ArticlePDF Available

#Me: Narcissism and its facets as predictors of selfie-posting frequency

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

"Selfies" are amateur photographs people take of themselves, usually with a smartphone. Sharing selfies on social media has become a popular activity, prompting questions about its psychological meaning and dispositionally-relevant motives. This study was performed to examine the association between narcissism, a personality trait characterized by inflated self-views and attempts to seek attention and admiration from others, and frequency of posting selfies on social networking sites. In addition, the association between posting selfies and three facets of narcissism (i.e., Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, Entitlement/Exploitativeness) was explored. These questions were addressed in a nationally representative sample of 1204 men and women who completed an online survey. Results showed that narcissism, as well as the Leadership/Authority and Grandiose Exhibitionism facets, but not Entitlement/Exploitativeness, exhibited positive and significant associations with selfie-posting frequency. Age did not moderate the predictive effects of narcissism or any of its three dimensions, indicating that the relationship between narcissism, its facets, and posting selfies is not age dependent. However, the more adaptive Leadership/Authority facet emerged as a stronger predictor of selfie posting among women than men, whereas the maladaptive Entitlement/Exploitativeness facet predicted selfie posting among men, but not women. Interpretations and implications of these findings are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
#Me: Narcissism and its facets as predictors of sele-posting frequency
Eric B. Weiser
Department of Psychology, Curry College, United States
abstractarticle info
Article history:
Received 27 April 2015
Received in revised form 29 June 2015
Accepted 3 July 2015
Available online 18 July 2015
Keywords:
Narcissism
Photo sharing
Seles
Social media
Social networking sites
Selesare amateur photographs people take of themselves, usually with a smartphone. Sharingseles on social
media has become a popular activity, prompting questions about its psychological meaning and dispositionally-
relevant motives. This study was performed to examine the association between narcissism, a personality trait
characterized by inated self-views and attempts to seek attention and admiration from others, and frequency
of posting seles on social networking sites. In addition, the association between posting seles and three facets
of narcissism (i.e., Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, Entitlement/Exploitativeness) was explored.
These questions were addressed in a nationally representative sample of 1204 men and women who completed
an online survey. Results showed that narcissism, as well as the Leadership/Authority and Grandiose Exhibition-
ism facets, but not Entitlement/Exploitativeness, exhibited positive and signicant associations with sele-
posting frequency. Age did not moderate the predictive effects of narcissism or any of its three dimensions, indi-
cating thatthe relationship between narcissism, its facets, and posting seles is not age dependent. However, the
more adaptive Leadership/Authority facet emerged as a stronger predictor of sele posting among women than
men, whereas the maladaptive Entitlement/Exploitativeness facet predicted sele posting among men, but not
women. Interpretations and implications of these ndings are discussed.
© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Photo sharing has become an important part of the online social ex-
perience. Over half of U.S. internet users post photos online (Pew
Research Center, 2013), perhaps owing to the increasing use of
smartphones; in the U.S., nearly two-thirds of adults own a smartphone,
compared to 35% in 2011 (Pew Research Center, 2015). These devices
simplify photo sharing through social media by enabling users to take
and post digital photographs on social networking sites (SNSs) instanta-
neously. This function is facilitated through photo-sharing sites such as
Instagram, a mobile photo platform allowing users to share photo-
graphs and videos over SNSs such as Facebook and Twitter. Instagram
hosts over 30 billion photographs, and, on average, 70 million photo-
graphs are posted to the site each day from its 300 million users
(Instagram, 2014).
The rise in photo sharing has helped spark a precipitous new social
phenomenon: the snapping and sending of amateur self-portrait photo-
graphs known as seles to SNSs. Seleis now part of today's ver-
nacular and is formally dened as a photograph that one has taken of
oneself, typically one taken with a smartphone or webcam and
uploaded to a social media website("Infographic: A Closer Look at
'Sele,'" 2013, para. 4). In 2013, Oxford Dictionaries added sele to its
lexicon and later named it their international Word of the Year
(Brumeld, 2013). Seles permeate the social media landscape; for
example, Instagram hosts over 238 million photographs hashtagged
with #sele and over 126 million hashtagged with #me.
The pervasiveness of sele posting raises questions concerning the
social and psychological motives behind this behavior. What important
needs and goals does selepostingreect, and from which dispositional
attributes do they emanate? The objective of this study is to extend pre-
vious work on trait predictors of SNS use and behavior by examining the
connection between sele posting and narcissism, a personality trait
marked by grandiosity and egocentrism, and by the constant pursuit
of veneration by others (Brown, Budzek & Tamborski, 2009; Campbell,
Reeder, Sedikides & Elliot, 2000). Photographs convey important social
information in numerous online domains (Ellison, Heino & Gibbs,
2006; Kapidzic, 2013; Whitty, 2008). Of the photographs shared online,
seles seem inherently to contain the most explicit elements of ostenta-
tion and self-propagation. Indeed, seles have been described as a
symptom of social media-driven narcissism(Pearlman, 2013, para.
20), reecting the intuitive assumption that the taking and posting of
such photographs constitute self-promoting gambits by the self-
absorbed. Empirically, however, sele posting and its association with
narcissism has been largely unexplored. Because narcissists are moti-
vated to gain others' attention and admiration to maintain their inated
self-views (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001), posting seles plausibly may
function as a novel psychological maneuver through which narcissistic
individuals attempt to meet these self-regulatory objectives. Hence, ex-
amining the relationship between narcissism and sele posting is an
issue of importance because it may shed light on the psychological
meanings of this activity in the social media milieu.
Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 477481
Department of Psychology, Curry College, Milton, MA 02186, United States.
E-mail address: eweiser@curry.edu.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.007
0191-8869/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Differences
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
1.1. Narcissism and SNS use
Narcissism has received particular attention in research examining
predictors of SNS use, the presumption being that SNSs may create or
reinforce narcissistic tendencies by serving as channels for self-
promotional displays (Buffardi & Campbell, 2008). Several studies
have revealed a positive relationship between narcissism and the fre-
quency of using SNSs (Panek, Nardis & Konrath, 2013; Ryan & Xenos,
2011), as well as narcissistic-like patterns of self-promoting behaviors
(e.g., status updates, number of Facebook friends, use of language and
photographs to draw attention to oneself) on these sites (Carpenter,
2012; DeWall, Buffardi, Bonser & Campbell, 2011; Mehdizadeh, 2010).
To date, there have been only two studies investigating the link be-
tween narcissism and posting seles. Although positive associations
were reported in both, investigators in one of these studies (Fox &
Rooney, 2015) assessed narcissism using a four-item subscale of the
Dirty Dozen (Jonason & Webster, 2010), a 12-item measure of Dark
Triad personality traits (Paulus & Williams, 2002). Moreover, participa-
tion in this study was limited to adult males age 40 and younger.
Sorokowski et al. (2015) used a Polish adaptation of theNarcissistic Per-
sonality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry, 1988), which has a different
factor structure than the original version, to examine how various com-
ponents of narcissism relate to sele-posting behavior. These investiga-
tors found that three of the four subscales specic to the Polish
adaptation (i.e., Vanity, Leadership, Admiration Demand) predicted
sele posting among men, whereas one (Admiration Demand) predict-
ed sele posting among women.
It is necessary to continue examining how narcissism and its compo-
nents are related to sele posting. Narcissism is a complex, multifaceted
construct consisting of socially adaptive and maladaptive components
(Ackerman et al., 2011; Emmons, 1987; Kubarych, Deary & Austin,
2004). Thus, viewing narcissism as a unidimensional rather than a nu-
anced, dimensional construct may obfuscate relationships between its
facets and important criterion variables (Brown et al., 2009). Further,
age and gender differences in narcissism have been documented; spe-
cically, recent generations are more narcissistic than previous genera-
tions (Twenge, Konrath, Foster, Campbell & Bushman, 2008), and men
are more narcissistic than are women (Grijalva et al., 2015). It is there-
fore crucial to examine and clarify how these demographic variables
moderate associations between narcissism, its facets, and outcome
measures.
1.2. The dimensions of narcissism
Factor analytic studies of the NPI, the primary self-report measureof
narcissism used in social andpersonality research (Cain, Pincus & Ansell,
2008), have revealed varying factor structures, with as few as two
(Corry, Merritt, Mrug & Pamp, 2008) and as many as seven (Raskin &
Terry, 1988) factors having been reported. Most recently, Ackerman
et al. (2011) demonstrated that the NPI assesses three robust and em-
pirically useful components of narcissism: Leadership/Authority, Gran-
diose Exhibitionism, and Entitlement/Exploitativeness.
Leadership/Authority (LA) reects self-perceptions of leadership,
dominance, and the capacity for social agency. Individuals who score
high on LA are motivated to lead and seek power and authority over
others. Grandiose Exhibitionism (GE) reects self-absorption, vanity,
and exhibitionistic propensities. Individuals who score high on GE are
in love with themselves and crave attention; they enjoy showing off
and actively seek opportunities for self-promotion. Entitlement/
Exploitativeness (EE) reects entitlement, a sense one deserves respect,
and a willingness to exploit others. Individuals who score high on EE
possess unmerited or unreasonable expectations of favorable treat-
ment; they believe they deserve whatever they wish and will take ad-
vantage of others in the service of their goals. Of these, LA is
considered adaptive in that it is related to indices of psychological ad-
justment (e.g., self-esteem), and unrelated to narcissism's more
interpersonally aversive elements. Conversely, GE and especially EE
are regarded as socially toxic and are more readily linked to maladaptive
social outcomes (Ackerman et al., 2011).
1.3. Overview and predictions
Given that narcissism is associated with the use of self-promoting
SNS behaviors, it was expected that narcissism would be positively re-
lated to the frequency of positing seles on SNSs. In addition, the NPI
subscales of LA, GE, and EE were analyzed to determine their differential
contributions to the prediction of sele posting and, thus, to gain insight
into the specic narcissistic needs this activity serves. As GE strongly
predicts self-promoting SNS behavior (Carpenter, 2012), it was expect-
ed that this relationship extends to sele posting. Therefore, it was fur-
ther predicted that, of the narcissism facets, GE would exhibit the
strongest association with sele-posting frequency.
Finally, age and gender were included in this study to test their po-
tential roles as moderators. As noted above, generational increases in
narcissism have been reported (Twenge et al., 2008); further, millen-
nialsaremore inclined toward posting seles than are previous gener-
ations (Pew Research Center, 2014a). Thus, if narcissism predicts sele
posting, this relationship may be stronger for younger than older indi-
viduals. In addition, although men score higher on all three narcissism
facets (Grijalva et al., 2015), women post more seles than do men
(Sorokowski et al., 2015). It is therefore necessary to examine gender
as a possible moderator of the hypothesized associations.
2. Method
2.1. Participants and procedure
A total of 1248 U.S. residents were recruited from Amazon's Me-
chanical Turk (Mason & Suri, 2012) to participate in the study in return
for a small payment. Participants completedan online survey developed
through QuestionPro. Forty-four participants failed to complete the sur-
vey fully and were excluded from the analysis. The nal sample (N=
1204) was predominantly White (75%) and female (65%), with an aver-
age age of 32.81 (range = 1674, SD =11.56).
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Time using SNSs
Participants indicated, on average, how manytimes per day they vis-
ited SNSs and the average duration in minutes per visit. The product of
these two open-ended responses yielded an estimate of the amount of
time participants spend using SNSs (Panek, Nardis & Konrath, 2013)
and was used as a covariate in later analyses.
1
2.2.2. Posting frequency
Frequency of participants' posting (e.g., status updates, comments,
pictures,links)onSNSswasassessedand used as an additional covariate
in subsequent analyses. Posting frequency was determined by a single
item using a response scale ranging from 1 (never)to8(more than once
per day).
2.2.3. Sele-posting frequency
Participants responded to the following item: Do you know what a
seleis?Those who indicated Nowere branched to an item provid-
ing a formal denition of a sele; those who indicated Yes(97%)
were branched to another item providing a denition, but beginning
with, As you know, a seleis…” Next, all participants responded to a
second yes-or-no item: Have you ever shared a sele on a photo
1
Examination of the distributions for each variable revealed that the amount of time
using SNSsexhibited substantial positiveskew; hence, this variable was subjectedto a log-
arithmic transformation. All other variables were normally distributed.
478 E.B. Weiser / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 477481
sharing or social networking site, such as Facebook, Twitter, Instagram,
Flickr, and Snapchat?Participants who indicated Nowere branched
to a subsequent section of the survey; those who indicated Yes
(64%) were branched to an item asking, How often would you say
that you share seles on photo sharing or social networking sites?Re-
sponse options consisted of: less than once a year;”“once a year;”“sev-
eral times per year, but less than once a month;”“once a month;”“afew
times a month, but less than once a week;”“once a week;”“a few times
per week;”“once a day;and more than once a day.
2.2.4. Narcissism
Participants completed the 40-item Narcissistic Personality Invento-
ry (NPI-40, Raskin & Terry, 1988), which assesses individual differences
in trait narcissism. The NPI-40 uses a forced-choice format, in that each
item asks respondents to choose one of two statements that is most self-
descriptive; one of the statements reects narcissistic sentiments and
the other does not (e.g., I am more capable than other people.vs.
There is a lot that I can learn from other people.). For each item, the
narcissistic statement is scored a 1 and the non-narcissistic statement
is scored a 0.
A conrmatory factor analysis was performed on the NPI to deter-
mine if the Ackerman et al. (2011) model t the data adequately.
Mplus 7.31 was used to estimate the three-factor model consisting of
the items for LA, GE, and EE. Because NPI indicators are dichotomous,
the model was estimated using the robust weighted least squares
(WLSMV) estimation method (Brown, 2006). In replication of
Ackerman et al. (2011), covariances between residuals for selected indi-
cators were specied. Results showed that the three-factor model pro-
vided acceptable t to the data (CFI = .941, TLI = .933, RMSEA = .048).
Along with NPI-40 total scores (α= .88), items pertaining to the
three factors were summed to form subscales: LA (11 items, α=.81),
GE (10 items, α= .79), and EE (4 items, α=.51).
2
3. Results
Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS 22.0. Intercorrelations, means,
and standard deviations are displayed in Table 1. Time using SNSs, post-
ing frequency, narcissism, and each of the three narcissism subscales all
correlated positively with sele-posting frequency; age, however, cor-
related negatively with self-posting frequency.
A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was performed to test the
hypothesis that narcissism predicts sele-posting frequency. Time using
SNSs, posting frequency, age, and gender served as covariates and were
entered in Step 1. Subsequently, narcissism was entered in Step 2, and
the two-way interactions of age, gender, and narcissism were entered
in Step 3. As recommended when testing interaction effects in multiple
regression, each predictor variable was centered, and interaction terms
were formed by multiplying together the centered predictors (Aiken &
West, 1991). Examination of variance ination factors (VIFs; Stevens,
2001) indicated that multicollinearity was not a problem in any of the
regression analyses.
Table 2 summarizes the regression model. Collectively, the covari-
ates explained a signicant proportion of the variance in sele-posting
frequency. After the covariates were entered, narcissism positively pre-
dicted sele-posting frequency (β=.21,t=8.15,pb.001) and ex-
plained a signicant proportion of the variance. Both interaction terms
entered in Step 3 were nonsignicant, indicating that neither age nor
gender moderated the effect of narcissism on sele posting.
A second hierarchical multiple regression tested the predictive ef-
fects of the three narcissism facets on sele posting. After covariates
were entered in Step 1, the LA, GE, and EE subscales were entered in
Step 2. In Step 3, all two-way interactions involving age, gender, and
the three subscales were entered; as before, each of the relevant vari-
ables were centered. As shown in Table 3, both LA (β= .12, t= 3.98,
pb.001) and GE (β=.13,t=4.55,pb.001) contributed signicantly
and positively to the prediction of sele-posting frequency.
Asignicant gender × LA interaction emerged in Step 3, indicating
that the predictive effect of LA was stronger among women than
among men. Specically, analysis of separate regression equations re-
vealed that although LA was signicantly positively related to posting
seles among both female and male participants, this relationship was
stronger among women (β=.20,pb.001) than among men (β=
.12, pb.01). Additionally, a signicant gender × EE interaction was ob-
served in Step 3, indicating that that the predictive effect of EE was
stronger among men than women. Analysis of simple regression equa-
tions demonstrated that EE was positively related to posting seles
among men (β=.13,pb.01) but not among women (β=.03, n.s.).
2
Levene's tests revealed that men's and women's total narcissism and subscale scores
were homoscedastic.
Table 1
Correlations between variables, means, and standard deviations.
Measure 1 2 3 45678MSD
1. Sele-posting frequency .22⁎⁎⁎ .41⁎⁎⁎ .32⁎⁎⁎ .32⁎⁎⁎ .26⁎⁎⁎ .27⁎⁎⁎ .16⁎⁎⁎ 3.49 2.38
2. Time using SNSs .28⁎⁎⁎ .07.12⁎⁎⁎ .09⁎⁎ .08⁎⁎ .06 100.96 156.36
3. Posting frequency .05 .18⁎⁎⁎ .16⁎⁎⁎ .12⁎⁎⁎ .10⁎⁎⁎ 5.32 2.05
4. Age .22⁎⁎⁎ .13⁎⁎⁎ .18⁎⁎⁎ .20⁎⁎⁎ 32.81 11.56
5. Narcissism .87⁎⁎⁎ .77⁎⁎⁎ .55⁎⁎⁎ 12.79 7.61
6. Leadership/Authority .50⁎⁎⁎ .36⁎⁎⁎ 4.41 3.08
7. Grandiose Exhibitionism .36⁎⁎⁎ 2.32 2.42
8. Entitlement/Exploitativeness 0.90 1.05
Note: Pearson product-moment correlations. M=mean;SD =standarddeviation.
pb.05.
⁎⁎ pb.01.
⁎⁎⁎ pb.001.
Table 2
Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analysis predicting sele-posting frequency
from narcissism.
Predictor βtΔR
2
F
chg
Step 1 .28 116.62⁎⁎
Time using SNSs .15⁎⁎ 5.11
Posting frequency .32⁎⁎ 11.49
Age .29⁎⁎ 11.63
Gender .083.12
Step 2 .04 66.39⁎⁎
Narcissism .21⁎⁎ 8.15
Step 3 b.01 2.58
Age × narcissism .04 1.94
Gender × narcissism .03 1.25
Total R
2
.32
N1204
Note. Gender was coded: 0 = female, 1 = male. βis the standardized regression coef-
cient. For Step 2, F(5, 1199) = 111.66, pb.001; for Step 3, F(7, 1197) = 80.71, pb.001.
pb.01.
⁎⁎ pb.001.
479E.B. Weiser / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 477481
4. Discussion
The results of this study are in accordance with research examining
narcissism and its relationship to attention-seeking and self-promoting
behaviors in social media environments (e.g., Buffardi & Campbell,
2008; Carpenter, 2012; DeWall, Buffardi, Bonser & Campbell, 2011),
and they replicate and extend previous ndings supporting a positive
association between narcissism and seles (Fox & Rooney, 2015;
Sorokowski et al., 2015). Overall, ndings from this investigation sug-
gest that posting seles, as with other self-promoting SNS behaviors,
represents an avenue through which narcissistic needs are expressed
through social media. For example, narcissistic individuals consider
themselves to be physically attractive (Bleske-Rechek, Remiker &
Baker, 2008) and, in fact, meta-analytic evidence suggests they are
(Holtzman & Strube, 2010). Hence, these individuals may consider
sele-posting as a conduit through which they can use their looks to
garner both attention and admiration.
However, the association between total NPI narcissism and posting
seles obscures which narcissism facets are driving this behavior. Spe-
cically, LA and GE predicted sele-posting frequency, whereas EE did
not. It seems, then, that for individuals high in narcissism, posting seles
constitutes a means to not only seek attention and admiration from
others (GE), but also to convey heightened perceptions of leadership,
authority, and dominance (LA). Combined, these dimensions capture
agentic, approach-oriented elements of narcissism (Ackerman et al.,
2011), suggesting that sele posting may be a manifestation of more
adaptive self-enhancement tendencies, rather than the more socially
noxious tendencies captured by EE. For their part, individuals high in
EE may regard other SNS behaviors (e.g., retaliating against others' neg-
ative comments) as viable means to satisfy narcissistic objectives
(Carpenter, 2012).
Unexpectedly, age did not moderate the predictive inuence of ei-
ther total narcissismand or its dimensions on posting seles, suggesting
that self-promoting displays on SNSs may not be age dependent. Given
the increasing engagement with SNSs among adults of all ages (Pew
Research Center, 2014b), sele posting might serve to fulll narcissistic
goals throughout much of adulthood. As previous investigations exam-
ining narcissistic behaviors on SNSs have often used college-age sam-
ples (e.g., Bergman, Fearrington, Davenport & Bergman, 2011;
Kapidzic, 2013; Mehdizadeh, 2010), future work might benetfrom
sampling participants whose ages are sufciently representative of the
adult population.
Of importance, gender moderated the predictive effect of LA, in that
sele-posting frequency was linked to higher levels of LA among women
than among men. LA appears to represent a dimension of personality re-
lated to psychological resilience and social potency (Ackerman et al.,
2011). It embodies agentic qualities (e.g., assertiveness, dominance)
elements incongruous with female gender stereotypes (Eagly & Karau,
2002)and, in fact, men exhibit stronger LA than do women (Grijalva
et al., 2015). Agentic women who violate communal gender role stereo-
types are often perceived negatively (Rudman & Glick, 2001), perhaps
discouraging women in leadership roles from presenting themselves
in ways that are distinctly masculine or feminine (Koenig, Eagly,
Mitchell & Ristikari, 2011). Because posting seles is neither inherently
masculinenor feminine,narcissistic women who harbor self-
perceptions of power, authority, and dominance over others may view
sele-posting as a viable way to satisfy their agentic needs without in-
curring social penalties. Equally important, sele-posting frequency
was signicantly associated with EE among men, but not women. Al-
though men score substantially higher than do women on EE, and gen-
der differences in narcissismare driven principally by this facet (Grijalva
et al., 2015), EE is not associated with the desire to self-promote on SNSs
(Carpenter, 2012). However, EE correlates with elements of pathological
narcissism, a trait viewed as distinct from the more normalnarcissism
assessed by the NPI (Pincus et al., 2009). These elements seem to capture
a fundamentally insecure and vulnerable aspect of personality and are
marked by contingent self-esteem, devaluation of others who do not
provide needed adulation, and anger when entitled expectations are
not met (Ackerman et al, 2011). It is therefore possible that, among nar-
cissistic men, sele posting might represent a pathological self-
regulatory maneuver aimed at boosting self-enhancement and valida-
tion. Ideally, future work addressing how unhealthyforms of narcis-
sism relate to SNS behavior will employ measures that assess such
forms explicitly (e.g., Pathological Narcissism Inventory [PNI]; Pincus
et al., 2009).
4.1. Strengths, limitations, and conclusions
Primary strengths of this study include the use of a large, nationally
representative sample of participants and theanalysis of both total nar-
cissism and its conceptually meaningful facets.
Nevertheless, several limitations bear mentioning. First, the low reli-
ability of theEE subscale may have attenuated the observed associations
between this facet and self-posting frequency. However, this subscale's
low alpha coefcient likely is attributable to its small number of total
items rather than its content (Ackerman et al., 2011). More practically,
the internal consistency of personality scales is of limited utility in eval-
uating their validity (McCrae, Kurtz, Yamagata & Terracciano, 2011).
Second, although measurement equivalence was presumed in this
study, the observed gender differences in NPI facets may have reected
differences in the ways men and women interpret or respond to NPI
items. It should be noted, however, that Grijalva et al. (2015) ruled
out the possibility that gender differences in the three facets are artifacts
of measurement bias, concluding that differences in men's and women's
scores on the NPI can be interpreted at face value(p. 20). Third, al-
though thepredictive effectof narcissism and its facets were statistically
signicant after controlling for empirically-relevant covariates, sele
posting may be associated with other factors not assessed here, such
as other personality traits (e.g., extraversion).
In sum, the present research compliments and extends prior work
demonstrating that personality and trait-related needs are powerful ar-
chitects of the behaviors on display in social media enclaves. Posting
seles is not a motivationally vacuous activity; instead, it is a form of
self-expression that carries with it social and psychological meaning.
Findings from this study demonstrate that seles tell us something im-
portant about those who share them and, as such, offer valuable insights
into the uses of social media in general.
Table 3
Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analysis predicting sele-posting frequency
from narcissism subscales.
Predictor βtΔR
2
F
chg
Step 1 .28 116.62⁎⁎
Control variables
a
Step 2 .05 24.03⁎⁎
LA .12⁎⁎ 3.98
GE .13⁎⁎ 4.55
EE .01 0.03
Step 3 .01 2.54
Age × LA .04 1.50
Gender × LA .092.31
Age × GE .04 1.37
Gender × GE .02 0.52
Age × EE .04 1.60
Gender × EE .082.17
Total R
2
.34
N1204
Note. Gender was coded: 0 = female, 1 = male. βis the standardized regression coef-
cient. LA = Leadership/Author ity; GE = Grandiose Exhibitionism; EE = Entitlement/
Exploitative ness. For Step 2, F(7, 1197) = 80.77, pb.001; for Step 3, F(13, 1191) =
45.06, pb.001.
a
Estimated values were the same as in the previous analysis.
pb.05.
⁎⁎ pb.001.
480 E.B. Weiser / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 477481
References
Ackerman, R.A., Witt, E.A., Donnellan, M.B., Trzesniewski, K.H., Robins, R.W., & Kashy, D.A.
(2011). What does the Narcissistic Personality Inventory really measure? Assessment,
18,6787.
Aiken, L.S., & West, S.G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bergman, S.M., Fearrington, M.E., Davenport, S.W., & Bergman, J.Z. (2011). Millennials,
narcissism, and social networking: What narcissists do on social networking sites
and why. Personality and Individual Differences,50,706711.
Bleske-Rechek, A., Remiker, M.W., & Baker, J.P. (2008). Narcissistic men and women think
they are so hot But they are not. Personality and Individual Differences,45,420424.
Brown, T.A. (2006). Conrmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford.
Brown, R.P., Budzek, K., & Tamborski, M. (2009). On the meaning and measure of narcis-
sism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,35,951964.
Brumeld, B. (2013, November 20). Sele named word of the year for 2013. CNN. Re-
trieved from http://www.cnn.com
Buffardi, L.E., & Campbell, W.K. (2008). Narcissism and social networking web sites.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,34,13031314.
Cain, N.M.,Pincus, A.L., & Ansell,E.B. (2008). Narcissism at the crossroads: Phenotypic de-
scription of pathological narcissism across clinical theory, social/personality psychol-
ogy, and psychiatric diagnosis. Clinical Psychology Review,28,638656.
Campbell, W.K., Reeder, G.D., Sedikides, C., & Elliot, A.J. (2000). Narcissism and compara-
tive self -enhancement strate gies. Journal of Research in Personality,34,329347.
Carpenter, C.J. (2012). Narcissism on Facebook: Self-promotional andanti-social behavior.
Personality and Individual Differences,52,482486.
Corry, N., Merritt, R.D., Mrug, S., & Pamp, B. (2008). The factor structure of the Narcissistic
Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment,90,593600.
DeWall, C.N., Buffardi, L.E., Bonser, I., & Campbell,W.K. (2011). Narcissism and implicit at-
tention seeking: Evidence from linguistic analyses of social networking and online
presentation. Personality and Individual Differences,51,5762.
Eagly, A.H.,& Karau, S.J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice towardfemale leaders.
Psychological Review,109,573598.
Ellison, N., Heino, R., & Gibbs, J. (2006). Managing impressions online: Self-presentation
processes inthe online dating environment. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communi-
cation,11,415441.
Emmons, R.A. (1987). Narcissism: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,52,1117.
Fox, J., & Rooney, M.C. (2015). The Dark Triad andtrait self-objectication as predictors of
men's use and self-presentation behaviors on socialnetworking sites. Personality and
Individual Differences,76,161165.
Grijalva, E., Newman, D.A., Tay, L., Donnellan, M.B., Harms, P.D., Robins, R.W., & Yan, T.
(2015). Gender d ifferences in nar cissism: A meta-analytic review. Psychological
Bulletin,141,261310.
Holtzman, N.S., & Strube, M.J. (2010). Narcissism and attractiveness. Journal of Researchin
Personality,44,133136.
Infographic: A closer look atsele. (2013, November 19). [Web log post]. Retrieved from
http://blog.oxforddictionaries.com/2013/11/an-infographic-of-sele/
Instagram (2014). Press news. Retrieved from http://instagram.com/press/
Jonason, P.K., & Webster, G.D. (2010). The Dirty Dozen: A concise measure of the Dark
Triad. Psychological Assessment,22,420432.
Kapidzic, S. (2013). Narcissism as a predictor of motivations behind Facebook prole pic-
ture selection. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking,16,1419.
Koenig, A.M., Eagly, A.H., Mitchell, A.A., & Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes mas-
culine? A meta-a nalysis of three research paradigms . Psychological B ulletin,137,
616642.
Kubarych,T.S., Deary, I.J., & Austin, E.J. (2004). The Narcissistic Personality Inventory: Fac-
tor structure in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Ind ividual Differences,36,
857872.
Mason, W., & Suri, S. (2012). Conducting behavioral research on Amazon's Mechanical
Turk. Behavior Research Methods,44,123.
McCrae, R.R., Kurtz, J.E., Yamagata, S.,& Terracciano, A. (2011). Internal consistency, retest
reliability, and their implications for personality scale validity. Personality and Social
Psychology Review,15,2850.
Mehdizadeh, S. (2010). Self-presentation 2.0: Narcissism and self-esteem on Facebook.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking,13,357364.
Morf, C.C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic
self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry,12,177196.
Panek, E.T.,Nardis, Y., & Konrath, S. (2013).Mirror or megaphone? How relationshipsbe-
tween narcissism and social networking site use diff er on Facebook and Twitte r.
Computers in Human Behavior,29,20042012.
Paulus, D.L., & Williams, K.M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machia-
vellianism, & psychopathy. Journal o f Research in Per sonality,36,556563.
Pearlman, J. (2013, Novembe r 19). Australian man inven ted the sele after drunken
night out..The Telegraph Retrieved from http://www.telegraph.co.uk.
Pew Research Cen ter (2013). Photos andvideo sharing grow online. Retrieved fromhttp://
www.pewinternet.org/2013/10/28/photo-and-video-sharing-grow-online/
Pew Research Center (2014a). Millennials in adulthood. Retrieved from http://www.
pewsocialtrends.org/2014/03/07/millennials-in-adulthood/
Pew Research Center (2014b). Social networking fact sheet. Retrieved from http://www.
pewinternet.org/fact-sheets/social-networking-fact-sheet/
Pew Research Center (2015). U.S. smartphone use in 2015. Retrieved from http://www.
pewinternet.org/2015/04/01/us-smartphone-use-in-2015/
Pincus, A.L., Ansell, E.B., Pimentel, C.A., Cain, N.M., Wright, A.G.C., & Levy, K.N. (2009). Ini-
tial constructi on and validation of the Patholog ical Narcissism I nventory.
Psychological Assessment,21,365379.
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Person-
ality Inventory and furtherevidence of its constructvalidity. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,54,890902.
Rudman, L.A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward
agentic women. Journal of Social Issues,57,743762.
Ryan, T., & Xenos, S. (2011). Who uses Facebook? An investigation into the relationship
between the Big Fi ve, shyness, narcissism, loneliness, and Faceboo k usage.
Computers in Human Behavior,27,16581664.
Sorokowski, P., Sorokowska, A., Oleszkiewicz, A., Frackowiak, T., Huk, A., & Pisanski, K.
(2015). Sele posting behaviors are ass ociated with nar cissism among men.
Personality and Individual Differences,85,123127.
Stevens, J. (2001). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (4th ed.). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Twenge, J.M., Konrath, S., Foster, J.D., Campbell, W.K., & Bushman, B.J. (2008). Egos inat-
ing over time: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of theNarcissistic Personality Invento-
ry. Journal of Personality,76,875901.
Whitty, M.T. (2008). Revealing the realme, searching for the actualyou: Presentations
of self on an internet dating site. Computers in Human Behavior,24,17071723.
481E.B. Weiser / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 477481
... Female scored high on Narcissism as compare to male. Previous research support that as you know that women are more emotional, sensitive and attention seeker as compare to men So that's why women have greater tendency of narcissism as compare to men (Weiser, 2015). Previous research support that as you know that women are more emotional, sensitive and attention seeker as compare to men. ...
... previous studies have shown that the levels of narcissism among study participants vary greatly by gender. As a result, women are generally more narcissistic than males, and research has shown there are substantial gender differences in narcissistic tendencies on social media (Weiser, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Aim of the Study: The aim of the study was to examine the connection between social comparison orientation in social networking sites and general health & well-being among university students. The study further evaluates how narcissism and self-esteem play a part in this relationship Methodology: The study based on correlational research design. A sample of 500 university students with (M =1.50 and SD=.50). Age range of participants was 18 to 30 years, taken from different universities students from Sargodha and Faisalabad, having an equal proportion of males and females. Only those university students who were currently using at least four social networking sites from last 6 months like Facebook, Instagram, snapchat, were selected. Unmarried university students and those with any physical and psychological illness and married were excluded from the study. Different measurement scales were used for the data collection like social comparison orientation Scale: (Gibbons & Buunk, 1999), General health & wellbeing scale short form of SF- 36 (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992), Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale (Hendin & Cheek, 2013), self-esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965). Findings: Result of study explained social comparison orientation and general health & well-being were positively correlated and subscales of social comparison orientation and general health & well-being. There is also a positive correlation between social comparison orientation and Narcissism and negative correlation with self-esteem among university students. Stepwise Regression analysis showed that General health & well-being was significantly predicted by social comparison orientation whereas narcissism was not identified as an important indicator in this outcome. Mean differences among study variables result showed that there were major differences in social comparison orientation, self-esteem while a non-significant difference on general health & well-being with relevance to gender and age among university students. Conclusion: Social comparison orientation effects negatively on general health and well-being in university students. It also relates negative self-esteem among them.
... According to past studies on selfies, there is a strong correlation between the attribute of narcissism and uploading selfies (Sung et al., 2016;Weiser, 2015). Therefore, one could argue that the invention of selfies sparked people's entrepreneurial skills, which in turn led to the narcissism of the selfie era. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: In the present digital age, young people place a greater emphasis on posting their images on social networking sites. As a result, the increasing usage of filters and photo manipulation to improve one's appearance in photos has sparked worries about potential mental health issues. Objectives: The present study seeks to explore the gender differences in photo-manipulation, along with examining the relationship of photo manipulation with self-esteem and narcissism among youth. Method: The sample consisted of young adults (N = 92) aged 18-29 years old who were active users of social media. The Google form consisting of the three psychometric tools namely, Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965), Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI-16) (Ames et al., 2006) and Self-Photo Manipulation Scale (McLean et al., 2015) was shared with the participants using convenience and snowball sampling technique. Independent samples t-test and Pearson's Product Moment Correlation were carried out for data analysis. Results: There were no significant gender differences in photo manipulation among males and females. Further, self-esteem and narcissism were not correlated with photo manipulation among youth. Conclusion: The findings of this study show that photo manipulation is used by both genders, and it is becoming normalized among today's generation.
... In a different study examining the relationship between selfie-taking behavior and narcissism in young people, high narcissism symptoms were observed in married people (Bhachech, 2021). Additionally, in another study on selfie and narcissism, it was found that selfies, which originate from social media, have a strong relationship with narcissism (Weiser, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective. The objective of this study was to adapt the NPI-13 for the Brazilian context, gathering psychometrics evidence. Method. A total of 520 participants completed the NPI-13, the Basic Values Questionnaire, the Five-Factor Inventory, and the Dirty Dozen. Results. The three-factor structure of the NPI-13 (Leadership/ Authority, Grandiosity/Exhibitionism, Entitlement/Exploitativeness) was supported (TLI > 0.90; RMSEA < 0.08). Correlations were found between the NPI-13 with achievement values and the dark dyad (i.e., psychopathy and Machiavellianism). The Leadership/Authority and Grandiosity/Exhibitionism factors were consistently correlated with extraversion, while Entitlement/Exploitativeness was correlated with neuroticism. In summary, evidence was gathered for the NPI-13 in the Brazilian context, indicating that it is a useful measure for capturing the multidimensionality of the construct in Brazil. Keywords. Narcissism, dark triad, personality, scale, psychometrics
Article
Full-text available
Selfitis is considered as an obsessive-compulsive desire to take photos of oneself and post them on social media either to enhance self-esteem or gain attention. Selfies refer to self-portraits taken by one using a digital camera or a smartphone (APA, January 2015; Social News Daily; Adobe Chronicle). Selfie-taking is increasingly popular among young adults without knowing that selfies reflect their owners’ personality traits and how people judge others’ personality. Thus, the current study aims to identify the different dimensions of self that get engaged and affected by mere selfie-taking behaviour. The research adopted a qualitative design and used a purposive sampling method to survey 1080 individuals aged 18-25 years, who were aware of and took selfies, to identify the various dimensions of self-associated with selfie taking. The thematic analysis of the responses indicated 11 dimensions of the individual self that get engaged and affected namely Self-Esteem, Happiness, Intimacy, Obsessive-Compulsive Behaviour, Narcissism, Sense of Independence, Self-Concept, Neuroticism, Attention Seeking, and Socio-Cultural Factors. These results help structuralise and comprehend the impact of selfie taking behaviour which help in self-assessment by excessive users and aid planning intervention.
Article
This study examines how influencers perform institutional entrepreneuring through social media. The study relies on the original institutional economics approach in order to explore the emulation of social identities expressed by the top ten profiles of women influencers on Instagram in 2019. 2946 posts from June to December 2019, are analyzed in this study. We classify these posts as selfies, beauty, lifestyle, culture, travel, social or environmental movements, donations, and product placement associated with technology. We address influencers as exhibitors of social identities and we analyze how their social media activity culminates in emulation as assets of institutional entrepreneuring. We were able to identify the main emulation items exposed by the influencers. The achieved results help us understand the social values expressed by influencers acting as institutional entrepreneurs in this age of social media.
Article
Full-text available
With rapid technological advances, social media has become an everyday form of human social interactions. For the first time in evolutionary history, people can now interact in virtual spaces where temporal, spatial, and embodied cues are decoupled from one another. What implications do these recent changes have for socio-cognitive phenotypes and mental disorders? We have conducted a systematic review on the relationships between social media use and mental disorders involving the social brain. The main findings indicate evidence of increased social media usage in individuals with psychotic spectrum phenotypes and especially among individuals with disorders characterized by alterations in the basic self, most notably narcissism, body dysmorphism, and eating disorders. These findings can be understood in the context of a new conceptual model, referred to here as ‘Delusion Amplification by Social Media’, whereby this suite of disorders and symptoms centrally involves forms of mentalistic delusions, linked with altered perception and perpetuation of distorted manifestations of the self, that are enabled and exacerbated by social media. In particular, an underdeveloped and incoherent sense of self, in conjunction with ‘real life’ social isolation that inhibits identify formation and facilitates virtual social interactions, may lead to use of social media to generate and maintain a more or less delusional sense of self identity. The delusions involved may be mental (as in narcissism and erotomania), or somatic (as in body dysmorphic disorder and eating disorders, encompassing either the entire body or specific body parts). In each case, the virtual nature of social media facilitates the delusionality because the self is defined and bolstered in this highly mentalistic environment, where real-life exposure of the delusion can be largely avoided. Current evidence also suggests that increased social media usage, via its disembodied and isolative nature, may be associated with psychotic spectrum phenotypes, especially delusionality, by the decoupling of inter and intra-corporeal cues integral to shared reality testing, leading to the blurring of self-other boundaries.
Chapter
Digital technologies have had disruptive effects on self, self-identity, community, race, and ethnicity imaginations. Where the conception of the digital communities have moved beyond the immediate geographic locations to group of networked persons across the geographies, race and ethnicity identification as such have also become sharper as evident from the increased identification of people with similar physical or racial attributes.
Article
Full-text available
Despite the widely held belief that men are more narcissistic than women, there has been no systematic review to establish the magnitude, variability across measures and settings, and stability over time of this gender difference. Drawing on the biosocial approach to social role theory, a meta-analysis performed for Study 1 found that men tended to be more narcissistic than women (d = .26; k = 355 studies; N = 470,846). This gender difference remained stable in U.S. college student cohorts over time (from 1990 to 2013) and across different age groups. Study 1 also investigated gender differences in three facets of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) to reveal that the narcissism gender difference is driven by the Exploitative/Entitlement facet (d = .29; k = 44 studies; N = 44,108) and Leadership/Authority facet (d = .20; k = 40 studies; N = 44,739); whereas the gender difference in Grandiose/Exhibitionism (d = .04; k = 39 studies; N = 42,460) was much smaller. We further investigated a less-studied form of narcissism called vulnerable narcissism-which is marked by low self-esteem, neuroticism, and introversion-to find that (in contrast to the more commonly studied form of narcissism found in the DSM and the NPI) men and women did not differ on vulnerable narcissism (d = -.04; k = 42 studies; N = 46,735). Study 2 used item response theory to rule out the possibility that measurement bias accounts for observed gender differences in the three facets of the NPI (N = 19,001). Results revealed that observed gender differences were not explained by measurement bias and thus can be interpreted as true sex differences. Discussion focuses on the implications for the biosocial construction model of gender differences, for the etiology of narcissism, for clinical applications, and for the role of narcissism in helping to explain gender differences in leadership and aggressive behavior. Readers are warned against overapplying small effect sizes to perpetuate gender stereotypes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2014 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated 3 broad classes of individual-differences variables (job-search motives, competencies, and constraints) as predictors of job-search intensity among 292 unemployed job seekers. Also assessed was the relationship between job-search intensity and reemployment success in a longitudinal context. Results show significant relationships between the predictors employment commitment, financial hardship, job-search self-efficacy, and motivation control and the outcome job-search intensity. Support was not found for a relationship between perceived job-search constraints and job-search intensity. Motivation control was highlighted as the only lagged predictor of job-search intensity over time for those who were continuously unemployed. Job-search intensity predicted Time 2 reemployment status for the sample as a whole, but not reemployment quality for those who found jobs over the study's duration. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
In an experiment, job description and applicants' attributes were examined as moderators of the backlash effect, the negative evaluation of agentic women for violating prescriptions of feminine niceness (Rudman, 1998). Rutgers University students made hiring decisions for a masculine or “feminized” managerial job. Applicants were presented as either agentic or androgynous. Replicating Rudman and Glick (1999), a feminized job description promoted hiring discrimination against an agentic female because she was perceived as insufficiently nice. Unique to the present research, this perception was related to participants' possession of an implicit (but not explicit) agency-communality stereotype. By contrast, androgynous female applicants were not discriminated against. The findings suggest that the prescription for female niceness is an implicit belief that penalizes women unless they temper their agency with niceness.
Article
Although many studies have investigated individual differences in online social networking, few have examined the recent and rapidly popularized social phenomenon of the “selfie” (a selfportrait photograph of oneself). In two studies with a pooled sample of 1296 men and women, we tested the prediction that individuals who score high on four narcissism sub-scales (Self-sufficiency, Vanity, Leadership, and Admiration Demand) will be more likely to post selfies to social media sites than will individuals who exhibit low narcissism. We examined three categories of selfies: own selfies; selfies with a romantic partner; and group selfies, controlling for non-selfie photographs. Women posted more selfies of all types than did men. However, women’s selfie-posting behavior was generally unrelated to their narcissism scores. In contrast, men’s overall narcissism scores positively predicted posting own selfies, selfies with a partner, and group selfies. Moreover, men’s Vanity, Leadership, and Admiration Demand scores each independently predicted the posting of one or more types of selfies. Our findings provide the first evidence that the link between narcissism and selfie-posting behavior is comparatively weak among women than men, and provide novel insight into the social motivations and functions of online social networking.
Article
An online survey of a nationally representative sample of U.S. men aged 18-40 assessed trait predictors of social networking site use as well as two forms of visual self-presentation: editing one’s image in photographs posted on social networking sites (SNSs) and posting “selfies,” or pictures users take of themselves. We examined the Dark Triad (i.e., narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy) and trait self-objectification as predictors. Self-objectification and narcissism predicted time spent on SNSs. Narcissism and psychopathy predicted the number of selfies posted, whereas narcissism and self-objectification predicted editing photographs of oneself posted on SNSs. We discuss selective self-presentation processes on social media and how these traits may influence interpersonal relationship development in computer-mediated communication.
Article
Abstract The rising popularity of social networking sites raises the question of whether and how personality differences are manifested on them. The present study explores this topic through an analysis of the relationship between narcissism and motivations behind Facebook profile picture selection. A survey that assesses motivations emphasizing physical attractiveness, personality, and social ties was conducted with 288 undergraduate students. The study found narcissism to be a significant predictor of the motivation for selecting profile pictures that emphasize attractiveness and personality for both men and women. The findings are discussed in terms of the dynamic self-regulatory processing model of narcissism.
Article
A survey (N = 292) was conducted that measured self-promoting Facebook behaviors (e.g. posting status updates and photos of oneself, updating profile information) and several anti-social behaviors (e.g. seeking social support more than one provides it, getting angry when people do not comment on one's status updates, retaliating against negative comments). The grandiose exhibitionism subscale of the narcissistic personality inventory was hypothesized to predict the self-promoting behaviors. The entitlement/explo-itativeness subscale was hypothesized to predict the anti-social behaviors. Results were largely consistent with the hypothesis for the self-promoting behaviors but mixed concerning the anti-social behaviors. Trait self-esteem was also related in the opposite manner as the Narcissism scales to some Facebook behaviors.