ArticlePDF Available

UNDERSTANDING MEMORY RECONSOLIDATION

Authors:
  • Coherence Psychology Institute
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
4
Understanding
Memory Reconsolidation
Bruce Ecker
Coherence Psychology Institute
This article is excerpted and adapted from
“Memory Reconsolidation Understood and Misunderstood” by Bruce Ecker in the
International Journal of Neuropsychotherapy, vol. 3, 2015.
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 5
The Neuropsychotherapist
EXTENSIVE RESEARCH BY NEUROSCIENTISTS since the late
1990s has found that the brain is innately equipped with a potent
process, known as memory reconsolidation, that can fundamen-
tally modify or erase a targeted, specic learning, even complex hu-
man emotional learnings formed subcortically, outside of awareness
(Pine, Mendelsohn, & Dudai, 2014; for reviews see, for example, Agren, 2014;
Reichelt & Lee, 2013). Such learnings are found to underlie and drive most
of the problems and symptoms that people address in psychotherapy and
counseling (Toomey & Ecker, 2007; Ecker & Toomey, 2008), so the relevance
and value of memory reconsolidation for the clinical eld are profound.
To describe a particular learning as “erased” means that its behavioral, emotional, cog-
nitive, and somatic manifestations disappear completely, and no further eort of any
kind is required to maintain this nullication permanently. Such lasting, transforma-
tional change is the therapeutic ideal. There is growing evidence that in erasure, the
neural encoding of the target learning is nullied (Clem & Huganir, 2010; Debiec, Díaz-
Mataix, Bush, Doyère, & LeDoux, 2010; Díaz-Mataix, Debiec, LeDoux, & Doyère,
2011; Jarome et al., 2012). The discovery of an erasure process was something of an
upheaval, reversing a rmly established conclusion, based on nearly a century of
research, that subcortical emotional learnings were indelible for the lifetime of the
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
6
work are susceptible to misconceptions.
I have been observing misconceptions as
they have developed for nearly a decade
as of this writing, and they are increasing
as awareness of the importance of recon-
solidation builds at an accelerating pace.
In fact, sizable conceptual errors are being
propagated widely in articles by science
journalists in the popular media, in articles
by psychologists in peer-reviewed jour-
nals, in posts by psychotherapists in online
clinical discussion groups, and, surprisingly,
even in articles and talks by some neurosci-
entists involved in reconsolidation research
(Ecker, 2014).
Thus there is a growing need for a clear
map of the new territory, showing where
the path of understanding branches o into
the various misunderstandings of memory
reconsolidation.
Such a guide has now been published
(Ecker, 2015), examining ten widespread
misconceptions. Three of those ten are dis-
cussed in the current article, which is ex-
cerpted and adapted from the longer one.
The three misconceptions addressed here
are these:
• Misconception: The reconsolidation
process is triggered by the reactivation of a
target learning or memory.
• Misconception: Anxiety, phobias and
PTSD are the symptoms that memory re-
consolidation could help to dispel in psycho-
therapy, but more research must be done
before it is clear how reconsolidation can be
utilized clinically.
• Misconception: What is erased thera-
peutically is the negative emotion that be-
came associated with certain event memo-
individual (LeDoux, Romanski, & Xagoraris,
1989; Milner, Squire, & Kandel, 1998).
I began studying reconsolidation re-
search ndings in 2005, at about the twen-
ty-year point of my psychotherapy practice.
Neuroscientists’ densely technical accounts
of their studies have been comprehensible
to me, for the most part, thanks to my rst
career of 14 years as a research physicist,
and it quickly became apparent to me that
knowledge of reconsolidation could drive
the evolution of the eld of psychotherapy
in major ways. The process that brings about
erasure is so fundamental for potent, eec-
tive psychotherapy, and so sweeping in the
advances that it delivers to the clinical eld,
that I refocused my clinical career on trans-
lating reconsolidation research into clinical
practice. This has produced a versatile, inte-
grative methodology of psychotherapy and
a conceptual framework that maps out how
knowledge of reconsolidation creates four
major advances for the clinical eld (Ecker,
2011; Ecker, Ticic, & Hulley, 2012, 2013a,b).
These advances are: a new level of eec-
tiveness for individual clinicians, the deep
unication of seemingly diverse methods
and systems of psychotherapy, clarication
of the much-debated role of attachment
in the therapeutic process, and a decisive
breakthrough beyond nonspecic common
factors theory and the almost 80-year-long
“dodo bird verdict” that has appeared to
limit all therapy systems to the same mod-
est level of ecacy.
Understanding memory reconsolidation
involves learning some new ways of think-
ing that dier from familiar concepts of psy-
chotherapeutic change and may even seem
counterintuitive initially. Therefore, vari-
ous aspects of the reconsolidation frame-
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 7
The Neuropsychotherapist
ries, and this negative emotion is erased by
inducing positive emotional responses to
replace it.
For readers not yet familiar with memory
reconsolidation, next is a short overview as
the context for discussion of the misconcep-
tions..
Memory Reconsolidation in Context
Memory reconsolidation is the brain’s
innate process for fundamentally revis-
ing an existing learning and the acquired
behavioral responses and/or state of mind
maintained by that learning. In the recon-
solidation process, a target learning is rst
rendered revisable at the level of its neural
encoding, and then revision of its encod-
ing is brought about either through new
learning or chemical agents (for reviews see
Agren, 2014; Reichelt & Lee, 2013). Through
suitably designed new learning, the target
learning’s manifestation can be strength-
ened, weakened, altered in its details, or
completely nullied and canceled (erased).
Erasure through new learning during the
reconsolidation process is the true unlearn-
ing of the target learning. When erasure
through new learning is carried out in psy-
chotherapy, the client experiences a pro-
found release from the grip of a distressing
acquired response (Ecker et al., 2012). The
use of chemical agents to produce erasure is
described later in this article.
In order to see the full signicance of
memory reconsolidation for psychothera-
py, it is necessary to recognize the extensive
role of learning and memory in shaping each
person’s unique patterns of behavior, emo-
tion, thoughts, and somatic experience.
Among the many types of learning and the
many types of memory, the type responsi-
ble for the great majority of the problems
and symptoms that bring people to psycho-
therapy is implicit emotional learning—es-
pecially the implicit learning of vulnerabili-
ties and suerings that are urgent to avoid,
and how to avoid them. These learnings
form usually with no awareness of learning
anything, and they form in the presence of
strong emotion, which greatly enhances
their power and durability (McGaugh, 1989;
McGaugh & Roozendaal, 2002; Roozendaal,
McEwen, & Chattarji, 2009).
For example, if a small child consistently
receives frightening anger from a parent
in response to the child expressing needs,
the child learns not to express or even feel
needs or distress and not to expect under-
standing or comfort from others. This learn-
ing can occur with no representation in
conscious thoughts or conceptualization,
entirely in the implicit learning system. The
child congures him- or herself according to
this adaptive learning in order to minimize
suering in that family environment. Later
in life, however, this same learned pattern
has life-shaping, extremely costly personal
consequences. The learnings in this exam-
ple are very well-dened, yet they form and
operate with no conscious awareness of the
learned pattern or its self-protective, coher-
ent emotional purpose and necessity. From
outside of awareness these learnings shape
the child’s and later the adult’s behavior, so
the individual is completely unaware of liv-
ing according to these specic learnings.
The neural circuits encoding these learn-
ings are mainly in subcortical regions of
implicit memory that store implicit, tacit,
emotionally urgent, procedural knowledge,
not mainly in neocortical regions of explic-
it memory that store conscious, episodic,
autobiographical, declarative knowledge
(Schore, 2003).
As in the example above, the vast ma-
jority of the unwanted moods, emotions,
behaviors, and thoughts that people seek
to change in psychotherapy are found to
arise from implicit emotional learnings, not
in awareness (Toomey & Ecker, 2007). Of
course, some psychological and behavio-
ral symptoms are not caused by emotional
The vast majority of the unwanted moods, emotions, behaviors, and
thoughts that people seek to change in psychotherapy are found to arise
from implicit emotional learnings, not in awareness
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
8
learnings—for example, hypothyroidism-
induced depression, autism, and biochemi-
cal addiction—but it is implicit emotional
learnings that therapists and their clients
are working to overcome in most cases.
It is the tenacity of implicit emotional
learnings, more than their ubiquity, that is
the real clinical challenge. On a daily basis,
psychotherapists encounter the extreme
durability of original emotional learnings
that fully maintain their chokehold dec-
ades after they rst formed. Researchers
too have observed that “A unique feature
of preferences [the authors use that term
to denote compelling, emotionally complex
avoidances and attractions] is that they re-
main relatively stable over one’s lifetime.
This resilience has also been observed ex-
perimentally, where . . . acquired preferenc-
es appear to be resistant to extinction train-
ing protocols” (Pine et al., 2014, p. 1). The
life-constraining grip of such patterns is the
bane of psychotherapists and their clients,
yet that very tenacity is a survival-positive
result of natural selection. In the course of
evolution, selection pressures crafted the
brain so that any learning accompanied by
strong emotion becomes encoded by en-
hanced, exceptionally durable synapses due
to the emotion-related hormones that inu-
ence synapse formation (McGaugh, 1989;
McGaugh & Roozendaal, 2002; Roozendaal
et al., 2009).
So durable are implicit emotional learn-
ings that they continue to function and drive
responses even during states of amnesia
and are only temporarily suppressed, not
erased, by the process of extinction (non-
reinforcement of a reactivated, learned ex-
pectation). Psychologists and neuroscien-
tists have amassed extensive evidence that
even after complete extinction of an emo-
tionally learned response, the extinguished
response is easily retriggered in various
ways. This revealed that extinction training
does not result in the unlearning, elimina-
tion, or erasure of the suppressed, origi-
nal learning (making the term “extinction”
something of a misnomer, suggesting as it
does a permanent disappearance). Rather,
the research found that extinction train-
ing forms a separate, second learning that
competes against, but does not change, the
original learning (see, e.g., Bouton, 2004;
Foa & McNally, 1996; Milner et al., 1998;
Myers & Davis, 2002).
Many decades of studying extinction led
researchers to the conclusion that implicit
emotional learnings are permanent and in-
delible for the lifetime of the individual once
they have been installed in long-term mem-
ory circuits through the process of consoli-
dation (reviewed in McGaugh, 2000). There
appeared to exist no form of neuroplasti-
city capable of unlocking the synapses of
consolidated implicit memory circuits. The
tenet of indelibility reached its peak inu-
ence with the publication of a research arti-
cle on extinction studies by neuroscientists
LeDoux, Romanski, and Xagoraris (1989)
titled “Indelibility of Subcortical Emotional
Memories.” The indelibility model soon en-
tered the literature of psychotherapy when
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 9
The Neuropsychotherapist
Year Authors Species Memory type Design and ndings
2004 Pedreira et al. Crab Contextual fear
memory
Reactivated learned expectation of visual threat must be sharply discon-
rmed for memory to be disrupted by cycloheximide.
2005 Frenkel et al. Crab Contextual fear
memory
New experience modies memory expression only if preceded by a
memory mismatch experience.
2005 Galluccio Human Operant condition-
ing
Memory is erased only by being reactivated along with a novel contin-
gency.
2005 Rodriguez-Or-
tiz et al.
Rat Taste recognition
memory
Novel taste following reactivation allows memory disruption by anisomy-
cin.
2006 Morris et al. Rat Spatial memory of
escape from danger
After reactivation, only a change of the learned safe position allows
disruption of original memory by anisomycin.
2006 Rossato et al. Rat Spatial memory of
escape from danger
After reactivation, only a change of the learned safe position allows
disruption of original memory by anisomycin.
2007 Forcato et al. Human Declarative memory Memory of syllable pairings learned visually is destabilized and impaired
by new learning only if, after reactivation by presentation of context, pre-
sentation of a syllable to be paired does not occur as expected, creating
mismatch.
2007 Rossato et al. Rat Object recognition
memory
Memory is disrupted by anisomycin only if reactivated in presence of
novel object.
2008 Rodriguez-Or-
tiz et al.
Rat Spatial memory of
escape from danger
After reactivation, only a change of the learned safe position allows
disruption of original memory by anisomycin.
2009 Forcato et al. Human Declarative memory Memory of syllable pairings learned visually is labilized and lost only if
reactivation is followed by learning revised novel pairings.
2009 Pérez-Cuesta
& Maldonado
Crab Contextual fear
memory
Reactivated learned expectation of visual threat must be sharply discon-
rmed for memory to be disrupted by cycloheximide.
2009 Winters et al. Rat Object recognition
memory
Memory is disrupted by MK-801 only if reactivated in presence of novel
contextual features.
2010 Forcato et al. Human Declarative memory Memory of syllable pairings learned visually destabilizes and incorpo-
rates new information only if, after reactivation, the expected opportunity
to match syllables does not occur, creating mismatch.
2011 Coccoz et al. Human Declarative memory Memory of syllable pairings learned visually destabilizes, allowing a mild
stressor to strengthen memory, only if, after reactivation, the expected
opportunity to match syllables does not occur, creating mismatch.
2012 Caffaro et al. Crab Contextual fear
memory
New experience modies memory expression only if preceded by a
memory mismatch experience.
2012 Sevenster et
al.
Human Associative fear
memory (classical
conditioning)
Reactivated fear memory is erased by propranolol only if prediction error
is also experienced.
2013 Balderas et al. Rat Object recognition
memory
Only if memory updating is required does reactivation trigger memo-
ry destabilization and reconsolidation, allowing memory disruption by
anisomycin.
2013 Barreiro et al. Crab Contextual fear
memory
Only if memory reactivation is followed by unexpected, mismatching
experience is the memory eliminated by glutamate antagonist.
2013 Díaz-Mataix
et al.
Rat Associative fear
memory (classical
conditioning)
Reactivated fear memory is erased by anisomycin only if prediction error
is also experienced.
2013 Reichelt et al. Rat Goal-tracking mem-
ory
Blockage of brain’s prediction error signals prevented target memory
destabilization or disruption by MK-801.
2013 Sevenster et
al.
Human Associative fear
memory (classical
conditioning)
Reactivated fear memory is destabilized, allowing disruption by proprano-
lol, only if prediction-error-driven relearning is also experienced.
2014 Sevenster et
al.
Human Associative fear
memory (classical
conditioning)
Reactivated fear memory is destabilized, allowing disruption by propran-
olol, only if prediction-error-driven relearning is also experienced, and
termination of prediction error terminates destabilzation.
Table 1. Studies demonstrating that both memory reactivation and memory mismatch (prediction error)
are necessary for inducing memory destabilization (deconsolidation) and reconsolidation, and that memory
reactivation alone is insucient.
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
10
van der Kolk (1994) published in the Harvard
Review of Psychiatry his seminal article “The
Body Keeps the Score: Memory and the
Evolving Psychobiology of Posttraumatic
Stress,” in which there was a section titled
“Emotional memories are forever.” The
conclusion that implicit emotional learn-
ings persist for a lifetime meant that people
could never become fundamentally free of
are-ups of childhood emotional condition-
ing. The worst experiences in an individual’s
past could at any time become reactivated
and seize his or her state of mind or behav-
ior in the present.
Then, several studies published from
1997 to 2000 suddenly overturned the mod-
el of irreversible memory consolidation and
indelibility. Neuroscientists in several dif-
ferent laboratories resumed studying the
eects of reactivating an established emo-
tional learning (Nader, Schafe, & LeDoux,
2000; Przybyslawski, Roullet, & Sara, 1999;
Przybyslawski & Sara, 1997; Roullet & Sara,
1998; Sara, 2000; Sekiguchi, Yamada, & Su-
zuki, 1997). Using sophisticated new tech-
niques as well as the eld’s advanced knowl-
edge of exactly where in the brain certain
emotional learnings form and are stored in
memory, researchers again demonstrated
the full elimination of any expression of a
target learning. In addition, they demon-
strated that such erasure of the learning
became possible because consolidated,
locked memory synapses had returned to a
deconsolidated, unlocked, unstable or “la-
bile” state, allowing erasure of the learning
by chemical agents that disrupt only syn-
apses that are in an unstable, nonconsoli-
dated condition. The longstanding tenet of
irreversible consolidation was disconrmed.
The destabilized state of deconsolidation
was found to exist only soon after the target
learning had been reactivated by a suitable
cue or reminder. Yet, long after such a reac-
tivation, an implicit learning is found to be
once again in a stable, consolidated state.
Thus the detection of a deconsolidated,
destabilized state of memory soon after its
reactivation implied the existence of a natu-
ral process of reconsolidation, the relocking
of the synapses of a destabilized memory,
returning the memory to stability. Subse-
quent studies found that the labile state of
deconsolidation lasts for about ve hours—
a period widely known now as the reconsoli-
dation window—during which the unstable
target learning can be modied or erased
(Duvarci & Nader, 2004; Pedreira, Pérez-
Cuesta, & Maldonado, 2002; Pedreira &
Maldonado, 2003; Walker, Brakeeld, Hob-
son, & Stickgold, 2003). When a learned, un-
wanted emotional reaction is erased, there
is no loss of memory of events in one’s life
(as shown by Kindt, Soeter, & Vervliet, 2009,
and as illustrated by a clinical example later
in this article).
With that background, we can now ex-
amine the misconceptions of the reconsoli-
dation process listed above.
Misconception: The Reconsolidation
Process Is Triggered by the Reactivation
of a Target Learning or Memory
In the reconsolidation discovery studies
of 1997 to 2000, a state of deconsolidation
was found to exist only soon after the target
learning had been reactivated by a suitable
cue or reminder. This observation was inter-
preted by the researchers to mean that each
reactivation of a target learning deconsoli-
dates its neural circuits, launching the re-
consolidation process.
That conclusion may have been sensible
based on the initial few studies, but it turned
out to be incorrect. Pedreira, Pérez-Cuesta,
and Maldonado (2004) were rst to show
that reactivation alone does not bring about
deconsolidation and reconsolidation. They
concluded, “at odds with the usual view,
retrieval per se is unable to induce labiliza-
tion of the old memory” (p. 581), and they
demonstrated that what the brain requires
to trigger the reconsolidation process is re-
activation plus another critical experience,
described below. Subsequently, this same
two-step requirement has been demon-
strated in at least twenty-two other studies
that I have tallied as of this writing. They are
listed in Table 1. In the discovery studies of
1997 to 2000, researchers had fullled this
two-step requirement without awareness of
doing so, as shown later in this section.
The early interpretation that reactivation
by itself produces deconsolidation spread
widely among both neuroscientists and sci-
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 11
The Neuropsychotherapist
ence journalists and became a reconsolida-
tion meme. Despite the post-2004 piling
up of decisive evidence revealing that this
original conclusion was incorrect, it has con-
tinued to be asserted in new writings by not
only science journalists but also by some
prominent researchers who were involved
in the original studies, as well as by many
later reconsolidation researchers.
What, then, is the second step that must
accompany reactivation? Pedreira et al.
(2004), followed by all of the studies listed
in Table 1, have shown that in order to in-
duce reconsolidation, reactivation must be
accompanied or followed soon by what re-
searchers term a mismatch experience or
prediction error experience. This is an expe-
rience of something distinctly discrepant
with what the reactivated target memory
“knows” or expects—a surprising new learn-
ing consisting of anything from a superu-
ous but salient novelty element to a direct
contradiction of what is known according to
the target learning. It makes sense from an
evolutionary perspective that deconsolida-
tion and reconsolidation, being the brain’s
process for updating learnings and memo-
ries, would be triggered only by new infor-
mation that is at odds with the contents of
an existing learning (Lee, 2009). Lee wrote,
reconsolidation is triggered by a violation
of expectation based upon prior learning,
whether such a violation is qualitative (the
outcome not occurring at all) or quantitative
(the magnitude of the outcome not being
fully predicted)” The studies listed in Table
1 have shown that the brain evolved so as
to launch de/reconsolidation only when an
experience of something discrepant with a
reactivated, learned expectation or model of
reality signals the need for an update of that
existing knowledge. This empirical nding of
a critical role of mismatch or prediction er-
ror can be regarded as a neurobiological val-
idation of a central feature of the learning
models of both Piaget (1955) and Rescorla
and Wagner (1972).
As stated by Agren (2014) in reviewing
research on reconsolidation of emotional
learnings in humans, “it would appear that
prediction error is vital for a reactivation
of memory to trigger a reconsolidation
process” (p. 73). Likewise, Delorenzi et al.
(2014) commented, “strong evidence sup-
ports the view that reconsolidation depends
on detecting mismatches between actual
and expected experiences” (p. 309). Exton-
McGuinness, Lee and Reichelt (2015, p. 375)
review the role of prediction errors in mem-
ory reconsolidation studies and sum up their
position by stating, “We propose that a pre-
diction error signal...is necessary for desta-
bilisation and subsequent reconsolidation
of a memory.” That is the research nding
that translates into major advances for the
psychotherapy eld (Ecker, 2011; Ecker et
al., 2012, 2013a,b).
For those advances to materialize, it is
necessary for clinicians to understand well
what the brain regards as an experience
of mismatch or prediction error. Miscon-
ceptions abound on this point as well. The
following example shows the meaning of
mismatch at the basic level of classical con-
ditioning in the laboratory, as demonstrated
by Pedreira et al. (2004) and other studies
listed in Table 1. Clinically relevant learnings
are often far more complex, and the guiding
of mismatch experiences in psychotherapy
looks very dierent, as a rule, from the labo-
ratory instances described in this article, but
the principles of mismatch are usefully clari-
ed at this basic level.
The study by Nader, Schafe and LeDoux
(2000), which repeated the basic design of
some other early studies (Przybyslawski et
al., 1997, 1999; Roullet et al., 1998), is often
regarded as the one that brought the initial
research to a tipping point of establishing
the reconsolidation phenomenon conclu-
sively. The mismatch requirement was dis-
covered four years later by Pedreira et al.
(2004), so Nader et al. were unaware of its
“it would appear that prediction error is vital for a reactivation of
memory to trigger a reconsolidation process”
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
12
presence in their procedure. They used a
classical conditioning procedure in which
rats learned to expect an electric footshock
during the last half-second of a 30-second
audible tone. One day later, their procedure
accomplished memory reactivation with
the onset of the 30-second tone, and it ac-
complished memory mismatch with the o-
set of the tone with no shock occurring. That
mismatch of what the reactivated target
learning expected quickly triggered destabi-
lization of the target learning and launched
the reconsolidation process. Erasure of the
learned fear of the tone then was accom-
plished by promptly following the mismatch
with administration of anisomycin, which
destroys non-consolidated synapses but
has no eect on stable, consolidated ones.
If administered six hours later, after recon-
solidation (restabilization) had occurred and
the reconsolidation window was no longer
open, anisomycin had no eect and the fear
learning persisted.
Understanding the mismatch require-
ment allows us to interpret correctly the
results of various studies that were misin-
terpreted by the researchers because they
analyzed their studies without reference to
the mismatch requirement. The simple logic
of the situation, as stated by Agren (2014),
is that “the studies that have shown eects
of reconsolidation . . . must somehow have
induced a prediction error” (p. 80). Ecker
et al. (2012) articulated the same princi-
ple: “Whenever the markers of erasure of
a learning are observed, both reactivation
and a mismatch of that learning must have
taken place, unlocking its synapses, or eras-
ure could not have resulted. This logic can
serve as a useful guide for identifying the
critical steps of process in both the experi-
ments of researchers and the sessions of
psychotherapists” (p. 23).
For example, failure to achieve destabi-
lization of a reactivated target learning has
been reported in many studies (e.g., Cam-
marota, Bevilaqua, Medina, & Izquierdo,
2004; Hernandez & Kelley, 2004; Mileusnic,
Lancashire, & Rose, 2005), and we can now
recognize that the target learning remained
stable due to an absence of mismatch or
prediction error in the procedure used. (For
example, a rat’s memory that pressing a
certain lever brings a sugar reward was in-
deed reactivated when the rat was once
again placed in the chamber with the lever,
pressed it and received a sugar pellet (Her-
nandez & Kelley, 2004), but this reactivation
provided the expected reinforcement and
entailed no experience of prediction error,
so memory destabilization did not occur.)
All 23 studies listed in Table 1 have shown
that reactivation alone does not launch the
reconsolidation process, but reactivation
plus mismatch does. This point was particu-
larly emphasized by Forcato, Argibay, Pe-
dreira, and Maldonado (2009) in titling their
article, “Human reconsolidation does not
always occur when a memory is retrieved,”
and by Sevenster, Beckers, and Kindt (2012),
who titled theirs “Retrieval Per Se Is Not Suf-
cient to Trigger Reconsolidation of Human
Fear Memory.”
Reconsolidation can also be triggered by
a mismatch of when events are expected
to occur, with no change in what occurs, as
demonstrated by Díaz-Mataix, Ruiz Martin-
ez, Schafe, LeDoux, and Doyère (2013). On
Day 1 in their study, rats heard a 60-s tone
and received a momentary electrical shock
at the 30-s point, midway through the tone.
For each rat this was repeated 10 times to
create a reliable conditioned response of
fear to the tone. On Day 2, each rat heard the
tone and received the shock again just once,
reactivating the learned association of tone
and shock. The shock occurred at the same
30-s point for some rats, but for others it oc-
curred at the 10-s point. Immediately after
this reactivation experience, researchers
administered a chemical agent (anisomy-
cin) that disrupts nonconsolidated memory
circuits. On Day 3, the tone was played again
Whenever the markers of erasure of a learning are observed, both
reactivation and a mismatch of that learning must have taken place,
unlocking its synapses, or erasure could not have resulted.
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 13
The Neuropsychotherapist
for each of the rats ve times with no ac-
companying shock, and the strength of fear
responses was measured. Rats that had un-
changed shock timing on Day 2 reacted with
fear on Day 3 fully as strongly as they had
done on Day 2, indicating that anisomycin
had no eect and, therefore, that the reac-
tivation without mismatch on Day 2 had not
destabilized the target learning. In contrast,
rats whose shock timing had been changed
on Day 2 reacted on Day 3 with only half as
many fear responses as on Day 2, indicating
that anisomycin had signicantly impaired
the target learning and, therefore, that the
reactivation with timing mismatch on Day 2
had indeed destabilized the target learning.
A target learning that has been destabi-
lized by mismatch can be erased not only
by chemical agents, but also by a counter-
learning experience with no use of chemi-
cal agents. It is this endogenous approach
that is most desirable for psychotherapeu-
tic use and which has been applied exten-
sively in that context (Ecker et al., 2012).
In laboratory studies, endogenous erasure
or modication of a target learning has
been demonstrated with both animal and
human subjects (e.g., Galluccio, 2005; Liu
et al., 2014; Monls, Cowansage, Klann, &
LeDoux, 2009; Schiller et al. 2010; Stein-
furth et al., 2014; Walker et al., 2003; Xue et
al., 2012).
The experimental procedures discussed
in this section in relation to the mismatch
requirement illustrate a principle that is crit-
ical for understanding reconsolidation phe-
nomena: What does, or does not, constitute
a mismatch experience depends entirely on
the specic makeup of the target learning at
the time of mismatch. That is a principle that
I will refer to henceforth as mismatch rela-
tivity. It is essential for understanding the
eects of reconsolidation procedures used
in both laboratory studies and therapy ses-
sions. However, I have never seen this prin-
ciple articulated explicitly in research arti-
cles. Mindfulness of mismatch relativity is
critical for consistent outcomes in utilizing
reconsolidation in psychotherapy to bring
about transformational change. Only by at-
tending closely to the specic elements of
a symptom-generating emotional learning
can a psychotherapist reliably guide mis-
match experiences that disconrm those
specic elements, as is necessary for their
nullication and dissolution.
A question often asked by clinicians
learning about reconsolidation is: When my
panicky therapy client drives on the highway
and the feared terrible ery crash doesn’t
happen, that seems to be a mismatch expe-
rience, as needed to launch reconsolidation,
yet it doesn’t unlock or erase the learned
fear. Doesn’t this show that the model is
incorrect? To clarify this, we need to apply
the mismatch relativity principle and exam-
ine whether or not a mismatch experience
actually took place. That begins with exam-
ining the detailed makeup
of the target learning in
question. In this case, the
target learning is not that a
car crash happens on eve-
ry drive; rather it is that a
crash might happen un-
predictably on any drive.
That learning is not mis-
matched or disconrmed
by an accident not hap-
pening on any one drive or
on any number of drives.
A safe, uneventful drive
creates no prediction er-
ror and therefore does not
induce deconsolidation, so
the target learning is not
revised and the model has
not failed to apply.
This example naturally
raises the question: For
that target learning, what
would be a mismatch ex-
perience? The knowledge
that a crash might happen
unpredictably on any drive
is true as a recognition of
existential reality, so no
mismatch or disconrma-
tion of that knowledge is
possible. However, that
knowledge is not the en-
tire learning maintain-
ing the panicky dread of a
ery car crash. Some other
learning is responsible for
that emotional intensity,
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
14
and it is for elements of that learning that
mismatches can be created. The most com-
mon form of this other learning, though
not the only possibility (see Ecker, 2003,
or Ecker & Hulley, 2000, for an account of
diverse learnings underlying anxiety and
panic symptoms), is suppressed traumatic
memory of the same or a similar kind, such
as a car crash, a ery explosion, the death
of high school classmates in a head-on col-
lision, a terrible scare from skidding on ice
on a mountain road or from being pulled
along very fast at 3 years old in a little wag-
on tied to the bicycle of an older sibling, and
so forth. The suppressed state of the trau-
matic memory preserves its emotionally
raw, unprocessed quality, including desper-
ate fear and helplessness. De-suppression
of the memory (in small enough steps to be
tolerable) reveals a set of specic elements,
each of which is a particular learning. It is
these component learnings that can now
be subjected to a mismatch experience. For
example, the helplessness felt and learned
in the original situation can in many cases
encounter a mismatch experience through
the technique of empowered reenactment,
which is widely used in trauma therapy to
create a vivid experience of potent self-pro-
tection in the original scene. For a detailed
clinical example of that kind, see Ecker et al.
(2012, pp. 86–91).
In summary of
this section, the
research ndings
on memory recon-
solidation represent
a nontheoretical
set of instructions
for bringing about
transformational
change in a target
learning. These in-
structions specify
that in order for
a target learning
to become desta-
bilized and sus-
ceptible to being
unlearned and nul-
lied, it must be
both reactivated
and subjected to a
mismatch or prediction error experience.
The mismatch relativity principle has been
introduced here, within the exercise of
analyzing the occurrence of mismatch in
published studies, to emphasize that what
is, and what is not, a mismatch experience
is always dened in relation to the specic
elements of the target learning and what
the target learning “knows” or expects.
This needed exercise of examining the role
of mismatch in published studies will con-
tinue in each of the next two sections. (For
numerous examples of creating mismatch
experiences in psychotherapy, see Ecker et
al., 2012, Chapters 3 to 6.)
Misconception: Anxiety, Phobias, and
PTSD Are the Symptoms That Memory
Reconsolidation Could Help to Dispel in
Psychotherapy, but More Research Must
Be Done Before It Is Clear How Reconsoli-
dation Can Be Utilized Clinically
Here we have a blend of two misconcep-
tions. First is the view that for clinical use,
reconsolidation could be suitable for help-
ing to dispel learned fears of various kinds,
with symptomology such as PTSD, phobias,
panic attacks and anxiety. This impression
probably stems from the consistent ten-
dency of researchers to comment in their
research articles that reconsolidation has
signicant potential for treatment of PTSD
and anxiety disorders. Researchers have to
be ultra-conservative in what they write so
that everything they propose is rmly based
on what is known according to the current
state of research Fear learnings are a rela-
tively safe zone for researchers when com-
menting on future application because fear
can be induced and measured under con-
trolled conditions in the laboratory more re-
liably and accurately than other emotions.
Consequently fear learning has been stud-
ied and understood far more thoroughly
than other emotional responses.
In fact, however, reconsolidation re-
search has already demonstrated that the
process applies to many types of learning
other than fear learnings—for example, ap-
petitive (pleasure) learnings (Stollho et al.,
2005), operant (instrumental) learnings (Ex-
ton-McGuinness, Patton, Sacco & Lee, 2014;
Gallucio, 2005), spatial learnings (Rossato
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 15
The Neuropsychotherapist
et al., 2006), object recognition learnings
(Rossato et al., 2007), motor task learnings
(Walker et al., 2003), taste recognition learn-
ings (Rodriguez-Ortiz, De la Cruz, Gutierrez,
& Bermidez-Rattoni, 2005), human declara-
tive learnings (Forcato et al., 2007), human
episodic learnings (Hupbach, Gomez, Hardt,
& Nadel, 2007), and emotionally compel-
ling human preferences (Pine, et al., 2014),
among others. In fact, to my knowledge, as
of this writing, all tested types of learning
and memory have been found to submit to
the process of reconsolidation.
That is extremely good news for psycho-
therapy, as the learnings that underlie and
drive individuals’ problems and symptoms
are of many dierent kinds and not neces-
sarily fear-based. Examples include: the ex-
pectation to be allowed no autonomy and
reliance on secrecy and lying to maintain
personal power; the expectation of severe
devaluing and derision by others for any
mistake or misstep, generating paralyzing
states of shame, inhibition and procrastina-
tion.
The second misconception in this cat-
egory is this: In reconsolidation research
articles, the authors typically comment that
much more research must be done before it
is clear how reconsolidation can be utilized
in psychotherapy. This is hardly the case. In
reality, for over a decade before neurosci-
entists’ discovery in 2004 of the sequence
of experiences that triggers reconsolida-
tion (Pedreira et al., 2004), psychothera-
pists had been knowingly guiding clients
through that sequence, having recognized
from clinical observations that it was re-
sponsible for transformational therapeutic
change (as described below). Furthermore,
since 2006, psychotherapists have been
translating reconsolidation research nd-
ings into successful therapeutic methodol-
ogy. In 2006 I gave a keynote address to a
conference of psychologists and psycho-
therapists (Ecker, 2006), describing the criti-
cal sequence of experiences that is required,
according to reconsolidation research, for
erasing a target emotional learning. In that
talk, a clinical case example from my prac-
tice illustrated the guiding of that sequence
and the resulting permanent disappearance
of a longstanding, intense emotional reac-
tion. In subsequent years, many articles and
conference talks have presented the critical
sequence in many clinical case examples of
using it to decisively dispel a wide range of
symptoms and problems (e.g., Ecker, 2008,
2010, 2013; Ecker, Ticic, & Hulley, 2012,
2013a,b; Ecker & Toomey, 2008; Sibson &
Ticic, 2014).
Note that according to current neurosci-
ence, memory reconsolidation is the only
known process and type of neuroplasticity
that can produce what we have been observ-
ing clinically: the abrupt, permanent disap-
pearance of a strong, longstanding, involun-
tary emotional and/or behavioral response,
with no further counteractive measures re-
quired. So, in psychotherapy we have been
guiding the same well-dened sequence of
experiences and observing the same distinc-
tive signs of erasure as reconsolidation re-
searchers have. We have applied the process
successfully to the real-life, highly complex
emotional learnings that underlie and main-
tain symptoms of many dierent types (see
citations in the previous paragraph). Also,
successful clinical use of protocols designed
to induce reconsolidation and erasure have
been reported by Högberg et al. (2011) and
Xue et al. (2012). The latter demonstrated,
in a controlled study, a strong degree of
elimination of heroin addicts’ cue-induced
craving for heroin.
Thus the new era of the psychotherapy of
memory reconsolidation is well underway. It
had a curious birth: From 1986 to 1993, my
clinical colleague Laurel Hulley and I closely
scrutinized the occasional therapy sessions
in our practices in which abrupt, liberating
change had somehow occurred—the last-
ing cessation of a problematic pattern of
Memory reconsolidation is the only known process and type of neuroplasticity
that can produce the abrupt, permanent disappearance of a strong,
longstanding, involuntary emotional and/or behavioral response, with no further
counteractive measures required.
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
16
emotion, behavior, cognition and/or somat-
ics. Finally we identied a sequence of ex-
periences that was always present, across
a wide range of clients and symptoms,
whenever such transformational change oc-
curred. We developed a system of therapy
focused on facilitating that key sequence
of experiences right from the rst session
of therapy, and found that working in this
way made our sessions far more consistent
in producing transformational therapeu-
tic breakthroughs. We began teaching this
methodology in 1993 at a workshop in Tuc-
son, Arizona, followed by our rst published
account of it in the volume Depth Oriented
Brief Therapy (Ecker & Hulley, 1996). Subse-
quently the same sequence of experiences
emerged in reconsolidation research, pro-
viding corroboration of our clinical observa-
tions by empirical, rigorous studies
Our psychotherapy system, now known
as coherence therapy, guides the series of
experiences required by the brain for re-
consolidation and erasure to occur, creating
transformational change (Ecker & Hulley,
2011). It is the only system of psychothera-
py that explicitly calls for and maps directly
onto the process identied in reconsolida-
tion research, but there are many other sys-
tems of therapy in which the same process
also takes place, albeit embedded within
methodologies conceptualized quite dier-
ently. It is clear that no single school of psy-
chotherapy “owns” the process that induces
memory reconsolidation, because it is a uni-
versal process, inherent in the brain. In any
therapy sessions, the occurrence of trans-
formational change can now be presumed
to mean that reconsolidation and erasure of
the target response have occurred, whether
or not the therapist was knowingly guiding
that process. Toward conrming that uni-
versality, we began an ongoing project of
explicitly identifying the embedded steps of
the reconsolidation and erasure process in
published case examples of various forms of
psychotherapy (Ecker et al., 2012, chapter 6;
for updated list, see http://bit.ly/15Z00HQ).
Thus, knowledge of memory reconsoli-
dation can enhance the eectiveness of in-
dividual psychotherapists and, in addition, it
translates into a unifying framework of psy-
chotherapy integration, in which the many
dierent systems of therapy form a huge
repertoire of ways to guide the brain’s core
process of transformational change. This
framework gives practitioners of dierent
therapies a shared understanding of their
action and a shared vocabulary for their ac-
tion.
Misconception: What Is Erased in Ther-
apy Is the Negative Emotion That Became
Associated With Certain Event Memories,
and This Negative Emotion Is Erased by
Inducing Positive or Neutral Emotional
Responses to Replace It
A case vignette from my psychotherapy
practice will serve to illustrate the clinical
guiding of memory reconsolidation and in
particular it will show that the what is erased
by the process is not a therapy client’s prob-
lematic emotion (or any other manifested
symptom), but rather the learned schema
or model of reality generating that emotion.
The client, a married woman, aged 50
and the mother of one child, sought therapy
to dispel her aversion to sexuality with her
husband, her depression, and her panic at-
tacks, all of which had been aicting her
for at least a decade. I was using coherence
therapy, in which the nonconscious, implicit
emotional learnings that underlie and drive
a given symptom are rst brought into di-
rect, explicit awareness, and then subjected
to the process of memory reconsolidation
and erasure, creating transformational
change.
Session by session, into explicit aware-
ness was emerging a complex array of un-
derlying, implicit emotional learnings, some
of which involved traumatic memories from
Knowledge of memory reconsolidation can enhance the
eectiveness of individual psychotherapists and, in addition, it
translates into a unifying framework of psychotherapy integration.
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 17
The Neuropsychotherapist
various developmental stages of her life. In
her rst session I found that she would dis-
sociate and become glazed and wooden in
response to even a small step of interior ex-
ploration. She had a total of 45 sessions and
was symptom-free at the end. This vignette
focuses only on the particular emotional
learning that emerged in her ninth session.
This learning had formed when she was 18
years of age and had become pregnant by
her boyfriend while living with her parents
in a conservative town. She was living in
shame and “desperate loneliness,” did not
want the baby or the boyfriend, and was
struggling to decide about having an abor-
tion when she had a miscarriage.
Wanting to nd the emotional learn-
ings she had formed in this ordeal, I gently
guided her into experientially revisiting and
reinhabiting that situation imaginally, and
voicing her thoughts and feelings in pre-
sent tense. This technique is often useful
for bringing the implicit meanings of the
original experience into explicit awareness.
She seemed absorbed in the subjective real-
ity of this material, and her voice was soft
but somber as she said, “In this town, a girl
who’s been pregnant outside of marriage is
just ruined, completely ruined.”
In order to elicit fully and explicitly the
learning she had formed, I asked softly,
“What does ‘ruined’ really mean? What’s go-
ing to happen to you now?”
After a silence, in an even quieter voice
she said, “The rest of my life as a woman is
ruined. I’ll never marry, and I’ll never have
children.” There it was, the specic learn-
ing she had formed. According to this learn-
ing, which had been implicit and outside of
awareness for decades, having sex had re-
sults that had ruined the rest of her life. Im-
mediately I understood that this dire model
of her future was a potent source of both
her depression and her sexual aversion.
With this clarity about the makeup of this
target learning, I saw a possible way to cre-
ate a contradictory experience: use of the
brain’s automatic detection of mismatches,
a background process that is always scan-
ning current conscious experience. So in
reply to her words, I said, “Please say that
again.”
Somberly, and
clearly feeling the
emotional reality of
the words, she said
again, “The rest of
my life as a woman
is ruined. I’ll never
marry, and I’ll never
have children.” As
soon as she spoke
the words this time,
her wider conscious
knowledge net-
works registered
this information,
which was new
to her conscious
networks though it was old in her implicit
memory system. Her head made an abrupt
movement, and in a sharper, louder voice
she said with obvious surprise, “Wait—that’s
not true! I did marry! I did have a child!”
This rst encounter between the target
learning and vivid contradictory knowledge
was the mismatch experience or prediction
error needed for deconsolidating the target
learning. This both-at-once experience of
the target learning and vivid contradictory
knowledge is termed a juxtaposition experi-
ence in coherence therapy to emphasize the
simultaneous activation of the two as co-
present conscious experiences.
Note that in this instance, the mismatch-
ing knowledge—“I did marry! I did have a
child!”—was familiar, ordinary knowledge
that was very real to her experientially, as
real and certain as her own existence, but
it was not inherently emotional in qual-
ity. It would not normally induce emotional
arousal by itself. For successful mismatch,
the knowledge or experience utilized must
feel decisively real to the person on the basis
of his or her own living experience, but that
does not require this mismatching knowl-
edge to be emotionally arousing in itself,
even though the target learning is strongly
emotional
Presumably the neural encoding main-
taining “My life as a woman is ruined, I’ll
never marry, I’ll never have children” was
now rapidly destabilizing, opening that set
of learned meanings to being rewritten and
erased by the knowledge, “I did marry! I did
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
18
have a child! My life isn’t ruined!”
She said in almost a whisper, “That just
feels huge.” Then her head tipped back
against the top of her chair, and she gazed
at the ceiling with blinking eyes. Then her
eyes closed, and after about ten seconds she
said, “I feel tingling and buzzing all over my
body. It’s weird—I can feel the skin between
my toes. It’s huge, it’s huge.” Internally she
was repeatedly beholding and marveling
at the new realization, which served as the
several repetitions of it needed for rewriting
the now deconsolidated target learning. For
good measure, I soon created an explicit,
out-loud repetition by jokingly saying, “I’m
seeing an image of you running down the
street waving your arms and shouting, ‘I did
get married! I did have a child! My life wasn’t
ruined!’” She laughed heartily at that, but
even before I said it, her mood had shifted
into a happy lilt that I had never seen in her
before. Her contradictory knowledge was
not emotional in itself, but the liberating ef-
fect of its use in the reconsolidation process
certainly was.
I then reminded her that in our previous
session she had raised a major question:
“Why did I start feeling unbearable sad-
ness and depression when I became preg-
nant with my son 13 years ago?” I asked her,
“Does today’s session help you see why?”
Her eyes widened with this further powerful
realization that the later pregnancy had ree-
voked her emotional memory of the much
earlier one, reimmersing her in the complex
emotional miseries that accompanied that
pregnancy and the miscarriage. She said,
“Ohhh—that’s an amazing insight.”
After that session, her longstanding de-
pressed mood was gone and did not return.
This conrmed that the targeted learning
had been producing that mood, and that
erasure or dissolution of that learning had
been accomplished—meaning that “I’ll nev-
er marry, I’ll never have children” no longer
felt real or true in any memory network. Her
depressed mood had been the conscious
surface of the unconscious despair and grief
generated by the target learning.
That session was also the beginning of
the end of her sexual aversion, which was
dispelled after several more sessions that
revealed a number of other episodes in her
life where great suering had resulted from
or accompanied sex. Finally she no longer
felt any urge to avoid her husband’s over-
tures, though she did feel vulnerable and
cautious about entering into a new level of
sexuality with him. Those of course were
natural, appropriate feelings, and I coached
her on expressing to him her need for him to
sensitively honor her pace and her cues.
Her panics attacks proved to be based in
yet other emotional learnings. They ceased
after the discovery and dissolution of those
other learnings through juxtaposition expe-
riences tailored to them.
The vignette illustrates the lifelong du-
rability of original emotional implicit learn-
ings or schemas, as well as their dissociated,
encapsulated state, which keeps them insu-
lated from and immune to new experiences
and new knowledge formed later in life.
By being retrieved into conscious, explicit
awareness, emotional implicit learnings be-
come fully available for contact with other,
disconrming knowledge that can induce
transformational change through juxtaposi-
tion (mismatch) experiences.
Thus, for consistently guiding decisive
change through the reconsolidation process
in therapy, the required reactivation of a
target learning has to be its reactivation as a
conscious, explicit experience of the retrieved,
specic elements of the target model (such
as “I’ll never marry or have children, so my
life as a woman is ruined”), not merely the
retriggering of a still nonconscious, unre-
trieved implicit schema. Such implicit learn-
ings are often retriggered in day-to-day life
without conscious awareness, and often
life also provides strong disconrmations,
but because the schema remains outside of
awareness, there is no juxtaposition experi-
ence—no conscious coexperiencing of both
the old and new knowledge of what’s real—
and therefore no change takes place.
As this clinical example shows, what is
erased through the reconsolidation process
is a specic, learned schema or model or
template of reality, verbalized in the exam-
ple as “I’ll never marry or have children, so
my life as a woman is ruined.” That schema
was the target for erasure, and the mis-
match that deconsolidated and then nul-
lied it consisted of experiencing a sharp
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 19
The Neuropsychotherapist
disconrmation of that specic schema. With disso-
lution of the schema, the negative emotions that it
was generating (despair, grief, and depression) dis-
appeared, though those emotions were not them-
selves the target for mismatch or erasure, and the
mismatch did not consist of creating a positive or
neutral emotion instead of despair and depression.
Notice also that the client’s negative emotion
was arising directly from her existing model of the
rest of her life, not from episodic memory (event
memory) of the traumatic pregnancy and miscar-
riage. In other words, the traumatic experience re-
sulted in her model (which is semantic memory),
and that model in turn generated and maintained
her emotional symptoms. Erasure of that model
caused no loss of autobiographical memory.
Therapy clients’ unwanted symptoms and prob-
lems are of course not limited to negative emo-
tions, but can also be behaviors, thoughts, disso-
ciated states, somatic sensations or conditions, or
any combination of these. In any case, the target for
erasure is not the manifested symptom or problem.
The target is the learned implicit schema or seman-
tic structure that underlies and drives production
of the symptom. Erasure occurs when the target
schema is activated as a conscious, explicit experi-
ence and is directly disconrmed by a concurrent,
vivid experience of contradictory knowledge. In
other words, erasure does not occur simply through
evoking a nonsymptomatic state when normally
the symptom would be occurring (One important
exception to that rule is the learned fear of fear that
often accompanies phobias.). The occurrence of
a symptom does not in itself bring the underlying,
symptom-generating schema into conscious, fore-
ground awareness, as is necessary for guiding the
erasure process in therapy, so methods for evoking
a nonsymptomatic state are not likely to disconrm
the underlying schema. The woman in our exam-
ple might arrive at a session in a depressed mood,
and there are techniques of somatic therapy, posi-
tive psychology, or mindfulness practice that could
be used to shift her into a depression-free sense of
well-being. However, that would not disconrm and
dissolve the underlying implicit schema maintaining
her depression, “I’ll never marry or have children, so
my life as a woman is ruined.” Her depression would
therefore recur.
An example of the misconception that nega-
tive emotion is erased by inducing positive or neu-
tral emotion is the view of Lane et al. (in press)
that “changing emotion with emotion” character-
izes how the system of psychotherapy known as
emotion-focused therapy carries out reconsolida-
tion and erasure. Rather, “changing old model with
new model” is the core phenomenology of erasure
through reconsolidation in any system of thera-
py. Emotions then change as a derivative eect of
change in semantic structures (models, rules, and
attributed meanings), just as in our example the cli-
ent’s depression disappeared as a direct result of
dissolution of her target schema. In therapy, mis-
match consists of, and erasure results from, a direct,
unmistakable perception that reality is fundamen-
tally dierent from what one currently knows and
expects reality to be.
Conclusion
The profound unlearning and cessation of ac-
quired behaviors and states of mind occur through
the process of memory reconsolidation, to the best
of our current scientic knowledge and as exten-
sive clinical experience bears out. However, there
are many possible misconceptions of memory re-
consolidation (see Ecker, 2015 for an extensive re-
view), so I hope this article will help motivate mental
health clinicians to pursue a sound understanding
and thereby gain a vital guide for facilitating lasting,
liberating change with maximum regularity.
References
Agren, T. (2014). Human reconsolidation: A reactivation
and update. Brain Research Bulletin, 105, 70-82. doi:
10.1016/j.brainresbull.2013.12.010
Balderas, I., Rodriguez-Ortiz, C. J., & Bermudez-Rattoni,
F. (2013). Retrieval and reconsolidation of object rec-
ognition memory are independent processes in the
perirhinal cortex. Neuroscience, 253, 398-405. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.09.001
Barreiro, K. A, Suárez, L. D., Lynch, V. M., Molina, V. A.,
& Delorenzi, A. (2013). Memory expression is inde-
pendent of memory labilization/reconsolidation.
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 106, 283-91.
doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.006
Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes
in extinction. Learning and Memory, 11, 485–494.
Caaro, P. A., Suarez, L. D., Blake, M. G., & Delorenzi, A.
(2012). Dissociation between memory reactivation
and its behavioral expression: scopolamine interferes
with memory expression without disrupting long-
term storage. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory,
98, 235–245. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.08.003
Cammarota, M., Bevilaqua, L. R. M., Medina, J. H., &
Izquierdo, I. (2004). Retrieval does not induce recon-
solidation of inhibitory avoidance memory. Learning
& Memory, 11, 572–578. doi: 10.1101/lm.76804
Clem, R. L., & Huganir, R. L. (2010). Calcium-permeable
AMPA receptor dynamics mediate fear memory
erasure. Science, 330, 1108-1112. doi: 10.1126/sci-
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
20
ence.1195298
Coccoz, V., Maldonado, H., & Delorenzi, A. (2011). The
enhancement of reconsolidation with a naturalistic
mild stressor improves the expression of a declara-
tive memory in humans. Neuroscience, 185, 61–72.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.04.023
Debiec, J., Díaz-Mataix, L., Bush, D. E. A., Doyère, V., &
LeDoux, J. E. (2010). The amygdala encodes specic
sensory features of an aversive reinforcer. Nature
Neuroscience, 13, 536–537. doi:10.1038/nn.2520
Delorenzi, A., Maza, F. J., Suárez, L. D., Barreiro, K.,
Molina, V. A., & Stehberg, J. (2014). Memory beyond
expression. Journal of Physiology, 108, 307-322. doi:
10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.07.002
Díaz-Mataix, L., Debiec, J., LeDoux, J. E., & Doyère, V.
(2011). Sensory specic associations stored in the
lateral amygdala allow for selective alteration of fear
memories. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 9538–9543.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5808-10.2011
Díaz-Mataix, L., Ruiz Martinez, R. C., Schafe, G. E.,
LeDoux, J. E., & Doyère, V. (2013). Detection of a
temporal error triggers reconsolidation of amygdala-
dependent memories. Current Biology, 23, 1-6. doi:
10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.053
Duvarci, S., & Nader, K. (2004). Characterization of fear
memory reconsolidation. Journal of Neuroscience, 24,
9269–9275. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2971-04.2004
Ecker, B. (2003). The hidden logic of anxiety: Look for
the emotional truth behind the symptom. Psycho-
therapy Networker, 27(6), 38–43, 58.
Ecker, B. (2006). The eectiveness of psychotherapy.
Keynote address, 12th Biennial Conference of the
Constructivist Psychology Network, University of
California, San Marcos, California. Transcript: www.
coherencetherapy.org/les/ecker2006cpnkeynote.
pdf
Ecker, B. (2008). Unlocking the emotional brain: Find-
ing the neural key to transformation. Psychotherapy
Networker, 32 (5), 42–47, 60.
Ecker, B. (2010). The brain’s rules for change: Translating
cutting-edge neuroscience into practice. Psychother-
apy Networker, 34 (1), 43–45, 60.
Ecker, B. (2011, January 13). Reconsolidation: A uni-
versal, integrative framework for highly eective
psychotherapy [Web log post]. Retrieved December
12, 2014, from http://bit.ly/1zjKtMr
Ecker, B. (2013). Unlocking the emotional brain: Memory
reconsolidation, therapeutic eectiveness and the
further evolution of psychotherapy. Keynote address,
49th Annual Conference of the California Associa-
tion of Marriage and Family Therapists, Sacramento,
California.
Ecker, B. (2014, July 14). Annals of memory reconsolida-
tion: Lagging accounts cause confusion [Web log
post]. Retrieved from http://www.neuropsycho-
therapist.com/annals-of-memory-reconsolidation-
lagging-accounts-cause-confusion/
Ecker, B. (2015, in press). Memory reconsolidation un-
derstood and misunderstood. International Journal of
Neuropsychotherapy, 3.
Ecker, B., & Hulley, L. (1996). Depth oriented brief thera-
py: How to be brief when you were trained to be deep,
and vice versa. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Ecker, B., & Hulley, L. (2000). The order in clinical “dis-
order”: Symptom coherence in depth oriented brief
therapy. In R. A. Neimeyer & J. D. Raskin (Eds.), Con-
structions of disorder: Meaning-making frameworks for
psychotherapy (pp. 63-89). Washington, DC: Ameri-
can Psychological Association Press.
Ecker, B., & Hulley, L. (2011). Coherence therapy practice
manual and training guide. Oakland, CA: Coherence
Psychology Institute. Online: www.coherencethera-
py.org/ resources/manual.htm
Ecker, B., Hulley, L., & Ticic, R. (2015). Minding the nd-
ings: Let’s not miss the message of memory recon-
solidation research for psychotherapy. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences (in press).
Ecker, B., Ticic, R., & Hulley, L. (2012). Unlocking the
emotional brain: Eliminating symptoms at their roots
using memory reconsolidation. New York: Routledge.
Ecker, B., Ticic, R., & Hulley, L. (2013a). A primer on
memory reconsolidation and its psychotherapeu-
tic use as a core process of profound change. The
Neuropsychotherapist, 1, 82-99. DOI: 10.12744/
tnpt(1)082-099
Ecker, B., Ticic, R., & Hulley, L. (2013b). Unlocking the
emotional brain: Is memory reconsolidation the key
to transformation? Psychotherapy Networker, 37 (4),
18–25, 46-47.
Ecker, B., & Toomey, B. (2008). Depotentiation of
symptom-producing implicit memory in coherence
therapy. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 21, 87-
150.
Foa, E. B., & McNally, R. J. (1996). Mechanisms of
change in exposure therapy. In R. M. Rapee (Ed.),
Current controversies in the anxiety disorders (pp.
329–343). New York: Guilford Press.
Forcato, C., Argibay, P. F., Pedreira, M. E., & Maldonado,
H. (2009). Human reconsolidation does not always
occur when a memory is retrieved: The relevance of
the reminder structure. Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory, 91, 50–57. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.011
Forcato, C., Burgos, V. L., Argibay, P. F., Molina, V. A.,
Pedreira, M. E., & Maldonado, H. (2007). Reconsoli-
dation of declarative memory in humans. Learning &
Memory, 14, 295–303. doi: 10.1101/lm.486107
Forcato, C., Rodríguez, M. L. C., Pedreira, M. E., &
Maldonado, H. (2010). Reconsolidation in humans
opens up declarative memory to the entrance of new
information. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory,
93, 77–84. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2009.08.006
Frenkel, L., Maldonado, H., & Delorenzi, A. (2005). Mem-
ory strengthening by a real-life episode during recon-
solidation: an outcome of water deprivation via brain
angiotensin II. European Journal of Neuroscience, 22,
1757–1766. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04373.x
Galluccio, L. (2005). Updating reactivated memories
in infancy: I. Passive- and active-exposure eects.
Developmental Psychobiology, 47, 1–17. doi: 10.1002/
dev.20073
Hernandez. P. J., & Kelley, A. E. (2004). Long-term
memory for instrumental responses does not un-
dergo protein synthesis-dependent reconsolidation
upon retrieval. Learning & Memory, 11, 748–754. doi:
10.1101/lm.84904
Högberg, G., Nardo, D., Hällström, T., & Pagani, M.
(2011). Aective psychotherapy in post-traumatic
reactions guided by aective neuroscience: memory
reconsolidation and play. Psychology Research and
Behavior Management, 4, 87–96. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.
S10380
Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., Hardt, O., & Nadel, L. (2007).
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 21
The Neuropsychotherapist
Reconsolidation of episodic memories: A subtle
reminder triggers integration of new informa-
tion. Learning & Memory, 14, 47–53. doi: 10.1101/
lm.365707
Jarome, T. J., Kwapis, J. L., Werner, C. T., Parsons, R. G.,
Gaord, G. M., & Helmstetter, F. J. (2012). The timing
of multiple retrieval events can alter GluR1 phospho-
rylation and the requirement for protein synthesis in
fear memory reconsolidation. Learning & Memory, 19,
300-306. doi: 10.1101/lm.024901.111
Kindt, M., Soeter, M., & Vervliet, B. (2009). Beyond
extinction: erasing human fear responses and pre-
venting the return of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 12,
256–258. doi: 10.1038/nn.2271
Lane, R. D., Ryan, L., Nadel, L., & Greenberg, L. (2015).
Memory reconsolidation, emotional arousal and the
process of change in psychotherapy: New insights
from brain science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences (in
press). doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14000041.
LeDoux, J. E., Romanski, L., & Xagoraris, A. (1989). Indel-
ibility of subcortical emotional memories. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 238–243. doi: 10.1162/
jocn.1989.1.3.238
Lee, J. L. (2009). Reconsolidation: Maintaining memory
relevance. Trends in Neuroscience, 32, 413–420.
doi:10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.002
Liu, J., Zhao, L., Xue, Y., Shi, J., Suo, L., Luo, Y., Chai, B.,
Yang, C., Fang, Q., Zhang, Y., Bao, Y., Pickens, C. L.,
& Lu, L. (2014). An Unconditioned Stimulus Retrieval
Extinction Procedure to Prevent the Return of Fear
Memory. Biological Psychiatry. doi: 10.1016/j.bi-
opsych.2014.03.027
McGaugh, J. L. (1989). Involvement of hormonal and
neuromodulatory systems in the regulation of
memory storage. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 2,
255–287. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.12.030189.001351
McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory—A century of consolida-
tion. Science, 287, 248–251. PMID: 10634773
McGaugh, J. L., & Roozendaal, B. (2002). Role of adrenal
stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the
brain. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 12, 205–210.
PMID: 12015238
Mileusnic, R., Lancashire, C. L., & Rose, S. P. R. (2005).
Recalling an aversive experience by day-old chicks is
not dependent on somatic protein synthesis. Learn-
ing & Memory, 12, 615–619. doi: 10.1101/lm.38005
Milner, B., Squire, L. R., & Kandel, E. R. (1998). Cognitive
neuroscience and the study of memory. Neuron, 20,
445–468. PMID: 9539121
Monls, M.-H., Cowansage, K. K., Klann, E., & LeDoux, J.
E. (2009). Extinction-reconsolidation boundaries: key
to persistent attenuation of fear memories. Science,
324, 951–955. doi: 10.1126/science.1167975
Morris, R. G., Inglis, J., Ainge, J. A., Olverman, H. J.,
Tulloch, J., Dudai, Y., & Kelly, P. A. (2006). Memory
reconsolidation: Sensitivity of spatial memory to
inhibition of protein synthesis in dorsal hippocampus
during encoding and retrieval. Neuron, 50, 479-489.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.012
Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., & LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Fear
memories require protein synthesis in the amyg-
dala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature, 406,
722–726. PMID: 10963596
Pedreira, M. E., & Maldonado, H. (2003). Protein synthe-
sis subserves reconsolidation or extinction depend-
ing on reminder duration. Neuron, 38, 863–869. doi:
10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00352-0
Pedreira, M. E., Pérez-Cuesta, L. M., & Maldonado, H.
(2002). Reactivation and reconsolidation of long-
term memory in the crab Chasmagnathus: Protein
synthesis requirement and mediation by NMDA-type
glutamatergic receptors. Journal of Neuroscience, 22,
8305–8311. PMID: 12223585
Pedreira, M. E., Pérez-Cuesta, L. M., & Maldonado, H.
(2004). Mismatch between what is expected and
what actually occurs triggers memory reconsolida-
tion or extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 579–585.
doi: 10.1101/lm.76904
Pérez-Cuesta, L. M., & Maldonado, H. (2009). Memory
reconsolidation and extinction in the crab: Mutual
exclusion or coexistence? Learning & Memory, 16,
714–721. doi: 10.1101/lm.1544609
Piaget, J. (1955). The child’s construction of reality. Lon-
don: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Pine, A., Mendelsohn, A., & Dudai, Y. (2014). Uncon-
scious learning of likes and dislikes is persistent,
resilient, and reconsolidates. Frontiers in Psychology,
5(1051), 1-13. doi: 10.3389/ fpsyg.2014.01051
Przybyslawski, J., Roullet, P., & Sara, S. J. (1999). Attenu-
ation of emotional and nonemotional memories after
their reactivation: Role of beta adrenergic recep-
tors. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 6623–6628. PMID:
10414990
Przybyslawski, J., & Sara, S. J. (1997). Reconsolida-
tion of memory after its reactivation. Behavior and
Brain Research, 84, 241–246. doi: 10.1016/S0166-
4328(96)00153-2
Reichelt, A. C., & Lee, J. L. C. (2013). Memory reconsoli-
dation in aversive and appetitive settings. Frontiers
of Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 1–18. doi: 10.3389/fn-
beh.2013.00118
Reichelt, A. C., Exton-McGuinness, M. T., & Lee, J. L.
(2013). Ventral tegmental dopamine dysregulation
prevents appetitive memory destabilisation. Journal
of Neuroscience, 33, 14205–14210. doi: 10.1523/ JNEU-
ROSCI.1614-13.2013
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pav-
lovian conditioning: variations in the eectiveness
of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Classical
Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory (Prokasy,
A. H., ed.), pp. 64–99. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts.
Rodriguez-Ortiz, C. J., De la Cruz, V., Gutierrez, R.,
& Bermidez-Rattoni, F. (2005). Protein synthesis
underlies post-retrieval memory consolidation to a
restricted degree only when updated information
is obtained. Learning and Memory, 12, 533–537. doi:
10.1101/lm.94505
Rodriguez-Ortiz, C. J., Garcia-DeLaTorre, P., Benavidez,
E., Ballesteros, M. A., & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. (2008).
Intrahippocampal anisomycin infusions disrupt
previously consolidated spatial memory only when
memory is updated. Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory, 89, 352-359. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.10.004
Roozendaal, B., McEwen, B. S., & Chattarji, S. (2009).
Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 10, 423-433. doi: 10.1038/nrn2651
Rossato, J. I., Bevilaqua, L. R. M., Medina, J. H., Izqui-
erdo, I., & Cammarota, M. (2006). Retrieval induces
hippocampal-dependent reconsolidation of spa-
tial memory. Learning & Memory, 13, 431–440. doi:
10.1101/lm.315206
The Neuropsychotherapist issue 10 January 2015
22
Rossato, J. I., Bevilaqua, L. R. M., Myskiw, J. C., Medina,
J. H., Izquierdo, I., & Cammarota, M. (2007). On the
role of hippocampal protein synthesis in the con-
solidation and reconsolidation of object recognition
memory. Learning & Memory, 14, 36–46. doi: 10.1101/
lm.422607
Roullet, P., & Sara, S. J. (1998). Consolidation of memory
after its reactivation: Involvement of beta noradren-
ergic receptors in the late phase. Neural Plasticity, 6,
63-68. PMID: 9920683
Sara, S. J. (2000). Retrieval and reconsolidation: Toward
a neurobiology of remembering. Learning & Memory,
7, 73-84. doi: 10.1101/lm.7.2.73
Schiller, D., Monls, M.-H., Raio, C. M., Johnson, D.
C., LeDoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (2010). Preventing
the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation
update mechanisms. Nature, 463, 49-53. doi: 10.1038/
nature08637
Schore, A. N. (2003). Aect dysregulation and disorders of
the self. New York: W.W. Norton.
Sekiguchi, T., Yamada, A., & Suzuki, H. (1997). Reacti-
vation-dependent changes in memory states in the
terrestrial slug Limax avus. Learning & Memory, 4,
356–364. PMID: 10706372
Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2012). Retrieval
per se is not sucient to trigger reconsolidation of
human fear memory. Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory, 97, 338-45. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.01.009
Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2013). Prediction
error governs pharmacologically induced amnesia for
learned fear. Science, 339, 830-833. doi: 10.1126/sci-
ence.1231357
Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2014). Prediction
error demarcates the transition from retrieval, to
reconsolidation, to new learning. Learning & Memory,
21, 580-584. doi: 10.1101/lm.035493.114
Sibson, P. & Ticic, R. (2014). Remembering in order to
forget. Therapy Today, 25 (2), 26–29.
Steinfurth, E. C. K., Kanen, J. W., Raio, C. M., Clem, R.
L., Huganir, R. L., & Phelps, E. A. (2014). Young and
old Pavlovian fear memories can be modied with
extinction training during reconsolidation in humans.
Learning and Memory, 21, 338–341. doi:10.1101/
lm.033589.113
Stollho, N., Menzel, R., & Eisenhardt, D. (2005).
Spontaneous recovery from extinction depends on
the reconsolidation of the acquisition memory in an
appetitive learning paradigm in the honeybee (Apis
mellifera). Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 4485–4492.
PMID: 15872095
Toomey, B., & Ecker, B. (2007). Of neurons and
knowings: Constructivism, coherence psychol-
ogy and their neurodynamic substrates. Journal
of Constructivist Psychology, 20, 201–245. doi:
10.1080/10720530701347860
Toomey, B., & Ecker, B. (2009). Competing visions of
the implications of neuroscience for psychotherapy.
Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 22, 95–140. doi:
10.1080/10720530802675748
van der Kolk, B. (1994). The body keeps the score: Mem-
ory and the evolving psychobiology of post traumatic
stress. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 1(5), 253–265.
PMID: 9384857
Walker, M. P., Brakeeld, T., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold,
R. (2003). Dissociable stages of human memory con-
solidation and reconsolidation. Nature, 425, 616–620.
PMID: 14534587
Winters, B. D., Tucci, M. C., & DaCosta-Furtado, M.
(2009). Older and stronger object memories are se-
lectively destabilized by reactivation in the presence
of new information. Learning & Memory, 16, 545-553.
doi: 10.1101/lm.1509909
Xue, Y.-X., Luo, Y.-X., Wu, P., Shi, H.-S., Xue, L.-F., Chen,
C., et al. (2012). A memory retrieval-extinction pro-
cedure to prevent drug craving and relapse. Science,
336, 241–245. doi: 10.1126/science.1215070
Bruce Ecker, MA, LMFT is co-originator of Coherence
Therapy, co-director of the Coherence Psychology Insti-
tute, and coauthor of Unlocking the Emotional Brain: Elim-
inating Symptoms at Their Roots Using Memory Reconsoli-
dation; the Coherence Therapy Practice Manual and Training
Guide; and Depth Oriented Brief Therapy. He is in private
practice in Oakland, California, gives clinical trainings in-
ternationally, and has taught graduate courses for many
years. Clarifying how lasting, transformational change
takes place is the theme of Bruce Ecker’s clinical career.
He has contributed extensive innovations in concepts and
methods of experiential psychotherapy, and has driven
the clinical eld’s recognition of how memory reconsolida-
tion research translates into new capabilities of consistent
therapeutic eectiveness and psychotherapy integration.
For more information, visit www.CoherenceInstitute.org.
www.neuropsychotherapist.com 23
The Neuropsychotherapist
www.tnptinstitute.com
The Neuropsychotherapy Institute.
The neuropsychotherapy institute learning platform has been created for psychothera-
pists, psychologists, and other mental health professionals, to educate them in the new par-
adigm of neuropsychotherapy for more eective clinical practice.
Neuropsychotherapy is a multidisciplinary perspective on mental well-being that looks
to neuroscience and other related elds of human biology and psychology to enhance the
clinical practice of talking therapies. The Neuropsychotherapy Institute will provide you with
a sound foundational understanding of the neurobiology of mental life and how that knowl-
edge can inform psychotherapy and increase the eectiveness of your practice. The Insti-
tute oers courses on a Continuing Education or Professional Development credit basis* as
each unit keeps you up-to-date with the latest science and practice of psychotherapy.
... Through the imagination, patients can undo the aloneness experienced by a younger version of the self, responding to their unmet needs in both action and words to help calm, relieve and soothe these parts (Medley, 2021). Ecker (2015) advanced the idea that the brain can process imagined experiences with similar circuitry to a person's lived experience, and that painful and traumatic memories can be healed and reconsolidated through imagination, as if they were experienced as real. Ecker et al. (2012) suggested that AEDP, as do other therapies that rest on recent research demonstrating the brain's capacity for lifelong "positive" neuroplasticity (Doidge, 2007;Hanson, 2017), makes use of mechanisms to alleviate suffering by "rewiring the brain". ...
Article
COVID-19 has not only killed and infected millions of people worldwide but has also resulted in unprecedented psychosocial stressors that continue to have profound mental health consequences for many people, exacerbating pre-existing psychological suffering and contributing to the onset of new stress related conditions. It has also resulted in a major revolution in the delivery of mental health treatment abruptly shifting psychotherapeutic practice to online technology. Psychotherapists need to be prepared for how their clinical work may change. This qualitative research study has been phenomenological in nature, attempting to capture and contribute to the literature on the lived experience of psychotherapists in navigating the transition through a global pandemic and exploring how the accompanying shift to telehealth has impacted clinical practice and the therapeutic relationship, if at all. A single-session, semi-structured interview lasting approximately one hour was conducted over Zoom with 15 mental health clinicians certified in an integrative psychotherapeutic attachment-based treatment model Accelerated Experiential Dynamic Psychotherapy (AEDP). Research findings and data were analyzed using a thematic coding process and principles of grounded theory. Significant findings of this study included the identification of factors that might negatively impact the online therapeutic relationship and the recognition of ways to strengthen and enhance telehealth effectiveness with an attachment-based and relational lens. Advantages and disadvantages of telehealth practice were identified and explored in addition to the effects of shared trauma on the therapeutic relationship and the post-traumatic growth and resilience of the therapist. Implications for theory, practice and social work education are discussed. Limitations included the small size and homogeneity of the study sample.
... Within AEDP this is referred to as a portrayal, similar to a retrieval used in other theoretical approaches. Using vivid imagery to return to a scene with emotional valence, and having the patient enter the scene as an agent to provide something corrective or assertive, the patient can experience a powerfully contrasting emotional experience which transforms the original traumatic memory (Ecker, 2015;Fosha, 2000;Fosha, Paivio, Gleiser, & Ford, 2009). Portrayals can be used to complete unprocessed emotion about a past experience, have a longed for experience that was interrupted or blocked, or to facilitate a reunion and relationship of care and attunement between the patient's adult self and their imagined younger self (Fosha, 2000;Fosha et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
The coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic created an unprecedented physical and mental health crisis on an international scale. Clients and therapists alike navigated the fears and uncertainty surrounding the virus, often in an environment of social isolation. The following paper presents a brief overview of the unique stressors and psychosocial factors impacting therapeutic work in the time of the coronavirus pandemic, including fears of the virus and social transmission, relational stressors due to isolation, grieving in isolation, fear of death, financial challenges, and the transition to telehealth. In addition, this paper aims to provide specific interventions and helpful approaches for psychotherapists navigating the novel challenges and demands to their clinical work through an AEDP (Accelerated Experiential Dynamic Psychotherapy) perceptive. This includes finding an entry point for accessing the pain: undoing aloneness; moving through the pain: dyadic affect regulation; and paying close attention to vitality within suffering: searching for transformance. Recommendations for the health and care of the clinician are also discussed, such as attuning to self-needs, anticipating and detecting dysregulation, staying with the good, meaning-making, identifying and responding to burnout, and the importance of receiving personal therapy and clinical supervision.
Thesis
In this dissertation I explore the dysregulation and regulation of affect over a period of two years. I start the dissertation explaining the motivations underlying my choice of dissertation topic. In the Literature Review, I outline the multifaceted theoretical landscape of Affect Regulation research. In the Methodology section I describe the methodological approaches I chose to carry out my exploration, followed by an outline of the methods applied. In Part One I recount my experiences in the PPD group over the course of two years sourcing diary entries, e-mails and essays. In Part Two I then outline my experiences of the one-on-one interview sessions that I purposely organized with the PPD group members six months after our course ended. Lastly, in the Discussion section I describe the experiential process of writing this dissertation and highlight the learning I derived from having undertaken this exploration.
Article
Full-text available
A memory trace in its active state is susceptible to interference by amnesic agents, such as hypothermia and electroconvulsive shock, and by NMDA receptor antagonists, suggesting that a time-dependent consolidation process occurs each time a memory is reactivated. The role of beta noradrenergic receptors in reconsolidation in rats was examined in both a positively reinforced radial maze task and a footshock-reinforced conditioned emotional response task. For the former, rats were trained over several days in a spatial reference memory task and received a single reactivation trial followed by propranolol. A temporally graded impairment was observed when propranolol treatment occurred after the memory reactivation trial. In the emotional task, memory impairing effects of propranolol were greater when the drug was administered after a reactivation trial than when administered immediately after the initial training. These results suggest that reactivation of memory triggers a beta receptor-dependent cascade of intracellular events, recapitulating that which occurs during initial postacquisition consolidation, thus permitting reorganization of the existing memory as a function of new information in the retrieval environment. This remarkable lability of an active memory trace provides a new basis for pharmacotherapeutic intervention in such syndromes as Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. beta adrenoreceptor antagonists may be promising pharmacological agents for attenuating debilitating memories at the time of their controlled reactivation.
Article
Full-text available
That memory reconsolidation is the process underlying decisive, lasting therapeutic change has long been our proposal, and the recognition of its critical role by Lane et al. is a welcome development. However, in our view their account has significant errors due to neglect of research findings and neglect of previous work on the clinical application of those findings.
Article
Full-text available
Memory reconsolidation is the brain's natural, neural process that can produce transformational change: the full, permanent elimination of an acquired behavior or emotional response. This article identifies and examines 10 common misconceptions regarding memory reconsolidation research findings and their translation into clinical practice. The research findings are poised to drive significant advancements in both the theory and practice of psychotherapy, but these benefits depend on an accurate understanding of how memory reconsol-idation functions, and misconceptions have been proliferating. This article also proposes a unified model of reconsolidation and extinction phenomena based on the brain's well-established requirement of memory mis-match (prediction error) for reconsolidation to be triggered. A reinterpretation of numerous studies published without reference to the mismatch requirement shows how the mismatch requirement and mismatch relativity (MRMR) model can account for diverse empirical findings, reveal unrecognized dynamics of memory change, and generate predictions testable by further research.
Article
Full-text available
Preferences profoundly influence decision-making and are often acquired through experience, yet it is unclear what role conscious awareness plays in the formation and persistence of long-term preferences and to what extent they can be altered by new experiences. We paired visually masked cues with monetary gains or losses during a decision-making task. Despite being unaware of the cues, subjects were influenced by their predictive values over successive trials of the task, and also revealed a strong preference for the appetitive over the aversive cues in supraliminal choices made days after learning. Moreover, the preferences were resistant to an intervening procedure designed to abolish them by a change in reinforcement contingencies, revealing a surprising resilience once formed. Despite their power however, the preferences were abolished when this procedure took place shortly after reactivating the memories, indicating that the underlying affective associations undergo reconsolidation. These findings highlight the importance of initial experiences in the formation of long-lasting preferences even in the absence of consciousness, while suggesting a way to overcome them in spite of their resiliency.
Article
Full-text available
Although disrupting reconsolidation is promising in targeting emotional memories, the conditions under which memory becomes labile are still unclear. The current study showed that post-retrieval changes in expectancy as an index for prediction error may serve as a read-out for the underlying processes engaged by memory reactivation. Minor environmental changes define whether retrieval induces memory reconsolidation or the initiation of a new memory trace even before fear extinction can be observed.
Article
Full-text available
The idea that memories are not invariable after the consolidation process has led to new perspectives about several mnemonic processes. In this framework, we review our studies on the modulation of memory expression during reconsolidation. We propose that during both memory consolidation and reconsolidation, neuromodulators can determine the probability of the memory trace to guide behavior, i.e. they can either increase or decrease its behavioral expressibility without affecting the potential of persistent memories to be activated and become labile. Our hypothesis is based on the findings that positive modulation of memory expression during reconsolidation occurs even if memories are behaviorally unexpressed. This review discusses the original approach taken in the studies of the crab Neohelice (Chasmagnathus) granulata, which was then successfully applied to test the hypothesis in rodent fear memory. Data presented offers a new way of thinking about both weak trainings and experimental amnesia: memory retrieval can be dissociated from memory expression. Furthermore, the strategy presented here allowed us to show in human declarative memory that the periods in which long-term memory can be activated and become labile during reconsolidation exceeds the periods in which that memory is expressed, providing direct evidence that conscious access to memory is not needed for reconsolidation. Specific controls based on the constraints of reminders to trigger reconsolidation allow us to distinguish between obliterated and unexpressed but activated long-term memories after amnesic treatments, weak trainings and forgetting. In the hypothesis discussed, memory expressibility - the outcome of experience-dependent changes in the potential to behave- is considered as a flexible and modulable attribute of long-term memories. Expression seems to be just one of the possible fates of re-activated memories.
Article
Full-text available
Extinction training during reconsolidation has been shown to persistently diminish conditioned fear responses across species. We investigated in humans if older fear memories can benefit similarly. Using a Pavlovian fear conditioning paradigm we compared standard extinction and extinction after memory reactivation 1 d or 7 d following acquisition. Participants who underwent extinction during reconsolidation showed no evidence of fear recovery, whereas fear responses returned in participants who underwent standard extinction. We observed this effect in young and old fear memories. Extending the beneficial use of reconsolidation to older fear memories in humans is promising for therapeutic applications.
Book
Psychotherapy that regularly yields liberating, lasting change was, in the last century, a futuristic vision, but it has now become reality, thanks to a convergence of remarkable advances in clinical knowledge and brain science. in Unlocking the Emotional Brain, authors Ecker, Ticic and Hulley equip readers to carry out focused, empathic therapy using the process found by researchers to induce memory reconsolidation, the recently discovered and only known process for actually unlocking emotional memory at the synaptic level. Emotional memory's tenacity is the familiar bane of therapists, and researchers have long believed that emotional memory forms indelible learning. Reconsolidation has overturned these views. It allows new learning to erase, not just suppress, the deep, unconscious, intensely problematic emotional learnings that form during childhood or in later tribulations and generate most of the symptoms that bring people to therapy. Readers will learn methods that precisely eliminate unwanted, ingrained emotional responses—whether moods, behaviors or thought patterns—causing no loss of ordinary narrative memory, while restoring clients' well-being. Numerous case examples show the versatile use of this process in AEDP, Coherence Therapy, EFT, EMDR and IPNB.