Content uploaded by Jukka A Salmi
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Jukka A Salmi on Jun 11, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
37New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
© by IAAF
24:1; 37-47, 2009
STUDY
Introduction
peed of reaction is a critical aspect
of many competitive sports, includ-
ing the sprint events in athletics. In
these events, the reaction time is considered
to be the time taken by the athlete to respond
to the start signal and begin pushing the start-
ing blocks. A simple auditory reaction such as
this is one of the fastest types of reaction,
although it is thought to be rarely less than
100 ms (THOMPSON et al., 1992). It is note-
worthy that this “definition” of reaction time
specifically focuses on the force production
on the blocks without necessarily referring to
IAAF Sprint Start Research
Project: Is the 100 ms limit
still valid?
By Paavo V. Komi, Masaki Ishikawa, Jukka Salmi
The current false start criterion used by
the IAAF is based on an assumed mini-
mum auditory reaction time. If an ath-
lete moves sooner than 100 ms after the
start signal, he/she is deemed to have
false-started. The purpose of this study,
which was commissioned by the IAAF,
was to examine neuromuscular reaction
to the auditory signal used in the sprint
start and to determine whether the
100 ms limit is correct. Seven national-
level Finnish sprinters took part. A com-
prehensive approach was used to study
force reaction on the blocks, the move-
ments of the arms and the activation
profiles of several muscles. The authors
found great variation in individual reac-
tion times and confirmed previous
reports of simple auditory reactions as
fast as 80 ms. They recommend that the
100 ms limit be lowered to 80 or 85 ms
and that the IAAF urgently examines
possibilities for detecting false starts
kinematically, so that judges’ decisions
are based on the first visible movement
regardless of the body part. This can be
done with a system of high-speed
cameras, which gives views of all the
athletes on the start line. With such a
system, it would be possible to change
the start rule so that no false starts are
permitted.
ABSTRACT Prof. Paavo V. Komi is the Founder of the
Neuromuscular Research Center,
Department of the Biology of Physical
Activity, University of Jyväskylä,
Jyväskylä, Finland.
Masaki Ishikawa, PhD, is a Senior Lec-
turer at Osaka University of Health and
Sport Sciences, Osaka, Japan. His
research topic is the neuromuscu-
loskeletal mechanics during human
locomotion.
Jukka Salmi is a Senior Researcher of the
Sports Technology Program at Neuro-
muscular Research Center, Department
of the Biology of Physical Activity, Uni-
versity of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland.
AUTHORS
S
movements or force production of other body
parts, such as head, neck, trunk or arms.
Physiologically, reaction time depends on
several factors: arrival of the start signal stim-
ulus at the sensory organ, conversion by the
sensory organ to a neural signal, neural trans-
missions and processing, muscular activa-
tion, soft tissue compliance and the selection
of the external measurement parameter (Fig-
ure 1). Each of these factors has an associat-
ed processing time that contributes to the
overall reaction time.
The current false start criterion used by the
International Association of Athletics Federa-
tions is based on an assumed minimum audi-
tory reaction time of 100 ms (IAAF, 2003). If an
athlete moves sooner than 100 ms after the
start signal, the firing of the starter’s gun,
he/she is deemed to have false-started. Until
recently, the reaction movement was deemed
to have started when the threshold of 25kg of
force above the baseline in the set position
was reached on either of the blocks. This has
now been changed so the reaction move-
ment is judged by the steepness of the rise of
the force curve.
These criteria seem to consider that all the
human beings will have more or less similar
results to an auditory reaction test, such as
the sprint start. However, the influence of
auditory stimulus on the initiation of complex
motor tasks is not fully understood. There are
several studies showing that simple auditory
reaction times of less than 100 ms can be
achieved (MERO & KOMI, 1990; ROTHWELL
& VALLS-SOLE, 2002; PAIN & HIBBS, 2007;
BROWN et al., 2008).
The purpose of the present study was to
examine the neuromuscular reaction to the
auditory signal used in the sprint start and to
determine whether the current 100 ms limit is
still justified. A comprehensive approach was
New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
38
Figure 1: Sequence of the auditory evoked reaction
used to study the force reaction on the blocks
and, using sophisticated EMG and kinematic
approaches, the movements of the arms and
the activation profiles of several muscles during
the start movement.
Methods
Protocols
Seven Finnish national-level sprinters (four
males and three women: age 24 ± 3 years;
mass 71.2 ± 14.2kg; height 177 ± 7cm) par-
ticipated in this study. They had previously
been informed of the procedures and all the
associated risks and all gave written consent.
The measurements were included in the ath-
letes’ training and testing programmes as
planned by their individual coaches.
The study was conducted in the biome-
chanics laboratory of the Neuromuscular
Research Center, Department of Biology of
Physical Activity, University of Jyväskylä. This
laboratory is equipped, among other things,
with a unique 10m long force platform sys-
tem, composed of two rows of 1m long indi-
vidual force plates placed in series, row by
row. The force plate area continued as a free
space for an additional 40m. Thus, the ath-
letes were able to perform the start naturally,
like in a real sprint competition. The force
plate and the extended 40m are covered with
tartan surface.
In the measurements, the athletes per-
formed a total of five to eight sprint starts sim-
ilar to the real race starts. Thus the conditions
were as close as possible to real competi-
tions. Figure 2 depicts how the starting bocks
were positioned on the force-plates. The two
individual front and rear start blocks were
measured separately for both vertical (Fz) and
horizontal (Fy) components of the ground
reaction forces. Similar measurements were
performed for the arm actions, again in the Fz
and Fy directions.
Parameters
In addition to the recording of the Fz and Fy
ground reaction forces (right and left sides)
individually for both legs and arms using the
39New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
Figure 2: Schematics of the sprint start measurement
force plates, electromyogram (EMG) activities
from the erector spinae (ES), vastus medialis
(VM), soleus (SOL) and tibialis anterior (TA)
muscles of both legs were recorded using
bipolar miniature-size surface electrodes
(diameter 6mm, interelectrode distance
21mm; Blue Sensor N-00-S/25, Medicotest,
Olstykke, Denmark). These EMG data were
amplified using the EMG telemetric recording
system (bandwidth 10Hz to 1kHz per 3dB;
model 16-2, EISA, Freiburg, Germany) and
were stored simultaneously together with
kinetic data on a personal computer via an AD
converter (Sampling rate 2kHz; Power 1401,
Cambridge Electronics Design Ltd, England).
To determine when the initial reactions to the
start signal took place, all start movements
were video-recorded with two high-speed
cameras at 300 fps (A600, Basler AG, Ahrens-
burg, Germany) from the rear leg side and from
the 45º diagonally forward direction. Reflective
markers were placed on the head, shoulder,
elbow, hand, trochanter major, centre of
rotation of the knee, lateral malleolus, heel, and
fifth metatarsal head. These points were then
digitised automatically (Motus, Vicon Peak
Performance, USA) and used to determine the
onset of the initial start movement.
An electronic pulse from the set to the start
gun firing was used to synchronise the kinet-
ic, kinematics, and EMG data.
Analyses
EMG signals were first full-wave rectified
and then filtered (Butterworth 4-order low-
New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
40
Figure 3: Time course data of the kinetics (arm and leg forces) and EMGs during the sprint start
(The zero (first dotted line) is the onset of the start signal.) VM=vastus medialis, MG=medial
gastrocnemius, SOL=soleus, TA=tibialis anterior, Bicep=biceps femoris, Abd=rectus abdominis,
Bra=brachioradialis
pass filter: 75Hz). The onset of EMG was
determined by visual inspection of the filtered
EMG signal with the researcher deciding
when the signal had changed from baseline
during the set position.
The resultant force during the start move-
ment was calculated from Fz and Fy data for
determining the onset and the 25kg threshold
of the rise of the resultant force curve.
Values are presented as means and stan-
dard deviations (SD) unless otherwise stated.
Results
Figure 3 shows a typical example of the
time course of the force and EMG data during
the sprint start. Table 1 gives the mean and
fastest reaction times for each parameter. The
onset of the arm force reaction (69 ms) was
faster than that of the leg force reaction
(98 ms). The time to reach the force detection
level (25kg) delayed the reaction time by
approximately 35 ms compared to the onset
of the leg force reaction. In the fastest start
condition, the onset of the muscle activation
for the measured EMGs occurred earlier than
in 100 ms.
In the 25kg force level detection, three sub-
jects (subject02, subject04, subject07) had
reaction times less than 100 ms in their fastest
trial (Figures 4.1 to 4.7). Subject 7 showed
mean reaction times of less than 100 ms
although the timing of her 25kg force de-
tection level was not fast due to the slow force
development.
After the start signal, the head and wrist
began to move at 110 ms and 108 ms,
respectively (Table 1). Note the large stan-
dard deviations (17 and 20). The legs began
to move after the head and wrist movements.
Discussion
The main results of this study are as follows:
1. In response to the start signal, the mean
reaction time on the blocks required to
reach the 25kg force level was generally
longer than 100 ms. However, in several
trials three subjects did have reaction
41New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
Mean (SD) Fastest (SD)
Kinetics
Onset of leg force reaction 98 (23) 78 (27)
Onset of arm force reaction 69 (12) 49 (14)
Force detection (25kg level) 133 (21) 114 (29)
Kinematics
Head movement 110 (17) 101 (21)
Hand movement 108 (20) 104 (17)
EMG
Erector spinae 87 (33) 69 (30)
Vastus medialis 94 (16) 73 (19)
Medial gastrocnemius 96 (20) 82 (26)
Soleus 112 (20) 91 (17)
Tibialis anterior 74 (18) 59 (13)
* time from the auditory stimulus (n=7)
Table 1: Reaction times (ms)
New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
42
Figure 4.1: The time course data of the resultant forces for the arms and legs of subject 01
Figure 4.2: The time course data of the resultant forces for the arms and legs of subject 02
43New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
Figure 4.3: The time course data of the resultant forces for the arms and legs of subject 03
Figure 4.4: The time course data of the resultant forces for the arms and legs of subject 04
New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
44
Figure 4.5: The time course data of the resultant forces for the arms and legs of subject 05
Figure 4.6: The time course data of the resultant forces for the arms and legs of subject 06
times less than 100 ms. These would
have been considered as false starts
according to the criteria used by the IAAF.
2. The time to the onset of muscle activation
in the fastest start reaction condition was
60-80 ms.
As shown in Introduction, the neuromuscu-
lar response of the auditory reaction time dur-
ing a sprint start can be less than 100 ms.
Generally, after taking 3-6 ms for the start sig-
nal discharge to travel 1m to the ear of ath-
lete, the sound stimulus can reach the motor
cortex through the brain stem and auditory
cortex in the time of 65 ms. Thereafter, the
time from the motor cortex to the spinal cord
and to the leg muscles can take 20-30 ms.
Including the mechanical delay (5-10 ms), the
total auditory reaction time takes approxi-
mately 100 ms (Figure 1).
However, as shown in a previous study
(ROTHWELL & VALLS-SOLE, 2002), there is
evidence that simple auditory reaction time of
less than 100 ms can be achieved. For exam-
ple, the startle reflex is thought to alter the
information processing loops so that the audi-
tory and motor cortices are bypassed and a
prepared motor programme is released sub-
cortically (BROWN et al., 2008). In this way,
portions of the typical auditory motor pathway
are bypassed, resulting in a decrease of reac-
tion time.
The currently limit of 100 ms in the IAAF
rules can be questioned for several reasons.
Firstly, in addition to the present data, there
are also other reports that demonstrate that
values much below 100 ms are possible in
good sprinters (PAIN & HIBBS 2007 and
BROWN et al.,2008). Both of these studies
are objective and very convincing. In fact PAIN
& HIBBS (2007) went as far as suggesting
that the auditory reaction time in the sprint
start could be as low as 85 ms. Our experi-
ence supports this suggestion. Secondly,
there is no question that the IAAF “rule” of
45New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
Figure 4.7: The time course data of the resultant forces for the arms and legs of subject 07
100 ms is based on something other than real
neuromuscular-physiological evidence. This
minimum reaction time limit does not take into
consideration the size and gender differences
among athletes. As the limit threshold is
based more or less on the ability to produce
the force (e.g. 25kg), the women are not able
to reach this level as quickly as men (KOMI &
KARLSSON, 1977).
The limit gives a signal that training cannot
reduce the minimum reaction time of 100 ms
at 25kg force level. Our Figure 1 is an attempt
to describe schematically the sequence of
delays in the neuromuscular system to audi-
tory evoked reaction. There are important
points in this scheme. First, the most uncer-
tain time-delay takes place between brain-
stem and auditory cortex. It is likely that this
delay, shown to last up to 50 ms, can most
probably be interpreted to be under influence
of training, for example. As this delay takes
about half of the entire reaction time, any
experimental data to verify its true value and
possible adaptation to genetic as well as envi-
ronmental conditions are more than welcome.
Until this becomes possible we can only rely
on objectively measured reaction time data
that the total reaction time in the sprint start
can be below 85 ms.
The sprint start reaction time as measured
from the reaction forces on the blocks is very
unnatural. The start action following the “sta-
tionary” set position is not just an action of the
legs. It is a very comprehensive whole body
movement. The examined muscles in the pres-
ent study showed variable response times
above the base line (Table 1), ranging, for
example, from 59 ms (group average in the fast
condition for the tibialis anterior muscle) to
91 ms (in the soleus muscle). The large stan-
dard deviations shown in Table 1 emphasise
possibilities for even lower values. Knowing the
relatively low value of electromechanical delay
of 10-12 ms in humans (NICOLl & KOMI, 1996),
the total reaction time for some muscles can
be well below 100 ms. It is important to note
that in the present study many of the athletes
had acceptable motor times in the range of 75
to 80 ms and below for the Vastus lateralis,
Gastrocnemius and Soleus muscles. All these
muscles are responsible for extension move-
ment of the legs against the starting block.
Conclusions
The following conclusions can be drawn
from the present study:
1. Great individual differences can be
observed in reaction times.
2. Reaction time in the sprint start can be
lower than the 100 ms IAAF criteria. The
values can in some cases be even below
80 ms (see the schematic presentation in
Figure 5).
3. Reaction time in the sprint start is param-
eter dependent and has different values in
different body parts. The values are usual-
ly lower in the arms than the legs.
4. As the reaction to the auditory stimulus in
the sprint start involves activation of sever-
al muscles in the whole body and conse-
quently activation and movement in the
various body parts (e.g. neck, head, shoul-
der, arms, back, abdomen, hip, knee and
ankle), the current application of the IAAF
rule does not take this important “whole
body involvement “ into consideration.
New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
46
Figure 5: The schematics of the possibilities
for the faster reaction (The horizontally sha-
ded area shows the reaction times that have
been observed experimentally in the sprint
start.)
5. As the start of muscle activation is the first
neuromuscular parameter to trigger the
joint movement (and force production),
the resulting kinematic changes should be
considered as a key possibility for solving
the complex problem of the current false
start criteria.
Recommendations for the IAAF
1. As the present study gives essentially the
same results as the ones published by
independent researchers in Britain (PAIN &
HIBBS, 2007) and Canada (BROWN et
al., 2008), it is now recommended that the
IAAF abandon the 100 ms minimum sprint
reaction time level and its measurement
with the current technical devices.
2. The level should be lowered to 80 or
85 ms, even if the block force production
is still used as the parameter to set the
level.
3. The IAAF should urgently examine possi-
bilities for detecting the false start kine-
matically, so that the decision is based on
the first visible movement regardless of
the body part. This can be done with a
system of high-speed cameras, which
gives views of all the athletes on the start
line. Modern technical possibilities are
numerous in this regard, and the human
eye can be considered as best to differen-
tiate the first movement (shown by the
high-speed camera) before the set mini-
mum reaction time. Figure 6 is our sug-
gestion for how the development project
could be started. The figure does not
show the cables to the monitor centre, in
which one or two persons can make the
decision a posteriori, but within 10-15
seconds after the gun firing.
4. Achieving point 3 above would lead to the
situation where the rule could be changed
so that no false starts are permitted.
Please send all correspondence to:
Prof. Paavo V. Komi
Paavo.Komi@sport.jyu.fi
47New Studies in Athletics • no. 1/2009
IAAF Sprint Start Research Project: Is the 100 ms limit still valid?
Figure 6: The schema of the proposed
camera detection system
BROWN, A.M.; KENWELL, Z.R.; MARAJ, B.K. &
COLLINS, D.F. (2008). "Go" signal intensity influences the
sprint start. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 40, 1142-1148.
IAAF (2003). Technical rules for international competitions
(available at: http://www.iaaf.org/newsfiles/23484.pdf).
KOMI, P.V. & KARLSSON, J. (1978). Skeletal muscle fibre
types, enzyme activities and physical performance in young
males and females. Acta Physiol Scand, 103, 210-218.
MERO A. & KOMI, P.V. (1990). Reaction time and electromyo-
graphic activity during a sprint start. European Journal of
Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 61, 73 – 80.
NICOL, C. & KOMI, P.V. (1998). Significance of passively
induced stretch reflexes on achilles tendon force enhance-
ment. Muscle Nerve, 21, 1546-1548.
PAIN, M.G. & HIBBS, A. (2007). Sprint starts and the min-
imum auditory reaction time. Journal of Sports Sciences,
25, 79 – 86.
ROTHWELL, J.C. & VALLS-SOLE, J. (2002). The startle
reflex, voluntary movement, and the reticulospinal tract. In
R. A. Schmidt & L. Latash, (Ed.), Progress in motor control,
Vol. 2: Structure – function relations in voluntary move-
ments (pp. 13 – 23). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
THOMPSON, P.D.; COLEBATCH, J.G.; BROWN, P.;
ROTHEWELL, J.C.; DAY, B.L.; OBESO, J.A. et al. (1992).
Voluntary stimulussensitive jerks and jumps mimicking
myoclonus or pathological startle syndromes. Movement
Disorders, 7, 257 – 262.
REFERENCES