ArticlePDF Available

A dual function of white coloration in a nocturnal spider Dolomedes raptor (Araneae: Pisauridae)

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Nocturnal animals frequently possess seemingly conspicuous colour patterns that can function in a variety of ways (e.g. prey attraction, camouflage, predator avoidance, etc.). The use of colour patterns in intraspecific signalling, especially reproductive activities, in nocturnal animals has received relatively little attention. This study tested for a dual function of colour in the nocturnal fishing spider, Dolomedes raptor (Araneae: Pisauridae), whose males develop dimorphic white stripes at sexual maturation. We tested for a role in foraging as well as mate assessment. First, quantifications of the natural variation of male stripes indicated a correlation between stripe area and male body size and weight. Subsequent diet experiments confirmed that the area of a male's white stripes are body size-dependent and thus could potentially convey information to choosey females about male quality. Field experiments used dummies resembling D. raptor in appearance to test a prey attraction function of the white stripes. We found that dummies with the white stripes present attracted significantly more prey than those without stripes. Finally, we used males with manipulated phenotypes in laboratory mating trials and found that males with intact white stripes were significantly more likely to be accepted by females than those with the white stripes eliminated. Together, our results document a nutrient-dependent trait that functions not only in strengthening foraging success, but also in a mating context, increasing male mating success. We suggest that the role of these male white stripes in reproduction has been facilitated by their function in foraging.
Content may be subject to copyright.
A dual function of white coloration in a nocturnal spider Dolomedes
raptor (Araneae: Pisauridae)
Tai-Shen Lin
a
, Shichang Zhang
a
, Chen-Pan Liao
a
, Eileen A. Hebets
b
, I-Min Tso
a
,
c
,
*
a
Department of Life Science, Tunghai University, Taichung, Taiwan
b
School of Biological Sciences, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE, U.S.A.
c
Center for Tropical Ecology &Biodiversity, Tunghai University, Taichung, Taiwan
article info
Article history:
Received 5 February 2015
Initial acceptance 5 March 2015
Final acceptance 8 June 2015
Published online
MS. number: 15-00096R
Keywords:
Dolomedes raptor
nocturnal animal
sexual selection
visual signal
wandering spider
Nocturnal animals frequently possess seemingly conspicuous colour patterns that can function in a va-
riety of ways (e.g. prey attraction, camouage, predator avoidance, etc.). The use of colour patterns in
intraspecic signalling, especially reproductive activities, in nocturnal animals has received relatively
little attention. This study tested for a dual function of colour in the nocturnal shing spider, Dolomedes
raptor (Araneae: Pisauridae), whose males develop dimorphic white stripes at sexual maturation. We
tested for a role in foraging as well as mate assessment. First, quantications of the natural variation of
male stripes indicated a correlation between stripe area and male body size and weight. Subsequent diet
experiments conrmed that the area of a male's white stripes are body size-dependent and thus could
potentially convey information to choosey females about male quality. Field experiments used dummies
resembling D. raptor in appearance to test a prey attraction function of the white stripes. We found that
dummies with the white stripes present attracted signicantly more prey than those without stripes.
Finally, we used males with manipulated phenotypes in laboratory mating trials and found that males
with intact white stripes were signicantly more likely to be accepted by females than those with the
white stripes eliminated. Together, our results document a nutrient-dependent trait that functions not
only in strengthening foraging success, but also in a mating context, increasing male mating success. We
suggest that the role of these male white stripes in reproduction has been facilitated by their function in
foraging.
©2015 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
The elaborate coloration and intricate visual patterns found in
many animal groups have inspired artists and intrigued scientists
for centuries (Andersson, 1994). While aesthetically often utterly
captivating, these colours and patterns can have very distinct
functions. For example, aposematic coloration in some animals can
be used to warn off predators (Stevens &Ruxton, 2012). The eye
spotsin the wings of some caterpillars can startle or intimidate
predators (Kodandaramaiah, 2011; Stevens, 2005)ordeect attacks
by predators to less vulnerable body parts (Prudic, Stoehr, Wasik, &
Monteiro, 2015). Another example is aggressive mimicry, in which
the colour pattern closely mimics certain visual signals that can be
used by some animals to lure prey (O'Hanlon, Holwell, &
Herberstein, 2014; Pietsch &Grobecker, 1978). Alternatively, elab-
orate visual components can function in reproduction, such as for
attracting mates (G
omez et al., 2009; Lim, Land, &Li, 2007). The
visual signals used in mate attraction are often sexually dimorphic,
with the most striking of these secondary sexual traits typically
observed in diurnally active males (Maynard Smith &Harper,
2004).
While not as immediately conspicuous or obvious to us, due, in
part, to our diurnal nature, nocturnal animals also possess colora-
tion and/or other visually conspicuous traits (G
omez et al., 2009;
Penteriani, del Mar Delgado, Alonso-Alvarez, &Sergio, 2007). For
example, many nocturnal animals, or those living in consistently
dark environments, produce and emit bioluminescence (Broadley
&Stringer, 2001; Lloyd, 1971). Bioluminescence, a phenomenon
in which animals produce light by themselves rather than reecting
light from external sources, is taxonomically widespread, as it is
seen in numerous marine vertebrates and invertebrates, fungi,
some bacteria and reies (Widder, 2010; Widder, Latz, Herring, &
Case, 1984). The many functions of bioluminescence include
mimicry, camouage, distraction, warning and even attracting
mates (Haddock, Moline, &Case, 2010; Lloyd, 1971; Widder, 2010).
In addition to bioluminescence, recent studies have shown that
*Correspondence: I-Min Tso, Department of Life Science, Tunghai University,
Taichung 40704, Taichung, Taiwan.
E-mail address: spider@thu.edu.tw (I. -M. Tso).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Animal Behaviour
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.07.001
0003-3472/©2015 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Animal Behaviour 108 (2015) 25e32
nocturnal animals may use colour signals in activities such as
claiming advantage in maleemale combat (e.g. eagle owl, Bubo
bubo,Penteriani et al., 2007; frog, Phyllomedusa boliviana,Jansen &
K
ohler, 2008), defence (e.g. glow-worm, Lampyris noctiluca,De
Cock &Matthysen, 2003;Motyxia millipedes, Marek, Papaj,
Yeager, Molina, &Moore, 2011), prey attraction (e.g. glow-worm,
Arachnocampa luminosa,Broadley &Stringer, 2001) and predator
avoidance (e.g. leiognathid sh, McFall-Ngai &Morin, 1991).
However, colour signals used in the context of reproduction of
nocturnal organisms have not been widely reported (G
omez et al.,
2009; Lewis &Cratsley, 2008). Recently, it was reported that in the
Neotropical spider Paratrechalea ornata (Trechaleidae) males were
more attractive to females when the chelicerae of the males were
painted white, implying that conspicuous visual signals can be
detected and may also play roles in the mating of nocturnal ar-
thropods (Trillo, Melo-Gonz
alez, &Albo, 2014).
Perhaps surprising to some, spiders are among the many taxo-
nomic groups that possess extraordinary sexual dimorphism, with
males of some species exhibiting colourful bodies and leg orna-
ments in addition to elaborate dynamic motion displays (Framenau
&Hebets, 2007; Hebets &Uetz, 2000; Uhl &Elias, 2011). Salticids
(jumping spiders), for example, can use sex-specic visually
mediated displays to court females (Li et al., 2008; Lim et al., 2007;
Lim, Li, &Li, 2008). Members of the diurnally active and diverse
genus Habronattus (ca. 100 species) are well known for their elab-
orate male secondary sexual traits which include spectacular colour
patterns, morphological ornamentsand complex courtship dis-
plays (Elias, Maddison, Peckmezian, Girard, &Mason, 2012; Girard,
Kasumovic, &Elias, 2011). It has been known in sexually dimorphic
animals that diet is closely related to the attractiveness of the
sexual traits that are preferred by females when choosing a mate
(Ferkin, Sorokin, Johnston, &Lee, 1997; Hill, 1992; McGlothlin,
Duffy, Henry-Freeman, &Ketterson, 2007). Related studies in spi-
ders, however, have rarely been reported. Hebets, Wesson, and
Shamble (2008) found in Schizocosa wolf spiders that males
receiving a high-quality diet matured more quickly and were
signicantly larger as adults than those receiving a low-quality diet,
and high-quality diet males had larger sexual traits and were more
successful in courting females than low-quality diet males. Yet, it is
unknown whether, in nocturnal organisms, diet would inuence
physical condition and potential sexual traits.
The conspicuous body coloration of spiders not only plays roles
in sexual selection, but also functions in foraging, for instance as a
prey lure. For example, the brightly coloured body parts of the giant
wood spider, Nephila pilipes, have been shown to function as a vi-
sual lure for attracting both diurnal and nocturnal prey (Chuang,
Yang, &Tso, 2007). The conspicuous yellow ventral spots in the
garden spider, Neoscona punctigera, can also lure prey at night
(Blamires et al., 2012). Aside from these examples, the role of
conspicuous coloration in luring prey at night has not been well
studied or known in nocturnal cursorial arthropods.
The shing spider, Dolomedes raptor (Araneae: Pisauridae), like
most members of its genus, is a nocturnal wandering species
inhabiting low-altitude streams. These nocturnal predators prey on
aquatic and semiaquatic invertebrates and vertebrates (Bleckmann
&Lotz, 1987; Zimmermann &Spence, 1989). The species exhibits
dramatic sexual colour dimorphism, with mature males displaying
two white stripes at the two margins of the cephalothorax (Fig. 1a).
Females lack these white stripes, but they do possess white spots at
the tips of their legs, especially the anterior two pairs (Fig. 1b). The
presence of white coloration in both sexes suggests a potential
function in prey attraction, while the distinct placement of color-
ation in males in combination with their courtship advances hints
towards a role in courtship communication. Here, we hypothesized
that the bright white patch on the body of male D. raptor plays
important roles in both foraging (i.e. prey attraction) and sexual
selection (i.e. a sexual character in female mate assessment). To test
these two hypotheses, we rst quantied the natural variation in
colour patterns of males that were collected from the eld. We next
conducted diet experiments in the laboratory to test whether the
colour pattern was nutrition dependent and thus potentially
capable of conveying information about a male's past foraging
history. Then, we conducted eld observations using different
dummies that resembled male D. raptor in size and colour. Finally,
we conducted mating trials using manipulated males. We predicted
that dummies with the colour pattern would attract more prey and
males with the colour pattern would be more successful in mating.
METHODS
Natural Variation in Male Coloration
The white stripes on the males are composed of tiny white hairs.
To determine the natural variation in the area of these white
stripes, we collected males (N¼32) at low-altitude streams near
Dongshi Forest Recreation Area in Dongshi, Taichung city, Taiwan
(120
52
0
03.96
0
E, 24
17
0
06.78
0
N) and brought them back to the
laboratory in Tunghai University. To measure the area of white
stripes on the carapace, we rst anaesthetized the spiders with
carbon dioxide and then used a digital camera (Olympus
m
1030
SW) to take images of the spiders. We used GIMP2 software (www.
gimp.org) to measure the foreleg length, carapace length, carapace
width and the area of the white stripe on the cephalothorax. The
body weights were also measured. We then calculated the standard
scores (i.e. [variable emean variable]/standard deviation of vari-
able) for these variables. To check the relationship between white
stripe area and body condition (i.e. body size and weight), we rst
applied a principal component analysis (PCA) based on the stan-
dard scores of four body characteristics (carapace length, carapace
Figure 1. (a) Male and (b) female Dolomedes raptor showing their dimorphic body
coloration patterns.
T.-S. Lin et al. / Animal Behaviour 108 (2015) 25e3226
width, foreleg length and spider weight). Then, we calculated the
score of body size and weightby extracting the rst principal
components of the PCA analysis. Finally, we tted the white stripe
area with the body size and weight scoreusing a simple linear
regression. Using the same method, we also calculated the score of
body sizeusing variables such as carapace length, carapace width
and foreleg length. We then tted the white stripe area with body
size scoreusing a quadratic regression to calculate the residuals.
Finally, we tted the white stripe area with these residuals by using
a simple linear regression.
To investigate whether the brightness of the white stripe
correlated with spider body size and weight, we rst measured the
reectance spectra of 32 mature males using the following
equation:
white stripe brightness ¼Z
700 nm
300 nm
Sdl;
where Sis the reectance spectra (%) of the white stripe and
l
is
wavelength (nm). We tted the white stripe brightness with body
size and weight scoreas well as residual of spider weight against
body size scoreby using simple linear regressions. The normality of
the residuals of the linear regression was checked by the Shapir-
oeWilk normality test, while the homoscedasticity of the variance
of the residuals was checked by the White's test.
Nutrition Dependence of Male Coloration
Sexual traits are often closely associated with body condition of
mate-searching individuals (Andersson,1994) and our aim here was
to determine whether the white stripes on male D. raptor were
nutrition dependent. To achieve this, subadult males weresubjected
to different levels of nutrient intake to see whether such treatments
would affect the area of white stripes when the spiders matured. We
collected juvenile males from the study site in Dongshi, Taichung
city, Taiwan and raised them individually to the subadult stage by
giving them one cricket (Acheta bimaculata; body length ca. 10 mm)
once a week. Before the nutrition manipulation, we measured the
body weight and white stripe area of subadults and randomly
assigned them to high-nutrition (fed one cricket every 2 days;
N¼19) and low-nutrition (fed one cricket every 7 days; N¼12)
treatments. When the spiders matured, we measured the body
weight, white stripe area and number of days taken to reach
maturity. The white stripe area of males in the two treatment groups
was compared by a general linear model in which the nutrition
treatment, spiders' pretreatment white stripe area and the interac-
tion term were considered. The homogeneity assumption of the
model was checked by a Bartlett's test, and we found that the vari-
ances among the four treatments were not (but very close to)
signicantly different. ShapiroeWilk normality tests showed that
the variables in the two groups were normally distributed, so we
used a Welch two-sample ttest to compare the number of days taken
to reach maturity of males in the two treatment groups.
Prey Attraction Function of Male Coloration
To investigate the function of the white stripes on males' bodies,
we rst tested the hypothesis that white stripes may function inprey
attraction. We conducted eld observations using dummies that
resemble male D. raptor in size and colour. We measured the chro-
matic properties of selected brown and white dummy construction
paper across a 300e700 nm spectrum using a USB4000 spectro-
photometer (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, U.S.A.; Chuang, Yang, &Tso,
2008). The spectral properties across a 300e70 0 nm spectrum were
also measured for the body (legs, palps, carapace and abdomen) and
the white stripe of live adult male D. raptor collected from Dongshi
(N¼5) so as to compare spectral properties between corresponding
dummy and live spider body parts.
The dummies were divided into two groups: control group (with
white stripes, N¼35) and experimental group (without the white
stripes, N¼46). In the control group, stripes made fromwhite paper
were pasted onto the cephalothorax region, while in the experi-
mental group, only the glue that was used to paste thewhite stripes
was applied on that region (Appendix Fig. A1). Field experiments
were conducted between 28 August and 1 September in 2009 and
between 17 and 24 July in 2010 at the study site where we collected
D. raptor. The habitat is characterized by the presence of many rocks
(20e40 cm in diameter) along the banks of calm streams with slow
water ow and relatively closed canopy. In the eld, male D. raptor
were frequently observed perching on the rocks. Therefore, we
randomly chose rocks of similar size and placed two types of
dummies on them alternately. Video cameras with infrared night
view scopes (Sony SR-100 and SR-62, Tokyo, Japan) were placed
approximately 1 m from each dummy to monitor them from 2000 to
0400 hours each night. The video footages were viewed in the lab-
oratory in Tunghai University to check for events such as prey
attraction and predator attraction. An attractionevent was dened
as an insect approaching directly within 5 cm of a dummy. We
dened prey attraction rate as the number of prey approaching
events per hour of footage. A negative binomial regressionwas used
to compare the prey attraction rates of two types of dummies.
Reproductive Function of Male Coloration
Finally, we tested the hypothesis that the white stripes of males
were involved in reproductive behaviour and thus would affect a
male's courting success. We collected subadult males and females
from the study site in Dongshi, Taichung city, Taiwanand each spider
was individually raised in a Plexiglas container (30 15 cm and
18 cm high). The bottom of the container was submerged in 2 cm of
water (water changed once a week) and a sponge was placed for the
spider to perch on. Opaque plastic sheets were placed between
containers to prevent visual contact between individual spiders. We
fed spiders one cricket (A. bimaculata, body length ca. 10 mm) once a
week until they reached maturity. The age of spiders used in the
experiment was no more than 1 month after sexual maturity.
We randomly assigned mature males into two groups. Males in
the experimental group were manipulated by covering the white
stripes with brown paint with a reectance spectrum similar to that
of the spiders' brown body colour (Appendix Fig. A2). For males in
the control group, we applied the same amount of brown paint on
the brown part of the cephalothorax. All the spiders were used only
once to exclude the potential inuence of prior experience. A glass
terrarium (30 30 cm and 15 cm high) with a cylindrical stage and
plastic board at the bottom was used as the arena for mate choice
trials. The arena was covered by a piece of glass to prevent the
spiders from escaping during the experiments. In each trial, a fe-
male was rst introduced to the arena for 1 h to let it adapt to the
environment and produce dragline silks, which may contain sex
pheromones, and then a male was introduced to the arena. The
behaviours and interactions of the spiders were recorded by
infrared video cameras. Preliminary experiments showed that the
courtship of a male D. raptor includes the following four steps: (1)
following female's dragline; (2) waving forelegs; (3) tapping fe-
male's legs; and (4) climbing onto female's back. It has been re-
ported that vibratory signals (transmitted by water surface wave)
play roles in the courtship of Dolomedes spiders (Arnqvist, 1992;
Bleckmann &Bender, 1987; Roland &Rovner, 1983). Thus, to
T.-S. Lin et al. / Animal Behaviour 108 (2015) 25e32 27
differentiate the role of the visual signal, we did not provide water
in the arena for the spider to generate vibratory signals. We
considered courtship successful when a male was able to climb
onto a female's dorsum without being kicked off or cannibalized. A
permutation Pearson chi-square test of homogeneity was used to
compare the courtship success of male D. raptor receiving different
treatments. All statistical tests were carried out in the R program-
ming environment version 3.1.2 (R Core Team, 2014). A general
linear mixed model was tted by using an R package lmerTest
version 2.0-20 (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, &Christensen, 2014).
Ethical Note
To minimize adverse impacts on their welfare, we treated the
spiders gently during the experiment and released them afterwards
to their original habitat. The experimental procedures strictly
adhered to the research ethics and animal treatmentlegal re-
quirements of Tunghai University.
RESULTS
Natural Variation in Male Coloration
In the eld, body size, body weight and white stripe area of
mature males varied considerably. The white stripe area was
signicantly and positively correlated with body size and weight
score (Fig. 2a, Appendix Table A1). This result indicated that the
white stripe area can be used as an indicator of spider body size and
weight. Spider weight was highly correlated with spider size
(R
2
¼0.954, P<0.0001; Fig. 2b, Appendix Table A1). However, the
white stripe area was not signicantly correlated with the residual
of spider weight against body size score (Fig. 2c, Appendix
Table A1). This result indicated that, after we controlled for the
spider body size, the spider body weight cannot predict the white
stripe area. The residuals of the simple linear regression were all
normally distributed, and the variances of these residuals were all
homogeneous.
Neither the body size and weight scorenor the residual of
spider weight against body size scorewas signicantly correlated
with white stripe brightness (Appendix Fig. A3,Table A2). These
results showed that the white stripe brightness cannot be predicted
by raw/standardized spider size.
Nutrition Dependence of Male Coloration
We found no signicant difference in the stripe area of the two
groups of spiders before the nutrition manipulation (Fig. 3,
Appendix Table A3). However, the stripe area of spiders in the two
groups varied greatly after the treatments. Males receiving the
high-nutrition treatment (N¼19) moulted faster (t
14
¼6.36,
P<0.001; Appendix Fig. A4) and had larger white stripe areas than
those receiving the low-nutrition treatment (N¼12; Fig. 3,
Appendix Table A3). These results indicate that the white stripe
area can reect the feeding history of adult males.
Prey Attraction Function of Male Coloration
From eld experiments conducted in 2009 and 2010, more than
600 h of video footage were obtained. A preliminary analysis
showed no signicant difference between results obtained from the
2 years, so we pooled the 2009 and 2010 data. A Pearson goodness-
of-t chi-square test showed that the prey attraction data tted
well with a negative binomial model (c
2
79
¼86:15, P¼0.273). Re-
sults of a negative binomial regression showed that dummies with
white stripes attracted signicantly more prey than those without
(Fig. 4a, Appendix Table A4).
Reproductive Function of Male Coloration
In the laboratory, 29 mating trials were performed (N¼14 fo r
males without white stripes and N¼15 for males with white stripes).
Males in the two groups did not differ signicantly in their body
–4 –2 0 2 4
0
0.05
0.1
0.15
Body size and weight score
White stripe area (cm²)
(a)
y = 0.0778 + 0.0120x, R² = 0.9285
–4 –2 0 2 4
0
0.2
0.4
Body size score
Spider weight (g)
(b)
y = 0.2230 + 0.0599x + 0.004x²
R² = 0.954
0
0.05
0.1
0.15
Residual of spider weight
a
g
ainst bod
y
size score
White stripe area (cm²)
(c)
y = 0.0778 + 0.0609x, R² = 0.0033
0.1
0.3
0.5
–0.06 –0.02 0.02 0.06
0 0.04–0.04
Figure 2. Relationships between (a) white stripe area and body size and weight score,
(b) spider weight and spider body size score, and (c) white stripe area and residual of
spider weight against body size score of adult males collected from the eld.
NS
**
High nutrition
Low nutrition
0.14
0.12
0.1
0.08
0.06
0.04
0.02
0Pretest Post-test
Area of white stripe (cm2)
Figure 3. White stripe area of male D. raptor before (pretest) and after (post-test)
receiving low-nutrition (N¼12) and high-nutrition (N¼19) treatments. **P<0.01.
T.-S. Lin et al. / Animal Behaviour 108 (2015) 25e3228
weight (ttest: t
27
¼0.366, P¼0.718). Males with white stripes were
more likely to be accepted by females (control group: 13 of 15) than
those in which the white stripes were covered up (experimental
group: 6 of 14; c
2
1
¼6:152, simulated P¼0.021, based on 5 0 00 000
replications; Fig. 4b). Males without white stripes had a high proba-
bility of being rejected or attacked by females (8 of 14, 57%).
DISCUSSION
This study empirically shows that colour signals in the nocturnal
predatory spider D. raptor have a dual function, foraging and
reproduction. The nutrition-dependent white stripes on male
D. raptor appear to function in both courtship success and prey
attraction. The increased prey attraction of spider dummies with
versus without white stripes suggests their role in prey attraction
while the nding that males with white stripes were more likely to
be accepted by females indicates that this conspicuous body colour
probably serves as a visual-based sexual signal, a rarely docu-
mented phenomenon in animals active at night. In this paper, we
used the term conspicuousto describe the colour pattern of the
body of the spiders, because the reectance of the white part
(above 40%) is much higher than that of other parts of the body
(around 5%; Appendix Fig. A2). However, the visual systems of
humans and arthropods are very different (Endler, 1990), so the
conspicuousness of colour patterns under the two systems may be
different as well, especially in dim light conditions. However, the
results of our dummy experiments showed that the sharp contrast
between the white and brown body parts did attract the attention
of prey insects. Therefore, we may safely state in this case that the
colour pattern is highly visible to these organisms.
In many nocturnal animals, intraspecic communication under
dim light conditions relies on pheromones (e.g. moths and rodents,
Wyatt, 2014), acoustic signals (e.g. crickets and frogs, Searcy &
Andersson, 1986), ultrasound (e.g. bats, Pfalzer &Kusch, 2003)or
bioluminescence (e.g. reies, Lewis &Cratsley, 2008). Nocturnal
spiders have been previously reported to employ mainly acoustic
signals (e.g. percussion and stridulation in wolf spiders; Hebets &
Uetz, 2000) and pheromones (Gaskett, 2007) in intraspecic
communication. The documented dual function of male white
stripes in D. raptor suggests that this prey attraction signal might
have been co-opted for a communicative function in reproductive
behaviour.
The potential role of visual signals in the courtship of nocturnal
animals has not been extensively explored, except in reies (Lewis
&Cratsley, 2008). In addition to a prior study on a tree frog (G
omez
et al., 2009), our present study on the shing spider D. raptor
suggests that colour signals may be more broadly employed in the
courtship of nocturnal animals than previously appreciated.
Numerous nocturnal animals have good colour vision and colour
cues have been demonstrated to be important in nonreproductive
aspects of their lives, such as foraging (Kelber, Balkenius, &
Warrant, 2002), navigation (Warrant &Dacke, 2011) and mutual-
istic interaction (Peng, Blamires, Agnarsson, Lin, &Tso, 2013).
However, so far it is unknown whether in nocturnal animals a
conspicuous body colour can serve as disruptive coloration under
dim night conditions. In theory, the contrasting colours can
enhance the disruptive effect (Cuthill et al., 2005), which may
enable these nocturnal species to avoid attack from their predators.
Therefore, we expect that, as more effort is invested in studying the
roles of body colour of nocturnal animals, the ndings may
dramatically change our perspective regarding the nature of
interspecic and intraspecic interactions at night.
The results of our study suggest that the conspicuous white
stripes in male D. raptor may represent a reliable visual indicator of
feeding history and body size/weight, and may be a visual signal
used by females during mate assessment. Exaggerated sexual traits
of males may be favoured by females because they may reect the
survival or competitive capacities of the owner (Emlen, 2008;
Emlen, Warren, Johns, Dworkin, &Lavine, 2012). In diurnal ani-
mals, the physical form of traits that may convey increased survival
or competitive capacities are very diverse, with visual signals
employed in many organisms (Andersson, 1994). In contrast to
diurnal animals, previously documented sexually selected traits
associated with male quality in nocturnal animals have typically
been acoustic or chemical in nature (Fisher, Swaisgood, &Fitch-
Snyder, 2003; M
arquez, Bosch, &Eekhout, 2008). Our study,
however, documented a clear example in which the white stripe
area of male D. raptor in the eld varied considerably, yet was
closely associated with body size/weight. Indeed, our manipula-
tions showed that nutrition could inuence a male's time to
maturation as well as the area of his white stripes, with high-
nutrition males displaying larger white stripe areas than low-
nutrition males. It is currently unknown whether females are in-
clined to choose males with larger white stripes when they are
given multiple options and research examining the preference of
females for males with different sizes of white stripe is needed.
Our eld experiment using dummies showed that the white
stripes on male D. raptor can attract prey, implying that this con-
spicuous colour signal has a foraging function. Conspicuous body
coloration has recently been reported to function as a prey lure in
nocturnal web-building spiders, such as N. pilipes (Chuang et al.,
2007) and N. punctigera (Blamires et al., 2012). In these spiders,
coloration can apparently increase foraging success (i.e. increase
the interception rate of the web) by attracting prey such as moths
(Blamires et al., 2012; Chuang et al., 2008). The conspicuous body
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0
Experimental
group
Control
group
Experimental
g
rou
p
Control
g
rou
p
Rejected by female
Accepted by female
100
80
60
40
20
0
Percentage
Prey attraction rate
(number / h)
(a)
(b)
***
*
Figure 4. (a) Mean þSE prey attraction rates (number of prey attracted per hour of
monitoring) of dummies in experimental (white stripe removed, N¼46) and control
(white stripe present, N¼35) groups. ***P<0.001. (b) Courting success rates of male
D. raptor in the experimental (white stripes covered by paint, N¼14) and control
(paint on brown part, N¼15) groups. *P<0.05.
T.-S. Lin et al. / Animal Behaviour 108 (2015) 25e32 29
colour of these nocturnal web-building spiders, however, is un-
likely to also have a sexual function because males of these species
are very myopic and dull in colour (Land, 1985). Therefore, the role
of conspicuous body coloration in orb-web spiders seems restricted
to prey attraction.
In Araneae, the true spiders, species using conspicuous body
colour in sexual activities have been mostly reported in the RTA
(retrolateral tibial apophysis) clade, such as spiders in the family
Lycosidae (Hebets &Uetz, 1999) and Salticidae (Lim et al., 2007),
taxonomic groups that also have good vision. We suspect that in
Pisauridae shing spiders (also an RTA taxon), the prey-attracting
conspicuous body colour may be endowed with a sexual function
in accordance with the evolution of good nocturnal visual ability in
these spiders, a hypothesis that requires additional testing.
Regardless, the sexually dimorphic white stripes on mature males
appear to function in mate attraction, suggesting that females at
least have the perceptual capacity to assess this trait. While
comparative work would be necessary to test this, we propose that
the white coloration on female and male D. raptor evolved initially
in a prey attraction context and then this coloration was co-opted
for a role in male courtship and female assessment. Examples
such as this of dual-function signals can help us understand the
evolutionary history and current functions of animal displays.
Although the conspicuous visual signal of white stripes appears
important for courting males, nothing is yet known of any costs they
might incur. Bright signals, even in nocturnal animals, can be
exploited by potential predatorsand parasitoids (Zuk &Kolluru,1998).
For example, the aquatic wasp, Anoplius depressipes,canactively
search for Dolomedes spiders that hide inside the curled-up leaves of
water lilies (Roble, 1985) and vertebrate predators with relatively
good low-light vision such as shes and frogs also pose a large threat
to shing spiders (Suter, 2003). In this particular system, these trade-
offs would be especially complex as the presence/absence of white
stripesinuences foraging success,which in turn inuencesthe size of
the white stripes, which then inuences a male's mating success.
Studies exploring the evolutionary and functional interplay between
all of these interactions would be illuminating.
Acknowledgments
The study was funded by the National Science Council, Taiwan
(NSC-99-2632-B-029-001-MY3, NSC-102-2311-B-029-001-MY3)
grants to I.M.T and the Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan
postdoctoral grant (MOST 103-2811-B029-001) to S.Z. We are
grateful to the two anonymous referees for their constructive
comments.
References
Andersson, M. (1994). Sexual selection. New Jersey, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Arnqvist, G. (1992). Courtship behavior and sexual cannibalism in the semi-aquatic
shing spider, Dolomedes mbriatus (Clerck) (Araneae: Pisauridae). Journal of
Arachnology, 20, 222e226.
Blamires, S. J., Lai, C. H., Cheng, R. C., Liao, C. P., Shen, P. S., & Tso, I. M. (2012). Body
spot coloration of a nocturnal sit-and-wait predator visually lures prey.
Behavioral Ecology, 23,69e74.
Bleckmann, H., & Bender, M. (1987). Water surface waves generated by the male
pisaurid spider Dolomedes triton (Walckenaer) during courtship behavior.
Journal of Arachnology, 15, 363e369.
Bleckmann, H., & Lotz, T. (1987). The vertebrate-catching behaviour of the shing
spider Dolomedes triton (Araneae, Pisauridae). Animal Behaviour, 35,641e651.
Broadley, R. A., & Stringer, I. A. N. (2001). Prey attraction by larvae of the New
Zealand glowworm, Arachnocampa luminosa (Diptera: Mycetophilidae). Inver-
tebrate Biology, 120,170e177.
Chuang, C. Y., Yang, E. C., & Tso, I. M. (2007). Diurnal and nocturnal prey luring of a
colorful predator. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210, 3830e3837.
Chuang, C. Y., Yang, E. C., & Tso, I. M. (2008). Deceptive color signaling in the night: a
nocturnalpredator attractsprey with visual lures. BehavioralEcology, 19,237e244.
Cuthill, I. C., Stevens, M., Sheppard, J., Maddocks, T., Parraga, C. A., & Troscianko, T. S.
(2005).Disruptive coloration and backgroundpattern matching. Nature, 434,72e74.
De Cock, R., & Matthysen, E. (2003). Glow-worm larvae bioluminescence (Coleop-
tera: Lampyridae) operates as an aposematic signal upon toads (Bufo bufo).
Behavioral Ecology, 14,103e108 .
Elias, D. O., Maddison, W. P., Peckmezian, C., Girard, M. B., & Mason, A. C. (2012).
Orchestrating the score: complex multimodal courtship in the Habronattus
coecatus group of Habronattus jumping spiders (Araneae: Salticidae). Biological
Journal of the Linnean Society, 105, 522e547.
Emlen, D. J. (2008). The evolution of animal weapons. Annual Review of Ecology
Evolution and Systematics, 39, 387e413.
Emlen, D. J., Warren, I. A., Johns, A., Dworkin, I., & Lavine, L. C. (2012). A mechanism
of extreme growth and reliable signaling in sexually selected ornaments and
weapons. Science, 337, 860e864.
Endler, J. A. (1990). On the measurement and classication of colour in studies
of animal colour patterns. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 41,
315e352.
Ferkin, M. H., Sorokin, E. S., Johnston, R. E., & Lee, C. J. (1997). Attractiveness of
scents varies with protein content of the diet in meadow voles. Animal
Behaviour, 53,133e141.
Fisher, H., Swaisgood, R., & Fitch-Snyder, H. (2003). Countermarking by male pygmy
lorises (Nycticebus pygmaeus): do females use odor cues to select mates with
high competitive ability? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 53,123e130 .
Framenau, V. W., & Hebets, E. A. (2007). A review of leg ornamentation in male wolf
spiders, with the description of a new species from Australia, Artoria schizo-
coides (Araneae, Lycosidae). Journal of Arachnology, 35,89e101.
Gaskett, A. C. (2007). Spider sex pheromones: emission, reception, structures, and
functions. Biological Reviews, 82,27e48.
Girard, M. B., Kasumovic, M. M., & Elias, D. O. (2011). Multi-modal courtship in the
peacock spider, Maratus volans (O.P. Cambridge, 1874). PLoS One, 6, e25390.
G
omez, D., Richardson, C., Lengagne, T., Plenet, S., Joly, P., L
ena, J. P., et al. (2009). The
role of nocturnal vision in mate choice: females prefer conspicuous males in the
European tree frog (Hyla arborea). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological
Sciences, 276, 2351e2358.
Haddock, S. H. D., Moline, M. A., & Case, J. F. (2010). Bioluminescence in the sea.
Annual Review of Marine Science, 2,443e493.
Hebets, E. A., & Uetz, G. W. (1999). Female responses to isolated signals from
multimodal male courtship displays in the wolf spider genus Schizocosa (Ara-
neae: Lycosidae). Animal Behaviour, 57, 865e872.
Hebets, E. A., & Uetz, G. W. (2000). Leg ornamentation and the efcacy of courtship
display in four species of wolf spider (Araneae: Lycosidae). Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology, 47, 280e286.
Hebets, E. A., Wesson, J., & Shamble, P. S. (2008). Diet inuences mate choice
selectivity in adult female wolf spiders. Animal Behaviour, 76,355e363.
Hill, G. E. (1992). Proximate basis of variation in carotenoid pigmentation in male
house nches. Auk, 109,1e12.
Jansen, M., & K
ohler, J. (2008). Intraspecic combat behavior of Phyllomedusa
boliviana (Anura: Hylidae) and the possible origin of visual signaling in
nocturnal treefrogs. Herpetological Review, 39, 290e293.
Kelber, A., Balkenius, A., & Warrant, E. J. (2002). Scotopic colour vision in nocturnal
hawkmoths. Nature, 419, 922e925.
Kodandaramaiah, U. (2011). The evolutionary signicance of buttery eyespots.
Behavioral Ecology, 22, 1264e1271.
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2014). lmerTest: Tests in linear
mixed effects models. R package version 2.0-20 http://CRAN.R-project.org/
package¼lmerTest.
Land, M. (1985). The morphology and optics of spider eyes. In F. G. Barth (Ed.),
Neurobiology of arachnids (pp. 53e78). Berlin, Germany: Springer Verlag.
Lewis, S. M., & Cratsley, C. K. (2008). Flash signal evolution, mate choice, and pre-
dation in reies. Annual Review of Entomology, 53, 293e321.
Li, J., Zhang, Z., Liu, F., Liu, Q., Gan, W., Chen, J., et al. (2008). UVB-based mate-choice
cues used by females of the jumping spider Phintella vittata.Current Biology, 18,
699e703.
Lim, M. L. M., Land, M. F., & Li, D. (2007). Sex-specic UV and uorescence signals in
jumping spiders. Science, 315,481.
Lim, M. L. M., Li, J., & Li, D. (2008). Effect of UV-reecting markings on female mate-
choice decisions in Cosmophasis umbratica, a jumping spider from Singapore.
Behavioral Ecology, 19,61e66.
Lloyd, J. E. (1971). Bioluminescent communication in insects. Annual Review of
Entomology, 16,97e122.
Marek, P., Papaj, D., Yeager, J., Molina, S., & Moore, W. (2011). Bioluminescent
aposematism in millipedes. Current Biology, 21,R680eR681.
M
arquez, R., Bosch, J., & Eekhout, X. (2008). Intensity of female preference quan-
tied through playback setpoints: call frequency versus call rate in midwife
toads. Animal Behaviour, 75,159e166.
Maynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. (2004). Animal Signals. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
McFall-Ngai, M., & Morin, J. G. (1991). Camouage by disruptive illumination in
Leiognathids, a family of shallow-water, bioluminescent shes. Journal of
Experimental Biology, 156,119e137.
McGlothlin, J., Duffy, D., Henry-Freeman, J., & Ketterson, E. (2007). Diet quality af-
fects an attractive white plumage pattern in dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis).
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61, 1391e1399.
O'Hanlon, J. C., Holwell, G. I., & Herberstein, M. E. (2014). Pollinator deception in the
orchid mantis. American Naturalist, 183,126e132 .
Peng, P., Blamires, S. J., Agnarsson, I., Lin, H. C., & Tso, I. M. (2013). A color-mediated
mutualism between two arthropod predators. Current Biology, 23,172e176.
T.-S. Lin et al. / Animal Behaviour 108 (2015) 25e3230
Penteriani, V., del Mar Delgado, M., Alonso-Alvarez, C., & Sergio, F. (2007). The
importance of visual cues for nocturnal species: eagle owls signal by badge
brightness. Behavioral Ecology, 18,143e147.
Pfalzer, G., & Kusch, J. (2003). Structure and variability of bat social calls: implica-
tions for specicity and individual recognition. Journal of Zoology, 261,21e33.
Pietsch, T. W., & Grobecker, D. B. (1978). The compleat angler: aggressive mimicry in
an antennariid anglersh. Science, 201, 369e370.
Prudic, K. L., Stoehr, A. M., Wasik, B. R., & Monteiro, A. (2015). Eyespots deect
predator attack increasing tness and promoting the evolution of phenotypic
plasticity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282, 1531.
R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna,
Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org/.
Roble, S. M. (1985). Submergent capture of Dolomedes triton (Araneae, Pisauridae) by
Anoplius depressipes (Hymenoptera, Pompilidae). Journal of Arachnology, 13,
391e392.
Roland, C., & Rovner, J. S. (1983). Chemical and vibratory communication in the
aquatic pisaurid spider Dolomedes triton.Journal of Arachnology, 11,77e85.
Searcy, W. A., & Andersson, M. (1986). Sexual selection and the evolution of song.
Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 17, 507e533.
Stevens, M. (2005). The role of eyespots as anti-predator mechanisms, principally
demonstrated in the Lepidoptera. Biological Reviews, 80, 573e588.
Stevens, M., & Ruxton, G. D. (2012). Linking the evolution and form of warning
coloration in nature. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279,
417e426.
Suter, R. B. (2003). Trichobothrial mediation of an aquatic escape response: direc-
tional jumps by the shing spider, Dolomedes triton, foil frog attacks. Journal of
Insect Science, 3,19e26.
Trillo, M. C., Melo-Gonz
alez, V., & Albo, M. J. (2014). Silk wrapping of nuptial gifts as
visual signal for female attraction in a crepuscular spider. Naturwissenschaften,
101,123e130.
Uhl, G., & Elias, D. O. (2011). Communication. In M. E. Heberstein (Ed.), Spider
behaviour: Versatility and exibility (pp. 127e190). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge
University Press.
Warrant, E., & Dacke, M. (2011). Vision and visual navigation in nocturnal insects.
Annual Review of Entomology, 56, 239e254.
Widder, E. A. (2010). Bioluminescence in the ocean: origins of biological, chemical,
and ecological diversity. Science, 328, 704e708.
Widder, E. A., Latz, M. I., Herring, P. J., & Case, J. F. (1984). Far red bioluminescence
from two deep-sea shes. Science, 225,512e514.
Wyatt, T. D. (2014). Pheromones and animal behavior: Chemical signals and signatures
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Zimmermann, M., & Spence, J. R. (1989). Prey use of the shing spider Dolomedes
triton (Pisauridae, Araneae): an important predator of the neuston community.
Oecologia, 80,187e194.
Zuk, M., & Kolluru, G. R. (1998). Exploitation of sexual signals by predators and
parasitoids. Quarterly Review of Biology, 73,415e438.
Appendix
Table A1
Results of simple linear regression (SLR) between different spider body parameters
SLR analysis Coefcient Estimate SE tdfP
White stripe area against body size and weight score DV: white stripe area (cm
2
)
Intercept 0.0778 0.0012 65.04 30 <0.0001
Body size and weight score 0.0120 0.0006 19.74 30 <0.0001
Spider weight against body size score DV: spider weight (g)
Intercept 0.2227 0.0063 35.276 29 <0.0001
Body size score 0.0599 0.0025 23.930 29 <0.0001
(Body size score)
2
0.0041 0.0016 2.592 29 0.015
White stripe area against residual of spider weight against body size score DV: white stripe area (cm
2
)
Intercept 0.0778 0.0045 17.420 30 <0.0001
Residual of spider weight against body size score 0.0609 0.1945 0.313 30 0.756
Table A2
Results of simple linear regression (SLR) between the brightness of the white stripe area and the body size or the residual of spider weight against body size score
SLR analysis Coefcient Estimate SE tdfP
White stripe brightness against body size and weight score DV: white stripe brightness
Intercept 24 949.4 552.3 45.175 30 <0.0001
Body size and weight score 236.8 279.9 0.846 30 0.404
White stripe brightness against residual of spider weight against body size
score
DV: white stripe brightness
Intercept 24 949.4 540.6 46.150 30 <0.0001
Residual of spider weight against body size
score
33 757.9 23 540.9 1.434 30 0.162
Table A4
Results of negative binomial regression comparing the prey attraction rates of dummies of the experimental (white stripes removed) and control group (white stripes intact)
Coefcient Estimate of
b
SE ZP
Intercept 0.9913 0.1795 5.522 <0.0001
Treatment*(Experiment vs control) 1.0592 0.2691 3.9368 <0.0001
*
The ratio between probabilities of two certain events was e
b
.
Table A3
Results of a general linear mixed model comparing the white stripe area of males receiving different nutrition treatments
Coefcient Estimate SE df t P
Intercept 0.06680 0.004018 16.627 <0.0001 <0.0001
b
1
[posthigh vs postlow] 0.02331 0.008377 33.96 2.783 0.009
b
2
[prehigh vs prelow] 0.001273 0.008377 33.96 0.152 0.880
b
3
[(prehigh þprelow)/2 vs (posthigh þpostlow)/2] 0.04477 0.002367 28.63 18.91 <0.0001
The nutrition treatment, spiders' pretreatment white stripe area and the interaction term were considered in this model.
T.-S. Lin et al. / Animal Behaviour 108 (2015) 25e32 31
Figure A1. A schematic drawing of dummies used in (a) control (white stripes present)
and (b) experimental (white stripes absent) groups.
Brown colour paint
Brown colour paper
Brown region of spider
White colour paper
White region of spider
20
15
10
5
300 400 500 600 700
Wavelength (nm)
300 400 500 600 700
Wavelen
g
th (nm)
100
80
60
40
20
Reflectance (%)
(a)
(b)
Figure A2. (a) The reectance spectra of the brown region of male D. raptor, the brown
paper used to construct dummies and the brown paint used to alter the colour signal of
white stripes. (b) The reectance spectra of the white stripes of male D. raptor and the
white paper used to construct white stripes on dummies.
–4 –2 0 2 4
10 000
25 000
40 000
Body size and weight score
White stripe brightness (%-nm)
y = 24949 + 236x, = 0.0233
(a)
–0.06 –0.02 0.02 0.06
Residual of spider weight
a
ainst bod
size score
y = 24949 + 33758x , = 0.0642
(b)
20 000
15 000
30 000
35 000
10 000
25 000
40 000
20 000
15 000
30 000
35 000
Figure A3. Relationships between the white stripe brightness and (a) body size and
weight scoreand (b) residual of spider weight against body size score.
***
80
60
40
20
0High Low
Nutrition treatment
Moulting time (day)
Figure A4. Mean þSE number of days taken to reach maturity of subadult males
receiving high-nutrition (N¼19) and low-nutrition (N¼14) treatments. ***P<0.001.
T.-S. Lin et al. / Animal Behaviour 108 (2015) 25e3232
... In D. sulfureus, three coloration morphs are known (Supplementary Figure S3; after Tanikawa and Miyashita, 2008). Although the function of the typical Dolomedes white lateral bands is unexplored, studies in D. raptor have linked them to foraging (Lin et al., 2015;Tso et al., 2016) (see Diet & Predation Behavior) and male mating success (Lin et al., 2015) (see Reproductive Behavior). The mechanism(s) behind Dolomedes color variation has only been studied in D. plantarius. ...
... In D. sulfureus, three coloration morphs are known (Supplementary Figure S3; after Tanikawa and Miyashita, 2008). Although the function of the typical Dolomedes white lateral bands is unexplored, studies in D. raptor have linked them to foraging (Lin et al., 2015;Tso et al., 2016) (see Diet & Predation Behavior) and male mating success (Lin et al., 2015) (see Reproductive Behavior). The mechanism(s) behind Dolomedes color variation has only been studied in D. plantarius. ...
... The use of body coloration in prey capture has also been suggested to help Dolomedes forage. For example, female D. raptor use distinct white patches of hairs on their legs to lure prey (Tso et al., 2016), while bright white stripes on male cephalothorax provide a similar function for prey attraction (Lin et al., 2015). Several other Dolomedes similarly possess such distinct leg patches in females (e.g. ...
Article
Full-text available
Dolomedes may easily be considered to be among the most charismatic spider taxa. Known colloquially as fishing or raft spiders, this clade of dolomedid cursorial hunters is speciose with about 100 valid species names. Most Dolomedes are large spiders that inhabit water bodies across all continents except Antarctica and, interestingly, South America. Dolomedes have captured the attention of researchers and the public alike for their ability to walk on and submerge under water, fish for prey (including small vertebrates), and for their often-bizarre mating behavior that includes examples of male spontaneous death and sexual cannibalism. In this review, we critically evaluate what is known of Dolomedes biology, focusing on their systematics and morphology, ecology, behavior, and conservation. Given their close association with water, Dolomedes may be particularly vulnerable to the impacts of anthropogenic change and provide an important group of indicator species for understanding the effect of pollution, habitat loss and climate change. We outline a roadmap for future studies that, in our view, will consolidate Dolomedes as an ideal model lineage among spiders for addressing a vast array of questions across multiple fields of biology.
... Although this issue has rarely been investigated, there is some evidence that body coloration can affect both prey attraction and sexual selection. For example, white stripes of males of the spider Dolomedes raptor plays a role in both prey attraction and mate choice (Lin et al. 2015). The presence and size of white stripes in males, which depend on body size and presumably reflect resource acquisition during juvenile growth, have a positive effect on both prey attraction and female mating acceptance (Lin et al. 2015). ...
... For example, white stripes of males of the spider Dolomedes raptor plays a role in both prey attraction and mate choice (Lin et al. 2015). The presence and size of white stripes in males, which depend on body size and presumably reflect resource acquisition during juvenile growth, have a positive effect on both prey attraction and female mating acceptance (Lin et al. 2015). The presence and intensity of the visual signal, such as the white stripes in D. raptor, might reliably indicate quality of potential sexual partners. ...
... For example, in species where body coloration plays a role in both sexual selection and prey attraction, there may be opposing selection forces shaping body coloration. Alternatively, body coloration could fulfill the two functions, which seems to be the case with the white coloration in the nocturnal spider Dolomedes raptor (Lin et al. 2015). Second, although previous studies have documented the ecological cost in terms of increased predation, little is known about the physiological and/or energetic cost of producing lures or attracting prey in general. ...
Article
Full-text available
Lures and other adaptations for prey attraction are particularly interesting from an evolutionary viewpoint because they are characterized by correlational selection, involve multicomponent signals, and likely reflect a compromise between maximizing conspicuousness to prey while avoiding drawing attention of enemies and predators. Therefore, investigating the evolution of lure and prey-attraction adaptations can help us understand a larger set of traits governing interactions among organisms. We review the literature focusing on spiders (Araneae), which is the most diverse animal group using prey attraction and show that the evolution of prey-attraction strategies must be driven by a trade-off between foraging and predator avoidance. This is because increasing detectability by potential prey often also results in increased detectability by predators higher in the food chain. Thus increasing prey attraction must come at a cost of increased risk of predation. Given this trade-off, we should expect lures and other prey-attraction traits to remain suboptimal despite a potential to reach an optimal level of attractiveness. We argue that the presence of this trade-off and the multivariate nature of prey-attraction traits are two important mechanisms that might maintain the diversity of prey-attraction strategies within and between species. Overall, we aim to stimulate research on this topic and progress in our general understanding of the diversity of predator and prey interactions.
... To date, very few studies have explored the role of body coloration in the courtship of spiders at night. Lin, Zhang, Liao, Hebets, & Tso (2015) reported that the conspicuous white stripes on the cephalothorax of males in the nocturnal, semiaquatic fishing spider Dolomedes raptor (Araneae: Pisauridae) have an important function in their courtship. Males with intact white stripes were significantly more likely to be accepted by females than those with the white stripes experimentally removed. ...
... Previous studies of the fishing spider D. raptor showed that the total area covered by their white stripes varied considerably between individuals and was closely associated with body size/ weight (Lin et al., 2015). The white stripe of this spider is also a good indicator of the nutritional history of the males as better fed males normally displayed larger white stripe areas than poorly fed males (Lin et al., 2015). ...
... Previous studies of the fishing spider D. raptor showed that the total area covered by their white stripes varied considerably between individuals and was closely associated with body size/ weight (Lin et al., 2015). The white stripe of this spider is also a good indicator of the nutritional history of the males as better fed males normally displayed larger white stripe areas than poorly fed males (Lin et al., 2015). In the present study, H. venatoria females may have assessed the quality of the males by the brightness of the white stripe as it may also be an indicator of male quality; however, further study is needed to investigate this relationship. ...
Article
Full-text available
Visual signals are widely used by animals in foraging, mating and predator avoidance. Body coloration plays an important role in the reproductive behavioural interactions of many diurnal animal species; however, its role in the courtship of nocturnal species has received little attention. We investigated the role of conspicuous body coloration, a white stripe on the forehead region, in the reproductive interactions of the nocturnal brown huntsman spider, Heteropoda venatoria. We performed a series of mating trials in which we manipulated the body coloration of males and the vision of females and determined the probabilities of mating success of males in different treatments. Results showed that males that courted females with intact vision had a significantly lower probability of mating success than males that courted females whose vision was blocked. Females with intact vision were also more likely to choose males with a white stripe than those whose stripe was removed. Therefore, we have unambig-uously demonstrated for the first time that body coloration plays an important role as a visual signal in the mating of nocturnal spiders.
... Because of its large body size and excellent diving ability, this species is thought to consume large aquatic and semi-aquatic organisms (Kosuge & Sasaki 2002). Interestingly, it has a white hair patch on its first and second legs; in the closely related species Dolomedes raptor Bösenberg & Strand, 1906 this patch visually attracts terrestrial prey insects (Lin et al. 2015;Tso et al. 2016). Dolomedes orion is therefore expected to have a broad diet that includes both aquatic and terrestrial organisms. ...
... Prey length ¼ total body length, excluding legs in insects, harvestman and spiders, carapace width in crabs and snout-vent length in amphibian and reptiles prey. The closely related species, D. raptor, captures insects such as grasshoppers by visually attracting them with the white patches on its legs(Lin et al. 2015;Tso et al. 2016). In D. orion, the presence or absence of white leg patches differs among islands: they are present on most individuals on Amami Oshima and Okinawa Island but are lacking on many individuals on Kume Island (A. ...
Article
Full-text available
Fishing spiders, Dolomedes Latreille, 1804 (Pisauridae), are thought to be important predators in semi-aquatic ecosystems, but relatively few studies have investigated feeding ecology of Asian species. Here, we report on the diet of the Okinawan fishing spider, Dolomedes orion Tanikawa, 2003, the largest cursorial spider in Japan, based upon field observations on Amami Oshima Island in Kagoshima Prefecture and a literature review. A total of 13 predation events were observed in field surveys and 10 records were found in the literature. Together, these indicate the consumption of a broad range of food items from arthropods to vertebrates, e.g., insects, harvestman, conspecific spiders, crustaceans (freshwater crab and prawn), two species of endemic frogs (Rana ulma, Odorrana narina), and an Okinawa tree lizard (Japalura polygonata polygonata). Because this species has substantial energy demands for growth, large prey items such as crustaceans and vertebrates seem to be its primary food resources with 10 of 23 prey items [=43%] belonging to these two large-sized prey groups.
... For example, several hummingbird (Family Trochilidae; Beltr an et al., 2021) and warbler (Leichty & Grier, 2006) species conceal their sexually selected colour patches when the signal is not being directed at a receiver, allowing their otherwise-cryptic body coloration to serve as protection from predators. Colour signals are primarily found in diurnal species owing to the limitations of colour discrimination in dim light conditions (Stang & McRae, 2009), although nocturnal species might use the contrast of white coloration in dim light for signalling in dark environments (Lin et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Otherwise, apparently cryptic mammals often have conspicuous patches of colour on distal regions of their body, possibly for signalling. To investigate ideas about communication within sociosexual contexts, we used a comparative dataset for 2726 terrestrial mammals to match the coloration and patterning of distal body areas (head, chest, rump and tail) to activity cycle, group size, sexual size dimorphism, mating system and congener overlap at both Class and Order levels. Using phylogenetic controls, we uncovered repeated associations between white colour patches and nocturnality, conspicuous rump patches and sexual selection and multicoloured, complex patterning on distal regions of the body being associated with small group size. Overall, however, we found rather few associations between conspicuous coloration and behavioural, social and morphological variables, suggesting that visual signalling is not a key driver of communication in mammals, perhaps because the majority are nocturnal and solitary, and rely on olfactory signalling.
... The evolutionary significance of different spider body colours is still a matter of conjecture. Some spiders use their body coloration to attract mates (Lin, Zhang, Liao, Hebets, & Tso, 2015), as prey lures (Blamires et al., , 2012Tso, Zhang, Tan, Peng, & Blamires, 2016;White & Kemp, 2016;Zhang, Chen, et al., 2015a), to avoid (Bonte & Maelfait, 2004;Brechbuhl, Casas, & Bacher, 2010;Bush, Yu, & Herberstein, 2008;Hoese, Law, Rao, & Herberstein, 2006), startle or confuse predators (Liu, Blamires, Liao, & Tso, 2014;Moya-Lorano, Taylor, & Fernandez-Montraveta, 2003;Zhang, Mao, et al., 2015b), for thermoregulation (Robinson & Robinson, 1978), or perhaps a complex interplay of these functions. Most of what is known about the function of spider body colours emanates from studies of single features, for example conspicuous bright stripes or spots (Blamires et al., 2012;Tso et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Animal body colours can be shaped by many factors, including the need to attract mates, avoid predators and lure prey. In some contexts, these needs might compete. A number of studies have recently demonstrated that the silver, white, yellow or red bodies of spiders attract mates, lure prey or startle predators. Nevertheless, when spider bodies display different colours, little is known about the multifunctionality of the colours and whether they interact. The Australasian coin spider, Herrenia multipuncta, displays unconventional body coloration, with orange, black and grey regions across its body. We hypothesized that its coloration serves a multifunctional role, with the dorsal orange bands on its prosoma attracting prey and its orange ventrum deterring predators. We tested our hypothesis with field and laboratory experiments using dummies and real spiders, and modelling the visibility of the various colours to different predators and prey. Our field experiment showed significant prey attraction towards the orange‐grey dorsal pattern during the day and night, while our laboratory experiment showed that the lizard Japalura swinhonis stared at spiders and hesitated before attacking spiders when the orange abdominal region was uncovered. Our various visual models confirmed our experimental results by showing that the orange and grey body parts were always visible when contrasted against their natural backgrounds. Combined, our analyses provide evidence to conclude that the orange body colour of H. multipuncta is multifunctional, serving in both prey attraction and predator avoidance. A plain language summary is available for this article.
Article
Full-text available
Visual signals in animals are often shaped by sexual selection. For aposematic species, advantages may result from advertising unpalatability to decrease predation and attracting mates to increase reproductive success. On Green Island, Taiwan, most Eupyrgops waltonianus weevils have metallic blue spots on the body; however, some weevils have no conspicuous coloration. The role of coloration in sexual selection and any disadvantages encountered by dull individuals are unknown. To understand whether morphological traits affect mating success in E. waltonianus, we compared different traits between mating and nonmating individuals in the field. A manipulation experiment was also used to test whether coloration affects mating choice and behaviour. The function of colour as an aposematic signal was tested. Results showed that body size and coloration were important indicators of female mating success in field and laboratory experiments, respectively. In the field, males preferred to mate with larger, more conspicuous females, and females preferred to mate with conspicuous males; however, no such preference was shown in the laboratory. Intrasexual competition was stronger in males than in females. Predators avoided colourful weevils. Thus, conspicuous coloration in both sexes is a multifunctional trait and could be an attractive or aposematic signal for different receivers.
Article
Pale lateral bands that contrast with somatic colouration are common to many semi-aquatic spider species and may contribute to camouflage. Dolomedes plantarius is dimorphic for the presence or absence of a broad, pale, lateral band on the abdomen and cephalothorax. Here, we investigate the heritability of this banding pattern by assessing the proportion of banded progeny in broods of spiderlings for which the phenotype of one or both parents was known. Our results indicate a single-gene system of inheritance with the banded allele dominant to the unbanded. This finding offers a simple way to investigate various aspects of the biology of this rare spider, which is classified as vulnerable to extinction. We consider the implications for further understanding the mating system of D. plantarius and for studying the function and maintenance of banding in wild populations.
Article
Full-text available
Body coloration serves a variety of purposes in animals. Diurnal and nocturnal predators such as spiders may use their body coloration to lure prey. We predicted here that the white patches on the forelegs on females of the nocturnal semi-aquatic spider Dolomedes raptor lure prey, explaining why they are primarily displayed when the spider forages along the water edge. To test our prediction, we developed a color vision model assessing whether the patches are visible to pygmy grasshoppers, the spider's primary prey. We conducted a field experiment using cardboard dummies that resemble D. raptor in size, shape, and color, but with half of them lacking leg patches, and we staged interactions between pygmy grasshoppers and D. raptor with and without leg patches in a greenhouse. We found the white patches to be visible to grasshoppers. The dummies with white patches attracted more grasshopper prey than the dummies without the patches. Moreover, grasshoppers were more attracted to spiders when their white patches were present. Our results supported the hypothesis that the white patches of D. raptor lure prey. Our findings, nevertheless, could not be explained as the spider's body coloration acting as a sensory trap but it should not be ruled out. More studies on a wider range of predators and prey will give more meaningful insights into the co-evolution of predatory lures and prey sensory modalities.
Article
Full-text available
Fishing spiders (Pisauridae) frequent the surfaces of ponds and streams and thereby expose themselves to predation by a variety of aquatic and semi-aquatic vertebrates. To assess the possibility that the impressive jumps of fishing spiders from the water surface function in evading attacks by frogs, attacks by bullfrogs ( Rana catesbiana ) and green frogs ( R. clamitans ) on Dolomedes triton were studied. Both the attack dynamics of the frogs and the evasive behaviors of the spiders were recorded at 250 frames per second. A freeze-dried bullfrog, propelled toward spiders with acceleration, posture, and position that approximated the natural attack posture and dynamics, was used to assess the spiders' behavior. Qualitatively, the spiders responded to these mock-attacks just as they had to attacks by live frogs: jumping (N=29 jumps, 56.9% of instances), rearing the legs nearest the attacking frog (N=15, 29.4%), or showing no visible response (N=7, 13.7%). Spiders that jumped always did so away (in the vertical plane) from the attack (mean =137° vs. vertical at 90° or horizontally toward the frog at 0°). The involvement of the trichobothria (leg hairs sensitive to air movements), and the eyes as sensory mediators of the evasion response was assessed. Spiders with deactivated trichobothria were significantly impaired relative to intact and sham-deactivated spiders, and relative to spiders in total darkness. Thus, functional trichobothria, unlike the eyes, are both necessary and sufficient mediators of the evasion response. Measurements of air flow during frog attacks suggest that an exponential rise in flow velocity is the airborne signature of an attack.
Article
Full-text available
One of the frequent questions by users of the mixed model function lmer of the lme4 package has been: How can I get p values for the F and t tests for objects returned by lmer? The lmerTest package extends the 'lmerMod' class of the lme4 package, by overloading the anova and summary functions by providing p values for tests for fixed effects. We have implemented the Satterthwaite's method for approximating degrees of freedom for the t and F tests. We have also implemented the construction of Type I - III ANOVA tables. Furthermore, one may also obtain the summary as well as the anova table using the Kenward-Roger approximation for denominator degrees of freedom (based on the KRmodcomp function from the pbkrtest package). Some other convenient mixed model analysis tools such as a step method, that performs backward elimination of nonsignificant effects - both random and fixed, calculation of population means and multiple comparison tests together with plot facilities are provided by the package as well.
Article
Full-text available
Many nocturnal spiders have paired conspicuous yellow ventral spots that contrast against their black and brown abdomens. A previous experiment, manipulating the coloration of the spots of the spider Neoscona punctigera, suggested that the spots lure prey. We conducted a field experiment in which we placed spider dummies that either mimicked an adult female N. punctigera in coloration, size, and shape (standard dummies) or mimicked N. punctigera but with the coloration of their ventral spots manipulated onto orb webs in the field at night and monitored them with infrared video cameras. Spectrophotometry confirmed that the coloration of the dummies resembled spider bodies, with the exception of the spots of the gray spotted dummies, where chromatic and achromatic contrast differences from spider spots were found. We used entirely yellow dummies to assess whether the spots represent a compromise between prey attraction and predator avoidance. We found that the standard dummies, mimicking adult female N. punctigera in coloration, size, and shape, attracted more prey than the dummies with gray spots, the entirely black dummies, and webs without spiders. The entirely yellow dummies attracted fewer prey than the standard dummies. These results show that the coloration of nocturnal spider ventral spots lures prey, although whether chromatic or achromatic cues are used could not be identified. The entirely yellow dummies did not lure more prey than standard dummies, so the spots are unlikely to represent a compromise between prey attraction and predator avoidance, in contrast to the coloration of certain diurnal orb web spiders.
Article
Signals used to attract mates are often conspicuous to predators and parasites, and their evolution via sexual selection is expected to be opposed by viability selection. Many secondary sexual traits may represent a compromise between attractiveness and avoidance of detection. Although such signal exploitation appears to be widespread, most examples come from species that use acoustic or olfactory mating signals, and relatively few cases of visual signal exploitation can be substantiated. Because males are usually the signaling sex, they are more at risk from predators or parasitoids that locate prey or hosts by sexual signals; this differential selection on the two sexes can affect the intensity of sexual selection on male ornamental traits. The notable exception to male signaling and female attraction occurs in pheromone-producing insects, particularly lepidopterans, which show an opposite pattern of female odor production. Exploitation of such sex pheromones is relatively rare. We discuss reasons for the reversal in sex roles in these species and its implications for signal exploitation. Changes in signals that appear to be adaptations to avoid predation include the use of different signal modalities, changes in signaling behavior, loss of signals, and alteration of signal characteristics such as pitch. Selection pressure from signal exploiters could lead to the production of a novel signal and thus facilitate speciation. Relatively little work has been done on adaptations on the part of the exploiting species, but such adaptations could indirectly influence the mating system of the predator or parasitoid. Signal exploitation is also expected to be a fruitful source of examples of coevolution. Finally, plants emit attractants analogous to secondary sex characters in animals, and may also be vulnerable to signal exploitation.
Chapter
A spider's life is guided by sensory information completely alien to human observers unless specialised equipment is applied. Even in the case of spiders guided by vision, a sensory mode that humans can boast great acuity in, a large body of evidence suggests that spiders are most sensitive to ultraviolet light, light completely imperceptible to humans. The spider's world is thus unknown and only in the last two decades have researchers begun to make strides into understanding these fascinating creatures. Communication research has been a critical piece of the puzzle in our embryonic understanding of spiders. Although spiders generally live a solitary life, it has long been accepted that communication plays an important role throughout their lifetime. Spiders are now the subjects of intensive scientific research as it becomes more and more obvious that their communication systems are unique, highly complex, plastic and versatile. Introduction. Generally, communication takes place when a signal is sent from one individual to another that alters the pattern of behaviour or the physiology in another organism (Wilson, 1975). Three processes are required for communication: the production of a signal or cue by a sender, its propagation through the environment via a transmission channel, and appropriate receptor sites to detect the signal by the receiver. The transmission channels used by spiders are chemical, tactile, acoustic and visual channels (Weygoldt, 1977, Witt and Rovner, 1982).