ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Creativity per se is a convoluted phenomenon. With regard to distinct manifestations, the impact of culture on creativity is discussed. The belief system is embedded in the creative productions across different cultures. The exertion of culture on creativity is presented within a reciprocal process that, alongside culture, also weaves historical, societal, and individual factors. Further suggestions for cross-cultural studies of creativity are also discussed. Résumé La créativité en soi est un phénomène compliqué. En ce qui concerne les manifestations distinctes, l'impact de la culture sur la créativité est discutée. Le système de croyance est ancrée dans les productions créatives à travers différentes cultures. L'effort de la culture sur la créativité est présentée dans un processus réciproque qui, aux côtés de la culture, tisse également des facteurs historiques, sociaux et individuels. D'autres suggestions pour les études transculturelles de la créativité sont également discutés.
15
ISSN 1712-8358[Print]
ISSN 1923-6700[Online]
www.cscanada.net
www.cscanada.org
Cross-Cultural Communication
Vol. 8, No. 2, 2012, pp. 15-20
DOI:10.3968/j.ccc.1923670020120802.1360
The Interplay Between Culture and Creativity
L’INTERACTION ENTRE LA CULTURE ET DE LA CREATIVITE
Kuan Chen Tsai1,*
1 Ph.D. Student, University of the Incarnate Word, USA.
Address: 6900 N Vandiver Rd J205, San Antonio, TX 78209, USA.
*Corresponding author.
Received 7 January 2012; accepted 22 April 2012.
Abstract
Creativity per se is a convoluted phenomenon. With
regard to distinct manifestations, the impact of culture
on creativity is discussed. The belief system is embedded
in the creative productions across different cultures. The
exertion of culture on creativity is presented within a
reciprocal process that, alongside culture, also weaves
historical, societal, and individual factors. Further
suggestions for cross-cultural studies of creativity are also
discussed.
Key words: Creativity; Culture; Cross-cultural
studies
Résumé
La créativité en soi est un phénomène compliqué. En
ce qui concerne les manifestations distinctes, l’impact
de la culture sur la créativité est discutée. Le système
de croyance est ancrée dans les productions créatives
à travers différentes cultures. L’effort de la culture sur
la créativité est présentée dans un processus réciproque
qui, aux côtés de la culture, tisse également des facteurs
historiques, sociaux et individuels. D’autres suggestions
pour les études transculturelles de la créativité sont
également discutés.
Mots clés: La créativité; La culture; Les études
transculturelles
Kuan Chen Tsai (2012). The Interplay Between Culture
and Creativity. Cross-Cultural Communication
, 8
(2), 15-20.
Available from URL: http://www.cscanada.net/index.
php/ccc/article/view/j.ccc.1923670020120802.1360
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3968/j.ccc.1923670020120802.1360.
INTRODUCTION
The study of creativity is pitfall, since creativity per se
is a convoluted phenomenon. Indeed, universal theory
of creativity does not exist (Craft, 2003; Ludwig, 1992;
Treffinger, 2004). Through a great deal of efforts and
works, creativity researchers now understand some
features of creativity (Treffinger, 2004). A number of
variables are identified to improve or impair creative
performance, which include personality, cognition,
knowledge, brain function, family background,
motivation, environment, and social context (Runco,
2004). Perhaps most of the contribution is that the extent
of ownership of creativity, which not only is limited to a
select few but also laypeople have that property; it exists
within the mundane life and beyond specific domains
(e.g., science, art, and literature) (Lubart & Sternberg,
1998; Simonton, 2000). “We all harbor within us creative
seeds that are capable of ourishing” (Edelson, 1999, p.7).
The interest in the research of creativity gained grounds
in North America since 1950 Guilford speech and shed
light on systematic empirical research of creativity. The
merits of Guilford early creativity research not only
provided the foundation of subsequent research on the
nature and assessment of creative thinking, but also
introduced key components of divergent thinking which
includes fluency, feasibility, and originality. Since then
the wave of research becomes an exciting research topic
for creativity researchers to explore different theoretical
or methodological framework and cross-disciplinary
methods (Feldman & Benjamin, 2006; Mumford, 2000;
Simonton, 2000; Torrance, 1977).
The present review specifically focuses on the
profound effects of cultural milieu on creativity. From the
lens of culture, the universality of creativity is distinct,
but the manifestation of creativity is diverse (Craft, 2008;
Simonton & Shing-Shiang, 2010). Different cultures have
different perceptions of creativity. Throughout this review
the conceptions of creativity, especially the similarityand
16
Copyright © Canadian Academy of Oriental and Occidental Culture
The Interplay Between Culture and Creativity
difference between East and West, will be examined.
Here, Chinese perception of creativity will be represented
as the major sample of East. The main reason is that
the Confucianism and Taoism have a great influence on
Asian countries (Cannon, 2010). Different culture values
attached to creativity also manifest on various creativity
outlets. Some concrete examples will be given. Then,
some theories related to social environment on creativity
will be brie y discussed. Following this line, some cross-
cultural studies will also be presented. Finally, possible
directions and further suggestions concerning the cross-
cultural researches on creativity will be outlined.
DIFFERENT CONCEPTUAL MAPS OF
CREATIVITY BETWEEN EAST AND
WEST
The similarity of concept of creativity across East and
West is both valuing the positive side of creativity and
praising creative individuals. For example, in India, God
Vishvakarna, is worshiped by the spirit and power of the
creative process (Lubart, 1990). It is said that perspectives
of creativity stem from cultural creation myths (Craft,
2008; Lubart, 1990). For instance, the Oriental common
theme of creativity includes development and ongoing
process toward the cosmic creation. In light of Chinese
belief systems, the world was created by the interaction
of yin-yang movement (yin means negative force; yang
means positive force), which in turn differentiates this
world and its being; namely yin-yang is the ultimate
creative source of everything (Niu & Sternberg, 2006).
The phenomenon of yin and yang manifests everywhere;
all events (including creativity) consist of opposites or
polarities (Moeller, 2006). On the other hand, Judaic and
Greek view this phenomenon as unexpected incident by
outsiders to bring the order (Lubart, 1990). Traditionally,
creativity was viewed as the divine force between East and
West (Craft, Gardner, & Claxton, 2008; Niu& Sternberg,
2006). With this view, the human beings cannot create
and only mimic the glory of God or are inspired by the
Muses (Ludwig, 1992; Niu & Sternberg, 2003; Simonton,
2000). In sum, “human do not create; God does” (Niu &
Sternberg, 2006, p.22).
The notion of creativity under the umbrella of the
divine entity was dominant in the history of Western
mindset for a long period of time (Craft et al., 2008).After
Enlightenment, the concept of creativity had shifted from
divine to individual, followed by achievement of science
and technology (Niu & Sternberg, 2006). In Western
view, currently the general consensus of creativity is
de ned as the individuals (creators), processes (creating),
and products (creations) with the features of usefulness,
appropriateness, and novelty (DiLiello & Houghton, 2006;
Ford, 1996; Hennessey &Amabile, 1988;Taylor, 1988;
Walberg, 1988). The definition of Western creativity is
product-oriented, which focuses on tangible, observable,
and measurable manifestation (Lubart, 1990). This utility-
orientated attribution is a good t for the Western process
model of cognitive problem-solving orientation (Lubart&
Sternberg, 1998). Additionally, the popular Torrrance
Tests of Creative Thinking lends support to the important
feature of observable product-oriented definition in the
West (Torrance, 1988).
In contrast, the Eastern conception of creativity
portrays a different picture. The “novelty” is not
a protagonist around the plot of creativity (Niu &
Sternberg, 2006; Rudowicz, 2003). The focus is more
inner development and inner state of fulfilment. It is
the journey of self-discovery and intuitive approach
rather than the manifest of wordily product (Rudowicz,
2003). In doing so individual could achieve a high level
of creativity. This value-based viewpoint also exhibits
on social and moral realm (Lubart & Sternberg, 1998;
Niu & Sternberg, 2003, 2006). For example, in lights
of ancient Chinese perspective, mostly reflected by
Taoism, creativity is not isolated but conceptualized in
a comprehensive universal power within and without
a person. Further, there are two approaches to develop
creativity: mediation (Taoist method) and self-cultivation
(Confucian method) (Niu & Sternberg, 2003; Shi, Qu &
Lin, 2007). In Indian philosophy, the conceptualization
of creativity is embedded in the “state of personal
ful llment or bliss”(Lubart, 1990, p.42). In other words,
creativity is viewed as the process of self-actualization or
enlightenment to some extent. The study of Indian artists
(Maduro, 1976; as cited in Lubart, 1990), for example,
further illustrates this contention that the creation journey
in some sense is integrating with self-unconsciousness.
THE EFFECT OF CULTURE ON CREATIVE
PRODUCTION
Cultural features have a catalyzing effect on creative
activity. The outlet of creative expression is defined
differently across cultures (Lubart & Sternberg, 1998;
Ludwig, 1992). Arab culture, for example, encourages
creativity on technology and verbal expression but visual
arts have been strictly prohibited by canons. However,
in India and China, the religious topics and idols are
admired and important genre. In Turkey, creativity is
strongly welcomed in science and technology but not
in traditional social rules and relationships (Rudowicz,
2003). In North America, it is a likelihood of compliment
of the creative expression in science and problem solving
but condemnation of that in politics or socioeconomic
theory (Lubart, 1990). There is a tendency that "the level
of creativity permitted on a topic is inversely related
to the topic's role in the maintenance of deep cultural
patterns"(Lubart, 1990, p.46). Sometimes, those cultural
constraints are more implicit. For example, Chinese
17
Kuan Chen Tsai (2012).
Cross-Cultural Communication, 8
(2), 15-20
novels place more focus on the external behavior, which
is parallel with the emphasis of interpersonal relationship
within Eastern culture, whereas American novels tend to
care about inner states of the characters, which re ects the
center of individual per se in Western societies (Ludwig,
1992).
THEORIES OF SOCIAL-
ENVIRONMENTAL EFFECTS ON
CREATIVITY
A battery of creativity researches are anchored to
theoretical frameworks addressing the position of
societies on creativity. Amabile (1996), for example,
proposed intrinsic motivation principle of creativity
from the perspective of social psychology of creativity.
This formulation supported by other colleagues (e.g.,
Hennessey, 1995) states that the primary driving force to
be highly creative is located in the intrinsic motivation
such as interest, enjoyment, satisfaction, and the like
rather than extrinsic motivation such as expected
evaluation, expected reward, competition, and the like.
Further Amabile (1998) contended that creativity is the
function of intrinsic motivation, domain-relevant skills,
and creativity-relevant skills. In sum, the overwhelming
evidences demonstrate that the social environment
plays a key role on the motivational orientation, which
in turn positively or negatively impacts the creativity.
Speci cally, intrinsic motivation is especially conducive
to creative expression, whereas extrinsic motivation is
detrimental to creative behavior; nevertheless, to some
extent extrinsic motivation might be beneficial for
creativity under some conditions (Hennessey, 2003). Most
important, a supportive social environment is the sine qua
non for developing these motivations (Amabile, 1998,
2001).
Another holistic view of creativity was suggested
by Csikszentmihalyi (1988, 1996) systems model of
creativity. He asserted three essential forces underline
creative endeavors: (1) a cultural domain, which contains
rules and norms of creative expression; (2) a social
field, in which recognition and evaluation of creative
ideas; and (3) the individual, who brings thoughts and
actions to the domain. If a creative person wants to
have some contributions to the system, he needs to
learn and recognize not only the rules and the content
of that domain, but also the criteria and preferences of
the field. For instance, Csikszentmihalyi identified three
major dimensions which are relevant to creativity: the
clarity of structure, the centrality within the culture, and
accessibility (p.38). Centrally, this theory denoted that
creative achievements are not exclusively located on
merits of individuals but on the interaction among those
three components. As Csikszentmihalyi (1988) clearly
put, “we cannot study creativity by isolating individuals
and their works from the social and historical milieu in
which their actions are carried out” (p.325).
Sternberg and Lubart (1995) investment theory of
creativity posited that the creative person should act like
a good investor and “buy low and sell high.” The creative
individual, metaphorically, buys low by rejecting accepted
ideas in the society, and then sells high when others realize
its value and follow the thread. The notion of selling ideas
signi es the social environment as an important variable
in creativity (Lubart & Sternberg, 1998). According to the
con uence model of creativity (Lubart, 1990; Sternberg &
Lubart, 1995), they also recognized six potential resources
that help or hinder creativity: intelligence, knowledge,
and thinking styles are cognitive resources; personality
and motivation are conative resources; and finally the
environment. A confluence of those resources is needed
for an individual portfolio of creative resources. In
essence, “creativity is in part the product of an interaction
between a person and his or her context” (Sternberg &
Lubart 1995, p.10). The environment that encourages and
stimulates creative thoughts and rewards those behaviors
is beneficial for creativity. Accordingly, they criticized
the society with high demands on standardized tests at the
expense of creative power. In his view, Sternberg (2006)
maintained that society should play the role to “increase
the rewards and decrease the costs” (p.97) for the sake of
development of creativity.
CROSS-CULTURAL STUDIES ON
CREATIVITY
A review of the literature has shown that Easterners
and Westerners hold similar but not identical
conceptualizations of creativity (Niu & Sternberg, 2006;
Rudowicz, 2003). Oral, Kaufman, and Agars’ (2007)
research noted that numerous ndings in Western contexts
on creativity are in line with another culture to some
extent. It implied that it is a consistency across culture
in terms of creative abilities. For instance, they found
that creativity had tendency to increase with age and the
importance of intrinsic motivation on creativity, which are
consistent with Western studies. Above all, this attribution
might be accessible for cross-cultural collaborations and
information sharing. In addition, in light of aesthetic
judgements, there is a significant consensus of opinion
among experts across cultures (Niu & Sternberg, 2001,
2003). One empirical study evidenced this intention.
Paletz and Peng (2008) found that both novelty and
appropriateness play an important role on ratings of
creativity across Chinese, Japanese, and Americans.
In particular, appropriateness was more important for
the Americans and Japanese than for the Chinese. Also
novelty weighed heavily overall for creativity. Their
findings confirmed that East Asian cultures are not
homogeneous. Moreover, Runco and Johnson(2002)
18
Copyright © Canadian Academy of Oriental and Occidental Culture
The Interplay Between Culture and Creativity
investigated how parents and teachers from the U.S.
and India perceived creative traits in their children and
students. The results displayed the similar pattern, which
was creative characteristics as favorable and uncreative
characteristics as undesirable. Specifically, U.S. parents
and teachers showed signi cantly more attention on those
clusters than their counterparts.
The study of cultural differenceon creativity, Ng (2003)
utilized the samples from China and U.S. and found that
the society of liberal individualism is more conducive
to people engaging in creative behavior than the society
of Confucianism. The possible reason is that the
psychological bound constrain the collectivistic members
to have premium to behave in a creative manner. It is said
that Confucian ideology has the tendency toward prizing
collectivism and social conformity (Dineen & Niu, 2008).
In addition, in Asian classroom, a paradox illustrated
that the Confucian tradition of learning reinforces the
conformity, which is detrimental to creative performance
(Craft, 2008). The emphasis of this peculiar conception
of learning is on moral cultivation: teachers as moral
exemplar and students as docile sponges. There is a clear-
cut hierarchical relationship between teacher and student
(Radclyffe-Thomas, 2007).
Ng and Smith(2004) confirmed this assertion by
comparing the attitudes of teachers on creativity behaviors
between conservative-autocratic teachers and liberal-
democratic teachers. The results showed that the latter
had a stronger tendency to encourage creativity behaviors
in class. They also found that cultural individualism
had a positive influence on liberal-democratic teaching
attitudes, whereas cultural collectivism had opposite
effects. They concluded that the constraint of Confucian
tradition might be a closing of the Asian creative mind.
Nevertheless, some beneficial evidence provides a
promising ground for promoting creativity in a relatively
conformist social context. For example, Niu and Sternberg
(2001, 2003) found the discrepancy in creative expression
between the Chinese and American students, but it is
still possible to temporarily enhance Chinese students’
creative performance through appropriate instructions.
They concluded that the environment has a potential and
important impact on individuals’ artistic creativity. In
line with these ndings, Dineen and Niu (2008) utilized
a creative pedagogic model developed in the U.K.,
which showed a considerable effect on perception and
production of creativity for Chinese students. The visual
arts works produced by Chinese students through teaching
intervention demonstrated high quality of creative
productions, comparing with traditional Chinese teaching
strategy. Further, the qualitative data also exhibited the
merits of this approach that boosted learners’ intrinsic
motivation and confidence, which is a vital component
of creative development. Because of the effectiveness
and appropriateness of this model, they suggested the
possibility of temporally boosting learners’ creativity
through the provision of a suitable learning environment.
CONCLUSION
The relationship between cultural and creative expression
is not only reciprocal but also the conflux of historical,
societal, and individual variables. This profound in uence
evinces perception of creativity and creative expression
(Niu & Sternberg, 2001; Rudowicz, 2003). Ludwig
(1992) recognized four powerful cultural impacts on
the engagement of creativity activities: subject, form
of expression, functions of expression serving, and
types of individuals selected. In addition, Lubart and
Sternberg (1998) underlined that within the framework
of social environment; it not only may provide physical
or social stimulation but also serves the function of
evaluation of creative products and performances. Thus,
the importance of taking culture into account while
conducting creativity research is identified by Lubart
(1990): With the administration of cross-cultural studies,
“we begin to discover how deeply creativity is bound to
cultural context”(p.55). By doing do, the benefit is that
researchers could have a more holistic picture of creativity
via incorporating sociocultural milieu in the framework of
analysis (Ludwig, 1992; Ng & Smith, 2004).
Given the literature review available at this juncture,
some further suggestions for the cross-cultural study
on creativity might be helpful to uncover the nature of
creativity. First, the majority of studies are based on
comparison cultural groups (e.g., East and West); the
analysis within cultural groups might provide valuable
insights (e.g., Japan and Taiwan). The main reason is
that even under the heritage of Confucianism, East Asian
nations are not homogeneous; within-group differences do
exist (Radclyffe-Thomas, 2007). Furthermore, different
level of cultural variations is also important subjects to be
examined, such as subcultural unites or speci c contexts
at the individual level (Lubart & Sternberg, 1998). In
brief, the research of place-related in uences on creativity
provides a promising ground to decrease blind spots when
researchers try to understand creativity.
With regard to assessing creative performance, Lubart
(1990) underscored that although Torrance tests are
widely popular in the cross-cultural creativity research,
the results are questionable as regards actual perceptions
of creativity embodied in the specific culture. Further,
as Amabile (1996) pointed out, the problem of a battery
of creativity tests is validity. Namely, the nature of those
assessments developed by the Western psychologists only
limits to narrow ranges of abilities that inappropriately
captures the general creative behaviors as well as good
indicators. Additionally, that is why sometimes the poor
performance of creativity tests developed by American
on people in different countries thanks to cultural bias in
the understanding of creativity (Paletz & Peng, 2008). As
19 Copyright © Canadian Academy of Oriental and Occidental Culture
Kuan Chen Tsai (2012).
Cross-Cultural Communication, 8
(2), 15-20
a result, Amabile (1996) concluded that eventually the
creativity test should take social, cultural, and historical
factors into consideration. Although it is a big challenge
for social psychologist and other creativity researchers, it
is a necessary process for further research and to develop
alternative context-suitable tests in order to authentically
re ect the creative performance in a speci c context.
For the purpose of pedagogy, intercultural
communication, especially in the education context, might
be also an important role to bridge to the cultural gap
(Radclyffe-Thomas, 2007). For example, if East educators
who want to promote creativity in their classrooms,
what strategies could they use? What pitfalls might they
face? How to overcome some limits of their educational
systems? Here, an action research conducted by Dineen
and Niu (2008) could serve as a good example.On the
one hand, Eastern educators could take advantage of
research ndings (e.g., Scott, Leritz, & Mumford, 2004)
form the West perspective as examples and grounds; on
the other hand, they also should take context and culture
into consideration. For example, a number of creativity
activities and workbooks for adult and children were
developed by Western (Davis, 2006). As a result, Asian
teachers could utilize those resources as a reference when
applying in their classroom. But they might change the
content and format to fit the context.The chances are
what work for their counterparts do not necessarily work
for their own situations. Taken together,more successful
creativity promoting in classroom across all subject areas
and cultures should be observed and documented. For
instance, the study from Dineen and Niu (2008) provides
a good example for implementation of U.K. model
on Chinese classroom.It would be useful to follower
researchers and practitioners at various points in their
journeys of teaching creativity in order to highlight
effective pedagogy for moving forward (Edelson, 1999).
In sum, creativity scholars are recognizing that
sociocultural values do play a key role in conceptualization
and actualization of creativity (Rudowicz, 2003). Under
the umbrella of globalization, nevertheless, to what extent
each society could resist this phenomenon in order for
preserve of its unique value and simultaneously leaves
some room for creative development.
REFERENCES
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in Context: Update to the
Social Psychology of Creativity. Boulder, Colorado:
Westview Press.
Amabile, T. M. (1998). How to Kill Creativity. Harvard Business
Review, 76(5), 76-87.
Amabile, T. M. (2001). Beyond Talent. American Psychologist,
56(4), 333.
Cannon M. J. (2010). Understanding Global Cultures:
Metaphorical Journeys Through 29 Nations, Clusters of
Nations, Continents, and Diversity. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage Publications.
Craft, A. (2003). The Limits to Creativity in Education:
Dilemmas for the Educator. British Journal of Educational
Studies, 51(2), 113-127. DOI:10.1111/1467-8527.t01-1-
00229
Craft, A. (2008). Tensions in Creativity and Education: Enter
Wisdom and Trusteeship?. In A. Craft, H. Gardner & G.
Claxton (Eds.), Creativity, Wisdom, and Trusteeship (p.16-
34). Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
Craft, A., Gardner, H. & Claxton, G. (2008). Nurturing
Creativity, Wisdom, and Trusteeship in Education. In A.
Craft, H. Gardner & G. Claxton (Eds.), Creativity, Wisdom,
and Trusteeship (pp. 1-13). Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin
Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, Culture, and Person: A
Systems View of Creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The
Nature of Creativity: Contemporary Psychology Perspectives
(pp. 325-339). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the
Psychology of Discovery and Invention. New York, NY:
Harper Collins.
Davis, G. A. (2006). Gifted Children and Gifted Education: A
Handbook for Teachers and Parents. Scottsdale, AZ: Great
Potential Press.
Dineen, R. & Niu, W. (2008). The Effectiveness of Western
Creative Teaching Methods in China: An Action Research
Project. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts,
2(1), 42-52. DOI:10.1037/1931-3896.2.1.42
Edelson, P. (1999). Creativity and Adult Education. New
Directions for Adult & Continuing Education, 81, 3.
Feldman, D. & Benjamin, A. C. (2006). Creativity and
Education: An American Retrospective. Cambridge Journal
of Education, 36(3), 319-336.
Ford, C. M. (1996). A Theory of Individual Creative Action in
Multiple Social Domains. The Academy of Management
Review, 21(4), 1112.
Hennessey, B. A. (1995). Social, Environmental and
Developmental Issues and Creativity. Educational
Psychology Review, 7(2), 163.
Hennessey, B. A. (2003). The Social Psychology of Creativity.
Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 47(3), 253.
Hennessey, B. A. & Amabile, T. M. (1988). The Conditions of
Creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The Nature of Creativity:
Contemporary Psychology Perspectives (pp. 11-38). New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Lubart, T. I. (1990). Creativity and Cross-cultural Variation.
International Journal of Psychology, 25(1), 39.
Lubart, T. I. & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Creativity Across Time
and Place: Life Span and Cross-cultural Perspectives. High
Ability Studies, 9(1), 59-59.
Ludwig, A. M. (1992). Culture and Creativity. American Journal
of Psychotherapy, 46(3), 454.
Moeller, H. G. (2006). The Philosophy of the Daodejing.
Chichester. NY: Columbia University Press.
20
Copyright © Canadian Academy of Oriental and Occidental Culture
The Interplay Between Culture and Creativity
Mumford, M. D. (2000). Something Old, Something New:
Revisiting Guilford's Conception of Creative Problem
Solving. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3, 4), 267-276.
Ng, A. K. (2003). A Cultural Model of Creative and Conforming
Behavior. Creativity Research Journal, 15(2/3), 223.
Ng, A. K. & Smith, I. (2004). The Paradox of Promoting
Creativity in the Asian Classroom: An Empirical
Investigation. Genetic, Social & General Psychology
Monographs, 130(4), 307-330.
Niu, W. & Sternberg, R. J. (2001). Cultural Influences
on Artistic Creativity and Its Evaluation.
International Journal of Psychology, 36(4), 225-241.
DOI:10.1080/00207590143000036
Niu, W. & Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Societal and School
Influences on Student Creativity: The Case of China.
Psychology in the Schools, 40(1), 103-114. DOI:10.1002/
pits.10072
Niu, W. & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The Philosophical Roots of
Western and Eastern Conceptions of Creativity. Journal of
Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 26(1), 18-38.
DOI:10.1037/h0091265
Oral, G., Kaufman, J. C. & Agars, M. D. (2007). Examining Creativity
in Turkey: Do Western Findings Apply?. High Ability
Studies, 18(2), 235-246. DOI:10.1080/13598130701709590
Paletz, S. B. F. & Peng, K. (2008). Implicit Theories of Creativity
Across Cultures: Novelty and Appropriateness in Two
Product Domains. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
39(3), 286-302. DOI: 10.1177/0022022108315112
Radclyffe-Thomas, N. (2007). Intercultural Chameleons or
the Chinese Way? Chinese Students in Western Art and
design Education. Art, Design & Communication in Higher
Education, 6(1), 41-55. DOI:10.1386/adch.6.1.41_1
Rudowicz, E. (2003). Creativity and Culture: A Two Way
Interaction. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research,
47(3), 273.
Runco, M. A. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of
Psychology, 55(1), 657-687. DOI:10.1146/annurev.
psych.55.090902.141502
Runco, M. A. & Johnson, D. J. (2002). Parents’ and
Teachers’Implicit Theories of Children’s Creativity: A
Cross-cultural Perspective. Creativity Research Journal,
14(3-4), 427-438.
Scott, G., Leritz, L. E. & Mumford, M. D. (2004). The
Effectiveness of Creativity Training: A Quantitative Review.
Creativity Research Journal, 16(4), 361-388. DOI:10.1207/
s15326934crj1604_1
Shi, J., Qu, X. & Lin, T. (2007). Creativity and Its Cultivation.
In A.G. Tan (Ed.), Creativity: A Handbook for Teachers (pp.
65-75). Hackensack, NJ: World Scienti c Publishing.
Simonton, D. (2000). Creativity: Cognitive, Personal,
Developmental, and Social Aspects. American Psychologist,
55(1), 151-158. DOI:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.151
Simonton, D. & Shing-Shiang, T. (2010). Creativity in Eastern
and Western Civilizations: The Lessons of Historiometry.
Management & Organization Review, 6(3), 329-350.
DOI:10.1111/j.1740-8784.2010.00188.x
Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Creative Thinking in the Classroom.
Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 47(3), 325.
Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The Nature of Creativity. Creativity
Research Journal, 18(1), 87-98. DOI:10.1207/
s15326934crj1801_10
Sternberg, R. J. & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the Crowd:
Cultivating Creativity in a Culture of Conformity. New
York, NY: The Free Press.
Taylor, C. W. (1988). Various Approaches to and De nitions of
Creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The Nature of Creativity:
Contemporary Psychology Perspectives (pp. 99-121). New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Torrance, E. P. (1977). Creativity in the Classroom: What
Research Says to the Teacher. West Haven, CT: National
Education Association.
Torrance, E. P. (1988). The Nature of Creativity as Manifest in
IitsTest. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The Nature of Creativity:
Contemporary Psychology Perspectives (pp. 43-75). New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Treffinger, D. J. (2004). Research on Creativity. In D. J.
Treffinger (Ed.), Creativity and Giftedness (pp. 87-96).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
DiLiello, T. C. & Houghton, J. D. (2006). Maximizing
Organizational Leadership Capacity for the Future: Toward
a Model of Self-leadership, Innovation and Creativity.
Journal of Managerial Psychology: Self-leadership, 21(4),
319-337.
Walberg, H. J. (1988). Creativity and Talent as Learning. In R.
J. Sternberg (Ed.), The Nature of Creativity: Contemporary
Psychology Perspectives (pp. 340-361). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
... Diante da literatura disponível sobre a questão, Tsai (2012) argumenta que a maior parte dos estudos se baseia na comparação entre grupos culturais, notadamente ocidentais e orientais, destacando que outro tipo de estudo, ainda pouco explorado, volta-se à análise de subgrupos. Tal constatação ampara-se no reconhecimento de que as nações não são homogêneas, fazendo-se notar variações culturais entre grupos dentro de um mesmo país, as quais podem ser destacadas como importante ponto a ser examinado. ...
Article
Full-text available
O presente estudo teve como objetivo investigar a influência do ambiente no desempenho criativo de crianças e adolescentes. Uma amostra composta por 1.250 estudantes provenientes das cinco regiões do Brasil (250 de cada uma) respondeu ao teste de criatividade figural infantil. De um modo geral, os resultados apontaram para a influência da variável região de moradia nos quatro fatores avaliados pelo instrumento, assim como na maior parte das 12 características criativas nele avaliado. Em todos os fatores, perfis de desempenho bastante diferenciados foram encontrados entre as regiões brasileiras, de modo a demonstrar que, no presente estudo, as influências ambientais parecem estar influenciando a produção criativa dos estudantes avaliados. Tais resultados confirmam os apontamentos da literatura de que a criatividade não pode ser compreendida sem considerar o contexto em que o indivíduo encontra-se inserido, sendo importante considerar tal informação no momento de avaliação.
... In light of the results of the study, a similar study might be conducted on different samples, and policy-makers attention might be directed to the inclusion of the research variables in the educational curricula. Davis et al., 2011;Kelemen, 2015;Manning, 2006;Runco, 2007;Yassin et al., 2012Selby et al., 2005 ( Dabrowski, 1970( Dabrowski, , 1972( Dabrowski, , 1973 (Mendaglio & Tillier, 2006: Smith, 2006 (Chan, 2005;Charyton et al., 2008;Tsai, 2012 ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to investigate the contributions of ‎overexcitabilities to the creative personalities of gifted and ‎ordinary secondary school students in the State of Kuwait. ‎Furthermore, to determine the significance of the differences in ‎overexcitability‏ ‏and creative personality, according to the ‎student's classification, gender, and the interaction between ‎them. To achieve the research objectives, the research measures, ‎namely: overexcitabilities, and creative personality, were ‎validated and administered to a sample of 545 secondary school ‎students (545, of whom 176 are gifted and 369 are ordinary), ‎during the second semester of the academic year 2019- 2020. ‎They were selected using the available sampling method (for the ‎gifted) and the cluster method (for the normal). A two-way ‎Anova revealed that there are statistically significant effects of ‎student’s classification on their estimations of creative ‎personality in favor of gifted students, while there is no ‎statistically significant effect of gender, or the interaction ‎between gender and student’s classification on their estimations. ‎A two-way Manova revealed that there is a statistically ‎significant effect of the student’s classification on the student’s ‎practice of overexcitabilities in favor of gifted students, while ‎there is no statistically significant effect of gender or the ‎interaction between gender and the student’s classification on ‎the student’s practice of overexcitabilities. Multiple regression ‎analysis revealed that mental overexcitability contributes to the ‎creative personalities of gifted students while sensory ‎overexcitability contributes to the creative personality of ‎ordinary students. It was observed that the contributions of ‎sensory overexcitability to the creative personality of the gifted ‎students are statistically stronger than those of their ‎counterparts. In light of the results of the study, a similar study ‎might be conducted on different samples, and policy-makers ‎attention might be directed to the inclusion of the research ‎variables in the educational curricula.‎ ‎(Keywords: Overexcitability, Creative Personality, Gifted ‎Students, Ordinary Students, State of Kuwait)
... However, recent work often describes the four factors of creative thinking as originality, flexibility, fluency, and elaboration (Torrance, 2008). The definition of creativity differs across cultures, especially from East to West (Tsai, 2012), as 120 definitions of creativity are categorized into four broad categories -generating ideas, deepening thoughts, openness, courage to explore ideas, and listening to one's inner voice (Treffinger et al., 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
The terminal objective of this study was to investigate the contribution of habits of mind (i.e. taking responsible risks, thinking flexibility, thinking about thinking [metacognition], persistence, striving for accuracy, creative self-efficacy and creative environment), on the creative personality. The questionnaire, which included eight scales, was administered to a sample of 205 secondary school students in the State of Kuwait. Data were analysed using Structural Equation Modelling (SEM). The results revealed that: (1) habits of mind contribute to the creative personality; (2) the creative environment and creative self-efficacy contribute to the creative personality; (3) the creative environment mediates the relationship between habits of mind (i.e. taking responsible risks, thinking flexibility, persistence and striving for accuracy) and the creative personality; and (4) creative self-efficacy mediates the relationship between habits of mind (i.e. taking responsible risks and persistence) and the creative personality.
... Researchers might overgeneralize among Asian countries-speaking of East versus West-and minimize between-country differences in the West or between-country differences in the East. Thus, future research should not only focus on cross-cultural, but also on within-country comparisons of several countries from different continents-and not only on the US and one East Asian country (see also Tsai, 2012). ...
... Despite the importance of creativity in many societies, researchers argue that individuals' creative expressions and experiences can differ across cultures (Mourgues et al., 2015;Tsai, 2012). Niu and Sternberg (2006) argue that individuals' concepts about creativity can differ in the East and West, given their distinct philosophical roots. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research examined the transactions between adolescents’ after school activities and their divergent thinking. Three times over the course of 18 months, American and Chinese adolescents (N = 566) filled out open-ended surveys about how they spend their time after school and completed a set of divergent thinking tasks. Adolescents’ reports of their after school activities were coded into 4 mutually exclusive categories: Personal academic (e.g., studying alone), personal nonacademic (e.g., watching TV alone), social nonacademic (e.g., playing sports with friends), and social academic (e.g., working on group projects). Compared with their American counterparts, Chinese adolescents engaged in more personal nonacademic activities; American (vs. Chinese) adolescents participated in a greater number of social academic activities and social nonacademic activities. Transactional processes were similarly evident in the United States and China: The more adolescents engaged in academic-oriented after school activities, the more they had heightened divergent thinking; in turn, divergent thinking was predictive of adolescents’ engagement in such activities over time.
... Japan has introduced policies such as yutori kyōiku-relaxed education or education free from pressure (Watabe & Hibbard, 2014), which may also lead to changes in how curricula are structured to foster creativity. Despite the importance of creativity in many societies, researchers contend that individuals' conceptions of creativity can differ across cultures (Mourgues et al., 2015;Tsai, 2012), which may result in differences in how creativity should be cultivated. Unfortunately, there is limited research on the variations of such conceptions among early adolescents residing in diverse cultural environments. ...
Article
Full-text available
Our research examined whether adolescents in the United States, China, and Japan differed in their conceptions of a creative person. Participants were American (n = 321), Chinese (n = 235), and Japanese (n = 393) adolescents in 7th and 8th grades who completed surveys at 3 time points. Using an open-ended questionnaire, adolescents were asked to list up to 10 attributes of a creative person. The responses were coded into 4 categories: action, emotion, characteristics, and self (vs. others). Results indicated that adolescents in the United States used more action and emotion descriptors (e.g., draws, happy) when conceptualizing a creative person, compared to Chinese and Japanese adolescents. In contrast, Chinese (vs. American and Japanese) adolescents were more likely to use descriptors about the characteristics of a person (e.g., hardworking, smart) in their conceptualizations of a creative person. Changes over time in adolescents’ conceptions of a creative person were evident, with the rates of such changes being uneven across countries. Findings provide evidence in support of the idea that adolescents’ implicit theories of creativity may be rooted in their cultural experiences.
... Indeed, some views of creativity are directly in tension with others. Creativity is studied from a wide range of perspectives, and its meaning varies across cultures, particularly from East to West (Tsai, 2012). ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
Are young people who believe that they are creative more likely to do good in the world? Using the concept of creative self-efficacy this review explores potential connections between creativity and meaningful social action. The review finds five potentially interesting connections which merit further investigation: 1. heightened imaginative awareness and potential empathy in the lives of others 2. levels of curiosity and interest in social issues 3. perseverance, self-efficacy and the likelihood of making a positive difference more widely 4. an interest in collaboration and a sense of social belonging 5. an ethic of excellence and a willingness to become involved in voluntary activities in areas of interest.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to develop a Turkish version of the Creative Self-Efficacy Scale (CSES) and to explore its psychometric properties. Participants were 489 preservice teachers enrolled in a public university in Turkey (n = 489). Two-hundred-thirty-five of 489 preservice teachers (48.06%) participated in the first study for the exploratory factor analysis (EFA), and 254 (51.94%) participated in the second study for the confirmatory factor analysis. Two-hundred-sixty (53.2%) of the participants were studying in preschool teaching and 132 (46.8%) were studying in classroom teaching programs. CSES, which is a 3-item Likert-type English questionnaire, was translated into Turkish by the researcher. Eight researchers who were expert in Turkish education, English language teaching, educational measurement and evaluation, elementary education and educational technology fields participated in the back-translation and expert review processes. Scale scores did not differ according to sex, age, grade or department of the respondent. However, creative self-efficacy was observed to be related to design self-efficacy.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
We consider creativity as the ability to see situations in an original and unusual way. Design thinking techniques are suitable to introduce the differences between divergent and convergent thinking to newcomers at creativity. However, these beginners might feel that the model of design thinking restricts them in this liberating activity. Also, the idea that by passing through the four steps of the design thinking model a perfect solution will be found, is neither realistic nor removes their uncertainties. Moreover, whether an emphasis on the future context of a designed object, and the implementation of different storytelling methods at the front end of innovation could be recognized as added value, must still be inquired. A two-day workshop that applied techniques from design thinking and futures studies combined with narrative techniques is developed to foster creativity among User Experience students in China. In particular, the participants are design thinking "newbies" who are also not used to an educational context of studentled discovery and collaborative learning. This is a reflective paper, based on empirical experience that articulates a new attitude towards the use of methods to stimulate creativity in non-design students.
Article
Full-text available
Creativity plays an important role in the advancement of all societies around the world, yet the role of cultural influences on creativity is still unclear. Following systems theory, activity theory, and ecocultural theory, semistructured interviews with 30 renowned artists (writers, composers, and visual artists) from Cuba, Germany, and Russia were conducted to explore the complexity of the creative process and potential cultural differences. All interviews were transcribed verbatim and analyzed using consensual qualitative research methodology. The following eight main domains resulted from the interviews: How I became an artist, What being an artist means to me, Creating as a cognitive process, Creating as an emotional process, Creating as a motivational process, Fostering factors of creativity, Hindering factors, and The role of culture in creating. Artists in the three countries similarly talked about creativity being a fluid process where ideas change, and elaborated on the role of intuition and the unconscious when creating art. Meaningful cross-cultural differences were seen among the artists of three cultural backgrounds in terms of attitudes about financial instability, in how they perceive themselves, in their art’s societal function, in the cognitive and in the emotional process of creating, and in terms of social connectedness. Results highlight (a) the complexity of the creative process going beyond cognitive factors and including motivational, emotional, and sociocultural factors, and (b) the cultural differences in the creative process. Results are beneficial for further developing a comprehensive theory of the creative process taking cultural differences into consideration.
Article
Full-text available
Although many psychologists have expressed an interest in the phenomenon of creativity, psychological research on this topic did not rapidly, expand until after J. P. Guilford claimed in his 1950 APA presidential address, that this topic deserved far more attention than it was then receiving. This article reviews the progress psychologists have mane in understanding creativity, since Guilford's call to arms. Research progress has taken place on 4 fronts: the cognitive processes involved in the creative act, the distinctive characteristics of the creative person, the development non manifestation of creativity across the individual life span, and the social environments most strongly associated with creative activity. Although some important questions remain unanswered, psychologists now know more than ever before about how individuals achieve this special and significant form of optimal human functioning.
Chapter
“This is a significant book… for a multitude of audiences, including scholars, practitioners, students, expatriates, travelers, and those who are simply interested in culture… This book is also an ideal reference tool, since the metaphors are easy to remember yet rich in contextual value and are presented in a logical structure for quick consultation. Overall, this book is enormously appealing, genuinely useful, and a worthy addition to any collection.” —Thunderbird International Business Review (2002) In Understanding Global Cultures, Fourth Edition, authors Martin J. Gannon and Rajnandini Pillai present the cultural metaphor as a method for understanding the cultural mindsets of individual nations, clusters of nations, and even continents. The fully updated Fourth Edition continues to emphasize that metaphors are guidelines to help outsiders quickly understand what members of a culture consider important. This new edition includes a new part structure, three completely new chapters, and major revisions to chapters on American football, Russian ballet, and the Israeli kibbutz. New and Continuing Features: Emphasizes clusters of national cultures and variations within each cluster, as well as both topic-oriented (authority-ranking cultures, market-pricing cultures, etc.) and cluster-focused descriptions Includes three new parts: India, Shiva, and Diversity; Scandinavian Egalitarian Cultures (Sweden, Denmark, and Finland); and Other Egalitarian Cultures (including Canada and Germany) Provides three completely new chapters: Finnish Sauna, Kaleidoscopic India and Diversity, and a final integrative summary chapter Integrates chapters through the frameworks of the GLOBE study, the Hofstede study, Hall, and Kluckholn and Strodbeck Highlights religious and ethnic diversity throughout AncillariesInstructor Resources are available on a password-protected website at www.sagepub.com/gannon4instr. These include applications, discussion questions, model examinations,100 exercises, and suggested syllabi. Qualified instructors may contact Customer Care to receive access to the site.Understanding Global Cultures: Metaphorical Journeys Through 29 Nations, Clusters of Nations, Continents, and Diversity is appropriate for courses in International Business and Management, Strategic Management and Planning, and Cultural Studies.
Article
Over the course of his career, Guilford produced a remarkable body of research on creative thought. Today, this research is remembered primarily for its articulation of the notion that divergent thinking plays a key role in creative thought. However, a number of other capacities relevant to understanding creative thought were identified in the course of this research effort. In this article, I review this research program as a whole with special reference to those capacities that warrant more attention in current studies of creativity. Implications for current research are discussed.
Article
Over the course of the last half century, numerous training programs intended to develop creativity capacities have been proposed. In this study, a quantitative meta-analysis of program evaluation efforts was conducted. Based on 70 prior studies, it was found that well-designed creativity training programs typically induce gaïns in performance with these effects generalizing across criteria, settings, and target populations. Moreover, these effects held when internal validity considerations were taken into account. An examination of the factors contributing to the relative effectiveness of these training programs indicated that more successful programs were likely to focus on development of cognitive skills and the heuristics involved in skill application, using realistic exercises appropriate to the domain at hand. The implications of these observations for the development of creativity through educational and training interventions are discussed along with directions for future research.
Article
Motivation plays a crucial role in the creative process. It is not enough to have unusually high levels of skill or a deep conceptual understanding. In order for students to reach their creative potential, they must approach a task with intrinsic motivation; they must engage in that task for the sheer pleasure and enjoyment of the activity itself rather than for some external goal. Researchers and theorists now understand that there is a direct relation between the motivational orientation brought to a task and the likelihood of creativity at that task. And it is particular features of the school environment and students' daily routine that in large part determine that motivation. The present paper outlines investigations revealing that the typical classroom is fraught with teaching practices and programme features that kill intrinsic motivation and creativity. Research designed to immunise students against the negative effects of these damaging classroom elements is reviewed. The argument is made that the undermining of intrinsic motivation and creativity of performance may be largely driven by an affective, rather than a cognitive, mechanism, and recent cross-cultural data gathered in a non-western educational setting are reviewed.
Article
Two studies were designed to compare (a) the rated creativity of artworks created by American and Chinese college students, and (b) the criteria used by American and Chinese judges to evaluate these artworks. The study demonstrated that the two groups of students differed in their artistic creativity. American participants produced more creative and aesthetically pleasing artworks than did their Chinese counterparts, and this difference in performance was recognized by both American and Chinese judges. The difference between the use of criteria by American and Chinese judges was small, and consisted mainly of the American judges' use of stricter standards in evaluating overall creativity. Moreover, in general, there was a greater consensus among Chinese judges regarding what constitutes creativity than among American judges. The study also revealed, but preliminarily, that the artistic creativity of Chinese students was more likely to be reduced as a function of restrictive task constraints or of the absence of explicit instructions to be creative. The results of this study seem to support the hypothesis that an independent self-oriented culture is more encouraging of the development of artistic creativity than is an interdependent self-oriented culture. Other possible explanations, such as differences in people's attitudes toward and motivation for engaging in art activities, or socioeconomic factors might also account for differences in people's artistic creativity.
Article
What is the relationship of creativity to gifted behavior? The author raises key questions relative to previous research in the field. He builds and defends a model for weaving creativity into the fabric of exceptional performance in any arena of endeavor.
Article
Although early childhood education in America has always had the creative child at the core of its approach, the field itself has generated little systematic research on the topic. In contrast, the scholarly field of creativity studies, with its focus on basic research and theoretical questions, has impacted education only slightly. Creativity and education may have been disconnected topics in a scholarly sense in the US primarily because (1) early childhood education has relied primarily on theory imported from abroad as a guide to practice; (2) psychometric research on creativity proved conceptually and methodologically flawed, short circuiting widespread use of creativity tests by American public schools; and (3) contemporary approaches to creativity have tended to be either broadly theoretical or primarily concerned with answering basic research questions. The article discusses contemporary creativity research; cautionary notes, drawn from the American experience, are also included.