ArticlePublisher preview available

The Emotions Sumanai, Gratitude, and Indebtedness, and Their Relations to Interpersonal Orientation and Psychological Well-Being Among Japanese University Students

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Sumanai is a Japanese word that refers to a feeling in response to a favor; it includes gratitude, along with sorrow and sometimes guilt for having put the benefactor to so much trouble. The purpose of the current research was to explore sumanai, gratitude, and indebtedness, and their relations to interpersonal orientation and psychological well-being. Results from Study 1 with 115 female Japanese university students confirmed the distinct meanings of gratitude, sumanai, and indebtedness in terms of their dissimilar correlations with other feelings. The results also revealed that when the benefactor’s expectation for repayment was manipulated, gratitude and sumanai, but not indebtedness, decreased with increasing benefactor expectations. In Study 2 with a sample of 179 female Japanese university students, sumanai had a significant positive correlation with interpersonal orientation but did not have a significant positive correlation with psychological well-being. Interpersonal orientation mediated the relation between indebtedness and psychological well-being, but did not mediate the relation between gratitude and psychological well-being. Gratitude had significant direct paths to psychological well-being and interpersonal orientation. An alternative model was also supported; gratitude mediated the relation between interpersonal orientation and psychological well-being. The contribution of gratitude to well-being and interpersonal orientation are consistent with the extant literature, pointing to gratitude’s ability to build and maintain social relationships.
The Emotions Sumanai, Gratitude, and Indebtedness, and Their
Relations to Interpersonal Orientation and Psychological Well-Being
Among Japanese University Students
Naoko Washizu and Takashi Naito
Ochanomizu University
Sumanai is a Japanese word that refers to a feeling in response to a favor; it includes
gratitude, along with sorrow and sometimes guilt for having put the benefactor to so
much trouble. The purpose of the current research was to explore sumanai, gratitude,
and indebtedness, and their relations to interpersonal orientation and psychological
well-being. Results from Study 1 with 115 female Japanese university students con-
firmed the distinct meanings of gratitude, sumanai, and indebtedness in terms of their
dissimilar correlations with other feelings. The results also revealed that when the
benefactor’s expectation for repayment was manipulated, gratitude and sumanai, but
not indebtedness, decreased with increasing benefactor expectations. In Study 2 with a
sample of 179 female Japanese university students, sumanai had a significant positive
correlation with interpersonal orientation but did not have a significant positive corre-
lation with psychological well-being. Interpersonal orientation mediated the relation
between indebtedness and psychological well-being, but did not mediate the relation
between gratitude and psychological well-being. Gratitude had significant direct paths
to psychological well-being and interpersonal orientation. An alternative model was
also supported; gratitude mediated the relation between interpersonal orientation and
psychological well-being. The contribution of gratitude to well-being and interpersonal
orientation are consistent with the extant literature, pointing to gratitude’s ability to
build and maintain social relationships.
Keywords: gratitude, indebtedness, moral emotion, interpersonal orientation, psychological
well-being
When receiving favors from others, people
may feel a variety of feelings such as happiness,
gratitude, and indebtedness. The current article
focuses on the interrelations among those emo-
tions, and a similar emotion sumanai, that may
be uniquely Japanese. In addition, we explored
the relations of those emotions with other vari-
ables, such as interpersonal orientation and psy-
chological well-being.
Here, we define the feelings of gratitude,
indebtedness, and the Japanese word sumanai.
We refer to gratitude as “a positive emotional
reaction to the receipt of a benefit that is per-
ceived to have resulted from the good intentions
of another” (Tsang, 2006a, p. 139). Indebted-
ness is “a state of obligation to repay another”
(Greenberg & Westcott, 1983, p. 85). Sumanai
is a Japanese word that is used for apology and
expressing thanks to others. Because of the
complex nature of the word, it is difficult to
clearly define the word, even in Japanese. We
tentatively define sumanai as a feeling of grat-
itude for someone’s help along with sorrow and,
sometimes guilt for having put them to so much
trouble.
The general purpose of the current article was
to examine the functions of the feelings of grat-
itude, indebtedness, and sumanai in Japanese
settings, where these three feelings are valued
and considered as moral emotions. The current
research is exploratory and aimed at examining
how the three feelings relate to psychological
well-being and interpersonal orientation. Inter-
personal orientation is defined as the “degree to
This article was published Online First May 4, 2015.
Naoko Washizu and Takashi Naito, Graduate School of
Humanities and Sciences, Ochanomizu University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Takashi Naito, Graduate School of Humanities
and Sciences, Ochanomizu University, 2-1–1, Otsuka, Bun-
kyoku, Tokyo, Japan. E-mail: naito.takashi@ocha.ac.jp
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, Consultation © 2015 American Psychological Association
2015, Vol. 4, No. 3, 209–222 2157-3883/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ipp0000037
209
... As further exploratory analyses, in GLM 2 we extracted and analyzed the parametric beta values of ROIs selected from previous studies on reciprocity in favor-receiving context 34,53 and reciprocity-related emotions, e.g., gratitude, 53 guilt 54 and obligation, 34 including left right dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC, peak MNI: À45, 5, 29), right DLPFC (peak MNI: 39,8,38); left inferior parietal lobule (IPL, peak MNI: À54, À40, 53), right IPL (peak MNI: 51, À28, 47), Right temporoparietal junction (TPJ, peak MNI: 48, À52, 31), left TPJ (À57, À61, 26), perigenual anterior cingulate cortex (pgACC, peak MNI: 9, 50, 1), ACC (peak MNI: À3, 20, 22), ACC (peak MNI: À5, 23, 28), anterior middle cingulate cortex (peak MNI: 0, 34, 16), dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (peak MNI: À9, 44, 41), left insula (peak MNI: À30, 16, 18), and right insula (peak MNI: 36, 30, À8). Following ANOVAs and correlation analyses were the same as GLM1. ...
... To examine the effects of TSD on reciprocity behavior depending not only on neural activities but also functional connectivities between brain regions, we performed PPI 55 using precuneus (peak MNI: À9, À64, 35), TPJ (peak MNI: 48, À52, 31) and DLPFC (peak MNI: 39,8,38) that were shown related to reciprocity behavior in the above analysis results as seed regions. Each seed was defined as a sphere with a radius of 3 mm centered on the coordinates of the peak point. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although the impact of sleep loss on social behaviors has been widely observed in recent years, the mechanisms underpinning these impacts remain unclear. In this study, we explored the detrimental effects of sleep deprivation on reciprocity behavior as well as its underlying psychological and neuroimaging mechanisms by combining sleep manipulation, an interpersonal interactive game, computational modeling and neuroimaging. Our results suggested that after sleep deprivation, individuals showed reduced reciprocity behavior, mainly due to their reduced weights on communal concern when making social decisions. At neural level, we demonstrated that sleep deprivation’s effects were observed in the precuneus (hyperactivity) and temporoparietal junction, dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (both hypoactivity), and reduced reciprocity was also accounted for by increased precuneus-thalamus connectivity and DLPFC-thalamus connectivity. Our findings contributed to the understanding of the psychological and neuroimaging bases underlying the deleterious impact of sleep deprivation on social behaviors.
... These intention inferences can produce different emotions in the beneficiary 39 . For example, if the benefactor's actions are perceived to be altruistic, the beneficiary may feel gratitude for receiving help, but this could also be accompanied by the feeling of guilt for personally burdening the benefactor [40][41][42][43] . Both feelings motivate reciprocity out of concern for the benefactor, which we refer to as "communal concern" throughout the paper 44,45 . ...
... Obligation can also motivate the beneficiary to reciprocate 13,14,21,46,47 , but unlike communal concern, it arises from external pressures, such as social expectations and reputational costs 48,49 and has been linked to feelings of pressure, burden, anxiety, and resentment [49][50][51] . Indebtedness has often been considered a unitary construct, defined singularly as either the feeling of guilt for personally burdening the benefactor [40][41][42][43] or as a sense of obligation to repay 13,14,21,46,47 . However, in everyday life, inferences about a benefactor's intentions are often mixed and we argue that indebtedness is a superordinate emotion that includes feelings of guilt for burdening the benefactor and social obligation to repay the favor. ...
Article
Full-text available
Receiving a favor from another person may induce a negative feeling of indebtedness for the beneficiary. In this study, we explore these hidden costs by developing and validating a conceptual model of indebtedness across three studies that combine a large-scale online questionnaire, an interpersonal game, computational modeling, and neuroimaging. Our model captures how individuals perceive the altruistic and strategic intentions of the benefactor. These inferences produce distinct feelings of guilt and obligation that together comprise indebtedness and motivate reciprocity. Perceived altruistic intentions convey care and communal concern and are associated with activity in insula, ventromedial prefrontal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, while inferred strategic intentions convey expectations of future reciprocity and are associated with activation in temporal parietal junction and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. We further develop a neural utility model of indebtedness using multivariate patterns of brain activity that captures the tradeoff between these feelings and reliably predicts reciprocity behavior.
... with Saudi Arabia being a collectivistic culture, it is possible that expressing gratitude may be accompanied by both positive and negative emotions-such as regret, indebtedness, and remorse-as it was found in collectivistic cultures [68,69]. This may explain why expressing gratitude may not directly lead to feelings of enjoyment but it seems that it does so through social support. ...
Article
Full-text available
This cross-sectional study aimed to investigate the relationship between gratitude and life satisfaction, and the mediation role of social support and enjoyment of life among older individuals. The measures employed include the Gratitude, Resentment, and Appreciation test; the Enjoyment of Life scale; the Satisfaction with Life scale; and the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. These were administered to a sample of 260 older individuals aged between 60 and 80. The main findings revealed a positive association between gratitude and life satisfaction, and social support was a partial mediator in this relationship. Although enjoyment of life was not a direct mediator in the relationship between gratitude and life satisfaction, the final model indicated a significant serial pathway from gratitude to social support and then through enjoyment of life to life satisfaction. In conclusion, enhancing gratitude, promoting the enjoyment of life, and social support to older individuals might increase their life satisfaction, which in turn might contribute to their successful aging.
... The mechanisms underlying this finding are to be found in the cultural patterns of Saudi Arabia. Saudi Arabia being a collectivistic culture, it is possible that expressing gratitude may be accompanied by both positive and negative emotions such as regret, indebtedness, and remorse as it was found in collectivistic cultures (Kimura, 1994;Washizu & Naito, 2015). This may explain why expressing gratitude may not directly lead to feelings of enjoyment, but it seems that it does so through social support. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This cross-sectional study was aimed to investigate the relationship between gratitude and life satisfaction, and the mediation role of social support and enjoyment of life among older individuals. The Gratitude Resentment and Appreciation Test, the Enjoyment of Life Scale, the Satisfaction with Life Scale, and the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support Scale were administered to a sample of 260 older individuals aged between 60 and 80. The main findings revealed a positive association between gratitude and life satisfaction, and social support was a partial mediator in this relationship. Although enjoyment of life was not a direct mediator in the relationship between gratitude and life satisfaction, the final model indicated a significant serial pathway from gratitude to social support and then through enjoyment of life-to-life satisfaction. In conclusion, enhancing gratitude, promoting enjoyment of life, and social support in older individuals might increase their life satisfaction, which in turn might contribute to their successful aging
Article
Gratitude interventions have been usually less effective for collectivistic cultures compared to individualistic cultures. The first study examined the impact of the target person in the efficacy of gratitude intervention in the Indian context. The second study examined whether conception of gratitude is similar between Indians and Anglo-Americans. In first study, 312 first-year undergraduate Indian students (Mage = 18.64, SD = 0.76) were randomly assigned to three groups. They wrote about a typical day in their life, gratitude toward a particular person of their choice and gratitude toward an immediate family member, respectively. Selected sub-scales of PANAS-X were administered before and after the intervention. In second study, 112 Indian participants (Mage = 27.63, SD = 5.82) and 80 Anglo-American (Mage = 38.48, SD = 11.94) participants took part in a survey whereby they needed to rate the centrality and positivity of some gratitude features. We found expressing gratitude toward an immediate family member was neither effective in increasing positive emotions nor effective in decreasing negative emotions. However, expressing gratitude toward any person of choice was effective in increasing positive emotions as well as decreasing negative emotions. Three prominent themes emerged for gratefulness toward any person, i.e., being an inspiration, providing a perspective in life, and emotional and financial support in success. Providing support to the hypotheses, indebtedness and its associated negative emotions were perceived more positively and more central to the concept of gratitude for Indian participants than Anglo-American participants.
Article
According to Yu et al. (2022, Study 2), beneficiaries are less likely to feel grateful toward benefactors who exhibit immoral behavior, indicating that gratitude is morally sensitive. As a preregistered conceptual replication study, the present study aimed to examine which specific immoral attribute of benefactors would weaken beneficiaries’ gratitude. The participants of this study were presented with a vignette, in which the benefactors offered to introduce a doctor to them who could help with their physical problems. Referring to moral foundation theory, we randomly assigned one of six moral traits of benefactors: immoral with respect to care, fairness, loyalty, authority, purity, or morally neutral. Preregistered analyses revealed that beneficiaries felt less grateful toward benefactors who were immoral with respect to loyalty and purity. Furthermore, immorality in those two moral foundations weakened beneficiaries’ other appraisals that were consistent with the findings of the original study, while immoral benefactors with respect to care significantly affected neither these appraisals nor gratitude. Results suggest that one should carefully discern the influence of certain kinds of immorality on gratitude.
Article
This study aimed to analyze whether sport-domain gratitude predicts the risk of burnout, even when controlling for positive affect. We administered a survey to 470 Japanese university athletes; The final sample comprised data from 412 athletes (men = 357, mean age =19.88 years, SD = 1.06). The participants completed questionnaires assessing burnout, positive affect, and sport-domain gratitude. A series of hierarchical multiple regressions was estimated to predict each of the following five dependent variables: burnout, interpersonal exhaustion, emotional exhaustion for athletic practice, lack of personal accomplishment (LPA), and devaluation toward club activity (DCA). Positive affect was entered in the first step; subsequently, sport-domain gratitude was entered in the second step, wherein burnout, LPA, and DCA were predicted by sport-domain gratitude. Our findings indicate that sport-domain gratitude has a distinct inhibitory effect on the risk of burnout that cannot be attributed to positive affect alone. Accordingly, coaches should promote not only to athletes’ feelings of positive affect in athletic life, but also feelings of gratitude in their competitive lives to reduce the risk of burnout.
Article
Zero-sum belief is the belief that someone’s gains are another’s losses. Assuming that beneficiaries’ zero-sum beliefs let them perceive benefactors’ cost resulting from giving benefits, this study examined whether the zero-sum belief increases the occurrences of grateful feelings and expression in apologetic form, which is represented by “sumimasen” in Japanese. We manipulated participants’ zero-sum beliefs and rewarded them for the task. Thereafter, we asked participants what they wanted to say, how they felt, and how much they perceived our (i.e., benefactors’) cost. The results revealed that participants whose zero-sum beliefs were experimentally strengthened were inclined to select the grateful expression in apologetic form from some options to convey what they wanted to say, though grateful feelings in apologetic form and perceived costs were not significantly affected. These results suggested the possibility that individuals’ zero-sum beliefs let them express their gratitude in apologetic form independently from the extent to which they have such feelings or perceive benefactors’ cost.
Article
Beneficiaries can have prior expectations of not receiving benefits and imagine ex‐post that benefits were not given. The present study examined the preregistered hypothesis that these two kinds of absence simulations increase gratitude. Both Studies 1 and 2 were conducted in a Japanese university. In Study 1, reading a manipulated vignette made participants in the absence simulation condition expect not to receive a benefit, while those in the control condition were made to expect to receive it. In Study 2, after reading a benefit‐receiving vignette, those in the absence simulation condition imagined the situation where they did not receive it, while those in the control condition again imagined receiving it. In both studies, all the participants rated their gratitude for certain fixed benefits in each vignette. Results showed that gratitude in neither of the two absence simulation conditions was higher than that of the respective control conditions, suggesting that simulating the absence of benefits does not increase gratitude.
Article
Full-text available
In four studies, the authors examined the correlates of the disposition toward gratitude. Study 1 revealed that self-ratings and observer ratings of the grateful disposition are associated with positive affect and well-being prosocial behaviors and traits, and religiousness/spirituality. Study 2 replicated these findings in a large nonstudent sample. Study 3 yielded similar results to Studies 1 and 2 and provided evidence that gratitude is negatively associated with envy and materialistic attitudes. Study 4 yielded evidence that these associations persist after controlling for Extraversion/positive affectivity, Neuroticism/negative affectivity, and Agreeableness. The development of the Gratitude Questionnaire, a unidimensional measure with good psychometric properties, is also described.
Article
The effect of a grateful outlook on psychological and physical well-being was examined. In Studies 1 and 2, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 experimental conditions (hassles, gratitude listing, and either neutral life events or social comparison); they then kept weekly (Study 1) or daily (Study 2) records of their moods, coping behaviors, health behaviors, physical symptoms, and overall life appraisals. In a 3rd study, persons with neuromuscular disease were randomly assigned to either the gratitude condition or to a control condition. The gratitude-outlook groups exhibited heightened well-being across several, though not all, of the outcome measures across the 3 studies, relative to the comparison groups. The effect on positive affect appeared to be the most robust finding. Results suggest that a conscious focus on blessings may have emotional and interpersonal benefits.
Article
Three studies were conducted to construct a scale to measure sensibilities to indebtedness and to examine the reliability and validity of the scale. In Study I, university students and adult non-students completed an original version of the Indebtedness Scale along with some other scales assessing tendencies theoretically correlated with sensibilities to indebtedness. Through item analyses, 18 items were selected as the final version of Indebtedness Scale (IS-18). The IS-18 showed a high internal consistency (α = .850) and a high test-retest reliability (r=.778). In Study I and Study II, significant correlations were observed between the IS-18 score and the measures of self-consciousness, formality ideology, social skills, and self-esteem. There was no correlation between the score and Social Desirability Scale. In Study III, university students placed themselves in the role of hypothetical students confronted with 7 different situations in which they had been helped from others. They answered a question regarding the magnitude of indebtedness in each situation. The total score of the magnitude of indebtedness in 7 situations was positively correlated with IS-18 score.
Article
本研究の目的は, 幅広い年代 (25~65歳) の成人女性の多様なライフスタイルについて, 複数の構成要素からなる心理的well-beingとの関連から検討することであった。まず研究1では, 成人期全般に適用でき, 理論的背景が確認されているRyffの概念に基づき, 人格的成長, 人生における目的, 自律性, 自己受容, 環境制御力, 積極的な他者関係の6次元を有する心理的well-being尺度が作成され, 6次元の信頼性・妥当性が確認された。また, 年代によって心理的well-beingの様相が異なり, 次元によっては発達的に変化することが示された。次に研究2では, ライフスタイル要因と心理的well-being各次元との関連について検討した。その主な結果は以下の通りである。(1) 年代と就労の有無, 社会活動参加度を独立変数, 心理的well-being各次元を従属変数とする分散分析を行った結果, 就労, 社会活動という家庭外での役割は, 成人女性の心理的well-beingとそれぞれ異なった形で関連していることが示された。特にこれまで家庭外役割としてほとんど焦点が当てられてこなかった社会活動が, 就労とは異なった形で心理的well-beingと強く関連していたことから, 成人女性の発達的特徴を考える際に, 就労以外の様々な活動にも目を向けることの必要性が示唆された。(2) 年代別に, 妻, 母親, 就労者, 活動者の各役割達成感と心理的well-being各次元との偏相関係数を検討した結果, 長期にわたる成人期においては, 各年代に応じた役割を獲得し, それによる達成感を得ることが心理的well-beingと強く関連することが明らかになった。この結果から, それぞれの役割の質的側面が成人女性のライフサイクルの中で異なった重要性を持つことが示唆された。