Content uploaded by Arnaud Wisman
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Arnaud Wisman on Aug 25, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Smell of Death: Evidence that Putrescine Elicits Threat Management
Mechanisms
Arnaud Wisman and Ilan Shrira
Journal Name: Frontiers in Psychology
ISSN: 1664-1078
Article type: Original Research Article
Received on: 27 May 2015
Accepted on: 10 Aug 2015
Provisional PDF published on: 10 Aug 2015
Frontiers website link: www.frontiersin.org
Citation: Wisman A and Shrira I(2015) The Smell of Death: Evidence that
Putrescine Elicits Threat Management Mechanisms. Front. Psychol.
6:1274. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01274
Copyright statement: © 2015 Wisman and Shrira. This is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other
forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor
are credited and that the original publication in this journal is
cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use,
distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply
with these terms.
This Provisional PDF corresponds to the article as it appeared upon acceptance, after rigorous
peer-review. Fully formatted PDF and full text (HTML) versions will be made available soon.
Emotion Science
1
1
2
The Smell of Death:
3
Evidence that Putrescine Elicits Threat Management Mechanisms
4
5
Arnaud Wisman1*, Ilan Shrira2
6
7
1School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, UK
8
2Department of Behavioral Sciences, Arkansas Tech University, Russellville, AR, USA
9
10
*Correspondence:
11
Dr. Arnaud Wisman
12
School of Psychology
13
University of Kent
14
Canterbury
15
Kent CT2 7NP
16
United Kingdom
17
a.wisman@kent.ac.uk
18
19
Keywords: olfaction, putrescine, threat, threat management, chemosensory cue
20
21
Abstract
22
23
The ability to detect and respond to chemosensory threat cues in the environment plays a vital
24
role in survival across species. However, little is known about which chemical compounds can
25
act as olfactory threat signals in humans. We hypothesized that brief exposure to putrescine, a
26
chemical compound produced by the breakdown of fatty acids in the decaying tissue of dead
27
bodies, can function as a chemosensory warning signal, activating threat management responses
28
(e.g., heightened alertness, fight-or-flight responses). This hypothesis was tested by gauging
29
people’s responses to conscious and non-conscious exposure to putrescine. In Experiment 1,
30
putrescine increased vigilance, as measured by a reaction time task. In Experiments 2 and 3, brief
31
exposure to putrescine (vs. ammonia and a scentless control condition) prompted participants to
32
walk away faster from the exposure site. Experiment 3 also showed that putrescine elicited
33
implicit cognitions related to escape and threat. Experiment 4 found that exposure to putrescine,
34
presented here below the threshold of conscious awareness, increased hostility toward an out-
35
group member. Together, the results are the first to indicate that humans can process putrescine
36
as a warning signal that mobilizes protective responses to deal with relevant threats. The
37
implications of these results are briefly discussed.
38
39
1. Introduction
40
41
When animals die they release an unpleasant smell. A pungent component of this scent is emitted
42
by putrescine, a volatile diamine that results from the breakdown of fatty acids in the putrefying
43
2
tissue of dead bodies (Hussain et al., 2013). Interestingly, animal research shows that putrescine
1
can function as a powerful chemosensory signal that prompts the perceiver to leave or avoid the
2
area (Prounis & Shields, 2013; Yao et al., 2009). The aim of the present research is to show that
3
humans respond in a similar way to putrescine, and more generally, that exposure to putrescine
4
triggers threat management behaviors (Blanchard et al., 2001; Neuberg et al., 2011).
5
6
A growing body of research suggests that humans can identify threats via chemosignals
7
(Ackerl et al., 2002; Chen & Haviland-Jones, 2000; de Groot et al., 2012; Mujica-Parodi et al.,
8
2009; Prehn et al., 2006; Zhou & Chen, 2009). For instance, when people are exposed to sweat
9
taken from donors during a fearful experience, perceivers show a heightened startle reflex (Pause
10
et al., 2009; Prehn et al., 2006) and interpret ambiguous facial expressions as fearful (Zhou &
11
Chen, 2009). This transmission of threat-arousing chemosignals is assumed to serve an adaptive
12
function by orienting us to impending dangers. Indeed, the ability to detect and process
13
chemosensory threat cues is vital for the survival of a wide range of species (Stevenson, 2010).
14
However, thus far there is little evidence that humans can, like other organisms, detect olfactory
15
threat cues in the environment through means other than the chemosignals (e.g., body sweat) of
16
conspecifics.
17
18
The decay of tissue and its resulting scent can function as a “necromone” cue that signals
19
an animal’s death to conspecifics. Alarm and avoidance behaviors (necrophobic behaviors) in
20
response to these scents are widespread in the animal kingdom and thought to have evolved at
21
least 420 million years ago (Yao et al., 2009). In fact, recent research shows that necrophobic
22
behavior may have innate underpinnings through the activation of trace amine-associated
23
receptors (TAARs), a group of specialized scent receptors in the olfactory epithelium (Horowitz
24
et al., 2014; Hussain et al., 2013; Li & Liberles, 2015). TAARs are known to detect specific
25
chemicals that evoke behavioral responses, without the need for prior exposure to the scents. For
26
example, in model vertebrates, certain TAARs respond to diamines (e.g., putrescine) by
27
producing avoidant behaviors that likely serve to defend against immediate dangers (Yoon et al.,
28
2015). Thus, it is feasible that we have a chemosensory sensitivity to diamines like putrescine (Li
29
& Liberles, 2015), given that their detection can aid survival (Stevenson, 2010).
30
31
A further advantage of examining putrescine as a threat stimulus is that we know what it
32
is. Despite the impressive amount of indirect support for human chemosignals amassed in recent
33
years, their chemical properties have yet to be identified (Wyatt, 2009). Focusing on a known
34
compound, putrescine, enables us to directly test whether it plays a causal role in human threat
35
responses. In a similar vein, although several studies have shown that chemosensory cues can
36
elicit greater readiness for behavior (Bradley et al., 2001; Prehn et al., 2005), thus far there is
37
little direct evidence that a specific chemical substance can cause overt behavioral changes in
38
humans (Wysocki & Preti, 2004). Since exposure to putrescine elicits specific behaviors in
39
animals (e.g., escape, avoidance), we can examine whether putrescine produces similar behaviors
40
in humans. In sum, putrescine appears to be well-suited to test as a specific chemical compound
41
that can act as a threat signal in humans.
42
43
Chemosensory cues can convey danger in at least two fitness-relevant domains: microbial
44
and predator threats (Stevenson, 2010). First, olfactory information is often central to identifying
45
the presence of pathogens. For example, pathogens can alter the scent of those who become
46
3
infected, which can be detected by conspecifics (Arakawa et al., 2010; Olsson et al., 2014; Tybur
1
et al., 2011). Similarly, the release of putrescine in decaying tissue co-occurs with the arrival of
2
bacteria, a motivation for others to eschew physical contact with the dead body. A number of
3
species exhibit necrophobic behaviors, and after detecting the scent emanating from dead bodies,
4
usually respond by leaving or avoiding the area (Prounis & Shields, 2013). Second, putrescine
5
released by decaying bodies can signal the risk of predation (Boissy et al., 1986). Since a large
6
proportion of deaths in the wild are the result of predator attacks, putrescine would be a useful
7
alarm cue to stay away (Misslin, 2003).
8
9
In humans, responses to specific scents can develop through learned associations between
10
odors and personal experiences (Degel et al., 2001; Stevenson et al., 1998). For example, based
11
on the cultural expression that when “something smells fishy” it is viewed suspiciously, exposure
12
to fish-like odors arouses suspicion toward others and reduces cooperation, an orientation that is
13
assumed to result from conditioned reactions to this scent (Lee & Schwarz, 2012). Since
14
putrescine can emanate from various sources (Yeoman et al., 2013), people may learn to
15
associate the smell of putrescine with threats, and it is plausible that occasional exposure to
16
putrescine, whenever it occurs, could lead to conditioned threat responses (Stevenson, 2010).
17
However, we render it unlikely that modern humans have strong conscious meaningful
18
associations with the scent of putrescine. Moreover, conscious scent evaluations are often
19
inaccurate, context dependent, and colored by other sensory modalities (Sela & Sobel, 2010). In
20
view of this, it is important to note that responses to aversive chemosensory cues do not require
21
prior learning or conscious evaluation (Dielenberg et al., 2001; Li et al., 2013; Miller & Maner,
22
2010). Indeed, scents can alter our perception, cognition, behavior, and physiology (e.g., heart
23
rate, skin conductance) even when there is no conscious scent detection (Krusemark & Li, 2012;
24
Li et al., 2007; Pause et al., 2009; Sela & Sobel, 2010), and even after olfactory adaptation has
25
set in (de Groot et al., 2012; Smeets & Dijksterhuis, 2014). Thus, neither prior associations with
26
olfactory signals, nor conscious processing, are necessary conditions for people to process them
27
as threatening (Pause, 2012; Williams et al., 2006; Köster, 2002; Sela & Sobel, 2010; Smeets &
28
Dijksterhuis, 2014).
29
30
At the most basic level, threat detection increases vigilance and sharpens our reactions to
31
events in the environment (Williams et al., 2006). For instance, detection of a predator’s scent
32
will interrupt foraging and increase behaviors (e.g., scanning the environment) that facilitate
33
predator detection (Woody & Szechtman, 2011). Once the threat management system is
34
engaged, it produces readiness for fight-or-flight behaviors (Blanchard et al., 1986; Cannon,
35
1927; Gray & McNaughton, 2003; Mobbs et al., 2009). Flight responses seek to escape the
36
situation, whereas fight responses—whether physical or verbal aggression—are typically only
37
used when escape is not possible. In contrast to popular belief that the dominant response to
38
threats is to fight, flight is actually far more common (Misslin, 2003), presumably because nature
39
selects more strongly for strategies that minimize risk. In one study, for example, when people
40
were confronted by a threatening out-group member, they responded with aggressive readiness
41
(fight), but only when there was little possibility of escaping; when given the option, though,
42
participants chose to distance themselves (flight) from the other person (Cesario et al., 2010).
43
44
2. Overview and Hypotheses
45
46
4
Coming full circle, we propose that putrescine can serve as a (non-conscious) signal that initiates
1
threat management responses. Specifically, we hypothesize that brief exposure to putrescine
2
increases vigilance, followed by the readiness to either escape (flight), or engage in aggressive
3
readiness (fight) when escape is not possible. Experiment 1 assessed whether putrescine (vs.
4
ammonia and a neutral scent) increased vigilance as measured by faster responses in a simple
5
reaction time task. Experiments 2 and 3 assessed whether brief exposure to putrescine (vs.
6
ammonia and neutral scent) caused participants to walk away faster from the exposure site after
7
completing the experiment (outdoors). Experiment 3 also tested whether putrescine evoked
8
cognitions related to escape and threat. Finally, Experiment 4 examined whether non-conscious
9
exposure to putrescine increased aggressive readiness (e.g., defensiveness toward an out-group
10
member). All four experiments adhered to the Declaration of Helsinki guidelines, and gained the
11
prior approval by the University Research Ethics Committee. Written consent was obtained from
12
all participants involved in these experiments, and all were fully debriefed.
13
14
3. Experiment 1: The effect of putrescine on vigilance
15
16
In Experiment 1, we tested whether brief exposure to putrescine increased vigilance. To measure
17
vigilance, we employed a task closely modeled after the shortened version of the Psychomotor
18
Vigilance Task (PVT; Dinges & Powell, 1985) that assessed participants’ reaction times to a red
19
dot that was presented at random intervals on a computer screen.
20
21
In addition, Experiment 1 was designed to determine whether ammonia served as an
22
appropriate aversive control condition. Our pilot testing revealed that ammonia, unlike other
23
aversive scents we had examined (i.e., skatole
1
and indole), was rated similarly to putrescine on
24
repugnance, familiarity, and intensity. Moreover, previous research has used ammonia (NH3;
25
ammonium hydroxide) as an aversive scent prime (Rieser et al., 1976; Wise et al., 2005).
26
Furthermore, ammonia can increase trigeminal nerve activation associated with vigilance and
27
sensory rejection, via activation of the sympathetic nervous system (Hummel & Kobal, 1992;
28
Sekizawa & Tsubone, 1994). However, some research suggests that unpleasant ambient odors
29
can also decrease reaction times on simple tasks like the current PVT (Millot et al., 2002). In
30
view of this, we made no specific prediction about whether ammonia, like putrescine, would
31
enhance vigilance relative to our scentless control condition.
32
33
3.1. Method
34
35
3.1.1. Participants and Procedure
36
37
A sample of sixty participants (43 females; Mage = 21.20, SD = 3.20) completed the study in
38
return for a financial incentive of 3 pounds (approximately $5).
39
40
1
In line with previous research (Wheatley & Haidt, 2005), we pilot-tested the so-called “fart spray” along with
skatole, indole, and ammonia, for suitability as an aversive control condition. These ratings are presented in Table 1.
As can be seen, ammonia and fart spray were rated similarly to putrescine on all three dimensions of repugnance,
familiarity, and intensity, whereas indole and skatole diverged from putrescine on at least one dimension. A
disadvantage of fart spray, however, is that we could not ascertain its precise chemical compounds—its
manufacturers were reluctant to disclose this information.
5
Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: putrescine (C4H12N2;
1
Sigma-Aldrich), ammonia (5%; NH3; Sigma-Aldrich), or water. One hour before the start of the
2
experiment, cotton wool pads were blotted with 2 ml of one of the three compounds, and stored
3
separately in small (100 ml) sealable amber jars. Participants were run in our lab individually,
4
and seated in different cubicles to avoid carryover effects of scents. The refreshment rate in each
5
cubicle was 4 to 5 air changes (cycles) per hour. Furthermore, participants were booked at least
6
30 minutes apart in order to ventilate the rooms—by opening the lab room’s window—between
7
sessions. When preparing materials for the experiment, one of the researchers marked the bottom
8
of each jar with a number code, so that the experimenters were unaware of the meaning of these
9
codes. This basic procedure was repeated in our subsequent experiments to keep the
10
experimenters blind to the conditions.
11
Participants were seated in front of a standard PC (equipped with Authorware 7.1
12
software) with a 17-inch screen. They were given instructions (on-screen) to open the jar, sniff
13
the scent inside for 10 seconds, and close the jar. After that, they rated the scent on its intensity
14
(“This scent is intense”; 1 = strongly disagree and 9 = strongly agree), repugnance (“This scent
15
is repugnant”; 1 = strongly disagree and 9 = strongly agree), and familiarity (“This scent is
16
familiar”; 1 = strongly disagree and 9 = strongly agree). Repugnance was included as evaluative
17
rating (alongside the standard measures of intensity and familiarity) because repugnance (or
18
disgust) is often a central component of aversive scents. Participants were then introduced to the
19
adapted PVT, which lasted about five minutes (see Loh et al., 2004). The task instructed them to
20
click on a red dot as quickly as possible whenever they saw the dot on the screen. Ten dots (each
21
measuring 1 cm) were shown at different locations on the screen, and the time between
22
appearances was randomized at variable intervals (2-45 sec). As soon participants clicked on the
23
red dot with the mouse, a screen appeared for five seconds with the message: “prepare for next
24
trial”. Participants received two practice trials first, to get them familiar with the main task of ten
25
trials. Finally, after completing the PVT and filling out a standard demographic questionnaire,
26
they were fully debriefed and thanked for their participation.
27
28
3.2. Results and Discussion
29
30
3.2.1. Hedonic Evaluations
31
32
We began by testing our prediction, based on our pilot testing, that putrescine and ammonia
33
would not differ from each other on repugnance, familiarity and intensity. As predicted, separate
34
one-way between-subjects ANOVAs revealed that there was no significant difference between
35
ammonia and putrescine on repugnance, F(1, 38) = 0.38, p = .54, η² = .01, familiarity, F(1, 38) =
36
0.26, p = .26, η² = .03, or intensity, F(1, 38) = 0.14, p = .71, η² = .004 (see Table 2, for
37
descriptive statistics). Moreover, the analyses reported below were not altered when entering all
38
hedonic evaluations as covariates.
39
40
3.2.2. Reaction Times
41
42
We examined our main prediction that putrescine, relative to the neutral control condition
43
(water), would elicit faster reaction times. In line with previous PVT research, we applied
44
reciprocal transformation to the raw data (i.e., 1/RT). This type of transformation is standard
45
within the PVT paradigm, as it reduces the impact of extreme scores and brings them into an
46
6
acceptable range (Dinges et al., 1987; Dorrian et al., 2004). A one-way between-subjects
1
ANOVA revealed a difference between the scent conditions, F(2, 57) = 4.32, p = .018, η² = .13.
2
Post hoc comparisons, with the raw means reported here, showed that putrescine produced faster
3
reaction times (M = 1.04, SD = .10) than the neutral scent (M = 1.24, SD = .35; p = .013), but not
4
compared to ammonia (M = 1.12, SD = .20; p = .28). No difference was found between the
5
neutral and ammonia conditions (p = .14).
6
7
In sum, only putrescine caused participants to react more quickly compared to the neutral
8
condition, supporting our hypothesis that putrescine increases vigilance. At the same time,
9
ammonia did not increase vigilance relative to the scentless control condition. Importantly, the
10
findings show that, consistent with our pilot study, ammonia and putrescine are evaluated similar
11
on repugnance, familiarity, and intensity, and were similar in the degree of vigilance they
12
elicited. Consequently, together with previous research (Rieser et al., 1976; Wise et al., 2005),
13
Experiment 1 indicated that ammonia would serve as an appropriate aversive control condition.
14
Experiments 2 and 3 investigated our hypothesis that putrescine activates the motivation to
15
escape the situation (flight).
16
17
4. Experiment 2: The effect of putrescine on escape behavior
18
19
Similar to Experiment 1, Experiment 2 first asked participants to rate a scent prime (putrescine
20
vs. ammonia vs. neutral) on three dimensions: intensity, familiarity, and repugnance, then we
21
observed whether it influenced the tendency to escape the situation. To avoid the biases
22
associated with some operationalizations of flight in prior research (e.g., self-reported intentions,
23
Gilbert & Gilbert, 2003), we employed an overt behavioral measure of escape (e.g., Ellsworth et
24
al., 1972; Wisman & Koole, 2003). Specifically, we assessed whether putrescine would cause
25
participants (who were under the impression the study was finished) to walk away more quickly
26
over a predetermined distance of 80 meters.
27
28
4.1. Method
29
30
4.1.1. Participants and Procedure
31
32
Forty-five participants (21 females and 24 males; Mage = 27.51, SD = 9.72) completed the study
33
on campus. We filled three empty felt-tip pens, each with one of the three compounds
34
(putrescine, ammonia, or water). To fill each pen, 10ml of liquid odor was injected onto the
35
pen’s fiber rod inside the pen. The pens were then re-assembled and left to stand upside down for
36
24 hours in order to allow the liquid to soak into the fiber rod. Just before the start of the
37
experiment, scent blotters were marked with the scent marker pens and stored in separate
38
sealable containers.
39
40
Participants were approached on a fixed spot on the campus and asked if they had time to
41
participate in a brief scent test of approximately ten minutes. Participants were tested
42
individually and randomly assigned to one of three conditions (putrescine, ammonia, or water).
43
The experimenter, blind to the conditions, presented one of the three containers to the
44
participant, who rated the scent on intensity (“This scent is strong”; 1 = strongly disagree and 5
45
= strongly agree), repugnance (“This scent is repugnant”; 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly
46
7
agree), and familiarity (“This scent is familiar”; 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree).
1
After finishing and being thanked for their participation, a second experimenter blind to the
2
condition and hypotheses of the experiment and out of sight of the participants— used a standard
3
stopwatch to time how many seconds it took participants to walk away over a distance of 80
4
meters (pre-measured before the experiment began). The recorded time constituted our
5
dependent variable. After they reached this distance, participants were re-approached, fully
6
debriefed and thanked again.
7
8
4.2. Results and Discussion
9
10
4.2.1. Hedonic Evaluations
11
12
Consistent with Experiment 1, separate one-way between-subjects ANOVAs revealed that there
13
was no significant difference between ammonia and putrescine on repugnance, F(1, 28) = 2.30, p
14
= .14, η² = .07, and familiarity, F(1, 28) = 0.04, p = .75, η² = .01. However, ammonia was rated
15
as relatively more intense (M = 4.73; SD = 0.46) compared to putrescine (M = 4.27; SD = 0.70; p
16
= .04; see Table 3). Once again, the results reported below were not altered when we entered the
17
intensity (nor the other hedonic) ratings into the analyses as covariates. We also note that the
18
results are similar whether participants rate how “intense” or “strong” the scent smells (see
19
Experiment 3 below).
20
21
4.2.2. Escape Behavior
22
23
To test our hypothesis that putrescine elicited an escape motivation, we compared our scent
24
conditions in a one-way ANOVA, using gender as a covariate
2
. The results yielded a significant
25
effect of the scent prime on the time it took to walk 80 meters, F(2, 41) = 19.03, p < .001, η² =
26
.48. The only significant differences occurred between putrescine (M = 56.40 seconds; SD =
27
4.19) and ammonia (M = 59.93, SD = 5.04), and between putrescine and the neutral scent prime
28
(M = 60.00, SD = 4.42; both ps < .005; see Figure 1). Thus, putrescine caused participants to
29
walk away more quickly, supporting our assumption that putrescine evoked a stronger
30
motivation to escape. Experiment 3 was conducted to replicate this finding, and furthermore to
31
test whether putrescine elicited implicit cognitions related to escape and threat.
32
33
5. Experiment 3: The effect of putrescine on escape behavior and thoughts
34
35
The procedure for Experiment 3 was similar to Experiment 2’s. First, we asked participants to
36
evaluate the scents on the different dimensions (repugnance, familiarity, intensity). In addition,
37
we gauged participants’ implicit threat-related associations using a word stem-completion task.
38
Specifically, this task measured the implicit accessibility of thoughts related to “escape” and
39
“threat.” We predicted that only putrescine would increase the accessibility of these cognitions.
40
Finally, we assessed whether putrescine would cause participants to walk away more quickly
41
over a predetermined distance of 60 meters.
42
2
Because previous research has shown that men and women tend to walk at different speeds (Chumanov, Wall-
Scheffler, & Heiderscheit, 2008), the results of Experiments 2 and 3 included gender as a covariate. In addition, we
analyzed the results of Experiments 2 and 3 with gender as a separate factor and this did not alter the significance of
the results.
8
1
5.1. Method
2
3
5.1.1. Participants and Procedure
4
5
Sixty participants (32 females and 28 males, Mage = 21.57, SD = 1.12) completed the study on
6
campus. Individuals were approached just outside campus on a path sloping downhill and asked
7
if they had time to participate in a brief scent test for about 15 minutes.
8
9
Participants were randomly assigned to one of the three scent conditions, then they rated
10
the scent on intensity, repugnance, and familiarity (“This scent is intense”; 1 = strongly disagree
11
and 9 = strongly agree), repugnance (“This scent is repugnant”; 1 = strongly disagree and 9 =
12
strongly agree), and familiarity (“This scent is familiar”; 1 = strongly disagree and 9 = strongly
13
agree). Then, to assess cognitions relevant to the concepts of “escape” and “threat,” participants
14
completed the word-stem completion task, a widely used and well-established measurement that
15
gauged the thought accessibility of these two concepts (Arndt et al., 1997; Greenberg et al.,
16
1994; Lozito & Mulligan, 2010; Migo et al., 2010). Participants were asked to complete 30 word
17
fragments, 20 of which were neutral (e.g., B_ NK could be BANK or BUNK) in terms of any
18
particular theme, five of which could be words related to “escape” (e.g., the fragment RU_ could
19
be completed as RUN or RUB, the latter a neutral word), and another five could be completed
20
with a word related to “threat” (e.g., _ _ RROR could be TERROR or MIRROR). We summed
21
the number of escape- (M = 2.73, SD = 1.07) and threat-related words (M = 1.90, SD = .66) that
22
participants completed to assess the thought accessibility of these concepts. Finally, participants
23
were again timed by a second experimenter, who was blind to the conditions and the hypotheses,
24
for how long it took them to walk away over a distance of 60 meters (Due to natural constraints a
25
slightly shorter distance than in Experiment 2).
26
27
5.2. Results and Discussion
28
29
5.2.1. Hedonic Evaluations
30
31
Separate one-way between-subjects ANOVAs revealed no difference between the chemosensory
32
primes on repugnance, F(1, 38) = .35 , p = .56, η² = .01, familiarity, F(1, 38) = .04, p = .85, η² =
33
.001, and intensity, F(1, 38) = 0.29, p =.59, η² = .008 (see Table 4). Thus, participants rated
34
ammonia and putrescine similarly to one another on intensity, repugnance, and familiarity.
35
Again, the results reported below were did not differ when we entered the hedonic evaluations
36
into the analyses as covariates.
37
38
5.2.2. Escape- and Threat-Related Cognitions
39
40
To test our hypothesis that putrescine elicited implicit cognitions related to escape and threat, we
41
analyzed the escape and threat word-completion results separately. The results revealed a
42
significant effect of scent prime on escape thought accessibility, F(2, 57) = 10.90, p < .001, η² =
43
.28 (see Table 5). Putrescine caused participants to complete word stems more frequently with
44
escape related words (M = 3.45, SD = .69) than both the ammonia (M = 2.45, SD = 1.05) and the
45
neutral scent (M = 2.15, SD = .99) primes (both ps < .005). Similarly, the scent primes affected
46
9
the accessibility of threat-related thoughts, F(2, 57) = 8.39, p < .001, η² = .23. Putrescine led to
1
more threat word-stem completions (M = 2.55, SD = .94) than ammonia (M = 1.73, SD = .64)
2
and the neutral scent (M = 1.68, SD = .65; all ps < .005).
3
4
5.2.3. Escape Behavior
5
6
Like Experiment 2, the analyses showed a significant effect of chemosensory primes on walking
7
speed, F(2, 56) = 9.11, p < .001, η² = .24 (see Figure 2). The pattern of results again showed that
8
putrescine (M = 33.38, SD = 2.99) caused people to walk more quickly than ammonia (M =
9
35.92, SD = 3.38) and the neutral scent prime (M = 37.67, SD = 3.13; p < .05). Again, no
10
difference was found between the ammonia and the neutral scent condition (p = .87).
11
12
Experiment 3 revealed that putrescine elicited implicit cognitions of escape and threat. In
13
addition, Experiment 3 replicated the finding that putrescine increased walking speed. Thus,
14
taken together, the results of Experiments 2 and 3 indicated that putrescine motivated (automatic)
15
escape behavior. An important feature of the settings in Experiments 2 and 3 was that
16
participants were outdoors and in a context that facilitated the possibility that they could distance
17
themselves from the scent.
18
19
6. Experiment 4: The effects of putrescine on defensive responses toward an out-group
20
21
Experiment 4 sought to extend our understanding of the effects of putrescine in two important
22
respects. First, we tested the hypothesis that non-conscious (unobtrusive) exposure to putrescine
23
could elicit threat management responses. As we highlighted in the Introduction, this possibility
24
is consistent with evidence that scent primes, even when presented at sub-threshold levels, can
25
influence brain activation (Sobel et al., 1999), learning (Koster et al., 2002), and physiological
26
state (Stern & McClintock, 1998). This applies similarly to aversive scent primes, which for
27
example, have the ability to alter skin conductance (Jacquot et al., 2004), social preferences (Li
28
et al., 2007), and cognitive performance (Epple & Herz, 1999) in ways that correspond to
29
supraliminal exposure to aversive stimuli (Sela & Sobel, 2010). Thus, we predicted that
30
subliminal presentation of putrescine would be capable of activating threat responses.
31
32
Second, Experiment 4 focused on the fight rather than the flight component of alarm
33
responses. Consistent with previous research showing that implicit threat cues increase
34
intolerance toward out-group members (Holbrook et al., 2011) and defensive responses
35
(Blanchard et al., 2001; Wheatley & Haidt, 2005), we hypothesized that putrescine would
36
increase defensiveness toward an out-group member, in a situation where there was no
37
immediate opportunity to escape (Cesario et al., 2010). Like Experiment 1, we conducted this
38
experiment in a laboratory setting. After priming the participants with one of the scents, they
39
filled out a standard positive and negative affect scale that gauged their mood. Although, our
40
pilot study (see Table 1) and some research (e.g., Knasko, 1993) revealed that aversive scent
41
primes do not alter mood on a conscious level, we intended to rule out the possibility that the
42
subliminal primes influenced participants’ feelings at a conscious level. After that, they read
43
about an out-group member—a foreign student who criticized the participants’ value system—
44
and were asked to evaluate the target. This evaluation was designed to assess how much hostility
45
participants felt toward the target.
46
10
1
6.1. Method
2
3
6.1.1. Participants and Procedure
4
5
Sixty-nine participants (39 females and 30 males, Mage = 24.00, SD = 8.38) were run in our lab
6
individually, in different cubicles (randomized) to avoid carryover effects of scents. Furthermore,
7
participants were booked at least 30 minutes apart in order to ventilate the rooms between
8
sessions. Upon arrival, participants were given the first of two questionnaire packets to complete.
9
This first questionnaire consisted of demographic questions and a number of filler items. We
10
then randomly assigned participants to their condition by marking one of the three liquid scents
11
(putrescine, ammonia, water) to the top of each page (0.5 ml) of the second questionnaire
12
participants were given. In the putrescine and ammonia conditions, this amounted to a very
13
subtle scent prime that was not meant to be detected. At the conclusion of the experiment, we
14
funnel debriefed participants to determine whether they noticed or smelled anything unusual
15
during the study. None of them reported being aware of the scents.
16
17
The second questionnaire assessed participants’ mood, and our dependent variables. First,
18
to rule out the possibility that our results could be explained by generalized affect, participants
19
began the second part of the questionnaire by completing the 20-item Positive and Negative
20
Affect Scale (PANAS; Tellegen et al., 1988). This scale measured the extent to which each of 10
21
positive affect descriptors (α = .86) and 10 negative affect descriptors (α = .85) reflected how
22
they felt at that moment (1 = very slightly or not at all, 5 = extremely). We computed the average
23
positive affect (M = 3.31, SD = .68) and negative affect (M = 1.61, SD =.59) scores for
24
everybody.
25
26
This was followed by the description and evaluation of the out-group member (Greenberg
27
et al., 2001; Navarrete et al., 2004; Norenzayan et al., 2007). Specifically, participants read an
28
essay supposedly written by a college student from the Middle East who was visiting the United
29
Kingdom to study English. In this essay, the student went on to criticize Western values,
30
predicting its eventual decline (see Norenzayan et al., 2007). Participants were then asked to
31
evaluate the author and his message by responding to four questions on a 9-point Likert scale
32
(“To what extent do you like the author”; ‘To what extent would you like to be friends with the
33
author”; “How much would you oppose the author teaching your (future) children”; and “How
34
much do you want the ideas of the author to be publicized”; 1 = very much, 9 = not at all). We
35
derived an overall out-group hostility index (M = 5.82, SD = 1.63) by averaging all items
36
together (α = .77), such that larger values indicated greater hostility. Finally, we measured
37
motivation to escape the situation by timing how long it took participants to complete the second
38
(scented) questionnaire followed by a standard demographic questionnaire (91% of the
39
participants were native to England, 3% Greece, 4% Ireland, and 1% to the United States).
40
41
6.2. Results and Discussion
42
43
6.2.1. Ancillary Analyses
44
45
11
A one-way ANOVA tested whether the chemosensory primes elicited different levels of self-
1
reported affect across the three conditions. However, the primes had no impact on positive affect
2
F(2, 66) = 1.87, p >.16, nor negative affect, F(2, 66) = .36, p > .70. Moreover, the analyses
3
below were no different when we used these affect measures as covariates, showing that any
4
effect of our primes on out-group defense was not mediated by mood.
5
6
6.2.2. Out-group Defense
7
8
As predicted, we found a significant effect of scent prime on defensiveness toward the author of
9
the essay, F(2, 66) = 11.83, p < .001, η² = .26 (see Figure 3). Post-hoc analyses found that
10
putrescine led to greater hostility (M = 6.98, SD = 1.42) compared to ammonia (M = 5.05, SD =
11
1.54) and the neutral conditions (M = 5.43, SD = 1.30; both ps < .005). There was no significant
12
difference between the ammonia and control conditions, p > .6.
13
14
Experiment 4 supported the hypothesis that non-conscious exposure to putrescine evoked
15
defensive responses toward an out-group member, and this effect was not due to conscious
16
awareness of the scents, mood, or to the motivation to escape the aversive scent primes
3
.
17
Although these results suggest that the scent primes elicited an odor percept (non-consciously),
18
future studies may wish to control the precise intensities of the stimulus odors that are presented
19
(e.g., using an olfactometer).
20
21
7. General Discussion
22
23
This research was designed to test the hypothesis that putrescine could serve as a warning signal
24
that mobilizes protective responses to deal with threats. In four experiments, we found support
25
for this idea: conscious and non-conscious exposure to putrescine elicited distancing and
26
defensive reactions (e.g., fight and flight responses). Putrescine increased vigilance (Experiment
27
1), heightened the accessibility of escape- and threat-relevant cognitions (Experiment 3), and
28
increased the speed participants walked away from the location of the scent (Experiments 2 and
29
3). Experiment 4 created a situation where immediate escape was not likely and gave participants
30
the opportunity to evaluate an out-group member. Subtle exposure to putrescine produced greater
31
defensiveness toward the out-group member, suggesting an aggressive readiness in participants
32
(Cesario et al., 2010). As a whole, the findings indicate that even brief exposure to putrescine
33
mobilizes threat management responses designed to cope with environmental threats.
34
35
These are the first results to show that a specific chemical compound (putrescine) can be
36
processed as a threat signal. Thus far, nearly all the evidence for threat chemosignals has come
37
from those that are transmitted by body sweat (de Groot et al., 2012; Pause et al., 2012).
38
Moreover, these are among the first studies that show that a specific chemical compound can
39
cause overt behavior in humans (Wysocki & Preti, 2004). Furthermore, an advantage of isolating
40
putrescine in threat management processes is that it may help in determining which sensory and
41
brain pathways are involved in chemosensory threat detection and processing. For instance,
42
research suggests that the central nucleus of the amygdala projects to the midbrain
43
3
When the amount of time participants took to complete the questionnaire was used as a covariate, the results
remained significant, F(2, 65) = 13.13, p < .001, η² = .29).
12
periaqueductal gray, the hypothalamus and the brainstem, which together coordinate to prepare
1
fight-or-flight responses to threatening stimuli (Misslin, 2003). We speculate that putrescine
2
activates a similar neurological pathway. Future research could include physiological
3
measurements (e.g., systolic blood pressure, heart rate) to test the thesis that the observed effects
4
of putrescine are modulated by processes originating in the sympathetic nervous system.
5
6
An important direction for future research will be to understand the precise nature of the
7
threat produced by putrescine (e.g., microbial, predatory). Our view is that putrescine is relevant
8
to both of these domains, though the immediate context should determine which type of threat is
9
more primary. Recent work on trace amine receptors (TAARs) has the potential to shed light on
10
some of these mechanisms, as the activation of different receptors may function to detect specific
11
threats, such as predators and pathogens (Li & Liberles, 2015; Pérez-Gómez et al., 2015). In
12
addition, this research suggests that cadaverine (a compound with a similar chemical structure as
13
putrescine; both are diamines) activates a similar pathway and produces similar escape and
14
avoidance responses (Hussain et al., 2013; Oliveira et al., 2014) in animals. Thus, we render it
15
likely that cadaverine evokes a similar threat response as putrescine (see Li & Liberles, 2015).
16
17
It would also be interesting to examine how putrescine detection affects sensitivity to
18
particular types of threat and whether it produces elevated responses to certain stimuli more than
19
others (e.g., fear- vs. disgust-based sensitivities). For instance, further research could elucidate
20
how putrescine activates sensory acquisition (typically associated with fear experiences) and
21
sensory rejection (associated with disgust) processes (Susskind et al., 2008), and whether
22
exposure to putrescine augments physiological responses (e.g., heart rate, pupil dilation) that
23
typically co-occur with adaptive responses to threats. This type of research would benefit from
24
including individual differences in both disgust and fear sensitivity (Garfinkel et al., 2014; Haidt
25
et al., 1994). By the same token, future work could clarify whether putrescine elicits discrete
26
emotions (e.g., fear versus disgust) or less specific affective states associated with negative
27
valence and high arousal (see also Li & Liberles, 2015; Smeets & Dijksterhuis, 2014). Our
28
findings, which showed that responses to putrescine were automatic, occurred after various
29
lengths of delay (Experiments 1-3), and when presented at sub-threshold levels (Experiment 4),
30
suggested that conscious evaluations are not at the heart of the observed responses to putrescine.
31
This is consistent with our theorizing and ample work showing that chemosensory cues influence
32
psychological and physiological operations outside conscious awareness (for extended reviews,
33
see Sela & Sobel, 2010; Smeets & Dijksterhuis, 2014). However, we hasten to add that more
34
research is needed to specify the exact nature of the effects produced by the sub-threshold
35
priming of putrescine, for instance, by varying the exposure times to putrescine, the delay after
36
the primes, and the intensity of the putrescine stimulus.
37
38
Another important question is how specific threat management responses develop.
39
Within non-olfactory sensory channels, for example, there may be an innate bias for humans to
40
detect certain biologically-relevant stimuli as threatening, such as the sight of snakes and spiders
41
(Ohman & Mineka, 2001). Although controversial in human research, some work suggests that
42
responses to chemosensory stimuli are innate (Hussain et al., 2013; Misslin, 2003; Dielenberg et
43
al., 2001). For instance, Soussignan et al. (1997) showed that soon after birth, butyric acid (a
44
malodorous scent) evoked disgust reactions in neonates, a finding they claim is consistent with
45
an innate predisposition toward ecologically-relevant scents. To test for possibility of innate
46
13
biases toward threatening chemosensory cues, it would be interesting to examine whether
1
putrescine triggers facial expressions associated with fear in infants. In fact, research indicates
2
that adults do not habituate so readily to the scent of putrescine emitted from rotting flesh
3
(Roberson et al., 2008), suggesting that there might be a bias to respond warily to it.
4
5
Although the innateness of responses to chemosignals is still controversial, humans’
6
ability to incorporate learned information into cultural practices is beyond question (Boyd &
7
Richerson, 2005). Consequently, the magnitude of specific chemosensory threat responses could
8
be different in cultures where people are exposed to putrescine more frequently. Likewise,
9
reactions to putrescine may differ between cultures with different burial practices (e.g.,
10
embalming practices, the duration before burial). These factors should remind us that the context
11
is critical to how people react to putrescine. How olfactory information modulates other sensory
12
inputs (Zhou et al., 2012) is no doubt central to whether it will be interpreted as threatening.
13
14
One alternative theoretical perspective of our findings on the effects of putrescine is
15
Terror Management Theory (TMT; Greenberg et al., 1994). According to this theory, reminders
16
of death are regulated by a “cultural anxiety buffer” that consists of beliefs and values that imbue
17
life with meaning and the promise of immortality. Interestingly, TMT argues that a great deal of
18
the darker side of human behavior (e.g., aggression, out-group prejudice, religious intolerance)
19
stems from the need to maintain and defend the integrity of this cultural anxiety buffer, due to its
20
vital role in managing existential angst. In this view, putrescine could function as a reminder of
21
mortality, and subsequently elicits similar defensive processes, as activated by reminders of
22
death. We do not rule out this possibility, but render it unlikely that chemosensory threats trigger
23
the same type of processes as those that originate from the unique human ability to reflect on the
24
conundrum of life and death (Landau et al., 2007). Nevertheless, examining whether putrescine
25
can be used as a subtle reminder of death, and whether it influences cultural beliefs, values, and
26
practices, would open up fascinating directions of research.
27
28
Most research has shown that humans process threats either visually or audibly, while
29
other animals inhabit the inaccessible world of scents. At the same time, we know that humans
30
are guided by many of the same olfactory processes, especially when they involve fitness-
31
relevant information. We believe that by identifying putrescine as one of these signals, a further
32
understanding of its mechanisms can shed light on more general processes that modulate
33
chemosensory signaling and threat management responses.
34
35
36
14
8. Conflict of Interest Statement
1
2
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or
3
financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
4
5
6
15
9. References
1
2
Ackerl, K., Atzmueller, M., & Grammer, K. (2002). The scent of fear.
3
Neuroendocrinology Letters, 23, 79-84.
4
Arakawa, H., Arakawa, K., & Deak, T. (2010). Sickness-related odor communication
5
signals as determinants of social behavior in rat: a role for inflammatory processes.
6
Hormones and Behavior, 57, 330-341.
7
Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1997). Subliminal exposure to
8
death-related stimuli increases defense of the cultural worldview. Psychological Science,
9
8, 379-385.
10
Blanchard, R. J., Flannelly, K. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1986). Defensive behaviors of
11
laboratory and wild Rattus norvegicus. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 101-
12
107.
13
Blanchard, C. D., Hynd, A. L., Minke, K. A., Minemoto, T., & Blanchard, R. J. (2001).
14
Human defensive behaviors to threat scenarios show parallels to fear-and anxiety-related
15
defense patterns of non-human mammals. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 25,
16
761-770.
17
Boissy, A., Terlouw, C., & Le Neindre, P. (1998). Presence of cues from stressed
18
conspecifics increases reactivity to aversive events in cattle: evidence for the existence of
19
alarm substances in urine. Physiology and Behavior, 63, 489-495.
20
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford University
21
Press, USA.
22
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I:
23
defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298.
24
Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an
25
alternative theory. The American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106-124.
26
Cesario, J., Plaks, J. E., Hagiwara, N., Navarrete, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (2010). The Ecology of
27
Automaticity How Situational Contingencies Shape Action Semantics and Social
28
Behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 1311-1317.
29
Chen, D., & Haviland-Jones, J. (2000). Human olfactory communication of emotion.
30
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 771-781.
31
Chumanov, E. S., Wall-Scheffler, C., & Heiderscheit, B. C. (2008). Gender differences in
32
walking and running on level and inclined surfaces. Clinical Biomechanics, 23, 1260-
33
1268.
34
Degel, J., Piper, D., & Köster, E. P. (2001). Implicit learning and implicit memory for
35
odors: the influence of odor identification and retention time. Chemical Senses, 26, 267-
36
280.
37
de Groot, J. H., Smeets, M. A., Kaldewaij, A., Duijndam, M. J., & Semin, G. R. (2012).
38
Chemosignals communicate human emotions. Psychological Science, 23, 1417-1424.
39
Dielenberg, R. A., Hunt, G. E., & McGregor, I. S. (2001). ‘When a rat smells a cat’: The
40
distribution of Fos immunoreactivity in rat brain following exposure to a predatory odor.
41
Neuroscience, 104, 1085-1097.
42
Dinges, D. F., Orne, M. T., Whitehouse, W. G., & Orne, E. C. (1987). Temporal placement of a
43
nap for alertness: contributions of circadian phase and prior wakefulness. Sleep, 10(4),
44
313-329.
45
Dinges, D. F., & Powell, J. W. (1985). Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable,
46
16
simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behavior Research Methods,
1
Instruments, & Computers, 17(6), 652-655.
2
Dorrian, J., Sweeney, M., & Dawson, D. (2004). A prior sleep/wake model of fatigue-related
3
accident risk in truck drivers. Journal of Sleep Research, 13, A194.
4
Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M., & Henson, A. (1972). The stare as a stimulus to flight in
5
human subjects: a series of field experiments. Journal of Personality and Social
6
Psychology, 21(3), 302.
7
Epple, G., & Herz, R. S. (1999). Ambient odors associated to failure influence cognitive
8
performance in children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35(2), 103-107.
9
Garfinkel, S. N., Minati, L., Gray, M. A., Seth, A. K., Dolan, R. J., & Critchley, H. D. (2014).
10
Fear from the heart: sensitivity to fear stimuli depends on individual heartbeats. The
11
Journal of Neuroscience, 34(19), 6573-6582.
12
Gilbert, P., & Gilbert, J. (2003). Entrapment and arrested fight and flight in depression: An
13
exploration using focus groups. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and
14
Practice, 76(2), 173-188.
15
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2003). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the
16
function of the septo-hippocampal system (No. 33). Oxford University Press.
17
Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Schimel, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (2001). Clarifying the
18
function of mortality salience-induced worldview defense: Renewed suppression or
19
reduced accessibility of death-related thoughts?. Journal of Experimental Social
20
Psychology, 37(1), 70-76.
21
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Simon, L., & Breus, M. (1994). Role of
22
consciousness and accessibility of death-related thoughts in mortality salience
23
effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(4), 627.
24
Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A
25
scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual
26
Differences, 16(5), 701-713.
27
Holbrook, C., Sousa, P., & Hahn-Holbrook, J. (2011). Unconscious vigilance: Worldview
28
defense without adaptations for terror, coalition, or uncertainty management. Journal of
29
Personality and Social Psychology, 101(3), 451.
30
Horowitz, L. F., Saraiva, L. R., Kuang, D., Yoon, K. H., & Buck, L. B. (2014). Olfactory
31
receptor patterning in a higher primate. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 12241-12252.
32
Hummel, T, & Kobal, G (1992). Differences in human evoked potentials related to
33
olfactory or trigeminal chemosensory activation. Electroencephalogr. Clin.
34
Neurophysiol., 84, 84-89.
35
Hussain A., Saraiva, L. R, Ferrero, D. M., Ahuja, G., Krishna, V. S., Liberles, S. D., &
36
Korsching, S. I. (2013). High-affinity olfactory receptor for the death associated odor
37
cadaverine. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 48, 19579-19584.
38
Jacquot, L., Monnin, J., & Brand, G. (2004). Unconscious odor detection could not be due to
39
odor itself. Brain Research, 1002(1), 51-54.
40
Knasko, S. C. (1993). Performance, mood, and health during exposure to intermittent
41
odors. Archives of Environmental Health: An International Journal, 48(5), 305-308.
42
Koster, E. P., Degel, J., & Piper, D. (2002). Proactive and retroactive interference in implicit
43
odor memory. Chemical Senses, 27, 191–207.
44
Krusemark, E. A., & Li, W. (2012). Enhanced olfactory sensory perception of threat in anxiety:
45
an event-related fMRI study. Chemosensory Perception, 5(1), 37-45.
46
17
Landau, M. J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (2007). On the compatibility of
1
terror management theory and perspectives on human evolution. Evolutionary
2
Psychology, 5, 476-519.
3
Lee, S. W., & Schwarz, N. (2012). Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical
4
effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and
5
Social Psychology, 103(5), 737-749.
6
Li, Q., & Liberles, S. D. (2015). Aversion and attraction through olfaction. Current Biology, 25,
7
R120-R129.
8
Li, W., Moallem, I., Paller, K. A., & Gottfried, J. A. (2007). Subliminal smells can guide social
9
preferences. Psychological Science, 18(12), 1044-1049.
10
Loh, S., Lamond, N., Dorrian, J., Roach, G., & Dawson, D. (2004). The validity of psychomotor
11
vigilance tasks of less than 10-minute duration. Behavior Research Methods, 36(2), 339-
12
346.
13
Lozito, J. P., & Mulligan, N. W. (2010). Exploring the role of attention during implicit memory
14
retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 387-399.
15
Migo, E. M., Roper, A., Montaldi, D., & Mayes, A. R. (2010). British English norms for the
16
spontaneous completion of three-letter word stems. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 470-
17
473.
18
Mobbs, D., Marchant, J. L., Hassabis, D., Seymour, B., Tan, G., Gray, M., et al. (2009). From
19
threat to fear: the neural organization of defensive fear systems in humans. The Journal
20
of Neuroscience, 29, 12236-12243.
21
Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2010). Scent of a woman: men’s testosterone responses to
22
olfactory ovulation cues. Psychological Science, 21, 276-283.
23
Millot, J. L., Brand, G., & Morand, N. (2002). Effects of ambient odors on reaction time in
24
humans. Neuroscience Letters, 322(2), 79-82.
25
Misslin, R. (2003). The defense system of fear: Behavior and neurocircuitry. Clinical
26
Neurophysiology, 33, 55-66.
27
Mujica-Parodi, L. R., Strey, H. H., Frederick, B., Savoy, R., Cox, D., Botanov, Y.,
28
Tolkunov, D., Rubin, D., & Weber, J. (2009). Chemosensory cues to conspecific
29
emotional stress activate amygdala in humans. PLoS One, 4, e6415.
30
Navarrete, C. D., Kurzban, R., Fessler, D. M., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2004). Anxiety and
31
intergroup bias: Terror management or coalitional psychology?. Group Processes &
32
Intergroup Relations, 7(4), 370-397.
33
Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2011). Human threat management systems: Self-
34
protection and disease avoidance. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1042-
35
1051.
36
Norenzayan, A., Dar‐Nimrod, I., Hansen, I. G., & Proulx, T. (2007). Mortality salience
37
and religion: Divergent effects on the defense of cultural worldviews for the religious and
38
the non‐religious. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 101-113.
39
Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module
40
of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483-522.
41
Olsson, M. J., Lundström, J. N., Kimball, B. A., Gordon, A. R., Karshikoff, B., Hosseini, N., et
42
al. (2014). The scent of disease: Human body odor contains an early chemosensory cue of
43
sickness. Psychological Science, 25, 817–823.
44
Pause, B. M. (2012). Processing of body odor signals by the human brain. Chemosensory
45
Perception, 5, 55-63.
46
18
Pause, B. M., Adolph, D., Prehn-Kristensen, A., & Ferstl, R. (2009). Startle response
1
potentiation to chemosensory anxiety signals in socially anxious individuals.
2
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74, 88-92.
3
Pérez-Gómez, A., Bleymehl, K., Stein, B., Pyrski, M., Birnbaumer, L., Munger, S. D., et al.
4
(2015). Innate predator odor aversion driven by parallel olfactory subsystems that
5
converge in the ventromedial hypothalamus. Current Biology, 25, 1-7.
6
Prehn, A., Ohrt, A., Sojka, B., Ferstl, R., & Pause, B. M. (2006). Chemosensory anxiety signals
7
augment the startle reflex in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 394, 127-130.
8
Prounis, G. S., & Shields, W. M. (2013). Necrophobic behavior in small mammals. Behavioural
9
Processes, 94, 41-44.
10
Rieser, J., Yonas, A., & Wikner, K. (1976). Radial localization of odors by human newborns.
11
Child Development, 47, 856-859.
12
Roberson, D. W., Neil, J. A., & Bryant, E. T. (2008). Improving wound care simulation with the
13
addition of odor: A descriptive, quasi-experimental study. OstomyWound Management,
14
54, 36-43.
15
Sekizawa, S. I., & Tsubone, H. (1994). Nasal receptors responding to noxious chemical irritants.
16
Respiration Physiology, 96(1), 37-48.
17
Sela, L., & Sobel, N. (2010). Human olfaction: A constant state of change-blindness.
18
Experimental Brain Research, 205(1), 13-29.
19
Smeets, M. A. M., & Dijksterhuis, G. B. (2014). Smelly primes–when olfactory primes do or do
20
not work. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 96.
21
Sobel, N., Prabhakaran, V., Hartley, C. A., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., Sullivan, E. V., &
22
Gabrieli, J. D. (1999). Blind smell: brain activation induced by an undetected air-borne
23
chemical. Brain, 122(2), 209-217.
24
Soussignan, R., Schaal, B., Marlier, L., & Jiang, T. (1997). Facial and autonomic responses to
25
biological and artificial olfactory stimuli in human neonates: Re-examining early hedonic
26
discrimination of odors. Physiology & Behavior, 62, 745-758.
27
Stern, K., & McClintock, M. K. (1998). Regulation of ovulation by human pheromones. Nature,
28
392, 177–179.
29
Stevenson, R. J. (2010). An initial evaluation of the functions of human olfaction. Chemical
30
Senses, 35, 3-20.
31
Stevenson, R. J., Boakes, R. A., & Prescott, J. (1998). Changes in odor sweetness
32
resulting from implicit learning of a simultaneous odor-sweetness association: An
33
example of learned synesthesia. Learning and Motivation, 29, 113-132.
34
Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., & Anderson, A. K. (2008).
35
Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nature Neuroscience, 11(7), 843-850.
36
Tellegen, A., Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures
37
of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social
38
Psychology, 54, 1063-1070.
39
Tybur, J. M., Bryan, A. D., Magnan, R. E., & Hooper, A. E. C. (2011). Smells like safe sex:
40
Olfactory pathogen primes increase intentions to use condoms. Psychological
41
Science, 22, 478-480.
42
Wise, P. M., Canty, T. M., & Wysocki, C. J. (2005). Temporal integration of nasal irritation from
43
ammonia at threshold and supra-threshold levels. Toxicological Sciences, 87(1), 223-231.
44
Wisman, A., & Koole, S. L. (2003). Hiding in the crowd: can mortality salience promote
45
19
affiliation with others who oppose one's worldviews?. Journal of Personality and Social
1
Psychology, 84(3), 511-526.
2
Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe.
3
Psychological Science, 16, 780-784.
4
Woody, E. Z., & Szechtman, H. (2011). Adaptation to potential threat: the evolution,
5
neurobiology, and psychopathology of the security motivation system. Neuroscience &
6
Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(4), 1019-1033.
7
Wyatt, T. D. (2009). Fifty years of pheromones. Nature, 457, 262-263.
8
Wysocki, C. J., & Preti, G. (2004). Facts, fallacies, fears, and frustrations with human
9
pheromones. The Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular, Cellular, and
10
Evolutionary Biology, 281, 1201-1211.
11
Yao, M., Rosenfeld, J., Attridge, S., Sidhu, S., Aksenov, V., & Rollo, C. D. (2009). The ancient
12
chemistry of avoiding risks of predation and disease. Evolutionary Biology, 36, 267-281.
13
Yeoman, C. J., Thomas, S. M., Miller, M. E., Ulanov, A. V., Torralba, M., Lucas, S., et al.
14
(2013). A multi-omic systems-based approach reveals metabolic markers of bacterial
15
vaginosis and insight into the disease. PloS One, 8(2), e56111.
16
Yoon, K. H., Ragoczy, T., Lu, Z., Kondoh, K., Kuang, D., Groudine, M., & Buck, L. B. (2015).
17
Olfactory receptor genes expressed in distinct lineages are sequestered in different
18
nuclear compartments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, E2403-
19
E2409.
20
Zhou, W., & Chen, D. (2009). Fear-related chemosignals modulate recognition of fear in
21
ambiguous facial expressions. Psychological Science, 20, 177-183.
22
Zhou, W., Zhang, X., Chen, J., Wang, L., & Chen, D. (2012). Nostril-specific olfactory
23
modulation of visual perception in binocular rivalry. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32,
24
17225-17229.
25
26
27
20
Table 1
1
Hedonic evaluations of putrescine, ammonia, indole, “fart spray,” and skatole1 (Pilot study).
2
3
Scent primes Putrescine Ammonia Indole Skatole Fart spray
4
5
Intensity2
6
M 5.98b 6.60b 5.25a 7.23c 5.52b
7
SD 2.50 2.46 2.15 2.08 2.07
8
Familiarity
9
M 4.98a 5.10a 6.88b 5.21a 4.90a
10
SD 2.71 2.95 2.46 2.56 2.69
11
Repugnance
12
M 5.94b 5.94b 3.65a 6.54b 5.31b
13
SD 2.65 2.55 1.78 2.94 2.63
14
Positivity
15
M 2.63b 2.69b 3.81a 2.50b 2.67b
16
SD 1.55 1.78 2.05 1.87 1.77
17
N 48 48 48 48 48
18
19
20
1 “How intense is this scent?”, 1 = Not at all and 10 = Very much; “How familiar is this
21
scent?”, 1 = Not at all and 10 = Very much; “How repugnant is this scent?”, 1 = Not at all and 10
22
= Very much; “How positive does this scent make you feel?”, 1 = Not at all and 10 = Very much.
23
2 Different subscripts on a hedonic dimension (within a row) indicate a significant
24
difference of p < .05.
25
26
27
21
Table 2
1
Scent ratings for the chemosensory primes (Experiment 1)
2
3
Chemosensory primes Neutral Ammonia Putrescine
4
5
Intensity
6
M 3.30 4.73 4.27
7
SD 1.81 1.45 1.92
8
9
Familiarity
10
M 6.00 5.10 4.40
11
SD .86 2.25 1.60
12
13
Repugnance
14
M 2.35 5.90 5.65
15
SD 1.46 1.34 1.23
16
N 20 20 20
17
18
22
Table 3
1
Scent ratings for the chemosensory primes (Experiment 2)
2
3
Chemosensory primes Neutral Ammonia Putrescine
4
5
Intensity
6
M 1.53 4.73 4.27
7
SD .64 .46 .70
8
9
Familiarity
10
M 4.75 1.60 1.67
11
SD .46 .51 .62
12
13
Repugnance
14
M 1.73 4.47 4.80
15
SD .70 .74 .41
16
N 15 15 15
17
18
23
Table 4
1
Scent ratings for the chemosensory primes (Experiment 3)
2
3
Chemosensory primes Neutral Ammonia Putrescine
4
5
Intensity
6
M 1.85 3.20 3.40
7
SD .99 1.32 .99
8
9
Familiarity
10
M 2.95 2.20 2.15
11
SD .83 .89 .75
12
13
Repugnance
14
M 2.60 3.70 3.50
15
SD .60 .98 1.15
16
N 20 20 20
17
18
19
24
Table 5
1
The ratings of escape-related and threat-related cognitions for the chemosensory primes
2
(Experiment 3).
3
4
Chemosensory primes Neutral Ammonia Putrescine
5
6
Escape cognitions
7
M 2.15 2.45 3.45
8
SD 0.99 1.05 0.69
9
10
Threat cognitions
11
M 1.68 1.73 2.55
12
SD 0.65 0.64 0.94
13
N 20 20 20
14
15
25
1
2
3
Figure 1. The number of seconds it took participants to walk 80 meters after exposure to the
4
scent prime (Experiment 2). Asterisks denote that two groups differ at **p < .005.
5
6
7
8
**
**
50
55
60
65
neutral ammonia putrescine
Walking time in seconds
26
1
2
3
4
Figure 2. The number of seconds it took participants to walk 60 meters after exposure to the
5
scent prime (Experiment 3). Asterisks denote that two groups differ at *p < .05.
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
*
*
25
30
35
40
neutral ammonia putrescine
walking time in seconds
27
1
2
3
Figure 3. Mean scores on the worldview defense scale for all three conditions (Experiment 4).
4
Higher scores reflect greater hostility toward the target. Asterisks denote two groups differ at **p
5
< .005.
6
7
8
9
10
11
**
**
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
neutral ammonia putrescine
outgroup defense