Conference PaperPDF Available

Charisma perception in political speech: A case study

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
Charisma perception in political speech: a case study
Rosario Signorello, Francesca D’Errico, Isabella Poggi, Didier Demolin, Paolo
Mairano
To cite this version:
Rosario Signorello, Francesca D’Errico, Isabella Poggi, Didier Demolin, Paolo Mairano.
Charisma perception in political speech: a case study. Heliana Mello, Massimo Pettorino,
Tommaso Raso. International Conference on Speech and Corpora (GSCP 2012), Feb 2012,
Belo Horizonte, Brazil. Firenze University Press, pp.343-348. <hal-00820359>
HAL Id: hal-00820359
https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00820359
Submitted on 18 May 2013
HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access
archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci-
entific research documents, whether they are pub-
lished or not. The documents may come from
teaching and research institutions in France or
abroad, or from public or private research centers.
L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est
destin´ee au epˆot et `a la diffusion de documents
scientifiques de niveau recherche, publi´es ou non,
´emanant des ´etablissements d’enseignement et de
recherche fran¸cais ou ´etrangers, des laboratoires
publics ou priv´es.
Charisma perception in political speech: a case study
Rosario SIGNORELLO
1,2
,FrancescaD'ERRICO
2
, Isabella POGGI
2
, Didier DEMOLIN
1
,
Paolo MAIRANO
1
1
GIPSA-Lab, UMR 5216 CNRS, Grenoble INP, UJF, U. Stendhal (Grenoble, France);
2
Dipartimento di Scienze
dell’Educazione,Universita`degliStudiRomaTre(Rome,Italy)
rosario.signorello@gipsa-lab.fr, didier.demolin@gipsa-lab.fr, fderrico@uniroma3.it, poggi@uniroma3.it,
paolomairano@gmail.com
Abstract
The charisma of the leader is conveyed through multiple aspects: his ideas and vision and his perceivable verbal and non verbal
behaviors. Among these perceivable behaviors there are the acoustic characteristics of speech. We present here a study on the
perception of charisma in political speech. We collected speech statements with different illocutionary value taken from two speeches
given by Umberto Bossi, the leader of an Italian party, before and after a stroke which caused him a voice disorder. Stimuli from the
two condition differed significantly in the acoustic-prosodic features. In the first part of the study 40 French listeners rated normal
speech stimuli (20 pre- and 20 post-stroke) and in the second part 22 French (11 pre- and 11 post-stroke) and 31 Italians (15 pre- and 16
post-stroke) rated the de-lexicalizedversionofthesamestimuli.ResultsforthefirstpartofthestudyshowthatpitchcontourinBossi’s
pre-stroke speech positively influence the perception of his speech as charismatic, as opposed to those some years after the stroke.
Results for the de-lexicalized speech confirm for French listeners our hypothesis of the influence of the pitch contour in Bossi’s
charisma perception but they are controversial for Italian participants that seem to perceive Bossi as more charismatic in the
post-stroke condition.
Keywords: charisma; political speech; intonation; illocution; voice disorder; speech synthesis.
1. Introduction
Charisma was firstly described by Weber as an
“extraordinaryquality”ofapersonwhoisbelievedtobe
endowed with superhuman properties thanks to which
s/he gets ac- knowledged as a leader Cavalli, 1995: 5).
Though no specific objective description of the
“extraordinary quality” was given in Webers studies,
some works started to study the perceivable behaviors of
charismatic leaders: some, e.g., (Boss, 1976), focus on
whatwecalledthe“charismaofthemind”(Signorelloet
al.,2012),thatdwellsinthestrengthofaleadersideas,
others, e.g., (Atkinson, 1984) try to find visually or
acoustically perceivable aspects of a leaders behaviors
thatwecalled“charismaofthebody”(Signorelloet al.,
2012). We suggest that both aspects of charisma, either
jointly or independently, are responsible for its conveying
and perception.
In the present study we focus on one aspect of the
charisma of the body: the speech. We assume here that
some of the perceivable acoustic-prosodic characteristics
of a leaders speech are specifically responsible for
conveying charisma. Our general goal is to characterize
acoustically and distinguish perceptually a charismatic
speech from a non- charismatic one.
Within previous work investigating the relationship
be- tween the acoustic-prosodic characteristics of a
political leaders speech and the perception of his/her
charisma, Rosenberg and Hirschberg, 2009) studied the
correlation between acoustic, prosodic, and
lexico-syntactic characteristics of political speech and the
perception of charisma; Touati (1993) investigated the
prosodic features of rhetoric utterances in French political
speech in pre and post- elections discourses. Other works
examined the relationship between prosodic features
andtheperceptionofaspeakerasa“goodcommunicator”
(Strangert & Gustafson, 2008) or analyzed the pitch
contour of French political leaders’ speech and its
idiosyncratic and contextual variations (Martin, 2009).
2. A hypothesis about charisma
According to Poggi (2005), in persuasive discourse the
speaker tries to convince the audience to do some action by
exploiting the three strategies posited by Aristotle (2011):
Logos (the rational argument), Pathos (the appeal to the au-
diences emotions), and Ethos (the character of the speaker).
According to the theory of Poggi (2005) and Poggi et al.
(2011), the dimension of Ethos also includes, for the
political leader, three sub-dimensions: Benevolence (the
tendency to act in the interest of the audience), Competence
(the capacity for rational foreseeing and planning), and
Dominance (the power to prevail in a competition).
The notion of charisma we proposed in (Signorello
et al., 2012) is based on this theoretical framework. We
de- fined charisma as a set of characteristics of a leader
that include his “having a vision” (a goal towards which
he wants to lead his followers), a “high level of
dominance” (look strong, persistent and fighting) and
“emotional intelligence”(the ability to feel and transmit
emotions, and to be and look empathic). The combination
of these features makes a leader charismatic, and is
displayed by his/her non-communicative and
communicative behavior..
3. What makes a speech charismatic?
To investigate the perception of charisma in political
speech we analyzed the acoustic and prosodic
characteristics in the speech of Umberto Bossi, an Italian
politician who in 2004, during his political career, had a
stroke that resulted in severe speech impairment. We
collected two samples taken from two speeches
performed, respectively, in 1994 (the pre-stroke condition,
PRE) and in 2011 (the post-stroke condition, POST). Our
hypothesis was that the important differences in
Heliana Mello, Massimo Pettorino, Tommaso Raso (edited by), Proceedings of the VIIth GSCP International Conference : Speech and Corpora
ISBN 978-88-6655-351-9 (online) © 2012 Firenze University Press.
344 ROSARIO SIGNORELLO, FRANCESCAD’ERRICO,ISABELLA POGGI, DIDIER DEMOLIN, PAOLO MAIRANO
DIMENSION
PRE
POST
Pathos
passionate, empathetic,
enthusiastic, reassuring
cold, indif-
ferent
Ethos
Benevolence
extraverted, positive,
spontaneous, trustwor-
thy, honest, fair, friendly,
easygoing, makes the
others feel important
untrustworthy
dishonest,
egocentric,
individ-
ualistic,
introverted
Ethos Com-
petence
visionary, organized,
smart, sagacious, cre-
ative, competent, wise,
enterprising, deter-
mined, resolute, who
propose, seductive,
exuberant, sincere, clear,
communicative
inefficient,
inadequate,
uncertain,
faithless,
unclear,
menacing
Ethos Domi-
nance
dynamic, calm, active,
courageous, confident,
vigorous, strong, leader,
authoritarian, captivat-
ing, who persuade, who
convince
apathetic,
timorous,
weak, con-
formist,
unimpor-
tant, who
scare
Emotional
Induction
Effects
charming, attractive,
pleasant, sexy, bewitch-
ing, eloquent, influential
boring
acoustic-prosodic characteristics of Bossi’s speech, in
samples of political speeches preceding and following the
stroke, give rise to a different perception of charisma. If
this hypothesis is validated perceptually we might
conclude that information about charismatic qualities are
borne by the acoustic-prosodic characteristics that differ
in the two samples.
In order to describe the charisma phenomenon
through common language adjectives we conducted a
qualitative study collecting adjectives describing what
charisma is and what it is not (a brief summary is
presented in section 3.1.. For the extensive study see
(Signorello et al., 2012). We then analysed Bossis
acoustic-prosodic features in the PRE and POST and
conducted a language-independent perceptual study on
French participants (section 3.2.4.). We then
de-lexicalized our stimuli by synthesis only preserving
the pitch contour, the duration and the intensity and
conducted a perceptual study on French and Italian
listeners. In isolating the pitch contour we could verify if
this is the aspect that influences the perception of
charismainBossisspeech(section3.3.).
3.1 Describing charisma
In a previous work (Signorello et al., 2012) we
constructed a questionnaire aimed to assess the perception
ofcharismainthesamplesofBossi’sspeechrequiredto
previously make up a list of adjectives that express
charismatic and non-charismatic qualities. To find out
such adjectives in an empirically grounded way, we
administered a questionnaire through Internet to 58
French participants (42 female, 16 male, mean age 30),
asking to freely generate adjectives connected to the idea
of what charisma is and what it is not. We obtained a list
of French adjectives, 106 describing charisma positively
and 105 describing what charisma is not. In order to make
a manageable questionnaire, we further selected 67
adjectives (Table 1) retaining only those occurring more
than once, 42 positively and 20 negatively related with
charisma. We then classified those adjectives in a
multidimensional scale of charisma under five
dimensions describing this phenomenon. An extended
report of this multidimensional scale of charisma and on
how adjectives describing charisma are classified in it can
be founded in (Signorello et al., 2012).
3.2 Normal Speech
3.2.1. Stimuli
Previous works about the perception of a speaker as a
good (Strangert & Gustafson, 2008) or charismatic
speaker (Rosenberg & Hirschberg, 2009) rely on the
acoustic analysis and the perceptual evaluation of stimuli
classified per speaker, topic and genre of speech. Our
approach is different. We chose 3 stimuli per condition
(PRE and POST) according to their illocutionary value:
an assertion, an in- citation and a rhetorical wh- question.
As we know the speaker shapes prosody differently in
relation to different speech acts (Firenzuoli, 2001). Our
hypothesis is that all three types of speech acts are
perceived as more charismatic in the PRE condition
thanks to prosodic features. Further we argue that
incitation might be perceived as more charismatic than
rhetorical question which in turn might be perceived as
more charismatic than assertion. Below we describe the
acoustic-prosodic features of our stimuli.
Table 1: The 67 positive and negative adjectives related
with charisma collected amongthena¨ıveFrench
participants (in English for clarity purposes). Reprinted
from Signorello et al., 2012)
3.2.2. Overall F0 measures
The PRE speech presents higher F0 means than the POST
speech: PRE (F0 mean 178.89 Hz; min 101.84 Hz; max
241.10 Hz), POST (F0 mean 120.20 Hz; min 91.78 Hz;
max 155.99 Hz). All means from the PRE differ
significantly from the POST (p<0.0001). Our findings
confirm and extend (Murry, 1978)’s findings on
significant differences in F0 measures between normal
and disordered voice. We argue that F0 values might be
positively correlated to charisma perception.
3.2.3. Pitch contour description
The assertion in the PRE condition (Figure 1a below)
presentsasyntacticfocuson“questo”[this],emphasized
by a high fall and separated by a pause from the rest of the
sentence. The right-side part of the tonal unit presents a
falling contour with a small peak on the last tonic syllable.
Instead, in the POST condition (Figure 2a below) the
CHARISMA PERCEPTION IN POLITICAL SPEECH: A CASE STUDY 345
sentence presents a moderate falling and flat pitch contour
with a peak on the third lexical word. The incitation in the
PRE condition (Figure 1b) includes two parts, each with a
(a)
(b)
(c)
Figure 1: Intonation contour, transcription, translation,
du- ration and F0 measures of PRE stimuli per speech act.
(a):Assertion.“Questo amici ereditiamo”[This,my
friends, is what we inherit]. 3,51s. F0 mean 52.62 Hz; SD
12.40 Hz; min 95.25 Hz; max 210.94 Hz; range 13 ST. (b):
Incitation.“Siritornaall’attacco,fuoridalletrincee”
[Let’stakeupagaintheoffensive,getoutofthetrenches].
4.27s. F0 mean 225.51 Hz; SD 38.58 Hz; min 107.74 Hz;
max 270.36 Hz; range 16 ST. (c): Rhetorical wh- question.
Ecomefacevamoafarlo?” [How could we have done
it?]. 1.81s. F0 mean 138.28 Hz; SD 27.98 Hz; min 96.07
Hz; max 189.39 Hz; range 11.72 ST. Spectrogram and
pitch contour graphics obtained with WinPitch software
(Martin, 2011)
pitch contour starting with high frequency and falling
sharply in the last tonic syllable. In the POST condition
instead the incitation (Figure 2b) presents two
rising-falling contours in the first part and falls gradually
in the right part of the tonal unit. The rhetorical wh-
question in the PRE condition (Figure 1c) presents two
contiguous pitch contour movements: the rising part
corresponds to the wh- element, the falling part
corresponds to the verb. A gradual falling movement
comes on the right side of the tonal unit. In the POST
statement (Figure 2c) a falling contour corresponds to the
wh- element and a rising contour to the verbal element,
with a gradual falling movement on the right side of the
tonal unit.
3.2.4. Perception experiment
Forty French participants with no knowledge of Italian
rated the stimuli presented in the section above via a
HTML/PHP browser-based interface. Twenty of them
listened to the PRE condition and twenty to the POST
condition stimuli. The test took place in an anechoic
chamber and participants wore a Sennheiser HD 25-13
headphone. After listening to each stimulus a participant
had to answer to some check questions to verify that the
perception of the acoustic signal was optimal and that the
semantic content was not understood. Then they had to
express their judgment about the stimuli through our
67-adjective inventory on a 7-point Likert scale
(a)
(b)
(c)
Figure 2: Intonation contour, transcription, translation,
du- ration and F0 measures of POST stimuli per speech
act. (a):Assertion.Noi siamo schiavi del centralismo
romano”[WeareslavesoftheRomancentralism].2,46 s,
F0 mean 116.77 Hz, SD 10.74 Hz, min 86.64 Hz, max
146.45 Hz, range 9 ST. (b):Incitation.“La Lega e`
pronto per conquistare la liberta` della padania”[The
Lega is ready to conquer the freedom of padania]. 6.61s,
F0 mean 142.02 Hz, SD 38.58 Hz, min 86.2 Hz, max
182.08 Hz, range 12 ST. (c): Rhetorical wh- question“E
come fanno a lavorare questa gente?”[How can these
people work?]. 1.89 s, F0 mean 117.93 Hz, SD 15.54 Hz,
min 90.56 Hz, max 192.99 Hz, range 13 ST. Spectrogram
and pitch contour graphics obtained with WinPitch
software (Martin, 2011)
(0 = “totally disagree”, 7 = “totally agree”), with some
adjectives from the list substituted by their reverses (e.g.,
warm instead of cold) to avoid answer habituation. The
average duration of the test was of
20 minutes.
3.2.5. Results
From our check questions it resulted that perception was
good and there was no semantic comprehension. Hence,
the differences between PRE and POST, that are mostly
significant (t-test, p<0.05), must be due only to acoustic
and not to semantic features. Out of the 67 adjectives used
to measure the perception of charisma, about 33
adjectives obtained significantly different values (t-test,
p<0.05) between PRE and POST speech, and most of
them were rated higher for the PRE condition (Table 2
below). This is consistent with our hypothesis about the
PRE speech as more charismatic than the POST thanks to
its acoustic features. The PRE speech is positively
correlated with most adjectives describing charismatic
qualities (Table 1 below). In the dimension of Pathos the
speaker is perceived as passionate, eloquent and
enthusiastic in the PRE and as indifferent in the POST. As
346 ROSARIO SIGNORELLO, FRANCESCAD’ERRICO,ISABELLA POGGI, DIDIER DEMOLIN, PAOLO MAIRANO
DIMENSION
PRE
POST
Pathos
passionate (5.02), enthu-
siastic (3.25)
indifferent
(2.83)
Ethos Benev-
olence
egocentric (4.51), dis-
honest (3.95), makes the
others feel important
(3.68), individualistic
(4.29)
trustworthy
(3.51), in-
troverted
(2.41)
Ethos Compe-
tence
competent (4.83), smart
(4.52), organized (4.75),
determined (5.51), exu-
berant (4.57), faithless
(3.57), clear (4.65), com-
municative (4.25), se-
ductive (3.17)
wise
(3.90),
unclear
(3.37)
Ethos Domi-
nance
dynamic (5.13), author-
itarian (5.73), confident
(5.89), leader (5.87),
captivating (3.57),
convincing (4.40),
captivating (4.78)
calm
(4.29)
Emotional
Induction
Effects
attractive (3.10), elo-
quent (4.68), charming
(4.78)
boring
(3.63)
to Ethos Benevolence results are quite inconsistent: the
adjectives attributed to the PRE speech include egocentric,
dishonest and individualistic, which in our previous
qualitative study (Table 1) are non-charismatic qualities.
Table 2: Adjectives describing the perception of charisma
intheBossi’sspeechbyconditionwithratingvalues
(t-test, p<.001)
Table 3: Adjectives describing the perception of charisma
intheBossi’sspeechbyspeechact(A=assertion,
I=incitation, Q=rethorical wh- question) and condition
with rating values and one-wayANOVAsvalues
(p<.001). Higher rates in bold
As for the dimensions of Ethos Competence and
Ethos Dominance our hypothesis is almost completely
validated: the speaker is perceived as competent, smart,
clear, seductive, etc. in the PRE and as unclear in the
POST; as dynamic, authoritarian, confident, leader in the
PRE and as boring in the POST speech. These results
validate our hypothesis on the attribution of charismatic
qualities to the PRE as opposed to the POST speech.
Taking into account the different types of speech act
both in the PRE and in the POST speech the different
illocutionary act elicitates a different perception. The
incitation is the one that influences the most the
perception of charisma. In particular for the dimension of
Ethos Competence the incitation elicitates adjectives as
competent (F(2, 123)=3.114; p<0.048), resolute (F(2,
123)=6.767; p<0.002), enterprising (F(2, 123)=8.515;
p<0.001), clear (F(2, 123)=3.046; p<0.05), exuberant
(F(2, 123)=4.232; p<0.017) and communicative (F(2,
123)=2.705; p<0.05). More than other speech acts the
incitation has a significant effect on the perception of the
speakers emotional state (see adjectives as passionate
(F(2, 123)=2.999; p<0.05), influential (F(2, 123)=9.359;
p<0.001) and enthusiastic (F(2, 123)=4.765; p<0.010)).
The assertion on the other hand evokes more
non-charismatic qualities like indifferent (F(2,
123)=3.459; p<0.035) and unclear (F(2, 123)=3.662;
p<0.029). Finally the rhetorical question seems to not
influence a specific dimension of charisma. However, if
we consider effect of both condition and a particular
speech act the results are quite different. Through a
one-way ANOVA we crossed the results of the condition
(PRE vs. POST) and the different types of speech act
(assertion, incitation and rhetorical wh- question) to study
the influence of the different illocutionary acts on the
perception of Bossi’s charisma (see Table 3). The
incitation makes Bossi to be perceived as more dynamic,
authoritarian, active, menacing, and energic in the PRE
condition and as extraverted and wise in the POST
condition. Through the Assertion he has been perceived
as individualistic in the PRE speech and as calm and
introverted in the POST speech. As for the rhetorical wh-
question the only significantly results is timorous in the
PRE speech.
3.3 Synthesized speech
3.3.1. Stimuli
We decided to carry out a perceptive test on de-lexicalized
stimuli in order to further validate our hypothesis that the
pitch contour is a relevant element for the perception of
charisma. In fact, our de-lexicalization procedure enables
us to isolate the pitch contour of a sentence from the
semantic content, segmental features and voice quality
characteristics. In this way, listeners are therefore forced
to give their judgments solely on the basis of intonation,
all other linguistic information being eliminated. The
de-lexicalized procedure we chose has been developed for
the AMPER (Atlas Multimedia Prosodique de l’Espace
Roman) project developed by Albert Rilliard on the basis
of scripts originally elaborated by Antonio Romano (see
Contini et al., 2002 for details). It consists in synthesizing
a periodic waveform with the original pitch, intensity and
duration values of the actual sentence (this is done by
taking three measures per vowel, respectively at the onset,
PRE
POST
AD J EC T I VE S
A
I
Q
A
I
Q
dynamic
5
5.45
4.42
2.09
3.09
2.14
authoritarian
6.19
6.42
4.57
3.61
4.04
3.61
calm
2.66
1.76
3.42
4.61
4.14
4.09
extraverted
1.66
1.23
2
2.14
3.14
2.9
timorous
3
2.38
3.95
3.76
3.38
3.42
wise
2.85
2.28
3.38
3.95
4.23
3.52
individualistic
4.81
4.61
3.42
3.28
3.33
3.71
active
4.9
5.66
4.28
2.28
2.81
3.52
introverted
1.52
1.14
2.33
2.81
2.19
2.23
menacing
4.57
5.33
3.33
3
2.9
2.85
energic
5.14
6.09
4.52
2
2.9
3.38
CHARISMA PERCEPTION IN POLITICAL SPEECH: A CASE STUDY 347
middle and offset-consonants are replaced with silence).
This procedure has been used by several authors working
on the AMPER project and has already proved its
efficacy.
3.3.2. Perception experiment
Twenty-two French (11 PRE, 11 POST) and thirty-one
Italian (15 PRE, 16 POST) listeners participated to a
perception analysis with the same methodology described
in section 3.2.4. Thus the only differences were the
de-lexicalized stimuli.
3.3.3. Results
The first results for the de-lexicalized stimuli perception,
compared to results for normal speech perceptions,
confirm in one hand our hypothesis of the influence of the
pitch con- tourinBossi’scharismaperceptionforFrench
participants but they are, in the other hand, controversial
for Italian participants. In fact French listeners describe
Bossi as charming, who propose, timorous, confident,
pleasant, introverted in the PRE speech and as inadequate,
spontaneous, active, leader in the POST speech (t-test,
p<0.05). For Italian participants we only performed the
perceptual test of de-lexicalized stimuli in order to avoid
semantic and ideology influence on the perception of
Bossi’sspeech.Italianlistenersperceivedthespeakeras
boring, indifferent and unimportant in the PRE speech
and as attractive, visionary, sexy, cold, passionate,
seductive in the POST speech (t-test, p<0.05). From these
preliminary results it seems that the pitch contour-only
stimuli elicit a different type of of Bossis charisma for
Italians listeners. In fact the POST speech is described
with adjectives positively related with charisma and the
PRE speech with adjectives describing charisma
negatively, a trend in results that goes against our theory
of pre-stroke speech as more charismatic than the
post-stroke.
4. Conclusion
In this study we aimed to demonstrate that the perception
of charisma in political speech is partly determined by the
acoustic characteristics of speech. To do so, we first
analyzed samples from the speech of the Italian politician
Umberto Bossi before and after a stroke; through a
qualitative study we singled out 67 adjectives describing
charismatic and non-charismatic qualities. finally we run
a perception study asking participants to rate Bossi’s
samples in terms of those adjectives. As resulted from the
acoustic analysis, the PRE speech, with its intonation
features as focus words, tonal jumps, and higher values,
dramatically differs from the POST. And since the results
of the perception study validate our hypothesis that
Bossi’s speech after the stroke is perceived as less
charismatic than before, we may reasonably conclude that
the characteristics of intonationthatdifferentiateBossi’s
PRE and POST speeches are an important factor in the
perception of charisma. And this hypothesis has been
validated once more through a perceptual experiment in
which we only tested the intonation contour influence on
the Bossi’s charisma perception. We also de-lexicalized
stimuli and preserved original pitch, intensity and
duration values and we tested French and Italian
participants. Results validate our hypothesis on the
intonation contour relevance on charisma perception of
the PRE speech for French participants but are
controversial for Italians. In any case our results on
synthesized speech are preliminary and they will be
statistically analyzed more in depth. Naturally we are
aware that the acoustic characteristics of speech also
include voice quality, which we think is relevant too. In
future work we will investigate the importance of voice
quality in determining the perception of charisma, while
trying to distinguish it from the contribution of intonation,
also through synthesis of speech fragments.
5. Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Christophe Savariaux (GIPSA-
Lab, CNRS, Grenoble, France) for his technical support
in acoustic analysis. Research partly supported by a
Doctoral Fellowship from the French Ministry of
National Education and Research (years 2010-2013) and
by the 7th Framework Program, European Network of
Excellence SSPNet (Social Signal Processing Network),
Grant Agreement N. 231287.
6. Acknowledgements
Aristotle. (2011). Rhetoric. The Internet Classics Archive.
Atkinson, M. (1984). OurMasters’Voices:Language
and Body Language of Politics. Routledge, London,
UK.
Bligh, M.C., Robinson, J.L. (2010). Was
Gandhi ”charismatic”? Exploring the rhetorical
leadership of Mahatma Gandhi. In The Leadership
Quarterly, 21(5), pp. 844--855.
Boss, P. (1976). Essential attributes of charisma. In
Southern Speech Communication Journal, 41(3), pp.
300--313.
Cavalli, L. (1995). Carisma: la qualita` straordinaria del
leader. Laterza, Rome, Italy.
Contini, M., Lai, J.-P., Romano, A., Roullet, S., de Castro
Moutinho, L., Coimbra, R., Bendiha, U. P. and Ruivo,
S.S. (2002).Unprojetd’atlasmultime´diaprosodique
del’espaceroman.InB.Bel,L.Marlien(Eds.),Speech
Prosody 2002, Aix- en-Provence, France, pp. 227--230.
Firenzuoli, V. (2001). Le forme intonative di valore
allocu- tivo dell’italiano parlato. Analisi sperimentale
di un cor- pus di parlato spontaneo (LABLITA). PhD
thesis, Uni- versita` degli Studi di Firenze.
Goleman, D.P. (1995). Emotional Intelligence: Why It
Can Matter More Than IQ for Character, Health and
Lifelong Achievement. Bantam Books, New York,
USA.
Martin, P. (2009). Autres styles: quelques variantes id-
iosyncratiques. In Intonation du Français, Armand
Colin, pp. 228--232.
Martin, P. (2011). WinPitch W7 by Pitch Instruments Inc.
Computer program. Available at:
< http://www.winpitch.com/>.
348 ROSARIO SIGNORELLO, FRANCESCAD’ERRICO,ISABELLA POGGI, DIDIER DEMOLIN, PAOLO MAIRANO
Mohammadi, G., Mortillaro, M., and Vinciarelli, A.
(2010). The voice of personality: Mapping nonverbal
vocal behavior into trait attributions. In International
Workshop on Social Signal Processing, pp. 17--20.
Murry, T. (1978). Speaking fundamental frequency
characteristics associated with voice pathologies. In
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 43, pp.
374--379.
Poggi, I. (2005). The goals of persuasion. In Pragmatics
& Cognition, 13(2), pp. 297--336.
Poggi,I.,D’Errico,F.,andVincze,L.(2011).Discrediting
moves in political debate. In P. Ricci-Bitti (Ed.),
Proceedings of UMMS 2011, Springer LNCS, pp.
84--99.
Rosenberg, A., Hirschberg, J. (2009). Charisma
perception from text and speech. In Speech
Communication, 51(7), pp. 640--655.
Signorello, R., D’Errico, F., Poggi, I., and Demolin, D.
(2012). How charisma is perceived from speech. A
multidimensional approach. In ASE/IEEE International
Conference on Social Computing 2012, Amsterdam,
The Netherlands.
Strangert, E., Gustafson, J. (2008). What makes a good
speaker? subject ratings, acoustic measurements and
per- ceptual evaluations. In Interspeech 2008, pp.
1688--1691.
Touati, P. (1993). Prosodic aspects of political rhetoric. In
ESCA Workshop on Prosody, Lund, Sweden, pp.
168--171.
... За умов, що склалися, соціологи (Bass, 1998;Howell, 1988 . До визначення особистісних характеристик харизматичного мовця долучились і лінгвісти, що було зумовлено необхідністю більш об'єктивного добору персоналій для лінгвістичних досліджень усного й письмового харизматичного мовлення (Signorello et al., 2012). Проте питання систематизації невербальних ознак харизматичної особистості публічного мовця та методологічних особливостей їх вживання в описах результатів міждисциплінарних досліджень свого остаточного вирішення ще не знайшло. ...
... Окрім того, науковці та експерти у галузі політології, менеджменту, соціології й психології відзначають важливість невербальних аспектів сприйняття аудиторією лідера як харизматичного, зокрема послідовники «зчитують» інформацію про лідера-мовця на основі певних фізичних характеристик: виразу обличчя (важливою для сприйняття харизми є посмішка), зовнішності, постави, положення тіла, голосу, жестів, погляду, зросту, ваги, одягу (Reh, 2016). Важливість невербальних ознак підтверджується і лінгвістичними доробками, зокрема, група науковців (Signorello et al., 2012) досліджувала, наскільки змінилося сприйняття аудиторією мовлення політика, який пережив інсульт: якщо до цього випадку він сприймався як харизматичний лідер завдяки зовнішнім ознакам та енергійному, швидкому, високотонному мовленню, то після хвороби, коли політик мав певні труднощі з артикуляцією, диханням і голосом, його харизматичність у сприйнятті послідовниками зникла. Крім того, під час експериментальних лінгвістичних досліджень було доведено, що стать лідера-мовця впливає на сприйняття такої особистості як харизматичної: згідно з результатами експерименту жінок і чоловіків сприймають як однаково харизматичних, незважаючи на те, що мовлення жінок-лідерів має більш високі показники характеристик, що визначають мовлення як харизматичне (Niebuhr et al., 2016;Jokisch et al., 2018). ...
Article
Стаття містить обґрунтування раціонального термінологічного мінімуму понять, які вже функціонують у науці та які доцільно вживати на позначення невербальних ознак харизматичної особистості публічного мовця під час опису результатів міждисциплінарних досліджень численних феноменів його комунікативної поведінки. Серед безлічі міждисциплінарних ознак харизматичної особистості публічного мовця, що функціонують наразі в гуманітарних науках, зокрема соціології, психології, лінгвістиці, авторкою сформовано матрицю їхніх альтернатив, до складу якої введено лише поняття, що є абстрагованими від галузі наукового знання, в якій вони функціонують, та поняття ознак харизматичної особистості публічного мовця, що сприймаються представниками аудиторії. До складу матриці не ввійшла певна кількість некоректних ознак, які мають синонімічний характер або статус другорядних і перебувають у причинно-наслідковому зв’язку з введеними до неї більш абстрактними ознаками (наприклад, уважність як передумова виникнення емпатії; розважливість як прямий наслідок наявності інтелекту; рішучість як наслідок заповзятості тощо). Доведено, що для вичерпного наукового опису результатів міждисциплінарних досліджень численних феноменів комунікативної поведінки харизматичного публічного мовця водночас із іншими мовленнєво-лінгвістичними, психологічними, культурно-соціальними і фізіологічними (погляд, пози, жести, голос, інтонація тощо) ознаками та їх відповідними комплексами доцільним є вживання таких понять, як інтелект, впевненість, заповзятість, натхненність, комунікабельність, домінантність, нарцисизм, візіонерство, що стосуються харизматичних якостей особистості мовця й є за своєю сутністю конвенціалізованими історично. Вказано на перспективність продовження подібних аналізів мовленнєво-лінгвістичних, психологічних, культурно-соціальних і фізіологічних ознак мовця.
... У психології і психоаналізі дослідники фокусувалися на конкретних ознаках особистості та психіки (Deluga, 1997;Popper, 2002;Williams et al., 2018). До визначення особистісних характеристик харизматичного мовця долучились і лінгвісти, що було зумовлено необхідністю більш об'єктивного добору персоналій для лінгвістичних досліджень усного й письмового харизматичного мовлення (Signorello et al., 2012). Проте питання систематизації невербальних ознак харизматичної особистості публічного мовця та методологічних особливостей їх вживання в описах результатів міждисциплінарних досліджень свого остаточного вирішення ще не знайшло. ...
... Окрім того, науковці та експерти у галузі політології, менеджменту, соціології й психології відзначають важливість невербальних аспектів сприйняття аудиторією лідера як харизматичного, зокрема послідовники «зчитують» інформацію про лідера-мовця на основі певних фізичних характеристик: виразу обличчя (важливою для сприйняття харизми є посмішка), зовнішності, постави, положення тіла, голосу, жестів, погляду, зросту, ваги, одягу (Reh, 2016). Важливість невербальних ознак підтверджується і лінгвістичними доробками, зокрема, група науковців (Signorello et al., 2012) досліджувала, наскільки змінилося сприйняття аудиторією мовлення політика, який пережив інсульт: якщо до цього випадку він сприймався як харизматичний лідер завдяки зовнішнім ознакам та енергійному, швидкому, високотонному мовленню, то після хвороби, коли політик мав певні труднощі з артикуляцією, диханням і голосом, його харизматичність у сприйнятті послідовниками зникла. Крім того, під час експериментальних лінгвістичних досліджень було доведено, що стать лідера-мовця впливає на сприйняття такої особистості як харизматичної: згідно з результатами експерименту жінок і чоловіків сприймають як однаково харизматичних, незважаючи на те, що мовлення жінок-лідерів має більш високі показники характеристик, що визначають мовлення як харизматичне (Niebuhr et al., 2016;Jokisch et al., 2018). ...
... Charisma perception in political speech: a case study. International Conference on Speech and Corpora(GSCP 2012, 343-348. 49. ...
Article
The paper covers a theoretical analysis of the existing scientific research findings which can serve as the basis for further interdisciplinary investigation of the interplay of verbal and non-verbal means in charismatic communication. The author presents a short diachronic review of how the notion of charisma has evolved and been functioning in the scientific domain from ancient times to modern humanitarian studies, in particular psychological, sociological, managerial ones, etc. The review focuses on linguistic research into mass communication, communicative strategies and tactics which can be employed in different types of mass communication, as well as the interplay of verbal, non-verbal and paralingual means in charismatic communication, whose results serve as a basis for further investigation of charismatic communication within the framework of Alla Kalyta’s speech energetic theory, as the theory allows us to consider a complex qualitative and quantitative picture of the self-development of any cognitive phenomenon in general and the specificity of the interplay of verbal and non-verbal means in charismatic communication in particular. The works reviewed in the paper provide the list of universal characteristics of charismatic communication which can be summarised as follows: the effect of a speaker’s charisma is achieved not through separate elements of one’s speech but with the complex interaction of charismatic prosody and speaker’s voice, loudness, tempo of speech, word stress, body language, mimics, gestures, speaker’s age, gender and physical attractiveness, etc. The author comes to the conclusion that the results of this retrospective analysis provide grounds for an interdisciplinary research of charismatic communication within the cognitive approach framework which accounts for all verbal, non-verbal and paralingual means (physiological, psychological, social, etc.) whose interplay allows the audience to perceive a speaker as a charismatic one.
... The voice is one of the perceivable behaviors of the "charisma of the body" and its acoustic parameters are responsible for conveying charisma. In previous works (e.g., [4]) we distinguished a charismatic voice from a non-charismatic one through samples of political speech. We also showed how different acoustic voice patterns convey different charisma traits and how they could influence perception in the process of persuasion in political discourse [3]. ...
... Also examining political speech, Signorello et al. [4] used speeches by Umberto Bossi, the leader of an Italian party, before and after a stroke. Results revealed that, for French listeners, the prosody in Bossi's pre-stroke speech sounded more charismatic (the pitch contour in particular). ...
... There are many phonetic studies dealing with political speakers. These studies have been carried out in the last three decades and have included speakers like John Kerry, Umberto Bossi, Hillary Clinton, Arnold Schwarzenegger, Donald Trump, Angela Merkel, Matteo Salvini, Barack Obama and many others (see for instance Touati 1993;Rosenberg 2005 andBiadsy et al. 2008;Signorello et al. 2012a and2012b;D'Errico et al. 2013 andHiroyuki & Rathcke 2016;Jokisch et al. 2018). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The present study investigates acoustic features of charismatic speech using a case study. There is an ongoing myth regarding the first ever televised presidential debate on September 26, 1960, between John F. Kennedy (who emerged as the winner of the debate) and Richard M. Nixon. The myth states that while Kennedy clearly won amongst the TV audience, Nixon was actually the winner among radio listeners. The question that appears is if the debate had only been broadcast over the radio (as in previous years), would Nixon's tone of voice have helped him to victory over Kennedy? In a nutshell, based on current research on speaker charisma, this myth turns out to be plausible with certain reservations: Our results indicate that Nixon's tone of voice corresponds more closely to what is nowadays known about charismatic voices as opposed to Kennedy's tone of voice.
Article
Full-text available
У статті на основі аналізу наявного в гуманітарних науках міждисциплінарного знання про явище харизми обґрунтовано особливості і ознаки його актуалізації у процесі комунікації та визначено лінгвістичний статус поняття «харизма». Для реалізації мети дослідження було проведено ретроспективний огляд тлумачення харизми як культурного, соціального і наукового явища та виокремлено його основні характеристики. За результатами теоретичного пошуку обґрунтовано необхідність визначення такого поняття харизми, яке відповідатиме нагальним вимогам міждисциплінарних гуманітарних наукових пошуків, а не окремих досліджень чи галузей знань. У праці здійснено стислий аналіз еволюції поняття харизми та його сучасних визначень у психології, соціології, лінгвістиці та виокремлено ті ознаки досліджуваного явища, які є важливими для кожної окремо взятої науки. На цих підставах у статті узагальнено провідні ознаки набуття мовцем харизми з урахуванням положень сучасної когнітивістики щодо перебігу процесу комунікації. За результатами узагальнення хронологічного розвитку думки про явище харизми та аналізу її наявних дефініцій автором у рамках когнітивного підходу до процесів комунікації з урахуванням комплексу зовнішніх, психологічних, фізіологічних і соціальних ознак мовця, які свідчать про його здатність як лідера задовольнити запити аудиторії, умов комунікації та лінгвальних і позалінгвальних засобів здійснення комунікативного впливу запропоновано власне міждисциплінарне визначення харизми. Вказано на перспективність практичного використання результатів проведеного теоретичного пошуку і доцільність подальшого вживання запропонованого міждисциплінарного визначення поняття «харизма» у гуманітарних дослідженнях.
Chapter
This collection advances the study of context-dependent characteristics of argumentative discourse by examining a variety of media genres in which text and image (and other semiotic modes) combine to create meaning. The chapters have been written by an international group of senior and junior scholars researching multimodal argumentation in the last two decades. In each chapter, a specific approach to argumentation and rhetoric is combined with insights from visual studies, metaphor theory, scientific visualization, cognitive science, semiotics, conversation analysis, or (documentary) film theory in order to explain how multimodal genres function argumentatively and rhetorically. Together the chapters present a state-of-the-art in the analysis of multimodal argumentation in such diverse genres as print advertisements, news photographs, scientific illustrations, political cartoons, documentaries, film trailers, political TV advertisements, public debates, and political speeches. The volume will be of interest to advanced students and scholars in argumentation studies, rhetoric, and multimodal communication.
Article
Full-text available
Phonetic research on the prosodic sources of perceived charisma has taken a big step towards making a speaker’s tone-of-voice a tangible, quantifiable, and trainable matter. However, the tone-of-voice includes a complex bundle of acoustic features, and a lot of parameters have not even been looked at so far. Moreover, all previous studies focused on political or religious leaders and left aside the large field of managers and CEOs in the world of business. These are the two research gaps addressed in the present study. An acoustic analysis of about 1,350 prosodic phrases from keynotes given by a more charismatic CEO (Steve Jobs) and a less charismatic CEO (Mark Zuckerberg) suggests that the same tone-of-voice settings that make political or religious leaders sound more charismatic also work for business speakers. In addition, results point to further charisma-relevant acoustic parameters related to rhythm, emphasis, pausing, and voice quality - as well as to audience type as a significant context factor. The findings are discussed with respect to implications for future perception oriented studies and perspectives for a computer-based measurement, assessment, and training of a charismatic tone of voice.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
A leader's charisma is conveyed by various multiple aspects of his perceivable behavior among which the acoustic-prosodic characteristics of speech. We present here a study on the perception of charisma in political speech that aims to investigate the notion of charisma and to validate a theoretical framework on a multidimensional scale of charisma perception. The study points out that a multidimensional approach of charisma allows to better analyze which factors are related to specific aspects of speech. We finally clustered the charismatic voice of an Italian political leader in three factors: Proactive-Attracting, Benevolent-Competent and Authoritarian.
Article
Full-text available
This paper is about a starting project, which involves already more than 10 laboratories from Latin European countries. Here are summarized some basic points of our methodology as a guideline for the organization and normalization of research to come. In this Conference we would like to present some beta results obtained by the research teams of the Universities of Grenoble and Aveiro of some of the Romance Varieties already taken into account.
Article
Full-text available
This paper presents a model of persuasion in terms of goals and beliefs. Among the various ways to influence people, that is, to raise or lower the likelihood for them to pursue some goal, ranging from threat to suggestion, persuasion is viewed as a case of communicative non-coercive goal hooking. A persuader leads a persuadee to pursue some goal out of a free choice, i.e., by convincing him/her that the proposed goal is useful for some other goal that the persuadee already has. It is argued that the Aristotelian persuasive strategies of logos, ethos and pathos (rational argumentation, the speaker's credibility and reliability, and the appeal to emotion) are always present in every persuasive discourse, and that they are exploited to raise the value of the goal proposed and to strengthen the believability of the link between it and the persuadee's previous goals. The paper proposes an analysis of discourse in terms of a hierarchy of goals as a tool to single out these strategies within the discourse structure. By applying this model to different kinds of persuasive messages (political discourse, advertising, dialogues in the health domain), it shows how, in the fragments presented, this kind of analysis allows to clarify the relationships between the persuader's and the persuadee's goals and to elucidate how much and how directly the persuader appeals to logos, ethos and pathos in his/her discourse.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This paper deals with subjective qualities and acoustic- prosodic features contributing to the impression of a good speaker. Subjects rated a variety of samples of political speech on a number of subjective qualities and acoustic fea- tures were extracted from the speech samples. A perceptual evaluation was also conducted with manipulations of F0 dynamics, fluency and speech rate with the sample of the lowest rated speaker as a basis. Subjects' ranking revealed a clear preference for modified versions over the original with F0 dynamics - a wider range - being the most powerful cue. Index Terms: prosody, speaker skill, subject ratings, acoustic measurements, synthesis evaluation
Article
Full-text available
Charisma, the ability to attract and retain followers without benefit of formal authority, is more difficult to define than to identify. While we each seem able to identify charismatic individuals – and non-charismatic individuals – it is not clear what it is about an individual that influences our judgment. This paper describes the results of experiments designed to discover potential correlates of such judgments, in what speakers say and the way that they say it. We present results of two parallel experiments in which subjective judgments of charisma in spoken and in transcribed American political speech were analyzed with respect to the acoustic and prosodic (where applicable) and lexico-syntactic characteristics of the speech being assessed. While we find that there is considerable disagreement among subjects on how the speakers of each token are ranked, we also find that subjects appear to share a functional definition of charisma, in terms of other personal characteristics we asked them to rank speakers by. We also find certain acoustic, prosodic, and lexico-syntactic characteristics that correlate significantly with perceptions of charisma. Finally, by comparing the responses to spoken vs. transcribed stimuli, we attempt to distinguish between the contributions of “what is said” and “how it is said” with respect to charisma judgments.
Article
Full-text available
This paper reports preliminary experiments on automatic attribution of personality traits based on nonverbal vocal behavioral cues. In particular, the work shows how prosodic features can be used to predict, with an accuracy up to 75% depending on the trait, the personality assessments performed by human judges on a collection of 640 speech samples. The assessments are based on a short version of the Big Five Inventory, one of the most widely used questionnaires for personality assessment. The judges did not understand the language spoken in the speech samples so that the influence of the verbal content is limited. To tho best of our knowledge, this is the first work aimed at inferring automatically traits attributed by judges rather than traits self-reported by subjects.
Article
This study investigated the relationship between pathologic and normal speaking fundamental frequency characteristics (SFF) in a group of 80 male subjects. The subjects were divided into four groups of 20 (1) vocal fold paralysis, (2) benign mass lesion, (3) cancer of the larynx, and (4) normal. The results of this study indicate that SFF standard deviation and semitone range of SFF are significantly reduced for patients with vocal fold paralysis as compared with normals. The parameters of mean SFF, number of frequency shifts, and age failed to separate the normals from the three groups of pathologic subjects.
Book
Everyone knows that high IQ is no guarantee of success, happiness, or virtue, but until "Emotional Intelligence," we could only guess why. Daniel Goleman's brilliant report from the frontiers of psychology and neuroscience offers startling new insight into our "two minds"--the rational and the emotional--and how they together shape our destiny.Through vivid examples, Goleman delineates the five crucial skills of emotional intelligence, and shows how they determine our success in relationships, work, and even our physical well-being. What emerges is an entirely new way to talk about being smart. The best news is that "emotional literacy" is not fixed early in life. Every parent, every teacher, every business leader, and everyone interested in a more civil society, has a stake in this compelling vision of human possibility.
Rhetoric. The Internet Classics Archive and Body Language of Politics Was Exploring the rhetorical leadership of Mahatma Gandhi
  • Aristotle Atkinson
  • M Routledge
  • Uk London
  • M C Bligh
  • J L Robinson
Aristotle. (2011). Rhetoric. The Internet Classics Archive. Atkinson, M. (1984). and Body Language of Politics. Routledge, London, UK. Bligh, M.C., Robinson, J.L. (2010). Was Exploring the rhetorical leadership of Mahatma Gandhi. In The Leadership Quarterly, 21(5), pp. 844--855.