ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Several past studies have found that media reports of suicides and homicides appear to subsequently increase the incidence of similar events in the community, apparently due to the coverage planting the seeds of ideation in at-risk individuals to commit similar acts. Here we explore whether or not contagion is evident in more high-profile incidents, such as school shootings and mass killings (incidents with four or more people killed). We fit a contagion model to recent data sets related to such incidents in the US, with terms that take into account the fact that a school shooting or mass murder may temporarily increase the probability of a similar event in the immediate future, by assuming an exponential decay in contagiousness after an event. We find significant evidence that mass killings involving firearms are incented by similar events in the immediate past. On average, this temporary increase in probability lasts 13 days, and each incident incites at least 0.30 new incidents (p = 0.0015). We also find significant evidence of contagion in school shootings, for which an incident is contagious for an average of 13 days, and incites an average of at least 0.22 new incidents (p = 0.0001). All p-values are assessed based on a likelihood ratio test comparing the likelihood of a contagion model to that of a null model with no contagion. On average, mass killings involving firearms occur approximately every two weeks in the US, while school shootings occur on average monthly. We find that state prevalence of firearm ownership is significantly associated with the state incidence of mass killings with firearms, school shootings, and mass shootings.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Contagion in Mass Killings and School
Shootings
Sherry Towers
1
*, Andres Gomez-Lievano
1
, Maryam Khan
2
, Anuj Mubayi
1,2
,
Carlos Castillo-Chavez
1
1 Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA, 2 Northeastern Illinois University, Chicago, IL, USA
*
smtowers@asu.edu
Abstract
Background
Several past studies have found that media reports of suicides and homicides appear to
subsequently increase the incidence of similar events in the community, apparently due to
the coverage planting the seeds of ideation in at-risk individuals to commit similar acts.
Methods
Here we explore whether or not contagion is evident in more high-profile incidents, such as
school shootings and mass killings (incidents with four or more people killed). We fit a conta-
gion model to recent data sets related to such incidents in the US, with terms that take into
account the fact that a school shooting or mass murder may temporarily increase the proba-
bility of a similar event in the immediate future, by assuming an exponential decay in con-
tagiousness after an event.
Conclusions
We find significant evidence that mass killings involving firearms are incented by similar
events in the immediate past. On average, this temporary increase in probability lasts 13
days, and each incident incites at least 0.30 new incidents (p = 0.0015). We also find signifi-
cant evidence of contagion in school shootings, for which an incident is contagious for an
average of 13 days, and incites an average of at least 0.22 new incidents (p = 0.0001). All
p-values are assessed based on a likelihood ratio test compari ng the likelihood of a conta-
gion model to that of a null model with no contagion. On average, mass killings involving
firearms occur approximately every two weeks in the US, while school shootings occur on
average monthly. We find that state prevalence of firearm ownership is significantly associ-
ated with the state incidence of mass killings with firearms, school shootings, and mass
shootings.
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 1/12
a11111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Towers S, Gomez-Lievano A, Khan M,
Mubayi A, Castillo-Chavez C (2015) Contagion in
Mass Killings and School Shootings. PLoS ONE 10
(7): e0117259. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259
Academic Editor: Joshua Yukich, Tulane University
School of Public Health and Tropical Medicine,
UNITED STATES
Received: May 16, 2014
Accepted: December 19, 2014
Published: July 2, 2015
Copyright: © 2015 Towers et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original author and source are
credited.
Data Availability Statement: The USA Today mass
killings data are publicly available from masskillings.
usatoday.com. The Brady Campaign data on school
shootings and mass shootings are freely available on
request from that organization, who can be contacted
at (202) 370-8101. Data have also been included in
the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding: The authors have no support or funding to
report.
Competing Interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
In recent years, tragedies involving mass killings in the US, such as the Aurora, CO movie the-
ater shooting in July 2012, and the Newtown, CT school shooting in December 2012, have
intensified societal focus on trying to understand the dynamics and contributing factors that
underlie such events, particularly since the per capita incidence of such events and other fire-
arm related mortality is significantly higher in the US than in any other industrialized country
[
14].
Statistics are not readily available on the incidence of mass killings and school shootings in
other industrialized countries, however studies have shown that the firearm homicide and sui-
cide rates in the US are several times higher than that of any other industrialized country [
4],
and the patterns appear to be due to higher rates of firearm ownership in the US compared
with other industrialized countries [
5]. Overall, the pediatric firearm mortality rate is five times
higher in the United States compared with any other industrialized country [
3], and 87% of all
children age 0 to 14 worldwide killed by firearms are children living in the US [
4], despite the
fact that less than 5% of the worlds children live in the US [
6]. In September, 2014, the Federal
Bureau of Investigation (FBI) released a report A Study of Active Shooter Incidents in the
United States Between 2000 and 2013 [
7]. In their report, they note that the incidence of mass
shooting incidents has been growing over the past 14 years.
Ready access to weaponry has been implicated in these trends. For instance, it ha s been
found that two thirds of school shooters obtained their firearms from their own home, or the
home of a relative [
8], and youths are significantly more likely to die by firearm homicide or
suicide in states where gun ownership is more prevalent. In addition, household gun ownership
has been shown to significantly raise the risk for homicide in the home [
9], and homicide rates
in general [
10]. Studies have also shown that the more access people have to firearms, the lower
the levels of social trust, and the higher the levels of homicide [
11, 12]. A strong association has
also been found between overall state suicide rates and firearm ownership rates [
13, 14],
Access to mental health care has also been viewed as a factor in at least some types of inci-
dents. For example, it has been shown that access to health care and the state rates of federal
aid for mental health services are strongly associated with reduced state suicide rates [
15].
Beyond the exongenous factors like mental health issues and ease of access to weaponry that
may be contributing to the frequency of events, there is also the possibility that the stressed
individuals may have, consciously or sub-consciously, been inspired to act on previously sup-
pressed urges by exposure to details of similar events. Such contagious ideation is not implausi-
ble; for example, vulnerable youth have been found to be susceptible to suicide ideation
brought on by influence of reports and portrayal of suicide in mass media [
1618], and media
reports on suicides and homicides have been found to apparently subsequently increase the
incidence of similar incidents in the community [
19, 20]. In addtion, in a broader public health
context, the temporal and geospatial clustering of many types of violence and crime have been
shown to be similar to the patterns seen in the spread of infectious disease [
21].
To study this issue, we examine data on mass killings (four or more people murdered, by
any means) in the US between 2006 to 2013 compiled by USA Today (N = 232, 176 of which
involved firearms), data on school shootings from 1998 to 2013 inclusive from the Brady Cam-
paign to Prevent Gun Violence (N = 188), and data on mass shootings (at least three people
shot, not necessarily killed) from February 2005 to January 2013, also from the Brady Cam-
paign (N = 477).
We fit a mathematical contagion model to the data sets, with model terms that take into
account temporal trends due to possible exogenous non-contagion factors, and a contagion
term that takes into account the fact that a school shooting or mass murder may temporarily
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 2/12
increase the probability of a similar event in the immediate future. We model the contagion
process assuming an exponential decay in contagiousness after an event. Contagion models
have been applied to financial markets [
22], spread of YouTube videos on social networks [23],
burglary [
2427], civilian deaths in Iraq [28], and terrorist attacks [29, 30], but this is the first
instance in which these models have been applied in the context of mass murders and school
shootings.
Because contagion is just one potential aspect contributing to these events, we also examine
the relationship between the incidence of these tragedies, and state prevalence of mental illness
and firearm ownership, and the rankings of the strength of state firearm legislation.
In the following sections we describe the sources of data used in this analysis, and the
modeling and statistical data analysis methods used. We then present results, with discussion
and summary.
Methods
Data
Here we describe the mass killing and school shooting sources of data used in this analysis,
along with a description of the data sources use to examine the relationship of such events to
state prevalence of mental illness and firearm ownership, and estimates of the strength of state
gun laws. There are currently no comprehensive federal repositories of data on mass killings
and school shootings in the US, thus for these studies we relied on data compiled by private
organizations. Data are available at
http://tinyurl.com/oql4lpy.
USA Today mass killing data. Data on mass killings in the US between 2006 to December
2013 were obtained from a USA Today study that examined Federal Bureau of Investigation
(FBI) data from the FBI Supplemental Homicide Reports, and hundreds of media reports and
police documents to compile a list of incidents that involved four or more people being killed,
not including the killer (data are available from, and described at,
masskillings.usatoday.com,
accessed March 2014). The USA Today study did not rely solely upon the FBI data, in part
because the FBI data are based on voluntary reports by local police agencies and are thus an
incomplete tally of mass murders in the US, and also because the data were found to only have
a 61% accuracy rate when compared with data available from local police documents, appar-
ently largely due to mistakes in transcription. For more details about the USA Today data set,
see
www.usatoday.com/story/new s/nation/2013/12/03/fbi-mass-killing-data-inaccurate/
3666953/ (accessed January 30, 2015).
The data consist of 232 events, of which firearms were involved in 176 (76%). Of these 232
incidents, the authors found only three apparent errors, where the date was accidentally tran-
scribed incorrectly by one day. The dates were corrected for this analysis. The data are shown
in
Fig 1.
Incidents showed no significant dependence on month of the year, but were significantly
more likely to occur on Saturdays (Pearson χ
2
p = 0.04). For the 176 incidents that involved
mass killings with firearms, 46% of the killers committed suicide (either by th emselves or by
suicide by cop). This is significantly higher than the estimated 5% to 10% suicide rate of
killers in all murders [
31]. On average, 22% more people were killed (not including the killer)
in events where the murderer committed suicide (Student-t two-sided p = 0.038).
This is in contrast to the 56 events involving mass killings without firearms, where only 8%
of such killers committed suicide, and there was no significant difference in the number killed
when the murderer committed suicide (Student-t two-sided p = 0.74).
Brady Campaign School Shooting Data. Data on school shootings in the US were
obtained from the Brady Campaign to Prevent Gun Violence, who examined media reports to
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 3/12
compile a list of 220 school shootings in the US between 1997 to 2013, inclusive. We excluded
from this data set foiled plots where individuals were caught actively planning a shooting, but
had not yet carried out their plan, incidents that did not actually occur on a school campus or
school bus, or incidents that occurred outside of school hours or outside the context of school-
related events (such as sports games), leaving 188 events. Of these 188 events, upon review of
the media reports the authors found four incidents for which the date was apparently mis-tran-
scribed by one day, and the dates were corrected for this analysis. The school shootings resulted
in an average of one person being killed per incident; only six of the school shootings thus over-
lap with the USA Today mass killing data set, and the two data sets are nearly independent.
The data are shown in
Fig 1.
Fig 1. Number of mass killings, school shootings, and mass shootings over time for the data samples used in these studies. For clarity of
presentation, all data samples are shown over the same time frame. Overlaid is the fit of the full model of
Eq 4 that also contains a contagion component
(red). The green line indicates the estimated portion of the data due to contagion. The points along the x axis for the first three samples indicate the date of
events that had number of people killed in the top 5
th
percentile for that sample.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259.g001
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 4/12
The incidents were significantly more likely to occur between September to April compared
with May to August (p < 0.001), and on weekdays compared with weekends (p < 0.001).
Of all events, 18% of the shooters committed suicide, with the fraction rising to 23% in inci-
dents in which at least one person was killed (not including the shooter). Again, this is signifi-
cantly higher than the estimated 5% to 10% suicide rate of killers in all homicides [
31]. There
was no significant difference in the number of people killed in incidents where the shooter did,
and did not, commit suicide.
Brady Campaign Mass Shooting Data. Additional data on 477 mass shootings in the US
between February 2005 to January 2013 were also obtained from the Brady Campaign. These
data include shootings where at least three people were shot or injured (but not necessarily
killed).
Incidents were excluded that did not mention a verifi able source, or were an agglome ration
of events rather than a single event (such as a mention of the total number of people killed by
firearms in particular city over a weekend), and we were unable to identify news stories con-
taining the details of the separate events. In addition, two of the entries appeared to be repeti-
tions of a previous entry and were thus excluded from the analysis. A total of 468 incidents
remained after these selections. Ten of the events had the date of the incident mis-transcribed
by a day or two, and the dates were corrected for this analysis. Of the 468 mass shootings, 92
involve 4 four or more people killed, 83 of which are also included in the 154 mass killings by
firearms in the USA Today data set that spans the same time frame. Because of this large over-
lap of the two data sets, in order to ensure that the Brady Campaign mass shooting data is inde-
pendent of the USA Today mass killing data we thus examine only the mass shootings in the
Brady Campaign data that have less than four people killed, yielding a total of 376 incidents.
The data are shown in
Fig 1.
Incidents showed no significant dependence on month of the year, but were significantly
more likely to occur on Saturday and Sunday (p < 0.001).
Of all events, 17% of the shooters committed suicide, with the fraction rising to 25% in inci-
dents in which at least one person was killed (not including the shooter). Again, this is signifi-
cantly higher than the estimated 5% to 10% suicide rate of killers in all homicides [
31]. On
average, 2.3 times more people were killed in events where the shooter committ ed suicide,
compared with events where the shooter did not (Student-t p < 0.001).
Correlation of data samples to exogenous variables. No official government statistics are
available for firearm ownership by state. In order to estimate the relative firearm ownership by
state, we thus examined the fraction of all suicides within each state that involved firearms
(past firearm suicide rates have been shown to be directly correlated to firearm ownership rates
[
13, 14], and are a good proxy for firearm ownership rates [32]). Suic ide data from 1999 to
2010 by state and cause used to calculate these estimates were obtained from the Centers for
Disease Control Web-based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting System (
http://www.cdc.
gov/injury/wisqars/index.html, accessed January 30, 2015). State populations in 2012 were
obtained from the US Census Bureau
www.census.gov (accessed January 30, 2015).
State prevalence rates of serious mental illness for all 50 states were obtained from the from
the National Alliance on Mental Illness (NAMI) [
33, 34].
The Brady Campaign to Prevent Gun Violence regularly publishes rankings of the states on
the basis of laws that can prevent gun violence, such as background checks on all gun sales, per-
mit-to-purchase requirements, limiting handgun purchases to one a month, and retention of
sales records. The most recent state rankings were obtained from
bradycampaign.org.
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 5/12
To study how the state variations in incidence of mass killings, school shootings, and mass
shootings may be related to exogenous variables such as state rates of firearm ownership, prev-
alence of mental illness, etc, we first calculated the national average rate of such incidents, μ.
For a state with observed number of incidents, k
i
, and population N
i
, we determined the signifi-
cance of the observed number of incidents by calculating the Binomial CDF
Fð k
i
jN
i
; mÞ¼
X
k
i
j¼0
N
i
j
!
m
j
ð1 mÞ
N
i
j
: ð1Þ
The result is between 0 and 1, with results close to 0 (1) indicating that a particular state has
rate of such incidents significantly lower (higher) than the national average.
Modeling and Statistical Methods
Self-Excitation Contagion Model. In a self-excitation contagion model, recent prior
events increase the probability of another event happening in the near future. In this analysis,
we employ an exponential probability distribution to simulate this process. Under the hypothe-
sis that past events incite future events, the increased probability of an event occurring during
the 24 hours of day t
j
due to prior event that occurred on day t
i
is thus
Pðt
j
jt
i
; T
excite
Þ¼
Z
t
j
t
i
t
j
t
i
1
dx
e
x=T
excite
T
excite
;
ð2Þ
where T
excite
is the average duration of the contagion process (in this analysis, measured in
days). This yields
Pðt
j
jt
i
; T
excite
Þ¼e
ðt
j
t
i
1Þ=T
excite
e
ðt
j
t
i
Þ=T
excite
:
ð3Þ
We consider a self-excitation contagion model with an additional baseline (i.e.; non-conta-
gion related) average number of events per day of N
0
(t). Taking into account all prior events in
some stochastic data realization, the total number of expected events, N
exp
, on day t
n
for that
realization is thus
N
exp
ðt
n
Þ¼N
0
ðt
n
ÞþN
secondary
X
8t
i
<t
n
Pðt
n
jt
i
; T
excite
Þ;
ð4Þ
where the summation is over all prior events. The parameters of this contagion model are the
average number of secondary events inspired by the contagion of a single event, N
secondary
, the
duration of the contagion process, T
excite
, and whatever parameters are needed to describe the
temporal evolution of the baseline number of events, N
0
(t). For instance, one can assume N
0
(t)
is a constant, or a straight line with a slope. One can also use a non-parametric approach where
N
0
(t) is calculated using a weighted running mean of the data itself (in S1 Appendix we provide
a detailed description of the weighted running mean ansatz).
The functional form of N
0
(t) can incorporate additional weights if needed in order to take
into account day-of-week or seasonal effects, and indeed, we incorporate such weights when it
is evident that there is significant evidence of dependence on season or day-of-week in a partic-
ular data set.
In
S1 Appendix, we describe the methods used to fit the parameters of the mode l of Eq 4 to
data, and the methods we used to simulate data with a self-excitation contagion model. We
also describe the methods used to validate the modeling and fitting methods, and the methods
used to cross-check the analysis.
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 6/12
Results
Based on the USA Today sample, mass killings involving firearms with four or more people
killed occur on average every 12.5 days in the US. Based on the Brady Campaign data, school
shootings occur on average every 31.6 days.
The results of the fits of the parameters of the self-excitation contagion model of
Eq 4 to the
various data samples are shown in
Fig 1, and are tabulated in Table 1. Evidence of significant
contagion is found for all samples but the Brady Campaign mass shooting sample with at most
three people killed.
Latitude and longitude data were obtained for the location of each incident in the data sets
used in this analysis. For all samples, the time between incidents was not significantly corre-
lated to the distance between them, indicating lack of evidence of temporal/geospatial cluster-
ing (as would happen, for instance, if an incident incited a similar incident in a nearby locale).
Additionally, the Mantel test for temporal/geo-spatial clustering in the samples did not return
significant p-values (p > 0.05 for all samples) [
35]. This lack of temporal/geo-spatial correla-
tion is consistent with what would be expected if the contagion process is potentially due, for
instance, to widespread media attention given to mass killings and school shootings.
The correlation of the state incidence of our data samples to firearm ownership prevalence,
prevalence of mental illness, and ranking of strength of state firearm legislation are summa-
rized in
Fig 2.
Discussion and Summary
In our analysis, we employ a self-excitation contagion model, and find significant evidence of
contagion in mass killings and school shootings. There is no significant evidence of contagion
in mass shootings that involve three or fewer people killed, possibly indicating that the much
higher frequency of such events compared with mass killings and school shootings reduces
their relative sensationalism, and thus reduces their contagiousness. This is the first analysis of
its kind to examine the potential of contagion in such incidents.
We find that state prevalence of firearm ownership is significantly associated with state inci-
dence of mass killings with firearms, school shootings, and mass shootings. Once state preva-
lence of firearm ownership has been taken into account, there is no significant association
between state incidence of these events and state prevalence of mental illness or ranking of
strength of firearm legislation. Mass killings not involving firearms are not significantly corre-
lated to any of these variables. Our noted apparent relationship of high-profile shooting inci-
dents to firearm ownership is in concordance with the results of other studies of firearm
violence, including homicide and suicide [
814, 36].
Table 1. Results of fits of the self-excitation contagion model in Eq 4 to the various data samples considered in these studies, using a running
mean calculation of N
0
(t). N
secondary
is the average number of new incidents. The p-value is obtained from the likelihood ratio test comparing the likelihood
of the full contagion model to the likelihood of the null hypothesis model of no contagion. The numbers in the square brackets indicate the 95% confidence
interval on the parameter.
Sample # in sample N
secondary
T
excite
pvalue
USA Today full sample 232 0.28 [0.10,0.56] 13.3 [4.6,58.6] p = 0.004
USA Today w/ rearms 176 0.30 [0.12,0.56] 13.2 [4.3,46.8] p = 0.002
USA Today w/o rearms 56 0.23 [0.04,0.64] 11.9 [3.2,89.5] p = 0.037
Brady Campaign school shootings 188 0.22 [0.10,0.42] 12.9 [5.4,53.3] p < 0.001
Brady Campaign mass shootings w/ 3 killed 373 0.28 [0.00,0.62] 38 [0, 90] p = 0.18
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259.t001
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 7/12
Given the significant correlation of the incidence of mass killings to firearm ownership
prevalence, it is worthwhile to explore the potential reasons for the apparent relationship. We
note that the overall effect of firearm right-to-carry laws on the prevalence of various types of
crime has been hotly debated (see, for instance, References [
3741]). However, it has been
shown that firearm regulations that reduce overall firearm availability, such as permit and
licensing regulations, appear to have a significant deterrent effect on suicide rates [
42], and
shall-issue laws that eliminate most restrictions on carrying a concealed weapon have been
associated with increased firearm homicide rates [
43]. In addition, firearm mortality rates were
found to be significantly lower in states with stricter gun laws [
44]. Secure gun storage practices
involving safes and trigger locks have been shown to decrease the risk of youth suicides and
unintentional firearm injuries [
45].
Fig 2. Relationship of state prevalence of firearm ownership, mental illness, and state rankings of strength of firearm legislation, to the state
incidence of mass killings, school shootings, and mass shootings. Correlations with jρj0.28 are significant to p < 0.05.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259.g002
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 8/12
We found no significant association between the rate of school and mass shootings and
state prevalence of mental illness. However, we note that in all our data samples but mass kill-
ings not involving firearms, the probability that the perpetrator committed suicide was several
times higher than the overall estimated perpetrator suicide rate of 5% to 10% for all homicides
[
31]. In addition, in our mass killings with firearms and mass shootings data samples, we have
found a significant positive association between the overall number killed and perpetrator sui-
cide. Further study of the reasons behind these patterns is indicated, including an examination
of whether or not problems with access to mental health treatment at the individual level
played a partial role in such incidents.
While our analysis was initially inspired by the hypothesis that mass media attention given
to sensational violent events may promote ideation in vulnerable individuals, in practice what
our analysis tests is whether or not temporal patterns in the data indicate evidence for conta-
gion, by whatever means. In truth, and especially because so many perpetrators of these acts
commit suicide, we likely may never know on a case-by-case basis who was inspired by similar
prior acts, particu larly since the ideation may have been subconscious.
It is also unclear whether or not the patterns we have observe d in our analysis are perhaps
spuriously caused by potential unaccounted-for biases in the data samples that were compiled
by private organizations. To further the understanding of the unde rlying root causes of these
events, and to confirm whether or not contagion truly plays a role, an official comprehensive
detailed, accurate, and publicly available federal database of incidents of all mass killings and
school shootings in the US is necessary. A database that includes, at a minimum, details on the
background events, mental health status and access to mental health treatment of the perpetra-
tors, exactly what kinds of weapons were used, where the perpetrators obtained their weapons,
and whether they did so legally or illegally. Several studies of firearm violence over the past
decade have pointed out the need for such a database (see, for instance, References [
46, 47]).
For the time being, while waiting for such a database to become available, studies such as this
must use what data are available, paying attention to cross-checks of the robustness of the
modeling methodology to potential biases, as we ha ve attempted to do here.
Studies into the prevention of such tragedies are also hampered by the freeze on federal
funding for research into gun violence in the United States, put in place by Congress in 1997
[
48, 49]. In January, 2013, President Obama issued a presidential memorandum directing the
Centers for Disease Control to resume studies into firearm violence. However, at the time of
this writing in September 2014, the majority of members of Congress have vowed to continue
to block allocation of federal funding to the studies. In the near term it thus appears that federal
legislation will not be put forward to address the need for the documentation and detailed
study of such events.
Supporting Information
S1 Appendix.
(PDF)
S1 Data. A compressed directory containing the data files used in this study.
(GZ)
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank the Brady Campaign to Prevent Gun Violence for making their data
available for this study.
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 9/12
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: ST AGL. Performed the experiments: ST AGL MK
AM. Analyzed the data: ST AGL MK AM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: ST
AGL AM CCC. Wrote the paper: ST AGL MK AM CCC. Developed concept of study: ST AGL.
Developed software: ST AGL. Conducted data collection and analysis of self-excitation model:
ST AGL MK AM. Conducted development and analysis interpretation of deterministic model:
ST AGL AM CCC.
References
1. Savastana MA. Tattle-telling on the United States: School violence and the international blame game.
Penn St Intl L Rev 2002; 21: 649.
2. Krug EG, Powell KE, Dahlberg LL. Firearm-related deaths in the United States and 35 other high-and
upper-middle-income countries. International Journal of Epidemiology. 1998; 27: 214221. doi:
10.
1093/ije/27.2.214 PMID: 9602401
3. Krug EG, Mercy JA, Dahlberg LL, Powell KE. Firearm-and non-firearm-related homicide among chil-
dren: An international comparison. Homicide Studies. 1998; 2: 8395. doi:
10.1177/
1088767998002001006
4. Richardson EG, Hemenway D. Homicide, suicide, and unintentional firearm fatality: comparing the
United States with other high-income countries, 2003. The Journal of Trauma and Acute Care Surgery.
2011; 70: 238243. doi: 10.1097/TA.0b013e3181dbaddf
5. Hemenway D, Miller M. Firearm availability and homicide rates across 26 high-income countries. Jour-
nal of Trauma and Acute Care Surgery. 2000; 49(6): 985988. doi:
10.1097/00005373-200012000-
00001
6. CIA Factbook. The World Factbook; 2010. See also:
http://www cia gov/library/publications/the-world-
factbook, accessed January 30, 2015.
7. Federal Bureau of Investigation. A Study of Active Shooter Incidents in the United States Between
2000 and 2013. 2014; Available at
www.fbi.gov/news/stories/2014/september (accessed January 30,
2015).
8. US Dept of Education. Final report and findings of the safe school initiative: Implications for the preven-
tion of school attacks in the United States, 2004.
9. Hepburn LM, Hemenway D. Firearm availability and homicide: A review of the literature. Aggression
and Violent Behavior. 2004; 9: 417440. doi:
10.1016/S1359-1789(03)00044-2
10. Siegel M, Ross CS, King III C. The relationship between gun ownership and firearm homicide rates in
the united states, 19812010. American journal of public health. 2013; 103: 20982105. doi:
10.2105/
AJPH.2013.301409 PMID: 24028252
11. Rosenfeld R, Baumer E, Messner SF. Social trust, firearm prevalence, and homicide. Annals of Epide-
miology. 2007; 17: 119125. doi:
10.1016/j.annepidem.2006.07.016 PMID: 17178232
12. Hemenway D, Kennedy BP, Kawachi I, Putnam RD. Firearm prevalence and social capital. Annals of
Epidemiology. 2001; 11: 484490. doi:
10.1016/S1047-2797(01)00235-6 PMID: 11557180
13. Miller M, Azrael D, Hemenway D. Household firearm ownership and suicide rates in the United States.
Epidemiology. 2002; 13: 517524. doi:
10.1097/00001648-200209000-00006 PMID: 12192220
14. Miller M, Azrael D, Hepburn L, Hemenway D, Lippmann SJ. The association between changes in
household firearm ownership and rates of suicide in the united states, 19812002. Injury Prevention.
2006; 12: 178182. doi: 10.1136/ip.2005.010850 PMID: 16751449
15. Tondo L, Albert MJ, Baldessarini RJ. Suicide rates in relation to health care access in the United States:
an ecological study. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006; 67: 517523. doi:
10.4088/JCP.
v67n0402 PMID: 16669716
16. Gould M, Jamieson P, Romer D. Media contagion and suicide among the young. American Behavioral
Scientist. 2003; 46: 12691284. doi:
10.1177/0002764202250670
17. Gould MS. Suicide and the media. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001; 932: 200224.
doi:
10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05807.x PMID: 11411187
18. Gould MS, Wallenstein S, Davidson L. Suicide clusters: A critical review. Suicide and Life-Threatening
Behavior. 1989; 19: 1729. PMID:
2652386
19. Romer D, Jamieson PE, Jamieson KH. Are news reports of suicide contagious? a stringent test in six
US cities. Journal of Communication. 2006; 56: 253270. doi:
10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00018.x
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 10 / 12
20. Cantor CH, Sheehan P, Alpers P, Mullen P. Media and mass homicides. Archives of Suicide Research.
1999; 5: 283290. doi:
10.1023/A:1009637817185
21. Slutkin, G. Violence is a contagious disease. Contagion of violence: Workshop summary. 2012; See
also:
http://cureviolence.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/01/iom.pdf, accessed January 30, 2015.
22. Aït-Sahalia Y, Cacho-Diaz J, Laeven RJ. Modeling financial contagion using mutually exciting jump pro-
cesses. Technical report, National Bureau of Economic Research. 2010
23. Mitchell L, Cates ME. Hawkes process as a model of social interactions: a view on video dynamics.
Journal of Physics A: Mathematical and Theoretical. 2010; 43: 045101. doi:
10.1088/1751-8113/43/4/
045101
24. Johnson SD, Bernasco W, Bowers KJ, Elffers H, Ratcliffe J, Rengert G, et al. Space-Time Patterns of
Risk: A Cross National Assessment of Residential Burglary Victimization. Journal of Quantitative Crimi-
nology. 2007; 23 (3), 201219. doi:
10.1007/s10940-007-9025-3
25. Johnson SD, Bowers KJ. The Burglary as Clue to the Future The Beginnings of Prospective Hot-Spot-
ting. European Journal of Criminology. 2004; 1 (2), 237255. doi:
10.1177/1477370804041252
26. Bowers KJ, Johnson SD. Who commits near repeats? a test of the boost explanation. Western Crimi-
nology Review. 2004; 5.
27. Mohler GO, Short MB, Brantingham PJ, Schoenberg FP, Tita GE. Self-exciting point process modeling
of crime. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 2011; 106. doi:
10.1198/jasa.2011.ap09546
28. Lewis E, Mohler G, Brantingham PJ, Bertozzi AL. Self-exciting point process models of civilian deaths
in Iraq. Security Journal. 2011; 25: 244264. doi:
10.1057/sj.2011.21
29. Townsley M, Johnson SD, Ratcliffe JH. Space time dynamics of insurgent activity in Iraq. Security Jour-
nal. 2008; 21: 139146. doi:
10.1057/palgrave.sj.8350090
30. Porter MD, White G. Self-exciting hurdle models for terrorist activity. The Annals of Applied Statistics.
2012; 6: 106124. doi:
10.1214/11-AOAS513
31. Lester D. Suicide in mass murderers and serial killers. Suicidology Online. 2010; 1: 1927.
32. Azrael D, Cook PJ, Miller M. State and local prevalence of firearms ownership measurement, structure,
and trends. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 2004; 20: 4362. doi:
10.1023/B:JOQC.0000016699.
11995.c7
33. Aron L, Honberg R, Duckworth K, Kimball A, Edgar E, Carolla B, et al. Grading the states 2009: a report
on Americas health care system for adults with serious mental illness. Arlington, VA: National Alliance
on Mental Illness. 2009.
34. Holzer III C, Jackson DJ, Tweed D. Horizontal synthetic estimation: a strategy for estimating small area
health related characteristics. Evaluation and Program Planning. 1981; 4: 2934. doi:
10.1016/0149-
7189(81)90051-3
35. Mantel N. The detection of disease clustering and a generalized regression approach. Cancer
research. 1967; 27(2 Part 1), 209220. PMID:
6018555
36. Hemenway D, Miller M. Association of rates of household handgun ownership, lifetime major depres-
sion, and serious suicidal thoughts with rates of suicide across US census regions. Injury Prevention.
2002; 8: 313316. doi: 10.1136/ip.8.4.313 PMID: 12460969
37. Lott JR. More guns, less crime: Understanding crime and gun control laws. University of Chicago
Press. 2013.
38. Hahn RA, Oleg Bilukha M, Alex Crosby M, Fullilove MT, Liberman A, Moscicki E, et al. Firearms laws
and the reduction of violence: A systematic review. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2005;
28: 4071. doi:
10.1016/j.amepre.2004.10.005 PMID: 15698747
39. Maltz MD, Targonski J. A note on the use of county-level UCR data. Journal of Quantitative Criminol-
ogy. 2002; 18: 297318. doi:
10.1023/A:1016060020848
40. Donohue J, Ayres I. Shooting down the more guns, less crime hypothesis. Stanford Law Review.
2003; 51.
41. Donohue J, Ayres I. Nondiscretionary concealed weapons law: a case study of statistics, standards of
proof, and public policy. American Law and Economics Review. 1999.
42. Rodríguez Andrés A, Hempstead K. Gun control and suicide: The impact of state firearm regulations in
the United States, 19952004. Health Policy. 2011; 101: 95103. doi:
10.1016/j.healthpol.2010.10.005
PMID:
21044804
43. Rosengart M, Cummings P, Nathens A, Heagerty P, Maier R, Rivara F. An evaluation of state firearm
regulations and homicide and suicide death rates. Injury Prevention. 2005; 11: 7783. doi:
10.1136/ip.
2004.007062 PMID: 15805435
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 11 / 12
44. Fleegler EW, Lee LK, Monuteaux MC, Hemenway D, Mannix R. Firearm legislation and firearm-related
fatalities in the United States. JAMA internal medicine. 2013; 173: 732740. doi:
10.1001/
jamainternmed.2013.9957 PMID: 23467753
45. Grossman DC, Mueller BA, Riedy C, Dowd MD, Villaveces A, Prodzinski J, et al. Gun storage practices
and risk of youth suicide and unintentional firearm injuries. JAMA. 2005; 293: 707714. doi:
10.1001/
jama.293.6.707 PMID: 15701912
46. Weiner J, Wiebe DJ, Richmond TS, Beam K, Berman AL, Branas CC, et al. Reducing firearm violence:
a research agenda. Injury Prevention. 2007; 13: 8084. doi:
10.1136/ip.2006.013359 PMID: 17446246
47. Wellford CF, Pepper JV, Petrie CV. Firearms and violence: a critical review. National Academies
Press. 2004.
48. Brent DA, Miller MJ, Loeber R, Mulvey EP, Birmaher B. Ending the silence on gun violence. Journal of
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013; 52: 333338. doi:
10.1016/j.jaac.
2013.01.006 PMID: 23571100
49. Kellermann AL, Rivara FP. Silencing the science on gun research. JAMA. 2013; 309: 549550. doi:
10.1001/jama.2012.208207 PMID: 23262635
Contagion in Mass Killings and School Shootings
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0117259 July 2, 2015 12 / 12
... Following this initial use of violence, the conflict has now moved from the realm of discourse into reality, and its medium has been defined. Resentment-driven conflicts typically follow in a pattern stemming from the initial incident, for several possible reasons: the copycat effect -copying what others do; media contagion -where media coverage of the incident inspires further incidents; and methodological justification -previous incidents legitimize the use of violence as a realistic option (Muschert & Ragnedda, 2011;Towers et al., 2015). Because the fundamental attribution error encourages a more forgiving interpretation of the actions of the ingroup, some may assume that circumstances made the use of violence necessary. ...
... Because the fundamental attribution error encourages a more forgiving interpretation of the actions of the ingroup, some may assume that circumstances made the use of violence necessary. The effect of the ingroup bias can cause even those who view violence more negatively to perceive its use by fellow ingroup members in a more positive light, retroactively legitimizing the violent action and establishing justification for the future (Fisher & Kelman, 2011;Towers et al., 2015). ...
Article
This article outlines a new model for the initiation of intergroup conflict, in which resentment is theorized as the motivating factor. This model – drawing from existing theories in social psychology and international relations – describes the process by which members of a group become willing to engage in violent conflict with a target group, beginning with feelings of resentment arising from a perceived experience of victimization. The model is then applied to the involuntary celibate (incel) ideology and the trend of mass shootings perpetrated by incels, with two case studies.
... Second, the role of recent stressful life events (such as separation, divorce, death of a relative, disruption in employment or in scholarship, being a victim of a crime) should be documented, as they could precipitate suicidal ideation and suicide and could also be risk factors for violent acts (Howarth et al., 2020;Mo et al., 2020). Third, particular attention should be given to the contagion of suicide in this population, especially since there could also be a contagion effect of mass shootings (IOM (Institute of Medicine) and NRC (National Research Council), 2013; Towers et al., 2015). Finally, an assessment of alcohol and drug use among mass murderers/terrorists, especially before violent acts, is necessary, as they increase both the risk of suicide and violent outcomes (Armoon et al., 2021;Zhong et al., 2020). ...
Chapter
Terrorism and mass murder are two forms of violence that have become international concerns in the twenty-first century. Despite their apparent differences, they share some similarities. This chapter aims to compare and contrast the two phenomena by examining their definitions, global impact, and the psychological profiles of terrorists and mass murderers. This chapter presents a comparative study of the psychological profiles of terrorists and mass murderers. Two systematic literature reviews were carried out to investigate mental disorders in these two populations. It examines the role of mental disorders in both populations and discuss their potential relationship with violent behavior. The case of Anders Behring Breivik, who perpetrated the 2011 terrorist attacks in Norway, is used to illustrate the complex interplay between terrorism, mass murder, and psychiatry. Finally, the chapter addresses the role of mental health professionals in understanding and preventing terrorism and mass murder, as well as the limitations of current research and the need for a dynamic and multidisciplinary approach to address these complex issues.
... These early scalar models, which assume homogeneous or "random" mixing, have later been enhanced by defining them on spatial patches of metapopulations to allow for movements of individuals (Arino, 2009) or by embedding them into networks (Pastor-Satorras et al., 2015) to more realistically represent the influence of contacts between individuals on the spread of the disease. In addition to infectious disease analysis, these models were also shown to be applicable for the study of the "spread" or adoption of new behaviors on social networks, including the spread of obesity (Hill et al., 2010), mass shootings (Towers et al., 2015), and smoking, alcohol consumption, and depression (Christakis & Fowler, 2013), among others. The focus of this paper is on making inferences about contact rates among individuals or sub-populations during the spread of an infectious disease based on observed infection count data using network models. ...
Article
A key challenge in epidemic modeling is the lack of adequate data on population interactions (such as traffic flow or mobility patterns) which result in the spread of infectious diseases. Knowledge of social contact patterns is crucial for public health professionals to devise effective non-pharmaceutical interventions to control epidemics. This paper focuses on inferring social contact rates from reported infection counts during the spread of an infectious disease, addressing the increased difficulty that arises when dealing with “sparse” contact networks where only a small subset of the edges have non-zero weights. Specifically, a new geographically constrained lasso approach for network reconstruction for non-homogeneous mixing Susceptible-Infected-Removed (SIR) disease spread models is presented. The new network reconstruction method can explicitly account for the spatial proximity of network nodes in estimating the disease transmission rates and predicting the future evolution of the epidemic dynamics. Extensive numerical experiments are presented to show the proposed method outperforms existing approaches in terms of accuracy of contact identification under various graph topologies. A case study based on real data from the COVID-19 pandemic is presented to demonstrate the application of the approach for inferring contact structures and a counterfactual scenario analysis to assess effectiveness of containment strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Mass shootings in the U.S. have been at the center of the public crisis debate for a long time. Combining information on mass shootings with background check reports from the Federal Bureau of Investigation, this study applies mass shootings as exogenous shocks and reveals that the demand for guns is especially strong in the month in which a shooting occurs, and it decays with time. In addition, results confirm a spatial spillover effect of mass shootings, in which a mass shooting in one state affects gun demand in other states. The magnitude of the effect depends on the distance between the states. Our analysis also explores the difference in the effects between states with loose regulations on handguns and long guns and those with strict regulations. In the former, gun demand increases significantly after mass shootings, whereas in the latter the increase is insignificant. Finally, this study shows that consumers respond heterogeneously given the different characteristics of mass shootings, such as the number of victims and location.
Article
Background: Although an important subset of mass shooters has admitted copying previous shooters, there has been almost no empirical research on the similarities between mass shooting role models and their copycats. Such analysis is essential for understanding who is most susceptible to the influence of high-profile mass shooters and what behaviors they are likely to copy. Methods: We first compiled all documented instances we could find globally of public mass shooters and active shooters becoming a role model for a copycat from 1966 to 2022 (n = 205) and calculated how often their risk profiles and behaviors were similar. Next, we ran simulated matches (n = 2000) and used binary logistic regression to test whether copycats were significantly more similar to their role models than to a random shooter. Findings: Compared to a random shooter, copycat attackers were significantly closer to their role models in age and more likely to share the same sex, race, country, incident location type, and offender outcome. Nearly 80% of copycats attacked more than one year after their role model, and the average temporal gap was approximately eight years. Copycats averaged significantly fewer victims killed and wounded than their role models. Conclusions: The risk that high-profile mass shooters influence copycat attackers persists for many years, with the most susceptible individuals sharing characteristics of the role model shooters themselves. These findings could be used to make media coverage of mass shootings safer and to inform triage and case prioritization for threat assessment and violence prevention.
Article
BACKGROUND Mental illness is a health challenge faced by adolescents that has grown worse after the Coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic. Research on social media and young people’s mental health has recently increased, and numerous studies have examined whether frequent use of social media is linked to issues such as anxiety, stress, depression, eating disorders, insomnia, frustration, feeling alone, and externalizing problems among adolescents. This influence of social media on adolescents’ lives is clear, with many platforms like Facebook, Instagram, and YouTube playing an important role in daily interactions and self-expression. Even though social media offers numerous benefits, such as connectivity and information sharing, excessive usage can have detrimental effects on mental health, particularly among adolescents. AIM To study the impact of social media on the mental wellbeing of adolescents, and the associated potential dangers in India. METHODS A total of 204 adolescents aged 14 years to 23 years were included in the study. This study explored the intricate relationship between social media usage and adolescent mental health in India. The study employs a cross-sectional survey design to capture a snapshot of adolescent mental health and social media usage patterns. Data collection involved administering structured questionnaires and the analysis utilized quantitative methods, including descriptive statistics. RESULTS Excessive use of social media is correlated with increased stress, anxiety, and depression. Adolescents engage in compulsive behaviors such as scrolling in the middle of the night, which negatively impacts their mental and physical health, and leads to significant sleep disruption. Findings from the study aim to provide insights into the current state of adolescent mental health and inform strategies to promote positive wellbeing in the Indian population. CONCLUSION The study underscores the need for further research to better understand the complex interplay between social media and adolescent mental health, and need for effective strategies to combat online harassment.
Preprint
Full-text available
This study investigates the impact of the Ukraine war on Polish public opinion, focusing on four main areas: public attention to the war, war anxiety, susceptibility to disinformation, and anti-refugee attitudes. Utilising a cross-sectional survey of over 10,000 respondents, the research analyses how these factors have evolved during the conflict through the lens of cognitive dissonance theory. Our results highlight the dynamic nature of public sentiment, the influence of disinformation campaigns, and the complex attitudes towards refugees within Poland. The findings offer valuable insights, emphasising the importance of addressing disinformation and fostering social cohesion amidst crises. Additionally, we are empirically verifying expectations based on cognitive dissonance theory, showing that this framework could be valuable for analysing public opinion during times of crisis and that it meets most empirical expectations.
Chapter
In this chapter, the authors examine what they call “alma mater” school shootings - those in which a current or former student returns to attack people at the school. In this case, the focus is on K-12 school shootings in the United States between 1990 and 2018 where two or more individuals were killed. They argue that many shooters target their alma mater because it is the place where they have experienced “place trauma” - emotional pain that is intertwined with a location. After discerning common demographic, personal, and spatiotemporal patterns, they discuss how the shooters acquired the firearms used to carry out the attack. Finally, the authors outline policy and practice considerations focusing on education, school funding, and legislation.
Article
Full-text available
Preface 1: Introduction 2: How to Test the Effects of Gun Control 3: Gun Ownership, Gun Laws, and the Data on Crime 4: Concealed-Handgun Laws and Crime Rates: The Empirical Evidence 5: The Victims and the Benefits from Protection 6: What Determines Arrest Rates and the Passage of Concealed-Handgun Laws? 7: The Political and Academic Debate 8: Some Final Thoughts Appendixes Notes Bibliography Index
Article
Full-text available
Our goal in this article is to characterize temporal patterns of violent civilian deaths in Iraq. These patterns are expected to evolve on time-scales ranging from years to minutes as a result of changes in the security environment on equally varied time-scales. To assess the importance of multiple time-scales in evolving security threats, we develop a self-exciting point process model similar to that used in earthquake analysis. Here the rate of violent events is partitioned into a background rate and a foreground self-exciting component. Background rates are assumed to change on relatively long time-scales. Foreground self-excitation, in which events trigger an increase in the rate of violence, is assumed to be short-lived. We explore the model using data from Iraq Body Count on civilian deaths between 2003 and 2007. Our results indicate that self-excitation makes up as much as 37–50 per cent of all violent events and that self-excitation lasts at most between two and six weeks, depending upon the district in question. Appropriate security responses may benefit from taking these different time-scales of violence into consideration.
Article
Full-text available
Research continues to demonstrate that vulnerable youth are susceptible to the influence of reports and portrayals of suicide in the mass media. The evidence is stronger for the influence of reports in the news media than in fictional formats. However, several studies have found dramatic effects of televised portrayals that have led to increased rates of suicide and suicide attempts using the same methods displayed in the shows. Recent content analyses of newspapers and films in the United States reveal substantial opportunity for exposure to suicide, especially among young victims. One approach to reducing the harmful effects of media portrayals is to educate journalists and media programmers about ways to present suicide so that imitation will be minimized and help-seeking encouraged. Recently released recommendations for journalists are attached as an appendix. Similar initiatives with the entertainment industry would be highly desirable.
Book
For years proposals for gun control and the ownership of firearms have been among the most contentious issues in American politics. For public authorities to make reasonable decisions on these matters, they must take into account facts about the relationship between guns and violence as well as conflicting constitutional claims and divided public opinion. In performing these tasks, legislators need adequate data and research to judge both the effects of firearms on violence and the effects of different violence control policies. Readers of the research literature on firearms may sometimes find themselves unable to distinguish scholarship from advocacy. Given the importance of this issue, there is a pressing need for a clear and unbiased assessment of the existing portfolio of data and research. Firearms and Violence uses conventional standards of science to examine three major themes - firearms and violence, the quality of research, and the quality of data available. The book assesses the strengths and limitations of current databases, examining current research studies on firearm use and the efforts to reduce unjustified firearm use and suggests ways in which they can be improved. © 2005 by the National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
Article
Background The Forty-Ninth World Health Assembly recenttly declared violence a worldwide public health problem. Improved understand of cross-national differences is useful for identifying risk factors and may facilitate prevention efforts. Few cross-national studies, however, have explored firearm-related deaths. We compared the incidence of firearm-related deaths among 36 countries. Methods Health officials in high-income (HI) and upper-middle-income countries (UMI) with populations greater than one million were asked to provide data using ICD-9 codes on firearm-related homicides, suicides, unintentional deaths and deaths of undetermined intent, as well as homicides and suicides for all methods combined. Thirty-six (78%) of the 46 countries provided complete data. We compared age-adjusted rates per 100 000 for each country and pooled rates by income group and geographical location. Results During the one-year study period, 88 649 firearm deaths were reported. Overall firearm mortality rates are five to six times higher in HI and UMI countries in the Americas (12.72) than in Europe (2.17), or Oceania (2.57) and 95 times higher than in Asia (0.13). The rate of firearm deaths in the United States (14.24 per 100 000) exceeds that of its economic counterparts (1.76) eightfold and that of UMI countries (9.69) by a factor of 1.5. Suicide and homicide contribute equally to total firearm deaths in the US, but most firearm deaths are suicides (71%) in HI countries and homicides (72%) in UMI countries. Conclusions Firearm death rates vary markedly throughout th industrialized world. Further research to identify risk factors associated with these variations may help improve prevention efforts.
Article
Around 100 studies have been conducted to examine the ‘Werther effect’ – the phenomenon whereby there is an increased rate of completed or attempted suicide following the depiction of an individual’s suicide in the media. These ‘media influence studies’ provide strong evidence for the existence of the Werther effect in the news media, and equivocal evidence for its existence in the entertainment media. Having established this, there is now a need to complement these media influence studies with inter-related studies that draw on approaches from a range of disciplines, particularly that of communication. The studies can be thought of as investigating the full spectrum of news and entertainment media processes and content, from how suicide stories are produced (news/entertainment production studies), to what information they contain and how this is framed (content analysis studies), to how this information is received and perceived (audience reception studies). This will assist in explicating the mechanisms by which the Werther effect might operate, and in designing and evaluating interventions to improve the practices of news and entertainment media professionals.
Article
Objectives: We examined the relationship between levels of household firearm ownership, as measured directly and by a proxy-the percentage of suicides committed with a firearm-and age-adjusted firearm homicide rates at the state level. Methods: We conducted a negative binomial regression analysis of panel data from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention's Web-Based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting Systems database on gun ownership and firearm homicide rates across all 50 states during 1981 to 2010. We determined fixed effects for year, accounted for clustering within states with generalized estimating equations, and controlled for potential state-level confounders. Results: Gun ownership was a significant predictor of firearm homicide rates (incidence rate ratio = 1.009; 95% confidence interval = 1.004, 1.014). This model indicated that for each percentage point increase in gun ownership, the firearm homicide rate increased by 0.9%. Conclusions: We observed a robust correlation between higher levels of gun ownership and higher firearm homicide rates. Although we could not determine causation, we found that states with higher rates of gun ownership had disproportionately large numbers of deaths from firearm-related homicides.
Article
Data from 26 industrialized countries are compared to understand the incidence of firearm-related and non-firearm-related homicide among children under 15 years of age. Comparisons were made between rates per 100,000 children for each country and for the United States and all non-U.S. countries combined. It was found that the U.S. rates far exceed those of other countries. The childhood homicide rate is five times higher in the United States than in all of the other countries combined (2.57 versus 0.51). The firearm-related homicide rate for children under 15 years of age is 16 times higher in the United States than in the 25 other countries (0.94 versus 0.06), and the non-firearm-related homicide rate is four times higher (1.63 versus 0.45). These findings suggest the value of conducting further research to explain cross-national differences.