ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

A variety of instruments have been developed to assess different dimensions of playing videogames and its effects on cognitions, affect, and behaviors. The present study examined the psychometric properties of the Game Transfer Phenomena Scale (GTPS) that assesses non-volitional phenomena experienced after playing videogames (i.e., altered perceptions, automatic mental processes, and involuntary behaviors). A total of 1,736 gamers participated in an online survey used as the basis for the analysis. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed to confirm the factorial structure of the GTPS. The five-factor structure using the 20 indicators based on the analysis of gamers’ self-reports fitted the data well. Population cross-validity was also achieved and the positive associations between the session length and overall scores indicate the GTPS warranted criterion-related validity. Although the understanding of GTP is still in its infancy, the GTPS appears to be a valid and reliable instrument for assessing non-volitional gaming-related phenomena. The GTPS can be used for understanding the phenomenology of post-effects of playing videogames. It can also be used in conjunction with diagnostic criteria for measuring problematic gaming and gaming addiction.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Published as: Ortiz de Gortari, A.B., Pontes, H.M. & Griffiths, M.D. (2015). The Game
Transfer Phenomena Scale: An instrument for investigating the non-volitional effects of
video game playing. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 18, 588-594.
Abstract
A variety of instruments have been developed to assess different dimensions of playing
videogames and its effects on cognitions, affect, and behaviors. The present study examined
the psychometric properties of the Game Transfer Phenomena Scale (GTPS) that assesses
non-volitional phenomena experienced after playing videogames (i.e., altered perceptions,
automatic mental processes, and involuntary behaviors). A total of 1,736 gamers participated
in an online survey used as the basis for the analysis. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was
performed to confirm the factorial structure of the GTPS. The five-factor structure using the
20 indicators based on the analysis of gamers’ self-reports fitted the data well. Population
cross-validity was also achieved and the positive associations between the session length and
overall scores indicate the GTPS warranted criterion-related validity. Although the
understanding of GTP is still in its infancy, the GTPS appears to be a valid and reliable
instrument for assessing non-volitional gaming-related phenomena. The GTPS can be used
for understanding the phenomenology of post-effects of playing videogames.
Keywords: Game Transfer Phenomena, videogame post-effects, non-volitional phenomena,
gaming assessment, confirmatory factor analysis
The proliferation of videogames has resulted in an increased interest in investigating
their effects1. A variety of standardized assessment tools for measuring different dimensions
of playing videogames have been developed.1-3 Current assessment tools can be categorized
into two broad types. Firstly, there are instruments that assess in-game behaviors and
phenomena experienced while gaming. For example, scales for assessing subjective sense of
presence4, dispositional flow5, game engagement6, cyber-sickness or simulator sickness
malaise (e.g., fatigue, headache, eyestrain, etc.)7, 8, motivations for playing3, 9-11, character
attachment12, and identification with avatars13. Secondly, there are instruments or tasks that
have been developed to better understand the psychosocial effects of gaming. These have
either focused on examining dysfunctional gaming involvement employing modified
diagnostic criteria for gambling, substance-induced disorders, and more recently Internet
gaming disorder to measure gaming addiction1, 14-17 or have been to explain the cognitive,
affective or behavioral effects of playing violent videogames.
Some of the better known are the homonymous decision task that assesses risk-related
cognitions by completing a list of words18, and the Taylor Competitive Reaction Time task
that assesses the level of hostility based on the intensity of the punishment provided to an
opponent (e.g., aversive noise blasts, making them eat spicy sauce)19, 20. Measures and
behavioral tests of aggression have been criticized for the way the results have been
interpreted and their lack of external validity21-23, although some evidence supports the
generalization of the results to real-world aggression24. Furthermore, the influence of
unrealistic depictions of real world in media on the perception of the real world have been
assessed25. Cultivation effects (i.e., generalized influence on estimates of the probability of
events, and judgments that reflect beliefs) have only been found in direct relation to
videogame content. In light of the debate about videogame playing and its potential effects on
gamers, it is important to develop new psychometrically sound assessment tools for
examining the direct outcomes of playing videogames, thus facilitating the examination of
causal effects.
Research into Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP) a multimodal research approach
for investigating the transfer of videogame experiences into the real world by examining
altered perceptions, spontaneous mental processes, and behaviors and actions experienced
mostly after stopping playing26 suggests that the effects of videogames tend to be directly
related to the content and experiences in the videogame27-31. The GTP research approach has
explored the relationship between videogame structural characteristics (e.g., visual or aural
features) and in-game activities directly related to gamers’ transfer of experiences. The GTP
framework makes distinction between the inner and outer manifestation of non-volitional
phenomena, and whether they are interpreted as self-generated or not (e.g., inner-speech,
auditory hallucinations), and if they occurred voluntarily or involuntarily (e.g., deliberate use
of videogame slang for amusement, involuntary verbal outbursts). GTP are divided in three
main modalities: altered perceptions, automatic mental processes, and behaviors and
actions26,33.
Altered perceptions are understood as perceptions and/or sensations related to the
videogame when not playing and can take place in all the sensory modalities, across
modalities or be multisensory. Altered perceptions related to playing videogames have been
identified in the following dimensions27-32:
Altered visual perceptions include mind visualizations, pseudo-hallucinatory
experiences (e.g., seeing game icons above people’s heads), visual adaptations (e.g.,
perceiving objects or environments distorted), and visual misperceptions (e.g.,
confuse physical objects with those in the game)28, 31.
Body and other altered perceptions experiences include prioperception (e.g.,
sensations of body or limb movement), tactile perception (e.g., pushing buttons of
gamepad) and cronoceptive perception (e.g., feeling time slow down)28.
Altered auditory perceptions include auditory involuntary imagery (e.g., hearing
auditory cues in the head), auditory hallucinations (e.g., hearing sounds coming from
objects), inner-speech (e.g., hearing one’s own thoughts preserving features from
videogame character’s voices), and auditory misperceptions (e.g., confusing physical
sounds with those from the game)29.
Automatic mental processes manifest as thoughts, urges, and automatic mental
actions. These range from thoughts about the game (e.g., thinking continuously about the
game) to cognitive biases (e.g., experiencing attention bias toward game-related cues,
jumping to conclusions bias), and source monitoring errors (e.g., confusing what an in-game
character said with what a person said)27, 31, 32.
Behaviors and actions can range from experiencing involuntary motor activations
(e.g., involuntary movements of limbs) to performing actions inspired by the videogame or
changes in behavior influenced by the videogame (e.g., avoiding specific places, mimicking
videogame characters, having verbal outburst)27, 31, 32.
Given the aforementioned theoretical underpinnings of GTP, the aim of the present
study was to examine the psychometric properties of the Game Transfer Phenomena Scale
(GTPS), the first ever theory-driven scale developed for measuring non-volitional phenomena
such as altered perceptions (i.e., visual, bodily, and auditory), automatic mental processes,
and behaviors and actions experienced after playing videogames and understanding the
underlying mechanisms of videogame effects.
Method
Participants and Procedure
A total of 1,736 gamers were recruited online and split into two groups for the
purposes of factor analysis (i.e., Sample 1 [S1], n = 1,078; Sample 2 [S2], n = 658) using
opportunity sampling and an online survey methodology. Participants were recruited via
online gaming forums, Facebook, and meetup.com groups. Ethical approval for the study was
granted by the research team’s University Ethics Committee.
Measures
Socio-demographics: The survey included questions regarding participants’ gender, age, and
occupation.
Gaming profile: Included questions about typical videogame session length and frequency of
videogame playing, as well as gamer type (i.e., newbie, causal, hard-core, or professional).
Game Transfer Phenomena Scale (GTPS): The GTPS included 20 items comprising five
different dimensions: altered visual perceptions, altered body perceptions, altered auditory
perceptions, automatic mental processes, and behaviors and actions. These were derived
based on a theoretical framework concerning GTP developed from previous analyses of over
1,600 gamers’ self-reports26-31. The participants’ responses are rated on a 5-point Likert scale:
1 (“never”), 2 (“once”), 3 (“sometimes”), 4 (“many times”), or 5 (“all the time”). Examples
of items included: “seen videogame images with eyes open when not playing”, “experienced
bodily sensations of movement as in a videogame”, “heard game music when not playing”,
“wanted or felt the urge to do something in real life after seeing something that reminded of
the videogame”, “acted differently in real life situation because an experience in a
videogame” (The final version of the GTPS can be obtained by contacting the first author).
The following modalities were assessed in the GTPS via five first-order latent variables:
The altered perceptions modality assesses (i) visual experiences (visualizing or seeing
images, visual pseudo-hallucinations, distorted perceptions and misperceptions of physical
objects and environments), (ii) auditory experiences (auditory involuntary imagery,
auditory/verbal hallucinations or inner-speech and auditory misperceptions, and (iii) body-
related experiences (motion sickness, tactile hallucinations, other body-related altered
perceptions/sensations, and altered perception of time). The mental processes modality
assesses automatic mental processes such as (i) perseverative mental actions after playing, (ii)
thoughts and urges either about wanting to use videogame elements in a real life context or
performing something from the game in physical contexts, and (iii) source monitoring errors
between videogame and real life events. The behaviors and actions modality assesses (i)
involuntary movements of limbs elicited by automatic associations, (ii) verbal outbursts, (iii)
performing behaviors influenced by a videogame, and (iv) change of behavior due to
previous videogame experiences.
Statistical Analysis and Analytical Strategy
Statistical analysis comprised (i) descriptive statistics of the main sample’s
characteristics and (ii) a psychometric examination of the GTPS. In order to assess the scale’s
psychometric properties, validity (i.e., construct, criterion-related, and population cross-
validity) and reliability (i.e., internal consistency and factor determinacy) were scrutinized.
Moreover, construct validity was investigated by performing a confirmatory factor analysis
(CFA) on the GTPS in S1; criterion-related validity was assessed by examining the
bootstrapped correlation coefficients with Bias-corrected accelerated 95% confidence
intervals (i.e., Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients) between the GTPS overall
scores and participants’ self-reported videogame session length across both samples; and
population cross-validity was further investigated by performing an additional CFA for
replication purposes on S2. Finally, reliability analysis comprised an in-depth examination of
the Cronbach’s alpha of the GTPS instrument as a whole and also across the five subscales in
both samples, while factor score determinacies for each latent variable were also computed.
All the aforementioned analyses were performed on both MPLUS 7.233 and IBM SPSS
Statistics Version 2034
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Table 1 summarizes the samples’ main socio-demographic characteristics. Most
participants were male (92.7% in S1 and 80.9% in S2) and were aged ‘between 18 to 22
years’ (52.9% in S1 and 42.1% in S2). Additionally, most participants reported being a
‘student’ (54.8% in S1 and 38.8% in S2). In regards to participants’ gaming-related habits
and behaviors, the majority were ‘hardcore’ players (65% in S1 and 55.8% in S2), played
videogames mostly ‘between 3 to 6 hours’ (41.2% S1 and 43.2% S2) and reported a weekly
gaming frequency of ‘2 to 4 days a week’ (42.6% in S1 and 28.3% in S2). However, in S2,
40.3% (n = 265) reported playing videogames ‘everyday’ (see Table 1).
Construct Validity
In order to address the construct validity of the GTPS and also further verify the
suitability of the five theoretical factors proposed, a CFA with maximum likelihood with
robust standard errors estimation method (MLR) was performed on S1 (n = 1.078) on the 20
GTPS indicators. Because there is no consensus on the fit indices for evaluating structural
equation modelssee 35, 36, 37, the goodness of fit was based on several fit indices using the
following thresholds: χ2/df [1;4], Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA)
[.05;.08], RMSEA 90% confidence interval with its lower limit close to 0 and the upper limit
below .08, p-close > .05, Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) [.05;.08],
Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis Fit Index (TLI) [.90;.95]. In light of the
aforementioned assumptions, all 20 indicators were entered into a five first-order factorial
solution (see Figure 1). As a result, the analysis of the first-order five factors model provided
an acceptable model fit for the GTPS with acceptable item loadings (i.e., .50). More
specifically, χ2[160] = 628.4, χ2/df = 3.9; RMSEA = .052 (90% CI: [.048–.056]), p-close =
.203; SRMR = .040, CFI = .94; TLI = .93 (see Table 2 and Figure 1).
Criterion-related Validity
Recent empirical findings suggested that GTP experiences are heightened by greater
videogame session length38. Therefore, an observed positive association between
participants’ session length and the overall score obtained in the GTPS would be indicative of
the scale’s criterion-related validity since these variables are expected to co-vary both at the
theoretical and empirical level. As shown in Table 3, positive statistically significant
associations between videogame session length and the overall GTP scores were found both
in S1 and S2 (see Table 3).
Population Cross-validity
Population cross-validity was assessed by examining if the results obtained in one
sample (i.e., S1) of a population could also be replicated in another sample (i.e., S2) drawn
from the same populatione.g., 39, 40. Therefore, in order to obtain evidence for population
cross-validity, a second CFA was performed on another sample recruited from the same
population (i.e., S2, n = 658) to test the initially underlying conceptual assumptions (i.e.,
first-order model with five latent variables) verified in the first CFA. Moreover, the results
obtained in S2 (χ2 [160] = 492.7, χ2/df = 3.1; RMSEA = .056, 90% CI: [.051–.062]; p-close =
.140; SRMR = .047; CFI = .93; TLI = .92) were highly consistent and comparable with the
results previously found in S1, providing further empirical evidence that the five-factor
model fits the data well, thus warranting population cross-validity.
Reliability
As shown in Table 4, the GTPS internal consistency as measured by the Cronbach’s
alpha was satisfactory (i.e., .60) at several levels. In most occasions, internal consistency
could not be improved by excluding any items and inter-item correlations were relatively
high (i.e., .30) in general. In regards to the GTPS factor determinacy, this coefficient
reflects the degree of the correlation among the indicators and their respective factors, with
values of .80 being indicative of a strong correlation33, 41, 42. Accordingly, factor
determinacies in the present study ranged from .93 (i.e., Factor 1) to .95 (i.e., Factor 3) (see
Table 2), further supporting the GTPS reliability (see Table 4).
Discussion
The purpose of the present study was to examine the psychometric properties of the
first ever instrument developed for measuring non-volitional phenomena (i.e., altered visual
perceptions, body and other altered perceptions, altered auditory perceptions, automatic
mental processes, and behaviors and actions) related to videogame playing. Accordingly, the
first-order model including the five dimensions proposed for the GTPS was confirmed given
the results obtained from the CFA in both samples yielded acceptable fit indices and factor
loadings. Additionally, the validity of the GTPS at the construct, criterion-related, and
population cross-validity level was warranted and its internal consistency was adequate.
As suggested by previous research43, game-biased perceptions and associations with
videogame content comprise physical objects (i.e., gaming memories triggered by objects or
people), sounds and music (i.e., gaming memories triggered by auditory cues or cravings for
playing), vocabulary and expressions (i.e., use of slang, abbreviations and expressions from a
game), daydreams (i.e., fantasies and thoughts with game contents that pop up), and night
dreams (i.e., dreams about the game or insertion of videogame elements into dreams). In the
present study, the five dimensions of the GTPS were found to be comparable to a certain
degree to those related to the concept of game-biased perceptions. Studies examining GTP
have demonstrated that game-related cues not simply elicit memories of the game but they
also trigger for example altered perceptions (e.g., seeing menus while in a conversation
because gamers expect to see them as in the game)26, 28.!
The present findings relating to the GTPS are still preliminary in nature and therefore
additional rigorous psychometric testing of the GTPS is paramount. A first descriptive
analysis using the GTPS showed very high prevalence of GTP (97%) when using the criteria
to endorse at least one of the 20 GTPS items, and most participants endorsed six to ten
different types of GTP (95%)44. When interpreting the GTPS’ scores it is recommended that
researchers take into consideration the frequency of the number of GTP experiences for
assessing the level of GTP strength, as well as correlating with variables that assess distress
or impairment in areas of functioning for understanding the effects of videogames. In
addition, the prevalence of GTP should be investigated using more representative samples of
gamers.
GTP appear to be a temporal and are relatively common phenomena among gamers.
Analysis of gamers’ self-reports has shown that gamers can perceive GTP as something both
positive and/or negative.26-29 In a survey of over 2,300 gamers, GTP were perceived as more
pleasant than unpleasant and some gamers even wanted the experiences to re-occur.
However, one in five (20%) reported that they had experienced distress and/or impairment in
important areas of functioning at some point as a consequence of GTP. It has been suggested
that the content of the game, the frequency of GTP and the circumstances where GTP were
experienced play a role in the consequences of GTP26,44. Further research should be
undertaken to better understand why some gamers experience distress due to GTP
experiences while others do not”. Moreover, the majority of the gamers surveyed that
reported having experienced GTP, were from a non-clinical population and had never used
drugs38 (or were under the influence of them) when GTP occurred44 . However, GTP have
been significantly associated with medical conditions, and a small number of those that have
experienced GTP (3.5%) consider they are problem gamers or suffer from gaming
addiction38. Gamers that have experienced GTP reported playing excessively but playing
excessively is not a requisite for experiencing GTP. Future studies should assess the
associations between GTP as measured by the GTPS and other measurable gaming-related
phenomena (e.g., immersion, game engagement, gaming addiction, etc.).
Limitations: The present study has a number of limitations. Currently, there are no similar
measures to further assess the GTPS validity (e.g., concurrent validity). Additionally, it is
necessary to ascertain the invariance of GTPS to determine if its psychometric properties
hold across both genders and different cultural contexts. Only one indicator was used to
assess criterion validity (i.e., length of gaming session). However, this is the only factor that
has been consistently been found to be associated with GTP in previously published
empirical studies. Further criterion testing could be done once other associated factors found
in future empirical GTP studies have been carried out. The present study was based on
retrospective self-report data and is therefore prone to well know biases (e.g., recall bias,
social desirability bias). Future studies could perhaps assess to what extent GTP experiences
may be related to normal or abnormal functioning and gaming-related behaviors. The
development of the GTPS provides a psychometric framework for further exploratory and
empirical research into GTP and associated behaviors.
Conclusion: The findings of the present study demonstrate that GTP as measured by the
GTPS represents a validly and reliably approach at several levels. The GTPS is the first
instrument developed that assesses a broad variety of post-play gaming-related sensorial
perceptions, cognitions, and behaviors. The GTPS may be an additional useful instrument to
use in studies examining the underlying mechanism of problematic gaming or gaming
addiction, and may help to differentiate between non-volitional phenomena induced by
gaming and symptoms of psychopathology.
References
1. Pontes H, Király O, Demetrovics Z, Griffiths MD. The conceptualisation and
measuramente of DSM-5 Internet Gaming Disorder: The development of the IGD-20 test
PloS ONE 2014; 10:e110137.
2. Sprong M, Buono F, Bordieri J, Mui N, Upton T. Establishing the Behavioral
Function of Video Game Use: Development of the Video Game Functional Assessment.
Journal of Addictive Behaviors, Therapy & Rehabilitation. 2014; 6:2-6.
3. Yee N. Motivations for play in online games. Cyberpsychology & Behavior. 2006;
9:772-5.
4. Witmer BG, Singer MJ. Measuring presence in virtual environments: A presence
questionnaire. Presence. 1998; 7:225-40.
5. Wang CKJ, Liu WC, Khoo A. The Psychometric Properties of Dispositional Flow
Scale-2 in Internet Gaming. Current Psychology. 2009; 28:194-201.
6. Brockmyer JH, Fox CM, Curtiss KA, McBroom E, Burkhart KM, Pidruzny JN. The
development of the Game Engagement Questionnaire: A measure of engagement in video
game-playing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2009; 45:624-34.
7. Bouchard S, Robillard G, Renaud P. Revising the factor structure of the Simulator
Sickness Questionnaire. Annual Review of CyberTherapy and Telemedicine. 2007; 5:128-37.
8. Kennedy RS, Lane NE, Berbaum KS, Lilienthal MG. Simulator Sickness
Questionnaire: An Enhanced Method for Quantifying Simulator Sickness. The International
Journal of Aviation Psychology. 1993; 3:203-20.
9. Yee N, Ducheneaut N, Nelson L. Online gaming motivations scale: development and
validation. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing
Systems: ACM; 2012. p. 2803-6.
10. Lafrenière M-AK, Verner-Filion J, Vallerand RJ. Development and validation of the
Gaming Motivation Scale (GAMS). Personality and Individual Differences. 2012; 53:827-31.
11. Sprong M, Buono F, Bordieri J, Mui N, Upton T. Establishing the Behavioral
Function of Video Game Use: Development of the Video Game Functional Assessment.
Addictive Behaviors, Therapy & Rehabilitation. 3: 4. of. 2014; 6:2.
12. Lewis ML, Weber R, Bowman ND. “They May Be Pixels, But They're MY Pixels:”
Developing a Metric of Character Attachment in Role-Playing Video Games.
Cyberpsychology & Behavior. 2008; 11:515-8.
13. Li DD, Liau AK, Khoo A. Player–Avatar Identification in video gaming: Concept and
measurement. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013; 29:257-63.
14. Demetrovics Z, Urbán R, Nagygyörgy K, et al. The Development of the Problematic
Online Gaming Questionnaire (POGQ). PloS one. 2012; 7:e36417.
15. Vadlin S, Åslund C, Nilsson KW. Development and content validity of a screening
instrument for gaming addiction in adolescents: The Gaming Addiction Identification Test
(GAIT). Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2015:n/a-n/a.
16. Pontes HM, Griffiths MD. Measuring DSM-5 internet gaming disorder: Development
and validation of a short psychometric scale. Computers in Human Behavior. 2015; 45:137-
43.
17. Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM, Gentile DA. The Internet Gaming Disorder Scale.
2015:n/a-n/a.
18. Fischer P, Greitemeyer T, Morton T, et al. The racing-game effect: Why do video
racing games increase risk-taking inclinations? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.
2009; 35:1395-409.
19. Barlett C, Branch O, Rodeheffer C, Harris R. How long do the short-term violent
video game effects last? Aggressive Behavior. 2009; 35:225-36.
20. Hasan Y, Bègue L, Bushman BJ. Viewing the world through “blood-red tinted
glasses”: The hostile expectation bias mediates the link between violent video game exposure
and aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2012; 48:953-6.
21. Ferguson CJ. Evidence for publication bias in video game violence effects literature:
A meta-analytic review. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 2007; 12:470-82.
22. Ritter D, Eslea M. Hot sauce, toy guns, and graffiti: A critical account of current
laboratory aggression paradigms. Aggressive Behavior. 2005; 31:407-19.
23. Ferguson CJ, Rueda SM. Examining the validity of the modified Taylor competitive
reaction time test of aggression. Journal of Experimental Criminology. 2009; 5:121-37.
24. Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. External validity of" trivial" experiments: The case of
laboratory aggression. Review of General Psychology. 1997; 1:19.
25. Potter JW. (2012) Media Effects. Santa Barbara: Sage Publication, Inc.
26. Ortiz de Gortari AB. Exploring Game Transfer Phenomena: A multimodal research
approach for investigating video games’ effects (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).
Nottingham, UK: Nottingham Trent University; 2015.
27. Ortiz de Gortari AB, Griffiths MD. Automatic mental processes, automatic actions
and behaviours in Game Transfer Phenomena: An empirical self-report study using online
forum data. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. 2014; 12:1-21.
28. Ortiz de Gortari AB, Griffiths MD. Altered visual perception in Game Transfer
Phenomena: An empirical self-report study. International Journal of Human-Computer
Interaction. 2014; 30:95-105.
29. Ortiz de Gortari AB, Griffiths MD. Auditory experiences in Game Transfer
Phenomena: An empirical self-report study. International Journal of Cyber Behavior,
Psychology and Learning. 2014; 4:59-75.
30. Ortiz de Gortari AB, Aronsson K, Griffiths MD. (2013) Game Transfer Phenomena in
video game Playing: A qualitative interview study. In: Zheng R, ed. Evolving Psychological
and Educational Perspectives on Cyber Behavior. Pennsylvania: IGI Global; 2013.
31. Ortiz de Gortari AB, Aronsson K, Griffiths MD. Game Transfer Phenomena in video
game playing: A qualitative interview study. International Journal of Cyber Behavior,
Psychology and Learning. 2011; 1:15-33.
32. Ortiz de Gortari AB. Targeting the Real life Impact of Virtual interactions: The Game
Transfer Phenomenon 42 video games players’ experiences (Unpublished Master disertation).
Stockholm: Stockholm University; 2010.
33. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. (2012) Mplus User’s Guide Seventh Edition. Los Angeles:
Muthén & Muthén.
34. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0. New York: IBM Corp;
2011.
35. Bollen KA, Long JS. (1993) Introduction. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, eds. Testing
Structural Equation Models. Newbury Park: Sage; 1993. pp. 1-9.
36. Boomsma A. Reporting Analyses of Covariance Structures. Structural Equation
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2000; 7:461-83.
37. Hoyle RH, Panter AT. (1995) Writing about structural equation models. In: Hoyle
RH, ed. Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues and Application. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage; 1995.
38. Ortiz de Gortari AB, Griffiths MD. Game Transfer Phenomena and its associated
factors: An exploratory empirical online survey study. (in press). 2015.
39. Raju NS, Bilgic R, Edwards JE, Fleer PF. Methodology Review: Estimation of
Population Validity and Cross-Validity, and the Use of Equal Weights in Prediction. Applied
Psychological Measurement. 1997; 21:291-305.
40. Raju NS, Bilgic R, Edwards JE, Fleer PF. Accuracy of population validity and cross-
validity estimation: An empirical comparison of formula-based, traditional empirical, and
equal weights procedures. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1999; 23:99-115.
41. Mónok K, Berczik K, Urbán R, et al. Psychometric properties and concurrent validity
of two exercise addiction measures: A population wide study. Psychology of Sport and
Exercise. 2012; 13:739-46.
42. Schembre SM, Geller KS. Psychometric Properties and Construct Validity of the
Weight-Related Eating Questionnaire in a Diverse Population. Obesity. 2011; 19:2336-44.
43. Poels K, Ijsselsteijn WA, de Kort Y. World of Warcraft, the aftermath: How game
elements transfer into perceptions, associations and (day)dreams in the everyday life of
massively multiplayer online role-playing game players. New Media & Society. 2014; 1:1-
17.
44. Ortiz de Gortari AB, Griffiths MD. Prevalence and characteristics of Game Transfer
Phenomena: A descriptive survey study. (Manuscript under review). 2015.
Table 1.
Socio-Demographic Characteristics of Sample 1 and Sample 2
Sample
Variables
1
2
N
1.078
658
Gender (male, n, %)
868(92.7)
478(80.9)
Age group (n, %)
18 to 22 years
472(52.9)
241(42.1)
23 to 27 years
231(25.9)
152(26.5)
28 to 32 years
130(14.6)
87(15.2)
33 to 38 years
54(6)
45(7.9)
39 to 43 years
1(0.1)
29(5.1)
44 to 48 years
3(0.3)
12(2.1)
49 to 53 years
1(0.1)
2(.3)
54 or older
1(0.1)
5(.9)
Occupational status (n, %)
Full-time employment
217(23)
198(33.2)
Part-time employment
62(6.6)
62(10.4)
Self-employed
85(9)
30(5)
Unemployed
40(4.2)
49(8.2)
Homemaker
6(.6)
12(2)
Student
518(54.8)
231(38.8)
Disabled to work
2(.2)
1(.2)
Other occupations
15(1.6)
13(2.2)
Self-reported type of player (n, %)
Newbie
19(1.8)
6(.9)
Casual
291(27)
234(35.7)
Hardcore
700(65)
366(55.8)
Professional
67(6.2)
50(7.6)
Average videogame session length (n, %)
Less than 1 hour
46(4.3)
19(2.9)
1 to 2:59 hours
484(44.9)
271(41.2)
3 to 5:59 hours
444(41.2)
284(43.2)
6 to 7:59 hours
67(6.2)
42(6.4)
More than 8 hours
36(3.3)
42(6.4)
Video gaming weekly frequency (n, %)
Less than once
31(2.9)
18(2.7)
Once
54(5)
36(5.5)
2 to 4
459(42.6)
186(28.3)
5 to 6
240(22.3)
153(23.3)
Everyday
293(27.2)
265(40.3)
Table 2.
Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the 20 items of the Game
Transfer Phenomena Scalea.
Itemsb
Factor 2
Factor 3
Factor 4
Factor 5
1
2
3
4
5
.71
6
.67
7
.71
8
.59
9
.81
10
.87
11
.76
12
.67
13
.71
14
.73
15
.71
16
.74
17
.68
18
.63
19
.71
20
.76
Correlation Between Factors
Factors
2
3
4
5
1
2
1
3
.73
1
4
.86
.72
1
5
.87
.73
.91
1
Further Psychometric Information
Factor determinacies
.94
.95
.94
.94
Mean
2,01
2.65
2.53
2.39
SD
0.95
1.10
1.10
1.03
Note: All factor loadings are significant at least at p < .0001. Factor 1: altered
visual perceptions; Factor 2: altered body perceptions; Factor 3: Altered auditory
perceptions; Factor 4: Automatic mental processes; Factor 5: Actions and
Behaviors.
a: Instructions: Have you ever experienced any of the following: visual GTP,
body sensation GTP, auditory GTP, automatic GTP, behavior GTP.
b: Item wording was omitted for the sake of clarity. The final version of the GTPS
can be obtained upon author’s request.
Table 3.
Bootstrapped1 correlation matrix with bias-corrected accelerated 95% confidence interval
between GTPS overall scores and videogame session length (VSL)
Measure
Sample
GTPS Overall Scores
BCa 95% CI
R2
VSL
1
.264*
[.202;.325]
26.4%
VSL
2
.249*
[.169;.328]
24.9%
1. Bootstrap results are based on 10,000 bootstrap samples
* Correlation is significant at .01
Table 4.
Reliability analysis of the GTPS across Sample 1 (n = 1,078) and Sample 2 (n = 658)
Factor
Sample
Internal Consistency (α)1
1
2
3
4
5
1
.94
.74
.76
.85
.81
.79
2
.93
.71
.79
.85
.82
.79
1: The Cronbach’s alpha provided relates to all 20 GTPS items (i.e., whole scale).
Notes: Cronbach’s alpha could not be improved upon exclusion of any item on most
occasions. Factor 1: Altered visual perceptions; Factor 2: Altered body perceptions; Factor
3: Altered auditory perceptions; Factor 4: Automatic mental processes; Factor 5: Actions
and Behaviors.
... Game Transfer Phenomena experiences have been measured with the Game Transfer Phenomena Scale (GTPS), comprising 20 items. The scale has been validated with an international sample (Ortiz de Gortari et al., 2015), a Turkish sample (Dindar and Ortiz de Gortari, 2017), and a Polish sample (Cudo et al., 2022). The GTPS assesses GTP in terms of a five-factor structure consisting of altered visual perceptions, altered auditory perceptions, altered body perceptions, automatic mental processes, and actions/behaviors factors (cf. ...
... The GTPS assesses GTP in terms of a five-factor structure consisting of altered visual perceptions, altered auditory perceptions, altered body perceptions, automatic mental processes, and actions/behaviors factors (cf. Ortiz de Gortari et al., 2015). Studies conducted with international samples using the original GTPS have shown a prevalence of GTP between 75 and 99% among video game players (Ortiz de Dindar and Ortiz de Gortari, 2017;Ortiz de Gortari, 2017;Ortiz de Gortari and Gackenbach, 2021;Cudo et al., 2022). ...
... In order to measure GTP, a total of 46 items were created using a five-point scale of frequency (never, once, a few times, many times, all the time). Involuntary phenomena reported by video game players via qualitative studies (Ortiz de Gortari and Griffiths, 2014a,b,c) were included together with the 20 items of the original GTP scale (Ortiz de Gortari et al., 2015), which were rephrased to assess broader characteristics of GTP. Table 1 shows examples of how items from the original GTP scale (Ortiz de Gortari et al., 2015) were transformed. ...
Article
Full-text available
Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP) refers to a cluster of involuntary phenomena related to playing videogames, including sensory and cognitive intrusions, transient changes in perception and self-agency. The Game Transfer Phenomena Scale (GTPS) has been used to measure the frequency of GTP with respect to five factors. The present study aimed to validate an instrument for assessing the multiple dimensions of GTP (GTP-MDS) that helps clarify the distinction between GTP experiences. GTP were contextualized onto the spectrum of intrusive cognitions, perceptual distortions, and dissociations. The relationship between GTP, involuntary phenomena without game content (INVWG) in terms of, e.g., hallucinations and perceptual distortions, and game-biased perceptions (GBPA), as well as the positive and negative impact of GTP and level of distress were also examined. The data were collected using a survey ( N = 1,301, male 83.4%, mean age = 28.14). Separate confirmatory factor analyses of the dimensions of “inner intrusions/misperceptions,” “outer intrusions/distortions,” and “dissociations/mix-ups” produced acceptable fit indices. The findings show that phenomena manifesting as internal experiences are more common, while those manifesting as externalized intrusions are less common. Correlations between the GTP dimensions, INVWG, and GBPA, such as the insertion of game elements in thoughts, perceptions, and dreams, supported convergent validity. The correlations between the GTP dimensions and hours played supported criterion validity. Distress was correlated with outer intrusions and dissociations/mix-ups, but not with inner intrusions. Taken together, these results support the validity and reliability of the proposed assessment of GTP constructs.
... The prevalence of GTP among video game players is high, between 82% and 96% [21,29,[33][34][35][36], although most players experience mild levels of GTP [37,38]. Endogenous forms of GTP are more common [21]. ...
... Awareness of GTP can help clinicians to demarcate GTP from psychotic features and identify their interactions, given the ensuing treatment implications. If GTP are identified, a more meticulous assessment of GTP can take place via validated screening tools for GTP (i.e., the GTP Scale; the multidimensional GTP scale) [21,36]. An early intervention can be followed based on the GD model for reducing excessive or addictive gaming behaviours, in conjunction with providing cognitive restructuring techniques targeting GTP [25]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Little is known about the interplay of Gaming Disorder (GD) with psychotic processes in schizophrenia. Only a few clinical cases involving video game playing and psychotic symptoms have been previously reported in literature. This case report describes a 24-year-old male diagnosed with paranoid schizophrenia and GD. Our case, Patient G, had premorbid excessive video game playing and Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP) prior to the onset of his schizophrenia illness. GTP are common among gamers and are characterised by abnormal perceptions, intrusive thoughts, and temporal change on behaviours related to the content of video games. However, GTP are not necessarily of delusional intensity for meeting the threshold of psychosis. The relapse in Patient G’s paranoid schizophrenia was associated with recent cannabis use, social withdrawal, and excessive video game playing. Patient G’s psychotic symptoms were influenced by video game themes and the movie “Matrix”, including the delusion that he was in a video game and that people around him were “non-playable characters”. Awareness of GTP can help clinicians to demarcate GTP from psychotic features and identify their interactions, given the ensuing treatment implications. Our case report highlights the importance of GTP, which in some cases may be an early sign of developing mental illness and could have implications for early intervention and prevention of illness onset and complications.
... Research on Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP) has emerged as an area of study that focuses on understanding the effects of exposure to game experiences, and game features on sensory perceptions, cognitions and behaviours [24,53,27,34]. ...
... Game Transfer Phenomena Scale (GTPS) (20-items) [34] was used to assess GTP during the last six months. The GTPS has a 5-point Likert scale of frequency ("Never" to "All the time"). ...
Article
During the COVID-19 pandemic, video game playing increased exponentially. The question if playing could offer benefits to cope with the pandemic stressors emerged. This study compares how non-players and players who may or may not re-experience (e.g., seeing, hearing) game content after playing [i.e., Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP)] cope with the pandemic stressors, emotion regulation and resilience. It also examines the impact of GTP on the perception of self and the world. A total of 567 completed a survey (59.6% male, MeanAge = 28.55). The measures include emotional regulation (ERC), resilience to stress (BRCS) and fear of contamination (PI). No differences between players and non-players on ERC, BRCS and PI were found. Players with moderate GTP levels were more likely to report contamination fears and show preventive COVID coping behaviours. The positive impact of GTP was associated with high resilience and cognitive reappraisal as an emotion regulation strategy. The results suggest that attention should be paid to players who experience GTP more frequently and with a negative impact. Maladaptive coping styles can exacerbate distress from GTP and situational stressors. Identifying methods of protecting vulnerable individuals from these psychological burdens can guide interventions and mitigate consequences in similar situations.
... The present study was part of a larger research project on disordered gaming and game transfer phenomena (see Ortiz de Gortari et al., 2015). Given the clear focus of the present investigation, only the variables needed to assess the psychometric properties of the Polish GDT were utilised and reported. ...
Article
Full-text available
In 2019, Gaming Disorder (GD) was officially recognised by the World Health Organization (WHO) as an official mental health disorder. Currently, the Gaming Disorder Test (GDT) is one of the most widely utilised psychometric tools to assess GD symptoms according to the WHO framework. In this context, the present study aimed to develop the Polish version of the GDT so that this tool can be used within the Polish cultural context. Additionally, the research aimed to ascertain the psychometric properties and appropriateness of the Polish GDT. To achieve this goal, the Polish GDT was investigated in terms of its validity, reliability, and gender measurement invariance through two studies. Accordingly, study 1 recruited a total of 675 gamers (340 female; Meanage = 31.74 years; SD = 7.75 years; range: 15-45 years) and study 2 comprised 575 gamers (275 female; M age = 29.45 years; SD = 4.25 years; range: 18-35 years). A Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was used to assess psychometric properties of the Polish GDT. The reliability was assessed using Cronbach's alpha, McDonald's omega, and Average Variance Extracted. Gender measurement invariance was investigated using multiple-group CFA, and the unidimensionality was tested using mean of item residual absolute loadings (MIREAL), explained common variance (ECV), and unidimensional congruence (UC). The results of the psychometric analysis showed that the Polish GDT had a one-factor structure. Additionally, the present study demonstrated that the Polish GDT exhibited adequate levels of convergent validity, reliability, and strict gender measurement invariance. These findings suggest that the Polish version of GDT is a suitable psychometric test to assess GD and facilitate research on GD. Supplementary information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s11469-022-00929-4.
... Several theories and approaches have been advanced for better understanding the potential of game-driven learning. For instance, the game transfer phenomena framework suggests that several players experience game elements in real life voluntarily as well as involuntarily, from visuals to sounds (Ortiz de Gortari et al., 2015). Otherwise, constructivist approaches have been deployed for explaining how games can become bridges for shared learning across in and off game contexts (Martí-Parreño et al., 2018;Pallavicini et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Online game communities are inhabited by millions of players daily with relevant learning opportunities and dynamics. However, little efforts have been done for exploring their potential. Objectives The goal of this study is to explore if and how social learning processes are occurring in online game communities. Methods n = 480 adult players completed a questionnaire including demographics, game community habits, and the game community of inquiry scale. Results were analysed by referring to social learning theory's processes and factors. Results and Conclusions Online game communities provide relevant instances of social learning, with an emphasis on soft skills and application to off‐game contexts. Moreover, the more a community is inclusive and supporting, the more individuals learn and benefit from it from a cognitive perspective. Major takeaways This study provides a wide overview of the processes through which players observe, filter, and reproduce behaviours from their game communities of reference. Moreover, findings point at which malleable factors should be considered for facilitate this social learning dynamic, which are active participation, community receptiveness, and being aware of the learning process itself.
... An initial sample of 1,129 English-speaking university students from the United Kingdom (UK) were recruited online using snowball sampling. After data cleaning (see the "Statistical analyses" subsection), the sample was randomly split into two subsamples; the first sub-sample (Sample 1, n = 501) was used in Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) and the second one (Sample 2, n = 500) in Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) to assess for population cross-validity (164,165). Participant recruitment took place through university lectures in exchange for university credit as well as on social media with a potential financial compensation in the form of a prize draw of Amazon vouchers through a pool of eligible participants. The online survey was developed and administered via the survey platform Qualtrics (Provo, UT, USA) and included an information sheet, a consent form, and self-report questions to assess eligibility. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Distraction is a functional emotion regulation strategy utilized to relieve emotional distress. Within the attention economy perspective, distraction is increasingly associated with digital technology use, performance impairments and interference with higher-order cognitive processes. Research on smartphone distraction and its association with problematic smartphone use is still scarce and there is no available psychometric assessment tool to assess this cognitive and emotive process parsimoniously. Method: The present study reports the development and evaluation of the psychometric properties of the Smartphone Distraction Scale (SDS) through exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, construct validity, gender invariance, and latent mean differences. The study was conducted in a sample of British university students (N = 1,001; M = 21.10 years, SD = 2.77). Results: The 16-item SDS was best conceptualized in a four-factor model solution comprising attention impulsiveness, online vigilance, emotion regulation, and multitasking. Construct validity was established using relevant psychosocial and mental health measures, with SDS scores being moderately associated with deficient self-regulation and problematic social media use. Gender measurement invariance was achieved at the configural, metric, and scalar levels, and latent mean differences indicated that females had significantly higher means than males across all four SDS latent factors. Discussion: The SDS presents with several strengths, including its theoretical grounding, relatively short length, and sound psychometric properties. The SDS enables the assessment of distraction, which appears to be one of the pathways to problematic smartphone use facilitating overuse and overreliance on smartphones for emotion regulation processes. The assessment of distraction in relation to problematic use in vulnerable populations may facilitate interventions that could encourage metacognition and benefit these groups by allowing sustained productivity in an increasingly disrupted work and social environment.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: research on game transfer phenomena (GTP) is focused on understanding the impact of video game features and experiences on sensory perception, thoughts, and behaviors rather than on excessive/uncontrollable playing regardless of negative consequences. However, studies have shown a relationship between GTP and gaming disorder, and there is a resemblance between some forms of GTP and the perceptual disturbances that result as side effects of hallucinogens. Video game players have reported experiencing sensory perception changes (e.g., perceptual distortions of objects or environments and visual, auditory, or tactile hallucinations from games), intrusive thoughts, involuntary actions, and behaviors (e.g., the movement of fingers when interacting with game elements or a keyboard) . Objective: first, to adapt and validate the Game Transfer Phenomena Scale (GTPS) in Spanish with a Mexican sample. Second, to examine the adequacy of the frequency rating scale used in the GTPS. Method: cross-sectional survey with 607 players; most were males between 18 and 27 years old. Results: the higher-order factor model showed acceptable results. The prevalence of GTP was 96%. The number of game genres played and session length was correlated with all GTPS subscales, confirming the criterion validity. The examination of the frequency rating scale via the item response theory showed that selecting the second level of the scale “once” should be treated with caution. Discussion and conclusions: confirmatory factor analysis showed that the GTPS adapted into Spanish and tested in the Mexican sample was valid and reliable. Experiencing GTP more than once may be more meaningful for establishing the prevalence of GTP.
Chapter
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a prevalent neurodevelopmental disability among gamers where individuals belonging to this group of conditions have difficulty understanding non-verbal cues. Though game accessibility is a focal point in the games industry, there has been a keen focus placed on developing accessibility. Consequently, this study examines the perspective of video games from individuals who have autism to gain further insight into the needs of these individuals. The preliminary study is to discover if autistic users' difficulty reading non-verbal cues extends to their perception of a game environment and if these individuals can experience sensory distress while playing video games. A prototype was created to further understand the non-verbal cues to help shape the foundation of accessibility framework. The preliminary results concluded that autistic users frequently misread or fail to pick up on the non-verbal cues used by developers to drive game flow and narrative (e.g., sign-posting), in addition to experiencing sensory distress while playing video games.
Article
Full-text available
The study of the effects of interactive media has mainly focused on dysregulated behaviors, the conceptualization of which is supported by the paradigms of addiction. Research into Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP) examines the interplay between video game features, events while playing, and the manipulation of hardware, which can lead to sensory-perceptual and cognitive intrusions (e.g., hallucinations and recurrent thoughts) and self-agency transient changes (e.g., automatic behaviors) related to video games. GTP can influence the interpretation of stimuli and everyday interactions and, in contrast to gaming disorder, are relatively common and not necessarily negative. However, some players have reported feeling distress due to their GTP. This study focuses on how dispositional and interactive media habit factors are related to GTP and two forms of problematic interactive media [problematic video game playing (PVG) and problematic social media use (PSMU)]. A sample of 343 university students who played video games completed an online survey (58.7% male, 19-25 years old). Not all who had experienced GTP were identified as exhibiting PVG or PSMU, but all of those in the PVG group had experienced GTP. Overall, the profiles of the groups, including GTP (91.4%), PVG (28.5%), and PSMU (24.8%), were in accordance with previous findings. Those in the GTP and the PVG groups were characterized by being male, being highly engaged in the game (either while playing or via game-related activities), and showed preferences for game-related activities. However, while those in the GTP group were significantly more likely to be fantasy-prone, those with PVG were the ones who played most per day. Those in the PSMU group were characterized by being female and/or extroverted, frequently using social/sharing platforms, and seldom playing video games. A hierarchical binary logistic regression revealed that males were more likely to experience GTP. Increases in PVG, fantasy proneness, and neuroticism increased the odds of GTP. Future work can benefit from considering the role of GTP in gaming disorder, since intrusive thoughts, cognitive biases, and poor impulse control are pivotal in the initiation and maintenance of dysfunctional playing behaviors.
Article
Full-text available
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a prevalent neurodevelopmental disability among gamers where individuals belonging to this group of conditions have difficulty understanding non-verbal cues. Though game accessibility is a focal point in the games industry, there has been a keen focus placed on developing accessibility. Consequently, this study examines the perspective of video games from individuals who have autism to gain further insight into the needs of these individuals. The preliminary study is to discover if autistic users' difficulty reading non-verbal cues extends to their perception of a game environment and if these individuals can experience sensory distress while playing video games. A prototype was created to further understand the non-verbal cues to help shape the foundation of accessibility framework. The preliminary results concluded that autistic users frequently misread or fail to pick up on the non-verbal cues used by developers to drive game flow and narrative (e.g., sign-posting), in addition to experiencing sensory distress while playing video games.
Article
Full-text available
The external validity of artificial "trivial" laboratory settings is examined. Past views emphasizing generalizability of relations among conceptual variables are reviewed and affirmed. One major implication of typical challenges to the external validity of laboratory research is tested with aggression research: If laboratory research is low in external validity, then laboratory studies should fail to detect relations among variables that are correlated with aggression in "real-world" studies. Meta-analysis was used to examine 5 situational variables (provocation, violent media, alcohol, anonymity, hot temperature) and 3 individual difference variables (sex, Type A personality, trait aggressiveness) in real-world and laboratory aggression studies. Results strongly supported the external validity of trivial laboratory studies. Advice is given on how scholars might handle occasional descrepancies between laboratory and real-world findings.
Article
Full-text available
Establishing the Behavioral Function of Video Game Use: Development of the Video Game Functional Assessment A great deal of attention has been focused on individuals who play video and computer games despite physical and psychological consequences. Unfortunately, instruments that have been developed to determine problematic gaming have not completely measured the behavioral component of video game addiction. Identifying the behavioral functions of video game play would allow for the development of behavioral interventions specifically targeted at reducing or replacing those functions. A behavioral perspective may provide valuable information as to why gamers continue to play video games despite encountering difficulties in other areas of their lives as a result of their video gaming habits. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine the four reinforcing functions that maintain video game play and develop an instrument to measure these functions. Results supported a three-factor measurement model associated with the functions that maintain continued video game play, including (a) escape, (b) tangible, and (c) attention. Discussion and implications are provided.
Article
Full-text available
This paper describes the phenomenon of game-biased perceptions and associations, or how, through intensive game play, elements from the game world can trigger thoughts and imagery outside the game world, influencing the perception and interpretation of stimuli in everyday life. Examples include the insertion of game elements in thoughts, perceptions and dreams, the use of specific vocabulary or slang – typical to the game world – in everyday conversations, and the association of real-life objects with game elements. Results from an online survey with habitual players of World of Warcraft confirm the existence of this phenomenon and show that the occurrence of these game-biased perceptions and associations is reinforced by increasing playing time and narrative involvement.
Article
Full-text available
This paper begins with an argument that most measure development in the social sciences, with its reliance on correlational techniques as a tool, falls short of the requirements for constructing meaningful, unidimensional measures of human attributes. By demonstrating how rating scales are ordinal-level data, we argue the necessity of converting these to equal-interval units to develop a measure that is both qualitatively and quantitatively defensible. This requires that the empirical results and theoretical explanation are questioned and adjusted at each step of the process. In our response to the reviewers, we describe how this approach was used to develop the Game Engagement Questionnaire (GEQ), including its emphasis on examining a continuum of involvement in violent video games. The GEQ is an empirically sound measure focused on one player characteristic that may be important in determining game influence.
Article
Previous qualitative studies suggest that gamers experience Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP), a variety of non-volitional phenomena related to playing videogames including thoughts, urges, images, sounds when not playing. To investigate (i) which types of GTP were more common and (ii) their general characteristics, the present study surveyed a total of 2,362 gamers via an online survey. The majority of the participants were male, students, aged between 18 and 27 years, and ‘hard-core’ gamers. Most participants reported having experienced at least one type of GTP at some point (96.6%), the majority having experienced GTP more than once with many reporting 6 to 10 different types of GTP. Results demonstrated that videogame players experienced (i) altered visual perceptions (ii) altered auditory perceptions (iii) altered body perceptions (iv) automated mental processes, and (v) behaviors. In most cases, GTP could not be explained by being under the influence of a psychoactive substance. The GTP experiences were usually short-lived, tended to occur after videogame playing rather than during play, occurred recurrently, and usually occurred while doing day-to-day activities. One in five gamers had experienced some type of distress or dysfunction due to GTP. Many experienced GTP as pleasant and some wanted GTP to happen again.
Article
Previous qualitative and quantitative studies examining Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP) have demonstrated that GTP experiences are common. These studies have shown that many gamers report altered perceptions, involuntary thoughts and behaviors after playing video games (e.g., pseudo-hallucinatory experiences, automatic motor activations, etc.). However, the factors associated with GTP are unknown. In the present study, a total of 2362 gamers were surveyed using an online questionnaire to examine the relationship between GTP and socio-demographic factors, gaming habits, individual characteristics, and motivations for playing. Results showed that having a pre-existing medical condition, playing for 3–6 h, and playing for immersion, exploration, customization, mechanics and escape from the real world were significantly associated with having experienced GTP. Those who were 33–38 years old, playing sessions for less than one hour, being a professional player, being self-employed, and never recalling dreams, were significantly more likely to have not experienced GTP. The findings suggest that attention should be paid to young adults and the length of gaming sessions, as well as taking into consideration underlying factors such as medical conditions that may make gamers more prone to GTP.
Article
Video game playing is a popular activity and its enjoyment among frequent players has been associated with absorption and immersion experiences. This paper examines how immersion in the video game environment can influence the player during the game and afterwards (including fantasies, thoughts, and actions). This is what is described as Game Transfer Phenomena (GTP). GTP occurs when video game elements are associated with real life elements triggering subsequent thoughts, sensations and/or player actions. To investigate this further, a total of 42 frequent video game players aged between 15 and 21 years old were interviewed. Thematic analysis showed that many players experienced GTP, where players appeared to integrate elements of video game playing into their real lives. These GTP were then classified as either intentional or automatic experiences. Results also showed that players used video games for interacting with others as a form of amusement, modeling or mimicking video game content, and daydreaming about video games. Furthermore, the findings demonstrate how video games triggered intrusive thoughts, sensations, impulses, reflexes, visual illusions, and dissociations.
Article
This study describes the development of a screening tool for gaming addiction in adolescents – the Gaming Addiction Identification Test (GAIT). Its development was based on the research literature on gaming and addiction. An expert panel comprising professional raters (n = 7), experiential adolescent raters (n = 10), and parent raters (n = 10) estimated the content validity of each item (I-CVI) as well as of the whole scale (S-CVI/Ave), and participated in a cognitive interview about the GAIT scale. The mean scores for both I-CVI and S-CVI/Ave ranged between 0.97 and 0.99 compared with the lowest recommended I-CVI value of 0.78 and the S-CVI/Ave value of 0.90. There were no sex differences and no differences between expert groups regarding ratings in content validity. No differences in the overall evaluation of the scale emerged in the cognitive interviews. Our conclusions were that GAIT showed good content validity in capturing gaming addiction. The GAIT needs further investigation into its psychometric properties of construct validity (convergent and divergent validity) and criterion-related validity, as well as its reliability in both clinical settings and in community settings with adolescents.