Content uploaded by Karen Spruyt
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Karen Spruyt on Nov 15, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
Sleep Disordered Breathing and
Academic Performance:
A Meta-analysis
Barbara Galland, PhDa, Karen Spruyt, PhDb,c, Patrick Dawes, MBChB, FRCSd, Philippa S. McDowall, PhDe,
Dawn Elder, MBChB, FRACP, PhDe, Elizabeth Schaughency, PhDf
abstract BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: Sleep-disordered breathing (SDB) in children is associated with daytime
functioning decrements in cognitive performance and behavioral regulation. Studies
addressing academic achievement are underrepresented. This study aimed to evaluate the
strength of the relationships between SDB and achievement in core domains and general
school performance.
METHODS: Data sources included PubMed, Web of Science, CINAHL, and PsycINFO. Studies of
school-aged children investigating the relationships between SDB and academic achievement
were selected for inclusion in a systematic literature review using Preferred Reporting Items
for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines. Data extracted were converted into
standardized mean differences; effect sizes (ES) and statistics were calculated by using
random-effects models. Heterogeneity tests (I
2
) were conducted.
RESULTS: Of 488 studies, 16 met eligibility criteria. SDB was significantly associated with poorer
academic performance for core academic domains related to language arts (ES –0.31; P,.001;
I
2
= 74%), math (ES –0.33; P,.001; I
2
= 55%), and science (ES –0.29; P= .001; I
2
= 0%), and
with unsatisfactory progress/learning problems (ES –0.23; P,.001; I
2
= 0%) but not general
school performance.
CONCLUSIONS: Variable definitions of both academic performance and SDB likely contributed to
the heterogeneity among published investigations. Clear links between SDB and poorer
academic performance in school-age children are demonstrated. ES statistics were in the small
to medium range, but nevertheless the findings serve to highlight to parents, teachers, and
clinicians that SDB in children may contribute to academic difficulties some children face.
Departments of aWomen’s & Children’s Health, and dSurgical Sciences, Dunedin School of Medicine, Universit y of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand; bDepartment of Developmental and Behavioral
Pediatrics, Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China; cFaculty of Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Brussel and School for Mental Health and Neurosciences, Maastricht University,
Maastricht, Netherlands; eDepartment of Paediatrics and Child Health, Wellington School of Medicine, Universit y of Otago, Wellington, New Zealand; and fDepartment of Psychology, University
of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
Dr Barbara Galland conceptualized and designed the project and drafted the initial manuscript; Dr Spruyt contributed to the data collection methods, carried out the data
analyses, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Drs Dawes and McDowall carried out the quality review of the papers included in the meta-analysis and reviewed and
revised the manuscript; Dr Elder contributed to the quality review of the papers included in the meta-analysis and reviewed and revised the manuscript, Dr Schaughency
contributed to the design of the project and data collection methods and reviewed and revised the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2015-1677
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1677
Accepted for publication Jul 21, 2015
Address correspondence to Barbara Galland, PhD, Department of Women’s & Children’s Health, University of Otago, PO Box 56, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand. E-mail:
barbara.galland@otago.ac.nz
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).
Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
REVIEW ARTICLE PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 4, October 2015
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
Sleep-disordered breathing (SDB) in
children covers a spectrum of
breathing abnormalities ranging from
habitual snoring, to upper airway
resistance syndrome, to frank
obstructive sleep apnea (OSA).
1
The
prevalence of habitual snoring
reported in the pediatric literature,
based mainly on parental report,
ranges from 1.5% to 27.6%
2–4
but is
most commonly estimated at 6% to
12%,
5
whereas OSA is estimated to
affect up to 1% to 4% of children.
6,7
Adenotonsillar hypertrophy is
a major contributor to SDB in
children, and removing these
(adenotonsillectomy) leads to
significant improvement in SDB
symptoms for most children,
8
but
obesity, craniofacial genetics, and
neural control mechanisms of upper
airway patency are other
contributory factors.
9
Since the late 1990s, a large body of
literature has focused on the
neurocognitive correlates of SDB.
Research indicates the most common
behavioral and neurocognitive effects
to be evident in measures of constructs
such as intelligence, memory,
executive function, attention, and
hyperactivity.
10–12
Poorer performance
in these domains has been suggested
to affect daytime functioning, leading
to suboptimal academic achievement
compared with healthy peers. For
example, links between attention-
executive functioning, an umbrella
term incorporating attentional control,
behavioral inhibition, working
memory, and academic achievement,
are well documented,
13,14
as are links
between the symptoms of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder and
academic underachievement.
15,16
Limited developmental growth in
executive functioning predicts
behavioral and emotional adjustment
difficulties and negative academic self-
concept
17
with potential deleterious
effects on later academic
achievement.
18
The neurocognitive sequelae of SDB
resolve to some extent when treated
by adenotonsillectomy.
19–21
Outcomes beyond 1 year are largely
unknown aside from 1 study of
school-age children that reported
improvements within some
neurocognitive domains 4 years after
adenotonsillectomy.
22
Although
the positive outcomes of
adenotonsillectomy are well
documented, studies addressing
academic performance as an outcome
are underrepresented compared with
other functional outcomes, and little
is known about the potentially
remedial effects of SDB treatment on
school performance. The majority of
studies report parent or teacher
ratings or global measures of school
performance, with fewer reporting
results of more labor- and time-
intensive standardized achievement
test results. Methodological factors
potentially contribute to obtained
results; observed links between
potential neurocognitive sequelae of
SDB and achievement may vary
depending on how each is
measured,
14
and measures used may
not be sensitive to treatment
effects.
23
Differences in cultural and
educational contexts add to the
complexity; nevertheless, research
consistently links potential
neurocognitive sequelae of SDB to
achievement across cultural
contexts.
24
However, many studies
fail to consider general underlying
traits in the relationship between SDB
and academic achievement, such as
the associations between sleep
problems and general intellectual
ability.
25
The purpose of this report is to
systematically review the literature in
respect of the association between
SDB and academic performance and,
through meta-analyses, quantify the
reported associations. Because there
are currently no universally accepted
disease cutoffs for diagnosing SDB in
children, and because of the wide
range of definitions used, we included
studies where either subjective
reports or objective measures defined
SDB. For academic performance, we
chose to only include studies that
either directly reported on school
performance (eg, grades) or assessed
performance in academic skill areas
(eg, reading, math). These data should
better inform medical and
nonmedical professionals who
address children’s educational needs
regarding the potential role of SDB in
children’s performance at school.
METHODS
The protocol and data extraction was
conducted according to the 2009
Preferred Reporting Items for
Systematic Reviews and Meta-
Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines.
26
Search Strategy
A search strategy was developed to
identify studies related to SDB and
academic performance in school-age
children/adolescents. An extensive
literature search of 4 electronic
databases up to March 1, 2015, was
conducted: PubMed, Web of Science,
CINAHL, and PsycINFO. The search
strategy used for PubMed is
presented in Supplemental Appendix
1. Titles and abstracts were examined
to extract potentially relevant articles,
subsequently examined in more
depth for inclusion/exclusion criteria
by the main author (B.G.) and the
research assistant (C.L.). Reference
lists of original research and review
articles were also examined to search
for relevant studies. Articles for
which no abstract was available
online were reviewed on the full text.
Full text was retrieved if a decision
could not be made on the basis of the
article abstract. All possible effort
was made to obtain full-text articles,
including contacting the authors by
e-mail.
Inclusion Criteria
For inclusion, studies had to fulfill the
following criteria: (1) an original
article; (2) a study that was
observational, experimental,
longitudinal, cross-sectional, or
a cohort study that investigated the
PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 4, October 2015 e935
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
relationship between SDB and
academic performance; (3)
participants were school-age children
or adolescents; (4) participants were
drawn from a general community
population or patients referred to
a clinic for SDB concerns; (4) both SDB
and non-SDB (control) children were
included in the participant sample; (5)
article included $1 of the following
academic variables of interest: global
school grade; test performance or
grade on math, language arts (reading,
writing, spelling), or science; parent or
teacher report or subjective rating of
academic performance; (6) SDB was
reported as children being
symptomatically at risk for SDB or
clinically diagnosed as having SDB;
(7) sample was well described
(eg, number of subjects, gender,
recruitment criteria, etc); (8) data for
variables of interest presented
numerically or graphically with
a measure of central tendency and
variance, correlational, odds ratios, or
proportions; (9) data were published
in a peer-reviewed journal; and (9)
article available in English (either
written in English or a translation
available). Where data were presented
graphically, the authors were
contacted to attempt to obtain
numerical data. No time limits were
applied.
Exclusion Criteria: Potentially
Relevant Articles
Studies were excluded if they were
(1) case reports; (2) clinic population
studies that were not related to SDB;
(3) retrospective studies; and (4) if
$1 published report from the same
study was available, we included only
1 with either the most detailed
information or better standard of
reporting.
Quality Assessment
Two independent reviewers assessed
risk of bias and precision for each
study using questions extracted from
the RTI Item Bank
27
; a 29-item
validated tool (organized into 11
domains) with specific instructions
for focusing evaluations of
observational studies of interventions
or exposures included in systematic
evidence reviews and can be adapted
for cross-sectional studies. This tool
allows customization from the
investigator to adapt it to the design
and requirements of the research
question of interest. Eight domains
were suitable for this study
comprising 15 items detailed in
Supplemental Appendix 2. Possible
responses to each item were
combinations of yes, no, partially,
cannot determine, and not applicable.
We grouped “cannot determine”and
“partially”into “unclear risk of bias.”
Two investigators (P.D. and P.M.)
scored the articles independently, and
discrepancies were resolved through
consultation with the other authors
(D.E. and B.G.).
SDB Category
Agreement was reached by 2 authors
(K.S. and B.G.) as to criteria for
inclusion in the study. Ideally, the
severity of SDB would be considered;
however, this was not possible given
the heterogeneity of SDB definitions
including different polysomnography
(PSG) indices used as cutoffs to
denote SDB or not, and no single
index is precise.
28
SDB included
objective and subjectively reported
snoring and OSA, including symptoms
of OSA used in reporting
classifications. Thus, the range
reported included primary snoring,
habitual snoring, symptomatic and
clinically diagnosed OSA, and
generalized SDB. In studies in which
data were presented with $2 levels
of severity of SDB (eg, mild, moderate,
and severe OSA or occasional and
habitual snoring), scores were
aggregated from all SDB groups.
Academic Performance Categories
Academic performance was measured
in several ways in the literature and
categorized broadly as follows: (1)
general performance (global
measures of self- parent- or school-
reported academic performance or
grade point average); (2) language
arts (reading, reading comprehension,
spelling, English [or other first
language in non-English speaking
countries], dictation); (3) math
(mathematics in general or number
operations); (4) science; and (5)
unsatisfactory progress (learning
problem, grade failure, poor school
performance). These categories were
agreed on by 2 authors (E.S. and B.G.).
Where there was $1 language art or
math measure available, data were
aggregated for each domain.
Data Analyses
Meta-analyses were performed by
using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis
software (version 2.2.064; Biostat,
Englewood, NJ). The statistical results
extracted from all studies were
converted into standardized mean
difference (d). Meta-analytic data
analyses require independence of
effect sizes (ES); therefore, when
studies reported multiple ES, these
were combined to independent ES by
intrastudy meta-analyses. Results
reflect relative weight under the
random effects model. Cohen’sd
classification of ES was used, such
that between group ES (within study)
of d#0.20 are small, d= 0.50 are
medium, and d$0.80 are large. The
average ES estimates for each
outcome variable of interest are
illustrated in Forest plots. Cohen’sU
3
index, which determines the percent
change in SDB group scores/grades
from the pooled mean estimates of
non-SDB groups, was used to
estimate the practical value of the
effect.
29
The percentage of observed
total variation across studies due to
real heterogeneity rather than chance
was evaluated by using the I
2
statistic
test. The value of I
2
ranges from 0%
to 100% with 0 indicating no
heterogeneity, 25% low
heterogeneity, 50% moderate
heterogeneity, and 75% high
heterogeneity. Heterogeneity and
statistical significance are reported on
plots to describe the variation
between studies.
e936 GALLAND et al
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
RESULTS
The search criteria from all databases
(with duplicates eliminated) yielded
488 articles (Fig 1). Of those, 218
were deemed to be of potential
interest by title and abstracts
examined to extract 79 articles of
interest. All articles were downloaded
to examine and apply the more
stringent inclusion/exclusion criteria.
Fifty-nine articles were excluded,
1 added (identified from a reference
list), leaving 21 articles for review
containing data suitable to extract for
meta-analysis. Authors of studies
were contacted to obtain a correction
to numerical data,
30
clarification of
the source providing academic
ratings,
31
and confirmation of
participant overlap within 2 studies
from 1 group,
32,33
2 studies from
a second group,
22,34
and 3 studies
from a third group.
35–37
Numerical
data were extracted from the graphs
provided by 1 study after
unsuccessful attempts to contact the
authors.
38
The final meta-analysis
included 16 articles.
Articles Reviewed
Table 1 presents the 16 articles
included in this review chronologically
and summarizes the important
aspects of each. All studies recruited
males and females, and all supplied
a breakdown. The review included
studies from 12 countries. The age of
the participants varied from 5 to 17
years. The number of participants in
each study ranged from 66 to 6349
with a median of 549.
Quality Assessment
Customization of the RTI Item Bank
questions for this study resulted in 8
domains suitable for use in this study
comprising 15 items given in
Supplemental Appendix 2. Results of
individual studies are given in
Supplementary Table 2 and
a graphical summary of the combined
results illustrated in Fig 2. All 16
studies were prospective, and the
majority of studies described the
study populations in detail with only
a few studies at unclear risk of bias by
providing partial information. We
chose to include PSG and individually
administered standardized
achievement tests as the highest
standards of measurement for SDB
and academic performance
respectively in consideration of the
studies using valid and reliable
measures. Eleven studies (69%) did
not use this standard of measurement
for SDB, and 14 (88%) did not use this
standard for academic performance
creating information bias. Two studies
(13%) were at high risk of selection
bias by not considering covariates, and
a further 7 (44%) only considered
some covariates in analyses, creating
an unclear risk of bias. Statistical
methods were evaluated as
appropriate in 75% of studies. In 10 of
the evaluated studies (63%), the
results were considered credible in
line with the individual study’s
limitations and 38% partially credible.
SDB Measures
Seven of the 16 studies used
habitual snoring as the core SDB
symptom based on parental
questionnaires
30,31,38–42
with only 2
studies providing evidence
supporting validity of questionnaires
used.
31,42
Witnessed apnea was used
as a core symptom of SDB,
43
and in
conjunction with habitual snoring, as
an addition/alternative symptom.
41
Validated questionnaire subscales
FIGURE 1
Systematic review search flow diagram based on Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews
and Meta-Analyses.
26
PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 4, October 2015 e937
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
TABLE 1 Summary of Studies Included in the Meta-analysis
Author (y) Country Subject Source, Age
Range or Grade,
nfor Study
SDB Defined/Diagnosed
Measure(s)
Academic Measure(s) Adjustment for
Covariates
Reported Findings
Ferreira
39
(2000)
Portugal Schools, 6–11 y,
n= 976
Parent reported:
frequency of loud
snoring
Parent reported: general
school performance
Unadjusted, however
stated that age,
gender, parental SES,
sleep duration all
nonsignificant
between SDB groups
Loud snoring not
associated with school
achievement
Arman
40
(2005)
Turkey Schools, 9.3–9.4 y
(mean), n= 286
Parent reported:
frequency of snoring
Parent reported: learning
disability, academic
performance failure
rating
Unadjusted; however,
stated that parent
education, parental
smoking, SES similar
across groups
No correlation between
HS and learning
disability or academic
performance failure
rating
Goodwin
37
(2005)
USA Schools, 11y,
n= 480
PSG: RDI $1 Parent reported:
frequent learning
problem
Adjusted for previous
factors with P,.1
predictive of SDB
Positive likelihood ratios
for learning problems
when SDB present
Ng
43
(2005) Hong
Kong
Schools, 6–12 y,
n= 3047
Parent reported:
witnessed apnea
Parent reported: general
performance in exam
results from previous
term; grade E for poor
grade
Adjusted for age, allergic
rhinitis, asthma,
mouth dryness when
rising
Witnessed apnea
marginal significance
for poor academic
results
Perez-Chada
41
(2007)
Argentina Schools, 10–15 y,
n= 1900
Parent reported:
frequency of snoring
Math and language
school grade
Adjusted for: age, gender,
BMI zscore, sleep
hours and specific
school
Occasional and frequent
snorers significantly
lower scores for math
and language
compared with
nonsnorers
Giordani
47
(2008)
USA Cases: otolaryngology
clinic; controls:
pediatric general
surgery clinic, 5–13 y,
n=66
PSG: scheduled for AT
with OAI $1 (AT/OSA+)
or OAI ,1 (AT/OSA–)
Child assessment: WIAT
for math, number
operations, spelling,
reading and reading
comprehension
Adjusted for age AT/OSA–had significantly
lower scores for math,
spelling, reading,
and reading
comprehension than
controls; AT/OSA+
groups significantly
lower scores for math
than controls
Sahin
31
(2009)
Turkey Schools, 7–13 y,
n= 1164
Parent reported:
frequency of snoring
Teacher reported:
examination results
from previous class
Adjusted for significant
demographic and
behavioral variables in
unadjusted model
No significant association
between HS and poor
school performance
Arslan
38
(2010)
Turkey Schools, 6–16 y,
n= 1952
Parent reported:
frequency of snoring
Parent reported: general
school performance
Unadjusted Significantly higher
occurrence of poorer
general school
performance in HS vs
never snorers
Li
42
(2010) China Schools, 5–14 y,
n= 6349
Parent reported:
frequency of snoring
Parent reported: poor
school performance
Adjusted for age, gender,
BMI zscore, atopic
symptoms, and
environmental and
SES factors
HS demonstrated to be
an independent
predictor of poor
school performance,
whereas occasional
snoring was not
Kim
44
(2011) Korea Schools, third grade,
n= 299
Parent reported: Sleep-
Related Breathing
Disorder Scale
School obtained
examination scores
for math, science and
language
Unadjusted Mean examination scores
for each subject
higher in controls
than SDB group, but
not significant
Ting
45
(2011) Taiwan Schools, 6–11 y,
n= 138
PSG: SDB severity based
on AHI .1 and #5,
or .5 and #15, and
AHI .15
Teacher-rated academic
performance on math
and language
Adjusted for age, gender,
BMI, parental
education, and
prematurity
Different severities of
SDB groups did not
show significant
associations with
math or language
e938 GALLAND et al
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
were used in 3 studies to identify
SDB.
44–46
Five studies used the
diagnosis of SDB based on PSG using
varying but related indices of
respiratory events; apnea hypopnea
index, respiratory disturbance index,
and obstructive apnea hypopnea
index with thresholds of .1or
$1.
32,34,37,47,48
Academic Performance Measures
Studies used different indicators of
school achievement; for general
school performance, mainly parent or
school reported general school
performance was used. Only 2 studies
directly assessed the children’s core
skills using standardized achievement
tests for math, spelling, or reading or
reading comprehension.
34,47
The
remainder used subject grade or
ratings as reported by the school,
teacher, or parent.
Meta-analyses
Forest plots for each outcome can be
found in Figs 3 and 4 illustrating the ES
estimates for SDB compared with
controls (non-SDB) on the academic
performance categories. A negative ES
estimate indicates those with SDB
performed worse on measures of
academic performance compared with
controls.
The pooled adjusted ES for general
school performance based on data
from 4 studies
30,38,39,45
were –0.15
(95% confidence interval [CI]: –0.38
to 0.01; Fig 3A). This ES did not
reach statistical significance
(P= .21). There was significant and
“moderate”evidence of
heterogeneity (I
2
= 67%; P= .03). All
studies were based on parent- or
teacher-reported school grades,
stated as the previous term or year
grade, or not stated.
Eight studies reported on language
arts measures that included spelling,
reading, reading comprehension, and
TABLE 1 Continued
Author (y) Country Subject Source, Age
Range or Grade,
nfor Study
SDB Defined/Diagnosed
Measure(s)
Academic Measure(s) Adjustment for
Covariates
Reported Findings
Bourke
34
(2011)
Australia Cases: pediatric sleep
clinic; controls:
community, 7–12 y,
n= 137
PSG: Primary snorers
(OAHI #1 with snoring
history or OAHI .1),
mild OSAS (1,OAHI
#5) and moderate/
severe OSAS
(OAHI .5)
Child assessment: WRAT-3
for math, spelling,
reading
Adjusted for SES Trends for all SDB groups
for elevated
impairment of
academic function
compared with
controls; statistically
significant for reading
and math for primary
snoring group and
math for moderate/
severe OSA
Kim
45
(2012) Korea Schools, 11–12 y,
n= 618
Parent reported: 4
questions on SDB
questionnaire;
preschool and present
School performance
score: sum of the
latest midterm and
final examination
scores
Adjusted for SDB stages
at preschool age and
gender
Examination scores not
significantly different
from controls for past,
present, or continuous
SDB
Brockmann
30
(2012a)
Chile Schools, 7–17 y,
n= 274
Parent reported:
frequency of snoring
School report grade for
math, language, and
average school grade
Adjusted for age, gender,
BMI, and hyperactivity
scale score
HS group compared with
never snorers had
significantly lower
grades for math and
average school grade,
but not language
Brockmann
32
(2012b)
Germany Schools, 9.5 y (mean),
n=92
PSG:OSA (AHI $1), UARS
(AHI ,1 and RDI = 1),
primary snoring (AHI
and RDI ,1 and
oxygen desaturation
index ,4)
School report form:
previous terms grade
for math, science, and
spelling
Adjusted for age, gender,
maternal and paternal
education, and class
membership
Compared with children
who never snored,
primary snorers and
UARS/OSA had
significantly lower
percentile grades for
math, science and
spelling
de Carvalho
46
(2013)
Brazil Schools, 7–10 y,
n= 2384
Parent reported: SBD
subscale of Sleep
Disturbance Scale for
Children
Teacher-reported grades
for math, and
language
Adjusted for age and
gender
Children with elevated
SBD had significantly
lower grades for math
and language
compared with those
with no symptoms of
sleep disorders
AHI, apnea hypopnea index; AT, adenotonsillectomy; HS, habitual snoring; NSD, no significant difference; OAHI, obstructive apnea hypopnea index; OAI, obstructive apnea index;
RDI, respiratory disturbance index; SBD, Sleep-breathing disorder; SES, socioeconomic status; UARS, upper airway resistance syndrome; WIAT, Wechsler Individualized Achievement Test ;
WRAT-3, Wide Range Achievement Test-3.
PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 4, October 2015 e939
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
English/other first language grades/
scores.
30,32,34,41,44,46–48
Tests from
validated performance measures
were used in just 2 studies. When
studies were combined for meta-
analyses, a significant (P,.001)
negative association between
SDB and language arts skills was
found (ES –0.31; 95% CI –0.46 to
–0.16) (Fig 3B) with a near “high”
level of heterogeneity (I
2
= 77%;
P,.001).
Thesame8studies
30,32,34,41,44,46–48
reporting on language arts also reported
math grades/scores showing a combined
significant negative association w ith
SDB (ES –0.33; 95% CI –0.45 to –0.22;
Fig 3C) with “moderate”heterogeneity
(I
2
=61%;P=.01).
Science grades were reported by just
2 studies
32,44
yielding a combined
significant negative association
with SDB (ES –0.29; 95% CI –0.49
to –0.10; Fig 3D). No obvious
heterogeneity was found (I
2
=0%;
P= .94).
Combining academic skill data from
general school performance, language
arts, math, and science data yields
a negative association with SDB with
a significant ES difference of –0.30
(CI –0.38 to –0.22) and no evidence of
heterogeneity (I
2
= 0%; P= .57). This
combined measure reflects
a generalized estimate of effect given
the heterogeneity of the data from
which the “overall”ES is calculated.
Five studies presented
unsatisfactory academic progress
data
31,37,40,42,43
(Fig 4). When these
studies were combined, SDB was
found to be significantly associated
with unsatisfactory school progress
(ES –0.23; 95% CI –0.35 to –0.10).
There was no evidence of
heterogeneity (I
2
=0%;P=.44).
Practical interpretation
To facilitate interpretation of the
practical significance of the outcomes,
Cohen’sU
3
index was used to
estimate the magnitude of change
from the pooled mean estimates for
significant outcomes. Thus SDB
scores/grades/ratings for language
arts, math and science, were 12.3%,
13.1% and 11.6% respectively below
non-SDB mean estimates. Combined
academic scores yielded
a deterioration of 11.8%.
DISCUSSION
This review combined evidence in the
literature to show that SDB in
children is significantly associated
with poorer academic performance in
domains related to core academic
language arts and numeracy skills,
and science ratings/grades. In
addition, SDB was significantly
associated with unsatisfactory
progress but not general school
performance. Integration of these
data through meta-analysis confirms
the frequently cited notion that SDB
in children is related to academic
underperformance. ES statistics are in
the small to medium range. Practical
application using the Cohen’sU
3
index
29
suggests 11.8% deterioration
in SDB groups for core academic
skills. Few studies assessing core
academic skills stated participants’
performance were outside of the
average range. However, our results do
have implications for children not
being able to reach their full academic
potential. All studies represent
achild’s academic performance at one
point in time. Additive negative effects
over time cannot be ruled out.
49
FIGURE 2
Risk of bias by 8 domains (in bold) summarized for all studies (n= 16) using 15 items from the RTI Item Bank.
27
Results in the right-hand panel show the
level of risk of bias (%) as high, unclear, or low risk.
e940 GALLAND et al
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
FIGURE 3
Effects sizes and 95% CIs from the included studies of academic performance measures of general performance (A), language arts (B), math (C), science
(D), and combined academic skills (E) comparing SDB to non‐SDB. Square represents the area proportional to the weight assigned to the study within
each meta‐analysis; diamond represents the overall summary estimate for the analysis (width of the diamond represents the 95% CI).
PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 4, October 2015 e941
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
Compared with the large number of
studies using standardized tests for
other functional outcomes (eg,
behavior and cognition), only a few
studies used standardized tests to
assess academic achievement. The
majority documented subject grade or
ratings as reported by the school,
teacher or parent. Teachers’ratings
could potentially reflect hyperactive
and disruptive classroom behavior
influenced by SDB.
50
Improvements in
cross-study comparisons could be
made if more studies evaluating
academic achievement used
standardized and validated
achievement measures. For large-
scale studies, costs and time to
administer tests were likely important
reasons individually administered
standardized achievement tests were
not used, pointing to the need for
considerations of both efficiency and
effectiveness in educational
assessment.
Variable definitions of both academic
performance and SDB measures may
have contributed to the heterogeneity
among published investigations. Only
5 studies reported SDB based on gold
standard PSG diagnoses. Just under
half the studies used habitual snoring
as the core SDB symptom from parent
report. Although parental reports
may be susceptible to reporting bias,
there is some objective evidence
that supports the reliability and
accuracy of the subjective reporting
of habitual snoring, at least in young
children.
51,52
Older children are not
as frequently observed during sleep
as younger children, and therefore
parent history to define SDB may be
less reliable in studies of older
children.
It was not possible to relate the
severity of SDB to outcomes of
academic achievement due to the
array of SDB definitions used;
however, some previous studies
have failed to demonstrate a dose-
response relationship between
PSG-derived severity of SDB with
learning
53
and other functional
outcomes.
54–56
Habitual snoring has
been related to as many, or more,
behavior and executive function
problems as moderate to severe
OSA, and sometimes across multiple
domains.
11,57
In contrast, others
have shown a dose-response
relationship with behavior and
cognition.
58,59
Where PSG has been
used, these differences may reflect
the fact that signs of obstruction,
such as airflow limitation and effort
of breathing, may be seen without
overtevidenceofscorableapnea.
The apnea index alone may not
be optimal to assess these
relationships.
Multivariate statistical analyses
incorporating adjustments for
important covariates were used in the
majority of studies. However, 2 did not
state that they adjusted for covariates,
and 3 adjusted for only 1 or 2
variables, but the fact that academic
achievement was not the primary
outcome of all studies may provide
some explanation. Maternal education
is an important covariate to consider
because of the strong link with child
academic achievement.
60–62
Only 4
studies stated specifically that they
took maternal or parental education
into account. Four studies considered
BMI. Previous studies have found an
association between the combined
obesity/OSA condition and poorer
academic functioning.
63–66
Other
common covariates included age,
gender, socioeconomic status, and
behavioral factors. Failure to consider
important covariates, particularly
maternal education, could mean the
ES were overestimated in some
studies.
The neurobehavioral pathway(s)
linking SDB to deficits in academic
performance are unclear, but data
from our laboratory indicate that
the relationships are most likely
mediated through domains of
executive function and/or language
skills. Thus, in children 1 year into
schooling (age 6), the relationship
between SDB and poorer academic
performance was not direct, but
linked to reduced performance in
domains important for learning
outcomes.
67
Domains such as
verbal and nonverbal intelligence,
memory, and executive functioning
contribute to children’s learning-
related skills and behavior.
68
Adverse effects on these domains
could potentially underlie
associations between SDB and
academic performance.
The meta-analytic data from studies
in which children were identified as
having unsatisfactory academic
progress (Fig 4) yielded a significant
ES of –0.23, adding further
support to the evidence that SDB
is associated with deficits in
educational performance with
children performing below grade-
level expectations. There is also
evidence that substantial gains in
school grades can be made after
adenotonsillectomy in children
who have SDB and are struggling
FIGURE 4
Effects sizes and 95% CIs from the included studies of academic performance measures of un-
satisfactory progress comparing SDB to non‐SDB. Square represents the area proportional to the
weight assigned to the study; diamond represents the overall summary estimate for the analysis
(width of the diamond represents the 95% CI).
e942 GALLAND et al
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
academically.
69,70
In 1 longitudinal
study, a history of SDB or behavioral
sleep disorders in the first 5 years of
life significantly increased the
likelihood of children needing special
education at age 8, even after
controlling for many confounders.
71
These data highlight the need to
identify children with SDB who
perform poorly at school.
72
It is not
uncommon for SDB to go untreated
because SDB symptoms are
underreported by parents at general
practice visits,
73
and although the
condition may resolve with
increasing age as airway size
increases relative to the size of the
adenoids and tonsils,
74
it can also
persist into adolescence and
adulthood.
At present, studies demonstrating
results of intervention effectiveness
for treatment of SDB to improve
educational outcomes are limited to
a few studies. One before-after
treatment study of children (7–11
years) reported significant
improvements in writing, reading,
and math at follow-up (1 and 6
months postadenotonsillectomy),
with ES ranging from 0.25 to 0.34.
75
Another before-after treatment
studyof44childrenreports
a 76% resolution of low
school performance 9 months
posttreatment.
60
The only study
comparing cases with controls
reports significantly greater
improvements in case children on
academic achievement tests 1 year
posttreatment.
76
The first
randomized controlled trial of
adenotonsillectomy treatment of
OSA was recently published but
has not reported on academic
performance specifically.
56
Results
to date report mean neurocognitive
performance in the average
range at baseline but no statistically
significant improvements at
7-month follow-up, although
statistically significant improvements
in functioning in the school setting
were found on teacher ratings.
Possible explanations for this pattern
include sensitivity of measures to
detect functional impairment
56
or
reflect treatment outcome.
23,77
Longer-term follow-up trials are
needed to determine whether
improvements in school achievement
and cognitive function are detected
and sustained over time and thus
promote future success.
Although it could be argued that the
strength of the study is seen within
the replication of findings from many
studies and across several age
ranges and cultures, interpretation
of the reported ES is limited by
a lack of detail in the study designs
employed regarding SDB and
academic performance measures and
the failure of some studies to
control for important covariates.
Contributory factors to the
measurement issues identified
within the quality evaluation may be
a lack of consensus on what
measures should be used to define
school achievement and the milieu of
definitions for SDB. Not only is this
problematic in data synthesis, it
also affects educational and clinical
settings, limiting an understanding
of the nature of treatment effects
because cross-study comparisons
are made difficult. Further
limitations were evident within the
evaluation of the quality of the
reviewed studies, reflecting some
degree of measurement error, also
demonstrated within the moderate
to high heterogeneity of included
studies within most specific
domains. In addition, only a small
number of studies could be included
in some of the meta-analyses,
for example, only 2 studies for
science. With this small number
of studies, adequate power to
detect heterogeneity would be
compromised. It has been reported
that a median of 4 studies are
required before a meta-analysis for
an outcome in a systematic review
settles down around a final value to
get within 10% of the final point
estimate.
78
Finally, although our best
attempts to search the literature
rendered few studies with no
association between SDB and
academic performance, potential
publication bias also needs to be
considered given the possibility that
research with null findings may not
be reported in the literature.
CONCLUSIONS
According to the results of the
present meta-analysis, there is clear
evidence for poorer academic
performance in school-age children
with SDB. The ES is within the small
to medium range, but nevertheless
the findings serve to highlight to
parents, teachers, and clinicians
thatSDBinchildrenmaycontribute
to academic difficulties some
children face. In light of these
findings, screening for SDB should
be included in pediatric and
multidisciplinary assessments of
children’slearningdifficulties, with
appropriate medical follow-up as
indicated. Future research should
consider the neurobehavioral
pathways through which SDB may
exert its effects on academic
performance. However, more
appropriate and standardized tools
for assessing academic performance
across studies are required, and far
more research is needed on the
comparative effectiveness of
interventions targeting children
with SDB.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank the valuable input of
research assistant Carmen Lobb
(C.L.) for database entry and
crosschecking final articles for the
study.
ABBREVIATIONS
CI: confidence interval
ES: effect size
OSA: obstructive sleep apnea
PSG: polysomnography
SDB: sleep-disordered breathing
PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 4, October 2015 e943
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: No external funding.
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
REFERENCES
1. Sinha D, Guilleminault C. Sleep
disordered breathing in children. Indian
J Med Res. 2010;131:311–320
2. Liu X, Liu L, Owens JA, Kaplan DL. Sleep
patterns and sleep problems among
schoolchildren in the United States and
China. Pediatrics. 2005;115(suppl 1):
241–249
3. Gill A, Schaughency E, Lobb C, Galland B.
Sleep-disordered breathing and
cognitive development, early learning
and behavioural adjustment in children
at age 3 and 4. J Sleep Res. 2012;21
(suppl 1):69–70
4. Petry C, Pereira MU, Pitrez PM, Jones MH,
Stein RT. The prevalence of symptoms of
sleep-disordered breathing in Brazilian
schoolchildren. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2008;
84(2):123–129
5. Marcus CL, Brooks LJ, Draper KA, et al;
American Academy of Pediatrics.
Diagnosis and management of childhood
obstructive sleep apnea syndrome.
Pediatrics. 2012;130(3). Available at:
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/130/
3/e714
6. Bonuck KA, Chervin RD, Cole TJ, et al.
Prevalence and persistence of sleep
disordered breathing symptoms in
young children: a 6-year population-
based cohort study. Sleep. 2011;34(7):
875–884
7. Rosen CL, Larkin EK, Kirchner HL, et al.
Prevalence and risk factors for sleep-
disordered breathing in 8- to 11-year-old
children: association with race and
prematurity. J Pediatr. 2003;142(4):
383–389
8. Bhattacharjee R, Kheirandish-Gozal L,
Spruyt K, et al. Adenotonsillectomy
outcomes in treatment of obstructive
sleep apnea in children: a multicenter
retrospective study. Am J Respir
Crit Care Med. 2010;182(5):
676–683
9. Arens R, Marcus CL. Pathophysiology
of upper airway obstruction:
a developmental perspective. Sleep.
2004;27(5):997–1019
10. Blunden S, Lushington K, Kennedy D,
Martin J, Dawson D. Behavior and
neurocognitive performance in children
aged 5–10 years who snore compared to
controls. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;
22(5):554–568
11. Beebe DW, Wells CT, Jeffries J, Chini B,
Kalra M, Amin R. Neuropsychological
effects of pediatric obstructive sleep
apnea. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;
10(7):962–975
12. O’Brien LM, Mervis CB, Holbrook CR, et al.
Neurobehavioral implications of habitual
snoring in children. Pediatrics. 2004;
114(1):44–49
13. Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations
between executive function and
academic achievement from ages 5 to 17
in a large, representative national
sample. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21(4):
327–336
14. Neuenschwander R, Röthlisberger M,
Cimeli P, Roebers CM. How do different
aspects of self-regulation predict
successful adaptation to school?
JExpChildPsychol. 2012;113(3):
353–371
15. Barry TD, Lyman RD, Klinger LG.
Academic underachievement and
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
the negative impact of symptom severity
on school performance. J Sch Psychol.
2002;40(3):259–283
16. Birchwood J, Daley D. Brief report: the
impact of attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) symptoms on academic
performance in an adolescent
community sample. J Adolesc. 2012;
35(1):225–231
17. Hughes C, Ensor R. Individual differences
in growth in executive function across
the transition to school predict
externalizing and internalizing behaviors
and self-perceived academic success at
6 years of age. J Exp Child Psychol. 2011;
108(3):663–676
18. Guay F, Marsh HW, Boivin M. Academic
self-concept and academic achievement:
developmental perspectives on their
causal ordering. J Educ Psychol. 2003;
95(1):124–136
19. Galland BC, Dawes PJ, Tripp EG, Taylor BJ.
Changes in behavior and attentional
capacity after adenotonsillectomy.
Pediatr Res. 2006;59(5):711–716
20. Li HY, Huang YS, Chen NH, Fang TJ, Lee LA.
Impact of adenotonsillectomy on
behavior in children with sleep-
disordered breathing. Laryngoscope.
2006;116(7):1142–1147
21. Mitchell RB, Boss EF. Pediatric
obstructive sleep apnea in obese and
normal-weight children: impact of
adenotonsillectomy on quality-of-life and
behavior. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34(5):
650–661
22. Biggs SN, Vlahandonis A, Anderson V,
et al. Long-term changes in
neurocognition and behavior following
treatment of sleep disordered breathing
in school-aged children. Sleep. 2014;
37(1):77–84
23. Phelps L, Brown R, Power T. Thomas J.
Pediatric Psychopharmacology:
Combining Medical and Psychosocial
Interventions. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association; 2002:23–46
24. Lan X, Legare CH, Ponitz CC, Li S,
Morrison FJ. Investigating the links
between the subcomponents of executive
function and academic achievement:
a cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and
American preschoolers. J Exp Child
Psychol. 2011;108(3):677–692
25. Geiger A, Achermann P, Jenni OG. Sleep,
intelligence and cognition in
a developmental context: differentiation
between traits and state-dependent
aspects. Prog Brain Res. 2010;185:
167–179
26. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG;
PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting
items for systematic reviews and meta-
analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann
Intern Med. 2009;151(4):264–269, W64
27. Viswanathan M, Berkman ND.
Development of the RTI Item Bank on risk
of bias and precision of observational
e944 GALLAND et al
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
studies. J Clin Epidemiol. 2012;65(2):
163–178
28. Spruyt K, Verleye G, Gozal D. Unbiased
categorical classification of pediatric
sleep disordered breathing. Sleep. 2010;
33(10):1341–1347
29. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for
the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY:
Academic Press; 1988
30. Brockmann PE, Ber trand P, Pardo T,
Cerda J, Reyes B, Holmgren NL.
Prevalence of habitual snoring and
associated neurocognitive consequences
among Chilean school aged children. Int
J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2012a;76(9):
1327–1331
31. Sahin U, Ozturk O, Ozturk M, Songur N,
Bircan A, Akkaya A. Habitual snoring in
primary school children: prevalence and
association with sleep-related disorders
and school performance. Med Princ
Pract. 2009;18(6):458–465
32. Brockmann PE, Urschitz MS, Schlaud M,
Poets CF. Primary snoring in school
children: prevalence and neurocognitive
impairments. Sleep Breath. 2012b;16(1):
23–29
33. Urschitz MS, Guenther A, Eggebrecht E,
et al. Snoring, intermittent hypoxia and
academic performance in primary
school children. Am J Respir Crit Care
Med. 2003;168(4):464–468
34. Bourke R, Anderson V, Yang JS, et al.
Cognitive and academic functions are
impaired in children with all severities
of sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep
Med. 2011;12(5):489–496
35. Goodwin JL, Babar SI, Kaemingk KL, et al;
Tucson Children’s Assessment of Sleep
Apnea Study. Symptoms related to sleep-
disordered breathing in white and
Hispanic children: the Tucson Children’s
Assessment of Sleep Apnea Study. Chest.
2003;124(1):196–203
36. Goodwin JL, Kaemingk KL, Fregosi RF,
et al. Clinical outcomes associated with
sleep-disordered breathing in Caucasian
and Hispanic children—the Tucson
Children’s Assessment of Sleep Apnea
study (TuCASA). Sleep. 2003;26(5):
587–591
37. Goodwin JL, Kaemingk KL, Mulvaney SA,
Morgan WJ, Quan SF. Clinical screening
of school children for polysomnography
to detect sleep-disordered breathing—
the Tucson Children’s Assessment of
Sleep Apnea study (TuCASA). J Clin Sleep
Med.2005;1(3):247–254
38. Arslan S, Ozsahin SL, Dogan OT, Berk E,
Akkurt I. Habitual snoring in primary
schoolchildren: prevalence, risk factors
and school performance. Health Med.
2010;4(2):310–315
39. Ferreira AM, Clemente V, Gozal D, et al.
Snoring in Portuguese primary school
children. Pediatrics. 2000;106(5).
Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/
content/full/106/5/E64
40. Arman AR, Ersu R, Save D, et al.
Symptoms of inattention and
hyperactivity in children with habitual
snoring: evidence from a community-
based study in Istanbul. Child Care
Health Dev. 2005;31(6):707–717
41. Perez-Chada D, Perez-Lloret S, Videla AJ,
et al. Sleep disordered breathing and
daytime sleepiness are associated with
poor academic performance in
teenagers. A study using the Pediatric
Daytime Sleepiness Scale (PDSS). Sleep.
2007;30(12):1698–1703
42. Li AM, Au CT, So HK, Lau J, Ng PC, Wing YK.
Prevalence and risk factors of habitual
snoring in primary school children.
Chest. 2010;138(3):519–527
43. Ng DK, Kwok KL, Cheung JM, et al.
Prevalence of sleep problems in Hong
Kong primary school children:
a community-based telephone survey.
Chest. 2005;128(3):1315–1323
44. Kim JK, Lee JH, Lee SH, Hong SC, Cho JH.
School performance and behavior of
Korean elementary school students with
sleep-disordered breathing. Ann Otol
Rhinol Laryngol. 2011;120(4):268–272
45. Kim JK, Lee JH, Lee SH, Hong SC, Cho JH.
Effects of sleep-disordered breathing on
physical traits, school performance, and
behavior of Korean elementary school
students in the upper grade levels. Ann
Otol Rhinol Laryngol. 2012;121(5):
348–354
46. de Carvalho LBC, do Prado LBF, Ferrreira
VR, et al. Symptoms of sleep disorders
and objective academic performance.
Sleep Med. 2013;14(9):872–876
47. Giordani B, Hodges EK, Guire KE, et al.
Neuropsychological and behavioral
functioning in children with and without
obstructive sleep apnea referred for
tonsillectomy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc.
2008;14(4):571–581
48. Ting H, Wong RH, Yang HJ, Lee SP, Lee SD,
Wang L. Sleep-disordered breathing,
behavior, and academic performance in
Taiwan schoolchildren. Sleep Breath.
2011;15(1):91–98
49. Bub KL, Buckhalt JA, El-Sheikh M.
Children’s sleep and cognitive
performance: a cross-domain analysis of
change over time. Dev Psychol. 2011;
47(6):1504–1514
50. Chervin RD, Dillon JE, Bassetti C, Ganoczy
DA, Pituch KJ. Symptoms of sleep
disorders, inattention, and hyperactivity
in children. Sleep. 1997;20(12):1185–1192
51. Montgomery-Downs HE, O’Brien LM,
Holbrook CR, Gozal D. Snoring and sleep-
disordered breathing in young children:
subjective and objective correlates.
Sleep. 2004;27(1):87–94
52. Gill AI, Schaughency E, Gray A, Galland
BC. Reliability of home-based
physiological sleep measurements in
snoring and non-snoring 3-year olds.
Sleep Breath. 2013;17(1):147–156
53. Kaemingk KL, Pasvogel AE, Goodwin JL,
et al. Learning in children and sleep
disordered breathing: findings of the
Tucson Children’s Assessment of Sleep
Apnea (tuCASA) prospective cohort
study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9(7):
1016–1026
54. Melendres MC, Lutz JM, Rubin ED,
Marcus CL. Daytime sleepiness and
hyperactivity in children with suspected
sleep-disordered breathing. Pediatrics.
2004;114(3):768–775
55. Chervin RD, Ruzicka DL, Giordani BJ,
et al. Sleep-disordered breathing,
behavior, and cognition in children
before and after adenotonsillectomy.
Pediatrics. 2006;117(4):e769–e778
56. Marcus CL, Moore RH, Rosen CL, et al;
Childhood Adenotonsillectomy Trial
(CHAT). A randomized trial of
adenotonsillectomy for childhood sleep
apnea. N Engl J Med. 2013;368(25):
2366–2376
57. Bourke RS, Anderson V, Yang JSC, et al.
Neurobehavioral function is impaired in
children with all severities of sleep
disordered breathing. Sleep Med. 2011;
12(3):222–229
58. Kennedy JD, Blunden S, Hirte C, et al.
Reduced neurocognition in children who
snore. Pediatr Pulmonol. 2004;37(4):
330–337
PEDIATRICS Volume 136, number 4, October 2015 e945
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
59. Mulvaney SA, Goodwin JL, Morgan WJ,
Rosen GR, Quan SF, Kaemingk KL.
Behavior problems associated with sleep
disordered breathing in school-aged
children—the Tucson children’s
assessment of sleep apnea study. J
Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31(3):322–330
60. Myrberg E, Rosén M. A path model with
mediating factors of parents’education
on students’reading achievement in
seven countries. Educ Res Eval. 2008;
14(6):507–520
61. Myrberg E, Rosén M. Direct and indirect
effects of parents’education on reading
achievement among third graders in
Sweden. Br J Educ Psychol. 2009;79(pt 4):
695–711
62. Carneiro P, Meghir C, Parey M. Maternal
education, home environments, and the
development of children and
adolescents. J Eur Econ Assoc. 2013;
11(suppl 1):123–160
63. Beebe DW, Ris MD, Kramer ME, Long E,
Amin R. The association between sleep
disordered breathing, academic grades,
and cognitive and behavioral functioning
among overweight subjects during
middle to late childhood. Sleep. 2010;
33(11):1447–1456
64. Rhodes SK, Shimoda KC, Waid LR, et al.
Neurocognitive deficits in morbidly
obese children with obstructive sleep
apnea. J Pediatr. 1995;127(5):741–744
65. Hannon TS, Rofey DL, Ryan CM, Clapper
DA, Chakravorty S, Arslanian SA.
Relationships among obstructive sleep
apnea, anthropometric measures, and
neurocognitive functioning in
adolescents with severe obesity. J
Pediatr. 2012;160(5):732–735
66. Tan E, Healey D, Schaughency E, Dawes P,
Galland B. Neurobehavioural correlates
in older children and adolescents with
obesity and obstructive sleep apnoea. J
Paediatr Child Health. 2014;50(1):16–23
67. Luo R, Schaughency E, Gill A, Lobb C,
Galland B. Sleep-disordered breathing,
executive and cognitive functioning, and
academic performance in a sample of
New Zealand children: a structural
equation modelling (SEM) approach.
Sleep. 2013;36(Abstract Supplement):
A344
68. Brock LL, Rimm-Kaufman SE, Nathanson
L, Grimm KJ. The contributions of “hot”
and “cool”executive function to
children’sacademic achievement,
learning-related behaviors, and
engagement in kindergarten. Early Child
Res Q. 2009;24(3):337–349
69. Gozal D. Sleep-disordered breathing and
school performance in children.
Pediatrics. 1998;102(3 pt 1):616–620
70. Gozal D, Pope DW Jr. Snoring during
early childhood and academic
performance at ages thirteen to
fourteen years. Pediatrics. 2001;107(6):
1394–1399
71. Bonuck K, Rao T, Xu L. Pediatric sleep
disorders and special educational need
at 8 years: a population-based cohort
study. Pediatrics. 2012;130(4):634–642
72. Spruyt K, Gozal D. A mediation model
linking body weight, cognition, and sleep-
disordered breathing. Am J Respir Crit
Care Med. 2012;185(2):199–205
73. Blunden S, Lushington K, Lorenzen B,
Wong J, Balendran R, Kennedy D.
Symptoms of sleep breathing disorders
in children are underreported by
parents at general practice visits. Sleep
Breath. 2003;7(4):167–176
74. Jeans WD, Fernando DC, Maw AR,
Leighton BC. A longitudinal study of the
growth of the nasopharynx and its
contents in normal children. Br J Radiol.
1981;54(638):117–121
75. Ikeda FH, Horta PA, Bruscato WL, Dolci JE.
Intellectual and school performance
evaluation of children submitted to
tonsillectomy and adenotonsillectomy
before and after surgery. Braz J
Otorhinolaryngol. 2012;78(4):17–23
76. Giordani B, Hodges EK, Guire KE, et al.
Changes in neuropsychological and
behavioral functioning in children with
and without obstructive sleep apnea
following tonsillectomy. J Int
Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18(2):212–222
77. Hunsley J, Mash EJ. Evidence-based
assessment. In: Barlow DH, ed. The
Oxford Handbook of Clinical Psychology.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press;
2011:76–97
78. Herbison P, Hay-Smith J, Gillespie WJ.
Meta-analyses of small numbers of trials
often agree with longer-term results. J
Clin Epidemiol. 2011;64(2):145–153
e946 GALLAND et al
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1677
; originally published online September 7, 2015; 2015;136;e934Pediatrics and Elizabeth Schaughency
Barbara Galland, Karen Spruyt, Patrick Dawes, Philippa S. McDowall, Dawn Elder
Sleep Disordered Breathing and Academic Performance: A Meta-analysis
Services
Updated Information & /content/136/4/e934.full.html
including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Supplementary Material
html
/content/suppl/2015/09/03/peds.2015-1677.DCSupplemental.
Supplementary material can be found at:
References
/content/136/4/e934.full.html#ref-list-1
at:
This article cites 75 articles, 12 of which can be accessed free
Subspecialty Collections
/cgi/collection/sleep_medicine_sub
Sleep Medicine /cgi/collection/cognition:language:learning_disorders_sub
Cognition/Language/Learning Disorders /cgi/collection/development:behavioral_issues_sub
Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
the following collection(s):
This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
Permissions & Licensing
/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
Reprints /site/misc/reprints.xhtml
Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1677
; originally published online September 7, 2015; 2015;136;e934Pediatrics and Elizabeth Schaughency
Barbara Galland, Karen Spruyt, Patrick Dawes, Philippa S. McDowall, Dawn Elder
Sleep Disordered Breathing and Academic Performance: A Meta-analysis
/content/136/4/e934.full.html
located on the World Wide Web at:
The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on November 14, 2016Downloaded from