnosis. Our study challenges the binary view of emotional influences on creativity, highlighting the role of individual differences and experiences in shaping creative outputs. Diversifying the counterfactual DT tasks and using more refined measures of rumination and mental health history can provide further nuance to this line of research between creativity and anxiety.
... Furthermore, Horan (2009) highlighted that meditation enhances creative incubation and enlightenment through transcendence and integration. Lebuda et al. (2015) conducted a meta-analysis of the empirically informed mindfulness-creativity link, and reported that the link between creativity and mindfulness is at the level of little-c creativity ("everyday life creativity", see Kaufman & Beghetto, 2009) with a small to medium effect size. They noticed that the main link between creativity and mindfulness relates to open-monitoring meditation and divergent thinking by improving working memory, originality, cognitive flexibility, and the ability to change perspective, and reducing the fear of judgement and responding in unusual ways. ...
... Finally, mindfulness meditations train several executive functions and improve attention test scores (Tang et al., 2007;Chiesa et al., 2010), working memory (Chiesa et al., 2011) and cognitive control (Fountain-Zaragoza et al., 2015). Mindfulness meditation also develops socio-emotional abilities (Malboeuf-Hurtubise et al., 2017), creativity (Colzato et al., 2012;Colzato et al., 2014;Capurso et al., 2014;Ding et al., 2015;Lebuda et al., 2015;Lippelt et al., 2014), prosocial emotions (Luberto et al., 2018) and prosocial behaviours (Berry et al., 2020;Donald et al., 2019;Malin, 2023;Schindler & Friese, 2022), but this process is not necessarily automatic, hence the importance of including ethical instructions during mindfulness meditation (Berry et al., 2020;Chen & Jordan, 2020). These five practices, whether combined or not, could help to develop creativity and responsibility in pupils. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Creativity and responsibility are enhanced by meditation among adults, but such effects have not been studied in adolescents. Moreover, the determinants of the ethical effect (such as responsibility) of meditation are unclear. Aims To address this gap by investigating the impact of digital in-class meditation programmes in middle school, focusing on intentions (self-centred vs. responsibility-centred), on adolescents' creative potential and sense of responsibility. These intentions are operationalized by different purpose-based meditations. Methods We conducted a cluster randomized controlled trial involving 107 year 7 adolescents from six classes, assigning them to two experimental groups and an active control group. Pre- and post-intervention assessments were conducted over an 11-week period, including a creativity (EPoC) test comprising four exercises (graphic and verbal, divergent and convergent thinking), a responsibility and a mindfulness scale. Results Our findings revealed no discernible effects on divergent thinking or self-reported mindfulness. However, we observed significant differences in graphic and verbal convergent creative thinking, as well as impacting responsibility scores, between a responsibility-centred meditation group and a self-centred meditation group. Moreover, distinctions were noted between control and self-centred meditation groups and between some classes. Effect sizes indicated that the interventions had a moderate but significant impact on the variables measured. Conclusion Our study reveals the effectiveness of digital meditation interventions in enhancing convergent creative thinking and responsibility among middle-school students. Notably, it shed new light on the importance of meditation intentions, which may be as significant as the form of meditation itself.
... Interestingly, the mediation model between spontaneous mind-wandering, mindfulness, and creativity was not significant (Abd-Eldayem & Shaheen, 2021), highlighting the importance of being able to intentionally shift one's attention between the present moment and creative thoughts. When drawing inferences between mindfulness and soft fascination, it is important to note that despite the similarities, both are distinct constructs and have been found to independently affect creativity (Lebuda et al., 2016). Some studies have also found that mindfulness is necessary for soft fascination to occur (Djernis et al., 2019), leaving the exact nature of the relationship between the two, and thus their relationship to creativity, unclear. ...
Article
Full-text available
The widespread benefits of creativity have become more salient in recent years. This has led to scholarly interest in finding ways to foster creativity. Nature immersion may be one way to enhance creativity, particularly as many individuals involved in creative pursuits have found nature to be a source of inspiration and a haven for restoration. Using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) extension for scoping reviews and the PRISMA-ScR 22 item checklist, we conducted a scoping review to examine the evidence on the relationship between nature and creativity as well as any underlying mechanisms facilitating this relationship. A scoping review was deemed appropriate given the infancy of the topic and the need to understand the broad landscape. Our aim was to present a range of interdisciplinary applications of nature interventions on different types of creative performance. We found 45 publications exploring the relationship between nature and creativity, with the majority (n = 36, 80%) published within the past seven years. Overall, the quantitative studies in our scoping review provided evidence of a significant positive relationship, or effect, between nature and creativity. These relationships held across various types of nature including for wilderness, rural and urban settings, as well as in different applications of creativity such as in the arts, workplace, education, and laboratory conditions. Qualitative and case study research also supported the positive relationship between nature and creativity including in diverse nature settings and in different applications such as in visual arts, design, writing, music, free play in kindergartens, and work. Qualitative studies highlighted potential mechanisms behind this relationship, including time in solitude, appreciation of impermanence, relaxation and inspiration, and disconnection from technology. Moreover, the degree of exposure to nature may be an important consideration for creative performance with more immersive, real-life experiences typical of outdoor nature, tending to be more effective than indoor spaces with window views or indoor plants. These preliminary findings suggest that more opportunities to connect with nature, especially in the outdoors, may help to stimulate creativity in a wide range of settings including education, the creative arts, and workplaces, although more high-quality research examining the sustained effects of nature on creativity is needed.
... As for the positive effects of individual mindfulness in aspects of individual performance, among them are attention (Hales and Chakravorty, 2016;Weick and Sutcliffe, 2006), flexibility and creativity (Baron et al., 2018;Lebuda et al., 2016), resistance to stress (Hülsheger et al., 2013), productivity (Ivtzan and Hart, 2016;Ivtzan and Hart, 2016) and in competences necessary for problem-solving (Kiken et al., 2015;Olafsen, 2017). These and other benefits associated with personal and professional life are described in Table 1. ...
Article
Purpose: The study of mindfulness is still shown to be of interest in different aspects of organizations and/or businesses. Therefore, this study aims to present an integrative, multi-level model of mindfulness based on a holistic approach that can contribute to better governance practices and lead to competitive advantages. Design/methodology/approach: To fulfil this aim, an extensive integrative review of the literature, from the main articles about this topic, was made. Findings: This study shows that the concept of mindfulness, a conscious presence or full attention and its relation with organisations or firms’ personal, behavioural and social characteristics, in the current context of great adversity, uncertainty and unpredictability, is of interest at the individual, organisational and social level. Practical implications: This conceptual study has important implications for both practice and theory. It demonstrates that mindfulness significantly impacts the manager/business person’s ecosystem at the individual, organizational and social levels, particularly in relation to sustainable development goals (SDGs). Originality/value: This study introduces a comprehensive theoretical model that explains this relationship and organizes information from a multi-level perspective. This approach can contribute to the advancement of theory by clarifying and discussing the role of mindfulness at the individual, organizational, and societal levels. It also identifies opportunities and outlines future research directions, aiming to promote more sustainable development.
... For example, past mindfulness research has shown that trait mindfulness may have different effects on different creative stages or processes (Henriksen et al., 2020). In particular, Lebuda et al. (2016) reported that the effect of trait mindfulness was greater for creativity measurements involving insight problem-solving tasks than for those involving divergent thinking tasks. In addition, research has illustrated that PsyCap, as a higher-order positive psychological construct that consists of four subdimensions (i.e., self-efficacy, optimism, hope, and resilience), is complex in that it promotes creativity (Yu et al., 2019;Yan et al., 2020), with some of its subcomponents contributing more to a certain aspect of creativity than others (e.g., Puozzo and Audrin, 2021;He and Wong, 2022;Lorenz et al., 2022;Lei and Lei, 2023;Sun et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
The positive mindfulness-creativity link has been widely documented; however, its underlying psychological mechanisms remain less understood. From the perspective of positive psychology, this study examined the mediating effect of psychological capital (PsyCap) on the effect of dispositional mindfulness on creative functioning. A total of 894 Chinese secondary school students in Hong Kong (50.8% female; Mage = 15.5 years) completed the study. A cross-sectional design was used, in which context PsyCap and dispositional mindfulness were assessed by the Chinese version of the revised Compound PsyCap Scale (CPC-12R) and the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), respectively. Moreover, by adopting the multiple-measurement approach to creativity, three commonly used creativity tests (i.e., the Wallach-Kogan Creativity Test/WKCT, the Test for Creative Thinking–Drawing Production/TCT–DP, and the Creative Problem-Solving Test/CPST) were applied to capture three aspects of creativity (i.e., divergent thinking, creative combination, and creative problem solving). The results suggest that (1) PsyCap partially but significantly mediated the mindfulness-creativity link for all three aspects of creative functioning, and (2) PsyCap demonstrated the strongest effect size in mediating the mindfulness-creativity link for creative problem solving, followed by creative combination and then divergent thinking. These results, on the one hand, support the positive psychology perspective by confirming a positive psychological resource mechanism regarding the relationship between mindfulness and creativity. On the other hand, the results regarding the varied sizes of the mediation effect further enrich the discourse on this perspective by showing that the mediation mechanism may function to different degrees depending on which aspect of creativity is under consideration. These findings illuminate the positive functioning of mindfulness, psychological resources/capital and creativity.
... It is noteworthy that techniques developed in fields other than education, integrated into didactic methodologies of teaching with a sustainable and/or ecosocial focus, proved effective in incorporating and developing creativity. Design thinking, creative problem solving, and mindfulness emerged from fields such as product or service design, marketing, psychology, and contemplative traditions, providing distinctive elements that positively impacted the achievement of creative solutions with sustainability criteria in educational contexts, for both teachers and students [17,78,111,112]. By including a problem situation as a learning trigger and presenting it to a student in a structured manner or in phases, whether ideation techniques are applied or not, their brain was able to unconsciously work on incubating ideas and possible solutions [18]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This systematic review identifies the contributions of creativity to learning and its integration into teaching within educational approaches with an ecosocial and/or sustainable perspective based on scientific publications in the Web of Science and SCOPUS databases, following the PRISMA methodology guidelines. This study establishes a comprehensive understanding of creativity from a sociocultural perspective and examines the relationship between creativity and educational approaches based on sustainable education. The results highlight that creativity is incorporated into teaching in a cross-cutting manner in the ideation, design, and implementation of pedagogical strategies that include diverse techniques, resources, and methodologies. Creativity contributes to learning by being multifaceted and transforming the student experience through the identification, understanding, appropriation, and applicability of concepts. Various strategies and methodologies are identified that promote creativity by involving students in collaborative and reflective processes. Creativity stimulates imagination, interdisciplinary and intercultural integration, and experimentation. The fundamental role of the teacher and the learning environment is emphasized. Finally, the need to integrate creativity into educational plans from these approaches is highlighted, fostering the adaptability to uncertainty and the generation of creative and sustainable responses. This review provides a synthesized and updated resource for teachers and individuals interested in the relationship between creativity, learning, and sustainability.
Article
Mental health is relevant to every individual. Poor mental health and well-being contribute significantly to personal suffering and a substantial burden on society. Art and cooking therapies in complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) are flexible approaches that emphasize engagement and growth to promote mental health and well-being, are easy to perform, accessible when needed, and can be self-administered. This review aims to explore art and cooking therapies focusing on intervention protocols, target populations, underlying mechanisms, limitations, and areas for future research. The literature search was conducted in PubMed, Google Scholar, and Semantic Scholar databases using predefined search terms. A total of 26 randomized controlled trials, 19 in art therapy, and seven in cooking therapy were included. Art and cooking therapies are promising interventions to improve mental health and well-being. They promote mindfulness and provide opportunities for self-expression and social support which can be explored in future studies and therapeutic settings.
Article
Full-text available
This article reports a meta-analysis of the relationships between creative self-beliefs (CSBs)-a broad set of characteristics including creative self-efficacy, creative personal identity, and self-rated creativity-and the Big Five (openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism) as well as the Huge Two (plasticity and stability) personality traits. A review of the literature identified 25 independent studies and more than 80 correlations per personality factor; these were analyzed using a 3-level meta-analysis model. Openness was consistently the strongest predictor of CSBs (r = .467; 95% CI: .402, .531), followed by Extraversion (r = .264; 95% CI: .225, .304), Conscientiousness (r = .133; 95% CI: .061, .204), Neuroticism (r = -.124; 95% CI: -.184, -.064), and Agreeableness (r = .070; 95% CI: .006, .134). These effects were moderated by CSB type; more specifically, the relationships were stronger for domain-general than for domain-specific measures of CSBs. A meta-analytical structural equation model demonstrated that Plasticity was strongly positively correlated with CSBs (β = .71), whereas Stability was a negative predictor (β = -.23). We discuss whether the relationship between personality and CSBs is causal or correlational and consider the discriminant validity of CSBs as currently defined. (PsycINFO Database Record
Article
Journals tend to publish only statistically significant evidence, creating a scientific record that markedly overstates the size of effects. We provide a new tool that corrects for this bias without requiring access to nonsignificant results. It capitalizes on the fact that the distribution of significant p values, p-curve, is a function of the true underlying effect. Researchers armed only with sample sizes and test results of the published findings can correct for publication bias. We validate the technique with simulations and by reanalyzing data from the Many-Labs Replication project. We demonstrate that p-curve can arrive at conclusions opposite that of existing tools by reanalyzing the meta-analysis of the “choice overload” literature.
Article
Scientific interest in meditation has significantly grown in the past years; however, so far, science has neglected the idea that different types of meditations may drive specific cognitive-control states. It has been shown that focused-attention (FA) and open-monitoring (OM) meditation exert specific effect on creativity; OM meditation induces a control state that promotes divergent thinking, a style of thinking that allows many new ideas to be generated, while FA meditation tends to support convergent thinking, the process of generating one possible solution to a particular problem. In the present study, by using creativity tasks tapping into convergent (compound Remote Associates Task; cRAT) and divergent thinking (Alternate Uses Task; AUT), we investigated whether this effect was modulated by prior meditation experience, by comparing a group of practitioners (n = 20) and a group of novices (n = 20). The enhancing effect of OM meditation on divergent thinking was found to be robust irrespective of prior experience. However, while solving convergent-thinking problems, practitioners used an insight strategy, as opposed to an analytical approach, significantly more often than the novices.