ArticlePDF Available

Male mate preference for female eyespan and fecundity in the stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis dalmanni

Authors:
  • Bristol Zoological Society

Abstract and Figures

Traditional views of sexual selection view males as the indiscriminate sex, competing for access to choosy females. It is increasingly recognized that mating can also be costly for males and they are therefore likely to exhibit choice in order to maximize their reproductive success. Stalk-eyed flies are model species in sexual selection studies. Males are sperm limited and constrained in the number of matings they are able to partake in. In addition, variation in female fecundity has been shown to correlate positively with female eyespan, so eyespan size could provide males with a reliable signal of female reproductive value. We examined male mate preference in the wild in the stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis dalmanni. In addition, we set up experiments in the laboratory allowing males a choice between females that varied in 1) eyespan (a proxy for fecundity) and/or 2) fecundity (manipulated through diet). We found that males exhibited preference for large eyespan females, both in the wild and laboratory studies. As well as using female eyespan as a mating cue, males were also able to assess female fecundity directly. Changes in fecundity among large eyespan females caused corresponding changes in male mate preference, whereas changes in the fecundity of small eyespan females had limited effect on their attractiveness. These results show that male mate preferences are a prevalent feature of a canonical example of female mate choice sexual selection and that males use multiple cues when they assess females as potential mates.
Content may be subject to copyright.
© The Author 2014. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of
the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. All rights reserved. For
permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com
The ocial journal of the
ISBE
International Society for Behavioral Ecology
Behavioral
Ecology
Original Article
Male mate preference for female eyespan and
fecundity in the stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis dalmanni
Alison J.Cotton,
a,b,c
SamuelCotton,
a
JenniferSmall,
a
and AndrewPomiankowski
a,b
a
Department of Genetics, Evolution & Environment, University College London, Gower Street,
London WC1E 6BT, UK,
b
CoMPLEX, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK,
and
c
Bristol Zoological Society, c/o Bristol Zoo Gardens, Clifton Down, Clifton, Bristol BS8 3HA, UK
Received 9 May 2014; revised 29 September 2014; accepted 2 October 2014.
Traditional views of sexual selection view males as the indiscriminate sex, competing for access to choosy females. It is increasingly
recognized that mating can also be costly for males and they are therefore likely to exhibit choice in order to maximize their reproduc-
tive success. Stalk-eyed flies are model species in sexual selection studies. Males are sperm limited and constrained in the number of
matings they are able to partake in. In addition, variation in female fecundity has been shown to correlate positively with female eye-
span, so eyespan size could provide males with a reliable signal of female reproductive value. We examined male mate preference in
the wild in the stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis dalmanni. In addition, we set up experiments in the laboratory allowing males a choice between
females that varied in 1)eyespan (a proxy for fecundity) and/or 2)fecundity (manipulated through diet). We found that males exhib-
ited preference for large eyespan females, both in the wild and laboratory studies. As well as using female eyespan as a mating cue,
males were also able to assess female fecundity directly. Changes in fecundity among large eyespan females caused corresponding
changes in male mate preference, whereas changes in the fecundity of small eyespan females had limited effect on their attractive-
ness. These results show that male mate preferences are a prevalent feature of a canonical example of female mate choice sexual
selection and that males use multiple cues when they assess females as potential mates.
Key words: fecundity, female ornament, male mate preference, mate choice, sexual selection, stalk-eyed fly.
INTRODUCTION
Sexual selection has been viewed classically as competition among
undiscriminating males for access to choosy females (Darwin 1871;
Bateman 1948; Trivers 1972). However, it is increasingly recog-
nized that this perspective is too simplistic and that mating can
be constrained or costly for males (Dewsbury 1982; Bonduriansky
2001; Webberley and Hurst 2002; Wedell et al. 2002; Andrade
2003). Males should, therefore, allocate their matings prudently so
as to maximize their reproductive success. This leads to the pre-
diction that it will often pay males, as for females, to discriminate
between individuals when choosing a mate (Bonduriansky 2001).
Parker (1983) noted that choosiness is favored when high vari-
ance in quality exists in the opposite sex; if there is little variation
in mate quality, then there will be few advantages accruing from
mate preference (Parker 1983; Gwynne 1991). In promiscuous spe-
cies, males are expected to select females on the basis of fecundity
(Bonduriansky 2001). Nonetheless, directional male mate prefer-
ence is expected to evolve for traits that reflect female reproductive
value even when signaling compromises female viability and male
mate preference is costly (Servedio and Lande 2006; Nakahashi
2008). It has been suggested that males may use female body size
as a proxy for fecundity, as size and fecundity tend to be corre-
lated (Honěk 1993). In several polygynous species, females display
ornament-like traits that may have initially evolved as a correlated
response to selection on homologous male ornaments (Lande and
Arnold 1985). These may subsequently have been co-opted as tar-
gets of male mate preference, given that such traits in females are
conspicuous, easy to evaluate, and often reflect aspects of female
quality linked to fecundity (Amundsen 2000). Male mating prefer-
ence for female ornaments has been shown in several insect species
where the ornament is thought to be an indicator of fecundity, like
dance flies (Funk and Tallamy 2000; LeBas et al. 2003) and the
mosquito Sabethes cyaneus (South and Arnqvist 2011).
The evolution of male mate preference is also likely to be
influenced by the constraints and costs arising from mating
(Bonduriansky 2001). If males are able to mate cost free with every
female they encounter, there is little reason for discrimination to
evolve. For preference to be favored, male mating investment must
be subject to limits. It is increasingly recognized that sperm and
ejaculates are not unlimited resources, but rather they become
depleted by repeated mating. This sets limits to the mating rate
Address correspondence to A.Pomiankowski. E-mail: ucbhpom@ucl.ac.uk.
Behavioral Ecology (2014), 00(00), 1–10. doi:10.1093/beheco/aru192
Behavioral Ecology Advance Access published November 14, 2014
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology
(Dewsbury 1982; Wedell et al. 2002) and to investment in subse-
quent matings (Preston etal. 2001; Wedell etal. 2002). Mating pref-
erence for particular female phenotypes implies lower male mating
success (Servedio and Lande 2006; Nakahashi 2008). As increasing
numbers of males court or mate with attractive females, each has a
smaller chance of success (i.e., of mating and of paternity). On the
other hand, the costs associated with finding and assessing females
must not be prohibitive (Nakahashi 2008). Selection will favor the
evolution of male mate preference when the costs of mate search-
ing and sampling are not high, for example, when the distribution
of females is clumped and males are able to assess them easily
(Forsberg 1987).
The Malaysian stalk-eyed fly Teleopsis dalmanni (Diopsidae;
Diptera) is an important model species for studies of sexual selec-
tion (Wilkinson and Dodson 1997; Maynard Smith and Harper
2003). Stalk-eyed flies are characterized by having their eyes dis-
placed laterally from the head on elongate “eyestalks,” in both
sexes. Eyespan (the distance between the eyes) is sexually dimorphic
in T.dalmanni, being much enlarged in males as a result of sexual
selection. In natural populations, T.dalmanni form nocturnal lekking
aggregations on root hairs that hang underneath the eroded banks
of rainforest streams (Burkhardt and de la Motte 1985; Wilkinson
and Dodson 1997; Cotton et al. 2010). Males fight for control of
these roosting sites (Wilkinson and Dodson 1997; Small etal. 2009),
and females prefer to alight on root hairs controlled by males with
large eyespan (Wilkinson and Reillo 1994; Hingle et al. 2001a,
2001b; Cotton etal. 2010). The vast majority of matings occur in
these aggregations during the dawn and dusk period, when males
attempt to mate with females in their harem (Burkhardt and de
la Motte 1988; Lorch etal. 1993; Small et al. 2009; Cotton etal.
2010). So T.dalmanni appears to be a textbook example of harem-
based polygyny, with choosy females and competitivemales.
However, evidence suggests that there is a high potential for male
mate preference in T.dalmanni. Laboratory experiments have shown
that female fecundity is sensitive to environmental (dietary) stress
(Hingle et al. 2001a), suggesting that variance in female quality is
high. This is borne out in the wild, where female fecundity is highly
variable (Cotton etal. 2010). Moreover, female eyespan is an accu-
rate and reliable indicator of female fecundity in wild females, even
after controlling for its covariation with body size (Cotton et al.
2010). Female eyespan in T. dalmanni, like the ornamental homo-
logue in males, is prominent and easily assessable suggesting that it
may serve as a useful cue for males to use in mating decisions. In
the related African stalk-eyed fly species Diasemopsis meigenii, males
mate for longer and transfer more sperm to females with larger eye-
span (Harley etal. 2013). As female eyespan is positively correlated
with fecundity in D.meigenii, males could gain a selective advantage
by investing more in large eyespan females.
In addition, there is evidence that male stalk-eyed flies suer con-
straints on multiple mating. In T.dalmanni, male mating frequency
is correlated both phenotypically and genetically with the size of
the accessory glands, the paired internal organs involved in sper-
matophore production (Baker et al. 2003; Rogers, Baker, et al.
2005). Accessory glands become depleted with successive matings
(Rogers, Chapman, et al. 2005). When males with larger acces-
sory glands are allowed to mate with multiple females over a short
period of time, they confer higher fertility on females, probably
because they mate at a higher rate (Rogers etal. 2008). In D.mei-
genii, males with larger eyespan (and hence larger accessory glands
and testes) show smaller reductions in spermatophore size and
number of sperm transferred over successive matings, relative to
the performance of small eyespan males (unpublished data). Taken
together, these experimental results suggest that there are limita-
tions on the mating rate of males that have attracted many females
to their lek sites. In the dawn period, when most mating occurs,
males have about 20–30 min in which to mate with females in their
harem. Yet typically, some females are observed to disperse from
lek sites before mating with the dominant male. Under such con-
straints, we expect that there is the opportunity for males to direct
their mating attempts toward those females that have the highest
reproductivevalue.
Given these features of the mating system in T. dalmanni, and
the fact that the harem-based mating system in this species allows
males to choose among groups of females with relatively low costs
of search and assessment, we hypothesized that males should dis-
criminate between females on the basis of their fecundity. We
investigated the potential for male mate preference in the flies’
native habitat in the Malaysian rainforest and also under labora-
tory conditions. This combined approach allowed us to examine
male mate choice both under biologically realistic conditions and
under controlled, experimentally manipulated conditions. In the
latter case, we manipulated the eyespan of experimental females in
order to assess whether this trait is used in male choice and alter-
ing the adult diet of females in order to vary female fecundity in a
controlled manner. We then used a series of mate choice tests to
examine whether males were primarily using female eyespan as an
indicator of fecundity or were able to assess fecundity directly.
METHODS
Male mate preference in field conditions
Field data were collected during 2 phases of fieldwork carried out
on a T. dalmanni population at Ulu Gombak, Peninsular Malaysia
(3°19N, 101°45E) during July/August in 2006 and 2007. Lek sites
(exposed root hairs) on the banks of a tributary of the Gombak river
were identified after dusk. Observations of male mating behavior
at focal harems were conducted during the following dawn period,
starting at approximately 06:55 when flies were still quiescent and
ending at approximately 07:45 when flies had usually dispersed into
the forest. In a few instances (N=3), more than 1 male was present
on the root hair. In order to obtain information from a single focal
male per harem, we carefully removed the additional males without
disturbing the other individuals (the most easily removed male was
chosen). We noted the harem size (number of females present) and
the frequency of successful matings with each female, defined as a
copulation ≥ 30 s, as shorter copulations do not usually result in
transfer of a spermatophore (Lorch etal. 1993; Rogers etal. 2006).
Females within each harem were categorized by inspection by
observers into large and small eyespan classes. Observers (S.C.and
J.S.) were experienced in judging fly size from prior experience with
field populations of T.dalmanni. Arelative measure was used to cat-
egorize females within a lek. Medium-sized eyespan females were
classified as small when the lek contained larger females and large
when there were no larger eyespan females. Data from harems in
which there were no observable dierences in female size were
excluded from the analysis. Eyespan of the focal male was mea-
sured in situ noninvasively using standardized digital photographs.
Images were taken with a digital SLR camera (Canon EOS 350D)
through a 180-mm macro lens set to its minimum focal distance,
which creates a fixed distance between the camera and the subject.
The focal male was kept perpendicular to the camera by keeping
Page 2 of 10
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Cotton etal. Male mate preference, female ornament, and fecundity
both eye bulbs in focus. Eyespan was then estimated from the
size of the resultant image relative to a known standard, photo-
graphed under identical conditions. This method is highly accurate
compared with controlled measurements of the same individuals
(repeatability > 0.93; Lessells and Boag 1987; Small etal. 2009).
Laboratory studies—origin of flies and
generation of flies for experiments
The flies used were from a population collected in Ulu Gombak,
Peninsular Malaysia (3°19N, 101°45E) in 2005 (S.C. and A.P.).
They have since been maintained in the laboratory in cage culture
(>200 individuals to minimize inbreeding) at 25 °C on a 12:12 h
light:dark cycle and fed pureed sweet corn twice weekly. Fifteen-
minute artificial dawn and dusk periods were created by illumina-
tion from a single 60-W bulb, at the start and end of the lightphase.
To obtain experimental flies, we collected eggs from the cage
cultures over a 3-week period and reared larvae on a variable
amount of pureed sweet corn to ensure high variance in eyespan
(David et al. 1998; Cotton et al. 2004a). On eclosion, flies were
anesthetized on ice and measured for their eyespan, defined as
the distance between the outermost lateral edges of the eye bulbs
(Cotton etal. 2004a). Following Rogers etal. (2006), females were
separated into large and small size classes, defined as having eye-
spans >5.8 or <5.4 mm, respectively. Intermediate size females
were discarded. To control for the well-documented eects of male
eyespan on female mating behavior (Wilkinson and Reillo 1994;
Hingle et al. 2001a, 2001b), we used only large eyespan males
(>8.5 mm) with a low sample variance (N=36, mean ± standard
deviation [SD]=8.94 ± 0.31 mm) in the subsequent assays of pref-
erence. In addition, previous work has shown that small eyespan
females are less able to discriminate among variation in male eye-
span (Hingle etal. 2001a), and this eect may extend to male abil-
ity to discriminate. Finally, because small eyespan males have fewer
opportunities to mate under field conditions (Cotton etal. 2010),
their mate preferences may dier compared with large eyespan
males. Though this is a topic of interest, it is beyond the scope of
the current study.
Male mate preference for female eyespan
Male mate preference under laboratory conditions was examined
using a specially designed cage. This comprised two 500-mL trans-
parent plastic pots, one inverted on top of the other and separated
by a pair of opaque removable partitions (Figure 1). A roosting
string hung from the ceiling of the upper pot extending down to
near the base of the lower pot. The base of the test cage contained
moist cotton wool to maintain high humidity. The focal male and
the pair of tester females, 1 large and 1 small, were introduced
during late afternoon on the day prior to the assay. The male was
placed in the upper half of the cage and the females in the lower
half of the cage, and the partitions were inserted to keep the sexes
segregated (Figure1).
At the beginning of the dawn period on the following morn-
ing, the partitions were removed allowing the flies to interact. Male
mating behavior was observed for 30 min. A mating was defined
as a copulation lasting ≥ 30 s (as in the field study). Asample of
N = 36 males were assayed for mate preference. Large and small
females were drawn at random from a population of each type of
tester female (N > 25 for each type). Tester females were therefore
used more than once in the trials. However, they were never used
more than once in any 48-h period.
All individuals used in the experiment were nonvirgins, hav-
ing been kept in mixed-sex groups prior to the mate preference
experiment. They were collected over a short period of time (3
weeks) and so were of similar age when used in the experiment
(~8+ weeks). Female T.dalmanni are highly promiscuous and mate
at high frequencies (Wilkinson et al. 1998; Rogers, Baker, et al.
2005). Female reproductive life span is in the order of months
(Wilkinson and Reillo 1994; Reguera etal. 2004), so the incidence
of female virgins under natural conditions is rare, which we aimed
to mimic in this design. Females lay eggs continually after reach-
ing maturity, irrespective of whether they have mated and/or are
fertilized (Baker et al. 2001; Reguera et al. 2004). Moreover, the
frequency of matings among mated females has no detectable
eect on egg output (Baker et al. 2001). So, we consider details
of female mating history prior to the experiment will have had a
minimal influence on the egg-laying rate. Likewise, male virgins
are also rare in nature, as they also mate promiscuously and live
for months (Wilkinson and Reillo 1994; Reguera etal. 2004). So,
we also housed our experimental males in mixed-sex groups. This
procedure further allowed comparison between field and labora-
tory experiments.
To examine male fitness benefits of mating with large and small
females, we measured the fecundity of a sample of females using a
previously developed protocol (Cotton et al. 2006), after the mate
preference assays were complete. Tester females (N=23 large and
N = 22 small females) were housed individually in 500-mL pots
with a roosting string hanging from the top and moist tissue paper
and a food tray at the base. The tissue and food were removed from
the containers every 2–3days and all the eggs on both substrates
were counted. Females were allowed to acclimatize in their new
pots for 3days, and their fecundity was measured over the subse-
quent 11days (N=5 collections per female).
Roosting
string
Focal
male
Tester
females
Removable
partitions
Figure1
Apparatus used for male mate preference assays in laboratory experiments.
A focal male was placed in the upper section and 2 tester females in the
lower section. The sexes were separated by removable partitions (cardboard)
until testing commenced. Asingle string resembling a rootlet runs the whole
length of the cage, providing a suitable roosting site.
Page 3 of 10
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology
Male mate preference for female eyespan and
fecundity
We examined the relative importance of female eyespan and fecun-
dity in determining male mate preference under laboratory condi-
tions. Large and small female eyespan classes were set up as defined
above. Fecundity was experimentally manipulated by placing females
on a reduced quality diet for 2 weeks prior to the start of the mate
preference assays and then throughout the remainder of the experi-
ment (N=50 for each eyespan class). The reduced diet consisted of
20% corn to 80% sucrose. The eect of this diet on fecundity was
compared with that for females on a high-quality diet of 80% corn
to 20% sucrose. To ensure that all the food had the same viscos-
ity, an indigestible bulking agent, carboxymethycellulose (3% w/v),
was added to the sucrose (25% w/v) solution (Rogers et al. 2008).
Half of the females from each eyespan class were placed on each
diet. To characterize the eect of the diet manipulation on egg pro-
duction, we assessed the fecundity of females after the completion
of the mate preference assays. Eggs were collected (as above) from
pots (N = 15) each containing 10 females (in each pot, all females
were either on the high or reduced quality diet). Eggs were collected
every 2–4days over a 15-day period (N=6 collections per female).
We examined male mate preference across 5 dierent treatment
groups (Table 1) using the same mate preference assays described
above. Treatments 1 and 2 were control treatments for high and
reduced fecundity, respectively, while varying eyespan. Treatments
3 and 4 controlled for large and small eyespan, respectively, while
varying fecundity. Treatment 5 manipulated both fecundity and
eyespan, by giving each focal male the choice between a large
female with reduced fecundity and a small female with high fecun-
dity. Asample size of N=28 males was set up for each treatment.
Statistical analysis
Male mate preference was assessed using an index based on the
dierence between the observed and expected numbers of copula-
tions with large females. In the field study, P
Field
was calculated for
individual males and allowed for multiple copulations and variable
numbers of large and small females on the lek,
Pc
n
Nc c
i
i
i
t
FieldL
L
LS
=−
+
=
1
1
,
where c
L
is the number of copulations with large eyespan females
and c
S
is the number of copulations with small eyespan females, n
Li
is the number of large eyespan females in the harem at the time
of mating i, and N
i
is the total number of females in the harem at
the time of mating i. This index takes into account the changing
composition of leks through time, as females occasionally flew away
between matings by the focal male. P
Field
is 0 under random mating,
P
Field
> 0 for males showing mating preference for large eyespan
females, and P
Field
< 0 for males showing preferences for small eye-
span males. The minimum/maximum values of P
Field
always dier
by 1 but are not necessarily symmetric about 0 due to the distribu-
tion of large and small females on the lek throughtime.
P
Field
values were not normally distributed, so we tested whether
the mean of the distribution of individual male P
Field
values was
dierent from 0 using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. In harems in
which more than 1 mating was observed, we tested whether the
observed probability of a mating with a large eyespan female in
the ith mating attempt was more likely than expected by chance,
calculating the expected mating probability n
Li
/N
i
for that mating.
We used a repeated measures approach to evaluate variation in
preference across matings by testing whether the mean P
Field(mating
i)
P
Field(mating i+1)
value was significantly dierent from 0 using a
Wilcoxon signed-ranktest.
In the studies of male mate preference under laboratory condi-
tions, we used a similar index of male mate preference. This was
simpler as females could not depart from the test cage, and there
was always 1 large and 1 small female perpot,
P
cc
cc
Lab
LS
LS
=
+2( )
.
As for the field index, P
Lab
equals 0 under random mating, P
Lab
>
0 for preference for large females, and P
Lab
< 0 for preference for
small females. But in this case, the minimum/maximum values of
P
Lab
are ±0.5 and are symmetric about 0.We used the same index
when the 2 females diered in fecundity, substituting the copulation
rate of females with high (c
H
) or reduced (c
R
) fecundity for those of
large (c
L
) and small (c
S
) females. As before, we tested whether the
distribution of individual P
Lab
scores had a mean that was signifi-
cantly dierent from 0 using Wilcoxon signed-rank tests and used
similar procedures to investigate whether there was change in pref-
erence across subsequent matings.
In the second laboratory study in which females diered in
fecundity and eyespan, we examined the ability of males to distin-
guish high fecundity females, using female eyespan as a covariate.
Pairs of treatments in which fecundity diered but the eyespan of
both females was large (treatment 3) or small (treatment 4) were
combined, after separate analysis of each treatment.
In the first laboratory experiment in which females diered
in eyespan alone, we examined potential fecundity dierences
between large and small females by performing a general linear
model (GLM) on the number of eggs laid per female (5 repeated
measures), nesting female identity within the eyespan variable (large
or small). In the second laboratory experiment, we evaluated the
eect of the diet manipulation by examining fecundity of females
on the 2 diet treatments. To do this, we performed a GLM on each
of the number of eggs laid per pot (6 repeated measures), nesting
pot identity within diet manipulation.
All statistical analyses were performed using JMP V.10.0.0 (SAS
Institute, Cary, NC).
RESULTS
Male mate preference in field conditions
Just over half of observed males mated multiply under field conditions
at dawn (13/25) with a mating frequency, mean ± SD=1.52 ± 0.51
Table1
Examination of multiple signals used in mate choice
Treatment Female eyespan Female fecundity
1 Large or small Both high
2 Large or small Both reduced
3 Both large High or reduced
4 Both small High or reduced
5 Large or small Reduced (large eyespan)
or high (small eyespan)
Attributes of paired females presented to focal males in each treatment
group. Females potentially diered in eyespan (large or small) and/or
fecundity (high or reduced).
Page 4 of 10
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Cotton etal. Male mate preference, female ornament, and fecundity
(range 1–2). Males preferred to mate with large eyespan females
(P
Field
: mean ± standard error [SE]= 0.24 ± 0.08, Wilcoxon signed-
rank=94.00, degrees of freedom [df]=24, P=0.0074; Figure2).
In harems in which the focal male mated twice, we compared
the eyespan of females across the 2 matings. In the first (Wilcoxon
signed-rank = 42.50, df = 12, P = 0.0012) and second (Wilcoxon
signed-rank=30.50, df=12, P=0.0303) matings, males copulated
more often with large eyespan females than expected given their
frequency in the harem. There was no significant dierence in the
strength of mate preference across the 2 mating attempts (Wilcoxon
signed-rank=−7.50, df=12, P=0.2500).
We found no relationship between male eyespan and P
Field
(F
1,15
= 0.0023, P = 0.9622), suggesting that our results are not
confounded by any eect of the focal male’s eyespan on male or
female behavior. The number or types of female in a focal male’s
harem could have influenced his ability to express mate preference.
However, we found no relationship between the proportion of large
eyespan females in the harem and P
Field
(F
1,15
=1.5589, P=0.2310)
or between harem size and P
Field
(F
1,23
=0.6375, P=0.4328).
Male mate preference for female eyespan
Within the half-hour period allowed, most males (33/36) mated
multiply (mating frequency: mean ± SD = 3.97 ± 1.63, range
1–7), well in excess of what was typical under natural conditions.
Males preferred to mate with large eyespan females (P
Lab
: mean
± SE = 0.18 ± 0.05, Wilcoxon signed-rank = 155.50, df = 35,
P = 0.0010; Figure 3). This result was not contingent on singly
mated males, as there was still preference for large eyespan females
when singly mated males were excluded (P
Lab
= 0.19 ± 0.05,
Wilcoxon signed-rank=137.50, df =32, P= 0.0006). There was
no relationship between male eyespan and P
Lab
(F
1,30
 = 0.4871,
P=0.4906), suggesting that by only using large eyespan males, we
had removed any potential confounding eect of variation in eye-
span of the focalmale.
When preference was examined in sequential matings,
we found preference for large eyespan females in the first
(P
Lab
 = 0.17 ± 0.08, N = 36, Wilcoxon signed-rank = 111.00,
P=0.0438) and second (P
Lab
=0.20 ± 0.08, N=33, Wilcoxon
signed-rank = 110.50, P = 0.0212) matings. But the patterns
of the third (P
Lab
= 0.16 ± 0.09, N = 29, Wilcoxon signed-
rank = 67.50, P = 0.0951) and subsequent matings (Wilcoxon
signed-rank < 34.50, P > 0.2080) were not significantly dif-
ferent than expected under random mating. This may partly
have been due to the reduced sample size of males that mated
more often. But it could have reflected a decline in preference
among males that mated more often. When this was explicitly
tested however we found no association between preference
0
–1.0 - –0.8 –0.8 - –0.6 –0.6 - –0.4 –0.4 - –0.2 –0.2 - 0.0
Male mate preference (P
Field
)
0.0 - 0.2 0.2 - 0.4 0.4 - 0.6 0.6 - 0.8 0.8 - 1.0
2
4
6
Frequency
8
10
12
Figure2
Frequency distribution of P
Field
, the preference function of wild males. P
Field
accounts for the harem size, the number of large and small females available, and
the dynamic changes in these variables between matings. P
Field
=0 indicates no preference, P
Field
< 0 indicates a preference for small eyespan females, and
P
Field
> 0 indicates a preference for large eyespan females.
Page 5 of 10
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology
and the number of matings a male engaged in (F
1,34
= 3.0292,
P = 0.0908).
Females from the large eyespan group laid 36% more eggs per
day than those from the small eyespan group (F
1,180
 = 9.4249,
P = 0.0025; mean ± SE daily egg output, large = 4.76 ± 0.49,
N=23; small=3.49 ± 0.50, N=22).
Male mate preference for female fecundity and
eyespan
We manipulated fecundity through diet, using either a reduced
(20% corn) or high-quality diet (80% corn). Females on the reduced
diet manipulation laid fewer eggs than those on the high-quality
diet (F
1,75
= 111.7045, P < 0.0001; mean ± SE daily egg output,
reduced=0.2640 ± 0.02, N=7, high quality=1.99 ± 0.10, N=8).
In line with the first laboratory study of male mate prefer-
ence (see above), most males mated multiply (128/138), but at an
even higher rate (mating frequency: mean ± SD = 6.33 ± 3.12,
range 1–16). In the control treatments where diet was standard-
ized but eyespan varied (treatments 1 and 2, Table 1), there was
a significant preference for large eyespan females (P
Lab
: mean
± SE = 0.25 ± 0.08, Wilcoxon signed-rank = 307.50, df = 55,
P= 0.0029). We found a significant dierence between the 2 diet
treatments, with stronger male mate preference for large eyespan
females when females were on the high-quality diet (P
Lab
(high-
quality diet) = 0.41 ± 0.10, P
Lab
(reduced diet) = 0.09 ± 0.11,
χ
2
(df = 1) = 4.4544, N=56, P=0.0348; Figure4).
We further investigated how males responded to dierences
in female fecundity by varying diet and standardizing eyespan
(treatments 3 and 4, Table 1). Overall, there was a significant
preference for fecund females in the absence of eyespan varia-
tion (P
Lab
=0.21 ± 0.09, Wilcoxon signed-rank= 217.50, df=54,
P = 0.0275). There was no dierence in preference when males
were presented with either 2 large or 2 small eyespan females
(
χ
1
2
1 4418=
.,
N=55, P=0.2298).
Finally, we studied how changes in fecundity aected prefer-
ence for large eyespan females. Comparing male mate prefer-
ence when only large eyespan females diered in diet (treatments
1 and 5, Table 1), we found overall preference was for large eye-
span females (P
Lab
 = 0.24 ± 0.08, Wilcoxon signed-rank = 252.50,
df = 54, P = 0.0031), and this was stronger when the large eye-
span female had high fecundity (P
Lab
(treatment one)=0.41 ± 0.10,
P
Lab
(treatment 5) = 0.06 ± 0.11,
χ
2
(df = 1) = 4.9108, N = 55,
P = 0.0267). In contrast, comparing male mate preference when
only small eyespan females diered in diet (treatments 2 and 5,
Table1), we found no overall preference for large eyespan females
(P
Lab
= 0.07 ± 0.08, Wilcoxon signed-rank = 87.50, df = 54,
P = 0.3743) or any dierence in preference between the treat-
ments when the small eyespan female had high or low fecundity
(
χ
1
2
0= 0.041 ,
N=55, P=0.8396).
DISCUSSION
Mate preference by males is predicted when there is exploitable
variation in female quality, limited male mating capacity, and low
costs of finding and assessing mates (Bonduriansky 2001). These
conditions are met in the stalk-eyed fly T. dalmanni: females vary
considerably in fecundity (Cotton et al. 2010), males have limited
ability to mate multiply over short periods of time (Rogers, Baker,
0
–0.5 - –0.4 –0.4 - –0.3 –0.3 - –0.2 –0.2 - –0.1 –0.1 - 0.0
Male mate preference (P
Lab
)
0.0 - 0.1 0.1 - 0.2 0.2 - 0.3 0.3 - 0.4 0.4 - 0.5
2
4
6
8
10
12
Figure3
Frequency distribution of P
Lab
, the preference function of laboratory males, when given the choice of mating with either a large or small female. In the
laboratory assays, there is no dynamic change in the number of females available because females cannot leave the test arena. For P
Lab
, P=0 indicates no
preference, P
Lab
< 0 indicates a preference for small eyespan females, and P
Lab
> 0 indicates a preference for large eyespan females.
Page 6 of 10
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Cotton etal. Male mate preference, female ornament, and fecundity
et al. 2005; Rogers, Chapman, et al. 2005), and the lek mating
system means that several females congregate closely with males
who can choose the order in which they mate with them. These
flies are well known for female mate preference for males with
large eyespans (Wilkinson and Reillo 1994; Cotton etal. 2010), so
we hypothesized that males might also use variation in eyespan to
assess females. This is logical because female eyespan has several
of the properties usually associated with sexual ornaments. Firstly,
female eyespan in sexually dimorphic stalk-eyed fly species is an
exaggerated trait in comparison to sexually monomorphic species
(Baker and Wilkinson 2001). Secondly, female eyespan is sensitive
to stress compared with nonsexual traits (e.g., wing size), even after
controlling for body size (Cotton etal. 2004b). In addition, female
eyespan is a reliable indicator of fecundity even after controlling for
the influence of body size (Cotton etal. 2010).
Male mate preference in T.dalmanni was investigated both in the
wild and in controlled laboratory experiments. The vast majority
of previous studies examining male mate preferences, especially
in Drosophila, have been performed in the laboratory (e.g., Byrne
and Rice 2006; Edward and Chapman 2013), and thus we have
remained largely ignorant on the importance of male mate prefer-
ence under natural conditions. Indeed, some authors have noted
that laboratory studies involving simultaneous choice tests may
result in inflated preference estimates as the abundance of “mates”
they encounter in the lab far exceeds that found in the wild (Barry
and Kokko 2010). Thus, our study provides important data on male
mating preferences under natural conditions. In the wild, males
arrive at lek sites at early dusk. Males fight to be the sole lek holder,
with the largest male typically being successful (Small etal. 2009).
Females then arrive and choose which lek to join and roost on, with
large eyespan males attracting more females (Cotton et al. 2010).
Males defend their harem during dusk from intrusions and mating
attempts by other nonlek holding males. The majority of the mat-
ings occur the following morning before flies disperse (Burkhardt
and de la Motte 1988; Lorch etal. 1993). In our wild leks, we found
that males with multifemale harems mated more frequently with
the largest eyespan females in the harem. This eect was indepen-
dent of harem size. We defined large female eyespan as the larg-
est eyespan available for the focal male to mate with, on the basis
that his assessment would be among those females in his harem.
In parallel, under experimentally controlled laboratory conditions,
we confirmed male mate preference for large female eyespan. In
these experiments, we gave males limited choices between pairs
of females, one with large and the other with small eyespan, and
restricted the dispersal of females. A large proportion of copula-
tions in the wild occur within 20–30 min of dawn (Lorch et al.
1993), and our laboratory experiments mirrored this, considering
only a 30-min window. We also constrained male eyespan in the
laboratory experiment, only using males that had large eyespan. So,
male preference may be dierent in small eyespan males though
our data from the field (where male eyespan was unconstrained) did
not reveal any variation in preference with male eyespan.
Given that female eyespan covaries positively with body size
(David et al. 1998), we cannot discount that our observation of
male mate preference for female eyespan arose indirectly from male
mate preference for large-bodied females. However, 2 lines of evi-
dence suggest that eyespan, rather than body size, is likely to be the
main cue that males use in their choice of mate. First, flies assess
each other face-on, meaning that the laterally elongated eyestalks
are more readily assessed than body size, which would require flies
High Fecundity
Male mate preference (P
Lab
)
–1.5
–1
–0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
Low Fecundity
Treatment
Figure4
The eect of fecundity on male mate preference (independent of eyespan). Male mate preference for large eyespan females (P
Lab
), when females were fed a
high-quality diet and had high fecundity (treatment one) or a reduced diet and had low fecundity (treatment 2). There was stronger male mate preference
when females were on the high-quality diet. The line represents the mean preference of the 2 diet treatments. *P<0.05.
Page 7 of 10
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology
to be oriented perpendicular to each other. Second, female eyespan
is a condition-dependent trait (Cotton etal. 2004b) and is a more
accurate signal of fecundity than body size alone (Cotton et al.
2010). It is also possible that cues other than eyespan, such as subtle
behavioral cues or chemical signals (Thomas 2011), influence mat-
ing behavior and might allow females to indicate their reproduc-
tive value to males. The disentanglement of such highly correlated
traits is a general problem faced by researchers of mate preference
in sexually dimorphic species (Hedrick and Temeles 1989).
Previous work has shown that males sire more ospring (fertile
eggs) following a single mating with a large eyespan female (Rogers
etal. 2006). Similarly, in wild-caught T.dalmanni (Cotton etal. 2010)
and in this laboratory experiment, female eyespan was a good indi-
cator of fecundity, and this is also the case in a related stalk-eyed fly
species, D. meigenii (Harley etal. 2013). These findings echo other
studies that have demonstrated male mate preference for females
with large body size or for female ornamental trait values that are
good predictors of female fecundity (Amundsen 2000; Amundsen
and Forsgren 2001; Bonduriansky 2001; Doutrelant et al. 2008;
Baldauf et al. 2011; Potti et al. 2013). Our own and these other
studies suggest that males with mating preference for large orna-
ments will, all things being equal, sire more ospring compared
with males who mate at random. However, fecund females are
likely to attract more matings by males, thereby increasing the
potential for sperm competition and diluting the gain in paternity
stemming from any particular male or particular mating. This has
been the subject of some theoretical consideration (Servedio and
Lande 2006; Nakahashi 2008), and our experimentation does not
directly address the range of fitness benefits and costs that follow
from male mate preference. For instance, males choosing larger
females may have a reduced likelihood of fertilizing any particular
egg, but this may be compensated by the fact that larger females
lay more eggs. The exact relationship between female ornaments,
female quality, and fitness needs to be elucidated through experi-
mentation in which the paternity gain to a male mating with an
attractive, large eyespan female is compared with a mating with a
less attractive small eyespan female. Afull analysis will also need to
consider the genetic and environmental inputs to female fecundity
and any interaction between these 2 factors. This will require fur-
ther analysis both in the field as well as using laboratory manipula-
tive experimentation.
In a further experiment, we manipulated diet as a way of alter-
ing fecundity independently of female eyespan (Hingle etal. 2001a,
2001b). Flies on reduced quality food had relatively low fecundity.
By constraining female eyespan while manipulating diet, we were
able to show male mate preference for females with higher fecun-
dity per se. These results lend support to the idea that males are
using multiple cues when assessing females. Perhaps males detect
the distension of the female abdomen that occurs when it harbors
many mature eggs. Another possibility is that females signal their
fecundity through scent as has been shown in other insects (Peeters
et al. 1999; Mitra and Gadagkar 2012) or other sensory modali-
ties. The use of multiple cues in mate preference decisions, such
as visual, chemical, and behavioral signals, has been the focus of
much interest with a key question being what information they sig-
nal (Candolin 2003; Bro-Jørgensen 2010).
These dietary manipulations also showed that the strength of
preference for fecundity dierences induced by diet did not dif-
fer when both tester females had large eyespan or both had small
eyespan. However, there were interactions between female eyespan
and fecundity. Male mate preference was weakened when the large
eyespan female was put on a reduced quality diet, but there was no
eect on preference of moving the small eyespan female between
diets. These results imply that fecundity dierences have a greater
eect on the attractiveness of large eyespan females than on that of
small eyespan females. However, this needs to be verified by further
investigation, involving direct measures of individual fecundity. In
this context, it is vital to further investigate how fecundity dier-
ences alter preferences among males in thewild.
It could be argued that the distribution of observed copula-
tions results from female behavior rather than male mate prefer-
ence, for example, if large females are more eager to mate. Indeed,
large females do need to mate more frequently than small females
although this has been interpreted as a reflection of their higher
fecundity and hence their need for more copulations to oset the
chronic sperm limitation typical of this species (Baker etal. 2001;
Rogers, Baker, et al. 2005; Cotton et al. 2010). However, several
lines of evidence suggest that eects of female behavior cannot
account (entirely) for the mating biases reported here. If females
compete among themselves for access to a male, then one might
expect that females with the largest eyespans would prevail, and
biased mating distributions would result from intrasexual competi-
tion rather than male mate preference. However, there is no evi-
dence that female eyespan influences contest outcome in female
T. dalmanni (Al-khairulla et al. 2003). In addition, observations of
lek sites reveal no obvious evidence that females compete for access
to males on the lek and it is indeed males who exhibit patrolling
behavior (personal observation). Likewise, we found no evidence
that harem size or the proportion of large females in the harem
correlated with preference in our wild experiment. In addition, in
our laboratory experiments, male eyespan was controlled to avoid
strong female mate preference influencing the outcome. Although
it is not possible to eliminate female eects, it seems likely that the
patterns in our data result primarily from male-controlled biases in
mating.
We have shown, using a combination of field studies and con-
trolled laboratory experiments, that males from a well-known
model species of harem-based polygyny exhibit strong preference
for female traits that indicate fecundity. We also provide evidence
that males can directly assess fecundity when variation in morpho-
logical traits associated with male mate preference is controlled
for. Males use multiple cues in their mate assessment. Future work
should capitalize on these initial findings and seek to explain the
variation that exists in male mate preference and estimate how this
aects the strength of sexual selection on male sexual ornaments.
The eect of male eyespan and condition on male mate preferences
(i.e., whether small eyespan males exhibit a dierence in preference)
should also be examined as condition-dependent male mate prefer-
ences could occur. Future work should also endeavor to understand
the cues used by females to attract male mating to provide a more
complete picture of how sexual selection operates in this species.
FUNDING
We acknowledge support from a variety of sources: A.J.C. by
a UCL IMPACT Studentship administered by the CoMPLEX
Systems Biology Programme, J.S.by a Biotechnology and Biological
Sciences Research Council Studentship, S.C. by a Natural
Environment Research Council (NERC) Research Fellowship (NE/
E012620/1), and A.P. by grants from Engineering and Physical
Sciences Research Council (EP/F500351/1, EP/I017909/1) and
NERC (NE/G00563X/1).
Page 8 of 10
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Cotton etal. Male mate preference, female ornament, and fecundity
The authors thank H. Eager, C. Liedtke, L. Bellamy, and M. Földvári
for help with data collection, K. Fowler for comments on the article,
and R. Hashim and the sta at the Ulu Gombak Field Research Centre,
University of Malaya, Kuala Lumpur, for their assistance.
Handling editor: Paco Garcia-Gonzalez
REFERENCES
Al-khairulla H, Warburton D, Knell RJ. 2003. Do the eyestalks of female
diopsid flies have a function in intrasexual aggressive encounters? J Insect
Behav. 16:679–686.
Amundsen T. 2000. Why are female birds ornamented? Trends Ecol Evol.
15:149–155.
Amundsen T, Forsgren E. 2001. Male mate choice selects for female color-
ation in a fish. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 98:13155–13160.
Andrade MCB. 2003. Risky mate search and male self-sacrifice in redback
spiders. Behav Ecol. 14:531–538.
Baker RH, Ashwell RIS, Richards TA, Fowler K, Chapman T,
Pomiankowski A. 2001. Eects of multiple mating and male eye span
on female reproductive output in the stalk-eyed fly, Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni.
Behav Ecol. 12:732–739.
Baker RH, Denni M, Futerman P, Fowler K, Pomiankowski A, Chapman
T. 2003. Accessory gland size influences time to sexual maturity and
mating frequency in the stalk-eyed fly, Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni. Behav Ecol.
14:607–611.
Baker RH, Wilkinson GS. 2001. Phylogenetic analysis of sexual dimor-
phism and eye-span allometry in stalk-eyed flies (Diopsidae). Evolution.
55:1373–1385.
Baldauf SA, Bakker TCM, Kullmann H, Thünken T. 2011. Female nuptial
coloration and its adaptive significance in a mutual mate choice system.
Behav Ecol. 22:478–485.
Barry KL, Kokko H. 2010. Male mate choice: why sequential choice can
make its evolution dicult. Anim Behav. 80:163–169.
Bateman AJ. 1948. Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila. Heredity. 2:349–368.
Bonduriansky R. 2001. The evolution of male mate choice in insects: a syn-
thesis of ideas and evidence. Biol Rev. 76:305–339.
Bro-Jørgensen J. 2010. Dynamics of multiple signalling systems: animal
communication in a world in flux. Trends Ecol Evol. 25:292–300.
Burkhardt D, de la Motte I. 1985. Selective pressures, variability, and
sexual dimorphism in stalk-eyed flies (Diopsidae). Naturwissenschaften.
72:204–206.
Burkhardt D, de la Motte I. 1988. Big ‘antlers’ are favoured: female choice
in stalk-eyed flies (Diptera, Insecta), field collected harems and laboratory
experiments. J Comp Physiol A. 162:649–652.
Byrne PG, Rice WR. 2006. Evidence for adaptive male mate choice in the
fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Proc R Soc B. 273:917–922.
Candolin U. 2003. The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biol Rev.
78:575–595.
Cotton S, Fowler K, Pomiankowski A. 2004a. Condition dependence of
sexual ornament size and variation in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dal-
manni (Diptera: Diopsidae). Evolution. 58:1038–1046.
Cotton S, Fowler K, Pomiankowski A. 2004b. Heightened condition depen-
dence is not a general feature of male eyespan in stalk-eyed flies (Diptera:
Diopsidae). J Evol Biol. 17:1310–1316.
Cotton S, Rogers DW, Small J, Pomiankowski A, Fowler K. 2006.
Variation in preference for a male ornament is positively associated with
female eyespan in the stalk-eyed fly Diasemopsis meigenii. Proc R Soc B.
273:1287–1292.
Cotton S, Small J, Hashim R, Pomiankowski A. 2010. Eyespan reflects
reproductive quality in wild stalk-eyed flies. Evol Ecol. 24:83–95.
Darwin C. 1871. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex.
London: John Murray.
David P, Hingle A, Greig D, Rutherford A, Pomiankowski A, Fowler K.
1998. Male sexual ornament size but not asymmetry reflects condition in
stalk-eyed flies. Proc R Soc B. 265:2211–2216.
Dewsbury DA. 1982. Ejaculate cost and male choice. Am Nat. 119:601–610.
Doutrelant C, Grégoire A, Grnac N, Gomez D, Lambrechts MM, Perret P.
2008. Female coloration indicates female reproductive capacity in blue
tits. J Evol Biol. 21:226–233.
Edward DA, Chapman T. 2013. Variation in male mate choice in Drosophila
melanogaster. PLoS One. 8:e56299.
Forsberg J. 1987. A model for male mate discrimination in butterflies.
Oikos. 49:46–54.
Funk DH, Tallamy DW. 2000. Courtship role reversal and deceptive sig-
nals in the long-tailed dance fly, Rhamphomyia longicauda. Anim Behav.
59:411–421.
Gwynne DT. 1991. Sexual competition among females: what causes court-
ship-role reversal? Trends Ecol Evol. 6:118–121.
Harley E, Birge LM, Small J, Tazzyman SJ, Pomiankowski A, Fowler K.
2013. Ejaculate investment and attractiveness in the stalk-eyed fly,
Diasemopsis meigenii. Ecol Evol. 3:1529–1538.
Hedrick AV, Temeles EJ. 1989. The evolution of sexual dimorphism in ani-
mals: hypotheses and tests. Trends Ecol Evol. 4:136–138.
Hingle A, Fowler K, Pomiankowski A. 2001a. The eect of transient food
stress on female mate preference in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni.
Proc R Soc B. 268:1239–1244.
Hingle A, Fowler K, Pomiankowski A. 2001b. Size-dependent mate prefer-
ence in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni. Anim Behav. 61:589–595.
Honěk A. 1993. Intraspecific variation in body size and fecundity in insects:
a general relationship. Oikos. 66:483–492.
Lande R, Arnold SJ. 1985. Evolution of mating preference and sexual
dimorphism. J Theor Biol. 117:651–664.
LeBas NR, Hockham LR, Ritchie MG. 2003. Nonlinear and correla-
tional sexual selection on ‘honest’ female ornamentation. Proc R Soc B.
270:2159–2165.
Lessells CM, Boag PT. 1987. Unrepeatable repeatabilities: a common mis-
take. Auk. 104:116–121.
Lorch PD, Wilkinson GS, Reillo PR. 1993. Copulation duration and sperm
precedence in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis whitei (Diptera: Diopsidae).
Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 32:303–311.
Maynard Smith J, Harper D. 2003. Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Mitra A, Gadagkar R. 2012. Queen signal should be honest to be involved
in maintenance of eusociality: chemical correlates of fertility in Ropalidia
marginata. Insect Soc. 59:251–255.
Nakahashi W. 2008. Quantitative genetic models of sexual selection by
male choice. Theor Popul Biol. 74:167–181.
Parker GA. 1983. Mate quality and mating decisions. In: Bateson P, editor.
Mate choice. New York: Cambridge University Press. p. 141–166.
Peeters C, Monnin T, Malosse C. 1999. Cuticular hydrocarbons cor-
related with reproductive status in a queenless ant. Proc R Soc B.
266:1323–1327.
Potti J, Canal D, Serrano D. 2013. Lifetime fitness and age-related female
ornament signalling: evidence for survival and fecundity selection in the
pied flycatcher. J Evol Biol. 26:1445–1457.
Preston BT, Stevenson IR, Pemberton JM, Wilson K. 2001. Dominant rams
lose out by sperm depletion. Nature. 409:681–682.
Reguera P, Pomiankowski A, Fowler K, Chapman T. 2004. Low cost of
reproduction in female stalk-eyed flies, Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni. J Insect
Physiol. 50:103–108.
Rogers DW, Baker RH, Chapman T, Denni M, Pomiankowski A, Fowler
K. 2005. Direct and correlated responses to artificial selection on male
mating frequency in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni. J Evol Biol.
18:642–650.
Rogers DW, Chapman T, Fowler K, Pomiankowski A. 2005. Mating-
induced reduction in accessory reproductive organ size in the stalk-eyed
fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni. BMC Evol Biol. 5:37.
Rogers DW, Denni M, Chapman T, Fowler K, Pomiankowski A. 2008.
Male sexual ornament size is positively associated with reproductive
morphology and enhanced fertility in the stalk-eyed fly Teleopsis dalmanni.
BMC Evol Biol. 8:236.
Rogers DW, Grant CA, Chapman T, Pomiankowski A, Fowler K. 2006.
The influence of male and female eyespan on fertility in the stalk-eyed fly,
Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni. Anim Behav. 72:1363–1369.
Servedio MR, Lande R. 2006. Population genetic models of male and
mutual mate choice. Evolution. 60:674–685.
Small J, Cotton S, Fowler K, Pomiankowski A. 2009. Male eyespan and
resource ownership aect contest outcome in the stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis
dalmanni. Anim Behav. 78:1213–1220.
South SH, Arnqvist G. 2011. Male, but not female, preference for an orna-
ment expressed in both sexes of the polygynous mosquito Sabethes cyaneus.
Anim Behav. 81:645–651.
Thomas ML. 2011. Detection of female mating status using chemical sig-
nals and cues. Biol Rev. 86:1–13.
Trivers RL. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell
B, editor. Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871–1971. Chicago
(IL): Aldine Publishing Co. p. 136–179.
Page 9 of 10
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology
Webberley KM, Hurst GD. 2002. The eect of aggregative overwintering
on an insect sexually transmitted parasite system. J Parasitol. 88:707–712.
Wedell N, Gage MJG, Parker GA. 2002. Sperm competition, male pru-
dence and sperm-limited females. Trends Ecol Evol. 17:313–320.
Wilkinson GS, Dodson GN. 1997. Function and evolution of antlers and
eye stalks in flies. In: Choe J, Crespi B, editors. The evolution of mating
systems in insects and arachnids. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University
Press. p. 310–328.
Wilkinson GS, Kahler H, Baker RH. 1998. Evolution of female mating
preferences in stalk-eyed flies. Behav Ecol. 9:525–533.
Wilkinson GS, Reillo PR. 1994. Female choice response to artificial selection
on an exaggerated male trait in a stalk-eyed fly. Proc R Soc B. 255:1–6.
Page 10 of 10
by guest on November 17, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
... All experimental females were virgins, 6-8 weeks old, and had reached sexual maturity (Baker et al., 2003). ST females were anesthetized on ice and their eyespans were measured (see below method) to exclude small flies and limit variation in size and fecundity that could influence sperm allocation strategies in males (Cotton et al., 2015). Only large females with an eyespan > 5.4 mm were used in mating trials (range 5.4-5.8 ...
... On the trial day, a single male was added to each container ~15 min after dawn, as this is the period during which mating is most likely . Males were allowed to mate, defined as a copulation lasting ≥ 30 s, as durations shorter than this are usually insufficient for sperm transfer (Cotton et al., 2015;Rogers et al., 2006). The mating duration was recorded. ...
... The effect of male thorax length (a proxy for body size) and relative eyespan (the variation in eyespan after controlling for thorax length) were also considered in the analysis. Both traits are strongly condition dependent and indicators of male genetic and phenotypic quality (Cotton et al., 2015;David et al., 2000;Howie et al., 2019). Whether these male trait sizes differed between genotypes was tested by fitting thorax length and relative eyespan as the response variable in linear models. ...
Article
Full-text available
Male X-linked meiotic drive systems, which cause the degeneration of Y-bearing sperm, are common in the Diptera. Sperm killing is typically associated with fitness costs that arise from the destruction of wildtype sperm and collateral damage to maturing drive sperm, resulting in poor success under sperm competition. We investigate X-linked meiotic drive fertility in the stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis dalmanni. Drive male paternity was measured in double mating trials under sperm competition against a wildtype male. Drive males sired the same number of offspring as wildtype males, both when mated first or second. This is the first evidence that drive males can compete equally with non-drive males in double matings, challenging the assumption that drive males inevitably suffer reduced fertility. The finding is in accord with previous work showing that the number of sperm per ejaculate transferred to females during non-competitive single matings does not differ between drive and wildtype males, which is likely due to the adaptive evolution of enlarged testes in drive males. Future experiments will determine whether the competitive ability of drive males is maintained under higher rates of female remating likely to be experienced in nature.
... All experimental females were virgins, 6-8 weeks old, and had reached sexual maturity (Baker et al., 2003). ST females were anesthetized on ice and their eyespans were measured (see below method) to exclude small flies and limit variation in size and fecundity that could influence sperm allocation strategies in males (Cotton et al., 2015). Only large females with an eyespan > 5.4 mm were used in mating trials (range 5.4-5.8 ...
... On the trial day, a single male was added to each container ~15 min after dawn, as this is the period during which mating is most likely . Males were allowed to mate, defined as a copulation lasting ≥ 30 s, as durations shorter than this are usually insufficient for sperm transfer (Cotton et al., 2015;Rogers et al., 2006). The mating duration was recorded. ...
... The effect of male thorax length (a proxy for body size) and relative eyespan (the variation in eyespan after controlling for thorax length) were also considered in the analysis. Both traits are strongly condition dependent and indicators of male genetic and phenotypic quality (Cotton et al., 2015;David et al., 2000;Howie et al., 2019). Whether these male trait sizes differed between genotypes was tested by fitting thorax length and relative eyespan as the response variable in linear models. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In male X-linked meiotic drive systems, the driver causes degeneration of Y-bearing sperm, leading to female-biased offspring sex ratios. This potentially leads to a two-fold transmission advantage to drive chromosomes. However, drive-bearing sperm often do poorly in sperm competition, limiting their ability to spread. We use the stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis dalmanni , to investigate the success of the X-linked Sex Ratio (SR) meiotic drive system. In this species, polyandrous matings, where a female mates with multiple males, are common. Recent findings demonstrate SR males transfer the same numbers of viable sperm as wildtype (ST) males during mating, implying that they do not necessarily have reduced fertility under sperm competition. Reciprocal mating trials were performed to measure the success of SR and ST sperm in double mated females, with either a SR or ST male mated first followed by a male of the alternative genotype. There was no significant difference in the number of offspring sired by SR and ST males. This equivalence held regardless of whether the SR male mated first or second. We show these results are consistent with previous studies that suggested SR male sperm do poorly in sperm competition. Future experiments will determine whether the competitive ability of SR males is maintained under higher stress conditions likely to be experienced in nature, in which females mate repeatedly with multiple males. The results from the current study helps to explain the high meiotic drive frequency of around 20% in wild populations in this species. Impact Summary Meiotic drive genes are selfish genetic elements that distort Mendelian patterns of inheritance to bias transmission in their favour. We use the stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis dalmanni , to investigate the fitness effects associated with a meiotic drive gene called Sex Ratio (SR), which is linked to the X chromosome. In males, SR destroys Y-bearing sperm, meaning only X-bearing sperm are viable, and females who mate with drive males sire all-female broods. This confers a two-fold transmission advantage to the SR gene, as it is transmitted to all offspring. We recently discovered that drive males have evolved compensatory mechanisms to cope with the sperm destruction caused by meiotic drive. They have greatly enlarged testes, allowing them to produce more sperm. When drive males mate with females, they deliver as many sperm and sire as many offspring as wildtype males. Building on this finding, we measured how drive male sperm performs against sperm from a non-carrier male in sperm competition – where the sperm from different males compete to fertilise an egg. Double mating trials were performed, where a single female was mated once to a drive and once to a non-carrier male. By genotyping offspring, we show that the number of offspring sired by the drive male was not different from the number sired by the non-carrier competitor. These findings contrast with those in other species. Typically, drive males do poorly in sperm competition and their spread is severely restricted by sperm competition. In stalk-eyed flies, female multiple mating with many males is the norm, but this does not appear to inhibit the fertility of drive males. The success of drive under sperm competition helps to explain the high frequency of drive around 20% in natural populations of T. dalmanni .
... Although less studied than female mate choice, there is evidence that male mate choice is common across taxa [15][16][17][18][19] and is often based on female traits that are correlated with fecundity [17][18][19][20][21]. Despite reproductive senescence causing female fecundity to decline with age, there is mixed evidence of how male mate choice is affected by female age. ...
... Male mate choice could be favoured in these systems because males are rare and likely to encounter multiple hermaphrodites simultaneously [27]. Males may be able to target mates that are more fecund [17][18][19][20][21], are genetically dissimilar to themselves [27] or are most receptive to outcrossing [28]. Reproductive senescence could be important in male mate choice because ageing in hermaphrodites could affect the viability and/or supply of both male and female gametes and their ability to self-fertilize (and tendency to outcross) [3,29,30]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Reproductive senescence is common across taxa and females often show a predictable decline in fecundity after maturity. Attending to these age-dependent cues could help males make optimal mate choice decisions. Here, we examined reproductive senescence and male mate choice in the androdioecious mangrove rivulus (Kryptolebias marmoratus), where self-fertilizing hermaphrodites exist with rare males. Hermaphrodites showed a strong decline in fecundity as they aged and genetic lineages varied in their fecundity at both young and old ages. Surprisingly, when given a simultaneous choice between genetically identical old and young hermaphrodites, males did not simply prefer younger hermaphrodites. Instead, male preference for younger versus older partners depended on the genetic lineage of the partners, resulting in a strong genotype × age interaction. For some genetic lineages, hermaphrodites were more attractive to males when younger, but for other genetic lineages, hermaphrodites were more attractive when older. Our results suggest that the genetic identity of the partner is key to how males weigh age-dependent changes in fecundity and that males are able to assess genetic variation in attractiveness over a partner’s reproductive lifespan. Exploring how gamete viability and outcrossing are affected by age across genetic lineages could help us further understand these male preferences.
... In insects, body size usually has positive effects on fitness (Fisher 1915(Fisher , 1958Andersson 1994;Cotton et al. 2015), and thus larger individuals have an advantage over smaller ones in terms of mate competition and sexual selection (Darwin 1871;Trivers 1972; Thornhill and Alcock 1983; Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved. ...
Article
Full-text available
Monochamus alternatus is a notorious pest of the pine forests across East Asian countries. Both large and small individuals of either sex coexist in the wild. However, whether and how body size affects sexual selection outcomes and longevity remains unknown. In this study, we allowed a male or a female of small or large size to choose between two mates of varying sizes, which were from a wild-caught population. Our results revealed that mating latency and frequency of mating attempts varied across different social environments. Both large males and large females preferred larger mates, whereas this preference was not observed for the small males and small females. Male–male competition had no significant effect on ejaculation duration and frequency or sperm number in ejaculates. When a single male had an opportunity to choose between two females of different sizes, the reproductive investment of small males towards the small females was almost doubled compared to that of the large males, i.e., longer ejaculation duration, higher ejaculation frequency, and greater numbers of sperm transferred. However, the greater mating efforts of small males did not translate to greater fecundity/fertility of small females. We also showed that regardless of their body size, females always had greater fertility when they mated with large males but evidence for a trade-off between fecundity and longevity was not found. Alternatively, female fecundity was positively associated with longevity, and fertility was negatively associated with longevity and fecundity. We discussed these results placing it in a broader context of mate choice evolutionary dynamics.
... Energy budgets are influenced by the cost of sperm production and quality, which may be particularly important in polygynous species in which there is no paternal care and males have relatively unlimited access to females (Reinhold, Kurtz, & Engqvist, 2002). Sperm limitation is the main argument used to explain why, in polygynous feral red junglefowl (Gallus gallus) and stalk-eyed flies (Teleopsis dalmanni), males transfer more sperm to females with larger combs (Cornwallis & Birkhead, 2007;Pizzari, Cornwallis, Lovlie, Jakobsson, & Birkhead, 2003) and larger eyespan (Cotton, Cotton, Small, & Pomiankowski, 2014), respectively. Consequently, abundant theoretical and empirical evidence favor the view that male mate choice is more widespread than previously thought in monogamous or polygynous species displaying conventional sex roles (Hare & Simmons, 2019;Kraaijeveld et al., 2007;Schlupp, 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Female ornamentation is frequently observed in animal species and is sometimes found as more evolutionary labile than male ornamentation. A complex array of factors may explain its presence and variation. Here we assessed the role of female cost of reproduction and paternal care. Both factors have been pinpointed as important by theoretical studies but have not been investigated yet in details at the interspecific level. We worked on 133 species of North temperate Passeriformes bird species for which both the clutch volume – here taken as the proxy of female cost of reproduction – and amount of paternal care are relatively well known. Using spectrometry, we measured the whole-body coloured plumage patches and quantified three metrics corresponding to brightness (i.e. achromatic component), colour chromaticity (i.e. intensity) and colour volume (i.e. diversity). We found a strong association between male and female colour metrics. Controlling for this association, we found additional small but detectable effects of both cost of reproduction and paternal care. First, females of species with more paternal care were slightly brighter. Second, the interaction between the level of paternal care and egg volume was correlated with female colour intensity: females with more paternal care were more chromatic, with this association mostly present when their investment in reproduction was low. Together these results suggest that female cost of reproduction and paternal care are part of the multiple factors explaining variation of female coloration, besides the strong covariation between male and female coloration. Abstract To a lesser extent than male conspicuous plumage, female plumage colouration may also vary across bird species. This study explores the role of egg production and male parental care in the evolution of female plumage colouration, using 133 species of songbirds. Pictures were provided by Justine Le Vaillant, David López-Idiáquez and Matthew Silk.
... This ambiguity has been suggested to be due to between-species variation in ornament-fecundity relationships deciding whether or not male mate choice is adaptive (Watson & Simmons, 2010). Studies published in the later years give ambiguous conclusions as well, with some studies being in accordance with (Cantarero et al., 2017;Cotton et al., 2015;Hernández et al., 2021;Lüdtke & Foerster, 2018, 2019, and others contrary to (Caro et al., 2021, andRigaill &Garcia, 2021) predictions from the direct selection hypothesis. Some studies give support to the genetic correlation hypothesis (Sganga & Greco, 2019), and the social selection hypothesis (Enbody et al., 2018, see also Kroken et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Females in mutually ornamented species are often less conspicuously ornamented than their male conspecifics. It has been hypothesized that offspring quality may decrease if females invest more resources into ornaments at the expense of resources in eggs. An experiment was carried out to test whether natural variation in carotenoid in the eggs from a wild population of Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus) was associated with survival and growth of their offspring until hatching. Wild Arctic charr were caught at a spawning ground during the spawning period. Eggs from two different females, one female with yellowish carotenoid-rich eggs and one with paler eggs, were fertilized by sperm from the same male. This was repeated until gametes were collected from 42 females and 21 males, giving a total of 21 groups. After fertilization, the zygotes from each of the two females were reared in four replicated groups. These 168 groups were reared separately until hatching when the surviving larvae were counted and their body length measured. For the two response variables survival and body length at hatching, no effect was demonstrated of any of the predictors (i) amount of carotenoid in the unfertilized eggs, (ii) the mothers' body condition, or (iii) ornament intensity of their red carotenoid-based abdominal ornament. Thus, this study gives no support for the hypothesis that females investing less carotenoid into their eggs suffer from decreased offspring quality until hatching. This lack of association between female ornament intensity and their fitness is not as expected if female ornaments evolved due to direct sexual selection from males on the more ornamented females ("direct selection hypothesis").
... Thus, non-contact cues, such as the morphologies and odors of their counterparts, do not seem to be useful for mate recognition in this species. Males of the stalk-eyed fly species Teleopsis dalmanni, who demonstrate sexual dimorphism in their eyespan, can differentiate the fecundities of females when given a choice (Cotton et al. 2015). Similarly, in S. detrahens, the presence of choices may also provide males with some information for better discrimination between potential mates. ...
Article
Full-text available
Among insects, males have evolved the ability to discriminate potential mates to avoid vain mating attempts. In Diptera, courtship and mating behaviors of species that do not discriminate potential mates have rarely been studied. In this study, we investigated copulation attempts of the stalk-eyed fly, Sphyracephala detrahens males, which do not engage in any observable pre-copulatory courtship behavior. We found that sexually mature males attempted to copulate with mature males and immature adults of both sexes, as frequently as with mature females. Successful copulation only occurred with mature females. Immature females rejected mounting males by vibrating their bodies and immature and mature males by incurving their abdomen downward. Comparison of the cuticular hydrocarbon components of individuals of different sexes and maturity levels indicated that the cuticular hydrocarbon profiles were similar regardless of the sex and sexual maturity. Therefore, we suggest that visual or chemical information are of little use for males S. detrahens in discriminating receptive conspecific mates. Since elaborate mate discrimination behaviors lead to additional time and energy costs when discrimination is difficult, S. detrahens may increase reproductive success simply by increasing the number of copulation attempts. Digital video images related to this article are available at http://www.momo-p.com/showdetail-e.php?movieid=momo211123sd01a, http://www.momo-p.com/showdetail-e.php?movieid=momo211123sd02a, http://www.momo-p.com/showdetail-e.php?movieid=momo211123sd03a, and http://www.momo-p.com/showdetail-e.php?movieid=momo211123sd04a.
... potential direct or indirect benefits of mating with a particular partner may be beneficial to both sexes, such as in species with biparental care and serial monogamy [16][17][18]. However, malemate choice has been shown to occur even in polygynous species, as evidenced by differential sperm transfer depending on a female's attractiveness [19][20][21]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Colourful traits in females are suggested to have evolved and be maintained by sexual selection. Although several studies have evaluated this idea, support is still equivocal. Evidence has been compiled in reviews, and a handful of quantitative syntheses has explored cumulative support for the link between condition and specific colour traits in males and females. However, understanding the potential function of females' colourful traits in sexual communication has not been the primary focus of any of those previous studies. Here, using a meta-analytic approach, we find that evidence from empirical studies in birds supports the idea that colourful female ornaments are positively associated with residual mass and immune response, clutch size and male-mate preferences. Hence, colourful traits in female birds likely evolved and are maintained by sexual selection as condition-dependent signals.
... Empirical support for the different hypotheses on evolution of ornaments in females of mutually ornamented species in general are ambiguous (reviewed by Amundsen, 2000, Kraaijeveld et al., 2007, Clutton-Brock, 2009, Nordeide et al., 2013, Svensson & Wong, 2011, Tobias et al., 2012, see also Lüdtke & Foerster, 2019, Lüdtke & Foerster, 2018, Cotton et al., 2014, Sganga & Greco, 2019, Enbody et al., 2018, LaPlante, 2015, Belliure et al., 2018. The shape of the association between ornaments and fecundity may vary among species and at least part of this variation may affect whether male mate choice of the more showy females is adaptive or not (Watson & Simmons, 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Conspicuous ornaments are often considered a result of evolution by sexual selection. According to the social selection hypothesis, such conspicuous traits may also evolve as badges of status associated with increased boldness or aggression toward conspecifics in conflicts about ecological resources. This study tested predictions from the social selection hypothesis to explain evolution of conspicuous red color of the pelvic spines of the three-spine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Wild nonreproducing sticklebacks were presented to pairs of dummies which differed at their pelvic spines, having either (i) normal-sized gray or red pelvic spines or (ii) normal-sized gray or large red pelvic spines. The experimental tank was illuminated by white or green light, since green light impedes the sticklebacks’ ability to detect red color. The dummies moved slowly around in circles at each end of the experimental tank. We quantified the parameters (i) which of the two dummies was visited first, (ii) time taken before the first visit to a dummy, (iii) distribution of the focal sticklebacks in the two zones close to each of the two dummies and in the neutral zone of the tank, (iv) close to which of the two dummies did the focal fish eat its first food-piece, and (v) time spent until the first piece of food was eaten. This was carried out for 22 females and 29 males sticklebacks. The results suggested no effect of the color or size of the dummies’ pelvic spines, on none of the five behavioral parameters. Moreover, neither the color of the pelvic spines of the focal sticklebacks themselves (as opposed to redness of the dummies’ spines) nor their body length was associated with behavior toward the dummies. Thus, this study did not support predictions from the social selection hypothesis to explain evolution of red pelvic spines in sticklebacks.
Article
Full-text available
Males are often predicted to prefer virgin over non‐virgin females because of the reduced risk of sperm competition. Does this prediction hold across studies? Our systematic meta‐analysis of 138 studies, mainly conducted in invertebrates, confirms that males generally prefer virgin females. However, males preferred virgin females even in species with last male sperm precedence, suggesting that sperm competition alone does not drive male preferences. Furthermore, our results suggest that males may reject mated females even when no alternative exists. Preference for virgins is unlikely to influence female reproductive success since virginity cannot be selected for, but strong preference for virgin females could swamp or reinforce selection on other traits. Our results add to growing evidence that males are not indiscriminate in mating. However, given the unexplained heterogeneity in effect sizes, we urge caution in assuming that males will prefer virgins and recommend considering the natural context of mating decisions.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Internal reproductive organ size is an important determinant of male reproductive success. While the response of testis length to variation in the intensity of sperm competition is well documented across many taxa, few studies address the importance of testis size in determining other components of male reproductive success ( such as mating frequency) or the significance of size variation in accessory reproductive organs. Accessory gland length, but not testis length, is both phenotypically and genetically correlated with male mating frequency in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni. Here we directly manipulate male mating status to investigate the effect of copulation on the size of both the testes and the accessory glands of C. dalmanni.Results: Accessory gland length was positively correlated with male mating frequency. Copulation induced a significant decrease in accessory gland size. The size of the accessory glands then recovered slowly over the next 8 - 48 hours. Neither testis length nor testis area was altered by copulation.Conclusion: These results reveal that the time course of accessory gland recovery corresponds to field observations of mating behaviour and suggest that accessory gland size may limit male mating frequency in C. dalmanni.
Chapter
Full-text available
Insects and arachnids display the most impressive diversity of mating and social behaviour among all animals. This book investigates sexual competition in these groups, and the variety of ways in which males and females pursue, persuade, manipulate, control and help one another, enabling us to gain a better understanding of how conflicts and confluences of interest evolve together. Each chapter provides a comprehensive review of mating systems in particular insect and arachnid groups, discusses intrinsic and extrinsic factors responsible for observed mating strategies, and suggests fruitful avenues for further research. The book culminates in a synthesis, reviewing the date in terms of the theory of sexual conflict. This broad-based book will be of immense value to students and researchers interested in reproductive strategies, behavioural ecology, entomology and arachnology.
Article
Full-text available
Queens of many social insect species are known to maintain reproductive monopoly by pheromonal signalling of fecundity. Queens of the primitively eusocial wasp Ropalidia marginata appear to do so using secretions from their Dufour’s glands, whose hydrocarbon composition is correlated with fertility. Solitary nest foundresses of R. marginata are without nestmates; hence expressing a queen signal can be redundant, since there is no one to receive the signal. But if queen pheromone is an honest signal inextricably linked with fertility, it should correlate with fertility and be expressed irrespective of the presence or absence of receivers of the signal, by virtue of being a byproduct of the state of fertility. Hence we compared the Dufour’s gland hydrocarbons and ovaries of solitary foundresses with queens and workers of post-emergence nests. Our results suggest that queen pheromone composition in R. marginata is a byproduct of fertility and hence can honestly signal fertility. This provides important new evidence for the honest signalling hypothesis.
Article
Full-text available
Quantitative genetic models for the evolution of exaggerated male traits through female mate choice predict that selection on male ornaments should cause a correlated response in female preferences. Furthermore, female selectivity should be inversely related to costs of mate choice. Here we use a stalkeyed fly, Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni (Diptera:Diopsidae), which exhibits pronounced sexual dimorphism in eye span, to evaluate these predictions. Field observations reveal that each evening females aggregate while males disperse among roosting sitcs where mating occurs. A positive regression between male relative eye span and the number of females in an aggregation suggests that sexual selection acts on male eye span. Mate choice experiments in the lab, using flies after 13 generations of bidirectional selection on male relative eye span, reveal that females from long eye-span lines and an unselected population preferred long eye-span males. Short eye-span line females, however, preferred short eye-span males, demonstrating a genetic correlation between female preference and a sexually selected male trait. Eye span of the largest male in a field aggregation correlated positively with female age, as estimated by amount of eye pigment, and was independent of egg number, thereby providing no evidence that mate choice impairs female survival or fecundity.
Article
In contrast to the extensively studied sexually transmitted diseases (STDs) of humans, little is known of the ecology or evolutionary biology of sexually transmitted parasites in natural systems. This study of a sexually transmitted parasite on an insect host augments our understanding of both the parasite's population dynamics and virulence effects. The impact of over-wintering was assessed on the prevalence of the parasitic mite Coccipolipus hippodamiae on the two-spot ladybird, Adalia bipunctata. First, the effect of infection on host survival was examined during the stressful overwintering period. Box experiments in the field revealed that the infected ladybirds, especially males, are less likely to survive overwintering. The study provides the first evidence that the parasite harms males and suggests revisions of theories on the adaptive virulence of sexually transmitted parasites. It also indicates the importance of using a range of experimental conditions because virulence can be dependent on host condition and sex. Box experiments were also used to examine whether transmission of the parasite occurs within overwintering aggregations. These revealed that substantial transmission does not occur in aggregations and that transmission is predominantly sexual. Overall, the virulence effects and the lack of transmission mean that the overwintering period acts to diminish parasite prevalence and will retard the spring epidemic associated with host reproductive activity.
Article
Age at first reproduction is an extremely important life-history trait. Several factors such as nutritional state and age-specific fecundity have been shown to influence time to sexual maturity; however, little work has been done in insects. We addressed this in a stalk-eyed fly (Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni), by testing the hypothesis that time to sexual maturity is associated with the development of male internal reproductive structures. We found that sexual maturity was attained after an increased rate of growth in the accessory glands, several days after mature sperm bundles, and motile sperm were observed in the testes. Although testis development is essential, the results suggest that accessory gland growth is more closely associated with the time taken to reach sexual maturity than is testis growth. When we manipulated the growth of testes and accessory glands via a dietary manipulation, we found that delayed growth rates increased the time taken to reach sexual maturity. Among the delayed individuals, sexually mature males had larger accessory glands, but not testes, than did immature males. In adult males, mating frequency was significantly positively correlated with accessory gland size, but not with testis length or body size. We conclude that accessory gland size is a critical determinant of sexual maturity and male mating frequency in this species. Copyright 2003.
Article
Male butterflies can only perform a limited number of matings during their lifetime, due to the fact that they transfer a substantial amount of energy and non-renewable nutrients to females during each copulation. At least in some species the males seem to need a longer or shorter time to recover after each mating. This makes it advantageous to male butterflies to mate preferentially with large, newly eclosed females, which usually have the highest expected egg production. However, the cost of discrimination is that searching time will increase in relation to the proportion of females that will be refused. In this paper I present a model that investigates how these two consequences of mate discrimination influence the expected number of offspring a male will have. The model predicts no mate discrimination in sparse populations or when mortality rate is high. When the density of available females is high or mortality is low, there is an advantage to males preferring newly eclosed, large females. In species with long male recovery time or low male mating capacity, male discrimination among females should be more frequent.
Article
The relationship between intra-specific variation in female body size and potential fecundity was investigated using the published literature on 57 oviparous species of Coleoptera, Diptera, Ephemeroptera, Heteroptera, Homoptera, Hymenoptera, Lepidoptera, and Trichoptera, and 11 species of larviparous Aphidina and Diptera. Female body sizes were converted to dry body weight. Variation in body weight and fecundity was expressed as percentage deviation from the median values. The increase in fecundity with body weight was similar in most taxa, with only a few important exceptions. The common regression for oviparous and larviparous species predicts a 0.95% increase in median fecundity for each 1% increase in dry body weight. The number of ovarioles (in 10 species of Coleoptera, Diptera, Hymenoptera and Orthoptera) also increased with body weight. The general relationship predicted a 0.81% increase in ovariole number for each 1% increase in dry body weight. The slope of ovariole number versus weight relationship was greater in species with many ovarioles than in species with few. The common slope of the fecundity/size relationship is close to 1 and this indicates that female size is a principal constraint on insect potential fecundity.
Book
Why are animal signals reliable? This is the central problem for evolutionary biologists interested in signals. Of course, not all signals are reliable; but most are, otherwise receivers of signals would ignore them. A number of theoretical answers have been proposed and empirical studies made, but there still remains a considerable amount of confusion. The authors, one a theoretician the other a fieldworker, introduce a sense of order to this chaos. A significant cause of confusion has been the tendency for different researchers to use either the same term with different meanings, or different terms with the same meaning. The authors attempt to clarify these differences. A second cause of confusion has arisen because many biologists continue to assume that there is only one correct explanation for signal reliability. The authors argue that the reliability of signals is maintained in several ways, relevant in different circumstances, and that biologists must learn to distinguish between them. In this book they explain the different theories, give examples of signalling systems to which one or another theory applies, and point to the many areas where further work, both theoretical and empirical, is required. John Maynard Smith is one of the most influential scientists of his generation and his theories have transformed our understanding of animal behaviour, whilst David Harper is a reknowned field ecologist. Animal signals are one of the hottest and most controversial subjects in animal behaviour, and are also of major importance to an understanding of human behaviour and the evolution of language.
Article
Sperm are generally delivered in batches (ejaculates or spermatophores) that may include many millions of gametes. Males are limited with respect to the number of ejaculates they can deliver and the time required to restore depleted reserves. Prudence would thus be expected in the allocation of ejaculates to females. Among the forces acting to limit the number of females with which a promiscuous male mates are high pregnancy initiation requirements, sperm competition, female choice and control, and the costs and risks of searching. Optimal strategies for ejaculate allocation will vary with the operational sex ratio. Males can and do discriminate among females as potential mating partners.-from Author