Content uploaded by Feng Kong
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Feng Kong on Feb 04, 2021
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=psns20
Download by: [Beijing Normal University] Date: 16 September 2015, At: 21:12
Social Neuroscience
ISSN: 1747-0919 (Print) 1747-0927 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/psns20
Brain regions involved in dispositional
mindfulness during resting state and their relation
with well-being
Feng Kong, Xu Wang, Yiying Song & Jia Liu
To cite this article: Feng Kong, Xu Wang, Yiying Song & Jia Liu (2015): Brain regions involved
in dispositional mindfulness during resting state and their relation with well-being, Social
Neuroscience, DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1092469
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1092469
Accepted online: 11 Sep 2015.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 5
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Accepted Manuscript
1
Publisher: Taylor & Francis
Journal: Social Neuroscience
DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1092469
Title: Brain regions involved in dispositional mindfulness during resting state and their
relation with well-being
Running title: Mindfulness and well-being
Authors: Feng Kong a,b, Xu Wang a,b, Yiying Song a,b, and Jia Liu c
Affiliation:
a State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute
for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
b Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, Beijing Normal
University, Beijing, China
c School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
Correspondence Author: Jia Liu, Ph.D.; Room 405, Yingdong Building, 19 Xinjiekouwai St,
Haidian District, Beijing 100875, China. Tel.: +86-10-58806154; fax: +86-10-58806154.
E-mail: liujia@bnu.edu.cn
Total number of words: 8664
Number of Pages: 38
Number of Figures/tables: 4/2
Funding
This study was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31230031,
91132703, 31221003), the National Basic Research Program of China (2014CB846103),
National Social Science Foundation of China (11&ZD187), and Changjiang Scholars
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
2
Programme of China.
The authors declare no competing interests.
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
1
Abstract
Mindfulness can be viewed as an important dispositional characteristic that reflects the
tendency to be mindful in daily life, which is beneficial for improving individuals’ both
hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. However, no study to date has examined the brain
regions involved in individual differences in dispositional mindfulness during the resting state
and its relation with hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. To investigate this issue, the present
study employed resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) to evaluate
the regional homogeneity (ReHo) that measures the local synchronization of spontaneous
brain activity in a large sample. We found that dispositional mindfulness was positively
associated with the ReHo in the left orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), left parahippocampal gyrus
(PHG) and right insula implicated in emotion processing, body awareness and self-referential
processing, and negatively associated with the ReHo in right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG)
implicated in response inhibition and attentional control. Furthermore, we found different
neural associations with hedonic (i.e., positive and negative affect) and eudaimonic
well-being (i.e., the meaningful and purposeful life). Specifically, the ReHo in the IFG
predicted eudaimonic well-being whereas the OFC predicted positive affect, both of which
were mediated by dispositional mindfulness. Taken together, our study provides the first
evidence for linking individual differences in dispositional mindfulness to spontaneous brain
activity and demonstrates that dispositional mindfulness engages multiple brain mechanisms
that differentially influence hedonic and eudaimonic well-being.
Keywords: Spontaneous brain activity; Eudaimonic well-being; Hedonic well-being;
Dispositional mindfulness
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
2
Introduction
Mindfulness has been defined as an attention to and awareness of one's ongoing sensory,
cognitive and emotional experiences in the context of an accepting, non-judgmental stance
toward those experiences (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007). A roughly
opposed concept is mind-wandering, which reflects intermittent shifts of attention away from
the task at hand toward internal information (Mrazek, Smallwood, & Schooler, 2012;
Smallwood & Schooler, 2006), in contrast to sustained attentiveness in mindfulness.
Although mindfulness can be cultivated through various forms of mindfulness meditation
training (e.g., Carmody & Baer, 2008; Falkenström, 2010; Keng, Smoski, & Robins, 2011),
mindfulness has also been conceptualized as a trait-like or dispositional characteristic, in
which mindfulness levels vary naturally in the general population, even without mindfulness
training (Brown & Ryan, 2003). A large body of evidence has shown that the self-reported
tendency to be mindful in daily life is positively correlated with psychological health and
well-being and negatively correlated with psychological symptoms and distress (Bao, Xue, &
Kong, 2015; Brown & Ryan, 2003; Brown et al., 2007; Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, &
Walach, 2004; Keng et al., 2011; Kong, Wang, & Zhao, 2014a; Schutte & Malouff, 2011). In
this study, we investigated the brain regions involved in individual differences in
dispositional mindfulness during the resting state and its relation with well-being.
The available neuroimaging literature on meditation has indicated an association with
altered structures or activities in brain regions involved in attention (e.g., anterior cingulate
cortex [ACC] and inferior frontal gyrus [IFG]), emotion processing (e.g., orbitofrontal cortex
[OFC] and ventromedial prefrontal cortex [PFC]), self-processing (e.g., parahippocampal
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
3
gyrus [PHG] and PFC), and interoception and body awareness (e.g., insula) (Farb et al., 2007;
Goldin & Gross, 2010; Grant, Courtemanche, Duerden, Duncan, & Rainville, 2010; Hölzel et
al., 2008, 2011; Lazar et al., 2000, 2005; Luders, Toga, Lepore, & Gaser, 2009; Lutz,
Brefczynski-Lewis, Johnstone, & Davidson, 2008; Manna et al., 2010; Ott, Hölzel, & Vaitl,
2011; Tang, Tang, & Posner, 2013; Klimecki, Leiberg, Lamm, & Singer, 2013; Zeidan,
Martucci, Kraft, Gordon, McHaffie, & Coghill,, 2011). Individual differences in dispositional
mindfulness may be reliably detected through self-report questionnaires, as mindfulness traits
may also be present in meditation-naive individuals (Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004). To our
knowledge, only several studies have explored the association between brain activity and
individual differences in dispositional mindfulness. For instance, several functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shown that higher levels of dispositional mindfulness
is mostly associated with reduced amygdala activation and increased PFC activation during
several emotion-processing tasks (Creswell, Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman,, 2007; Lutz et
al., 2013; Modinos, Ormel, & Aleman, 2010). However, it remains unknown whether and
how the local synchronization of spontaneous brain activity could predict dispositional
mindfulness. Therefore, here we explored the neural correlates of individual differences in
dispositional mindfulness using resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI), a promising tool for exploring
the intrinsic functional architecture of the brain based on measurements of spontaneous
low-frequency fluctuations (LFFs, 0.01–0.10 Hz) in the blood oxygenation level dependent
signal (Biswal, 2012; Fox & Raichle, 2007; Raichle, 2010). It has been used to explore the
neural basis of individual differences in behavioral performance such as intelligence, conflict
adaptation and personality traits (Tian et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2014; Wang, Song, Jiang,
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
4
Zhang, & Yu, 2011; Wei et al., 2011). Because it is a data-driven method that requires no a
priori hypotheses about regions of interest, it can identify non-anticipated or transient
task-related components (Zang, Jiang, Lu, He, & Tian, 2004).
The extant literature suggests that mindfulness is beneficial for improving well-being.
Generally, well-being is composed of hedonic well-being and eudaimonic well-being, which
have been long distinguished by philosophers (Aristotle, 1925). A hedonic approach of
well-being equates well-being with pleasure and happiness and defines well-being in terms of
pleasure attainment and pain avoidance (Ryan & Deci, 2001). Alternatively, a eudaimonic
approach of well-being conceptualizes well-being in terms of the realization of one’s true
potential (Ryff & Keyes, 1995) and the experience of purpose or meaning in life (Ryff, 1989).
Contemporary psychological research has continued to distinguish these two types of
well-being (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2001), with empirical studies demonstrating that they are
related but independent constructs using confirmatory factor analysis techniques (Gallagher,
Lopez, & Preacher, 2009; Linley, Maltby, Wood, Osborne, & Hurling, 2009). Correlational
studies have shown that measures of dispositional mindfulness are associated with higher
levels of positive affect and eudaimonic well-being and lower levels of negative affect (e.g.,
Brown & Ryan, 2003; Keng et al., 2011; Kong et al., 2014a; Schutte & Malouff, 2011; Wang
& Kong, 2014). Furthermore, numerous studies suggest that with an increase in mindfulness
through interventions (e.g., meditation), individuals’ well-being also tends to increase (e.g.,
Falkenström, 2010; Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008; Geschwind, Peeters,
Drukker, van Os, & Wichers, 2011; Keng et al., 2011; Zeidan, Johnson, Gordon, &
Goolkasian, 2010). Although previous studies have shown the associations between
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
5
mindfulness and well-being, specific neurobiological mechanisms through which
mindfulness exerts beneficial effects on well-being have not been understood. Given that
hedonic and eudaimonic well-being are conceptually and empirically distinct constructs
(Gallagher et al., 2009; Linley et al., 2009), spontaneous brain activity related to dispositional
mindfulness might differentially predict both forms of well-being. Previous studies have
shown that hedonic well-being is mostly associated with limbic regions such as the OFC
(Berridge & Kringelbach, 2013; Kringelbach, 2005), whereas eudaimonic well-being is
mostly associated with attention-related regions including the ACC and lateral PFC (Heller et
al., 2013; Kong, Hu, Xue, Song, & Liu, 2015; van Reekum et al., 2007). Furthermore, a
recent fMRI study found that compassion meditation training elicited activity in the OFC and
increased positive affect, even in response to witnessing others in distress (Klimecki et al.,
2013). Thus, we speculated that the OFC might predict hedonic well-being through
dispositional mindfulness, whereas attention-related areas including the ACC and lateral PFC
might predict eudaimonic well-being through dispositional mindfulness.
To investigate these issues, this study used well-validated behavioral measures of
dispositional mindfulness and well-being, and rs-fMRI methodology. Here, we employed
regional homogeneity (ReHo) to measure the local synchronization of spontaneous brain
activity during rest, and then investigated the relationship between ReHo and dispositional
mindfulness as measured by the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS, Brown & Ryan,
2003). On the basis of previous neuroscience findings on mindfulness, we hypothesized that
individual differences in dispositional mindfulness would be correlated with the ReHo in
multiple brain regions associated with executive attention, emotion processing, body
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
6
awareness and self-related processing. Furthermore, we examined the extent to which
individual differences in dispositional mindfulness are able to mediate the relationship
between mindfulness-related neural substrates and two types of well-being.
Methods
Participants
Two hundred and ninety healthy university students (157 females; mean age = 21.56
years, standard deviation (SD) = 1.01) from Beijing Normal University were recruited as paid
participants. All participants reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders.
Most of the participants were right-handed (n=272) based on a single-item handedness
questionnaire (“Are you (a) right-handed, (b) left-handed, (c) mixed-handed?”). Previous
neuroimaging studies have found that a limited number of left-handed participants has little
influence on the research results when the sample is large (e.g., Kong et al., 2014b; Li et al.,
2014); therefore we did not remove data from left-handed participants. Both behavioral and
MRI protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Beijing Normal
University. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the study.
Psychological measures
To assess dispositional mindfulness we administered the Mindful Attention Awareness
Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003), which consists of 15 brief statements. Items describe
mindlessness in everyday situations (e.g., “I tend to walk quickly to get where I’m going
without paying attention to what I experience along the way.” and ‘‘I tend not to notice
feelings of physical tension or discomfort until they really grab my attention.’’). Respondents
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
7
were asked to rate their agreement on a 6-point Likert-type scale (1 = almost always, 6 =
almost never). Item scoring was reversed so that a higher score reflects a higher level of
mindfulness. The scale has high internal consistency and validity (Bao et al., 2015; Brown &
Ryan, 2003; Kong et al., 2014a; Lu et al., 2014). In this study, the scale exhibited adequate
reliability (α = 0.85).
Emotional well-being (sometimes called hedonic well-being) refers to the emotional
quality of an individual’s everyday experience (Ryan & Deci, 2000; Kahneman & Deaton,
2010). It was assessed by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark,
& Tellegen, 1988). The PANAS consists of a word list describing two different affect states
(10 positive and 10 negative), for example, “excited” and “upset.” Participants were
instructed to indicate the extent to which they generally feel each affect using a 5-point Likert
scale. Positive and negative affect scores were calculated separately, with higher scores
indicating that participants feel more of that affect. The scale has high internal consistency
and validity (Crawford & Henry, 2004; Sun & Kong, 2013; Watson et al., 1988). In this study,
the positive affect scale (α = 0.81) and negative affect scale (α = 0.79) exhibited adequate
reliability. One participant was excluded because of missing data.
Eudaimonic well-being was assessed with the 42-item Scales of Psychological Well-being
(SPWB, Ryff et al., 2007). It is composed of the six dimensions (autonomy, environmental
mastery, self-acceptance, positive relations with others, purpose in life, and personal growth).
The SPWB is a highly reliable measure to assess eudaimonic well-being (Gallagher et al.,
2009; Ryff, 1989, 2007). It includes items such as, ‘‘Some people wander aimlessly through
life, but I am not one of them” and ‘‘I think it is important to have new experiences that
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
8
challenge how you think about yourself and the world.” Each dimension was measured with
seven items and participants responded to each item using a 6-point Likert scale with
response options ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. To compute an overall
score of eudaimonic well-being, scores across the six subscales were calculated. Higher
scores indicate higher levels of eudaimonic well-being. In the present study, the SPWB
exhibited adequate internal reliability (α = 0.92). Six participants were excluded because of
missing data.
rs-fMRI data acquisition
The rs-fMRI scan was performed on a 3T scanner (Siemens Magnetom Trio, A Tim
System) with a 12-channel phased-array head coil at Beijing Normal University Imaging
Center for Brain Research, Beijing, China. During rs-fMRI scanning, participants were
instructed to close their eyes, keep still, and not think about anything systematically or fall
asleep. The resting state scanning consisted of 240 contiguous EPI volumes (TR = 2000ms;
TE = 30ms; flip angle = 90°; number of slices = 33; matrix = 64 × 64; FOV = 200 × 200 mm2;
acquisition voxel size = 3.1 × 3.1 × 3.6 mm3). Moreover, High-resolution T1-weighted
images were acquired with magnetization prepared gradient echo sequence (MPRAGE:
TR/TE/TI = 2530/3.39/1100 ms; flip angle = 7 degrees; matrix = 256 × 256) for spatial
registration. One hundred and twenty-eight contiguous sagittal slices were obtained with 1 ×
1 mm2 in-plane resolution and 1.33 mm slice thickness.
Data preprocessing
We used FSL (www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/) to preprocess the resting-state functional images.
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
9
The first four volumes of the rs-fMRI data were discarded for signal equilibrium.
Preprocessing steps of rs-fMRI data included motion correction (by aligning each volume to
the middle volume of the image with MCFLIRT), intensity normalization, removing linear
trends and temporally band-pass filtering (0.01< f < 0.1 Hz). One participant whose head
motion was greater than 3.0 mm displacement in any of the x, y, or z directions or 3.0° of any
angular motion throughout the rs-fMRI scan was excluded from further analyses. Registration
of each participant’s functional images to the high-resolution anatomical images was carried
out with FLIRT to produce a six-degrees-of-freedom affine transformation matrix.
Registration of each participant’s high-resolution anatomical image to a common stereotaxic
space (the Montreal Neurological Institute 152-brain template with a resolution of 2 × 2 × 2
mm3, MNI152) was accomplished using a two-step process (Andersson, Jenkinson, & Smith,
2007).
ReHo analysis
ReHo was performed on a voxel-by-voxel basis by calculating Kendall’s coefficient of
concordance (KCC) of time series of each of the 26 neighbors surrounding the center voxel
(see Zang et al., 2004 for details). A KCC value was then attributed to the central voxel
representing the ReHo, and individual ReHo maps were obtained. A larger ReHo value for a
given voxel indicates a higher local synchronization of rs-fMRI signals among neighboring
voxels. Then, individual ReHo maps were registered to MNI152 standard space by applying
the previously calculated transformation matrix for further groupwise analyses. For
standardization purposes, the individual data was transformed to Z score (i.e., minus the
global mean value and then divided by the standard deviation) other than simply being
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
10
divided by the global mean.
ReHo-behavior correlation analysis
To detect the brain regions whose ReHo values predict individual differences in
dispositional mindfulness, we conducted a whole-brain correlation analysis with age and
gender as the confounding covariates, and the score of dispositional mindfulness as the
covariates of interest. Multiple comparison correction was performed using the 3dClustSim
program in AFNI (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/dist/doc/program_help/3dClustSim.html). A
corrected cluster threshold of p < 0.01 (single voxel p < 0.01, cluster size ≥ 91.4 voxels;
731.2 mm3) was set. All significant correlations were presented in MNI coordinates.
Prediction analysis
To examine the robustness of the relation between ReHo and behavioral performance, a
machine-learning approach with balanced fourfold cross-validation combined with linear
regression conducted was used (Cohen et al., 2010; Qin, Young, Duan, Chen, Supekar, &
Menon, 2014; Supekar et al., 2013). Mindfulness as a dependent variable and ReHo of the
regions as an independent variable were treated as input to a linear regression algorithm. The
r (predicted, observed) was used to measure how well the independent variable predicts the
dependent variable. The correlation was estimated using a balanced fourfold cross-validation
procedure in following steps. First, data were divided into four folds such that the
distributions of these variables were balanced across folds. Second, a linear regression model
was built using three folds, leaving out one fold, and this model was then used to predict the
data in the left-out fold (i.e. predicted values). This procedure was repeated four times to
compute a final r (predicted, observed) representing the correlation between the values predicted by
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
11
the regression model and the observed values. Nonparametric testing approach was used to
assess the statistical significance of the model. The empirical null distribution of r (predicted,
observed) was estimated by generating 1,000 surrogate datasets under the null hypothesis that
there was no association between ReHo and behavioral performance. We generated each
surrogate dataset Di of size equal to the observed dataset by permuting the labels on the
observed data points (i.e. the score of mindfulness). The r (predicted, observed)i (i.e., r (predicted,
observed) of Di) was computed using the observed labels of Di and predicted labels using the
fourfold-balanced cross-validation procedure described above. This procedure produced a
null distribution of r(predicted, observed) for the regression model. The statistical significance (P
value) of the model was then determined by counting the number of r (predicted, observed)i greater
than r (predicted, observed) and then dividing that count by the number of Di datasets (i.e., 1,000).
Results
The neural correlates of dispositional mindfulness
Table 1 shows means, SDs, skewness, and kurtosis for all questionnaires. The kurtosis
and skewness of all the scores ranged from -1 and +1, which indicated the normality of the
data (Marcoulides & Hershberger, 1997). Correlation analysis revealed that dispositional
mindfulness was positively correlated with the ReHo in three clusters including the right
insula (MNI coordinate: 36, 14, -10; r = 0.22; z =3.64; Cluster size = 920 mm3; p < 0.01), left
PHG (MNI coordinate: -20, -32, -14; r = 0.23; z =3.41; Cluster size = 880 mm3; p < 0.01),
and left OFC (MNI coordinate: -24, 14, -26; r = 0.21; z =3.86; Cluster size = 768 mm3; p <
0.01). Dispositional mindfulness was also negatively correlated with the ReHo in a cluster
including the right IFG (MNI coordinate: 60, 10, 28; r = -0.26; z = -4.10; Cluster size = 1024
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
12
mm3; p < 0.01) (Fig. 1 and 2, Table 2). No other significant relations were observed. To
examine the robustness of the relation between ReHo and mindfulness, we applied a
cross-validation method with a machine-learning algorithm (see Methods). We found that the
ReHo values of the IFG (r(predicted, observed) = .22; p < .001), PHG (r(predicted, observed) = .20; p
< .001), OFC (r(predicted, observed) = .19; p < .001) and insula (r(predicted, observed) = .21; p < .001)
reliably predicted individual differences in dispositional mindfulness.
----Insert Table 1 here ----
----Insert Table 2 here ----
In order to assess the joint effects of the ReHo in the IFG, PHG, OFC and insula on
dispositional mindfulness, we performed a multiple linear regression analysis. The dependent
variable was participants’ dispositional mindfulness scores, and the independent variables
were the average ReHo values of the clusters including the PCG, PHG, OFC and insula.
When all independent variables were simultaneously entered into the regression, the
regression model explained 14% of the variance in dispositional mindfulness (R2 =0.14; F[4,
285] = 12.32; p < 0.001) and all regression coefficients for these clusters were significant (β
= .14, −.21, ps < 0.05). After sex and age were regressed out, all regression coefficients
remained significant (β = .12, −.20, ps < 0.05). These findings suggest that resting-state
activity in the IFG, PHG, OFC, and insula may independently predict individual differences
in dispositional mindfulness.
----Insert Figure 1 here ----
----Insert Figure 2 here ----
Dispositional mindfulness mediated the association between spontaneous brain activity and
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
13
well-being
To test the relationship between dispositional mindfulness, well-being and spontaneous
brain activity, we first performed a correlation analysis between dispositional mindfulness
and well-being. We found that dispositional mindfulness was positively correlated with
positive affect (r = 0.29, p < 0.001, FDR corrected) and eudaimonic well-being (r = 0.52, p <
0.001, FDR corrected), and negatively correlated with negative affect (r = –0.35, p < 0.001,
FDR corrected). These results support the notion that dispositional mindfulness contributes to
both hedonic and eudaimonic well-being.
Next, we carried out a correlation analysis to examine whether the brain regions involved
in dispositional mindfulness are differentially associated with various forms of well-being.
We found that eudaimonic well-being was significantly correlated with the ReHo in the IFG
(r = –0.15, p = 0.030, FDR corrected). Positive affect was significantly correlated with the
ReHo in the OFC (r = 0.17, p = 0.011, FDR corrected). No significant correlation between
negative affect and these clusters was obtained. Furthermore, all these significant correlations
remained after controlling for the effects of age and sex. In addition, we also applied a
cross-validation method with a machine-learning algorithm to examine the robustness of the
relation between ReHo and well-being. We found that the ReHo of the IFG reliably predicted
individual differences in eudaimonic well-being (r(predicted, observed) = 0.12; p = .003), while the
ReHo in the OFC reliably predicted individual differences in positive affect (r(predicted, observed)
= 0.15; p = .001). The results indicated that the neurobiological systems subserving
dispositional mindfulness differentially predicted hedonic and eudaimonic well-being.
The aforementioned results indicated that well-being, dispositional mindfulness and brain
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
14
regions were closely linked to one another. However, the role of dispositional mindfulness in
the relationships between intrinsic brain activity and well-being remains unknown. Thus, we
further examined the association between the ReHo in these regions and well-being when
controlling for the mindfulness factor. The associations observed in the previous analysis
were no longer significant (ps > 0.05), suggesting a possible mediating role of dispositional
mindfulness in the associations between the ReHo in these regions and well-being.
To examine this intuition, an INDIRECT macro implemented in SPSS (Preacher & Hayes,
2008) was used. With sex and age as covariates in the model, the mediation analysis showed
that dispositional mindfulness significantly mediated the effect of the OFC on positive affect
(95% confidence interval = [0.02, 0.11], p < 0.05), based on the criterion that an empirical 95%
confidence interval of the significant indirect effect does not include zero with a bootstrap
simulation (n = 5,000) (Fig. 3). Besides, dispositional mindfulness also significantly mediated
the effect of the IFG on eudaimonic well-being (95% confidence interval = [-0.21, -0.08], p <
0.05) (Fig. 4).
----Insert Figure 3 here ----
In addition, we also used the same mediation analysis procedure to examine the
mediational model where relevant brain regions mediated the relationship between
dispositional mindfulness and well-being. The results showed that the empirical 95% CI of all
the indirect effects included zero, indicating that these brain regions did not mediate the
relationship between dispositional mindfulness and either form of well-being. Therefore, it is
dispositional mindfulness that mediated the association between spontaneous brain activity
and well-being.
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
15
Discussion
The present study employed the ReHo approach to investigate the neural correlates of
individual differences in dispositional mindfulness and its associations with hedonic and
eudaimonic well-being in a large sample. Behavioral results showed that dispositional
mindfulness was related to hedonic (i.e., positive and negative affect) and eudaimonic
well-being, which is in line with previous studies reporting the beneficial effects of
mindfulness on well-being (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Brown et al., 2007; Grossman et al., 2004;
Keng et al., 2011; Kong et al, 2014; Schutte & Malouff, 2011). ReHo results showed that
dispositional mindfulness was positively associated with the ReHo of rs-fMRI signals in the
left OFC, left PHG, right insula, and negatively associated with the ReHo in the right IFG.
Interestingly, we found that the ReHo in the IFG predicted eudaimonic well-being, whereas
the OFC predicted positive affect, both of which were mediated by dispositional mindfulness.
To our knowledge, our findings provides initial evidence for linking individual differences in
dispositional mindfulness to spontaneous brain activity and demonstrate possible
neurobiological mechanisms that influence individual’s well-being through dispositional
mindfulness.
The finding that dispositional mindfulness is positively associated with the ReHo in the
left OFC, left PHG, right insula, and negatively associated with the ReHo in the right IFG is
consistent with previous studies in which state and dispositional mindfulness are related to
structural changes or altered activities of the insula (Hölzel et al., 2008; Lazar et al., 2005;
Lutz et al., 2013; Murakami et al., 2012; Zeidan et al., 2011), OFC (Klimecki et al., 2012;
Luders et al., 2009; Zeidan et al., 2011), PHG (Leung, Chan, Yin, Lee, So, & Lee, 2013;
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
16
Murakami et al., 2012; Lazar et al., 2000) and IFG (Ding et al., 2015; Hasenkamp et al., 2012;
Lee et al., 2012; Manna et al., 2010; Mascaro, Rilling, Negi, & Raison, 2013; Tang et al.,
2013). Thus, these findings suggest that state and dispositional mindfulness might share
common neural mechanisms. In addition, low ReHo in the OFC, PHG and insula among
individuals with low mindfulness scores might reflect reduced cognitive processing in the
affected regions, thus leading to low spontaneous brain activity among these individuals.
High ReHo in the IFG among individuals with low mindfulness scores might imply a
reorganization mechanism for neuroplasticity and cortical remapping or disruption of
inhibitory mechanisms. Future studies are needed to investigate the physiological
significance of ReHo before we can more accurately interpret the direction of the results.
Importantly, we found that the ReHo in the IFG, PHG, OFC and insula accounted for the
unique variation in dispositional mindfulness, suggesting that these regions likely contribute
to levels of mindfulness through different cognitive processes. First, the OFC has been
implicated in regulating affective responses by reframing the contextual evaluation of sensory
events (Rolls & Grabenhorst, 2008; Zeidan et al., 2011) and encoding reward value of pain or
pleasure (Kringelbach et al., 2005; Leknes & Tracey, 2008; O'Doherty, Kringelbach, Rolls,
Hornak, & Andrews, 2001; Peters & Büchel, 2010). Therefore, the mindfulness-related OFC
involvement may reflect altered processing of reward and hedonic experience (e.g., positive
affect) (O’Doherty et al., 2001; Peters & Büchel, 2010). Second, overwhelming evidence
suggests that the functions of response inhibition and attentional control are localized to the
right IFG (Aron, Fletcher, Bullmore, Sahakian, & Robbins, 2003; Aron, Robbins, & Poldrack,
2004; Hampshire, Chamberlain, Monti, Duncan, Owen, 2010). Behaviourally, mindfulness
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
17
has been shown to relate to response inhibition and attentional control (Chambers, Lo, &
Allen, 2008; Heeren, Van Broeck, & Philippot, 2009; Lee & Chao, 2012). Thus, response
inhibition and attentional control might be an important mechanism for achieving
mindfulness. Third, as a key node of the “default mode network” (DMN; Buckner,
Andrews-Hanna, & Schacter, 2008; Fair et al., 2008), the PHG has been involved in
self-processing such as self-evaluation and autobiographical memory retrieval (e.g., Levine,
Turner, Tisserand, Hevenor, Graham, & McIntosh, 2004; Masaoka, Sugiyama, Katayama,
Kashiwagi, & Homma, 2012; Northoff, Heinzel, de Greck, Bermpohl, Dobrowolny, &
Panksepp, 2006; Pauly, Finkelmeyer, Schneider, & Habel, 2012). Behavioral studies have
documented changes in self-concept such as more positive self-representation and higher
self-acceptance following meditation practice (Hölzel et al., 2011). Therefore, the PHG
involvement may reflect the beneficial changes in perspective on the self. Finally, the insula
has been shown to be implicated in interoception and body awareness as well as emotional
awareness (Craig, 2009; Singer, Critchley, & Preuschoff, 2009). Behaviorally, meditation
practitioners report improved capability for body awareness (Hölzel et al., 2011). Such roles
are consistent with the aspect of mindfulness that involves the focus of internal experience
including sensory experiences of breathing and those related to emotions or other body
sensations. These functions of all the brain regions seem to support Hölzel et al.’s (2011)
theoretical view that mindfulness exerts its beneficial effects through an array of cognitive
mechanisms, namely, executive attention, body awareness, emotion processing, and change
in perspective on the self.
Interestingly, we found that dispositional mindfulness influenced individuals’ well-being
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
18
through different brain mechanisms. On one hand, our finding fits nicely with our hypothesis
that the OFC is associated with hedonic well-being through dispositional mindfulness. This is
consistent with the recent finding that compassion training elicits OFC activity and increases
positive affective experiences, in response to others’ suffering (Klimecki et al., 2013).
Previous imaging studies have demonstrated the role of the OFC in positive affect. For
example, Welborn, Papademetris, Reis, Rajeevan, Bloise and Gray (2009) reported that larger
OFC volume is associated with expressivity of positive emotions (but not of negative).
Furthermore, trait positive affect corresponding to the personality dimension of extraversion
was reported to be associated with structural changes or altered activation in the OFC
(Cremers et al., 2011; Mobbs, Hagan, Azim, Menon, & Reiss, 2005; Omura, Constable, &
Canli, 2005; Rauch, Milad, Orr, Quinn, Fischl, & Pitman, 2005). Overwhelming evidence
suggests that the OFC represents the reward value, expected reward value and subjective
pleasantness of foods and other reinforcers, all of which are essential for hedonic experience
(Gottfried, O'Doherty, & Dolan, 2003; Kringelbach et al., 2005; O’Doherty et al., 2001;
Peters & Büchel, 2010). Thus, the mindfulness-related OFC involvement may reflect
increased capacities for processing hedonic experience, which leads to higher levels of
positive affect.
On the other hand, the ReHo in the IFG significantly predicted eudaimonic well-being,
which was entirely mediated by dispositional mindfulness. On one hand, this result indicates
the important roles of the IFG in eudaimonic well-being. This is consistent with the previous
studies exploring the neural basis of eudaimonic well-being. For example, eudaimonic
well-being is found to be associated with activities of the PFC in response to affective stimuli
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
19
(Heller et al., 2013; van Reekum et al., 2007). On the other hand, our results substantiate that
dispositional mindfulness may serve as a potential mechanism that explains the impact of the
IFG on eudaimonic well-being. Previous studies have demonstrated the functions of the
mindfulness-related region in response inhibition and attentional control (Aron et al., 2003,
2004; Hampshire et al., 2010). All these IFG-related processes are of great value for
providing purpose and self-relevant goals as well as building social connections, which may
lead to high levels of eudaimonic well-being.
Several limitations of the present study should be mentioned. First, because of the
cross-sectional nature of this study, we cannot determine the direction of causation between
dispositional mindfulness, well-being and the ReHo of brain regions. Longitudinal studies
will be particularly useful to disentangle complex relationships between dispositional
mindfulness, well-being and relevant brain regions. Second, this study used a sample of
college students. Although college students are commonly chose as participants (e.g., Kong et
al., 2014b; Murakami et al., 2012), the narrow age range might limit the generalizability of
the results. Further investigation should use wider age ranges to explore the influence of age
on these results. Third, the present findings are based on a single dimensional measure of
mindfulness. Although the MAAS has been a widely used index of dispositional mindfulness
in various studies of mindfulness (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Kong et al., 2014a; Wang & Kong,
2013), we believe that it would be important for the field to continue to explore other
measures of mindfulness, to characterize and represent this multi-faceted construct more
accurately. Finally, the scales used in the study are not direct measures of either hedonic
or eudaimonic well-being; therefore, further studies are needed to examine the relation
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
20
between mindfulness and hedonic and eudaimonic well-being with direct measures.
To conclude, we employed the ReHo approach to investigate the potential neurobiological
mechanisms subserving the contribution of dispositional mindfulness to hedonic and
eudaimonic well-being of healthy individuals. We found that the ReHo values of the left OFC,
left PHG, right insula, and right IFG could predict individual differences in dispositional
mindfulness, revealing a potential resting-state neural mechanism for dispositional
mindfulness. Moreover, although dispositional mindfulness contributed to both forms of
well-being, there were apparently different neurobiological mechanisms through which
dispositional mindfulness influenced various forms of well-being. Specifically, dispositional
mindfulness mediated the effects of the IFG on eudaimonic well-being and that of the OFC
on positive affect (i.e., hedonic well-being). Our findings might have important implications
for how to implement mindfulness-related interventions aimed at enhancing various forms of
well-being. Hölzel et al. (2011) have mentioned that various types of mindfulness practice
may emphasize the different components of mindfulness. For example, breath awareness
practice might mainly involve attention control and body awareness whereas loving-kindness
and compassion meditation might involve emotion processing and the change in perspective
on the self. Thus, further studies may develop appropriate types of mindfulness practice for
different forms of well-being individuals would like to cultivate.
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
21
References
Andersson, J. L., Jenkinson, M., & Smith, S. (2007). Non-linear registration, aka Spatial
normalisation. FMRIB Analysis Group Technical Reports.
Aristotle. (1925). Nicomachean Ethics (D. Ross, Trans.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Aron, A. R., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2003).
Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans.
Nature neuroscience, 6(2), 115-116. doi: 10.1038/nn1003
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal
cortex. Trends in cognitive sciences, 8(4), 170-177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report the
Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment, 11(3), 191-206. doi:
10.1177/1073191104268029
Bao, X., Xue, S., & Kong, F. (2015). Dispositional mindfulness and perceived stress: The role
of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 78(0), 48-52. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.01.007
Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2013). Neuroscience of affect: brain mechanisms of
pleasure and displeasure. Current opinion in neurobiology, 23(3), 294-303. doi:
10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.017
Biswal, B. B. (2012). Resting state fMRI: a personal history. Neuroimage, 62(2), 938-944.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.090
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The Benefits of Being Present: Mindfulness and Its
Role in Psychological Well-Being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4),
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
22
822-848. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations
and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 211-237. doi:
10.1080/10478400701598298
Buckner, R. L., Andrews‐Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain's default
network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 1-38. doi:
10.1196/annals.1440.011
Carmody, J., & Baer, R. (2008). Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of
mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness-based
stress reduction program. Journal of behavioral medicine, 31(1), 23-33. doi:
10.1007/s10865-007-9130-7
Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness
training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 32(3), 303-322. Doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0
Cohen, J. R., Asarnow, R. F., Sabb, F. W., Bilder, R. M., Bookheimer, S. Y., Knowlton, B. J.,
& Poldrack, R. A. (2010). Decoding Developmental Differences and Individual
Variability in Response Inhibition Through Predictive Analyses Across Individuals.
Frontiers in human neuroscience, 4, 47. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00047
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 59-70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555
Crawford, J. R., & Henry, J. D. (2004). The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS):
Construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
23
sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43(3), 245-265. doi:
10.1348/0144665031752934
Cremers, H., van Tol, M.-J., Roelofs, K., Aleman, A., Zitman, F. G., van Buchem, M. A., . . .
van der Wee, N. J. A. (2011). Extraversion Is Linked to Volume of the Orbitofrontal
Cortex and Amygdala. PloS one, 6(12), e28421. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028421
Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural correlates
of dispositional mindfulness during affect labeling. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(6),
560-565. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3180f6171f
Ding, X., Tang, Y.-Y., Cao, C., Deng, Y., Wang, Y., Xin, X., & Posner, M. I. (2015).
Short-term meditation modulates brain activity of insight evoked with solution cue. Social
cognitive and affective neuroscience, 10(1), 43-49. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu032
Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Dosenbach, N. U. F., Church, J. A., Miezin, F. M., Barch, D. M., . . .
Schlaggar, B. L. (2008). The maturing architecture of the brain's default network.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(10), 4028-4032. doi:
10.1073/pnas.0800376105
Falkenström, F. (2010). Studying mindfulness in experienced meditators: A
quasi-experimental approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(3), 305-310. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.022
Farb, N. A., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A. K.
(2007). Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of
self-reference. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 2(4), 313-322. doi:
10.1093/scan/nsm030
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
24
Fox, M. D., & Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed
with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(9),
700-711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts
build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build
consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5),
1045-1062. doi: 10.1037/a0013262
Gallagher, M. W., Lopez, S. J., & Preacher, K. J. (2009). The Hierarchical Structure of
Well‐Being. Journal of personality, 77(4), 1025-1050. doi:
10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00573.x
Geschwind, N., Peeters, F., Drukker, M., van Os, J., & Wichers, M. (2011). Mindfulness
training increases momentary positive emotions and reward experience in adults
vulnerable to depression: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of consulting and clinical
psychology, 79(5), 618-628. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024595
Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on
emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Emotion, 10(1), 83-91. doi:
10.1037/a0018441
Gottfried, J. A., O'Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Encoding predictive reward value in
human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science, 301(5636), 1104-1107. doi:
10.1126/science.1087919
Grant, J. A., Courtemanche, J., Duerden, E. G., Duncan, G. H., & Rainville, P. (2010).
Cortical thickness and pain sensitivity in zen meditators. Emotion, 10(1), 43-53. doi:
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
25
10.1037/a0018334
Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress
reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 57(1),
35-43. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7
Hampshire, A., Chamberlain, S. R., Monti, M. M., Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2010). The
role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage,
50(3), 1313-1319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109
Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Duncan, E., & Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Mind
wandering and attention during focused meditation: a fine-grained temporal analysis of
fluctuating cognitive states. Neuroimage, 59(1), 750-760. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008
Heeren, A., Van Broeck, N., & Philippot, P. (2009). The effects of mindfulness on executive
processes and autobiographical memory specificity. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
47(5), 403-409. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.01.017
Heller, A. S., van Reekum, C. M., Schaefer, S. M., Lapate, R. C., Radler, B. T., Ryff, C. D., &
Davidson, R. J. (2013). Sustained striatal activity predicts eudaimonic well-being and
cortisol output. Psychological science, 24(11), 2191-2200. doi:
10.1177/0956797613490744
Hölzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011).
How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a
conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 537-559.
doi: 10.1177/1745691611419671
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
26
Hölzel, B. K., Ott, U., Gard, T., Hempel, H., Weygandt, M., Morgen, K., & Vaitl, D. (2008).
Investigation of mindfulness meditation practitioners with voxel-based morphometry.
Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 3(1), 55-61. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm038
Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not
emotional well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(38),
16489-16493. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011492107
Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness on psychological
health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical psychology review, 31(6), 1041-1056. doi:
10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C., & Singer, T. (2013). Functional neural plasticity and
associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cerebral Cortex, 23(7),
1552-1561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs142
Kong, F., Ding, K., Yang, Z., Dang, X., Hu, S., Song, Y., & Liu, J. (2014b). Examining gray
matter structures associated with individual differences in global life satisfaction in a
large sample of young adults. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. doi:
10.1093/scan/nsu144
Kong, F., Hu, S., Xue, S., Song, Y., & Liu, J. (2015). Extraversion mediates the relationship
between structural variations in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and social well-being.
Neuroimage, 105, 269-275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.062
Kong, F., Wang, X., & Zhao, J. (2014a). Dispositional mindfulness and life satisfaction: The
role of core self-evaluations. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 165-169. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.002
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
27
Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic
experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(9), 691-702. doi: 10.1038/nrn1747
Lazar, S. W., Bush, G., Gollub, R. L., Fricchione, G. L., Khalsa, G., & Benson, H. (2000).
Functional brain mapping of the relaxation response and meditation. Neuroreport, 11(7),
1581-1585.
Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R., Greve, D. N., Treadway, M. T., . . .
Benson, H. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness.
Neuroreport, 16(17), 1893-1897. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19
Lee, T. M., Leung, M.-K., Hou, W.-K., Tang, J. C., Yin, J., So, K.-F., . . . Chan, C. C. (2012).
Distinct neural activity associated with focused-attention meditation and loving-kindness
meditation. PloS one, 7(8), e40054.
Lee, Y.-C., & Chao, H.-F. (2012). The role of active inhibitory control in psychological
well-being and mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(5), 618-621. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.001
Leknes, S., & Tracey, I. (2008). A common neurobiology for pain and pleasure. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 314-320. doi: 10.1038/nrn2333
Leung, M.-K., Chan, C. C., Yin, J., Lee, C.-F., So, K.-F., & Lee, T. M. (2013). Increased gray
matter volume in the right angular and posterior parahippocampal gyri in loving-kindness
meditators. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 8(1), 34-39. doi:
10.1093/scan/nss076
Levine, B., Turner, G. R., Tisserand, D., Hevenor, S. J., Graham, S. J., & McIntosh, A. R.
(2004). The functional neuroanatomy of episodic and semantic autobiographical
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
28
remembering: a prospective functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
16(9), 1633-1646. doi: 10.1162/0898929042568587
Li, W., Li, X., Huang, L., Kong, X., Yang, W., Wei, D., . . . Qiu, J. (2014). Brain structure
links trait creativity to openness to experience. Social cognitive and affective
neuroscience, nsu041. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu041
Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Wood, A. M., Osborne, G., & Hurling, R. (2009). Measuring
happiness: The higher order factor structure of subjective and psychological well-being
measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 878-884. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.010
Luders, E., Toga, A. W., Lepore, N., & Gaser, C. (2009). The underlying anatomical
correlates of long-term meditation: larger hippocampal and frontal volumes of gray matter.
Neuroimage, 45(3), 672-678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.061
Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Regulation of the
neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: effects of meditative expertise.
PloS one, 3(3), e1897. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001897
Lutz, J., Herwig, U., Opialla, S., Hittmeyer, A., Jäncke, L., Rufer, M., . . . Brühl, A. B. (2013).
Mindfulness and emotion regulation—an fMRI study. Social cognitive and affective
neuroscience, nst043. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst043
Manna, A., Raffone, A., Perrucci, M. G., Nardo, D., Ferretti, A., Tartaro, A., . . . Romani, G.
L. (2010). Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation.
Brain research, 82(1), 46-56. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.03.001
Marcoulides, G. A., & Hershberger, S. L. (1997). Multivariate statistical methods: A first
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
29
course. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Masaoka, Y., Sugiyama, H., Katayama, A., Kashiwagi, M., & Homma, I. (2012).
Remembering the past with slow breathing associated with activity in the
parahippocampus and amygdala. Neurosci Lett, 521(2), 98-103. doi:
10.1016/j.neulet.2012.05.047
Mascaro, J. S., Rilling, J. K., Negi, L. T., & Raison, C. L. (2013). Compassion meditation
enhances empathic accuracy and related neural activity. Social cognitive and affective
neuroscience, 8(1), 48-55. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss095
Mobbs, D., Hagan, C. C., Azim, E., Menon, V., & Reiss, A. L. (2005). Personality predicts
activity in reward and emotional regions associated with humor. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 102(45), 16502-16506. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0408457102
Modinos, G., Ormel, J., & Aleman, A. (2010). Individual differences in dispositional
mindfulness and brain activity involved in reappraisal of emotion. Social cognitive and
affective neuroscience, 5(4), 369-377. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0408457102
Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering:
finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12(3), 442-448. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026678
Murakami, H., Nakao, T., Matsunaga, M., Kasuya, Y., Shinoda, J., Yamada, J., & Ohira, H.
(2012). The structure of mindful brain. PloS one, 7(9), e46377. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0046377
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J.
(2006). Self-referential processing in our brain—a meta-analysis of imaging studies on
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
30
the self. Neuroimage, 31(1), 440-457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002
O'Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., & Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract
reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature
neuroscience, 4(1), 95-102. doi: 10.1038/82959
Omura, K., Constable, R. T., & Canli, T. (2005). Amygdala gray matter concentration is
associated with extraversion and neuroticism. Neuroreport, 16(17), 1905-1908. doi:
10.1097/01.wnr.0000186596.64458.76
Ott, U., Hölzel, B. K., & Vaitl, D. (2011). Brain structure and meditation: How spiritual
practice shapes the brain Neuroscience, consciousness and spirituality (pp. 119-128):
Springer.
Pauly, K., Finkelmeyer, A., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2012). The neural correlates of
positive self-evaluation and self-related memory. Social cognitive and affective
neuroscience, nss086. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss086
Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2010). Neural representations of subjective reward value.
Behavioural brain research, 213(2), 135-141. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.031
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing
and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior research methods,
40(3), 879-891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879
Qin, S., Young, C. B., Duan, X., Chen, T., Supekar, K., & Menon, V. (2014). Amygdala
subregional structure and intrinsic functional connectivity predicts individual differences
in anxiety during early childhood. Biol Psychiatry, 75(11), 892-900. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.10.006
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
31
Raichle, M. E. (2010). Two views of brain function. Trends in cognitive sciences, 14(4),
180-190. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.008
Rauch, S. L., Milad, M. R., Orr, S. P., Quinn, B. T., Fischl, B., & Pitman, R. K. (2005).
Orbitofrontal thickness, retention of fear extinction, and extraversion. Neuroreport,
16(17), 1909-1912. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186599.66243.50
Rolls, E. T., & Grabenhorst, F. (2008). The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to
decision-making. Progress in neurobiology, 86(3), 216-244. doi:
10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.001
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research
on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual review of psychology, 52(1), 141-166. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141
Ryff, C., Almeida, D. M., Ayanian, J. S., Carr, D. S., Cleary, P. D., Coe, C., . . . Marks, N. F.
(2007). Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS II), 2004–2006. Ann Arbor,
MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR).
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of
psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6),
1069-1081. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069
Ryif, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The Structure of Psychological Well-Being Revisited.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(4), 719-727. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719
Schutte, N. S., & Malouff, J. M. (2011). Emotional intelligence mediates the relationship
between mindfulness and subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences,
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
32
50(7), 1116-1119. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.01.037
Singer, T., Critchley, H. D., & Preuschoff, K. (2009). A common role of insula in feelings,
empathy and uncertainty. Trends in cognitive sciences, 13(8), 334-340. doi:
10.1016/j.tics.2009.05.001
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The Restless Mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6),
946-958. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946
Sun, P., & Kong, F. (2013). Affective mediators of the influence of gratitude on life
satisfaction in late adolescence. Social Indicators Research, 114(3), 1361-1369. doi:
10.1007/s11205-013-0333-8
Supekar, K., Swigart, A. G., Tenison, C., Jolles, D. D., Rosenberg-Lee, M., Fuchs, L., &
Menon, V. (2013). Neural predictors of individual differences in response to math tutoring
in primary-grade school children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
110(20), 8230-8235. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1222154110
Tang, Y.-Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2013). Brief meditation training induces smoking
reduction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(34), 13971-13975. doi:
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1311887110
Tian, L., Ren, J., & Zang, Y. (2012). Regional homogeneity of resting state fMRI signals
predicts Stop signal task performance. Neuroimage, 60(1), 539-544. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.098
van Reekum, C. M., Urry, H. L., Johnstone, T., Thurow, M. E., Frye, C. J., Jackson, C. A., . . .
Davidson, R. J. (2007). Individual differences in amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal
cortex activity are associated with evaluation speed and psychological well-being. Journal
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
33
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(2), 237-248. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.2.237
Wang, L., Song, M., Jiang, T., Zhang, Y., & Yu, C. (2011). Regional homogeneity of the
resting-state brain activity correlates with individual intelligence. Neurosci Lett, 488(3),
275-278. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.11.046
Wang, T., Chen, Z., Zhao, G., Hitchman, G., Liu, C., Zhao, X., . . . Chen, A. (2014). Linking
inter-individual differences in the conflict adaptation effect to spontaneous brain activity.
Neuroimage, 90, 146-152. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.055
Wang, Y., & Kong, F. (2014). The role of emotional intelligence in the impact of mindfulness
on life satisfaction and mental distress. Social Indicators Research, 843-852. doi:
10.1007/s11205-013-0327-6
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-1070. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Wei, L., Duan, X., Yang, Y., Liao, W., Gao, Q., Ding, J.-r., . . . Lu, G. (2011). The
synchronization of spontaneous BOLD activity predicts extraversion and neuroticism.
Brain research, 1419, 68-75. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.08.060
Welborn, B. L., Papademetris, X., Reis, D. L., Rajeevan, N., Bloise, S. M., & Gray, J. R.
(2009). Variation in orbitofrontal cortex volume: relation to sex, emotion regulation and
affect. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 4(4), 328-339. doi:
10.1093/scan/nsp028
Zang, Y., Jiang, T., Lu, Y., He, Y., & Tian, L. (2004). Regional homogeneity approach to
fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage, 22(1), 394-400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.030
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
34
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Gordon, N. S., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Effects of brief and sham
mindfulness meditation on mood and cardiovascular variables. J Altern Complement Med,
16(8), 867-873. doi: 10.1089/acm.2009.0321
Zeidan, F., Martucci, K. T., Kraft, R. A., Gordon, N. S., McHaffie, J. G., & Coghill, R. C.
(2011). Brain mechanisms supporting the modulation of pain by mindfulness meditation.
Journal of Neuroscience, 31(14), 5540-5548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5791-10.2011
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
35
Figure Legends
Fig. 1. Brain regions that correlated with individual differences in dispositional
mindfulness. Dispositional mindfulness was negatively correlated with the ReHo in right
inferior frontal gyrus (A) and positively correlated with the ReHo in the left parahippocampal
gyrus (B), right insula (C) and left orbitofrontal cortex (D). The coordinate is shown in the
MNI stereotactic space.
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
36
Fig. 2. Scatter plots depicting correlations between ReHo and individual differences in
dispositional mindfulness. Scatter plots depicting correlations between dispositional
mindfulness and ReHo in the right inferior frontal gyrus (A, r = -0.26, p < 0.001), left
parahippocampal gyrus (B, r = 0.23, p < 0.001) right insula (C, r = 0.22, p < 0.001) and left
orbitofrontal cortex (D, r = 0.21, p < 0.001) after adjusting for age and sex.
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
37
Fig. 3. Dispositional mindfulness mediates the association between spontaneous brain
activity and hedonic well-being. Depicted is the path diagram (including standard regression
coefficients) of mediation analysis demonstrating that spontaneous brain activity affects
individuals’ positive affect through dispositional mindfulness. *: p < 0.05. **: p < 0.01. ***:
p < 0.001.
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
38
Fig. 4. Dispositional mindfulness mediates the association between spontaneous brain
activity and eudaimonic well-being. Depicted is the path diagram (including standard
regression coefficients) of mediation analysis demonstrating that spontaneous brain activity
affects individuals’ eudaimonic well-being through dispositional mindfulness. *: p < 0.05. **:
p < 0.01. ***: p < 0.001.
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
39
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for mindfulness, hedonic and eudaimonic well-being
Minimum Maximum Mean SD Skewness Kurtosis
Mindfulness 21 83 56.33
9.14
-0.17 0.80
Positive affect 20 48 34.14
4.80
-0.07 -0.14
Negative affect 12 40 24.40
4.72
0.41 0.36
Eudaimonic well-being 129 240 175.13
19.45
0.05 -0.21
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015
Accepted Manuscript
40
Table 2
Brain regions that correlated with individual differences in dispositional mindfulness
Region Side MNI coordinate
x y z
Z Cluster size
(mm3)
Positive correlation
Insula R 36 14 –10 3.64 920*
PHG L –20 –32 –14 3.41 880*
OFC L –24 14 –26 3.86 768*
Negative correlation
IFG R 60 10 28 -4.10 1024*
Note: MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute; L = left; R = right. OFC = Orbitofrontal cortex;
PHG = Parahippocampal gyrus; IFG = Inferior frontal gyrus. * p < 0.01 (corrected).
Downloaded by [Beijing Normal University] at 21:12 16 September 2015