ArticlePDF Available

Persevering with Positivity and Purpose: An Examination of Purpose Commitment and Positive Affect as Predictors of Grit

Springer Nature
Journal of Happiness Studies
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Grit, defined as a passion and perseverance for one's goals, has been consistently demonstrated as an adaptive resource across multiple domains. Less explored, however, are the correlates of and sources from which grit is derived. The current studies examined two plausible candidates for promoting grit, positive affect and commitment to a purpose, using college student samples from Canada and the United States. Study 1 confirmed our predictions that grittier students tended to report greater positive affect and purpose commitment , and demonstrated that these variables appear to be unique and independent predictors of grit. Study 2 examined these claims using two-wave data collected across a semester, and found that while both purpose and positive affect were initially correlated with grit, only initial levels of purpose predicted grit at wave two. In other words, having a life direction may help more than positive affect when predicting who is likely to become grittier over a college semester. Implications of these findings are discussed. Keywords Grit Á Purpose Á Positive affect Á Emerging adulthood
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH PAPER
Persevering with Positivity and Purpose:
An Examination of Purpose Commitment and Positive
Affect as Predictors of Grit
Patrick L. Hill Anthony L. Burrow Kendall Cotton Bronk
ÓSpringer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014
Abstract Grit, defined as a passion and perseverance for one’s goals, has been consistently
demonstrated as an adaptive resource across multiple domains. Less explored, however, are
the correlates of and sources from which grit is derived. The current studies examined two
plausible candidates for promoting grit, positive affect and commitment to a purpose, using
college student samples from Canada and the United States. Study 1 confirmed our pre-
dictions that grittier students tended to report greater positive affect and purpose commit-
ment, and demonstrated that these variables appear to be unique and independent predictors
of grit. Study 2 examined these claims using two-wave data collected across a semester, and
found that while both purpose and positive affect were initially correlated with grit, only
initial levels of purpose predicted grit at wave two. In other words, having a life direction
may help more than positive affect when predicting who is likely to become grittier over a
college semester. Implications of these findings are discussed.
Keywords Grit Purpose Positive affect Emerging adulthood
1 Introduction
When identifying which individuals are prone to success, the most obvious candidates are
those who have a passion for their long-term goals and persevere towards their attainment
P. L. Hill (&)
Department of Psychology, Carleton University, A515 Loeb Building, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa,
ON K1S 5B6, Canada
e-mail: Patrick.Hill@Carleton.ca
A. L. Burrow
Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA
K. C. Bronk
Department of Psychology, Claremont Graduate University, Claremont, CA, USA
123
J Happiness Stud
DOI 10.1007/s10902-014-9593-5
even in the face of obstacles. Researchers have defined this combination of passion and
perseverance as ‘‘grit,’’ a dispositional tendency that helps account for individuals’ success
above and beyond cognitive functioning (Duckworth et al. 2007). The effects of grit appear
most prominently in academic contexts, with grittier individuals achieving higher grade
point averages, higher levels of educational attainment, and greater success in scholastic
competitions (Duckworth et al. 2007; Duckworth and Quinn 2009). Indeed, the positive
relationship between grit and achievement even appears at the physiological level (Silvia
et al. 2013), predicting nervous system responses to given achievement tasks.
Given the clear promise of possessing grit, particularly in the classroom, research is
needed to better understand the cross-sectional and prospective correlates of grit. Using
two samples from different countries, the current studies considered two potential pre-
dictors of grit among college students. These hypotheses are not necessarily competing, but
both could explain the cross-sectional and prospective correlates of grit. First, the purpose
commitment hypothesis suggests that compared to students who lack a sense of direction in
life, those who have committed to a purpose or direction should report higher levels of grit,
in order to help them strive toward their life goals. Second, the positive emotions
hypothesis predicts that having a strong base of positive emotionality should better equip
individuals to develop abilities and skills that promote later success, such as grit. To date,
relatively little research has examined either purpose or positive affect as correlates of grit,
and thus we elaborate below upon the rationale why these posited relationships might be
present.
1.1 The Purpose Commitment Hypothesis
Having a purpose in life entails a commitment to an ultimate life goal that serves to
organize and plan the individual’s daily and long-term activities (McKnight and Kashdan
2009), and individuals oriented toward a set of life goals tend to demonstrate consistency
(i.e., continued commitment) to their choices over several years (Hill et al. 2010). While
purpose has been typically viewed as an indicator of positive adult development (e.g., Ryff
1989), there is increasing evidence that individuals may begin to commit to a purpose
during the adolescent and emerging adult years, and that doing so can serve as a catalyst
for adaptive development (e.g., Bronk 2013; Damon et al. 2003; Hill et al. 2013). While
most of this work has focused on well-being, finding a direction for life and knowing which
life goals to strive toward should build a greater perseverance and passion for these goals
(i.e., grit). Indeed, recent research suggests that grit is associated with a stronger orientation
toward deeper, more meaningful rather than hedonically pleasing activities (Von Culin
et al. 2014). Without having meaningful goals or benchmarks, one might be left without
clear targets to persevere toward, and thus fewer contexts and environments in which to
inculcate a gritty disposition.
In this respect, a purpose can be thought of as a press, which guides one to changing in
ways that help achieve success toward a long-term commitment, similarly to how adopting
social roles appears to influence personality development (see e.g., Jackson et al. 2012;
Hudson et al. 2012). When considering purpose and grit, being a college student appears a
particularly valuable social role for investigation. Purpose development is in flux during
the adolescent and emerging adult years (Hill et al. 2013), suggesting that its role on
personality traits (like grit) might be strongest during this period. Accordingly, we focused
on this context for the current studies, although it is likely that grit correlates positively
with purpose throughout life, given that purposeful adults tend to be more conscientious
and hardworking (Scheier et al. 2006; Siegler and Brummett 2000).
P. L. Hill et al.
123
2 The Positive Emotions Hypothesis
Similarly, research has only begun to examine the link between positive affect and grit,
with only one investigation thus far to our knowledge (Singh and Jha 2008), which itself
only was cross-sectional in nature. In line with that study, one should anticipate that grittier
individuals would experience greater positive affect. Positive correlations should be
expected for at least three reasons. First, gritty individuals have a more positive personality
profile, including lower levels of neuroticism and higher levels of extraversion (Duckworth
and Quinn 2009), traits known to be related to emotional well-being (see Steel et al. 2008).
Second, individuals may exhibit greater interest in their long-term goals if they are
building this passion from an existing base of positivity and optimism. This prediction
follows from a literature that suggests one function of positive emotions, such as interest
and inspiration, is to help build physical, intellectual, and social resources (Fredrickson
2001), which presumably would allow individuals a greater ability to deal with potential
obstacles to their long-term goals. In other words, positive affect can provide a foundation
from which individuals are better able to persevere in their goal pursuit, which could prove
more difficult without a base of positive emotionality.
Third, this second hypothesis builds from recent findings suggesting that well-being can
potentially drive changes in personality development. For instance, individuals more sat-
isfied with their lives are more prone to adaptive personality changes (e.g., gains on those
traits that promote success across life domains; Specht et al. 2013). These findings also
have been extended to other components of subjective well-being, such as positive and
negative affect (Soto 2013). Typically, this work on well-being and personality change is
summarized with respect to the notion that individuals who live happier or more satisfied
lives are more likely to act in ways that enhance or maintain that well-being. Grit seem-
ingly is one such candidate insofar that it promotes success in academic and work domains,
and as such deepening or developing this characteristic may lead to greater satisfaction and
well-being, which in turn serves as feedback for the need to be gritty in the future.
Accordingly, positive affect might promote the development of positive traits, such as grit,
leading to these two variables being positively correlated both cross-sectionally and pro-
spectively. The current studies focus on testing this claim with respect to both positive
affect and purpose commitment.
3 Study 1
Study 1 focused on addressing three important points regarding the role of purpose and
positive affect in predicting levels of grit. First, we sought to establish the predicted
positive relationships between all three variables, as research has yet to fully link purpose
and positive affect to grit even in cross-sectional data. Moreover, in line with previous
work (e.g., Duckworth et al. 2007; Duckworth and Quinn 2009), we sought to demonstrate
that grit presents with these relations unique from the Big Five personality traits (John and
Srivastava 1999). As noted above, levels of grit tend to correlate with a more positive
personality profile (greater agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, extra-
version, and openness to experience; Duckworth et al. 2007; Duckworth and Quinn 2009).
Moreover, the relation between grit and conscientiousness often proves strong enough to
lead researchers to suggest that grit may be best viewed as simply a component of the
higher-order trait (Roberts et al. 2014). As such, given the recent and widespread interest in
grit research (see Tough 2012), research is needed not only to establish a relationship
An Examination of Purpose Commitment and Positive Affect
123
between grit and outcomes like positive affect or purpose, but also to demonstrate that
these relations hold beyond just the Big Five, given that the Big Five traits consistently
correlate with well-being (see Hill, Mroczek and Young for a review) as well as purpose in
life (Hill and Burrow 2012; Schmutte and Ryff 1997).
Second, to conduct tests of unique predictive value, we performed a multiple regression
analysis to simultaneously consider purpose and positive affect as predictors of grit. Third,
though not necessarily expected, we explored potential interactive effects for purpose and
positive affect. For instance, purposeful individuals might be especially likely to be grittier
when they start with a foundation of positive affect. Alternatively, the role of positive
affect on grit could be greater for those who are striving toward a set of committed life
goals. A compensatory effect also could occur, insofar that positive affect or purpose in life
play a stronger role on grit when in the absence of the other variable. However, these
analyses were exploratory in nature, and thus we withhold any predictions.
4 Methods
4.1 Participants and Procedure
Three hundred thirty-seven undergraduates (M
age
=20.26 years, SD =3.96, range
17–45 years) at a large Canadian university took part in the survey for either course credit
or $5 Canadian (approximately $4.52 USD). Around two-thirds of participants ended up
choosing the course credit option. The sample was predominantly female (75 %), in their
first or second year of school (82 %), and White (62 %). All participants completed the
survey online at their leisure, but were restricted to roughly a three-week window in which
to complete the study. The survey contained questions related to different aspects of the
self (purpose, identity, personality), as well as indices of health and wellbeing. We report
below on the primary measures of interest for the current study. Participants were allowed
to skip items as they wished, and thus the sample sizes reported differ slightly between
analyses; however, very little missing data occurred overall as most participants completed
the full inventory.
4.2 Purpose Commitment
Purpose commitment was assessed using a 15-item measure developed by Bundick et al.
(2006), which borrows items from previous inventories (Crumbaugh and Maholick 1967;
Keyes et al. 2002; Steger et al. 2006). Participants rated their agreement on a scale from 1
(Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) to items such as ‘‘My life has a clear sense of
purpose’’ and ‘‘I do many things that give my life meaning.’’ Similar to previous work
(Bronk et al. 2009; Burrow et al. 2010), this measure demonstrated strong reliability
(a=.91).
4.3 Positive Affect
Positive affect was assessed using the 10-item subscale from the PANAS measure (Watson
et al. 1988). Participants rated how frequently they generally feel emotions such as
‘Interested’’ and ‘‘Excited’’ on a scale from 1 (Very slightly or not at all) to 5 (Extremely).
Reliability for the measure was strong in the current sample (a=.89).
P. L. Hill et al.
123
4.4 Grit
Grit was assessed using the 8-item brief measure developed by Duckworth and Quinn
(2009). Throughout both studies, we focus on the full scale rather than separating this
measure into its subscales. Participants rated their agreement to items such as ‘‘Setbacks
don’t discourage me’’ and ‘‘I am a hard worker’’ on a scale from 1 (Very Much Like Me) to
5 (Not Like Me at All), with scores reversed to allow higher values to indicate greater
levels of grit. Reliability in the current sample was good (a=.71).
4.5 Big Five Personality Traits
Personality was assessed using the Big Five Inventory (John 2008; John and Srivastava
1999). Participants are asked to rate 44 characteristics with respect to whether they per-
sonally apply on a scale from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Agree Strongly). All traits
demonstrated strong reliabilities in the current sample: extraversion, sample item: ‘‘is
talkative’’, a=.86; agreeableness: ‘‘is helpful and unselfish with others’’, a=.80; con-
scientiousness: ‘‘is a reliable worker’’, a=.81; neuroticism: ‘‘is depressed, blue’’,
a=.83; openness: ‘‘is curious about many different things’’, a=.77).
5 Results
5.1 Bivariate Correlations
To first test whether grit and purpose are related positively, bivariate correlations were
performed, and Table 1presents these across all the variables of interest. As expected, grit,
positive affect, and purpose all correlated positively. In addition, all three variables cor-
related with a largely adaptive Big Five personality profile, with significant correlations in
all cases except for the relation between grit and openness to experience. As such, it is
worth noting that even when controlling for all Big Five traits, partial correlations with grit
were significant for both purpose [r(319) =.21] and positive affect [r(313) =.15].
Accordingly, both variables appear positively related to levels of grit, even when con-
trolling for these shared personality correlates.
Table 1 Correlations and descriptive statistics for constructs of interest in Study 1
12345678
Grit (1)
Purpose (2) .44*
Positive Affect (3) .38* .60*
Extraversion (4) .16* .40* .47*
Agreeableness (5) .27* .35* .30* .08 –
Conscientiousness (6) .60* .43* .36* .18* .32*
Neuroticism (7) -.37* -.35* -.39* -.31* -.22* -.23* –
Openness (8) .05 .17* .34* .18* .16* .07 -.07 –
Mean 3.18 3.65 3.38 3.16 3.75 3.46 3.17 3.53
SD 0.58 0.79 0.72 0.80 0.65 0.65 0.78 0.60
*p\.05; n’s for correlations range from 320 to 332
An Examination of Purpose Commitment and Positive Affect
123
5.2 Multiple Regression Analyses
Next, we performed a multiple regression analysis to examine whether purpose commit-
ment and positive affect are unique or overlapping correlates of grit. Table 2presents the
results from these regression analyses, controlling for age and gender. Both purpose and
positive affect provided unique associations with grit in this study, demonstrating that these
two potential predictors are not overlapping. Finally, we included an interaction term to
test any potential moderator effects. However, this term failed to reach significance when
added to the model (B =-.02, t\1).
6 Discussion
Study 1 provided three important additions to the literature. First, it provided initial evi-
dence for our suggested relations between grit and both purpose and positive affect.
Moreover, these relations held even when controlling for the Big Five, demonstrating that
they cannot be explained by these higher-order traits. Second, the multiple regression
findings suggest that purpose commitment may prove the stronger correlate of grit;
however, this point will receive further attention in Study 2. More importantly, the two
variables do appear to explain unique variance in levels of grit. Third, we found no
evidence of any moderation effects, suggesting that purpose and positive affect are largely
unique and non-interactive predictors of grit.
7 Study 2
In Study 2, we sought to replicate and extend these findings using a sample of college
students in the United States. First, at the start of the semester, we expected to confirm the
positive relationships between the three constructs of interest (grit, purpose, and positive
affect). Second, we examined the longitudinal relationships between the constructs using
two-wave cross-lagged models, linking their data at the start and end of the semester.
Following the purpose commitment hypothesis, one might anticipate those higher initially
on purpose might be prone to increase on grit across the semester. The positive emotions
hypothesis though would suggest that positive affect may catalyze such changes. In
addition, we tested the alternative hypotheses, namely that grit could predict changes on
purpose commitment or positive affect. This investigation provides an initial step toward
understanding how purpose and positive affect might influence changes in grit.
8 Methods
8.1 Participants
One hundred sixty-five undergraduates at a mid-sized public university in the Midwest
United States completed paper-and-pencil surveys in class during the start of a college
semester. Our sample included relatively equal numbers of participants who were 19 years
old (19.4 %), 20 years (22.4 %), 21 years (20.6 %), and over 21 years (23.6 %), with
fewer individuals under 19 years of age (13.9 %). Participants tended to be female
(63.6 %) and white (93.3 %). Of this initial sample, 121 participants (73.3 % retention)
P. L. Hill et al.
123
Table 2 Multiple regression analysis predicting grit from purpose, positive affect, and control variables in Study 1 (left section), cross-sectionally in Study 2 (middle
section), and prospectively in Study 2 (right section)
Predictor Study 1 Study 2 T1 Study 2 T1 ?T2
B (s.e.) bt B (s.e.) bt B (s.e.) bt
Full model F(4, 310) =24.35* F(4, 160) =17.14* F(4, 310) =24.35*
Age 00 (.01) -.02 -0.33 .02 (.03) .05 0.79 .00 (.03) .01 0.07
Gender (1—Male) -.06 (.07) -.04 -0.86 .01 (.08) .01 0.07 .06 (.09) .05 0.67
Purpose commitment .21 (.04) .38 6.11* .28 (.06) .36 4.76* .24 (.07) .33 3.54*
Positive affect .13 (.05) .16 2.68* .30 (.08) .27 3.50* -.14 (.10) -.12 -1.37
Grit T1 .48 (.09) .47 5.36*
*p\.05. Multiple R’s for each analyses are .49, .55, and .49 respectively across columns
An Examination of Purpose Commitment and Positive Affect
123
completed the survey again at the end of the semester, roughly 3 months later. The second
wave (T2) sample had fewer participants in the older age brackets [v
2
(4) =27.57,
p\.05], but did not differ on the primary variables under investigation. All cross-sectional
analyses conducted were from the full sample, while longitudinal analyses included only
those participants with T2 data.
8.2 Measures
Grit, purpose commitment, and positive affect were assessed using the same scales as in
Study 1. For grit, reliability was good at both time points (T1 a=.79; T2 a=.80), and
the scale demonstrated a strong test–retest correlation (r=.61, p\.05). For purpose
commitment, reliability was good at both time points (T1 a=.89; T2 a=89), and the
scale demonstrated a strong test–retest correlation (r=.66, p\.05). For positive affect,
reliability was good at both time points (T1 a=.80; T2 a=.81), and the scale demon-
strated a strong test–retest correlation (r=.51, p\.05).
9 Results
9.1 Wave 1 Correlations and Description of Change over Time
Following predictions, all three constructs were strongly positively correlated at T1. Grit
was associated with greater purpose commitment (r=.49, p\.05), and higher levels of
positive affect (r=.45, p\.05). In addition, positive affect and purpose commitment
were positively related (r=.49, p\.05). Moreover, these magnitudes changed little when
controlling for age, gender, or racial status (white or minority). These potential control
variables were unrelated to T1 or T2 levels of grit, and thus were not considered further in
the analyses.
None of the three variables exhibited significant mean-level change (all t’s \1.6,
p’s [.05, difference scores across the waves reported below), as may be expected given
the relatively short time frame, and lack of an intervention. However, individual-level
change was apparent in this sample. Over the semester, 19 % of participants changed at
least one standard deviation (using T1 metrics) on grit (M
DIFF
=.00), along with 31 % for
similar changes on positive affect (M
DIFF
=-.07) and 18 % for purpose commitment
(M
DIFF
=-.03).
9.2 Longitudinal Relations between Purpose and Grit
Next, we fit a cross-lagged model in MPlus 7.1 (Muthe
´n and Muthe
´n1998–2012) to
examine whether purpose and grit were linked longitudinally, with respect either to pre-
dictive or correlated change effects, represented in Fig. 1. Given the sample size, manifest
variables were employed instead of latent ones to avoid issues associated with having very
few participants relative to the number of parameters to be estimated with a latent model.
The cross-lagged model allows us to examine (a) whether initial levels of one construct
predict end-of-semester scores on another construct, controlling for initial levels of the
second construct, and (b) if residualized change scores between any two constructs cor-
relate, which would serve as evidence that the two tend to change together.
P. L. Hill et al.
123
Results are presented in the top half of Fig. 1. The primary results of interest are with
respect to the cross-lag predictions and the T2 correlation. Initial levels of grit failed to
predict end-of-semester levels of purpose commitment (B =.11), but purpose commit-
ment at T1 predicted higher grit at T2 (B =.21, p\.05) even when controlling for initial
levels. Moreover, the residualized scores at T2 were significantly correlated (r=.30,
p\.05), suggesting the potential that changes on these constructs co-occurred over the
course of the semester (i.e., that they change in tandem).
9.3 Longitudinal Relations Between Positive Affect and Grit
Figure 1, bottom panel, presents the same model fit for positive affect instead of purpose
commitment. Neither cross-lagged effects reached significance, although the T2 correla-
tion between residualized scores was significantly positive (r=.26, p\.05). In other
words, while initial levels of either construct failed to predict change on the other variable,
there was evidence that these variables may change in tandem.
9.4 Comparing Purpose and Positive Affect as Predictors of Grit
Finally, we again performed a multiple regression analysis to investigate the unique roles
of purpose and positive affect in predicting levels of grit; results are presented in the
middle and right sections of Table 2. Using the T1 data, results again suggested unique
significant roles for both purpose (B =.28) and positive affect (B =.30, both p’s \.05).
However, only purpose (B =.24) and not positive affect (B =-.14) significantly pre-
dicted T2 levels of grit, when controlling for initial levels. Therefore, both variables appear
uniquely predictive of grit when examined concurrently, but, similar to the findings from
the cross-lagged models, only purpose commitment appears predictive of changes in grit
across the semester.
Fig. 1 Results of cross-lagged models in Study 2 for examining the prospective relations between grit and
purpose commitment (top half) or positive affect (bottom half) across the semester. Regression coefficients
are reported as unstandardized betas with the standard errors in parentheses
An Examination of Purpose Commitment and Positive Affect
123
10 Discussion
The current studies sought to examine two potential correlates and catalysts for the
development of grit, a disposition demonstrated to predict success across a number of
important life domains (Duckworth et al. 2007; Duckworth and Quinn 2009; Eskreis-
Winkler et al. 2014). Namely, we examined whether individuals could build perseverance
from a foundation of positive affect or purpose commitment. Across the studies, three
findings were of particular importance. First, grittier individuals tended to report higher
levels of both purpose commitment and positive affect, establishing the basic relations
among these constructs, even when controlling for broader personality traits. Second, we
found that these effects held across two samples, using college students from the United
States and Canada to test these claims. Third, our cross-lagged models suggest that changes
in either positive affect or purpose commitment are likely to coincide with changes in grit.
The broader implications of our findings provide important advances to research on
purpose, positive affect, and grit. Indeed, this study again points to the value of finding a
purpose and direction for one’s life. The current findings provide evidence that committing
to a purpose in life may encourage individuals to develop those characteristics that help
them to achieve their long-term aims, such as a gritty disposition. Though this finding
might appear immediately intuitive, it is worth noting that this need not have been the case,
as grit could be easily demonstrated across multiple long-term pursuits (e.g., getting a
degree or job), without having a sense that these pursuits form a meaningful, self-defining
directive. For instance, while grittier students have been shown to perform better in a
spelling bee context (Duckworth et al. 2007), it is possible but not necessarily the case that
individuals view spelling performance as part of their direction for life in order for this
effect to occur. As such, it is a particularly valuable step for future research to have
demonstrated that purposeful individuals tend to score higher on this adaptive specific trait.
Purpose in life has been linked to having a more adaptive personality profile (e.g., being
conscientious, emotionally stable, etc.; Hill and Burrow 2012; Scheier et al. 2006; Siegler
and Brummett 2000). However the current work is among the first studies to look at the
relationship between purpose and personality traits longitudinally, though focused on grit
as the trait of interest. As such, Study 2 is encouraging for future research that characterizes
purpose in life as a catalyst rather than simply an outcome of development. Instead of
being viewed as ‘‘merely’’ a component of psychological well-being, our findings support
the notion that purpose serves a force that ‘‘organizes and stimulates goals, manages
behaviors, and provides a sense of meaning’’ (McKnight and Kashdan 2009). This research
provides another foothold from which to consider purpose commitment as a source for self-
agency and self-development.
In addition, these findings follow recent research on personality development and well-
being by providing an initial examination of how positive affect could induce greater
perseverance with time. While the prospective effects failed to reach significance, this is
potentially due to the short timeframe and small sample size. That said, it is important to
note we found initial evidence for a correlated change effect. Similar to past work, it
appears that personality and well-being may manifest with reciprocal relationships (Soto
2013), or at least change in tandem. Moreover, it will be valuable to examine the gener-
alizability of the effects, and whether they are specific to given aspects of subjective well-
being. An alternative approach would be to focus on the ‘‘broaden’’ notion of Fredrickson’s
(2001) perspective and test whether individuals higher on positive affect are better at
applying their grit across different domains, instead of simply whether they report higher
levels overall.
P. L. Hill et al.
123
Finally, these results provide valuable contributions to research on grit, above and
beyond demonstrating two potential concurrent and prospective correlates. First, Study 1
provided further evidence that the positives associated with grit are not merely reducible to
its conceptual and empirical links with the Big Five (see also Duckworth et al. 2007). In
line with the developmental focus of the current research, though, it remains a question for
future research to examine how grit fluctuates in tandem with the Big Five, particularly
with respect to whether it demonstrates unique trajectories with conscientiousness and its
facets. Second, the current study provides one of the first investigations into whether and
how grit fluctuates over time, demonstrating that even over the course of a semester,
students report reliable changes on the trait. That said, we also provide some initial evi-
dence that the dispositional trait, as one would expect, retains high rank-order consistency
over the span of a few months. Third, it provides some insights into how educators can help
their students increase on the disposition, indicating that it might prove more valuable to
help them commit to life goals than merely bolstering their well-being. The college years
may prove particularly valuable in this respect, as it is a period where students begin to
winnow down their options for potential life goals (e.g., Lu
¨dtke et al. 2009).
However, our studies are not without limitations. First, it would be valuable to replicate
Study 2 with a larger, more diverse sample with at least three time points, in order to allow
for broader generalizations, and more sophisticated methods (e.g., latent growth modeling)
for analyzing longitudinal change. Second, a more thorough investigation of the positive
emotions hypothesis could include a lengthier measure of positive affect, which more fully
assesses specific affects. That said, it is worth noting that in post hoc analyses, our findings
suggest that most of PANAS items were positively correlated with grit, suggesting that the
trait is positively associated with multiple specific affects. Third, it would be valuable to
supplement these findings with more objective markers of grit (e.g., performance on tasks
that require persistence), as well as test whether such outcomes are influenced after
experimentally manipulating a sense of purpose, in order to make stronger causal argu-
ments. That said, it is unlikely that alternative approaches would lead to different results,
and it remains an open question in general how reliably these constructs can be assessed
using alternative methods. These caveats aside, the current studies provide several initial
insights into the unique correlates of grit and its development over a semester. Specifically,
when developing grit, it appears as important if not more to know the direction one is
going, than to have a base of positive emotion from which to embark.
Acknowledgments This research was funded in part through an Insight Development Grant awarded to
the first and second authors by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (Award
Number: 430-2013-000029).
References
Bronk, K. C. (2013). Purpose in life: A component of optimal youth development. New York: Springer.
Bronk, K. C., Hill, P., Lapsley, D., Talib, T., & Finch, W. H. (2009). Purpose, hope, and life satisfaction in
three age groups. Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 500–510.
Bundick, M., Andrews, M., Jones, A., Mariano, J. M., Bronk, K. C., & Damon, W. (2006). Revised youth
purpose survey. Stanford, CA: Unpublished instrument, Stanford Center on Adolescence.
Burrow, A. L., O’Dell, A. C., & Hill, P. L. (2010). Profiles of a developmental asset: Youth purpose as a
context for hope and well-being. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(11), 1265–1273.
Crumbaugh, J. C., & Maholick, L. T. (1967). An experimental study in existentialism: The psychometric
approach to Frankl’s concept of noogenic neurosis. In V. E. Frankl (Ed.), Psychotherapy and exis-
tentialism (pp. 183–197). New York: Washington Square Press.
An Examination of Purpose Commitment and Positive Affect
123
Damon, W., Menon, J., & Bronk, K. C. (2003). The development of purpose during adolescence. Applied
Developmental Science, 7, 119–128.
Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., & Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for
long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 1087–1101.
Duckworth, A. L., & Quinn, P. D. (2009). Development and validation of the Short Grit Scale (Grit-S).
Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 166–174.
Eskreis-Winkler, L., Duckworth, A. L., Shulman, E. P., & Beal, S. (2014). The grit effect: Predicting
retention in the military, the workplace, school, and marriage. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 36.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build
theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
Hill, P. L., & Burrow, A. L. (2012). Viewing purpose through an Eriksonian lens. Identity, 12, 74–91.
Hill, P. L., Burrow, A. L., Brandenberger, J. W., Lapsley, D. K., & Quaranto, J. C. (2010). Collegiate
purpose orientations and well-being in early and middle adulthood. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 31(2), 173–179.
Hill, P. L., Burrow, A. L., & Sumner, R. A. (2013). Addressing important questions in the field of adolescent
purpose. Child Development Perspectives, 7(4), 232–236.
Hudson, N. W., Roberts, B. W., & Lodi-Smith, J. L. (2012). Personality trait development and social
investment in work. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 334–344.
Jackson, J. J., Thoemmes, F., Jonkmann, K., Lu
¨dtke, O., & Trautwein, U. (2012). Military training and
personality trait development: Does the military make the man, or does the man make the military?
Psychological Science, 23, 270–277.
John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative big five taxonomy. In O.
P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed.,
pp. 114–158). New York: Guilford Press.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical
perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd
ed., pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press.
Keyes, C. L. M., Shmotkin, D., & Ryff, C. D. (2002). Optimizing well-being: The empirical encounter of
two traditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 1007–1022.
Lu
¨dtke, O., Trautwein, U., & Husemann, N. (2009). Goal and personality development in a transitional
period: Assessing change and stability in personality development. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 35, 428–441.
McKnight, P. E., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Purpose in life as a system that creates and sustains health and
well-being: An integrative, testable theory. Review of General Psychology, 13, 242–251.
Muthe
´n, L. K., & Muthe
´n, B. O. (1998–2012). Mplus user’s guide, 7th. Los Angeles, CA: Muthe
´n and
Muthe
´n.
Roberts, B. W., Lejuez, C., Krueger, R. F., Richards, J. M., & Hill, P. L. (2014). What is conscientiousness
and how can it be assessed? Developmental Psychology, 50, 1407–1425.
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081.
Scheier, M. F., Wrosch, C., Baum, A., Cohen, S., Martire, L. M., Matthews, K., et al. (2006). The life
engagement test: Assessing purpose in life. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29, 291–298.
Schmutte, P. S., & Ryff, C. D. (1997). Personality and well-being: Reexamining methods and meanings.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 549–559.
Siegler, I. C., & Brummett, B. H. (2000). Associations among NEO personality assessments and well-being
at midlife: Facet-level analyses. Psychology and Aging, 15, 710–714.
Silvia, P. J., Eddington, K. M., Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., & Kwapil, T. R. (2013). Gritty people try
harder: Grit and effort-related cardiac autonomic activity during an active coping challenge. Inter-
national Journal of Psychophysiology, 88, 200–205.
Singh, K., & Jha, S. D. (2008). Positive and negative affect, and grit as predictors of happiness and life
satisfaction. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology, 34, 40–45.
Soto, C. J. (2013). Is happiness good for your personality? Concurrent and prospective relations of the Big
Five with subjective well-being. Journal of Personality. doi:10.1111/jopy.12081.
Specht, J., Egloff, B., & Schmulke, S. C. (2013). Examining mechanisms of personality maturation: The
impact of life satisfaction on the development of the Big Five personality traits. Social Psychology and
Personality Science, 4, 181–189.
Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-
being. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 138–161.
Steger, M. F., Frazier, T. B., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The Meaning in Life Questionnaire: Assessing
the presence of and search for meaning in life. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 80–93.
P. L. Hill et al.
123
Tough, P. (2012). How children succeed: Grit, curiosity, and the hidden power of character. Houghton
Mifflin: New York.
Von Culin, K. R., Tsukayama, E., & Duckworth, A. L. (2014). Unpacking grit: Motivational correlates of
perseverance and passion for long-term goals. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 9, 306–312.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive
and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
An Examination of Purpose Commitment and Positive Affect
123
... Drawing on the broaden-and-build theory as a guiding framework (Fredrickson, 2001;Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005), we develop a model that examines how grit influences venture performance through bricolage. We posit that grit, which is associated with positive affectthat enable entrepreneurs to keep moving forward despite setbacks (Datu et al., 2017;Hill et al., 2016), can broaden an individual's thought-action repertoire and facilitatecreative use of resources (that is, bricolage) (Fredrickson, 2001;Fredrickson, 2013). We also examine how IU moderates the relationship between grit and bricolage. ...
... Specifically, Fredrickson (2004Fredrickson ( ,p. 1369 statesthat positive emotions such as joy and interest expand individual thinking and behaviours, creating an urge "to play, push the limits and be creative" and "explore, take in new information and experiences, and expand the self."This widens the range of perspective and opens individuals to new possibilities and ideas, allowing them to explore new opportunities and activities, leading to growth and development.In contrast, negative emotions like fear and anger tend to narrow the focus and lead to more rigid, entrenched thinking andbehaviour.Individuals possessing grit have a positive disposition which allows them to persist despite encountering difficulties and setbacks (Datu et al., 2017;Hill et al., 2016). This not only helps them cope with challenges (Tugade et al., 2004) but also enables them to creatively solve problems (Isen, 2015). ...
... In contrast, entrepreneurs with grit are proactive, driven, and competitive (Houston et al., 2021), thereby demonstrating a predisposition for action and motivation to overcome problems, even in the absence of resources (Senyard et al., 2014). Additionally, entrepreneurs who possess grit tend to have positive disposition that allows them to overcome challenges and setbacks (Datu et al., 2017;Hill et al., 2016). This positive disposition also broadens their thought-action repertoire (Fredrickson, 2004). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates the relationship between grit, bricolage, and venture performance in a sample of 230 entrepreneurs with chronic diseases and physical disabilities. Building on the broaden-and-build theory, we posit that grit influences venture performance through bricolage and that the effect of grit on bricolage is contingent upon varying levels of illness uncertainty (IU). The results demonstrate broad support for our proposed hypotheses. This study contributes to the entrepreneurship literature by examining the relationship between grit and venture performance through bricolage. Furthermore, we examine the moderating effect of IU on this relationship, Our findings have implications for both practitioners and policymakers.
... thus, purpose provides clarity about the reason for which goal is being pursued. the linkage of purpose with grit has been emphasized in previous literature (hill et al., 2016;Miller, 2017). Patience helps suspend temptations and the desires associated with sensory experiences by not falling for what seems to be a source of immediate gratification. ...
Article
Full-text available
The hitherto research on grit (i.e. passion and perseverance for long-term goal achievement) and its measurement, in psychological science, suffers from the issues of dimensionality and the possibility of different componential structures being present in collectivist culture vis-a-vis individualist culture. To address this, the present study aims to explore the factors influencing long-term goal achievement, an important aspect of the science of grit. The study further aims to examine the nature of grit in terms of its constituents from a qualitative perspective. A total of 48 respondents including five positive psychology experts and 43 higher education students from the Indian educational institutions took part in the study. Two different sets of open-ended questionnaires were used to gather data from the experts and the students. Inductive content analysis was used for analyzing the qualitative data. The findings revealed that both intrinsic factors (personal strengths, motivational factors, and goal factors) and extrinsic factors (socio-economic factors and familial factors) were essential for achieving long-term goals. Further, the components of grit included four constant strengths of perseverance, passion, purpose, and patience, and one variable strength of psychological flexibility. The present study contributes to the understanding of grit and long-term goal achievement from an in-depth qualitative perspective, broadening the current understanding of grit comprising of just perseverance and passion facets. The current study being a novel endeavor offers insights into the various possible constituents of grit existing in a collectivist setting which differ from the individualist context. Such a comprehensive understanding would enable the intervention developers to consider grit in its totality instead of just limiting to the original two-factor structure; while taking into account the role of personal, familial, and socio-economic factors in exhibiting the success behavior.
... Grit also serves as a buffer against burnout, as students with higher grit levels are more resilient and better equipped to manage academic stress. They are less likely to experience emotional exhaustion or disengagement [31,109]. However, academic burnout can erode grit over time, diminishing students' passion and perseverance [22,25]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the relationships among Foreign Language Learning Enjoyment (FLLE), L2 grit, academic burnout, and motivated behavior in English as a Foreign Language (EFL) learners, using a mixed-methods approach. A total of 534 Chinese undergraduate EFL learners participated in the quantitative phase, which used a cross-sectional survey, followed by narrative inquiry with 15 participants in the qualitative phase. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) analysis revealed that FLLE positively influenced motivated behavior while reducing academic burnout. L2 grit mediated the relationships between FLLE, burnout, and motivation, indicating that enjoyment promotes perseverance and resilience. Burnout was found to negatively impact motivation, underscoring the role of emotional well-being in maintaining engagement. The qualitative findings further supported these results, offering insights into learners’ experiences of joy, perseverance, and recovery from burnout. Participants described how moments of enjoyment reignited their long-term motivation, while grit helped them persevere through challenges. Burnout was linked to emotional exhaustion, but positive experiences often helped restore motivation. These findings offer a deeper understanding of the emotional and psychological factors influencing language learning success. The study provides important implications for educators, highlighting the need to create positive learning environments, promote resilience, and address burnout to support students’ sustained commitment to language learning.
... Said differently, those with a strong sense of purpose in life are likely to set meaningful aims which in turn helps facilitate adaptive self-regulation techniques as they work towards said aims, something that is highly relevant in the context of university . Purposeful university students are more likely to report high levels of perseverance as well as increases in perseverance over the course of a semester (Hill, Burrow & Bronk, 2016;. A sense of purpose is also associated with students reporting more positive relationships with others (Pfund et al., 2022, better emotion regulation strategies over the first semester of university (Lohani et al., 2022), and more adaptive relationships with parents when they are away at university . ...
Article
Full-text available
Individual differences are important predictors of academic success. A sense of purpose in life is gaining increasing attention as a key individual difference factor to foster in university students. The current study examined whether a sense of purpose in life, a dispositional tendency to pursue goals and activities in line with one’s overarching life direction, predicted better academic success across several years of university. Students (n = 769) at a large, U.S. public university were asked to complete a baseline survey in the summer prior to entering university, which included measures for a sense of purpose and background characteristics. Students were then followed throughout their first three years of university. Results demonstrated that higher levels of purpose were associated with a higher grade point average (GPA), more credits earned, less credits dropped, and an increased odds of persisting through the first three years of university. A sense of purpose also appeared to buffer the negative effect of low entrance scores on university GPA. These findings support cultivating a strong sense of purpose prior to entering university as an effective means of improving a variety of academic outcomes.
Article
Research on Quality of Life (QOL) encompasses a variety of dimensions, with some studies examining the factors that influence QOL and others focusing on methods to enhance it. One of the challenges in this field is understanding how to improve QOL across different economic contexts. Within this spectrum, capability stands out as a key determinant of QOL. The substantial correlation between capability and QOL has been established by numerous previous studies. This research primarily explores aspects related to capability. The goals of this study were manifold. First, we aimed to investigate the link between poverty and various capability variables. Second, we intended to assess the relationship between capability and QOL. The third objective was to introduce grit as a mediator in the capability-QOL relationship. Lastly, we explored the comparison between two classes within the overall model. The Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) analysis indicated that capability variables do have an impact on QOL in the baseline model. However, when comparing models, the one representing individuals under the poverty line showed no significant relationship between capability and QOL. The introduction of a mediation variable led to the identification of a fully mediated model in the context of poverty within this study. The distinctive contribution of this research is highlighted by the mediation findings between the two models, underscoring the nuanced interplay between capability, grit, and QOL in the backdrop of economic challenges.
Article
Full-text available
Article type: Research Article The current research endeavored to discern factors leading to the sense of purpose among male elites in school and university. The study implemented a grounded theory research design with the objective of conceptualizing the ordering process, utilizing a constructivist worldview and a qualitative approach. The investigation concentrated on males aged 25 to 45 who were members of the Iranian elite community and resided primarily in Tehran. Data were gathered through 2022 to 2023 through online or in-person semi-structured interviews with sixteen participants who were purposively selected. The Charmaz method was employed to execute analysis and coding in two stages: initial and focused coding, which was facilitated by the MAXQDA 2018 software. In general, three primary themes were identified, along with their respective categories and subcategories: Diversity, effective relationships, and the experience of agency. Effective relationships are defined as the dynamics and individuals that exist within the context of the purposeful individual, including teachers and professors. Taking responsibility in school and university is an example of the experience of agency, which is characterized by self-efficacy and a sense of influence. Experiencing diversity, the final term, denotes an understanding of various scientific and practical disciplines. The results obtained indicate that in order to establish an environment that is conducive to purposefulness, schools and universities must offer contexts that incorporate a variety of scientific, practical, and relational experiences. This will encourage their audience to engage in problem-solving and activation within each domain, thereby enabling students to develop a more autonomous level of self-awareness and contextual understanding, as well as to establish meaningful objectives and actively pursue them.
Article
Meaning in life protects individuals from mental distress during social upheaval. We posit that a growth mindset and consistency of interest positively predict meaning in life during social upheaval. The present research tested the hypothesis that among adolescence living in a period of social upheaval, the presence of a growth mindset (the belief in malleability of valued personal attributes) positively predicts persistent engagement in purpose‐congruent interests (consistency of interest), which in turn positively predicts the feeling that life is meaningful (presence of meaning in life). To test this hypothesis and to clarify the temporal causal connections among growth mindset, consistency of interest, and presence of meaning in life, we conducted a 3‐wave longitudinal cross‐lagged panel study with 275 Hong Kong adolescents between late 2017 and early 2020, which covered a period of social upheaval. As hypothesized, having a growth mindset predicted meaning in life two years later through the mediating effect of consistency of interest. These findings extended past findings by identifying a new causal pathway for the temporal causal effects of a growth mindset on the presence of meaning in life over two years in a population and context with high vulnerability to mental distress. (193 words).
Article
Full-text available
In a high school classroom, there are many factors that may influence academic achievement. One such factor may be due to the grit of individual learners. While much of the literature related to grit is focused on deficit ideological elements, structural elements, which are often overlooked, may also be present and could impact a student’s ability to be ‘gritty’ and successful in school. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to understand, from a teacher’s perspective, whether these structural elements, in addition to deficit elements, also impact student achievement. This autoethnographic study explores the culture of grit and student success in relation to three former students enrolled in Grades 11 and/or 12 Physics as they progress in their coursework. While deficit ideological elements exist within my autoethnographic narratives, structural ideological elements also implicate crucial moments when a student’s grit and success either radically improved or declined. Consequently, for those who support learners, the argument put forth in this paper suggests that being mindful of structural circumstances is essential if educators are to use grit to reinforce achievement. Keywords: grit, student success, high school, physics, deficit ideology, structural ideology
Article
Full-text available
Positive and Negative Affect, and Grit as predictors of Happiness and Life Satisfaction Kamlesh Singh and Shalini Duggal Jha This study explores the relationship between the concepts of Happiness, Life Satisfaction, Positive and Negative Affect and Grit. Happiness is the average level of satisfaction over a specific period, the frequency and degree of positiveaffect manifestations or the extent to which an individual experiences positive emotional states, and the relative absence of negative affect. Life Satisfaction is one’s evaluation of satisfaction with life in general and Grit refers to the character strength of perseverance described in Positive Psychology. The study was carried out on 254 undergraduate students of technology. Results revealed that the concepts of Grit, Positive Affect, Happiness and Life Satisfaction are significantly positively correlated. Negative Affect showed a significantly negative correlation with Grit, Happiness and Life Satisfaction. Stepwise regression analysis showed that Positive Affect, Grit and Negative Affect together account for 19% of the variance in Life Satisfaction. Grit, Negative Affect and Positive Affect account for 11% of the total variance in Happiness
Article
Full-text available
In two cross-sectional studies, we explored the motivational orientations correlates of the character strength of grit and its two component facets: perseverance of effort and consistency of interests over time. Specifically, we examined how individual differences in grit are explained by distinct approaches to pursuing happiness in life: pleasure in immediately hedonically positive activities, meaning in activities that serve a higher, altruistic purpose, and engagement in attention-absorbing activities. In both samples, grit demonstrated medium-sized associations with an orientation toward engagement, small-to-medium associations with an orientation toward meaning, and small-to-medium (inverse) associations with an orientation toward pleasure. These motivational orientations differentially related to the two facets of grit: pursuing engagement was more strongly associated with perseverance of effort, whereas pursuing pleasure was more strongly (inversely) associated with consistency of interests over time. Collectively, findings suggest that individual differences in grit may derive in part from differences in what makes people happy.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, the author describes a new theoretical perspective on positive emotions and situates this new perspective within the emerging field of positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory posits that experiences of positive emotions broaden people's momentary thought-action repertoires, which in turn serves to build their enduring personal resources, ranging from physical and intellectual resources to social and psychological resources. Preliminary empirical evidence supporting the broaden-and-build theory is reviewed, and open empirical questions that remain to be tested are identified. The theory and findings suggest that the capacity to experience positive emotions may be a fundamental human strength central to the study of human flourishing.
Article
Full-text available
Remaining committed to goals is necessary (albeit not sufficient) to attaining them, but very little is known about domain-general individual differences that contribute to sustained goal commitment. The current investigation examines the association between grit, defined as passion and perseverance for long-term goals, other individual difference variables, and retention in four different contexts: the military, workplace sales, high school, and marriage. Grit predicted retention over and beyond established context-specific predictors of retention (e.g., intelligence, physical aptitude, Big Five personality traits, job tenure) and demographic variables in each setting. Grittier soldiers were more likely to complete an Army Special Operations Forces (ARSOF) selection course, grittier sales employees were more likely to keep their jobs, grittier students were more likely to graduate from high school, and grittier men were more likely to stay married. The relative predictive validity of grit compared to other traditional predictors of retention is examined in each of the four studies. These findings suggest that in addition to domain-specific influences, there may be domain-general individual differences which influence commitment to diverse life goals over time.
Article
In this article, the author describes a new theoretical perspective on positive emotions and situates this new perspective within the emerging field of positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory posits that experiences of positive emotions broaden people's momentary thought-action repertoires, which in turn serves to build their enduring personal resources, ranging from physical and intellectual resources to social and psychological resources. Preliminary empirical evidence supporting the broaden-and-build theory is reviewed, and open empirical questions that remain to be tested are identified. The theory and findings suggest that the capacity to experience positive emotions may be a fundamental human strength central to the study of human flourishing.
Article
This volume integrates and makes sense of the growing body of theoretical and empirical research conducted on purpose across the lifespan. It opens with a comprehensive yet detailed discussion of the definitions of purpose most commonly used in studies on the topic. In addition to defining the construct, the author also discusses its philosophical roots and distinguishes it from related concepts, including meaning, goals, and ultimate concerns. This volume discusses the disparate perspectives on the construct and addresses the tendency to position purpose in the broader frame of positive psychology. It synthesizes distinct strands of research on purpose across the lifespan, it explores studies on the daily and longer-term experience of a purposeful existence, and it delves deeply into the wide range of measurement tools that have been used to assess the purpose construct. Further, it examines the prevalence and forms of purpose among diverse groups of youth and discusses the developmental trajectory of the construct. Other topics discussed include the central role of purpose in supporting optimal well-being and positive youth development. The book closes with empirically-supported steps adults, educators, and mentors can take to effectively and intentionally foster purpose among young people and makes recommendations for future research on the topic. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
In this review, we discuss three questions about the importance of studying purpose as a construct relevant to adolescence. We consider how finding a purpose can help youth answer questions such as “Who am I?” and “Where am I going?” and summarize the benefits of finding a purpose during adolescence. Purposeful youth are more agentic and driven in their lives, as well as happier and less susceptible to risks common in adolescence. We conclude by identifying areas that merit study, including the need to understand which contexts best scaffold the development of purpose among youth and the need for experimental research to strengthen causal claims.