ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to describe some of the basic features of attachment theory, and explore how they relate to the development of the “social mind” and the work of therapeutic communities (TC). Design/methodology/approach – The author describes the essentials of attachment theory in humans; and the development of both secure and insecure states of mind. The author will set out how insecure attachment systems are associated with deficits in mentalising processes which are fundamental to the activity of the social mind. Findings – The author suggests how attachment to a TC can promote mentalising processes. The author draws on the work of other speakers in the conclusions about how to “grow” secure minds and societies. Research limitations/implications – This paper is a brief over view only and does not address attachment process to TC in any depth. Practical implications – Attachment theory could help both service users and therapists who work in TCs understand some of the difficulties people have in engaging at the start. Attachment theory also gives a guide to what a “good enough” experience in a TC might look like. Originality/value – There is little existing discussion of the application of attachment theory to TCs.
Security and the social mind: attachment
and therapeutic communities
Gwen Adshead
Dr Gwen Adshead is
Consultant Forensic
Psychiatrist at the Forensic
Psychiatry Department,
Southern Health Foundation
Trust, Fareham, UK.
Abstract
Purpose The purpose of this paper is to describe some of the basic features of attachment theory, and explore
how they relate to the development of the social mindand the work of therapeutic communities (TC).
Design/methodology/approach The author describes the essentials of attachment theory in humans;
and the development of both secure and insecure states of mind. The author will set out how insecure
attachment systems are associated with deficits in mentalising processes which are fundamental to the
activity of the social mind.
Findings The author suggests how attachment to a TC can promote mentalising processes. The author
draws on the work of other speakers in the conclusions about how to growsecure minds and societies.
Research limitations/implications This paper is a brief over view only and does not address attachment
process to TC in any depth.
Practical implications Attachment theory could help both service users and therapists who work in TCs
understand some of the difficulties people have in engaging at the start. Attachment theory also gives a guide
to what a good enoughexperience in a TC might look like.
Originality/value There is little existing discussion of the application of attachment theory to TCs.
Keywords Security, Therapeutic communities, Psychoanalytic techniques, Groups, Attachment, Social mind
Paper type Viewpoint
Introduction
It is truly a privilege to have contributed to the annual Windsor Conference in September 2014;
and then to be able to offer some written reflections, enriched by reading those of others. I am a
group therapist by training, and my experience so far has repeatedly confirmed for me that my
best work is always a group effort. So too here, this paper should be seen very much as the
product of a group process: fed by the thinking of others, and the experience of being in all kinds
of groups, both social and therapeutic.
Like Rex, my training began with an encounter with the therapeutic community (TC) movement,
which occurred completely by chance. It was the summer after a year of house-jobs, and I was a
hapless houseman sitting in the doctors mess, reading the paper (an experience that must now
seem Elysian to current foundation year doctors who do not have rooms to sit in where you can
talk to peers or just be). I noticed an advertisement for a Richmond Fellowship Conference
with a programme of speakers that included Thomas Szasz, Antony Clare, R.D. Laing and
Metropolitan Anthony of Sourouzh. Even in my completely uneducated state, I knew this was
a line-up that no would-be psychiatrist should miss, so I booked myself in.
The conference was amazing and I was spell bound. R.D. Laing was difficult to follow, however;
and I was struggling to make sense of what he was saying when a deep pleasant voice rumbled
in my ear, saying Do you think Ronnie is drunk?. I had nothing to offer in response to this; but
my neighbour continued to engage me in interesting conversation and invited me to join him and
The author is grateful to Dr Steve
Pearce for his editorial assistance.
PAGE 12
j
THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES: THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES
j
VOL. 36 NO. 1 2015, pp. 12-20, © Emerald Group Publishing Limited, ISSN 0964-1866 DOI 10.1108/TC-02-2015-0009
his wife for a drink and supper. He introduced himself as Maxwell Jones, and over dinner, I had a
crash course in TC theory and methodology, and the social psychiatry challenge to mainstream
psychiatry. I had no idea what he was talking about, but the democratic ideal sounded just right to
me, and I was moved and impressed. I was even impressed when Max, pursuing the spirit of
openness, honesty and transparency, rang me up at one oclock in the morning to ask me how
many lovers I had had.
I took this spirit of openness, honesty and transparency with me into my first psychiatric job, and
I have tried to nurture it ever since. My early exposure to TC thinking and process acted as an
essential corrective to the psychiatric training, which (then as now) encouraged junior
psychiatrists to categorise distressed people into diagnostic categories and to try to medicate
their distress away. I was not lucky enough to do the junior psychiatry job at the Henderson,
but I had lots of exposure to people who did; and to the luminaries who worked there, like Estela
Welldon, Stuart Whiteley and Kingsley Norton, who supervised and taught me well, despite my
general ineptitude and resistance.
1. Attachment theory
I came across attachment theory first when I was working at the Trauma Clinic at the Maudsley, in
the context of trying to understand why different people responded differently to traumatic
events. The epidemiology was very clear; not everyone got PTSD after a trauma, and some (very
odd) people had no distress at all (Kessler et al., 1995). Further, some people seemed to be able
to make good use of the therapies we were offering (then a mixture of CBT and more supportive,
reflective work) and some seemed to get much worse. I was greatly helped by reading Scott
Hendersons paper on abnormal care eliciting behaviours (Henderson, 1974) and by reading
Bowlby; or more accurately, Jeremy Holmesexcellent biography of Bowlby (Holmes, 1993).
I tried to think about how attachment theory might be useful to the jobbing clinician; and
especially how it might help us understand those people with the fruitless label of personality
disorder (Adshead, 1998). I am very grateful to those patients with that label whom I met as junior
trainee in forensic psychiatry; the learning process was scary but what I learnt was invaluable.
In his excellent Quintessence paper (this issue) Rex Haigh comments that attachment is not a
cuddly process. He insightfully reminds us that attachment processes are closely related to
containment of emotions, which in turn involve the exercise of boundaries and rules in the
creation and maintenance of relationships. I will now try to distil some key features of attachment
theory from the vast literature on this subject that I think are particularly important for TC practice.
I start from the assumption that what Rex has memorably called The -TC-in-the headis closely
related to what Robin Dunbar calls the social mind( Dunbar, 2003). Attachment theory had its
roots in ethology of social bonds in non-human primates; first in the work of Harry Harlow, and
continued by Stephen Suomi. Like our non-human primate neighbours, with whom we share so
much genetic material, we are group animals, whose survival depends on attachment to, and
membership of, groups: so much so that prolonged human social isolation is associated with
early death, typically by violence or suicide (Wilkinson and Marmot, 2003). Dunbars work (1993,
1995; Aiello and Dunbar, 1993) suggests that the expanded volume of the human neocortex
developed in proportion to the size of the groups that humans could be members of; and
relatedly to the use of language, which makes social relationships and group membership
possible across distance and time.
Humans need to attach to something or someone; the social self is grounded in relationships with
others. Important aspects of identity are relational and constructed in relationships: especially
roles involved in caring such as parent, health care professional or therapist. Attachment to a
group facilitates a creative engagement between the individual and the group, and regulates both
cooperation and competition between individuals. One could argue that attachment to a group is
the essence of the pro-social experience; it balances the hierarchical tendency and fear based
impulses to hostile competition with affiliation and cooperative behaviours.
The first research on attachment bonds was carried out in Harlows primate laboratory; and
found that early disruptions to the development of social bonds in monkeys had profound social
VOL. 36 NO. 1 2015
j
THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES: THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES
j
PAGE 13
effects when those monkeys grew up. Monkeys who were either raised in social isolation, or who
had peer not maternal rearing, showed very abnormal behaviours with other monkeys when they
were adults: specifically, they struggled to make sexual pair bonds, attacked their offspring if they
had any, and mounted suicidal attacks on senior monkeys who were higher in the hierarchy.
These behaviours in monkeys were depressingly similar to the behaviours that got people
referred to the forensic clinics that I knew best. Reading his work, I noted a tragically ironic
comment by Harlow in which he comments that the dreadful social isolation to which he exposed
baby monkeys could never happen to human children. I had already worked out that most of my
forensic patients, especially those with personality disorder, had as children suffered similar types
of social isolation and much, much worse.
2. The clinical aspect of attachment theory: a brief overview
It may be helpful to think of attachment systems operating in both mind and brain: as
neurobiological and psychobiological systems that develop in childhood and influence care
eliciting and care giving behaviours across the life span and the regulation of the feelings that
arises when people seek care or give care to others. These systems are activated when we
experience illness, fear, anxiety and any other situation where we feel vulnerable and/or perceive a
discrepancy between our power and someone elses. Attachment systems are relational, and
rely on both conscious and unconscious processes of readingother peoples intentions
towards us: one aspect of the term mentalising. Without mentalising, there would be no social
mind because mentalising entails awareness and reflection on the existence of othersminds as
being as real as our own (Allen, 2006; Frith, 2007).
Mary Ainsworth et al.s (2014) research drew on attachment theory and ethology, and set out to
study how small children (18-36 months) react to a relational stressor; namely being left alone in a
strange place, and also being left with a stranger. This research established a number of crucial
facts: first, that children could be reliably divided into different groups on the basis of their
behaviour in this strange situation; and second, that some children showed odd and disturbing
behaviours when reunited with their mothers. The majority of children (60 per cent) showed a
securepattern of behaviour when stressed; they protested, they cried, and they clung to their
attachment figure on reunion. In contrast, the insecuregroup (40 per cent) either seemed not to
notice when mother left or reappeared; or seemed ambivalent in their response; or displayed very
odd behaviours: freezing, aggression and apparent spacing out. These insecure groups were
categorised as avoidant, ambivalent and disorganised, respectively.
From Ainsworths work has grown a vast research enterprise; either looking at what happens in
the long term to insecurely attached children, and the difference between them and securely
attached children; or studying the neurobiological nature of attachment bonds, both secure and
insecure. Research from the latter perspective have demonstrated that the attachment system is
a neurobiological one, involving specific neural circuits connecting the neocortex to the amygdala,
and other parts of the brain that regulate negative feelings.
In terms of the former work, it is clear that irrespective of temperament, relationships determine
relationships. Early attachment sets a pattern for future adult relationships (school, peers,
romance), especially those relationships that involve care giving or care eliciting. Attachment
security status is not immutable: securely attached children can become insecure if exposed to
trauma and adversity, and some insecure children can earnsecurity if they have positive
experiences of care in later childhood or adulthood (Weinfield et al., 2000). Ainsworths research
has been replicated in many studies in different countries and cultures, and the normative data
base is impressive: a review of data from 10,000 people confirms that a substantial sub-group
(40 per cent) of the general community have insecure attachment systems, and this proportion
greatly increases in clinical populations (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn, 2009).
3. Security of attachment: regulation of relational stress and distress
Why should insecurity of attachment be a risk factor for psychological and psychiatric morbidity?
The explanation lies in the role of attachment systems in regulating emotions and feelings,
PAGE 14
j
THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES: THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES
j
VOL. 36 NO. 1 2015
especially negative feelings that are activated by relational stress. Attachment systems are
activated across the life-cycle, whenever we seek care or provide it to others with whom we
are in caring relationships. A secure attachment system allows the individual to enjoy a healthy
mature dependency on others when vulnerable. Attachment relationships act as hidden
regulatorsof distress responses at times of crisis and loss: distress responses that are
physiological, psychological, and sociological. The attachment system is both psychological and
embodied; as Antonio Damasio has suggested, we experience emotions in our bodies, and these
emotions are converted into psychological feelings and representations of relational experience
(Damasio et al., 1996).
A child with a secure attachment system learns first that they can reliably access good enough care
from a carer when they are frightened and distressed; that they can safely approach and get close to
their carer when they are vulnerable. Years of good enough-care allow the child to build up a
psychological representation of how to get care when vulnerable, and how to give care; and how to
readthe mind of a carer and experience their own mind being read. By the age of five, a securely
attached child can name and articulate distress; seek care effectively, and even soothe themselves to
some degree ( which make school possible as well as the development of social bonds outside the
family). A secure child can construct a self-narrative of their experience of distress, which generally
becomes more reflective and includes other peoples perspectives as they get older. A securely
attached child is well placed to manage the pubertal challenge to identity and experience, as bodies
and selvesbecome sexed, gendered and autonomous in very definite and social ways.
Attachment security is vulnerable in adolescence because of the massive neurophysiological and
psychological changes over this period. However, most securely attached adolescents negotiate
this period to become secure young adults; ready for their roles as care givers to themselves and
others, and ready to attach to their own social group. Most importantly, they can manage normal
distress, and know how to get help if they are struggling.
The capacity to manage relational stress and distress is essential for life long success. Bluntly,
fear and distress are not optional in a long happy mammalian life; managing response to fear,
personal threat and loss is part of life in relationships. For humans, loss of attachment figures is a
potent threat, as evidenced by the huge psychological and psychiatric costs of bereavement:
often rather dismissed but significant. A 2014 study of the long-term effects of bereavement
found that 30 per cent of people suffered real psychiatric morbidity in the year after the loss of a
loved one; including new severe mental illness such as psychotic breakdown (Kaplow and Layne,
2014). My point here is not to medicalise bereavement especially but to note that attachment
disruptions can seriously disorganise your mind, often for quite long periods. Simply put, your
mental security rests on the stability of your relationships with others: your mind is social.
Intriguingly, our closest attachments nearly always involve ambivalence and negative feelings
towards our attachment figures, so that managing hostile feelings towards those we care for is
part of the security of the attachment system. Other people can be experienced as a form of
threat; sometimes just because we depend on them, and are vulnerable with them. Winnicott
(1949) described this in his famous paper about hate in the counter-transference; it may also
make more sense to understand Kleins account of infantile envy as really a fearful response to an
awareness of neediness and power disparities. However, conceptualised, in our social
relationships, others can be experienced as both a source of support and threat; and a secure
attachment system makes this ambivalence and ambiguity easier to manage and bear.
4. Risky business: insecurity of attachment and clinical risk
Allen Schore (2002) has described how security of attachment helps infants develop a properly
regulated sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system capability for stress responding. If
you have a secure attachment system, you can cope with distress, loss, life-threat by getting help
and being able to be present with your own distress. You do not become violent or psychotic or
somatise (very much). You eventually reorganise around your loss , and can symbolise what you
think and communicate it in words or images with others.
It follows from this that insecurity of attachment systems is a significant handicap to optimal
development of the social mind. Insecurely attached children have insecure neurophysiological
VOL. 36 NO. 1 2015
j
THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES: THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES
j
PAGE 15
responses to relational stress, as well as insecure psychological beliefs and cognitions about
how to access care. Insecure children tend to become insecure adolescents who become
insecure adults (Weinfield et al., 2000), who manage their distress in dysfunctional ways.
The dysfunction is manifest in disturbances of affect regulation and metacognition, especially in
relation to other minds.
Insecure avoidant individuals tend to dismiss distress ( their own and other peoples) and may
even be derogatory about their distress. They deactivate their attachment systems, and are
hypo-aroused to stress. They claim to be fineand may seem fineon the surface; they will often
be reluctant to seek help from others and avoid getting close or being vulnerable. In the general
population about 20 per cent of people show this attachment style: but this proportion is much
greater in clinical populations, especially offender populations (Pfafflin and Adshead, 2003). Their
self-narratives are often thin, sparse and contradictory; for example, when asked about their early
life, they may cheerfully claim to have no memories of childhood, while also saying that it was
perfect or excellent.
Insecure ambivalent individuals oscillate between getting close to others when they are
distressed and pulling away. Rapid swings between positive attachment and hostile withdrawal
characterise the ambivalent state of attachment; and these individuals frequently seek care and
closeness to others, only to reject it very soon afterwards. Attachment systems are hyper-
aroused and activated on a frequent basis, and metacognition is limited, with real problems in
distinguishing what seemsfrom what is. Reality testing may be poor for both avoidant and
ambivalent individuals, but can be especially compromised in the ambivalent group as they
become emotionally hyper-aroused. In the general populations, ambivalent attachment is found
in about 16 per cent of the population; but again, this proportion is much higher in those seeking
medical, psychiatric or psychotherapeutic help: for obvious reasons. This group are also called
preoccupiedbecause their self-narratives are preoccupied with their relationships with others;
sometimes in an angry way. Their attachment narratives often focus on how powerful other
people are, and others are failing or exploiting them, while they are helpless to act.
The disorganised attachment group are still not well studied in adulthood. The key feature of this
group is that they have highly disorganised and irregular responses to stress and distress; becoming
unpredictably hypo-aroused or hyper-aroused in unreliable and inconsistent ways. Their capacity to
self-soothe is poor, and disorganised children grow up to be adolescents who develop controlling
strategies for relating to others: either controlling care giving or controlling care eliciting. These
adolescents often describe dissociative experiences, suggesting that their regulation of conscious
awareness and reality testing may be compromised. This group are at risk for substance misuse, as
they struggle to manage bodily feelings of distress, which may be stimulated by what would seem to
others to be innocuous events or experiences. Their self-narratives are often highly incoherent, and
sometimes characterised by what have been called Hostile-helplessstates of mind in relation to
others (Lyons-Ruth et al., 2007). Such states of mind are stimulated by care giving roles such as
parenting; or when receiving care from care givers.
These brief clinical descriptions of insecure states of mind may well be ringing clinical bellsfor
the reader, and rightly so. Insecure states of mind have been described in high proportions in
many clinical groups, especially people with personality disorder and psychosis. Many
commentators have suggested that personality disorder would be better renamed as adult
attachment disorder or relational disorders. There is also good evidence that attachment security
influences the presentation and treatment of people with psychotic illnesses and physical
disorders like diabetes (Berry et al., 2007; Read and Gumley, 2008; Maunder and Hunter, 2008).
The obvious question is how insecurity of attachment develops in young children; and the answer
is a multifactorial one. It is likely that there is a genetic component, especially in relation to those
genes that affect the regulation of neuro-peptides like dopamine and oxytocin that are active
in processes of craving, desire and closeness: although the research findings are mixed
(Van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2006). What is absolutely clear is that childhood
adversity, and/or social isolation and deprivation in childhood is a potent risk factor for insecurity
of attachment. Children exposed to childhood maltreatment (especially physical abuse and
neglect) are at much greater risk of developing disorganised attachment in childhood; and
PAGE 16
j
THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES: THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES
j
VOL. 36 NO. 1 2015
growing up then to be disorganised adults. A study by Murphy et al. (2014) found a very strong
relationship between increasing childhood adversity and insecurity of attachment.
A key aspect of attachment insecurity is the inability to mentalise effectively, i.e to be able to read
and be aware of their own intentions as well as other peoples; to be able to reflect on them; to be
comfortable with ambiguity and uncertainty, and to be able to regulate thinking and feeling
together. Mentalising function is low in many insecure individuals (Levy et al., 2006), especially
those with disorganised attachment. This is important for two reasons: poor mentalising function
may make it difficult for people to relate effectively to care-givers (e.g. in therapy); and carers with
poor mentalising function are not likely to be able to give the good-enough care that will promote
the development of secure attachment in their off spring. Several studies have shown that women
with insecure attachment tend to have babies who have exactly the same insecure attachment
pattern as they do; and this is detectable from as early as six weeks.
It will now be obvious what people with insecure attachment systems will struggle with. They
struggle with the experience of distress, and their response to that distress. Adults with insecure
attachments cannot put negative feelings into words, but avoid trying to manage those feelings:
either by locating them in the body or by externalising them, and seeking an external solution to
an internal problem. Their self-narratives are incoherent and lack a sense of agency in relation to
others, or to their own distress. Linguistically, their narratives show many markers of incoherence
of thought and of self-experience; and lack meta-cognitive skills, such appearance reality
distinction or acknowledging the possibility of change of view over time or context.
The good news is that it seems that it is possible to help people with insecure attachment improve
their mentalising function ( Levy et al., 2006). The bad news is that it probably takes longer than
the six weeks currently allocated to distressed people by most mental health services. The other
badish news is that the progress of therapy may be stormy and uncomfortable for all parties
involved as the therapeutic process itself activates the attachment system (Liotti, 2007; Fonagy
and Adshead, 2012). For the patient, there is a paradoxical agenda here for them to do the
opposite of what they have done for so long: avoidant people have to experience more affect
without running away, and ambivalent people have to stay with ambivalence and ambiguity in
relationships without running away. In this sense, avoidant and ambivalent attachment insecurity
can be understood as defensive ways of coping with a global dysregulation of the attachment
system, which are organised and complementary; whereas the disorganised attachment style is
evidence of a lack of any sort of defensive structure that will work to effectively reduce stress.
5. Attachment and TCs: mentalisation is the social mind in action
Mentalisation is a key psychological process for the activation and experience of a social mind.
Without mentalising skills, other people will not be realto you, and your own mind may also not
seem very real. Poor mentalising also affect the ways that we relate to our bodily experience,
especially when anxious. Those who mentalise poorly may act in ways that alienate others: just
when they need them most.
The attachment literature claims that the capacity to mentalise begins with attachment to a
person who can be a secure basefor the child who has to growa social mind. I would argue
that the TC (however, configured) can act as a secure base for insecure people to attach to, so
they can learn to develop a social mind. TCs already offer therapeutic approaches which are
associated with secure attachment: truthfulness, open communication and tolerance of
negativity and ambiguity. The TC offers itself as a regulator of all those who participate in it; while
recognising that the participants are co-regulating each other and generating a matrix of security.
If these people with insecure attachment can tolerate the anxiety of engagement ( which is
considerable), then there is ample evidence that they can learn to develop mentalising skills in
relation to themselves and others. Boundary setting and maintenance, group relating and
dialogue create a matrix of relating that enables mentalising skill to develop and grow. It is
noteworthy that mentalisation based therapy (MBT) is essentially group therapy with individual
support (which is a model that TCs are familiar with); what is important is the evidence that MBT is
effective in helping people manage their distress better. MBT has been tested on the same
VOL. 36 NO. 1 2015
j
THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES: THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES
j
PAGE 17
populations of people who use TCs; and I suspect that the proven effectiveness of TCs is partly
due to the improvement in mentalising that TCs enable.
But as both Rex Haigh and Hanna Pickard (forthcoming) suggested in their papers, there is something
even more important about the process of belonging to a TC; more important than mentalising. It is the
process of becoming more agentic, more present and active in terms of engaging in social life. It entails
a reworking of the self-narrative from one that is highly passive ( in which things just keep happening to
you) to a narrative which acknowledges that there are choices to be made, and ownership of actions
that make up identity. This is not an easy process, and it is uncomfortable to do as an adult; I suspect
that for people with secure attachments, the move from a passive voice to an active voice in the
self-narrative happens organically as people age and develop. When I was a child, I thought as a child,
I spoke as a child, but when I became a man, I put away childish things: the difficulty for people with
insecure attachments is that their self-narratives are full of childish themes, of powerful adults who do
not care, of giants that oppress and terrorise, of helpless children threatened by nightmares and fears.
Their challenge in therapy is to gradually let go of the cover-story of their lives, and rebuild a new self-
narrative, a more adult one; which is co-constructed in dialogue with other people who have living out
similar challenges; where vulnerability and dependence are respected and supported; where sadness
and grief are accepted as events that shape identity but do not determine it.
As I have said, this is hard work: as more experienced TC residents and workers will attest. I am
mindful that it is possible; that people can and do change their narratives of themselves to become
both more of an author of their own lives and also connected to group identity. Work by Maruna
(2001), Adler et al. (2008) and Lysaker et al. (2007) suggest that people with considerable handicaps
and distress can and do change their narratives of themselves with therapeutic help. Lysakerswork
with people with psychosis in particular resonates with Val Jacksons description of the work of open
dialogue. My colleagues from Broadmoor and I have been privileged to observe how attachment to a
group can facilitate subtle but important changes of identity that are crucial for people who are both
victims and perpetrators of trauma (Adshead et al., 2015).
I also wonder if TCs offer people new opportunities to learn how both how to relate to care givers,
and how to give care to others. Insecure attachment disrupts both care giving and care eliciting
systems; learning how to give care to others is a crucial challenge to the delusional belief that
there are needy dependent people (called patients) and there are strong powerful people (called
doctorsor therapists) and these groups have nothing in common and can never know each
others worlds. The recent market-based narratives of health care, so beautifully described in
Penny Camplings paper, strongly suggest that the NHS is full of useless carers who have be
harassed into taking care of helpless patients who have simultaneously every choice and no
choice. Disorganisationof attachment is being mirrored in disorganisation of health care: which
we might expect, given that the NHS is a national attachment system for the regulation of care.
6. Conclusion
We all need a community to attach to. George Vaillants (2008) work on psychological defences
across the life span suggests that those who enjoy old age are those who belong to some sort of
group. TCs may be able to give people with insecure attachments a place to attach to, and create
new self-narratives. As we face a national election, and fears about international insecurity, we
may want to ask ourselves: what makes a secure group and community? What makes a secure
country and government? What I have taken away from my limited but valuable experience of TC
work is that the flattening of hierarchies is evidence of growing up; that there is a type of security
achieved when power is earned and shared; and that mature dependence entails respect and
support for vulnerability and power disparity. What we see at present, at the highest level of
government, is a contempt for vulnerability and need, and derogation of health care provision and
carers, which in turn suggests a type of mass insecurity at the political level. Perhaps it is always so.
I was struck that several of my co-presenters used horticultural metaphors to describe what
works in a TC, and the contrast between a horticultural model of mind and a mechanistic.
It reminded me of an image that was sent to me by a friend who found it on the internet,
unattributed and unsourced. Its message was simple, but profound: we come from the earth, we
return to the earth; and in between, we garden.
PAGE 18
j
THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES: THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES
j
VOL. 36 NO. 1 2015
References
Adler, J.M., Skalina, L.M. and McAdams, D.P. (2008), The narrative reconstruction of psychotherapy and
psychological health,Psychotherapy Research, Vol. 18 No. 6, pp. 719-34.
Adshead, G. (1998), Psychiatric staff as attachment figures. Understanding management problems
in psychiatric services in the light of attachment theory,The British Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 172 No. 1,
pp. 64-9.
Adshead, G., Ferrito, M. and Bose, S. (2015), Recovery after homicide narrative shifts in therapy with
homicide perpetrators,Criminal Justice and Behavior, Vol. 42 No. 1, pp. 70-81.
Aiello, L.C. and Dunbar, R.I. (1993), Neocortex size, group size, and the evolution of language,Current
Anthropology, Vol. 34 No. 2, pp. 184-93.
Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E. and Wall, S. (2014), Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological
Study of the Strange Situation, Psychology Press, New York, NY.
Allen, J. and Fonagy, P.P. (Eds) (2006), Handbook of Mentalisation-Based Treatment, Guilford,
New York, NY.
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. and van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2009), The first 10,000 adult attachment
interviews: distributions of adult attachment representations in clinical and non-clinical groups,Attachment &
Human Development, Vol. 11 No. 3, pp. 223-63.
Berry, K., Barrowclough, C. and Wearden, A. (2007), A review of the role of adult attachment style in
psychosis: unexplored issues and questions for further research,Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 27 No. 4,
pp. 458-75.
Damasio, A.R., Everitt, B.J. and Bishop, D. (1996), The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible
functions of the prefrontal cortex (and discussion),Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:
Biological Sciences,Vol. 351 No. 1346, pp. 1413-20.
Dunbar, R.I. (1993), Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans,Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, Vol. 16 No. 4, pp. 681-94.
Dunbar, R.I. (1995), Neocortex size and group size in primates: a test of the hypothesis,Journal of Human
Evolution, Vol. 28 No. 3, pp. 287-96.
Dunbar, R.I. (2003), The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective,Annual
Review of Anthropology, Vol. 34, pp. 163-81.
Fonagy, P. and Adshead, G. (2012), How mentalisation changes the mind,Advances in Psychiatric
Treatment, Vol. 18 No. 5, pp. 353-62.
Frith, C.D. (2007), The social brain?,Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
Vol. 362 No. 1480, pp. 671-8.
Henderson, S. (1974), Care-eliciting behavior in man,The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, Vol. 159
No. 3, pp. 172-81.
Holmes, J. (1993), John Bowlby and Attachment Theory, Routledge, London.
Kaplow, J.B. and Layne, C.M. (2014), Sudden loss and psychiatric disorders across the life course: toward a
developmental lifespan theory of bereavement-related risk and resilience,American Journal of Psychiatry,
Vol. 171 No. 8, pp. 807-10.
Kessler, R.C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M. and Nelson, C.B. (1995), Posttraumatic stress disorder
in the national comorbidity survey,Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 52 No. 12, pp. 1048-60.
Levy, K.N., Meehan, K.B., Kelly, K.M., Reynoso, J.S., Weber, M., Clarkin, J.F. and Kernberg, O.F. (2006),
Change in attachment patterns and reflective function in a randomized control trial of transference-focused
psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder,Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Vol. 74
No. 6, p. 1027.
Liotti, G. (2007), Internal working models of attachment in the therapeutic relationship,The Therapeutic
Relationship in the Cognitive Behavioral Psychotherapies, pp. 143-61.
Lyons-Ruth, K., Melnick, S., Patrick, M. and Hobson, R.P. (2007), A controlled study of hostile-helpless
states of mind among borderline and dysthymic women,Attachment & Human Development, Vol. 9 No. 1,
pp. 1-16.
VOL. 36 NO. 1 2015
j
THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES: THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES
j
PAGE 19
Lysaker, P.H., Dimaggio, G., Buck, K.D., Carcione, A. and Nicolò, G. (2007), Metacognition within narratives
of schizophrenia: associations with multiple domains of neurocognition,Schizophrenia Research, Vol. 93
No. 1, pp. 278-87.
Maruna, S. (2001), Making Good, American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Maunder, R.G. and Hunter, J.J. (2008), Attachment relationships as determinants of physical health,Journal
of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis and Dynamic Psychiatry, Vol. 36 No. 1, pp. 11-32.
Murphy, A., Steele, M., Dube, S., Bate, J., Bonuck, K., Meissner, P., Goldman, H. and Steele, H. (2014),
Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) questionnaire and adult attachment interview (AAI): implications for
parent child relationships,Child Abuse & Neglect, Vol. 38 No. 2, pp. 224-33.
Pfafflin, F. and Adshead, G. (Eds) (2003), A Matter of Security: The Application of Attachment Theory to
Forensic Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Vol. 25, Jessica Kingsley Publishers, London.
Pickard, H. (forthcoming), Stories of recovery: the role of narrative and hope in overcoming PTSD and PD,
in Fulford, K.W.M. et al. (Eds), Oxford Handbook of Psychiatric Ethics.
Read, J. and Gumley, A. (2008), Can attachment theory help explain the relationship between childhood
adversity and psychosis?,Attachment: New Directions in Psychotherapy and Relational Psychoanalysis,
Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 1-35.
Schore, A.N. (2002), Dysregulation of the right brain: a fundamental mechanism of traumatic attachment and
the psychopathogenesis of posttraumatic stress disorder,Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
Vol. 36 No. 1, pp. 9-30.
Vaillant, G.E. (2008), Aging Well: Surprising Guideposts to a Happier Life from the Landmark Study of Adult
Development, Little, Brown, London.
Van Ijzendoorn, M.H. and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. (2006), DRD4 7-repeat polymorphism moderates
the association between maternal unresolved loss or trauma and infant disorganization,Attachment &
Human Development, Vol. 8 No. 4, pp. 291-307.
Weinfield, N.S., Sroufe, L.A. and Egeland, B. (2000), Attachment from infancy to early adulthood in a highrisk
sample: continuity, discontinuity, and their correlates,Child Development, Vol. 71 No. 3, pp. 695-702.
Wilkinson, R.G. and Marmot, M.G. (Eds) (2003), Social Determinants of Health: The Solid Facts, World Health
Organization, Denmark.
Winnicott, D.W. (1949), Hate in the countertransference,International Journal of Psychoanalysis, Vol. 30
No. 2, pp. 69-74.
About the author
Dr Gwen Adshead is a Forensic Psychiatrist and Psychotherapist. She is also a qualified member
of the Institute of Group Analysis. She worked at Broadmoor Hospital for nearly 20 years and now
works in a medium secure unit with men who have committed acts of violence when mentally
unwell. She has a long standing interest in attachment theory applied to offenders and violence
perpetrators. Dr Gwen Adshead can be contacted at: gwen@gwenadshead.org
For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
PAGE 20
j
THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES: THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPEUTIC COMMUNITIES
j
VOL. 36 NO. 1 2015
... A TC is a hierarchically structured environment designed to facilitate behaviour change in the context of community living, whereby the community is the primary therapeutic instrument (Adshead, 2015;DeLeon, 2000;Veale et al., 2015). The model is designed to provide the individual with a physically and psychologically safe environment in which they are supported in a holistic recovery. ...
... housing, meals, clothing and medical care) are met, and the community structure aims to recreate the main characteristics of a prosocial and positive family environment, such as boundaries, routine, acceptance and encouragement (DeLeon, 2000). Through constructive feedback from TC staff and peers, time in the TC is intended to provide the corrective emotional experiences required to enable the individual to develop self-understanding and self-esteem, and to acquire skills in adaptive and respectful interpersonal functioning (Adshead, 2015;Warren et al., 2012) to facilitate the goal of long-term abstinence. ...
Article
Purpose The treatment of severe and chronic substance dependence is challenged by high rates of treatment attrition, highlighting the need to identify factors that hinder treatment retention. Design/methodology/approach The present study examined certain neurocognitive and personality traits in relation to treatment retention in a sample of 46 residents of an Australian Therapeutic Community (TC). The traits examined were previously found to be associated with problematic substance use in non-clinical samples and were also previously shown to differentiate TC clients from social drinkers.The hypothesis was thus that traits that appear to be risk factors for addictions are also likely to impact on TC treatment retention. Findings Group comparisons of those retained for more than the recommended 90 days versus those who left treatment prematurely showed that after controlling for the influence of depression, those who left treatment prematurely reported significantly higher levels of trait impulsivity, punishment sensitivity, and executive cognitive dysfunction. There was a very high rate of alexithymia in the sample (52%), but alexithymia was unrelated to retention. Research limitations/implications The final sample size was less than planned but reflected the strict participation criteria and temporal limitations of this study. No statistical assumptions were violated and the reliability indices of the scales completed by clients ranged from acceptable to excellent. Another limitation was that dropout cannot be assumed to mean relapse, as the reasons for client dropout were not available. Originality/value Findings highlight the important roles of trait factors in TC treatment retention in addition to the motivational and interpersonal factors identified in previous work.
... These findings suggest that attention to interpersonal difficulties and close relationships may be an important focus of therapy in cases that are at risk of poor response to treatment (Probst et al., 2020). Moreover, the psychotherapy literature insists that social support is critical in helping traumatized people, and that psychotherapy sessions in themselves, while useful, are not always sufficient for substantial improvement, especially if therapists are not working with a broad and flexible approach (Adshead, 2015;G. Holmes, 2012;Grossman et al., 2017;Schnyder et al., 2015;Sweeney & Taggart, 2018;Van der Kolk, 2014;Yehuda et al., 2016). ...
Article
In our paper we discuss the therapeutic dilemma that arises when relational patterns and attachment bonding are based exclusively on positive transference. When the harmful absence of negative transference is overlooked, rage and aggression become sequestered in the unconscious field. It is also well understood in the broader psychoanalytic community that conflict and aggression, when experienced, acknowledged, and processed inside a safe therapeutic relationship, is a powerful antidote to helplessness and hopelessness. We also suggest that an overemphasis on the impact of trauma may run the risk of making it the “essence” of the therapy, unconsciously encouraging clients to feel nailed to their wounds, as if they have completely lost their capacity for self-agency. This is the story of Claire, a young woman with severe and chronic relational trauma with whom Adriano worked for six years. When both Adriano and Claire suffered a loss of faith and the therapy became stymied by its limitations in providing an answer to this dilemma, Adriano called upon the philosophy of Hegel, whose valuing of the restorative power of “tarrying” with despair gave him support and comfort as he struggled with his own helplessness and fear in the face of Claire’s abject darkness. As a footnote, we are happy to report that at the time of this paper’s publication, Claire is now a practising psychologist and has been married to her husband for three years.
... It is widely acknowledged that attachment-related deficits can negatively affect relationships over the individual's lifespan and can shape later risk for offending (Ogilvie et al., 2014;Rich, 2006). Adshead (2015) asserted that TCs can act as a place of attachment for those who have developed maladaptive coping styles, where they can learn to develop healthy social relationships. TCs facilitate a culture of belonging and support the establishment of social relationships with peers and staff in which individuals can evaluate maladaptive beliefs affecting how they relate to the self and others (Haigh, 2002). ...
Chapter
This chapter provides a succinct review of the literature demonstrating that the general mental health literature has largely integrated the principles of positive psychology and that the importance of including strengths is firmly entrenched in mental health as standard, evidence‐based practice. It considers the importance of integrating strength‐based approaches as well as the alignment of existing models (such as RNR) to facilitate desistance from aggression and crime. The Safewards Model, Recovery Model, Reasoning and Rehabilitation, Enhanced Thinking Skills, Tidal Model, Therapeutic Communities, and Changing Lives and Changing Outcomes are outlined. In addition, Promising practices such as art therapy, mindfulness, meditation and yoga are reviewed. The chapter concludes that there is a growing body of solid research evidence to support the use of strength‐based approaches for the treatment of offenders with mental illness.
... The problem is the kind of outcomes clients are reporting. I encourage people in managing their pain more than getting rid of it because I think that the way people live with pain has a critical role in human balance (Adshead, 2015), while other colleagues might be more inclined to suggest that clients look ahead and think positive (Lomas & Ivtzan, 2016). This is especially clear with complex trauma, where people rebuild the painful narrative (Grossman et al., 2017) and "acknowledge, experience, and bear the reality of life" (Van der Kolk, 2014, p. 25). ...
Article
Full-text available
To help people own who they are, to get to their emotional basics, hold what they feel and be able to think even under the heaviest interpersonal pressure—this is my first and main therapeutic goal. The social consequences are that emotionally skillful people stop ignoring the inconsistencies they perceive in relationships; they ask for clarification once they no longer accept that their signaling is dismissed as “wrong” behavior. Their asking gives rise to fair conflicts, but a fair conflict is disrupting, because it involves a lot of bilateral exploration about established assumptions: relationships constantly exceed reached conclusions, if people have the courage to look at them with a fresh eye. This rupture-repair process is in turn based upon a degree of incongruence in human interactions as a source of endemic conflict: the mind is growing through its bridging of interactive gaps. Bearing the impact of full-range emotions arising from rupture-and-repair is anything but a comfortable societal ritual
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Long-term retention is a reliably well-studied factor associated with enhanced outcomes in addiction therapeutic communities (ATCs). Staying no less than three months is considered to be a critical time for program effectiveness. I plan to estimate retention rates of Saudi AATCs for three months, completion of therapy (stay at least six months), and early abandonment and investigate its correlations in this study. Methods A cohort retrospective study where data of all residents admitted to all Saudi ATCs since their establishment in 2000 through September 2014 were collected from their AATCs files. At the time of the study, there were five AATCs, two of them in Dammam, one in Riyadh, one in Jeddah, and the fifth one was in Taif. Date of admission, date of discharge, socio-demographic variable, and type of drug used of all of the five ACS were reported. Retention rate at 3 and 6 months and dropout in the first week were calculated. Results out of the 2050 files, 2003 of data was suitable for analysis. All of the residents were male adults. More than two-thirds of patients were younger than 40 years of age and most of them were singles (64%), unemployed (68%), and had intermediate or secondary school education (73%). Forty-six percent of patients reported Opioids use, 36% hash, 34% amphetamine, and 19% reported alcohol abuse. The retention rate for three months and six months was 45% and 28% respectively, and 8.3% dropped outs in the first week. The median duration of stay was 77 days. Residences in TC 1, TC 2, and TC 4 were less likely to stay for more than three months. Unemployment, and being students was associated with completion of treatment (stay > 6 months), while admission to TC 1 and TC-2 was associated with drop out prior completing the treatment program. Conclusion Three-month retention rates of 45% in Saudi addiction ATCs is reasonable and consistent with reported rates worldwide. In addition, treatment completion rate and drop out within the first week are at least comparable to the average rates recorded elsewhere. These rates can be considered as indicators for successful Saudi ATCs programs. However, there have been substantial variation between the different Saudi AATCs which may require further exploration to determine the factors related to these disparities.
Article
Full-text available
This review examines the perceptual and behavioural influences that pet-attachment has on the ways in which owners view risk, appraise threat, and respond to environmental hazards. Understanding how human-companion animal relationships function in this context has profound implications for the welfare of both people and their animals. Despite originating from human-attachment models, current perspectives on relationships with companion animals commonly adopt a unidimensional view of pet-attachment as a singular bond. This bypasses important aspects of attachment, ignoring the diversity evident in these relationships and, consequently, differences in risk processes. We argue that by adopting a pet-attachment ‘communities’ model that more closely approximates human-attachment theory, a nuanced understanding of perceptual and behavioural risk propensities that distinguishes between different types of ‘stronger’ and ‘weaker’ or insecure attachments can be achieved. We consider how research regarding pet- and human-attachment can be used to identify potential communities of the pet-attached. A community perspective upon pet-owner risk propensity will contribute to a social-ecological understanding of these relationships as potential protective factors when confronting environmental threats. Finally, we propose that future research relating to pet-attachment can benefit from current human-attachment findings regarding the wider social nature of attachment relationships.
Article
Full-text available
A key theme in mental health is the principle of recovery. However, it is not clear how this might apply to forensic mental health services, which offer mental health care to men and women who have offended when mentally unwell. In this article, we explore how discussion of the index offense fits into recovery paradigms and how reflection on offender identity relates to recovery. Using clinical material from therapy groups for homicide perpetrators, we discuss how narratives of agency and responsibility change (or not) in therapy, and how narrative shifts link with the concept of “recovery” in mental health. © 2014 International Association for Correctional and Forensic Psychology.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we offer an account of how various psychological therapies address dysfunctional mentalisation and identify pitfalls of therapy that could lead to an impasse or even to negative consequences associated with psychological treatment. Some practical recommendations follow from our model, particularly in relation to the careful monitoring of the intensity of the patient's attachment and the use of interventions aimed at promoting mentalising.
Article
Full-text available
Although Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) are linked to increased health problems and risk behaviors in adulthood, there are no studies on the association between ACEs and adults' states of mind regarding their early childhood attachments, loss, and trauma experiences. To validate the ACEs questions, we analyzed the association between ACEs and emotional support indicators and Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) classifications in terms of unresolved mourning regarding past loss or trauma and discordant states of mind in cannot classify (U/CC) interviews. Seventy-five urban women (41 clinical and 34 community) completed a questionnaire on ACEs, which included 10 categories of abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction, in addition to emotional support. Internal psychological processes or states of mind concerning attachment were assessed using the AAI. ACE responses were internally consistent (Cronbach's α=.88). In the clinical sample, 84% reported≥4 ACEs compared to 27% among the community sample. AAIs judged U/CC occurred in 76% of the clinical sample compared to 9% in the community sample. When ACEs were≥4, 65% of AAIs were classified U/CC. Absence of emotional support in the ACEs questionnaire was associated with 72% of AAIs being classified U/CC. As the number of ACEs and the lack of emotional support increases so too does the probability of AAIs being classified as U/CC. Findings provide rationale for including ACEs questions in pediatric screening protocols to identify and offer treatment reducing the intergenerational transmission of risk associated with problematic parenting.
Book
Pioneering research has been carried out over the last decade on mentalization and the promotion of mentalizing capacity - the ability to interpret the behavior of oneself and others as based on intentional mental states, such as needs, desires, feelings, and beliefs. This book is a consolidation of current knowledge and clinical applications, bringing together a group of international experts who have been on the ground floor of theory and research to clarify the concept, review pertinent neurobiological and psychosocial research, and explore its diverse clinical applications. Four sections will cover Conceptual Foundations, Developmental Psychopathology, Intervention and Prevention. A biopsychosocial approach will be used, integrating new research in neuroimaging with psychodynamic and cognitive perspectives. Clinical issues covered will include parent-child interactions, personality disorders, traumatic brain injury, bullying and at-risk children.
Article
Background: Data were obtained on the general population epidemiology of DSM-III-R posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), including information on estimated lifetime prevalence, the kinds of traumas most often associated with PTSD, sociodemographic correlates, the comorbidity of PTSD with other lifetime psychiatric disorders, and the duration of an index episode.Methods: Modified versions of the DSM-III-R PTSD module from the Diagnostic Interview Schedule and of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview were administered to a representative national sample of 5877 persons aged 15 to 54 years in the part II subsample of the National Comorbidity Survey.Results: The estimated lifetime prevalence of PTSD is 7.8%. Prevalence is elevated among women and the previously married. The traumas most commonly associated with PTSD are combat exposure and witnessing among men and rape and sexual molestation among women. Posttraumatic stress disorder is strongly comorbid with other lifetime DSM-III-R disorders. Survival analysis shows that more than one third of people with an index episode of PTSD fail to recover even after many years.Conclusions: Posttraumatic stress disorder is more prevalent than previously believed, and is often persistent. Progress in estimating age-at-onset distributions, cohort effects, and the conditional probabilities of PTSD from different types of trauma will require future epidemiologic studies to assess PTSD for all lifetime traumas rather than for only a small number of retrospectively reported "most serious" traumas.