ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

We hypothesised that belief in conspiracy theories would be predicted by the general tendency to attribute agency and intentionality where it is unlikely to exist. We further hypothesised that this tendency would explain the relationship between education level and belief in conspiracy theories, where lower levels of education have been found to be associated with higher conspiracy belief. In Study 1 (N = 202) participants were more likely to agree with a range of conspiracy theories if they also tended to attribute intentionality and agency to inanimate objects. As predicted, this relationship accounted for the link between education level and belief in conspiracy theories. We replicated this finding in Study 2 (N = 330), whilst taking into account beliefs in paranormal phenomena. These results suggest that education may undermine the reasoning processes and assumptions that are reflected in conspiracy belief.
Belief in conspiracy theories 1
Someone is Pulling the Strings: Hypersensitive Agency Detection and Belief in
Conspiracy Theories
Karen M. Douglas*, Robbie M. Sutton, Mitchell J. Callan,
Rael J. Dawtry and Annelie J. Harvey
Author notes:
* Karen M. Douglas, School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, CT27NP,
United Kingdom. Ph: +44 1227 824758. Email: k.douglas@kent.ac.uk.
Robbie M. Sutton, School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, CT27NP,
United Kingdom. Ph: +44 1227 823080. Email: r.sutton@kent.ac.uk.
Mitchell J. Callan, Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Colchester,
CO43SQ. Ph: +44 1206 873817, Email: mcallan@essex.ac.uk.
Rael J. Dawtry, School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, CT27NP, United
Kingdom. Ph: +44 1227 823181. Email: rd299@kent.ac.uk.
Annelie J. Harvey, Department of Psychology, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge,
CB11PT. Ph: +44 8451 962731, Email: annelie.harvey@anglia.ac.uk.
* Corresponding author
Belief in conspiracy theories 2
Abstract
We hypothesized that belief in conspiracy theories would be predicted by the general
tendency to attribute agency and intentionality where it is unlikely to exist. We further
hypothesized that this tendency would explain the relationship between education level
and belief in conspiracy theories, where lower levels of education have been found to be
associated with higher conspiracy belief. In Study 1 (N=202) participants were more
likely to agree with a range of conspiracy theories if they also tended to attribute
intentionality and agency to inanimate objects. As predicted, this relationship accounted
for the link between education level and belief in conspiracy theories. We replicated this
finding in Study 2 (N=330), whilst taking into account beliefs in paranormal phenomena.
These results suggest that education may undermine the reasoning processes and
assumptions that are reflected in conspiracy belief.
Keywords:
Conspiracy theories, hypersensitive agency detection, intentionality bias, paranormal
beliefs, education
Belief in conspiracy theories 3
Someone is Pulling the Strings: Hypersensitive Agency Detection and Belief in
Conspiracy Theories
Conspiracy theories are attempts to explain the ultimate causes of events as secret
plots by powerful and malevolent groups rather than as overt activities or natural
occurrences (Byford, 2011; McCauley & Jacques, 1979; Sunstein & Vermeule, 2009).
For example, popular conspiracy theories suppose that the 9/11 attacks on the Twin
Towers were an ‘inside job’ (Swami, Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2010; Wood &
Douglas, 2013), that Princess Diana was murdered by elements within the British
establishment (Douglas & Sutton, 2008; 2011), that climate change is a hoax (Jolley &
Douglas, 2014a; Lewandowsky, Oberauer & Gignac, 2013) and that Lee Harvey Oswald
acted on behalf of the CIA in assassinating U.S. President John F. Kennedy (McCauley
& Jacques, 1979; McHoskey, 1995). Conspiracy theories are a prominent characteristic
of contemporary culture, aided by the ease and speed of Internet communication (Coady,
2006). They capture public interest, typically drawing attention away from official
explanations in favour of elaborate plots and schemes.
Some conspiracy theories may have positive consequences. For example, they
may allow individuals to question social systems and in turn encourage government
transparency (e.g., Clarke, 2002; Fenster, 1999; Swami & Coles, 2010). They may also
reveal inconsistencies in official accounts of events (e.g., Clarke, 2002) uncovering new
information for examination (Miller, 2002). However, recent research suggests that
conspiracy theories may also have detrimental effects on health decisions, political
engagement, and pro-environmental behavior. For example, exposure to anti-vaccine
conspiracy theories decreases people’s intentions to vaccinate (Jolley & Douglas, 2014b),
an effect mediated by feelings of powerlessness, disillusionment and mistrust. Further,
belief in the theory that birth control and HIV/AIDS are forms of genocide against
Belief in conspiracy theories 4
African Americans is associated with negative attitudes toward contraception (Bird &
Bogart, 2003; Bogart & Thorburn, 2006). Also, exposure to anti-government and climate
change conspiracy theories has been found to negatively influence political engagement
and pro-environmental intentions respectively, effects explained by feelings of
powerlessness (Jolley & Douglas, 2014a).
With both the positive and negative consequences of conspiracy theories in mind,
it is important to understand the psychological factors associated with conspiracy belief.
In recent years, psychologists have made significant progress in understanding these
factors. For example, people are likely to believe in a particular conspiracy theory if they
also believe in others (Goertzel, 1994). This phenomenon occurs even when conspiracy
theories contradict each other (Sutton & Douglas, 2014; Wood, Douglas & Sutton, 2012).
Also, researchers have uncovered a range of individual differences and personality
variables associated with conspiracy belief, such as anomie, distrust in authority, political
cynicism, powerlessness (Abalakina-Paap, Stephan, Craig & Gregory, 1999; Goertzel,
1994; Swami et al., 2011), Machiavellianism (Douglas & Sutton, 2011) and schizotypy
(Darwin, Neave & Holmes, 2011). The growing psychological ‘profile’ of the typical
conspiracy believer therefore appears to be that of a morally deficient, psychologically
deficient and cynical individual (e.g., Groh, 1987; Plomin & Post, 1997).
However, this picture is rather too negative and simplistic given the popularity of
conspiracy theories in the general population. Also, whilst previous research provides
valuable insight into the personality characteristics of people who are prone to
conspiracist ideation, it provides little information about the cognitive mechanisms that
lead to conspiracist ideation, and therefore the cognitive mechanisms that are likely to
enable people to resist the conspiracy theories that may be harmful. Recently, social
scientists have focused more on what these psychological processes, cognitive biases and
Belief in conspiracy theories 5
thinking styles might be. For example, Douglas and Sutton (2011) found that the
psychological process of projection contributes to conspiracy belief, in that people
endorsed conspiracy theories more when they felt they themselves would be willing to
conspire. van Prooijen and Jostmann (2013) found that inducing uncertainty led people
to heightened conspiracy belief. Further, Brotherton and French (2014) found that
people who endorsed conspiracy theories were more likely to commit the conjunction
fallacy the tendency to overestimate the likelihood of co-occurring events (Tversky &
Kahneman, 1983). Belief in conspiracy theories may also be in part a result of the
proportionality bias the tendency to attribute small, mundane causes to insignificant
events, and large, major causes to more significant events (Leman & Cinnirella, 2007;
van Prooijen & van Dijk, 2014). Further, conspiracy beliefs have been consistently
found to be associated with supernatural beliefs such as religious beliefs and beliefs in
the paranormal (e.g., Darwin et al., 2011; Newheiser, Farias & Tausch, 2011; Stieger,
Gumhalter, Tran, Voracek & Swami, 2013; Swami, Coles et al., 2013). Belief in
conspiracy theories therefore appears to be associated with the way people perceive the
world around them, in particular when events are unclear or uncertain.
The view that conspiracy belief can be explained by thinking and reasoning is
further underscored by the fact that belief in conspiracy theories has been found to be
associated with education level, such that people with higher levels of education tend to
be less likely to endorse conspiratorial explanations for events (Bird & Bogart, 2003;
Goertzel, 1994; Oliver & Wood, 2014; Uscinski & Parent, 2014). Improving education
in digital literacy (Bartlett & Miller, 2011) and in health and science (e.g., Bogart &
Thorburn, 2005, 2006) are often cited as methods of overcoming conspiracy theorizing.
Some research also points to the more general cognitive benefits of education. For
example, when controlling for other natural confounds such as self-selection into further
Belief in conspiracy theories 6
education, schooling is a significant predictor of IQ (Brinch & Galloway, 2012). Further,
between-sibling differences in IQ can be partly explained by learning-to-read age
(Ritchie, Bates & Plomin, 2015). Also, some evidence suggests that education correlates
negatively (albeit weakly) with irrational beliefs such as attributions of events to fate and
the belief in karma (Banerjee & Bloom, 2014). Logical errors and biases in thinking
style, including conspiracy belief, may therefore be improved by education.
Another such cognitive bias is hypersensitive agency detection (HAD) the
tendency to attribute agency and intentionality where it does not exist or is unlikely to
exist (Barrett, 2004, 2007). Research has shown that people commonly interpret the
movement of two-dimensional geometric shapes as being intentional and agentic (e.g.,
Heider & Simmel, 1944; Scholl & Tremoulet, 2000). In the absence of typical indicators
of agency such as faces or hands, people describe the movement of shapes as they might
normally explain the intentional actions of human beings. Barrett (2004) argued that
people do this because the brain is hard-wired to be hypersensitive to agency in the
environment. Specifically, since humans have evolved in an environment that contains
many agents (e.g., friends, enemies and dangerous predatory animals) hypersensitivity to
agency may be adaptive because it makes people wary in their interactions with the
environment around them, reducing vulnerability to unexpected outcomes and avoiding
risk from potentially dangerous factors (Barrett, 2004; Guthrie, 1993). Being able to
detect and understand an event and react quickly, or respond quickly to an ambiguous
situation, is important for physical and social survival.
HAD has been used to explain why people believe in the existence of invisible
agents such as spirits and gods, and why people are religious (Barrett, 2007). A general
tendency to overestimate the presence of other agents reinforces the belief in an
omnipresent and invisible supernatural agent (Barrett, 2007). HAD has also been linked
Belief in conspiracy theories 7
to superstition the belief that there is a cause and effect between unconnected events
(e.g., that a particular number brings good luck; Lanman, 2012). Similarly, although
much of human behavior is intentional, researchers have argued that people show a
tendency to judge all actions of other people to be intentional by default (Rosset, 2008).
Further, people are prone to explain natural phenomena (e.g., that some rocks are pointy)
as having a purpose (e.g., so that animals do not sit on them) rather than a natural cause
(e.g., the composition of the rock piled up over time; Kelemen, 1999). Much of the time
individuals are able to override or inhibit these tendencies, but under conditions of
limited processing ability, people commonly attribute intentionality where it does not
exist and see natural phenomena as having some hidden purpose (Kelemen & DiYanni,
2005; Rosset, 2008).
As is the case for HAD, there is a robust relationship between conspiracy belief
and belief in supernatural phenomena (Darwin et al., 2011; Newheiser et al., 2011;
Stieger et al., 2013; Swami, Coles et al., 2013). Also, by definition, conspiracy theories
assume purpose, agency and intentionality. Specific human agents are implicated either
when it is unlikely to have been their responsibility, they are likely to have done nothing,
or when the event was most likely the result of an accident. With limited information to
understand events, people are likely to attribute intentionality and agency to attempt to
make sense of what happened. There is some preliminary evidence to suggest that this
indeed occurs. Imhoff and Bruder (2014, Study 4) asked participants to rate their
agreement with conspiracy theories and also to complete a scale of individual differences
in anthropomorphism the tendency to attribute human characteristics to anything other
than a human being as a proxy of the tendency to attribute intentionality and agency
(Waytz, Cacioppo & Epley, 2010). There was a significant positive relationship between
the tendency to anthropomorphize and to believe in conspiracy theories.
Belief in conspiracy theories 8
In the current paper, we aim to replicate and build upon this finding. Specifically,
we argue that the link between HAD and conspiracy belief may have a broader
significance. In particular, this thinking style may enable us to explain why individuals
with lower levels of education tend to be more likely to endorse conspiracy theories.
Specifically, belief in conspiracy theories has been found to be negatively associated with
analytic cognitive style, which reflects the propensity to put aside intuitions when
engaging in problem solving (Swami, Voracek, Steiger, Tran & Furnham, 2014).
Analytic cognitive style, which allows people to reject false inferences that have been
based on instincts and hunches (processes arguably very similar to resisting the automatic
detection of agency), is positively correlated with education level (Pennycook, Cheyne,
Seli, Koehler & Fugelsang, 2012). Further, analytic cognitive style has been found to be
negatively correlated with supernatural belief (Pennycook et al., 2012). We know that
HAD is associated with belief in the supernatural (Barrett, 2007; Riekki, Lindeman &
Raji, 2014) and conspiracy belief (Imhoff & Bruder, 2014), and also that conspiracy
belief is associated with supernatural belief (Darwin et al., 2011; Newheiser et al., 2011;
Stieger et al., 2013; Swami, Coles et al., 2013). Therefore, people with lower levels of
education may be more likely to endorse conspiratorial explanations for events because
they also show a greater tendency to attribute intentionality and agency in general. This
tendency, and in turn conspiracy theorizing, may be somewhat addressed by education.
In two studies, we examined the association between education, HAD and
conspiracy belief. Study 1 (N=202) tested the hypothesis that the tendency to attribute
agency and intentionality would predict conspiracy belief, and that it would also explain
the relationship between education level and belief in conspiracy theories. Study 2
(N=330) tested the hypothesis that these relationships would hold even when beliefs in
paranormal phenomena are taken into account. A range of demographic factors have
Belief in conspiracy theories 9
also been found to be associated with conspiracy theories, such as age (Swami, 2012),
annual income (Bird & Bogart, 2003), religiosity (Furnham, 2013), and political
orientation (Furnham, 2013; Oliver & Wood, 2014). Although effects are not consistent
across studies and it was therefore difficult to form specific hypotheses concerning these
variables, we included them as covariates in our analyses.
Study 1
The first study was designed to replicate Imhoff and Bruder’s (2014, Study 4)
finding that anthropomorphism and beliefs in conspiracy theories are related. We
predicted that the tendency to anthropomorphize would be significantly associated with
the tendency to believe popular conspiracy theories, and would explain the relationship
between education level and conspiracy beliefs.
To extend on this work, we included an additional task to measure the attribution
of intentionality and agency based on the classic study by Heider and Simmel (1944). In
this method, participants were asked to watch a video of shapes moving about a screen
and were asked to rate the extent to which the shapes display human-like characteristics
(e.g., consciousness) and whether their ‘behaviors’ are the result of human-like
characteristics such as free will. This task therefore measures the spontaneous perception
of intentionality in the behavior of inanimate objects rather than more general judgments
about perceived intentionality. Nevertheless, we could expect responses on this task to
be associated with the tendency to anthropomorphize and the tendency to endorse
conspiracy theories. We also tentatively predicted that responses on this task would also
explain the relationship between education and conspiracy belief.
Belief in conspiracy theories 10
Method
Participants and Design
Two hundred and two workers from Amazon’s Mechanical TurkTM were
recruited to complete an online questionnaire (102 women, 99 men, 1 transgender/rather
not say, Mage = 32.4, SD = 12.20). Of this sample, 78% were White/Caucasian, 7%
African American, 7% Asian, 5% Hispanic, 0.5% Pacific Islander and 2.5% Other. Fifty
one percent indicated that they had no religion or were atheist, 39% were Christian (e.g.,
Catholic, Baptist, Protestant, Methodist), 3% agnostic, 3% Jewish, 1% Muslim, 1%
Buddhist, 1% Hindu and 1% Other (including ‘spiritual’ and Wiccan). They were each
paid US $0.75. The design of the study was correlational. The predictor variables were
anthropomorphism, the tendency to attribute intentions to inanimate objects using the
Heider and Simmel (1944) task, and a range of demographic factors. The dependent
variable was belief in conspiracy theories.
Materials and Procedure
The questionnaire was designed and administered using the Qualtrics
questionnaire design software. Participants were first presented with an information page
where they were asked to give their informed consent. They were then asked to complete
a series of measures in random order, except for the demographic measures which always
appeared in the same order at the end of the questionnaire. 1
Belief in conspiracy theories
Participants were asked to read a series of statements related to well-known
conspiracy theories. These were adapted from Douglas and Sutton’s (2011) scale. There
were seven statements (e.g., “Scientists are creating panic about climate change because
it is in their interests to do so”; “The attack on the Twin Towers was not a terrorist action
but a governmental plot”; 1 = strongly disagree, 7 =strongly agree; α = .82).
Belief in conspiracy theories 11
Perceived intentionality and agency
Participants completed the individual differences in anthropomorphism scale
(IDAQ; Waytz et al., 2010). Individual differences in anthropomorphism predict the
amount of responsibility that is placed on an agent (Waytz et al., 2010) and as such
provide a measure of the extent to which individuals afford animals, objects and
situations with the ability to perform actions intentionally. There were 15 questions (e.g.,
“To what extent does the average mountain have free will?”; “To what extent does the
environment experience emotions?”; 1 = not at all, 10 = very much; α = .88).
Participants were also asked to view the animation used by Heider and Simmel
(1944) in which three shapes (a large triangle, a small triangle and a small circle) moved
around the screen and in and out of an opening and closing rectangular box. The
animation was 1:30 minutes long. In the original study using this animation, Heider and
Simmel found that the majority of participants described the movement of shapes as they
would describe the purposeful behavior of animals and humans. In this original work,
participants often created a story about the shapes (e.g., a person chasing another person),
the emotions they experienced (e.g., fear, frustration), and their personal characteristics
(e.g., bravery, aggression). This task therefore provides an additional opportunity to
measure individuals’ tendency to perceive intentionality and agency. After viewing the
animation, participants were asked to answer some questions about the shapes and the
behavior’ of the shapes. Five questions were asked about the shapes (e.g., “To what
extent did you think the shapes were purposeful”; “To what extent did you think the
shapes were conscious?”, α = .87) and five were asked about the ‘behavior’ of the shapes
(e.g., “Did you think the behavior of the shapes was intentional?”; “Did you think the
behavior of the shapes was the result of conscious decisions?”, α = .95; in each case 1 =
not at all, 5 = very much). The two types of questions were significantly correlated,
Belief in conspiracy theories 12
r(202) = .72, p <.001. Using oblique rotation (promax), an exploratory factor analysis
revealed one factor explaining 65.15 per cent of the variance. Several of the items cross-
loaded onto a smaller second factor (eigenvalue = 1). We therefore combined the two
types of questions into a single 10-item scale (α = .94). Note however that the two
measures of HAD (anthropomorphism and perceived intentionality) were not
significantly correlated, r(202) = .11, p = .202.
Education level
Participants rated their education level on a five-point scale (1 = no formal
education, 2 = primary level education, 3 = secondary level education, 4 = college
education, bachelor’s degree, 5 = college education, graduate degree).
Demographics
Finally, participants were asked to provide some demographic details. In addition
to age, gender, ethnicity and religion, participants were asked to rate their religiosity (1 =
not at all religious, 2 = somewhat religious, 3 = moderately religious, 4 = very religious).
Note that this is a measure of the degree to which participants feel that they are religious
(which may include religious practices and behaviors) rather than a measure of religious
belief. They also rated their political orientation (1 = very left-wing, 2 = moderately left-
wing, 3 = slightly left-wing, 4 = center, 5 = slightly right-wing, 6 = moderately right-
wing, 7 = very right-wing) to give a measure of political conservatism. They also rated
their annual household income within 11 brackets ranging from <$10,000 to $100,000 or
more. Finally, they rated their perceived socioeconomic status (1 = working class, 8 =
upper class). At the completion of the questionnaire, participants were debriefed,
thanked and paid.
Belief in conspiracy theories 13
Results and Discussion
Only one person indicated that they were transgender or would rather not say.
We therefore examined if there was a gender difference in conspiracy belief between
females and males. No such gender difference existed, t(199) = .81, p = .418 and gender
was therefore not included as a factor in further analyses. We also did not analyse results
for ethnic or religious differences since numbers were too unequal. Means and standard
deviations for all measures, and correlations between variables, are presented in Table 1.
There were significant positive correlations between conspiracy belief and
anthropomorphism, perceived intentionality, and political conservatism. Conspiracy
belief and age were negatively correlated. Conspiracy beliefs were also marginally
negatively correlated with education level and annual household income. 2
We entered all variables into a step-wise regression where Step 1 included
demographic variables (R2 change = .12, F(5,196) = 6.31, p < .001), Step 2 further
included education level (R2 change = .12, F(1,195) = 1.40, p = .239), and Step 3 further
included anthropomorphism and perceived intentionality (R2 change = .19, F(2,193) =
9.60, p < .001). There was no evidence of collinearity (all tolerances > .70 and all VIF <
1.5). Findings of the final model are presented in Table 2. The overall regression model
was significant F(8, 201) = 6.89, p < .001 and accounted for 22% of the variance. As
hypothesized, anthropomorphism and perceived intentionality each significantly
predicted belief in conspiracy theories. With all variables entered into the regression, age
and political conservatism also remained as significant predictors.
The predicted relationship between education level and belief in conspiracy
theories was present across the two studies reported in this paper. We proceeded to test
whether this was mediated by HAD. To do so, bootstrapped mediation analyses (10,000
resamples) examined the indirect effect of education level on conspiracy belief via
Belief in conspiracy theories 14
anthropomorphism and perceived intentionality using the PROCESS macro for SPSS
(model 4; see Hayes, 2013). Bootstrapping procedures have several advantages over
traditionally used parametric approaches such as regression. They make no assumptions
about distribution and are more robust up to sample sizes of approximately 1,000.
Unlike the traditional approach to testing mediation (Baron and Kenny, 1986), the
decision to undertake these tests is not contingent upon observation of significant effects
in preliminary tests (see Hayes, 2009, p. 414). This procedure calculates the total and all
possible specific indirect effects of the IV on the DV. In this procedure, an indirect
effect is estimated as being significant if zero is not contained within the 95% lower
(LLCI) and upper (ULCI) confidence intervals. Results are presented in Table 3.
Including age, religiosity, political conservatism, annual household income and perceived
SES as covariates, there was an indirect effect of education level on belief in conspiracy
theories via anthropomorphism but not responses on the Heider and Simmel task.
This study therefore first replicates the finding observed by Imhoff and Bruder
(2014) that belief in conspiracy theories are predicted by hypersensitive agency detection
in the form of anthropomorphism. In this study, both anthropomorphism and HAD as
measured using the Heider and Simmel task predicted the extent to which participants
endorsed conspiracy theories. We also found support for the hypothesis that HAD
mediates the relationship between education level and belief in conspiracy theories.
Individuals with higher levels of education were less likely to be hypersensitive to
agency (but only in the form of anthropomorphism) and were therefore less likely to
believe in conspiracy theories.
It should be explained why the effect was observed for anthropomorphism but not
for perceived intentionality as measured by the Heider and Simmel task. It may be the
case that although they both provide a measure of HAD that is associated with
Belief in conspiracy theories 15
conspiracy belief, they are nevertheless measuring subtly different phenomena.
Crucially, the tendency to attribute intentionality to the movement of shapes was not
associated with education and did not mediate the relationship between education level
and conspiracy belief. There is one reason why this may be the case. In contrast to
anthropomorphism, the Heider and Simmel task asks respondents to make an immediate
judgment of causality about behavior they have just observed, rather than a more general
judgment about hidden intentionality. Perhaps education suppresses more immediate
judgments of causality but not teleological thinking more generally (Kelemen, 2004).
Specifically, it may be the case that the Heider and Simmel task taps into people’s
tendency to impute intention onto a specific, concrete situation. Judgments on this task
may reflect errors in reasoning less so than responses to the anthropomorphism scale,
which measures judgments of dispositions possessed by many features of the non-human
world (e.g., the weather, animals). The broader set of judgments may be a clearer
reflection of thinking that can be influenced by long-term educational benefits. 3
Study 2
In Study 2, we aimed to replicate and extend these findings. We again examined
the extent to which HAD mediates the link between education and belief in conspiracy
theories. We also examined the extent to which HAD predicts conspiracy belief
independently of a specific type of irrational belief the belief in paranormal
phenomena. If education simply makes people more equipped to be rational thinkers,
then education should negatively predict conspiracy belief. Also, the effect of education
on conspiracy belief should be mediated by both HAD and paranormal belief. However,
if the attribution of intentionality reflects a unique aspect of thinking style that predicts
conspiracy belief, we would expect only HAD (and not paranormal belief) to mediate the
relationship between education and conspiracy belief.
Belief in conspiracy theories 16
Like conspiracy belief, belief in the paranormal partly reflects a rejection of
conventional or rational understandings of causality. As several studies have shown, just
like conspiracy belief (Swami et al., 2014), belief in the paranormal is negatively
associated with analytical thinking style (Gervais & Norenzayan, 2012; Pennycook et al.,
2012; Shenhav, Rand & Greene, 2012). Further, paranormal beliefs strongly correlate
with endorsement of conspiracy theories (Darwin et al., 2011; Newheiser et al., 2011;
Stieger et al., 2013). It is therefore possible that the relationships observed in Study 1 are
spurious because the tendency to attribute agency and intentionality in the environment is
a specific example of a paranormal belief.
However, we argue that there is a crucial difference between conspiracy and
paranormal beliefs. Specifically, unlike belief in the paranormal, belief in conspiracy
theories does not depend on magical associations. People may be predisposed to invoke
intentional, teleological explanations of natural phenomena and find these explanations
satisfying (Kelemen, 2004) but this does not mean that these explanations must be about
magical or supernatural occurrences. Therefore, whilst the preference for agency and
intentionality explanations may be a specific example of a broader set of beliefs, it may
be a unique type of cognitive error associated with education and belief in conspiracy
theories. The tendency to attribute agency and intentionality may therefore explain the
link between education and conspiracy belief whilst taking into account broader
paranormal belief. Study 2 was therefore designed to examine whether the attribution of
intentionality and agency uniquely accounts for the relationship between education level
and conspiracy belief when taking into account the extent to which people generally
believe in paranormal phenomena.
The study followed a similar procedure to Study 1. Participants were asked to
complete the anthropomorphism scale (Waytz et al., 2010), the adapted Heider and
Belief in conspiracy theories 17
Simmel (1944) task and a measure of agreement with well-known conspiracy theories
(Douglas & Sutton, 2011). Finally, participants completed a scale measuring the extent
to which they show belief in paranormal phenomena such as superstition, clairvoyance,
telepathy and a ‘6th sense’ (Eckblad & Chapman, 1983) and were asked to complete the
same demographic measures as in Study 1.
Method
Participants and Design
Three hundred and thirty workers from Amazon’s Mechanical TurkTM were
recruited to complete an online questionnaire (170 women, 158 men, 2 transgender/rather
not say, Mage = 35.45, SD = 13.05). Of this sample, 79.5% were White/Caucasian, 7%
African American, 7% Asian, 3% Hispanic, 1% Native American, 0.5% Pacific Islander
and 2% Other. Forty five per cent indicated that they were Christian (e.g., Catholic,
Baptist, Protestant, Methodist), 42% had no religion or were atheist, 3.5% agnostic, 2.5%
Jewish, 2% Buddhist, 1% Hindu and 4% Other (e.g., ‘spiritual’ and wiccan). They were
each paid US $1 for their time. The design of the study was correlational. The predictor
variables were anthropomorphism, the tendency to perceive intentionality in inanimate
shapes, paranormal beliefs, and a range of demographic factors as measured in Study 1.
The dependent variable was belief in well-known conspiracy theories.
Materials and Procedure
As in Study 1, the online questionnaire was designed using the Qualtrics
software and first presented participants with an information page where they were asked
to give their informed consent. Participants then completed the measures in random
order, except for the demographic measures which always appeared in the same order at
the end of the questionnaire. 4 They were asked to read a series of statements related to
Belief in conspiracy theories 18
conspiracy theories as in Study 1, but this time there were 17 statements in total (Douglas
& Sutton, 2011; α = .87). 5
As in Study 1, participants also completed the individual differences in
anthropomorphism scale (IDAQ; Waytz et al., 2010; α = .89). They were also asked to
view the animation used by Heider and Simmel (1944) and answer the same questions as
in Study. As in Study 1, the two types of questions used in the Heider and Simmel task
were significantly correlated, r(330) = .80, p <.001 and an exploratory factor analysis
using oblique rotation (promax) revealed one factor explaining 69.62 per cent of the
variance. As in Study 1, we therefore combined the two types of questions into a single
10-item scale (α = .95). In contrast to Study 1, we note here that the two measures of
HAD (anthropomorphism and perceived intentionality) were significantly correlated,
r(330) = .25, p <.001.
Participants also completed a scale of paranormal beliefs (Eckblad & Chapman,
1983). Here, they read a series of 30 statements (e.g., “Some people can make me aware
of them just by thinking about me”, “I have sometimes felt that strangers were reading
my mind”) and were asked to rate their agreement on a five-point scale (1 = strongly
disagree, 5 = strongly agree; α = .87). Finally, participants were asked to provide the
same demographic details as in Study 1. At the completion of the questionnaire,
participants were debriefed, thanked and paid.
Results and Discussion
Only two people indicated that they were transgender or would rather not say.
We therefore examined if there was a gender difference in conspiracy belief between
females and males. No such gender difference existed, t(326) = -0.47, p = .641. There
was also no relationship between age and belief in conspiracy theories, r(328) = -.08, p =
Belief in conspiracy theories 19
.155. Gender and age were therefore not analysed further. As in Study 1, we also did not
analyse results for ethnic or religious differences since numbers were too unequal.
Means and standard deviations for all variables, and correlations between
variables, are presented in Table 4. Beliefs in conspiracy theories were positively
correlated with paranormal beliefs, anthropomorphism, perceived intentionality,
religiosity, and political conservatism. They were negatively correlated with education
level, annual income and socioeconomic status. Also, paranormal beliefs were positively
correlated with the two different measures of agency detection.
We entered all variables into a step-wise regression where Step 1 included
demographic variables (R2 change = .13, F(5,321) = 10.88, p < .001), Step 2 further
included education level (R2 change = .17, F(1,322) = 14.69, p < .001), and Step 3 further
included anthropomorphism, perceived intentionality and paranormal belief (R2 change =
.40, F(3,318) = 43.20, p < .001). Findings of the final model are presented in Table 5.
There was no evidence of collinearity (all tolerances > .60 and all VIF < 1.7). The overall
regression model was significant, F(9, 327) = 25.46, p < .001 and accounted for 40.2% of
the variance in conspiracy belief. As in Study 1, anthropomorphism was a significant
predictor, but this time perceived intentionality was a marginal predictor (p = .06).
Paranormal beliefs, political conservatism, education level and socioeconomic status also
remained as significant predictors. Crucially however, despite the inclusion of
paranormal beliefs and demographic variables that significantly predicted conspiracy
belief, anthropomorphism and perceived intentionality remained significant predictors.
That is, hypersensitivity to agency appears to be a unique predictor of beliefs in
conspiracy theories.
To test the predicted pattern of mediation between education level and belief in
conspiracy theories via attributions of intentionality, bootstrapped mediation analyses
Belief in conspiracy theories 20
(10,000 resamples) again examined the indirect effect of education level on conspiracy
belief via anthropomorphism and perceived intentionality using the PROCESS macro for
SPSS (model 4; see Hayes, 2013). Including age, religiosity, political conservatism,
annual household income and perceived SES as covariates, there was an indirect effect of
education level on belief in conspiracy theories via anthropomorphism but not responses
on the Heider and Simmel task or paranormal belief (see Table 6). These results mirror
those of Study 1.
As in Study 1, anthropomorphism and perceived intentionality were significant
predictors of belief in conspiracy theories. Again, anthropomorphism accounted for the
relationship between education level and belief in conspiracy theories. Crucially
however, despite the inclusion of paranormal beliefs, anthropomorphism still uniquely
accounted for the relationship between education level and belief in conspiracy theories.
General Discussion
The current studies demonstrate that education level predicts the extent to which
people endorse conspiracy theories and that this relationship is mediated by the general
tendency to attribute intentionality and agency where it does not exist, or is unlikely to
exist. In Study 1, people with lower levels of education were more likely to endorse
conspiratorial explanations for well-known events, and this occurred because they were
also more likely to attribute agency and intentionality to inanimate objects. In Study 2,
we replicated this effect and showed that it occurred even when taking into account more
general beliefs in the paranormal. The current findings therefore suggest that conspiracy
theorizing may partially be a consequence of a specific thinking style assuming that
events have an underlying intentional cause when they most likely do not and that this
thinking style may be somewhat changed by education.
Belief in conspiracy theories 21
Researchers have put forward a variety of suggestions for dealing with the
influence of conspiracy theories, when negative consequences are anticipated (e.g.,
radicalization). For example, Sunstein and Vermeule (2009) proposed that governments
may wish to ban conspiracy theories or impose a tax upon people who release material
about conspiracy theories into the public domain. Alternatively, governments may put
forward counterarguments or hire private parties for help, or to communicate
counterarguments. Finally, officials could engage in “cognitive infiltration” by entering
conspiracist groups and planting ideas to tempt people away from conspiracy theories.
However, we know that conspiracy theories are resistant to correction (Sunstein &
Vermeule, 2009; Jolley & Douglas, 2014b), so many of these suggestions may be in
addition to being arguably undemocratic and somewhat impractical ineffective in
addressing the consequences of conspiracy theories.
Although the correlations between education level and conspiracy belief were
modest in the current studies, and therefore many other factors must predict belief in
conspiracy theories, the current findings suggest that education may be one alternative
factor in addressing any negative influence of conspiracy theories. Specifically, in the
current studies individuals with higher levels of education were less likely to see
intentionality everywhere and less likely to believe in conspiracy theories. Consistent
with this finding, Swami et al. (2014) have recently proposed that making people more
analytical thinkers may be one way to address potentially harmful conspiracy theories.
They provide evidence that encouraging analytical thinking in a laboratory setting
reduces conspiracy belief at least temporarily. There is also some evidence that time
spent in education decreases susceptibility to the reasoning bias known as the ‘myside
bias’ in which people evaluate, generate and test hypotheses in a way that is biased
toward their own prior opinions and attitudes (Toplak & Stanovich, 2003).
Belief in conspiracy theories 22
However, education may not only equip people with analytical thinking skills. If
this was the case in our research, we could perhaps expect the link between education
and conspiracy belief to be mediated also by paranormal belief. Instead, education may
equip people with the unique skills to understand non-intentional causality that not
everything happens intentionally, or for a purpose. What these exact tools may be is an
important subject of future research. One potential approach may be to target critical
thinking skills, which involve the ability and willingness to analyze, synthesize and
evaluate arguments or evidence. Critical thinking skills allow individuals to determine
whether conclusions follow logically from evidence, and to consider alternative
explanations (e.g., Griggs, Jackson, Marek & Christopher, 1998). Perhaps critical
thinking skills would enable individuals, long-term, to separate realistic, non-intentional
causal explanations from those that attribute blame to others with little or no justification.
Further research is needed to understand exactly why and how higher levels of education
predict lower levels of conspiracy belief. Research may also consider ways in which
other distal predictors of conspiracy belief may be intervened upon to reduce conspiracy
beliefs that are potentially harmful.
Douglas, Sutton, Jolley and Wood (in press) propose such a proactive method of
allaying the potentially harmful effects of conspiracy theories. As Douglas et al. noted,
many of the known predictors of conspiracy belief are alterable. One of these predictors
is the tendency to make errors in logical and probabilistic reasoning (Brotherton &
French, 2014), and another is the tendency toward magical thinking (e.g., Darwin et al.,
2011; Newheiser et al., 2011; Stieger et al., 2013; Swami, Coles et al., 2013). It is not
clear whether these tendencies can be corrected (Eckblad & Chapman, 1983; Peltzer,
2003), but evidence suggests that they can be reduced by training in logic and in
probability specifically (e.g., Agnoli & Krantz, 1989; Sedlmeier & Gigerenzer, 2001).
Belief in conspiracy theories 23
The current findings suggest that interventions targeting the automatic attribution of
intentionality may be effective in reducing the tendency to believe in conspiracy theories.
In conclusion, it is important to note that addressing conspiracy theories in a
broader educational context does not necessarily encourage people to reject conspiracy
theories completely. Whilst some conspiracy theories may cause problems, some hold
true and play an important part in making authorities responsible for their actions. It is
therefore somewhat a value judgment whether intervening on conspiracy theories is ever
appropriate. Rather, addressing conspiracy theories in an educational context may allow
people to learn the tools to critically examine information about the causes of events and
make informed rather than automatic judgments that the causes must have been
intentional.
Belief in conspiracy theories 24
References
Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, L. (1999). Beliefs in
conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20, 637647. doi: 10.1111/0162-895X.00160
Agnoli, F., & Krantz, D. H. (1989). Suppressing natural heuristics by formal instruction:
The case of the conjunction fallacy. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 515-550. doi:
10.1016/0010-0285(89)90017-0
Banerjee, K., & Bloom, P. (2014). Why did this happen to me? Religious believers’ and
non-believers’ teleological reasoning about life events. Cognition, 133, 277-303.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.017
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in
social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical
considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-
1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
Barrett, J. L. (2004). Why would anyone believe in God? Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira
Press.
Barrett, J. L. (2007). Is the spell really broken? Bio-psychological explanations of
religion and theistic belief. Theology and Science, 5, 57-72. doi:
10.1080/14746700601159564
Bartlett, J., & Miller, C. (2010). The power of unreason: Conspiracy theories, extremism
and counter-terrorism. London: Demos.
Bird, S. T. & Bogart, L. M. (2003). Birth control conspiracy beliefs, perceived
discrimination, and contraception among African Americans. Journal of Health
Psychology, 8, 263276. doi: 10.1177/1359105303008002669
Belief in conspiracy theories 25
Bogart, L. M. & Thorburn, S. T. (2005). Are HIV/AIDS conspiracy beliefs as barrier to
HIV prevention among African Americans? Journal of Acquired Immune
Deficiency Syndromes, 38, 213-218.
Bogart, L. M. & Thorburn, S. T. (2006). Relationship of African Americans’ socio
demographic characteristics to belief in conspiracies about HIV/AIDS and birth
control. Journal of the National Medical Association, 98, 11441150.
Brinch, C. N., & Galloway, T. A. (2012). Schooling in adolescence raises IQ
scores. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 425-430. doi:
10.1073/pnas.1106077109
Brotherton, R., & French, C. C. (2014). Belief in conspiracy theories and susceptibility to
the conjunction fallacy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 238-248. doi:
10.1002/acp.2995
Brotherton, R., French, C. C., & Pickering, A. D. (2013). Measuring belief in conspiracy
theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 279.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279
Burger, J. M., & Cooper, H. M. (1979). The desirability of control. Motivation and
Emotion, 3, 381-393.
Byford, J. (2011). Conspiracy theories: A critical introduction. Basingstoke UK:
Palgrave MacMillan.
Callan, M. J., Sutton, R. M., Harvey, A. J., & Dawtry, R. J. (2014). Immanent justice
reasoning: Theory, research and current directions. Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology, 49, 105-161. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-800052-6.00002-0
Clarke, S. (2002). Conspiracy theories and conspiracy theorizing. Philosophy of the
Social Sciences, 32, 131150. doi: 10.1177/004931032002001
Belief in conspiracy theories 26
Coady, D. (2006). Conspiracy theories: The philosophical debate. Hampshire, UK:
Ashgate.
Cozzolino, P. J. (2011). Trust, cooperation and equality: A psychological analysis of the
formation of social capital. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 203-320.
doi: 10.1348/014466610X519610
Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories. The role of
paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Personality and Individual
Differences, 50, 1289-1293. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2008). The hidden impact of conspiracy theories:
Perceived and actual influence of theories surrounding the death of Princess
Diana. Journal of Social Psychology, 148, 210221. doi:
10.3200/SOCP.148.2.210-222
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of
conspiracy theories is influenced by personal willingness to conspire. British
Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 544-552. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-
8309.2010.02018.x
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Jolley, D., & Wood, M. J. (in press). The social, political,
environmental and health-related consequences of conspiracy theories: Problems
and potential solutions. In M. Bilewicz, A. Cichocka, W. Soral (Eds.), The
psychology of conspiracy. Abingdon, Oxford, UK: Taylor and Francis.
Eckblad, M., & Chapman, L. J. (1983). Magical ideation as an indicator of schizotypy.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 215-225. doi: 10.1037/0022-
006X.51.2.215
Fenster, M. (1999). Conspiracy theories: Secrecy and power in American culture.
Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Belief in conspiracy theories 27
Furnham, A. (2013). Commercial conspiracy theories: A pilot study. Frontiers in
Psychology, 4, 379. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00379
Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Analytic thinking promotes religious
disbelief. Science, 336, 493 - 496. doi: 10.1126/science.1215647
Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731742.
Griggs, R. A., Jackson, S. L., Marek, P. and Christopher, A. N. (1998) Critical thinking
in introductory psychology texts and supplements. Teaching of Psychology, 25,
25466.
Groh, D. (1987). The temptation of conspiracy theory or: why do bad things happen to
good people? Part I: Preliminary draft of a theory of conspiracy theories. In:
Graumann. C.F. & Moscovici, S. (Eds.). Changing conceptions of conspiracy.
(pp. 1-13). New York: Springer.
Guthrie, S. E. (1993). Faces in the clouds: a new theory of religion. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new
millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408-420. doi:
10.1080/03637750903310360
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation and conditional process
analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.
Heider, F. & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American
Journal of Psychology, 57, 243-259.
Imhoff, R., & Bruder, M. (2014). Speaking (un-)truth to power: Conspiracy mentality as
a generalised political attitude. European Journal of Personality, 28, 25-43. doi:
10.1002/per.1930
Belief in conspiracy theories 28
Jonason, P. K. & Webster, G. D. (2010). The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark
triad. Psychological Assessment, 22, 420-432. doi: 10.1037/a0019265
Jolley, D., & Douglas, K.M. (2014a). The social consequences of conspiracism:
Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases the intention to engage in politics and
to reduce one’s carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 35-56. doi:
10.1111/bjop.12018
Jolley, D., & Douglas, K.M. (2014b). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on
vaccination intentions. PLOS ONE, 9(2): e89177. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0089177
Kelemen, D. (1999). The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition,
70, 241-272. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00010-4
Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning about purpose and
design in nature. Psychological Science, 15, 295-301. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-
7976.2004.00672.x
Kelemen, D., & DiYanni, C. (2005). Intuitions about origins: Purpose and intelligent
design in children’s reasoning about nature. Journal of Cognition and
Development, 6, 3-31. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0601_2
Lanman, J. A. (2012). The importance of religious displays for belief acquisition and
secularization. Journal of Contemporary Religion, 27, 49-65. doi:
10.1080/13537903.2012.642726
Leman, P. J. & Cinnirella, M. (2007). A major event has a major cause: Evidence for the
role of heuristics in reasoning about conspiracy theories. Social Psychological
Review, 9, 18-28.
Belief in conspiracy theories 29
Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., & Gignac, G. (2013). NASA faked the moon landing-
Therefore (climate) science is a hoax: An anatomy of the motivated rejection of
science. Psychological Science, 24, 622-633. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457686
Lipsey, M., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. California: Sage
Publications.
McCauley, C. & Jacques, S. (1979). The popularity of conspiracy theories of presidential
assassination: a Bayesian analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology. 37, 637-644. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.5.637
McClure, J., Hilton, D. J., & Sutton, R. M. (2007). Judgements of voluntary and physical
causes in causal chains: Probabilistic and social functionalist criteria for
attributions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 879-901. doi:
10.1002/ejsp.394
McHoskey, J. W. (1995). Case closed? On the John F. Kennedy assassination: Biased
assimilation of evidence and attitude polarization. Basic and Applied Social
Psychology, 17, 395409. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp1703_7
Miller, S. (2002). Conspiracy theories: public arguments as coded social critiques.
Argumentation and Advocacy, 39, 40-56.
Newheiser, A. K., Farias, M., & Tausch, N. (2011). The functional nature of conspiracy
beliefs: Examining the underpinnings of belief in the Da Vinci Code conspiracy.
Personality and Individual Differences, 58, 1007-1011. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.011
Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass
opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 58, 952-966. doi:
10.1111/ajps.12084
Belief in conspiracy theories 30
Peltzer, K. (2003). Magical thinking and paranormal beliefs among secondary and
university students in South Africa. Personality and Individual Differences, 35,
1419-1426. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00359-8
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2014). The
role of analytic thinking in moral judgements and values. Thinking and
Reasoning, 20, 188-214. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2013.865000
Plomin, R. S. & Post, J. M. (1997). Political paranoia. New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Riekki, T., Lindeman, M., & Raji, T. T. (2014). Supernatural believers attribute more
intentions to random movement that skeptics: An fMRI study. Social
Neuroscience, 9, 400-411. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2014.906366
Ritchie, S. J., Bates, T. C., & Plomin, R. (2015). Does learning to read improve
intelligence? A longitudinal multivariate analysis in identical twins from age 7 to
16. Child Development, 86, 23-36. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12272
Rosset, E. (2008). It’s no accident: Our bias for intentional explanations. Cognition, 108,
771-780. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.001
Rubin, Z. & Peplau, L. A. (1975). Who believes in a just world? Journal of Social Issues,
31, 65-89. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1975.tb00997.x
Scholl, B. J., & Tremoulet, R. D. (2000). Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 4, 299-309. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01506-0
Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (2001). Teaching Bayesian reasoning in less than two
hours. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 380-400. doi:
10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.380
Belief in conspiracy theories 31
Shenhav, A., Rand, D. G., & Greene, J. D. (2012). Divine intuition: Cognitive style
influences belief in god. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 423-
428. doi: 10.1037/a0025391
Snyder, C. R. & Fromkin, H. L. (1977). Abnormality as a positive characteristic: The
development and validation of a scale measuring need for uniqueness. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 86, 518-527. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.86.5.518
Stieger, S., Gumhalter, N., Tran, U. S., Voracek, M., & Swami, V. (2013). Girl in the
cellar: A repeated cross-sectional investigation of belief in conspiracy theories
about the kidnapping of Natascha Kampusch. Frontiers in Psychology, 4,
297. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.0029
Sunstein, C. R. & Vermeule, A. (2009). Conspiracy theories: Causes and cures. Journal
of Political Philosophy, 17, 202227. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9760.2008.00325.x
Sutton, R. M., & Douglas, K. M. (2014). Examining the monological nature of
conspiracy theories. In J-W. van Prooijen & P. A. M. van Lange (Eds.), Power,
politics and paranoia: Why people are suspicious of their leaders pp. 254-272.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Swami, V. (2012). Social Psychological Origins of Conspiracy Theories: The Case of the
Jewish Conspiracy Theory in Malaysia. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 280.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00280
Swami, V., & Coles, R. (2010). The truth is out there: Belief in conspiracy theories. The
Psychologist, 23, 560-563.
Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Unanswered questions: A
preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of
9/11 conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749761.
doi: 10.1002/acp.1583
Belief in conspiracy theories 32
Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., Voracek, M.
(2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological
belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and
real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 102,
443-463. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02004.x
Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic
thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133, 572-585.
10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006
Thompson, M. M., Naccarato, M. E., & Parker, K. E. (1989). Assessing cognitive need:
The development of the personal need for structure and personal fear of
invalidity scale. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting for the Canadian
Psychological Society. Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada.
Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2003). Associations between myside bias on an
informal reasoning task and amount of post-secondary education. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 17, 851-860. doi: 10.1002/acp.915
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The
conjunction fallacy in probability judgement. Psychological Review, 90, 293-
315. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.90.4.293
Uscinski. J. E., & Parent, J. M. (2014). American conspiracy theories. New York: Oxford
University Press.
van Prooijen, J.-W., & Jostmann, N. B. (2013). Belief in conspiracy theories: The
influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. European Journal of Social
Psychology , 43, 109-115. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1922
Belief in conspiracy theories 33
van Prooijen, J.-W., & van Dijk, E. (2014). When consequence size predicts belief in
conspiracy theories: The moderating role of perspective taking. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 63-73. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.06.006
Waytz, A., Cacioppo, J., & Epley, N. (2010). Who sees human? The stability and
importance of individual differences in anthropomorphism. Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 5, 219-232. doi: 10.1177/1745691610369336
Welzel, C., Inglehart, R., & Deutsch, F. (2005). Social Capital, Voluntary Associations
and Collective Action: Which Aspects of Social Capital Have the Greatest 'Civic'
Payoff? Journal of Civic Society, 2, 121-146. doi: 10.1080/17448680500337475
Wood, M. J., & Douglas, K. M. (2013). “What about building 7?” A social psychological
study of online discussion of 9/11 conspiracy theories. Frontiers in Psychology,
4, 409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00409
Wood, M., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2012). Dead and alive: Beliefs in
contradictory conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality
Science, 3, 767-773. doi: 10.1177/1948550611434786
Yamagishi, T. & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and commitment in the United States and
Japan. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 129-166.
Belief in conspiracy theories 34
Table 1. Means, (standard deviations) and correlations between all variables measured in Study 1.
Measures
M (SD)
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
1. Conspiracy
belief
2.75 (1.28)
--
.35**
.17*
-.20**
-.09
.27**
-.13+
-.13+
-.06
2. Anthropo-
morphism
3.67 (1.55)
--
.11
-.28***
-.36***
.12
-.21**
-.11
-.09
3. Perceived
intentionality
3.23 (1.18)
--
.02
-.02
.12+
.03
-.02
.05
4. Age
32.4 (12.20)
--
.23**
.12+
.21**
.21**
.04
5. Religiosity
1.74 (.90)
--
.34***
.19**
-.02
.16*
6. Political
conservatism
3.46 (1.46)
--
.02
-.01
.07
7. Education level
3.68 (0.78)
--
.28***
.29***
8. Annual
household
income
5.08 (2.98)
--
.46***
9. Socioeconomic
status
2.96 (1.73)
--
* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001, + p < .10
Conspiracy theories 35
Table 2. Regression coefficients for the analyses of Study 1 (dependent variable is
conspiracy belief)
_______________________________________________________________________
Predictor variable β t
_______________________________________________________________________
Anthropomorphism .249 3.65***
Perceived intentionality .130 2.00*
Age -.160 -2.27*
Religiosity .078 1.10
Political conservatism .224 3.26**
Education level -.051 -.73
Annual household income .030 .40
Socioeconomic status -.037 -.50
_______________________________________________________________________
* p < .05
** p < .01
*** p < .001
Conspiracy theories 36
Table 3. Simple mediation of the indirect effects of education level on belief in
conspiracy theories through anthropomorphism, and responses on the Heider and Simmel
task (Study 1).
Effect Boot SE BootLLCI BootULCI
_____________________________________________________________________
TOTAL -.0566 .0436 -.1573 .0174
Anthropomorphism -.0631 .0369 -.1593 -.0075
Heider and Simmel .0066 .0171 -.0212 .0504
______________________________________________________________________
Conspiracy theories 37
Table 4. Means, (standard deviations) and correlations between all variables measured in Study 2.
* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001, + p < .10
Measures
M (SD)
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
1. Conspiracy
belief
2.40 (1.19)
--
.41**
.22*
-.08
.19**
.28**
-.26**
-.12*
-.16
.52***
2. Anthropo-
morphism
3.64 (1.59)
--
.25***
-.23**
.09+
.07
-.19**
-.07
-.05
.49***
3. Perceived
intentionality
2.95 (1.26)
--
.03
.03
.00
-.05
.02
-.01
.22***
4. Age
35.45 (13.05)
--
.26***
.20***
.09
.08
-.01
-.25***
5. Religiosity
2.01 (1.07)
--
.33***
.00
-.04
-.04
.14*
6. Political
conservatism
3.58 (1.67)
--
.03
.13*
.10
.02
7. Education level
3.71 (0.80)
--
.31***
.27***
-.15**
8. Annual
household
income
5.39 (3.04)
--
.59***
-.16**
9. Socioeconomic
status
3.34 (1.60)
--
-.09
10. Paranormal
belief
3.93 (0.68)
--
Conspiracy theories 38
Table 5. Regression coefficients for the analyses of Study 2 (dependent variable is
conspiracy belief).
_______________________________________________________________________
Predictor variable β t
_______________________________________________________________________
Anthropomorphism .166 3.21**
Perceived intentionality .085 1.89+
Age .007 .144
Religiosity .021 .45
Political conservatism .263 5.68***
Education level -.134 -2.77**
Annual household income .050 .919
Socioeconomic status -.150 -2.77**
Paranormal belief .394 7.71***
_______________________________________________________________________
+ p = .060
* p < .05
** p < .01
*** p < .001
Conspiracy theories 39
Table 6. Simple mediation of the indirect effects of education level on belief in
conspiracy theories through anthropomorphism, responses on the Heider and Simmel
task, and paranormal belief (Study 2).
Effect Boot SE BootLLCI BootULCI
_____________________________________________________________________
TOTAL -.1055 .0479 -.2036 -.0143
Anthropomorphism -.0444 .0221 -.1028 -.0121
Heider and Simmel -.0084 .0095 -.0380 .0030
Paranormal belief -.0526 .0344 -.1234 .0122
______________________________________________________________________
Conspiracy theories 40
Footnotes
1 The current study was part of a larger-scale investigation including measures that form
part of a separate project. These were the extent to which participants felt that they
themselves would take part in real-world conspiracies if placed in the situation of the
alleged conspirators (Douglas & Sutton, 2011), personal need for structure
(Thompson, Naccarato & Parker, 1989), desirability of control (Burger & Cooper,
1979), just world beliefs (Rubin & Peplau, 1975), trust (Goertzel, 1994; Yamagishi &
Yamagishi, 1994), the dark triad of personality (Jonason & Webster, 2010), and
immanent justice reasoning based on ambiguous scenarios (Callan, Sutton, Harvey &
Dawtry, 2014). We also included paranormal beliefs in this study (Eckblad &
Chapman, 1983) but due to an error in the randomization of scales, we were not able
to analyse the results.
2 A meta-analysis showed that the weighted mean correlation between conspiracy belief
and education level across Studies 1 and 2 (fixed effects) was significant, r = -.21,
95% CI = [-.29, -.13], Z = 4.93, p < .001 (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
3 We measured these variables again in Study 2, and in contrast to Study 1 they were
significantly correlated. Note however that a Fisher r-to-z transformation revealed
that the coefficients for Study 1 and Study 2 were not significantly different (z = -.161,
p = .107). Therefore, the different finding across the two studies could simply be due
to variation around conventional p-values.
4 As was the case for Study 1, Study 2 was part of a larger-scale investigation including
measures that form part of a separate project. These were uniqueness-seeking (Snyder
& Fromkin, 1977), preference for intentional causes as attributions (McClure et al.,
2007), social capital (Cozzolino, 2011; Welzel, Ingelhard & Deutsch, 2005), just
Conspiracy theories 41
world beliefs (Rubin & Peplau, 1975), and immanent justice reasoning based on
ambiguous scenarios (Callan et al., 2014).
5 Participants also completed a scale of generic conspiracist ideation (Brotherton,
Pickering & French, 2013) where they were asked to rate the extent to which they
think that 10 statements are true (e.g., “The government is involved in the murder of
innocent citizens and/or well-known public figures, and keeps this a secret”, 1 =
definitely not true, 5 = definitely true; α = .95). Brotherton et al. have argued that
measuring conspiracy beliefs via agreement with popular conspiracy theories can be
problematic because many conspiracy theories are culture-specific. However, the
patterns of results were the same as for the well-known conspiracy theories scale we
report so we do not report the results for the Brotherton et al. (2013) scale here.
... Human beings can be susceptible to thinking that there is agency or intention within or beyond nature, a cognitive bias called "hypersensitive agency detection" [4,5] or "intentionality bias" [5,6]. Since the Scientific Revolution of the 17th and 18th centuries, the role of science (as stipulated by the philosophers and scientists of the time, such as Diderot and Buffon) has been to overcome this cognitive bias and explain nature by examining its properties. ...
... Human beings can be susceptible to thinking that there is agency or intention within or beyond nature, a cognitive bias called "hypersensitive agency detection" [4,5] or "intentionality bias" [5,6]. Since the Scientific Revolution of the 17th and 18th centuries, the role of science (as stipulated by the philosophers and scientists of the time, such as Diderot and Buffon) has been to overcome this cognitive bias and explain nature by examining its properties. ...
Article
Full-text available
Effective bioinspiration requires dialogue between designers/engineers, and biologists, and this dialogue must be rooted in a shared scientific understanding of living systems. To support learning from “nature’s overarching design lessons” the Biomimicry Institute has produced ten “Unifying Patterns of Nature”. These patterns have been developed to engage with those interested in finding biologically inspired solutions to human challenges. Yet, although well-intentioned and appealing, they are likely to dishearten biologists. The aim of this paper is to identify why and propose alternative principles based on evolutionary theory.
... The correlates encompass sociodemographic factors (age, sex, education, and perceived socioeconomic status), differences in worldview (ideology, religiosity, right-wing authoritarianism, and social dominance orientation), cognitive sophistication (cognitive reflection, preferences for intuitive and effortful thinking, and science literacy), and personality traits (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy). Existing literature shows that the dark triad personality traits (Hughes and Machan, 2021;March and Springer, 2019;Uscinski et al., 2022) and indications of low socioeconomic status, such as lower income (Constantinou et al., 2021;Freeman and Bentall, 2017) and lower education level (Douglas et al., 2016;van Prooijen, 2017), are associated with belief in conspiracy theories. Beliefs and biases such as ideology (Imhoff et al., 2022;Sutton and Douglas, 2020;Uscinski et al., 2022), religiousness, (Alper et al., 2020;Frenken et al., 2023), right-wing authoritarianism (Bruder et al., 2013;Grzesiak-Feldman and Irzycka, 2009;Prichard and Christman, 2020;Swami, 2012), and social dominance orientation (Bruder et al., 2013;Swami, 2012) are some of the important factors that are associated with conspiracy beliefs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Past research has shown that perceived scientific consensus (or lack thereof) on an issue predicts belief in misinformation. In the current study ( N = 729), we investigated how perceived consensus among both experts and laypeople predicts beliefs in localized and specific conspiracy theories in Turkey, a non-WEIRD country. Participants in our study were found to overestimate consensus among both experts and laypeople regarding baseless conspiracy theories surrounding the alleged secret articles of the Lausanne Treaty and unused mining reserves in Turkey. Notably, conspiracy believers exhibited a higher tendency to overestimate consensus compared to non-believers. Furthermore, perceived expert consensus had a stronger association with conspiracy beliefs than perceived laypeople consensus. We also explored the correlates of conspiracy beliefs and perceived consensus, including socioeconomic factors, worldview, cognitive sophistication, and personality. The results further indicate that the correlations between belief and perceived consensus manifest with comparable magnitudes, irrespective of the specific conspiracy theories under consideration. These findings support the potential of perceived consensus as an important factor for understanding conspiracy beliefs.
... Conversely, individuals with high levels of paranoid ideation might integrate irrelevant cues as cheating cues, increasing probability of occurrence of cheating. The incorporation of incorrect inputs in the detection of cheating could also explain the link between sensitivity to agency and the belief in conspiracy theories (Brotherton & French, 2015;Douglas et al., 2016). Indeed, social exchange theory proposes that cheating detection mechanisms are particularly sensitive to the detection of intentionality in order to distinguish between an accidental and an intentional violation of social contracts (Cosmides et al., 2010;Cosmides & Tooby, 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Research on conspiracy theories sometimes tends to pathologise this phenomenon with a focus on the impact of (sub) pathological predictors. However, socio-political factors also play a significant role in predicting belief in specific conspiracy theories. The aim of this article is to bridge these two perspectives through a unified cognitive mechanism. Based on an overlap between cheating and conspiracy concepts, we assume a cheating detection mechanism likely to underlie belief in conspiracy theories. Starting from the adaptive challenges of cheating detection, we explore the workings of this mechanism using signal detection theory and error management theory. The probability of cheating and decision bias according to the asymmetry of error costs in cheating detection could lead individuals to infer conspiracy theories. This functional mechanism not only explains the links between socio-political predictors and adherence to conspiracy theories but also helps us deduce alterations that may foster a stable inclination towards believing in conspiracy theories. These alterations, in turn, offer an explanation for the links between (sub)pathological predictors and conspiracy mentality. By integrating existing literature, our proposed model sheds light on the mechanisms underlying belief in conspiracy theories and presents new predictions to guide future research.
Preprint
Full-text available
During times of crisis, conspiracy theories tend to proliferate – especially in polarized political environments. Existing research has shown that both the COVID-19 pandemic and the Russian invasion of Ukraine have spawned a surge in conspiracy theories. However, there is a notable gap in academic research concerning the interconnection between these two sets of conspiracy theories. Our preregistered representative survey study conducted in Hungary (N = 1000) investigates the interconnections between conspiracy theories about COVID-19 and the war in Ukraine, and the moderating role of trust, conspiracy mentality, political preference, and socio-demographic variables in this relationship. Hungary, a nation marked by exceptionally high levels of tribal polarization, and plagued by pervasive disinformation presents an ideal case for examining the polarizing nature of conspiracy theories. Contrary to the prevailing notion that a generalized mistrust links unrelated conspiracy theories, the research finds that specific, logically unrelated conspiracy theories can be horizontally connected to each other. We found that conspiracy theories about COVID-19 and vaccines were strongly related to conspiracy theories about the Russian invasion of Ukraine. This relationship persists independently of party preference, and neither mistrust in media nor Conspiracy Mentality can explain this relationship. Surprisingly, trust in domestic media positively predicted war-related conspiracy beliefs. These results, even if correlational, raise the possibility that conspiracy theories about COVID-19 (which preceded the invasion of Ukraine in February 2022) could serve as a "gateway theory," leading to the acceptance of war-related conspiracies. Our findings, in line with some previous research on “gateway theories” and the monological nature of conspiracy theories, suggest that communication interventions should address specific narratives rather than solely focusing on the “root cause” such as mistrust in institutions – which not always go hand in hand with conspiracy theorizing. Furthermore, building trust in official sources may worsen the problem in contexts where mainstream media spreads conspiracy theories. Thus, a nuanced, context-sensitive communication approach is essential in combating conspiracy theories effectively.
Article
The motto of the conspiracist, “Do your own research,” may seem ludicrous to scientists. Indeed, it is often dismissed as a mere rhetorical device that conspiracists use to give themselves the semblance of science. In this perspective paper, we explore the information-seeking activities (“research”) that conspiracists do engage in. Drawing on the experimental psychology of aha experiences, we explain how these activities, as well as the epistemic experiences that precede (curiosity) or follow (insight or “aha” experiences) them, may play a crucial role in the appeal and development of conspiracy beliefs. Aha moments have properties that can be exploited by conspiracy theories, such as the potential for false but seemingly grounded conclusions. Finally, we hypothesize that the need for autonomous epistemic agency and discovery is universal but increases as people experience more uncertainty and/or feel epistemically excluded in society, hence linking it to existing literature on explaining conspiracy theories.
Article
Conspiracy theories allege secret plots between two or more powerful actors to achieve an outcome, sometimes explaining important events or proposing alternative understandings of reality in opposition to mainstream accounts, and commonly highlight the threat presented by the plot and its conspirators. Research in psychology proposes that belief in conspiracy theories is motivated by a desire to understand threats and is predicted by increased anxiety. Morbid curiosity describes the tendency to seek out information about threatening or dangerous situations and is associated with an interest in threat‐related entertainment and increased anxiety. Across three studies, we investigated the relationship between morbid curiosity and conspiracy theories in US‐based samples. We found that higher trait morbid curiosity was associated with higher general conspiracist beliefs (Study 1) and the perceived threat of conspiratorial explanations of events (Study 2). Using a behavioural choice paradigm, we found that participants who chose to investigate morbidly curious stimuli were more likely to choose to learn about conspiratorial explanations for events (Study 3). Greater curiosity about the minds of dangerous people was consistently the strongest predictor of conspiratorial ideation and interest. These results suggest that morbid curiosity is an important but hitherto unstudied predictor of conspiratorial interest and belief.
Chapter
QAnon has emerged as the defining conspiracy group of our times, and its far-right conspiracies are extraordinary for their breadth and extremity. Bringing together scholars from psychology, sociology, communications, and political science, this cutting-edge volume uses social science theory to investigate aspects of QAnon. Following an introduction to the 'who, what, and why' of QAnon, Part I focuses on the psychological characteristics of QAnon followers and the group's methods for recruiting and maintaining these followers. Part II includes chapters at the intersection of QAnon and society, arguing that society has constructed QAnon as a threat and the social need to belong motivates its followers. Part III discusses the role of communication in promoting and limiting QAnon support, while Part IV concludes by considering the future of QAnon. The Social Science of QAnon is vital reading for scholars and students across the social sciences, and for legal and policy professionals.
Article
Full-text available
The present study focused on whether Dark Tetrad characteristics (machiavellianism, narcissism, psychopathy, and sadism) predict conspiracy beliefs (COVID-19 related and general) in the Slovak population. Three independent samples were used for the study. The first sample consisted of 804 (73.9% women) university students of the middle-sized Slovak university. The second sample consisted of 489 (61.2% women) Facebook users. The third group was recruited from an online panel of a research agency and included 600 respondents (49% women). The results provided different findings for the three research samples. Results showed that machiavellianism was a positive predictor of conspiracy beliefs (COVID-19 and general) among respondents from a research agency and a university (general only). Psychopathy positively predicted COVID-19 and general conspiracy beliefs only among respondents from the Facebook sample. Narcissism positively pre dicted COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs among university students. The results of the study suggest that there is no consistent pattern of relationship between Dark Tetrad and conspiracy beliefs across different samples in Slovakia. Future research should focus on whether these inconsistencies are due to sample characteristics or they express lack of stable relationships between these two psychological variables.
Article
The majority of psychological research highlights the role of lapses in rational thinking as a significant contributing factor to conspiracy beliefs (CBs). However, historical examples demonstrate that not all CBs are inherently incorrect. In this paper, we propose a more nuanced approach, suggesting that instead of universally labelling CBs as false or adopting an agnostic stance towards their veracity, it is beneficial to assess the plausibility of each CB individually along a continuum. To achieve this, we recommend utilizing two key indicators: (1) the prior probability of a specific conspiracy theory being true, considered independently of any supporting or contradicting evidence, and (2) the extent to which proponents of a CB demonstrate actively open-minded thinking. We delve into the rationale behind employing such a nuanced approach to evaluating CB plausibility and present several strategies for calculating and integrating plausibility ratings within conspiracy research. Consistent with our proposed framework, we argue that conspiracy researchers possess both the capability and responsibility to differentiate certain CBs as comparatively more irrational than others, provided that such differentiation is based on principled and probabilistic grounds.
Article
Full-text available
Sixty-four participants were asked to read one of four variations on a vignette that reported the assassination or attempted assassination of a hypothetical President and then to rate the likelihood that each of eight statements relating to the vignettes was accurate. Participants were more likely to endorse a conspiracy theory to account for events when the consequences were major (the President dies) rather than comparatively minor (the President survives). Levels of belief in real-world conspiracy theories did not relate to this inference-making process even though participants whose level of belief in conspiracy theories was high were more likely to doubt the veracity of the reported details of the event.
Article
Full-text available
Surveyed 348 residents of southwestern New Jersey and found that most believed that several of a list of 10 conspiracy theories were at least probably true. Ss who believed in 1 conspiracy were more likely also to believe in others. Belief in conspiracies was correlated with anomia, lack of interpersonal trust, and insecurity about employment. Blacks and Hispanics were more likely to believe in conspiracy theories than were Whites. Younger Ss were slightly more likely to believe in conspiracy theories, but there were few significant correlations with gender, educational level, or occupational category. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
In the present research, we examined people's tendency to endorse or question belief in conspiracy theories. In two studies, we tested the hypothesis that the perceived morality of authorities influences conspiracy beliefs, particularly when people experience uncertainty. Study 1 revealed that information about the morality of oil companies influenced beliefs that these companies were involved in planning the war in Iraq, but only when uncertainty was made salient. Similar findings were obtained in Study 2, which focused on a bogus newspaper article about a fatal car accident of a political leader in an African country. It is concluded that uncertainty leads people to make inferences about the plausibility or implausibility of conspiracy theories by attending to morality information. Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Article
The authors present and test a new method of teaching Bayesian reasoning, something about which previous teaching studies reported little success. Based on G. Gigerenzer and U. Hoffrage's (1995) ecological framework, the authors wrote a computerized tutorial program to train people to construct frequency representations (representation training) rather than to insert probabilities into Bayes's rule (rule training). Bayesian computations are simpler to perform with natural frequencies than with probabilities, and there are evolutionary reasons for assuming that cognitive algorithms have been developed to deal with natural frequencies. In 2 studies, the authors compared representation training with rule training; the criteria were an immediate learning effect, transfer to new problems, and long-term temporal stability. Rule training was as good in transfer as representation training, but representation training had a higher immediate learning effect and greater temporal stability.
Chapter
Human beings are continually getting into situations wherein they can no longer understand the world around them. Something happens to them that they feel they did not deserve. Their suffering is described as an injustice, a wrong, an evil, bad luck, a catastrophe. Because they themselves live correctly, act in an upright, just manner, go to the right church, belong to a superior culture, they feel that this suffering is undeserved. In the search for a reason why such evil things happen to them, they soon come upon another group, an opponent group to which they then attribute certain characteristics: This group obviously causes them to suffer by effecting dark, evil, and secretly worked out plans against them. Thus the world around them is no longer as it should be. It becomes more and more an illusion, a semblance, while at the same time the evil that has occurred, or is occurring and is becoming more and more essential, takes place behind reality. Their world becomes unhinged, is turned upside down, in order to prevent damage to or destruction of their own group (religion, culture, nation, race) they must drive out, render harmless, or even destroy those—called “conspirators”—carrying out their evil plans in secret. Such orgies of persecution and annihilation against imagined or imaginary enemies accompany the history of Europe from, at the latest, the era of the persecution of the Jews and the Inquisition of the High Middle Ages up through the genocides of the recent past. In comparison to the belief in conspiracies—which is called the theory of conspiracy—belief in magic and witches associated with the so-called primitive cultures and with the European folk-culture seems harmless, especially in regard to the consequences for the conspirators.
Article
If we wish to more folly account for how conspiracy theories function in twenty-first century America, then we must be able to move beyond treating conspiracy theories solely as flawed arguments. This essay will argue that conspiracies fulfill two roles—the argumentative role traditionally studied that asserts that some powerful entity is engaged in a grand scheme to control or deceive the masses, and what I shall call the coded social critique role-an underlying message that critiques various social, political, or economic institutions and actors. In other words, the point of dispute in the competing theories and government accounts is equally over the different institutions' ethos and legitimacy as it is over the facts of the crash itself.
Chapter
Immanent justice reasoning involves causally attributing a deserved outcome to someone’s prior moral deeds or character, even when such a causal connection is physically implausible. This chapter describes a body of work showing that immanent justice reasoning is (a) motivated, in part, by the need to construe outcomes as deserved; (b) driven by intuitive more than controlled mental processes; and (c) more openly expressed among individuals who believe in supernatural phenomena. This review also documents several additional lines of inquiry exploring key assumptions about the nature, origins, and functions of immanent justice reasoning, including immanent justice reasoning for self-relevant fortuitous outcomes, the social-communicative function of immanent justice reasoning, and the interplay between immanent justice and normative causal reasoning. Early research portrayed immanent justice reasoning as unique to children, but this chapter identifies several conditions under which it is predictably displayed by adults. Immanent justice reasoning serves important psychological functions in adulthood, and is underpinned by reasoning processes and metaphysical assumptions that are not put away when children become adults.
Article
People often believe that significant life events happen for a reason. In three studies, we examined evidence for the view that teleological beliefs reflect a general cognitive bias to view the world in terms of agency, purpose, and design. Consistent with this hypothesis, we found that individual differences in mentalizing ability predicted both the tendency to believe in fate (Study 1) and to infer purposeful causes of one's own life events (Study 2). In addition, people's perception of purpose in life events was correlated with their teleological beliefs about nature, but this relationship was driven primarily by individuals' explicit religious and paranormal beliefs (Study 3). Across all three studies, we found that while people who believe in God hold stronger teleological beliefs than those who do not, there is nonetheless evidence of teleological beliefs among non-believers, confirming that the perception of purpose in life events does not rely on theistic belief. These findings suggest that the tendency to perceive design and purpose in life events-while moderated by theistic belief-is not solely a consequence of culturally transmitted religious ideas. Rather, this teleological bias has its roots in certain more general social propensities.