ArticlePublisher preview available

Saying "No" to Temptation: Want-to Motivation Improves Self-Regulation by Reducing Temptation Rather Than by Increasing Self-Control

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Self-regulation has been conceptualized as the interplay between controlled and impulsive processes; however, most research has focused on the controlled side (i.e., effortful self-control). The present studies focus on the effects of motivation on impulsive processes, including automatic preferences for goal-disruptive stimuli and subjective reports of temptations and obstacles, contrasting them with effects on controlled processes. This is done by examining people's implicit affective reactions in the face of goal-disruptive "temptations" (Studies 1 and 2), subjective reports of obstacles (Studies 2 and 3) and expended effort (Study 3), as well as experiences of desires and self-control in real-time using experience sampling (Study 4). Across these multiple methods, results show that want-to motivation results in decreased impulsive attraction to goal-disruptive temptations and is related to encountering fewer obstacles in the process of goal pursuit. This, in turn, explains why want-to goals are more likely to be attained. Have-to motivation, on the other hand, was unrelated to people's automatic reactions to temptation cues but related to greater subjective perceptions of obstacles and tempting desires. The discussion focuses on the implications of these findings for self-regulation and motivation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved).
Saying “No” to Temptation: Want-to Motivation Improves Self-Regulation
by Reducing Temptation Rather Than by Increasing Self-Control
Marina Milyavskaya
McGill University
Michael Inzlicht
University of Toronto
Nora Hope and Richard Koestner
McGill University
Self-regulation has been conceptualized as the interplay between controlled and impulsive processes; how-
ever, most research has focused on the controlled side (i.e., effortful self-control). The present studies focus
on the effects of motivation on impulsive processes, including automatic preferences for goal-disruptive
stimuli and subjective reports of temptations and obstacles, contrasting them with effects on controlled
processes. This is done by examining people’s implicit affective reactions in the face of goal-disruptive
“temptations” (Studies 1 and 2), subjective reports of obstacles (Studies 2 and 3) and expended effort (Study
3), as well as experiences of desires and self-control in real-time using experience sampling (Study 4). Across
these multiple methods, results show that want-to motivation results in decreased impulsive attraction to
goal-disruptive temptations and is related to encountering fewer obstacles in the process of goal pursuit. This,
in turn, explains why want-to goals are more likely to be attained. Have-to motivation, on the other hand, was
unrelated to people’s automatic reactions to temptation cues but related to greater subjective perceptions of
obstacles and tempting desires. The discussion focuses on the implications of these findings for self-regulation
and motivation.
Keywords: self-regulation, goal motivation, self-control, temptations, goal pursuit
Self-regulation plays a key role in many domains in our life, from
maintaining a healthy diet to being productive at work to resisting
getting angry at your spouse after a tiring day. Indeed, many of
today’s societal ills are attributed to poor self-regulation— eating too
much junk food, smoking, drugs, corruption, and violence are only
some of the problems that (at least according to popular thought)
would disappear if only people were better at controlling their im-
pulses. Attempts at self-regulation typically involve setting a specific
goal to pursue: “Lose 10 lbs.,” “write two hours per day,” “spend
more time with my kids,” or “quit smoking.” However, although such
goals can help people in guiding their actions, they are by no means
a guarantee of successful self-regulation.
Typically when people talk about self-regulation, they usually
mean “self-control,” which can be defined as the effortful inhibi-
tion of impulses or the overcoming of temptations (but see Fujita,
2011, for an alternate definition). Thus, self-regulation is often
thought to rely exclusively on the application of effort. However,
we prefer to use a broader definition of self-regulation that focuses
on goal-directed behavior in general (Carver & Scheier, 1982,
2011). Specifically, we use “self-regulation” as a broad term
referring to all manner of goal pursuit, which can include both
effortful control of behavior, but also effortless, automatic, or
habitual forms of goal directed behavior (de Ridder, Lensvelt-
Mulders, Finkernauer, Stok, & Baumeister, 2012;Fujita, 2011). As
such, self-regulation often consists of balancing self-control efforts
against the presence and strength of temptations and impulses that
can distract or impede one’s goal pursuits and must be effortfully
overcome. Although as important as self-control, this second “im-
pulse modulation” component of self-regulation is frequently over-
looked. How frequent and strong is the craving for the cigarette or
for the chocolate cake? Furthermore, could the strength and fre-
quency of these cravings depend on how people construe their
self-regulatory goals? In this article we investigate this question,
looking at the role of goal motivation on the impulsive and
effortful components of self-regulation.
Dual Models of Self-Regulation
The process of self-regulation can be thought of as a seesaw. On
one side are the impulses and desires—reaching for a chocolate,
craving a cigarette, sneaking a peak at Facebook. On the other side are
all the reasons to resist these desires—wanting to lose weight, trying
to quit smoking, trying to get work done; these are typically demar-
cated by the long-term goals that may be jeopardized by giving in to
This article was published Online First May 18, 2015.
Marina Milyavskaya, Department of Educational and Counselling Psy-
chology, McGill University; Michael Inzlicht, Department of Psychology
and Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto; Nora Hope
and Richard Koestner, Department of Psychology, McGill University.
As of July 1, 2015, Marina Milyavskaya will be affiliated with the
Department of Psychology, Carleton University.
This research was supported in part by grants from the Social Sciences
and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) to Marina Milyavs-
kaya and Richard Koestner.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marina
Milyavskaya, Department of Psychology, Carleton University, 550 Loeb
Building, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, ON, Canada, K1S 5B6. E-mail:
marina.milyavskaya@carleton.ca
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2015 American Psychological Association
2015, Vol. 109, No. 4, 677–693 0022-3514/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000045
677
... Zasobem pracownika z HP jest motywacja wewnętrzna. Milyavskaya, Inzlicht, Hope i Koestner (2015) podjęli problem zależności pomiędzy motywacją a samoregulacją (umiejętnym regulowaniem poziomu pobudzenia i odbudowywania zasobów energii). Wskazali, że motywacja wewnętrzna może chronić przed postrzeganiem przeszkód w działaniu, sprzyjając tym samym samoregulacji, co w efekcie wiąże się z mniejszym poczuciem wyczerpania (Milyavskaya i in., 2015). ...
... Milyavskaya, Inzlicht, Hope i Koestner (2015) podjęli problem zależności pomiędzy motywacją a samoregulacją (umiejętnym regulowaniem poziomu pobudzenia i odbudowywania zasobów energii). Wskazali, że motywacja wewnętrzna może chronić przed postrzeganiem przeszkód w działaniu, sprzyjając tym samym samoregulacji, co w efekcie wiąże się z mniejszym poczuciem wyczerpania (Milyavskaya i in., 2015). Z kolei motywacja zewnętrzna, specyficzna dla OP, była związana z postrzeganiem większej liczby przeszkód w dążeniu do celu czy sprzecznych pragnień. ...
... Z kolei motywacja zewnętrzna, specyficzna dla OP, była związana z postrzeganiem większej liczby przeszkód w dążeniu do celu czy sprzecznych pragnień. Prowadziła do poczucia wydatkowania energii oraz przekonania o towarzyszących zwiększonych wyzwaniach i przeszkodach w dążeniu do celu, które nie przynoszą korzyści (Milyavskaya i in., 2015). Ponadto Milyavskaya, Galla, Inzlicht i Duckworth (2018) dowiedli, że angażowanie się w aktywność, która stanowiła zainteresowanie osoby, pojęcie wykazujące konceptualne podobieństwo z pasją, przewidywało zmniejszenie uczucia zmęczenia, nawet po samym zaangażowaniu. ...
... The second hypothesis (H2) suggests that higher self-regulation skills reduce distracted driving (Milyavskaya et al., 2015). To account for this, we add SRS as an independent variable: ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: This study examines the impact of Mobile Phone Use (MPU) on Distracted Driving (DD) and the moderating role of Self-Regulation Skills (SRS) among drivers. With the increasing use of smartphones, distracted driving has become a critical issue, contributing to numerous road accidents. This research aims to explore how MPU affects DD and how SRS can mitigate these risks. Methods: A cross-sectional survey was conducted with 220 participants aged 15-58, including both professional and non-professional drivers. Data were collected through questionnaires measuring MPU frequency, self-regulation skills, and self-reported instances of DD. Statistical analyses, including regression and ANOVA, were used to test the hypotheses. Results: The findings show a significant positive relationship between MPU and DD, with a coefficient of -0.699 (p < 0.001). Self-regulation skills significantly moderated this relationship, reducing the impact of MPU on DD (coefficient = 0.304, p < 0.001). Differences in distraction levels were observed based on gender and vehicle type, with males and four-wheeler drivers exhibiting higher distraction rates. These findings highlight the importance of targeted therapies that account for demographic differences in distraction susceptibility. Conclusion: The study concludes that MPU is a key contributor to distracted driving, but strong self-regulation skills can help mitigate its negative effects. These results highlight the need for targeted awareness campaigns, self-regulation training, and stricter policies to enhance road safety. Ultimately, the study emphasizes the significance of improving self-regulation skills, conducting public awareness efforts, enforcing stronger policies to reduce MPU-related distractions, and improving road safety.
... Moreover, for real-life goal progress, motivational processes may be more important than the level of cognitive control. If individuals are intrinsically motivated for a task, they may be less tempted by conflicting goals (Milyavskaya et al., 2015;Werner & Milyavskaya, 2019;Inzlicht et al., 2021). It must be noted, however, that associations might be stronger in samples with higher levels of cognitive control problems such as clinical samples with addictive behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
Following the (revised) latent state–trait theory, the present study investigates the within-subject reliability, occasion specificity, common consistency, and construct validity of cognitive control measures in an intensive longitudinal design. These indices were calculated applying dynamic structural equation modeling while accounting for autoregressive effects and trait change. In two studies, participants completed two cognitive control tasks (Stroop and go/no-go) and answered questions about goal pursuit, self-control, executive functions, and situational aspects, multiple times per day. The sample (aged 18–30 years in both studies) consisted of 21 participants (14 female) in the pilot study and 70 participants (48 female) in the main study. Findings indicated poor within-subject reliability for the Stroop task error rate and reaction time difference between congruent and incongruent trials and moderate to good within-subject reliability for the go/no-go task error rate and reaction time. Occasion specificity—the systematic variance accounted for by state residuals—was at a modest level (between 1.4% and 11.1%) for the Stroop error rate and reaction time difference, and at a moderate level (between 16.1% and 37.2% for the go/no-go error rate and reaction time) in the two studies. Common consistency—the variance accounted for by latent trait variables—was at a moderate to high level for all of the investigated scores. Indicative of construct validity, the Stroop and go/no-go task error rates correlated positively with each other on the within- and between-subject level. Within-subject correlations between task scores and subjective self-control measures were very small and mostly nonsignificant.
... Agency is a core aspect of the self (Baumeister, 2010), and greater agency has been linked to enhanced productivity, more relationship success, less internalizing symptoms (e.g., depression), better interpersonal skills, and reduced reliance on self-defeating behaviors, such as alcoholism or gambling (Baumeister & Tierney, 2012;Moffitt et al., 2011;Tangney et al., 2004). Constructs linked to agency (e.g., self-control, self-discipline, or, more generally, conscientiousness) appear to instill the rewarding feelings of intrinsic motivation or engaging in action because of one's own wants (Converse et al., 2019;Milyavskaya et al., 2015;Thomaes et al., 2017), which may be central to happiness (Ryan & Deci, 2000). ...
Article
Full-text available
Liberals tend to indicate being less happy than conservatives; although one perspective suggests this happiness gap reflects liberals’ enhanced concern over the systemic maltreatment of others, the “maladjustment perspective” suggests it reflects, in part, liberals’ tendencies to reject aspects of conservatism that support agency and produce some life benefits. Some evidence seems to favor the maladjustment perspective; but, here, we sought to build on this perspective by examining its predictions in the context of Left-wing Authoritarianism (LWA). LWA is positively associated with liberalism, but, relative to liberalism, it encompasses aspects more rejecting of conservatism. US adults (N = 446) completed measures of LWA, happiness, agency, life impairment, and antagonistic personality constructs (e.g., sadism). The data supported the maladjustment perspective in the context of LWA: After accounting for liberalism (and demographics inclusive of age and sex), LWA was negatively related to happiness and positively related to agency deficiencies and life impairments; the latter two characteristics (reflective of maladjustment) completely accounted for LWA’s negative relation to happiness. These findings were generally consistent across LWA’s three facets. LWA also had a positive relationship to antagonistic personality constructs; hence, LWA is probably not conducive to enhanced concern over the systemic maltreatment of others. Additional analyses revealed that the anti-conventional facet of LWA completely accounted for relationships between liberalism and happiness, agency deficiencies, and life impairments. The results offer some novel insight into the ideological happiness gap.
... In terms of self-motivation, this means that people prefer to do what they want to do, and are not overly enthusiastic about following rules they are forced to or must follow. The idea that people with high self-motivation are more likely to follow rules even in stressful situations, is supported by both, classical views about human motivation [35] and also by more recent research [36]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
We discuss the negative impact of high levels of stress on a person's cognitive situation awareness and management capability. This is a critical aspect to consider when designing people-centered Cyber-Physical-Social Systems (CPSS). High levels of stress can negatively affect how people perceive and interpret information, resulting in an inability to understand the situation adequately and inadequate decision-making. The paper highlights that high levels of stress can also make it challenging for people to follow rules and regulations in crises situations. We discuss several aspects of how CPSS could help people in crisis situations to better follow rules and regulations.
... Whereas these kind of goals are important for all adolescents, albeit in varying degree, they are not always within immediate reach as goals by definition signal a discrepancy between one's current state and a desired future state (Carver & Scheier, 1998;Inzlicht et al., 2021). Making progress toward one's personal goals is important for well-being (De Ridder & Gillebaart, 2017;Sheldon & Elliot, 1999) and the employment of self-regulatory strategies may have beneficial effects on well-being through their positive effect on goal progress (Brunstein, 1993;Mann et al., 2013;Milyavskaya et al., 2015). Self-regulatory strategies can be defined as the 'means through which individuals, in order to help themselves achieve their goals, actively alter their cognitive, motivational, affective, or behavioral reactions to a self-regulatory challenge' (Hennecke et al., 2019). ...
Article
Purpose Generation Z (GenZers) significantly influences sustainability-related issues in emerging economies like India. Literature on sustainability suggests exploring ways to enhance their sustainable behavior, especially for food and clothing products, which have detrimental consequences despite being essential. Mindfulness is often proposed as a factor that could potentially influence sustainability. However, the empirical association between mindfulness and sustainable consumption behavior (SCB) and the underlying pathways remain underexplored. Therefore, this study aims to advance the empirical understanding of how GenZers’ trait mindfulness affects their SCB for food and clothing. Design/methodology/approach Based on a time-lagged study involving responses from 519 Indian GenZers (college students aged 18–24 years) the authors examined two models (one for SCB related to food and one for clothing) to explore the direct relationships and mediating factors between trait mindfulness and SCB. Findings GenZers with higher levels of trait mindfulness are more likely to influence their SCB for food and clothing. Besides, in both models, self-regulation, self-compassion and prosocialness mediate the association between trait mindfulness and SCB. Research limitations/implications Mindfulness and its intervening variables are promising for positively driving SCB for food and clothing. Marketers and policymakers can consider these findings to strategize and encourage sustainable consumption, particularly among GenZers. Originality/value This seminal study uses the reperceiving theory to empirically validate the relationship between GenZers’ trait mindfulness and SCB for food and clothing.
Article
Introduction Depressive symptoms, goal progress, and goal characteristics are interrelated, but the directionality of these relationships is unclear. Methods In a 6‐wave longitudinal study ( N = 431; 2002 total surveys), we examine the bidirectionality of the relationships between depressive symptoms, goal characteristics (commitment, self‐efficacy, and perception of other's support), and goal progress for academic and interpersonal goals at 2‐week intervals. Separate random‐intercept cross‐lagged panel models were tested for each goal characteristic across both goals. Results At the within‐person level, goal progress significantly positively predicted commitment, self‐efficacy, and perception of others' support for the goal. Most of the other hypothesized paths were nonsignificant, including paths between depressive symptoms and progress. At the between‐person level, all variables were significantly correlated, with some effects significantly larger for the interpersonal than the academic goal. Discussion The results suggest that when it comes to depressive symptoms and goal pursuit, general tendencies may be more important than variations over 2‐week intervals.
Article
Research on perfectionism and workplace procrastination has typically examined perfectionism through two dimensions: perfectionist strivings (PS) and perfectionist concerns (PC), which are inversely associated with procrastination. Recognizing that these two dimensions often coexist in varying degrees within individuals, our study conducted an interaction analysis to elucidate the associations between individual dimensions and their combined presence across four distinct perfectionism profiles. Based on self-determination theory, we examined the interaction between the two dimensions on procrastination through three psychological mediators: autonomous motivation, controlled motivation, and psychological distress. In a three-wave study involving 376 employees, we observed that the associations between PS and mediators vary with the level of PC, thus responding to the contentious debate of whether PS are beneficial. Additionally, we employed a three-way interaction analysis to explore how different profiles of perfectionism react under external performance pressure, particularly in relation to their levels of procrastination. As ambitious perfectionists (high PS, low PC) emerged as the optimal profile under stressors, our study further identified the particularly beneficial role of PS, especially in the presence of stressors. Our analyses shed new light on the relationships between trait perfectionism and workplace procrastination behavior. We discussed theoretical and practical implications.
Article
Full-text available
A neglected question regarding cognitive control is how control processes might detect situations calling for their involvement. The authors propose here that the demand for control may be evaluated in part by monitoring for conflicts in information processing. This hypothesis is supported by data concerning the anterior cingulate cortex, a brain area involved in cognitive control, which also appears to respond to the occurrence of conflict. The present article reports two computational modeling studies, serving to articulate the conflict monitoring hypothesis and examine its implications. The first study tests the sufficiency of the hypothesis to account for brain activation data, applying a measure of conflict to existing models of tasks shown to engage the anterior cingulate. The second study implements a feedback loop connecting conflict monitoring to cognitive control, using this to simulate a number of important behavioral phenomena.
Article
Full-text available
Two studies used the self-concordance model of healthy goal striving (K. M. Sheldon & A. J. Elliot, 1999) to examine the motivational processes by which people can increase their level of well-being during a period of time and then maintain the gain or perhaps increase it even further during the next period of time. In Study I, entering freshmen with self-concordant motivation better attained their 1st-semester goals, which in turn predicted increased adjustment and greater self-concordance for the next semester's goals. Increased self-concordance in turn predicted even better goal attainment during the 2nd semester, which led to further increases in adjustment and to higher levels of ego development by the end of the year. Study 2 replicated the basic model in a 2-week study of short-term goals set in the laboratory. Limits of the model and implications for the question of how (and whether) happiness may be increased are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Many psychological theories suggest a link between self-regulation and identity, but until now a mechanistic account that suggests ways to improve self-regulation has not been put forth. The identity-value model (IVM) connects the idea from social psychology, that aspects of identity such as core values and group affiliations hold positive subjective value, to the process-focused account from decision-making and behavioral economics, that self-regulation is driven by a dynamic value integration across a range of choice attributes. Together, these ideas imply that goal-directed behaviors that are identity-relevant are more likely to be enacted because they have greater subjective value than identity-irrelevant behaviors. A central hypothesis, therefore, is that interventions that increase the degree to which a target behavior is perceived as self-relevant will improve self-regulation. Additionally, identity-based changes in self-regulation are expected to be mediated by changes in subjective value and its underlying neural systems. In this paper, we define the key constructs relevant to the IVM, explicate the model and delineate its boundary conditions, and describe how it fits with related theories. We also review disparate results in the research literature that might share identity-related value as a common underlying mechanism of action. We close by discussing questions about the model whose answers could advance the study of self-regulation.
Article
Full-text available
People approach pleasure and avoid pain. To discover the true nature of approach–avoidance motivation, psychologists need to move beyond this hedonic principle to the principles that underlie the different ways that it operates. One such principle is regulatory focus, which distinguishes self-regulation with a promotion focus (accomplishments and aspirations) from self-regulation with a prevention focus (safety and responsibilities). This principle is used to reconsider the fundamental nature of approach–avoidance, expectancy–value relations, and emotional and evaluative sensitivities. Both types of regulatory focus are applied to phenonomena that have been treated in terms of either promotion (e.g., well-being) or prevention (e.g., cognitive dissonance). Then, regulatory focus is distinguished from regulatory anticipation and regulatory reference, 2 other principles underlying the different ways that people approach pleasure and avoid pain.
Article
Self-control is typically viewed as a key ingredient responsible for effective self-regulation and personal goal attainment. This study used experience sampling, daily diary, and prospective data collection to investigate the immediate and semester-long consequences of effortful self-control and temptations on depletion and goal attainment. Results showed that goal attainment was influenced by experiences of temptations rather than by actively resisting or controlling those temptations. This study also found that simply experiencing temptations led people to feel depleted. Depletion in turn mediated the link between temptations and goal attainment, such that people who experienced increased temptations felt more depleted and thus less likely to achieve their goals. Critically, results of Bayesian analyses strongly indicate that effortful self-control was consistently unrelated to goal attainment throughout all analyses.
Article
Although observers of human behavior have long been aware that people regularly struggle with internal conflict when deciding whether to behave responsibly or indulge in impulsivity, psychologists and economists did not begin to empirically investigate this type of want/should conflict until recently. In this article, we review and synthesize the latest research on want/should conflict, focusing our attention on the findings from an empirical literature on the topic that has blossomed over the last 15 years. We then turn to a discussion of how individuals and policy makers can use what has been learned about want/should conflict to help decision makers select far-sighted options. © 2008, Association for Psychological Science. All rights reserved.
Article
Six studies explore the role of goal shielding in self-regulation:by examining how the activation of focal goals to which the individual is committed inhibits the accessibility, of alternative goals. Consistent evidence was found for such goal shielding, and a number of its moderators were identified: Individuals' level of commitment to the focal goal, their degree of anxiety and depression, their need for cognitive closure, and differences in their goal-related tenacity. Moreover, inhibition of alternative goals was found to be, more pronounced when they serve the same overarching purpose as the focal goal, but lessened when the alternative goals facilitate focal goal attainment. Finally; goal shielding was shown to have beneficial consequences for goal pursuit and attainment.
Article
Intrinsic and extrinsic types of motivation have been widely studied, and the distinction between them has shed important light on both developmental and educational practices. In this review we revisit the classic definitions of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in light of contemporary research and theory. Intrinsic motivation remains an important construct, reflecting the natural human propensity to learn and assimilate. However, extrinsic motivation is argued to vary considerably in its relative autonomy and thus can either reflect external control or true self-regulation. The relations of both classes of motives to basic human needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness are discussed.