ChapterPDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Happiness is the main goal; most individuals reach out for a happy life and many policy makers aim at greater happiness for a greater number. This pursuit of happiness calls for understanding of conditions for happiness and for that reason the subject has received much attention in the history of western thought. The study of happiness has long been a playground for philosophical speculation. Due to the lack of empirical measures of happiness, it was not possible to check propositions about the matter. Hence, understanding of happiness remained speculative and uncertain. During the last decades, survey-research methods introduced by the social sciences have brought a breakthrough. Dependable measures of happiness have developed, by means of which a significant body of knowledge has evolved. This chapter presents an account on this field. The literature on happiness can be framed within some key questions that can be ordered as steps in the process for creating greater happiness for a greater number. (1) What is happiness precisely? (2) Can happiness be measured? (3) How happy are people presently? (4) What causes us to be happy or unhappy? (5) Can happiness be raised lastingly?
Content may be subject to copyright.
HAPPINESS:
Also known as ‘life-satisfaction’ and ‘subjective well-being’
Ruut Veenhoven, Erasmus University, Rotterdam.
In: Kenneth C. Land, Alex C. Michalos, and M. Joseph Sirgy (Eds.) Handbook of
Social Indicators and Quality of Life Research. 2012 Dordrecht, Netherlands:
Springer Publishers. page 63-77. DOI 10.1007/978-94-007-2421-1_3.
Happiness is a main goal, most individuals reach out for a happy life and many policy
makers aim at greater happiness for a greater number. This pursuit of happiness calls for
understanding of conditions for happiness and for that reason the subject has received
much attention in the history of western thought. The study of happiness has long been a
playground for philosophical speculation. By lack of empirical measures of happiness, it
was not possible to check propositions about the matter. Hence, understanding of
happiness remained speculative and uncertain. During the last decades, survey-research
methods introduced by the social sciences have brought a break-through. Dependable
measures of happiness have developed, by means of which a significant body of
knowledge has evolved. This chapter presents an account of this field.
The literature on happiness can be framed within some key-questions that can be
ordered as steps in the process for creating greater happiness for a greater number. 1)
What is happiness precisely? 2) Can happiness be measured? 3) How happy are people
presently? 4) What causes us to be happy or unhappy? 5) Can happiness be raised
lastingly?
1. WHAT IS ‘HAPPINESS’?
The word 'happiness' is used in various ways. In the widest sense it is an umbrella term
for all that is good. In this meaning it is often used interchangeably with terms like
'wellbeing' or 'quality of life' and denotes both individual and social welfare. This use of
words suggests that there is one ultimate good and disguises differences in interest
between individuals and society. It further suggests that all merits can be integrated in
one final scale of worth, which is not the case. The term is merely an umbrella for
different notions of what is good. Below I will delineate four qualities of life and show
that the concept of happiness fits only one of these.
1.1 Four qualities of life
Quality-of-life concepts can be sorted using two distinctions, which together provide a
fourfold matrix. That classification is discussed in more detail in Veenhoven (2000).
The first distinction is between chances and outcomes, that is, the difference
between opportunities for a good life and the good life itself. A second difference is
between outer and inner qualities of life, in other words between 'external' and 'internal'
Correspondence: Prof. Dr. Ruut Veenhoven Erasmus University Rotterdam, Faculty of Social Sciences,
P.O.B. 1738 3000 DR Rotterdam, Netherlands. www2.eur.nl/fsw/research/veenhoven
Printed version : www.SpringerLink.com
features. In the first case the quality is in the environment, in the latter it is in the
individual. Lane (1994) made this distinction clear by distinguishing 'quality of society'
from 'quality of persons'. The combination of these two dichotomies yields a fourfold
matrix. This classification is presented in table 1.
Livability of the environment
The left top quadrant denotes the meaning of good living conditions, shortly called
‘livability’.
Ecologists see livability in the natural environment and describe it in terms of
pollution, global warming and degradation of nature. Currently, they associate livability
typically with preservation of the environment. City planners see livability in the built
environment and associate it with such things as sewer systems, traffic jams and ghetto
formation. Here the good life is seen as a fruit of human intervention.
In the sociological view, society is central. Livability is associated with the quality of
society as a whole and also with the position one has in society.
Livability is not what is called happiness here. It is rather a precondition for
happiness and not all environmental conditions are equally conducive to happiness.
Life-ability of the person
The right top quadrant denotes inner life-chances. That is: how well we are equipped to
cope with the problems of life. Sen (1992) calls this quality of life variant 'capability'. I
prefer the simple term 'life-ability', which contrasts elegantly with 'livability'.
The most common depiction of this quality of life is absence of functional
defects. This is 'health' in the limited sense, sometimes referred to as 'negative health'.
Next to absence of disease, one can consider excellence of function. This is referred to
as 'positive health' and associated with energy and resilience. A further step is to
evaluate capability in a developmental perspective and to include acquisition of new
skills for living. This is commonly denoted by the term 'self-actualization'. From this
point of view a middle-aged man is not 'well' if he behaves like an adolescent, even if he
functions without problems at this level. Since abilities do not develop along side
idleness, this quality of life is close to the 'activity' in Aristotle's concept of eudemonia.
Lastly, the term 'art of living' denotes special life-abilities; in most contexts this quality
is distinguished from mental health and sometimes even attributed to slightly disturbed
persons. Art of living is associated with refined tastes, an ability to enjoy life and an
original style of life.
Ability to deal with the problems of life will mostly contribute to happiness as
defined here, but is not identical. If one is competent in living one has a good chance at
happiness, but this endowment does not guarantee an enjoyable outcome.
Utility of life
The left bottom quadrant represents the notion that a good life must be good for
something more than itself. This assumes some higher values. There is no current
generic for these external outcomes of life. Gerson (1976: 795) refers to these effects as
'transcendental' conceptions of quality of life. Another appellation is 'meaning of life',
which then denotes 'true' significance instead of mere subjective sense of meaning. I
prefer the simpler 'utility of life', while admitting that this label may also give rise to
misunderstanding.
When evaluating the external effects of a life, one can consider its functionality
for the environment. In this context, doctors stress how essential a patient's life is to its
Ruut Veenhoven
2
HAPPINESS
intimates. At a higher level, quality of life is seen in contributions to society. Historians
see quality in the addition an individual can make to human culture, and rate for
example the lives of great inventors higher than those of anonymous peasants. Moralists
see quality in the preservation of the moral order, and would deem the life of a saint to
be better than that of a sinner. As an individual's life can have many environmental
effects, the number of such utilities is almost infinite.
Apart from its functional utility, life is also judged on its moral or esthetic value.
For instance, most of us would attribute more quality to the life of Florence Nightingale
than to that of a drunk, even if it appeared in the end that her good works had a negative
result in the end. In classic moral philosophy this is called 'virtuous living', and is often
presented as the essence of 'true happiness'.
Here the focus is on mere 'experiential' happiness; on how much one likes the
life one lives. The difference is well expressed in the earlier mentioned statement of
Mill (1863) that he preferred an unhappy Socrates to a happy fool. Moral excellence is
clearly not the same as feeling good.
Core meaning: Subjective enjoyment of life
Finally, the bottom right quadrant represents the inner outcomes of life. That is the
quality in the eye of the beholder. As we deal with conscious humans, this quality boils
down to subjective enjoyment of life. This is commonly referred to by terms such as
'subjective wellbeing', 'life-satisfaction' and 'happiness' in a limited sense of the word.
This is the kind of happiness the utilitarian philosophers had in mind and it is also the
kind of happiness addressed here.
Humans are capable of evaluating their life in different ways. We have in
common with all higher animals that we can appraise our situation affectively. We feel
good or bad about particular things and our mood level signals overall adaptation. As in
animals these affective appraisals are automatic, but unlike other animals it is known
that humans can reflect on this experience. We have an idea of how we have felt over
the last year, while a cat does not. Humans can also judge life cognitively by comparing
life as it is with notions of how it should be.
Most human evaluations are based on both sources of information, that is:
intuitive affective appraisal and cognitively guided evaluation. The mix depends mainly
on the object. Tangible things such as our income are typically evaluated by
comparison; intangible matters such as sexual attractiveness are evaluated by how it
feels. This dual evaluation system probably makes the human experiential repertoire
richer than that of our fellow-creatures.
In evaluating our life we typically summarize this rich experience in overall
appraisals. For instance we appreciate several domains of life. When asked how we feel
about our work or our marriage we will mostly have an opinion. Likewise, most people
form ideas about separate qualities of their life, for instance how challenging their life is
and whether there is any meaning in it. Such judgments are made in different time-
perspectives, in the past, the present and in the future.
Mostly such judgments are not very salient in our consciousness. Now and then,
they pop to mind spontaneously. Though not in the forefront of consciousness all the
time estimates of subjective enjoyment of life can be recalled and refreshed when
needed. This makes these appraisals measurable in principle.
Ruut Veenhoven
3
HAPPINESS
1.2 Four kinds of satisfaction
Even when we focus on subjective satisfaction with life, there are still different
meanings associated with the word happiness. These meanings can also be charted in a
fourfold matrix. In this case, that classification is based on the following dichotomies:
Life-aspects versus life-as-a-whole and passing delight versus enduring satisfaction.
Above, we have seen that appraisals of life can concern aspects, such as
marriage or work-life, and one's life-as-a-whole. The word 'happiness' is used in both
contexts. Obviously, such appraisals are linked. Enjoyment of aspects of life will
typically contribute to the satisfaction with life as a whole (so-called bottom-up effect),
and enjoyment of one's life-as-a-whole appears to foster the satisfaction with life-
aspects (top-down). Still, these are not identical matters. One can have a happy marriage
but still be dissatisfied with life-as-a-whole, or be satisfied with life-as-a-whole in spite
of an unhappy marriage.
Next, the experience of enjoyment can be short-lived or enduring. Again, the
word happiness is used for both phenomena. Sometimes it refers to passing moods and
on other occasions to stable satisfaction. Once more, these matters are related but not
the same.
When combined, these distinctions produce the fourfold matrix presented in
table 2. The distinction between part and whole is presented vertically, and the
distinction between passing and enduring enjoyment horizontally.
Instant satisfaction
The top-left quadrant represents passing enjoyments of life-aspects. Examples would be
delight in a cup of tea at breakfast, the satisfaction of a chore done or the enjoyment of a
piece of art. I refer to this category as 'instant-satisfactions'. Kahneman (1999:4) calls it
'instant-utilities'. This quadrant represents hedonistic happiness, especially when the
focus is on sensory experience. The concept of happiness used here is broader however.
It concerns both overall satisfaction and life-as-a-whole. Though fleeting enjoyment
obviously contributes to a positive appreciation of life it is not the whole of it.
Domain satisfaction
The top right quadrant denotes enduring appreciation of life-aspects, such as marriage
satisfaction and job-satisfaction. This is currently referred to as domain-satisfactions.
Though domain-satisfactions depend typically on a continuous flow of instant-
satisfactions, they have some continuity of their own. For instance, one can remain
satisfied with one's marriage even if one has not enjoyed the company of the spouse for
quite some time. Domain-satisfactions are often denoted with the term happiness: a
happy marriage, happy with one's job, etc. Yet in this chapter the term happiness is used
in the broader sense of satisfaction with life-as-a-whole. One would not call a person
happy who is satisfied with marriage and job, but still dissatisfied on the whole because
his health is failing. It is even possible that someone is satisfied with all the domains
one can think of, but nevertheless feels depressed.
Top-experience
The bottom right quadrant denotes the combination of passing experience and appraisal
of life-as-a-whole. That combination occurs typically in top-experiences, which involve
short-lived but quite intense feelings and the perception of wholeness. This is the kind
of happiness poet's write about. Again this is not the kind of happiness aimed at here. A
moment of bliss is not enduring appreciation of life. In fact such top-experiences even
Ruut Veenhoven
4
HAPPINESS
seem detrimental to lasting satisfaction, possibly because of their disorientating effects
(Diener et. al. 1991).
Core meaning: lasting satisfaction with one's life-as-a-whole
Lastly, the bottom-right quadrant represents the combination of enduring satisfaction
with life-as-a-whole. This is what I mean with the word happiness and is then
synonymous with 'life-satisfaction'. This is the meaning the utilitarian philosophers had
in mind when talking about happiness as the 'sum' of pleasures and pains. In such
contexts they denoted a balance over time and thus a durable matter.
1.3 Definition of happiness
Overall happiness is the degree to which an individual judges the overall quality of
his/her own life-as-a-whole favorably. In other words: how much one likes the life one
leads. This definition is explained in more detail in Veenhoven (1984:22-25).
1.4 Components of happiness
When evaluating the favorableness of life, we tend to use two more or less distinct
sources of information: our affects and their thoughts (Veenhoven 2009). One can
decide that one feels fine most of the time and one can also judge that life seems to meet
ones (conscious) demands. These appraisals do not necessarily coincide. We may feel
fine generally, but nevertheless be aware that we failed to realize our aspirations. Or one
may have surpassed ones aspirations, but nevertheless feel miserable. Using the word
'happiness' in both these cases would result in three different kinds of happiness, the
overall judgment as described above and these two specific appraisals. Therefore the
components are referred to as 'hedonic level of affect' and 'contentment'. To mark the
difference with the encompassing judgment I will refer to happiness (the core concept)
as overall happiness. A synonym for overall happiness is 'life-satisfaction'.
Hedonic level of affect
Hedonic level of affect is the degree to which various affects that someone experiences
are pleasant in character. Hedonic level of affect is not the same as 'mood'. We
experience different kinds of mood: elated moods, calm moods, restless moods, moody
moods, etc. Each of these moods is characterized by a special mixture of affective
experience, one of which is 'hedonic tone' or 'pleasantness'. The concept of hedonic
level concerns only the pleasantness experienced in affects; that is, the pleasantness in
feelings, in emotions, as well as in moods. So a high hedonic level may be based on
strong but passing emotions of love, as well as on moods of steady calmness.
A person's average hedonic level of affect can be assessed over different periods
of time: an hour, a week, a year, as well as over a lifetime. The focus here is on
'characteristic' hedonic level. That is so to say: the average over a long time-span such
as a month or a year. The concept does not presume subjective awareness of that
average level.
Contentment
Contentment is the degree to which an individual perceives his/her aspirations are met.
The concept presupposes that the individual has developed some conscious wants and
has formed an idea about their realization. The factual correctness of this idea is not at
stake. The concept concerns the individual's subjective perception.
Ruut Veenhoven
5
HAPPINESS
1.5 Synonyms
The above defined concept of ‘overall happiness’ is denoted with different words. In the
1950s the words adjustment and morale were sometimes used in this meaning and since
the 1960s the term life-satisfaction came into use for this purpose. In 1984 Ed Diener
introduced the term subjective well-being 1, abbreviated as SWB, and this term is still
dominant in psychology.
The term life-satisfaction is mostly used for ‘overall happiness’, but refers in
some cases particularly to its cognitive component and is than synonymous with
‘contentment’. In such context, the term happiness is typically used for the affective
appraisal of life and then synonymous with ‘hedonic level of affect’.
The term subjective well-being is also used in wider meanings than happiness as
defined here. Sometimes the term refers to good mental functioning and then denotes
the meaning of life-ability in the top right quadrant of scheme 1. At other occasions the
term is used as a generic for all subjective enjoyment and then covers all the quadrants
of scheme 2.
2. CAN HAPPINESS BE MEASURED?
Measurement has long been understood as `objective' and `external' assessment,
analogous to the measurement of blood-pressure by a doctor. By now we know that
happiness cannot be measured that way. Steady physiological correlates have not been
discovered, and probably never will be. Nor have any overt behaviors been found to be
consistently linked to inner enjoyment of life. Like most attitudinal phenomena,
happiness is only partially reflected in behavior. Suicidal behavior is probably more
indicative of happiness. Almost all people who attempt or commit suicide are quite
unhappy. However, not all the unhappy seek resort to suicide. In fact, only a fraction
does.
Inference from overt behavior being impossible, we must make do with
questioning. That is, simply asking people how much they enjoy their life-as-a-whole.
Questioning is an appropriate method of measurement in this case since happiness is
defined as something we have on our mind.
Questions on happiness can be posed in various contexts; clinical interviews,
life-review questionnaires and common survey interviews. The questions can be posed
in different ways; directly or indirectly, and by means of single or multiple items.
2.1 Common questions
Some common questions are presented in Table 3.
2.2 Validity doubts
Critics have suggested that responses to questions on happiness actually measure other
phenomena. Rather than indicating how much the respondent enjoys life, answers
would reflect his normative notions and desires.
No notion
One of the misgivings is that most people have no opinion at all about their happiness.
They would be more aware of how happy they are supposed to be, and report that
instead. Though this may happen incidentally, it does not appear to be the rule. Most
people know quite well whether or not they enjoy life. Eight out of ten Americans think
Ruut Veenhoven
6
HAPPINESS
1 Ed Diener defines subjective well-being as being satisfied with one’s life, while feeling good and
this conceptualization also involves both cognitive and affective appraisals of life.
of it every week. Responses on questions about happiness tend to be prompt. Non-
response on these items is low; both absolutely (± 1%) and relatively to other attitudinal
questions. `Don't know' responses are infrequent as well.
A related assertion is that respondents mix up how happy they actually are, with
how happy other people think they are, given their situation. If so, people considered to
be well off would typically report to be very happy, and people regarded as
disadvantaged should characterize themselves as unhappy. That pattern is observed
sometimes, but it is not general. For instance, in The Netherlands good education is seen
as a pre-requisite for a good life, but the highly educated appear slightly less happy in
comparison to their less educated counterparts.
Colored answers
Another objection concerns the presence of systematic bias in responses. It is assumed
that questions on happiness are interpreted correctly, but that responses are often false.
People who are actually dissatisfied with their life would tend to answer that they are
quite happy. Both ego-defense and social-desirability would cause such distortions.
This bias is seen to manifest itself in over-report of happiness; most people claim
to be happy, and most perceive themselves as happier than average. Another indication
of bias is seen in the finding that psycho-somatic complaints are not uncommon among
the happy. However, these findings allow other interpretations as well. Firstly, the fact
that more people say to be happy than unhappy does not imply over-report of happiness.
It is quite possible that most people are truly happy (some reasons will be discussed
below). Secondly, there are also good reasons why most people think that they are
happier than average. One such reason is that most people are like critical scientists and
think that unhappiness is the rule. Thirdly, the occurrence of head-aches and worries
among the happy does not prove response distortion. Life can be a sore trial some times,
but still be satisfying on a balance.
The proof of the pudding is in demonstrating the response distortion itself. Some
clinical studies have tried to do so by comparing responses to single direct questions
with ratings based on depth interviews and projective tests. The results are generally not
different from responses to single direct questions posed by an anonymous interviewer.
2.3 Reliability doubts
Though single questions on happiness seem to measure what they are supposed to
measure, they measure it rather imprecisely.
When the same question is asked twice in an interview, responses are not always
identical. Correlations are about +.70. Over a period of a week, test-retest reliability
drops to circa +.60. Though responses seldom change from `happy' to `unhappy',
switches from `very' to `fairly' are rather common. The difference between response-
options is often ambiguous. The respondent's notion about his/her happiness tends to be
global. Thus the choice for one answer-category or the next is sometimes haphazard.
Because choice is often arbitrary, subtle differences in interrogation can exert
considerable effect. Variations in place where the interview is held, characteristics of
the interviewer, sequence of questions and precise wording of the key-item can tip the
scale to one response or the other. Such effects can occur in different phases of the
response process; in the consideration of the answer as well as in the communication of
it.
Ruut Veenhoven
7
HAPPINESS
Bias in appraisal
Though most people have an idea of how much they enjoy life, responding to questions
on this matter involves more than just bringing up an earlier judgment from memory.
For the most part, memory only indicates a range of happiness. Typically, the matter is
re-assessed in an instant judgment. This re-appraisal may be limited to recent change
(are there any reasons to be more or less happy than I used to be?), but it can also
involve quick re-evaluation of life (what are my blessings and frustrations?). In making
such instant judgments, people use various heuristics. These mental simplifications are
attended with specific errors. For instance the `availability' heuristic involves orientation
on pieces of information that happen to be readily available. If the interviewer is in a
wheelchair, the benefit of good health is salient. Respondents in good health will then
rate their happiness somewhat higher and the correlation of happiness-ratings with
health variables will be more pronounced. Several of these heuristic effects have been
demonstrated by Schwarz and Strack (1991).
Bias in response
Once a respondent has formed a private judgment, the next step is to communicate it. At
this stage reports can be biased in various ways as well. One source of bias is inherent to
semantics; respondents interpret words differently and some interpretations may be
emphasized by earlier questions. For example, questions on happiness are more likely to
be interpreted as referring to `contentment' when preceded by questions on success in
work, rather than items on mood. Another source of response-bias is found in
considerations of self-presentation and social-desirability. Self-rating of happiness tends
to be slightly higher in personal interviews than on anonymous questionnaires.
However, direct contact with an interviewer does not always inflate happiness reports. If
the interviewer is in a wheel-chair, modest self-presentation is encouraged.
Much of these biases are random, and balance out in large samples. So in large
samples, random error does not affect the accuracy of happiness averages. Yet it does
affect correlations, random error 'attenuates' correlations. Random error can be
estimated by means of multiple-trait-multiple-method (MTMM) studies, and
correlations can be corrected (disattenuated) on that basis. A first application on
satisfaction measures is reported by Saris et. al. (1996).
Some biases may be systematic; especially bias produced by technique of
interrogation and sequence of questions. Bias of that kind does affect the reliability of
distributional data. In principle it does not affect correlations, unless the measure of the
correlate is biased in the same way (correlated error). To some extend, systematic error
can also be estimated and corrected. See also Saris et al. (1996).
2.4 Comparability across nations
Average happiness differs markedly across nations. In scheme 4 we will see that
Russians score currently 5.4 on a 0-10 scale, while in Canada the average is 7.7. Does
that mean that Russians really take less pleasure in life? Several claims to the contrary
have been advanced. Elsewhere I have checked these doubts (Ouweneel & Veenhoven
(1991), Veenhoven (1993). The results of that inquiry are summarized below.
The first objection is that differences in language hinder comparison. Words like
`happiness' and `satisfaction' would not have the same connotations in different tongues.
Questions using such terms would therefore measure slightly different matters. I
checked that hypothesis by comparing the rank orders produced by three kinds of
questions on life-satisfaction: a question about `happiness', a question about
Ruut Veenhoven
8
HAPPINESS
`satisfaction with life' and a question that invites to a rating between `best- and worst
possible life'. The rank orders appeared to be almost identical. I also compared
responses on questions on happiness and satisfaction in two bi-lingual countries, and
found no evidence for linguistic bias either.
A second objection is that responses are differentially distorted by desirability-
bias. In countries where happiness ranks high in value, people would be more inclined
to overstate their enjoyment of life. I inspected that claim by checking whether reported
happiness is indeed higher in countries where hedonic values are most endorsed. This
appeared not to be the case. As a second check, I inspected whether reports of general
happiness deviate more from feelings in the past few weeks in these countries; the
former measure being more vulnerable for desirability distortion than the latter. This
appeared not to be the case either.
A third claim is that response-styles distort the answers dissimilarly in different
countries. For instance, collectivistic orientation would discourage `very' happy
responses, because modest self-presentation is more appropriate within that cultural
context. I tested this hypothesis by comparing happiness in countries differing in value-
collectivism, but found no effect in the predicted direction. The hypothesis failed
several other tests as well.
A related claim is that happiness is a typical western concept. Unfamiliarity with
it in non-western nations would lead to lower scores. If so, we can expect more `don't
know' and `no answer' responses in non-western nations. However, that appeared not to
be the case.
The issue of `cultural bias in the measurement' of happiness must be distinguished from
the question of `cultural influence on the appraisal' of life. Russians can be truly less
happy than Canadians, but be so because of a gloomier outlook-on-life, rather than as a
result of an inferior quality-of-life. This latter matter will be discussed in section 4 of
this chapter.
3. HOW HAPPY ARE WE?
Throughout time, social critics have bemoaned the miseries of life. Man is said to be
unhappy, and real happiness is projected in past paradise or future utopia. Optimists,
who stressed human adaptability and social progress, have always denounced such
bilious claims. By lack of an empirical gauge, the discussion remained inconclusive.
During the last few decades many surveys have been carried out, some drawing on
world samples (available in the World Database of Happiness). Together, the data
support the optimist view.
Most people are happy
Figure 1 presents the distribution for responses to the 10-step question on life-
satisfaction in the USA. The most frequent responses are 7, 8 and 9 and less than 2 %
scores below neutral. The average is 7,4. This result implies that most people must feel
happy most of the time. That view has been corroborated by yearly follow-up studies
over many years (Ehrhardt et al 2000) and by studies that use the technique of
experience sampling (Schimmack, U. & Diener, E. 2003).
The high level of happiness is not unique to the USA. Table 4 shows similar averages in
Ruut Veenhoven
9
HAPPINESS
other western nations. In fact, average happiness tends to be above neutral in most
countries of the world. So happiness for a great number is apparently possible.
No mere resignation
Nevertheless, some social critics are still reluctant to believe that modern man is happy.
Reported happiness is discounted as sullen adjustment. Rather than really enjoying their
life, people would just give up hope for a better one and try to adjust to the inevitable
(e.g. Ipsen 1978). Various defensive strategies would be used: simple denial of one's
misery, downward comparison and a tendency to see things rosier than they actually
are. Depressives would see the world more realistic. In addition to the above discussion
on validity, two counter-arguments can be mentioned:
Firstly, such resignation must give itself away in a discrepancy between the
`adjusted' judgment of life and `raw' affective experience. Appraisal of affect is
probably less vulnerable to cognitive adaptation, because it is a direct experience and
thus less open to defensive distortion. It is also less threatening to admit that one felt
depressed in the last few weeks than to admit disappointment in life. Various surveys
have assessed both general happiness and last week’s affect-balance. The results do not
suggest that people claim to be happy but actually feel lousy (research reviewed in
Veenhoven 1984: 106/113). Time sampling of mood-states also shows that pleasant
affect dominates unpleasant affect (see e.g. Bless & Schwarz 1984 for a meta-analysis
of 18 studies).
Secondly, people are typically unhappy when they live in miserable conditions.
As we have seen, unhappiness is the rule in poor third world countries. In western
nations happiness is typically lower where adverse conditions accumulate, such as in
persons who are poor, lonely and ill (Glatzer & Zapf 1984:282-397).
Together these findings suggest that people tend to enjoy their lives once
conditions are tolerable. From an adaptive-biological point of view, this does not seem
strange. Nature is unlikely to have burdened us with chronic unhappiness. Like `health',
happiness would seem to be the normal condition.
Why still so many complaints?
The prevalence of happiness does not wash away the multitude of suffering and
complaining. Even the happy are not without complaints. The German Welfare Survey
found that half of the subjects who say to be satisfied with their life-as-a-whole report
frequent worries (Glatzer & Zapf 1984:180). If not due to response distortion, what else
can explain this pattern of worried happiness?
Firstly, it is important to note that happiness and complaining do not exclude
each other logically. One can be satisfied with life-as-a-whole, but still be aware of
serious deficits. In fact, both stem from a reflection on life. Secondly, worrying may
contribute to happiness in the end. Only through realistic acknowledgement of danger
can we cope effectively with the problems of life.
4. WHAT CAUSES US TO BE MORE OF LESS HAPPY?
Having established that people differ in happiness, the next question is why. So far, the
determinants of happiness are only dimly understood. Still, it is clear that various levels
of human functioning are involved; collective action and individual behavior, simple
sensory experiences and higher cognition, stable characteristics of the individual and his
Ruut Veenhoven
10
HAPPINESS
environment as well as freaks of fate. Table 5 presents a tentative ordering of factors
and processes in a sequence-model.
The model presumes that the judgment of life draws on the flow of life-
experiences, particularly on positive and negative experience. This is what the utilitarian
philosophers referred to as "pleasures and pains". The flow of experiences is a mental
reaction to the course of life-events. This includes major one-time events, such as
marriage or migrations, as well as repetitious mundane events, like getting up in the
morning and doing the dishes. The events that happen in life are partly a matter of good
or bad luck, such as in the case of accidents. The occurrences of life-events also depend
on given conditions and capacities. Traffic accidents are less frequent in well-organized
societies and among attentive persons. Thus, the chances of `rewarding' and `aversive'
events are not the same for everybody. This is commonly referred to as life-chances.
Present life-chances root in past events and chance-structures, in societal history as well
as individual development.
An example may illustrate this four-step model: A person's life-chances may be
poor, because he/she lives in a lawless society, is in a powerless position in that society,
and is personally neither smart nor nice (step 1). That person will run into many adverse
events. He/she will be robbed, duped, humiliated and excluded (step 2). Therefore that
person will frequently feel anxious, angry and lonely (step 3). Based on this flow of
experience that person will judge life-as-a-whole negatively (step 4). Causality can skip
a step. For instance, poor legal protection (step 1) may instigate feelings of anxiety (step
3) directly, because the person anticipates on events that are likely to happen, but have
not occurred. Life-chances (step 1) can even enter the evaluation of life (step 4) right
away, when comparisons enter the judgment. Likewise, not all life-events in step 2
follow from life-chances at step 1. Some events are a matter of good or bad luck and
happen irrespective of social position or psychological capabilities. Nor is the flow of
life-experiences (step 3) entirely shaped by the course of events (step 2). How pleasant
or unpleasant we feel also depends on dispositions and interpretations.
4.1 Livability of the environment
Research on happiness has focused on its relation to life-chances. Below is a review of
the main findings up to 2010.
Quality of society
Average happiness differs greatly across nations. We have seen earlier that differences
cannot be explained by cultural bias in the measurement of happiness. We will see later
that they can neither be attributed to cultural variation in outlook on life. On the other
hand, there is a strong basis for interpretation of this variation in terms of differential
livability of society. Scheme 6 presents many strong correlations between average
happiness and societal qualities.
Much of the above mentioned correlates of average happiness are part of the
'modernity' syndrome. Hence, similar patterns emerge if we consider further indicators
of modernity, such as urbanization, industrialization, Informatisation and
individualization. The more modern the country, the happier its citizens are. This
finding will be a surprise to prophets of doom, who associate modernity with anomie
and alienation. Though modernization may involve problems indeed, its benefits are
clearly greater (Veenhoven 2005).
Ruut Veenhoven
11
HAPPINESS
Individual position in society
Numerous studies all over the world have considered differences in individual
happiness within countries. Because most of these studies are inspired by egalitarian
social policy, the emphasis is often on social differences, such as in income, education
and employment. Contrary to expectation these positional differences bear little
relationship to happiness, at least not in modern affluent society. Together positional
variables explain mostly no more than 10% of the variance in happiness. The main
findings are summarized in table 7.
4.2 Life-ability of the individual
The strongest correlations observed are at the psychological level; happy people are
typically better endowed than the unhappy. The common variance explained by such
variables tends to be around 30%. Some main findings are summarized in table 8. Much
of the findings on individual variation in happiness boil down to a difference in ability
to control ones environment and this pattern seems to be universal.
4.3 Course of life-events
The effect of life-events on happiness has received little attention. One of the few
sophisticated studies that considered the matter is the four-wave `Australian Quality of
Life Panel Study' by Heady & Wearing (1992).
First, this study showed that the course of life-events is not the same for
everybody. Some people find troubles repeatedly; they have accidents, are laid off,
quarrel with family, fall ill, etc. On the other hand there are also people who are lucky
most of the time; they meet nice people, get promoted, have children who do well, etc.
These systematic differences in the course of events depend to some extent on life-
chances. In the study, favorable events appeared to happen more often to persons who
were well educated and psychologically extraverted. Adverse events were more
frequent among neurotics, but occurred less to people with good intimate attachments.
Both favorable and unfavorable events happened more to persons who were young and
psychologically open. Together, the life-chances considered explained about 35% of the
variation in life-events over eight years.
The study also demonstrated that the course of life-events affects the appraisal of
life. First, it was found that the balance of favorable and adverse events in one year
predicts reported happiness in the next year. The more positive that balance, the greater
the satisfaction with life. Life-events explained some 25% of the differences in life-
satisfaction, of which about 10% were independent of social position and personality.
Next, longitudinal analysis indicated that change in characteristic pattern of events was
followed by change in happiness. Respondents who shifted to a more positive balance
became happier.
4.4 Flow of experience
As of yet, hedonic experience is not well understood. Though the feelings of disgust and
delight are quite tangible, it is not clear how they come about and why.
Function of hedonic experience
Much of our likes and dislikes seem to be inborn reactions to situations that are good
and bad for human survival. Evolution has probably eliminated our forefathers who did
Ruut Veenhoven
12
HAPPINESS
not enjoy food, shelter and company, or lacked dislikes for danger. As such, certain life-
events are likely to elicit pleasant experiences while others invoke unpleasant feelings.
Playing tennis with friends is typically more fun than sitting in jail alone.
Though it is quite plausible that hedonic experience reflects the gratification of basic
needs, it is not so clear what these needs are precisely. Current theory suggests that
there are various `organic needs' (food, shelter, sex), `social needs' (belonging, esteem)
and broader `self-actualizing' needs (mastery, control, variety, meaning, etc).
Conceptions differ however, and it is difficult to establish to what extent these strivings
are inborn and how they are linked to hedonic experience.
Cognitive theories suggest that pleasant experience can also be induced by
perceived realization of goals. For instance, that we enjoy playing tennis because we
successfully execute an intention and dislike the jail because it does not fit our plans.
The gratifying effects of perceived reality-want fits may draw on an underlying need for
control.
Pleasant and unpleasant events
Many adverse events evoke similar reactions in most people, particularly events that
exceed human adaptability. Everybody suffers when burned or starved. However,
within the limits of human faculties, reactions tend to differ. For instance, not
everybody feels equally as bad when his/her house burns down. Reactions vary with
earlier exposure to hardship, with meaning attributed to the event and with
psychological resilience. Still most people get more or less upset by the loss of their
property. Variability is probably greater in the pleasurable experiences people derive
from life-events. Though most of us enjoy feasts, this is not true for everybody. Some
people lack the social skills required for feasting, are not accepted by the participants, or
are have a limited capacity for enjoyment anyway.
The various personal characteristics that mold experiential reactions on life-
events belong to the same class of `life-chances' that also influence the course of events.
Low social status may result both in few invitations for feasts and in uneasy feelings at
the occasional celebrations one attends. Still, the life-events evoke experience and not
the life chances. Effects of daily events on daily experiences have been studied by
means of time sampling. In this method, respondents note several times during the day
how well they feel at that moment and what they are doing. Based on such studies
Csikszentmihalyi (1991) found that we tend to feel better in company than we do alone
and finer in leisure activities than we do at work. Structured leisure-activities such as
sporting appeared more rewarding than unstructured pastimes, such as television
viewing. This pattern is probably universal. Personality explains about 30% of the
variance in pleasant affect; situations explain another 10% and person-situation
interaction 20%.
Inner manufacturing of feeling
Though it is clear that events evoke experiences, it is not so clear how such effects come
about. In fact, little is known of how likes and dislikes are processed. We have some
idea about the psychophysiology of sensations, but the inner fabrication of affective
experience is hardly understood. Psychology has been more successful in grasping
thinking than affect.
In the 1960's, the discovery of pleasure centers in the brain seemed to promise a
break-through (Olds 1954, Rolls, 1979). That promise has become somewhat bleak by
now. There is no such thing as a single happiness gland. Pleasurable experience seems
Ruut Veenhoven
13
HAPPINESS
to result from different bio-chemical signals in both the body and the brain, the
interactions of which are still largely unknown.
Capacity for enjoyment
Wherever situated, the human capacity for enjoyment is great. Reward areas in the brain
seem to be greater than areas that produce unpleasant experience and most people tend
to feel good most of the time (Bless & Schwarz 1984). Suffering may be more salient
than satisfaction, but it is not more frequent.
There is some logic in this phenomenon. Why would nature doom us to be
unhappy most of the time? If experiences of like and dislike serve to indicate conditions
that are good and bad for the organism, we should expect that happiness is the rule.
Evolution tends to produce a good fit of species to its environment, which will is
reflected in predominance of pleasurable experiences. Dysphoric experience is to keep
away from harmful situations; it instigates withdrawal. Therefore, unhappiness can be
permanent only in adverse living conditions from which no escape is possible. In such
conditions species tend to die out. In this view, chronic unhappiness can at best be a
temporary phenomenon in the declining stage. However, the human species does not
seem to be drawing to its end, and if we get extinct that will be due to ecological
disaster rather than to mal-adaptation to our living environment.
The organic disposition to enjoy things may not be as strong in everybody.
There can be temperamental differences in happiness-proneness. Twin-studies show
greater resemblance in happiness between monozygotic twins than di-zygotic twins,
even when reared apart. However, this does not mark happiness itself as a
temperamental trait; the similarity in enjoyment of life can also result from other traits
that are instrumental to happiness, such as heritable variation in `energy' or `resilience'.
The results of a follow-up study from birth on do not suggest that there is a marked
temperamental disposition to be happy or unhappy. Babies observed to be cheerful did
not appear to be more likely to report high life-satisfaction in adulthood. (Research
reviewed in Veenhoven 1994)
4.5 Inner process of evaluation
What goes on in people when they evaluate their life? Speculations on these matters
were a main issue in antique philosophy of happiness. This issue enjoys a renewed
interest nowadays. It is not just curiosity about the inside of the black box that draws the
attention, but rather the far-reaching consequences of the different points of view for the
possibilities of creating greater happiness (to be discussed in the next section).
Calculus or inference?
Utilitarian philosophers spoke of happiness as the "sum of pleasures and pains",
established in a "mental calculus". This view on the evaluation process is still dominant
nowadays. Happiness is seen to be assessed in a similar way as accountants calculate
profit. We would count our blessings and blights and then strike a balance. The
judgment is then a bottom-up process, in which appraisals of various aspects of life are
combined into an overall judgment.
In this line, Andrews & Withey (1976) suggested that satisfaction with life-as-a-
whole is calculated from satisfactions with life-domains. In this view, we first evaluate
domains of life, such as our job and marriage, by means of comparing the reality of life
with various standards of success, like `security' and `variation'. Next, we would
compute an average, weighted by perceived importance of domains. Andrews & Withey
Ruut Veenhoven
14
HAPPINESS
demonstrate high correlations between satisfaction with life-as-a-whole and life-domain
appraisals, but found no evidence for the presumed weighing. Michalos' (1985)
Multiple-Discrepancy-Theory also depicts happiness as the sum of various sub-
evaluations. In his thinking, sub-evaluations are assessments of discrepancy between
perceptions of how ones life 'is' with notions of how it 'should be'. The five main
comparison standards are presented as: what one `wants', what one `had' earlier in life,
what one `expected' to have, what one thinks `other people' have, and what one thinks is
`deserved'. Michalos provides ample evidence that small discrepancies are accompanied
by high satisfaction with life-as-a-whole. Multiple regression analysis showed that
happiness is primarily a function of perceived discrepancy between reality and `wants'.
Though enjoyment of life-as-a-whole is statistically correlated with appraisals of
various aspects of life, it has not been established that happiness is causally determined
by these sub-evaluations. The correlation can also be due to top-down effects. For
instance, when assessing his job-satisfaction a person can reason "I am generally happy,
so apparently I like my job". Panel-analysis has demonstrated strong effects of this kind.
Actually, the effect of happiness on perception of have-want discrepancies is greater
than the effect of gap-size on happiness (Heady et al 1991).
Inference on the basis of feeling
A rival theory is that evaluations of life draw on cues that provide indications of the
quality of life-as-a-whole. An internal cue of this kind is how well one generally feels; if
pleasant affect dominates, life cannot be too bad. An external cue is how happy other
people think one is (reflected appraisal).
The available evidence suggests that internal affective cues are far more
important than external social ones. Happiness is much more related to matters of mood
than to reputation. In assessing how we generally feel, we seem to focus on the relative
frequency of positive and negative affects, rather than on the remembered intensity of
joy and suffering (Diener et al 1991). A typical heuristic seems to involve departing
from the mood of the moment, which can be read quite vividly, and next considering
how representative that mood is for general affective experience (Schwarz 1991)
Schwarz & Strack (1991) showed that evaluations of life-as-a-whole draw on how one
generally feels. This facilitates the judgmental task. Most people know well how they
generally feel. The alternative of `calculating' happiness is more difficult and time
consuming. It requires selection of standards, assessments of success and integration of
the appraisals into an overall judgment. Not only does this involve more mental
operations, but it also entails many arbitrary decisions. Still, people sometimes choose
to follow this more difficult road. A condition that encourages calculative evaluation is
uncertainty about one's typical mood. For instance, in depression it is hard to estimate
how one generally feels. Another factor that invites to the calculative approach may be
the availability of salient information for comparison, such as the earlier mentioned
confrontation with a person in a wheelchair.
Evaluations of specific aspects of life can less well be derived from estimates of
general affect. One can be satisfied with ones job, but still feel generally lousy, because
of a bad marriage and poor health. On the other hand, calculating is less difficult when
specific life-domains are concerned. The field is easier to oversee and the standards are
usually more evident.
Ruut Veenhoven
15
HAPPINESS
5. POSSIBILITY OF GREATER HAPPINESS
Much of the research on happiness is prompted by the hope of finding ways to create
greater happiness for a greater number. However, several theories about happiness
which imply that improvement of living conditions will not reduce discontent.
One such theory is that happiness is relative. Another is the theory that happiness is a
trait. Both theories have been tested and have been rejected (Veenhoven 1995). Another
comforting finding is that average happiness can be as high as 8 on a 0-10 scale.
Remember Scheme 5 that shown an average of 8.2 in Denmark and 8.1 in Switzerland.
What is possible in these countries should also be possible in other nations.
6. CONCLUSION
Happiness can be defined as subjective enjoyment of life-as-a-whole. Empirical studies
on happiness show considerable difference; both difference in average happiness across
countries and differences between citizens within countries. At its present stage, our
understanding of happiness already shows that greater happiness for a greater number is
possible in principle, and indicates some ways for achieving that goal.
Ruut Veenhoven
16
HAPPINESS
REFERENCES
Andrews, F.M. & Withey, S.B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being. (New York,
U.S.A.: Plenum Press)
Bless, H. & Schwarz, N. (1984). Ist schlechte Simmung die Ausnahme? Eine Meta-
analyse von Stimmungsuntersuchungen (Is bad mood exceptional? A meta
analysis). (Paper, University of Heidelberg, Dpt. Of Psychology, Germany)
Cantril, H. (1965). The pattern of human concern. (New Brunswick, U.S.A.: Rutgers
University Press)
Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Mei-Ha Wong (1991). The situational and personal correlates
of happiness: a cross national comparison. (In: Strack, F. et. al. (Eds.),
‘Subjective wellbeing’ (pp. 193-121). London, UK: Pergamon.)
Diener, E., Emmons, R.A., Larsen, R.J. & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life
scale.
Journal of Personality Assessment, Vol. 49, 71-75
Diener, E., Pavot, W. & Sandvik, E. (1991). Happiness is the frequency, not intensity of
positive and negative affect. (In: Strack, F. et. al. (Eds.), ‘Subjective
wellbeing’. London, UK: Pergamon.)
Ehrhardt, J. et al (2000). Stability of life satisfaction over time Analysis of change in
ranks in national population. Jounal of Happiness Studies, Vol. 1, pp. 177-205
Gerson, E.M. (1976). On quality of life. American Sociological Review, Vol. 41, pp.
793-806
Glatzer, W. & Zapf, W. eds. (1984). Lebensqualität in der Bundesrepublik (Quality of
life in West-Germany). (Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Campus Verlag)
Heady, B, Veenhoven, R. & Wearing, A (1991). Top-down versus Bottom-up: Theories
of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, Vol. 24, pp. 81-100
Headey, B. & Wearing, A. (1992). Understanding happiness: A theory of subjective
well-being. (Melbourne Australia: Longman Cheshire)
Ipsen, D. (1978). Das Konstrukt Zufriedenheit (The construct of satisfaction).
Soziale Welt, Vol. 29, pp 44-53
Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. (In: Kahneman, D., Diener, E. & Schwarz,
N. (Eds.), ‘Wellbeing: foundations of hedonic psychology’ (pp.3-25). New
York U.S.A.: Russell Sage Foundations.)
Lane, R.E. (1994). Quality of life and quality of persons: A new role for government?.
Political theory, Vol. 22, pp. 219-252
Ruut Veenhoven
17
HAPPINESS
Mill. J.S. (1863). Utilitarianism. (London: Fontana Press 1990 (20th print)
Ouweneel, P. & Veenhoven, R. (1991). Cross-national differences in happiness:
Cultural bias or societal quality?. (In: Bleichrodt, N. & Drenth, P.J. (Eds.),
‘Contemporary issues in cross-cultural psychology’ (pp. 168-184). Lisse,
Netherlands: Swets & Zeilinger.)
Olds, J. & Milner, O. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation
of septal areas and other regions of the rat brain. Journal of Comparative and
Physiological Psychology, Vol. 47, pp. 419-427
Rolls, E.T. (1979). Effects of electrical stimulation of the brain of behavior.
Psychological Surveys, Vol. 2, pp. 151-169
Saris, W., Scherpenzeel, A, & Veenhoven, R. (Eds.) (1996). Satisfaction in Europe.
(Budapest, Hungary: Eötvös University Press)
Schimmack, U. & Diener, E. (Eds.) (2003). Experience Sampling Method.
Special Issues Journal of happiness Studies, Vol. 4, nr 1
Schwarz, N. & Strack, N. (1991). Evaluating one’s life: a judgment model of subjective
well-being. (In: Strack, F. et. al. (Eds.), ‘Subjective wellbeing’ (pp. 27-48).
London, UK: Pergamon.)
Sen, A: (1992). Capability and wellbeing. (In: Sen, A. & Nussbaum, M. (Eds.), ‘The
quality of life’ (pp. 30-53 ). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.)
Veenhoven, R. (1984). Conditions of happiness. (Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer (now
Springer)
Veenhoven, R. (1988). The utility of happiness. Social Indicators Research, Vol. 20, pp.
333-354
Veenhoven, R. (1991). Is happiness relative?. Social Indicators Research, Vol. 24, pp.
1-34
Veenhoven, R. (1993). Happiness in nations: Subjective appreciation of life in 56
nations 1946-1992. (The Netherlands: Erasmus University Rotterdam, Risbo,
Studies in Social and Cultural Transformation. )
Veenhoven, R. (1994). Is happiness a Trait? Tests of the theory that a better society
does not make people any happier. Social Indicators Research, Vol. 32, p 101-
160
Veenhoven, R. (1995). The cross-cultural pattern of happiness. Test of predictions
implied in three theories of happiness. Social Indicators Research, Vol. 34 p
33-68
Ruut Veenhoven
18
HAPPINESS
Veenhoven, R. (1996). Happy life-expectancy: A comprehensive measure of quality-of-
life in nations. Social Indicators Research, Vol.39, pp. 1-58
Veenhoven, R. (1997). Progrés dans la compréhension du bonheur. Revue Québécoise
de Psychologie, Vol 18, pp 29-74
Veenhoven, R. (1998). Vergelijken van geluk in landen (Comparing happiness across
nations). Sociale Wetenschappen, Vol. 42, pp. 58-84
Veenhoven, R. (2000). The four qualities of life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1:. 1-39
Veenhoven, R (2005). Is life getting better? How long and happy do people live in
modern society? European Psychologist, special section on 'Human
development and Well-being', Vol. 10, pp. 330-343
Veenhoven, R. (2008). Healthy happiness: Effects of happiness on physical health and
consequences for preventive healthcare. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9: 449-
464
Veenhoven, R. (2009) How do we assess how happy we are? in: Dutt, A. K. & Radcliff,
B. (eds.) ‘Happiness, Economics and Politics: Towards a multi-disciplinary
approach’, Edward Elger Publishers, Cheltenham UK, page 45-69
Veenhoven, R. (2010) World Database of Happiness: Continuous register of research
on subjective enjoyment of life, Erasmus University Rotterdam, The
Netherlands. Available at: http://worlddatabaseofhappiness.eur.nl
Ruut Veenhoven
19
HAPPINESS
Table 1: Four qualities of life
Outer qualities
Inner qualities
Life chances
Livability of environment
Life-ability of the person
Life results
Utility of life
Satisfaction with life
Ruut Veenhoven
20
HAPPINESS
Table 2: Four kinds of satisfaction
Passing
Enduring
Part of life
Pleasure
Domain satisfaction
Life as a whole
Top experience
Happiness
Ruut Veenhoven
21
HAPPINESS
Table 3: Some currently used questions about happiness
Single questions
Taking all together, how happy would you say you are: very happy, quite happy,
not very happy, not at all happy?
(standard item in the World Value Studies)
How satisfied are you with the life you lead? Very satisfied, fairly satisfied, not
very satisfied, not at all satisfied?
(standard item in Euro-barometer surveys)
Here is a picture of a ladder. Suppose the top of the ladder represents the best
possible life for you and the bottom of the ladder the worst possible life. Where
on the ladder do you feel you personally stand at the present time? (0-10 ladder
like rating scale)
(Cantril's (1965) present life ladder rating)
Multiple questions (summed)
Same question asked twice: at the beginning and at the end of interview
How do you feel about your life-as-a-whole? Delighted, pleased, mostly
satisfying, mixed, mostly dissatisfying, unhappy, terrible?
(Andrews & Withey's (1976) Life 3)
Five questions, rated on a 1-7 scale ranging from strongly agree to strongly
disagree.
(Diener's 1985 Satisfaction With Life Scale SWLS 2)
- In most ways my life is close to ideal.
- The conditions of my life are excellent.
- I am satisfied with my life.
- So far I have gotten the important things I want in life.
- If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing
_________________________________________________________________________
_________________________________________________________________________
Ruut Veenhoven
22
HAPPINESS
2 In my view this last item is not appropriate. One can be quite satisfied with life, but still be open for the
opportunity to try something else.
Table 4: Life-satisfaction in nations around 2005 Average scores on scale 0-10
Denmark 8,3
Switzerland 8,0
Canada 7,6
USA 7.4
Israel 7,0
Japan 6,5
Korea (South) 6,0
India 5,5
Ukraine 5,0
Afghanistan 4,1
Zimbabwe 3,0
Togo 2,6
_____________________________________________________________________
Source: World Database of Happiness (Veenhoven 2010) Rank Report Average Happiness
_________________________________________________________________________
Ruut Veenhoven
23
HAPPINESS
Table 5: Evaluation of life: a sequence model of conditions and processes
LIFE-CHANCES COURSE OF EVENTS FLOW OF EXPERIENCE EVALUATION OF LIFE
Societal resources
economic welfare
social equality
political freedom
cultural lush
moral order
etc...
Personal resources
social position
o material property
o political influence
o social prestige
o family bonds
o etc..
individual abilities
o physical fitness
o psychic fortitude
o social capability
o intellectual skill
o etc...
Confrontation with:
deficit or affluence
attack or protection
solitude or company
humiliation or honor
routine or challenge
ugliness or beauty
etc...
Experiences of:
yearning or satiation
anxiety or safety
loneliness or love
rejection or respect
dullness or excitement
repulsion or rapture
etc...
Appraisal of average affect
Comparison with standards
of the good life
Striking an overall balance
of life
conditions for happiness appraisal process
Ruut Veenhoven
24
HAPPINESS
Table 6: Happiness and characteristics of nations around 2005
Condition in nation
Correlation with average happiness
Zero-order Wealth
controlled
N
Wealth
Purchasing power per head* +.67 - 137
Freedom
Economic* +.57 +.36 128
Political* +.50 +.31 130
Personal +.40 -.03 85
Equality
Disparity in incomes -.10 +.23 119
Discrimination of women -.79 -.62 99
Disparity in happiness -.52 -.16 97
Brotherhood
Tolerance +.48 +.39 77
Trust in people +.14 -.38 50
Voluntary work -.09 +.17 74
Social security +.44 +.15 107
Justice
Rule of law* +.64 +.30 146
Respect of civil rights* +.52 +.28 130
Corruption* -.65 -.32 146
Explained variance (R)
.70
122
Data: World Database of Happiness (Veenhoven 2010), States of Nations
* = included in regression
Ruut Veenhoven
25
HAPPINESS
Table 7: Happiness and position in society
________________________________________________________________________
Correlation Similarity of
correlation
within western nations across all nations
___________________________________________________________________________
Social rank
Income +
Education ±
Occupational prestige + +
Social participation
Employment ± +
Participation in associations + +
Primary network
Spouse ++ +
Children 0 ?
Friends + +
___________________________________________________________________________
++ = Strong positive + = Similar correlations
+ = Positive ± = Varying
0 = No relationship - = Different
correlations
- = Negative
? = Not yet investigated ? = No data
___________________________________________________________________________
Source: World Database of Happiness (Veenhoven 2010), Correlational Findings.
Ruut Veenhoven
26
HAPPINESS
Table 8: Happiness and life-abilities
___________________________________________________________________________
Correlation Similarity of correlation
within western nations across all nations
___________________________________________________________________________
Proficiencies
Physical health + +
Mental health ++ +
IQ 0 +
Personality
Internal control + +
Extraversion + +
Conscientiousness + ?
Art of living
Lust acceptance + +
Sociability ++ +
___________________________________________________________________________
++ = Strong positive + = Similar correlations
+ = Positive ± = Varying
0 = No relationship - = Different
correlations
- = Negative
? = Not yet investigated ? = No data
___________________________________________________________________________
Source: World Database of Happiness (Veenhoven 2010), Correlational Findings.
Ruut Veenhoven
27
HAPPINESS
Figure 1: Life-satisfaction in the USA 2007
dissatisfied satisfied
Source: World Value Survey 5
Ruut Veenhoven
28
HAPPINESS
... Happiness is often described as a subjective state of well-being marked by positive emotions, life satisfaction, and a sense of fulfillment (Veenhoven, 2011). It can be divided into two categories: shortterm happiness, derived from external stimuli and momentary joys, and long-term happiness, which reflects a deeper sense of purpose and overall life satisfaction. ...
... Happiness, a central concept in psychology and philosophy, is broadly defined as subjective wellbeing characterized by positive emotions, life satisfaction, and a sense of fulfillment (Veenhoven, 2011;Jam et al., 2010). Research in positive psychology identifies two primary types: short-term or immediate happiness and long-term or enduring happiness. ...
... This research underscores a crucial understanding of happiness, suggesting that instead of solely pursuing hedonic pleasures, individuals may find greater fulfillment by focusing on eudaimonic aspects of life, such as nurturing relationships and fostering personal growth. The concept of happiness, a central theme in psychology and philosophy, encompasses subjective well-being, positive emotions, life satisfaction, and a sense of fulfillment (Veenhoven, 2011). Positive psychology identifies two main types of happiness: short-term, linked to transient joy and pleasure, and longterm, reflecting enduring well-being beyond mere external stimuli (Mandishekwa, 2021). ...
... It is also used with terms like 'wellbeing' or 'quality of life' and denotes both individual and social welfare. Happiness is the degree to which people judges the quality of their lives, or how much one likes the life they leads (Veenhoven, 2012). ...
... Average Happiness (0-10) in Nations 2000-2009. (Veenhoven, 2012 If we want to get maximum happiness, we must utilize our time in such ways that ignite social connections. The time we spent with friends and family strengthens our relationships which brings the greatest happiness, we can also get happiness from: 1) People who are not associated with happiness e.g workplace friends. 2) Engaging in volu ...
Article
Full-text available
The problem understudy was "Rethinking Public Spaces: A Community Driven Compulsive Urban Design", with the objectives to evaluate the effectiveness of public spaces by correlating social interaction, happiness and public spaces in the selected communities. The urban population of Abbottabad district, Khyber Pakhtunkhwa Pakistan was taken as the study area, we calculated the population sample and conducted the study in three different communities in Abbottabad district. The duration of the data collection and community survey was two months. A detailed information on the selected variables was obtained by carefully filling the questionnaire designed for this purpose in a survey of the selected areas. Data obtained through surveys was analyzed and Correlation was computed from the collected data and was interpreted. All the information collected was analyzed and presented through tables and figures
... The concept of happiness in previous studies has tended to be reduced to welfare. Many studies focus more on referring to happiness as life satisfaction or well-being (Veenhoven, 2011). However, advances in research are developing the theory of happiness not only psychologically but also economically and socially (Yilmaz, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Happiness is now one of the indicators of successful development. Every country measures the happiness index of its people and competes to be the country with the happiest population. This research tries to map out happiness-related research trends in countries around the world, seeing how the results of these studies are published and making them the basis for the preparation of research related to happiness in Indonesian society. This research is a literature study with a bibliometric approach. The analysis was guided by 203 Scopus publications with the keywords “happiness,” “gender,” and “culture,” which were then narrowed down with the provisions of articles on economic subjects published between 2014-2024 and open access so that 75 articles were obtained which were then selected 5 (five) articles with the most citations. The results show that the increase in publications for the keywords “happiness,” “gender,” and “culture” continues to increase, and most publications come from the US and UK. On average, research related to happiness is more often published in psychology journals, although the content of the articles relates happiness to economic, demographic, and cultural variables
... However, there is a distinction to be made between happiness and life satisfaction [36]. While happiness can be delineated by the frequency and intensity of positive versus negative affect, life satisfaction is not a direct measure of emotion per se, but rather an overarching evaluative judgment that encompasses various aspects of life [37]. Despite these conceptual differences, life satisfaction is generally understood to encompass a range of factors including family relationships, communication, socio-economic status, leisure, self-actualization, values, living environment, health, and parental relations. ...
Article
Full-text available
In investigating indoor virtual golf's impact on self-efficacy and life satisfaction, this study surveyed South Korean adults from March 1 to March 15, 2024. This study examined the motivational factors driving participation in indoor virtual golf and the subsequent effects on self-efficacy and life satisfaction. Data analysis showed that skill development significantly improved self-efficacy (β = .601, p < .001), while increased physical activity surprisingly showed a negative correlation (β = -.523, p < .001). Joyfulness was a strong positive predictor of life satisfaction ( β = 1.161, p < .001), suggesting that the enjoyment derived from playing indoor virtual golf could contribute to overall life contentment. Furthermore, self-efficacy fully mediated the relationship between the intention to participate in indoor virtual golf and life satisfaction, indicating the critical role of self-belief in enhancing personal well-being. This study concluded that the participation intention in indoor virtual golf significantly boosts individuals' confidence in their capabilities and overall satisfaction with life, emphasizing the importance of psychological factors in virtual sports activities, which suggests a focus on skill development and fostering joy to enhance participant satisfaction.
... The idea of happiness has multiple definitions that vary among researchers and experts. Numerous studies have found that various ideas represent their views of happiness (Oishi et al., 2013;Veenhoven, 2012). Happiness is defined as experiencing bliss and delight; it is an aim for a lot of individuals. ...
Article
Full-text available
Abstract Background: Boosting university students' happiness is essential for personal development, familial harmony, social equilibrium, and national progress. The present study aimed to determine the significant influential factors of happiness, as well as whether gender and marital status have a moderating effect on the relationship between religiosity and happiness among Bangladeshi university students. Methods: A total of 464 students were recruited for the present study, they were selected by multistage random sampling. Bangla-adapted version of the MUNSH scale was used to measure the happiness of university students. We asked a question to respondents for measuring the religiosity, “Do you practice your religion?” (Yes, considered as higher religious, and No, considered as lower religious or no). T-test and correlation analysis were used to analyze the data. The PROCESS macro (Model 1) was applied to see the moderating effect of gender and marital status. Results: Among 464 participants, 48.30 % and 51.70 % were males and females, respectively. The mean age of the students was 21.07 ± 2.60 years. Firstly, we found that gender, marital status, and religiosity significantly influenced the happiness of university students. Secondly, the correlation analysis revealed that relationship between religiosity and happiness was positively significant. Thirdly, the results also revealed that the moderating effect of gender between religiosity and happiness was negative but not significant (β = −0.65, p > 0.05, 95 % CI = [−3.23, 1.94]). Fourthly, the moderation analysis showed that marital status was the positive and significant moderator in the association between religiosity and happiness (β = 5.17, p < 0.01, 95 % CI = [2.05, 8.28]). Finally, the selected model of moderation could able to explain the variation of happiness by 38.92 %. Conclusions: This study suggests that happiness depends on changes in many socio-demographic variables, such as gender, marital status, and religiosity. Marital individuals show a higher moderating effect than unmarried individuals in the association between religiosity and happiness. Both health authorities and university authorities may consider the findings to address the problem faced by university students.
... the philosophical foundation of the concept is more hedonistic, utilitarian, or eudaimonistic. The CA develops an understanding of wellbeing that is informed by questions about satisfaction with life and opportunities, hence using an eudaimonistic approach (see Veenhoven 2012, Exenberger & Juen 2014. Wellbeing is not to be understood only in the psychological or physiological sense (e.g. in MacDonald et al. 2012). ...
Preprint
There is no doubt that Bourdieu’s work and theories have had an enormous impact on sociolog-ical and educational research in terms of describing and identifying educational and social ine-qualities, especially with respect to cultural capital and engagement in the arts. In this article, Amartya Sen’s capability approach will be proposed as a complementary framework which can be applied to a sociological analysis of musical participation that offers useful implications for cultural educational policy and music education.
Article
School belonging is an important avenue for exploration due to its associations with positive outcomes across behaviour, academic achievements and mental health. This study aimed to investigate differences between looked after and non-looked after young people, the associations between school belonging and related outcomes across groups, and factors that help young people feel they belong at school. Quantitative analysis of data from an anonymous online survey completed by looked after young people and matched participants revealed that school belonging was related to mental wellbeing outcomes for all participants. While school belonging and mental health scores did not differ between groups, looked after young people reported lower peer support. Based on within group correlations, three relationships were examined using moderation analysis which showed that the relationship between bullying and school belonging was moderated by care status. Based on qualitative data, young people across groups highlighted friends, teachers, school atmosphere and personal characteristics as important for their school belonging. The research was limited by sample size, but results suggest that a focus on mental wellbeing and school belonging remains important; this is discussed in relation to current educational practice and supporting school belonging within education settings.
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: This study aimed to examine the differences in relationships among social capital components, health status, sociodemographic characteristics, and subjective well-being (SWB) among older adults in institutionalized versus non-institutionalized care environments. Methods: A cross-sectional survey was conducted involving 1,037 older adults aged 65-95 years from nine communities and nine nursing homes across three regions of Zhejiang Province, China. Social capital and SWB were assessed using the Social Capital Scale and the Memorial University of Newfoundland Scale of Happiness (MUNSH), respectively. Propensity score matching (PSM, 1:1, caliper width 0.02) was applied to balance key sociodemographic characteristics and health status between community-dwelling and nursing home residents. Multiple linear regression was utilized to analyze the relationships among social capital components, health status, sociodemographic factors, and SWB in both groups. Results: PSM identified 290 older adults in community dwellings and a comparable group (n = 290) in nursing homes. Comparative analysis showed that nursing home residents demonstrated lower SWB. Multiple linear regression revealed that social connection, trust, and cohesion positively associated with SWB in both groups. However, social participation was significantly linked only with community dwellings residents. Both groups showed a positive relationship between SWB and self-rated health, but no association was found with the number of chronic conditions. Additionally, higher income (≥3000 RMB) and education level (middle) were linked to increased SWB among community-dwelling older adults, whereas family structure—specifically, having no partner and three or more children—negatively impacted SWB in the nursing home group. Conclusion: Social capital and health status showed a strong and consistent association with SWB in both groups. Strengthening social connections, trust, and cohesion, along with maintaining positive health perceptions, is expected to enhance the well-being of older adults, particularly for those in institutional settings. Notably, differences in how sociodemographic factors influence SWB across settings. These findings indicate the necessity for tailored interventions that address the unique needs of each care environment to promote healthier aging experiences.
Article
Full-text available
Everyone by nature remains in search of happiness, a mental state of well being. As happiness is an expectation, entropy persists as a reality. Indian farmers are supposed to bear the humongous responsibility of food security of millions but seldom do we care for their happiness. This review paper examines different aspects of farmers' happiness in terms of stress and satisfaction, poverty and hunger, climate change and resource denudation, mobility and migration, the income and empowerment all are deeply intrigued into the context of farmer's happiness. Globally on happiness-index India's position is just close to the lowest one. The conceptual heterodoxy of happiness has been well examined. Both the physical and mental well being of the farmers are extremely important for augmenting and sustaining our food security and economic growth. So the inquiry is necessary to extract the causes of the unhappiness, the farmers are passing through and essentials to make them happy as per their terms and conditions.
Book
Full-text available
This book is about the degree to which people take pleasure in life: in short 'happiness'. It tries to identify conditions that favor a positive appreciation of life. Thus it hopes to shed more light on a longstanding and intriguing ques­ tion and, possibly, to guide attempts to improve the human lot. During the preceding decades a growing number of investigations have dealt with this issue. As a result there is now a sizable body of data. Yet it is quite difficult to make sense of it. There is a muddle of theories, concepts and indicators, and many of the findings seem to be contradictory. This book attempts to bring some order into the field. The study draws on an inventory of empirical investigations which involved valid indicators of happiness; 245 studies are involved, which together yield some 4000 observations: for the main part correlational ones. These results are presented in full detail in the simultaneously published 'Databook of Happiness' (Veenhoven 1984). The present volume distils conclusions from that wealth of data. It tries to assess the reality value of the findings and the degree to which correlations reflect the conditions of happiness rather than the consequences of it. It then attempts to place the scattered findings in context. As such, this work is not a typical study of literature on happiness.
Article
Full-text available
One of the ideological foundations of the modern welfare states is the belief that people can be made happier by providing them with better living conditions. This belief is challenged by the theory that happiness is a fixed 'trait', rather than a variable 'state'. This theory figures both at the individual level and at the societal level. The individual level variant depicts happiness as an aspect of personal character; rooted in inborn temperament or acquired disposition. The societal variant sees happiness as a matter of national character; embedded in shared values and beliefs. Both variants imply that a better society makes no happier people. Happiness can be regarded as a trait if it meets three criteria: (1) temporal stability, (2) cross-situational consistency, and (3) inner causation. This paper checks whether that is, indeed, the case. The theory that happiness is a personal-character-trait is tested in a (meta) analysis of longitudinal studies. The results are: (1) Happiness is quite stable on the short term, but not in the long run, neither relatively nor absoloutely. (2) Happiness is not insensitive to fortune or adversity. (3) Happiness is not entirely built-in: its genetic basis is at best modest and psychological factors explain only part of its variance. The theory that happiness is a national-character-trait is tested in an analysis of differences in average happiness between nations. The results point in the same direction: (1) Though generally fairly stable over the last decades, nation-happiness has changed profoundly in some cases, both absolutely and relatively. (2) Average happiness in nations is clearly not independant of living conditions. The better the conditions in a country, the happier its citizens. (3) The differences cannot be explained by a collective outlook on life. It is concluded that happiness is no immutable trait. There is thus still sense in striving for greater happiness for a greater number.
Article
Full-text available
Résumé Un des buts des politiques sociales est de créer un plus grand bonheur pour le plus grand nombre de personnes. La réalisation de cet objectif ambitieux nécessite la compréhension du bonheur. Les cinq questions suivantes appellent des réponses -. 1) Précisément, qu’est-ce que le bonheur ? 2) Le bonheur peut-il être mesuré ? 3) Jusqu'à quel point les gens sont-ils heureux ? 4) Qu'est-ce qui nous amène à être heureux ou malheureux ? 5) Le bonheur peut-il être atteint de façon permanente ? Au fil des siècles, les philosophes se sont confrontés à ces questions. Depuis les années 60, elles font l’objet de recherches empiriques. Cet article tente de répondre à ces questions en se basant sur les progrès réalisés. Summary One of the goals of social policy is to create greater happiness for a greater number. Realization of this ambition requires understanding of happiness. The following questions must be answered: 1) What is happiness precisely? 2) Can happiness be measured? 3) How happy are people presently? 4) What causes us to be happy or unhappy? 5) Can happiness be raised lastingly? Through the ages philosophers have toiled with these questions. Since the 1960's they are subject of empirical research. This paper takes stock of the progress in answering these questions.
Article
Full-text available
Human society has changed much over the last centuries and this process of 'modernization' has profoundly affected the lives of individuals; currently we live quite different lives from those forefathers lived only five generations ago. There is difference of opinion as to whether we live better now than before, and consequently there is also disagreement as to whether we should continue modernizing or rather try to slow the process down. Quality-of-life in a society can be measured by how long and happy its inhabitants live. Using these indicators I assess whether societal modernization has made life better or worse. Firstly I examined findings of present day survey research. I started with a cross-sectional analysis of 90 nations and found that people live longer and happier in today's most modern societies. Secondly I examined trends in 10 modern nations over the last 30 years and found that happiness and longevity have increased in most cases. Thirdly I considered the long-term and review findings from historical and comparative anthropology, which show that we lived better in the early hunter-gatherer society than in the later agrarian society. Together these data suggest that societal evolution has worked out differently for the quality of human life, first negatively, in the change from a hunter-gatherer existence to agriculture, and next positively, in the more recent transformation from an agrarian to an industrial society. We live now longer and happier than ever before.
Book
This volume presents a unique and interesting study of happiness from both economic and political perspectives... This interdisciplinary volume represents a distinctive contribution to the relatively large and clearly increasing literature of the subject. It will prove a worthy reading for all those, students or researchers, with a special interest in the analysis of happiness and human well-being. - Elena E. Nicolae, Journal of Philosophical Economics. © Amitava Krishna Dutt and Benjamin Radcliff 2009. All rights reserved.
Article
In this chapter, we shall compare a group of high school students from the US and from Italy, when answering the following questions bearing on the issue of happiness. First, does happiness have the same phenomenological meaning in the two cultures? Second, are external conditions-the kind of activities pursued, the type of companions present-related in the same way to moment-by-moment fluctuations of happiness in the two groups? Third, does the perception of the ratio of challenges and skills have the same effect on happiness in the two groups? Fourth, are there differences between happy and less happy individuals in the choice of situations (i.e. types of activities and companions) and in subjective interpretations of experience (i.e. degree of perceived choice, and perception of the challenges and personal skills in daily activities)? © 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht. All rights reserved.
Article
This article reports the development and validation of a scale to measure global life satisfaction, the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS). Among the various components of subjective well-being, the SWLS is narrowly focused to assess global life satisfaction and does not tap related constructs such as positive affect or loneliness. The SWLS is shown to have favorable psychometric properties, including high internal consistency and high temporal reliability. Scores on the SWLS correlate moderately to highly with other measures of subjective well-being, and correlate predictably with specific personality characteristics. It is noted that the SWLS is suited for use with different age groups, and other potential uses of the scale are discussed.
Article
Two traditional approaches to conceptualizing quality of life are distinguished: the individualist which, emphasizes the position and activities of individuals; and the transcendentalist, which emphasizes the overall order of society. Both of these approaches are shown to be inadequate because they presuppose the logical separation of individual and society. A third approach, which conceives of individuals and society generating each other via a continuing process of negotiation is proposed. Such an approach leads to a "quality of life" defined in terms of the outcomes of these negotiations; at a single point in time these are called the sovereignties of individuals and settings. Sovereignties are thus conceptualized as patterns of commitment made by individuals among settings, and conversely. It is suggested that these patterns of commitment be measured as the joint allocation of money, time, skill and sentiment by individuals and settings. Assessment of quality of life in chronic illness is used as an example.
Article
If the obstacles to human development lie in the paucity of resources, in insuperable technical barriers, the task would be hopeless. We know instead that it is too often a lack of political commitment, not of resources, that is the ultimate cause of human neglect. United Nations, Human Development Report, 1991