ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The social organization of mobile hunter-gatherers has several derived features, including low within-camp relatedness and fluid meta-groups. Although these features have been proposed to have provided the selective context for the evolution of human hypercooperation and cumulative culture, how such a distinctive social system may have emerged remains unclear. We present an agent-based model suggesting that, even if all individuals in a community seek to live with as many kin as possible, within-camp relatedness is reduced if men and women have equal influence in selecting camp members. Our model closely approximates observed patterns of co-residence among Agta and Mbendjele BaYaka hunter-gatherers. Our results suggest that pair-bonding and increased sex egalitarianism in human evolutionary history may have had a transformative effect on human social organization. Copyright © 2015, American Association for the Advancement of Science.
Content may be subject to copyright.
REPORT
HUMAN BEHAVIOR
Sex equality can explain the
unique social structure of
hunter-gatherer bands
M. Dyble,*G. D. Salali, N. Chaudhary, A. Page, D. Smith, J. Thompson, L. Vinicius,
R. Mace, A. B. Migliano
The social organization of mobile hunter-gatherers has several derived features,
including low within-camp relatedness and fluid meta-groups. Although these features
have been proposed to have provided the selective context for the evolution of human
hypercooperation and cumulative culture, how such a distinctive social system may have
emerged remains unclear. We present an agent-based model suggesting that, even if all
individuals in a community seek to live with as many kin as possible, within-camp
relatedness is reduced if men and women have equal influence in selecting camp members.
Our model closely approximates observed patterns of co-residence among Agta and
Mbendjele BaYaka hunter-gatherers. Our results suggest that pair-bonding and increased
sex egalitarianism in human evolutionary history may have had a transformative effect
on human social organization.
Contemporary mobile hunter-gatherers co-
operate extensively with unrelated individ-
uals across multiple social and economic
domains. Many communities of mobile
hunter-gatherers (hereafter hunter-gatherers)
share food extensively within camp and hunt,
gather, and fish cooperatively (1). Alloparenting
is also commonplace (2,3).Theimportanceof
cooperative activities is reflected in many hunter-
gatherer societies as a pervasive ethic of egalitarian-
ism (4,5). Like a number of non-human primate
species, humans live in multimale, multifemale
groups (6). However, we maintain enduring pair
bonds, resulting in what have been described as
multifamilygroups (7). In addition, and in con-
trast to the bounded and territorial groups of
chimpanzees (8,9), bonobos (10) andgorillas (11),
contemporary hunter-gatherers have fluid social
networks where family units are relatively auton-
omous, with couples and their children moving
often between bands (12), living with kin of
either the husband or the wife. This residence
pattern has been described as either bilocalor
multilocal(13).
As well as being highly mobile, contemporary
hunter-gatherer camps include a significant pro-
portion of unrelated individuals (14) and are less
closely related than groups of non-foraging small-
scale societies (15). Given the inclusive fitness
benefits of living with kin, why hunter-gatherers
live with unrelated individuals is a puzzle, even
more so if one considers that hunter-gatherers
show a preference for living with siblings (13)and
preferentially include kin in their campmate choices
and social networks (16). Therefore, the mecha-
nisms by which contemporary hunter-gatherers
attempt to maximize co-residence and cooperation
with kin, but nonetheless end up residing mostly
with unrelated individuals, remain unclear.
Here, we offer a solution for this apparent par-
adox by demonstrating that, even where all indi-
viduals are actively assorting with kin, within-group
relatedness is reduced if both sexes have influ-
ence over camp composition, as is the case among
egalitarian, multilocal hunter-gatherers. We present
a simulation of camp assortment where individ-
uals attempt to reside with as many kin as pos-
sible under two conditions. In the egalitarian
condition, men and women have equal influ-
ence on camp composition, whereas in the non-
egalitarian condition only one sex has influence.
We compared the results with previously unpub-
lished data from two hunter-gatherer groups, the
Palanan Agta (N= 4055 dyads) and Mbendjele
BaYaka (5)(N=1863dyads),aswellasonefarm-
ing population, the Paranan (N=1049dyads).
We demonstrate that low within-camp relatedness
emerges naturally from men and women seeking
to maximize the presence of related kin. In con-
trast, in societies where decision-making on co-
residence rests on one sex only, as in the case
of patrilocal farmers, low relatedness does not
emerge. Our model offers a mechanism that re-
conciles individual-level preferences for kin with
reduced camp-level relatedness. Assuming that
extant hunter-gatherers live in social structures
resembling the ones existing in past hominins,
our model explains how the shift from an an-
cestral hierarchical, female-dispersal system, to
a multilocal, egalitarian one would provide the
selective context for expanded social networks,
cumulative culture, and cooperation among un-
related individuals.
RESEARCH
SCIENCE sciencemag.org 00 MONTH 2015 VOL 000 ISSUE 0000 1
University College London (UCL) Anthropology, 14 Taviton
Street, London WC1H 0BW, UK.
*Corresponding author. E-mail: mark.dyble.12@ucl.ac.uk
Self
Primary
kin
Distant
kin
Spous
e
Spouse's
primary kin
Spouse's
distant kin
Primary
kin's
spouse
Distant
affines
No
relation
Self
Primary
kin
Distant
kin
Spouse
Spouse's
p
rimar
y
kin
Spouse's
distant
kin
Primary
kin's
spouse
Distant
affines
No
relation
Self
Primary
kin
Distant
kin
Spouse
Spouse's
primary
kin
Spouse's
distant kin
Primary
kin's
spouse
Distant
affines
No
relation
Self
Primary
kin
Distant
kin
Spouse
Spouse's
primary
kin
SDK
Primary
kin's
spouse
Distant
affines
No
relation
Self
Primary
kin
Distant
kin
Spouse
Spouse's
primary
kin
Spouse's
distant
kin
Primary
kin's
spouse
Distant
affines
No
relation
Fig. 1. Co-residence patterns across modeled and observed egalitarian populations. Chart area represents the proportion of all dyads across nine
categories of relatedness for the egalitarian model (left), Agta (middle left), Mbendjele (middle right), Ache (bottom right), and Ju/hoansi (top right). Ache
and Ju/hoansi data redrawn from Hill et al. (2011).
MS no: REaaa5139/CF/ANTHRO
Among the Agta, we collected data from 191
adults across 11 camps, coding a total of 4055
dyadic relationships. Among the Mbendjele, we
collected data from 103 adults across nine camps,
totaling 1863 dyadic relationships. Mean experi-
enced camp size was 18.09 adults (SD = 8.62) for
the Mbendjele and 21.23 adults (SD = 8.61) for
the Agta. Both populations were multilocal, with
husbands and wives living with similar numbers
of consanguineal (genetic) kin (table S1 and fig.
S1). In both groups, around 25% of dyads repre-
sented consanguineal kin, 25% were close affinal
kin, and around 50% of dyads were distant af-
final kin or unrelated individuals (
F1 Fig. 1 and
table S2). These results are similar to those re-
ported for the Ache and Ju/hoansi by Hill and
colleagues (14); see Fig. 1.
In contrast to the unbiased residence patterns
of the Agta and Mbendjele, Paranan farmers (n=
49 adults, 1049 dyads) demonstrate a significant
male bias in residence, with men living with a
larger number of primary kin (n= 23, mean =
2.65, SD = 2.29) than women (n=26,mean=
1.27, SD = 2.05; P=0.031).Despitehavingacom-
parable group size of 21.4 adults (SD = 9.30), the
Paranan live with fewer unrelated individuals
than the hunter-gatherers (4.2% versus 16.7%)
(c
2
=108.93,P< 0.001) (F2 Fig. 2).
Although it is possible that low within-camp
relatedness could result from random dispersal,
with households moving randomly between camps
and living with related individuals only by chance,
our results do not suggest that this is the case.
Rather, the observed frequency of primary kin
co-residence was significantly higher than would
be expected if individuals assorted randomly
across camps (Mbendjele, c
2
=451.62,P<0.001;
Agta, c
2
= 982.00, P< 0.001). Thus, hunter-
gatherer co-residence patterns are notable not
only in their low-relatedness but because this
low relatedness occurs despite the positive assort-
ment of kin.
We developed a model to understand how
hunter-gatherers come to co-reside with a large
number of unrelated individuals at the group
level, despite a preference toward living with kin
at the individual level. We ran two versions of the
model: one egalitarian, where both husband and
wife have equal influence over where their house-
hold resides, and a non-egalitarian one, where
only one sex has influence. Even at relatively
small population sizes, these two conditions re-
sult in large differences in group composition.
Across 100 simulations at a population size of 20,
for example, there was a significantly larger
proportion of unrelated dyads in the modeled
egalitarian camps (12.0% T8.4) compared with
the non-egalitarian, single-sex dispersal camps
(0.6% T1.5) (c
2
= 4372.36, P<0.001;Figs.1and2).
Although it is known that group relatedness de-
creases with increased group size (15), modeled
egalitarian camps show higher proportions of
unrelated individuals irrespective of camp size
(
F3 Fig. 3).
The modeled co-residence patterns also mirror
our observed data. The proportion of unrelated
dyads in the model at a comparable group size
(n=20agents)(12.0%T8.4) was not significantly
different from the observed proportion of unre-
lated co-residency among the Agta (11.2%, c
2
=
1.98, P= 0.016). Although the Mbendjele had sig-
nificantly larger numbers of unrelated individuals
inthecamps(28.6%)thanpredictedbythemodel
(c
2
= 440.76, P< 0.001), this was in the direction
consistent with our hypothesis. The obser ved
proportion of unrelated dyads among the Paranan
(4.2%) was larger than the modeled proportion
(0.6% T1.5, c
2
=183.41,P<0.001),butitwaslower
than either of the observed hunter-gatherer pop-
ulations (see above) and the egalitarian model
(c
2
= 58.65, P<0.001).
Our results suggest that pair-bonding alone is
not sufficient to explain the low levels of related-
ness seen in hunter-gatherer groups. Rather, both
pair-bonding and sex equality in residential decision-
making act together to constrain the overall re-
latedness of groups, leading to the co-residence
of individuals unrelated through either genetic
or affinal ties.
It has been proposed elsewhere that the com-
bination of pair-bonding, cooperation among un-
related males, and increased mobility derived from
male alliances could account for the low related-
ness of hunter-gatherer camps (7). We argue instead
that low within-camp relatedness is a consequence
of sex equality in hunter-gatherer couples, with
husbands and wives having an equal influence
over camp composition. Given sex equality, we
have shown that unrelated individuals come to
200 MONTH 2015 VOL 000 ISSUE 0000 sciencemag.org SCIENCE
Self
Primary
kin
Distant
kin
Spouse
Spouse's
primary kin
Spouse's
distant kin
Primary
kin's
spouse
Distant
affines
Self
Primary
kin
Distant
kin
Spouse
Spouse's
primary kin
Spouse's
distant kin
Primary
kin's
spouse
Distant
affines
No
relation
Fig. 2. Co-residence patterns across modeled and observed non-egalitarian populations. Chart
area represents the proportion of all dyads across nine categories of relatedness for the non-egalitarian
model (left) and Paranan (right).
Fig. 3. Modeling re-
latedness and equal-
ity. Results of the
egalitarian model (top)
and non-egalitarian
model (bottom)at
camp sizes between
10 and 80. From
bottom to top, areas
represent consanguineal
(genetic) kin, affinal kin,
distant affinal kin, and
unrelated individuals.
Exact proportions are
given in tables S3
and S4.
0%
20%
40%
60%
80%
100%
10 20 30 40 50 60 70
No relation
Distant affines
Affines
Consangineal kin
0%
20%
40%
60%
80%
100%
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80
No relation
Distant affines
Affines
Consangineal kin
RESEARCH |REPORT
MS no: REaaa5139/CF/ANTHRO
co-reside even when they display a strong indi-
vidual preference to live with kin, exemplified in
hunter-gatherers by the frequent co-residence of
brothers and sisters (14)andthehigherfrequen-
cy of related individuals in campmate and gift
networks (16). Therefore, our simulations provide
a mechanism for the emergence of low within-
camprelatednessinhunter-gatherersbysolving
the apparent contradiction between individual-
level preferences for living with kin and group-
level co-residence with non-kin. Gender inequality
reappeared in humans with the transition to
agriculture and pastoralism (17). Once heritable
resources, such as land and livestock, became
important determinants of reproductive success,
sex-biased inheritance and lineal systems started
to arise, leading to wealth and sex inequalities
(18). This predicted effect was demonstrated in
our non-egalitarian model and data from Paranan
agriculturalists. Our results also provide further
evidence that multilocality, rather than patrilocal-
ity, is the norm among mobile hunter-gatherers.
Understanding hunter-gatherer sex egalitari-
anism and the shift from hierarchical male philo-
patry typical of chimpanzees and bonobos to a
multilocal residence pattern is key to theories of
human social evolution. A possible clue for the
evolution of sex equality in the hominin lineage
was the increase in the cost of human reproduc-
tion associated with larger brain sizes in early
Homo (19). Higher offspring costs would require
investment from both mothers and fathers (20),
as seen among extant hunter-gatherers (3,21).
The need for biparental investment predicts in-
creased sex equality (22), reflected in the high
frequency of monogamy and the reproductive
schedules of male hunter-gatherers, who typi-
cally stop reproducing early and exhibit long
life spans after their last reproduction, in con-
trast to male farmers and pastoralists, whose
reproductive spans extend well into late life (23).
The recognition of affinal ties throughout our
long life span has been argued to be an impor-
tant step in human social evolution, and house-
hold residence may also be influenced by a tug of
war between a husband and his affinal kin, who
maywanttolivewiththeirdaughterorsister(7).
The possibility of recruiting help from both ma-
ternal and paternal kin by moving camps might
have been an important adaptation to meet
reproductive costs in unpredictable environments,
for example, by increasing the frequency of co-
residence with grandmothers, who have an im-
portant provisioning role in many hunter-gatherer
societies (24). Increased reproductive costs, coop-
erative breeding, and sex equality in residential
decision-making can explain why hunter-gatherer
parents live in groups containing multiple mated
pairs, why hunter-gatherers recruit help both from
related and unrelated individuals, and why hunter-
gatherer camps exhibit low levels of relatedness.
Sex equality and the resulting low within-camp
relatedness had many important consequences.
Co-residence with unrelated individuals set the
selective environment for the evolution of hyper-
cooperation and prosociality (25). Sex equality
suggests a scenario where cooperation among
unrelated individuals can evolve in the absence
of wealth accumulation, reproductive inequalities,
and intergroup warfare (26). Couples freely mov-
ing between camps and sharing interests with
kin and affines would be able to maintain coop-
eration without the need for more complex sys-
tems, such as cultural group selection and altruistic
punishment (27).
Last, this social system may have allowed
hunter-gatherers to extend their social networks,
buffering environmental risk and promoting lev-
els of information exchange required for cumu-
lative culture (2831).
REFERENCES AND NOTES
1. M. Gurven, Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 543583 (2004).
2. P. K. Ivey, Curr. Anthropol. 41, 856866 (2000).
3. C. L. Meehan, R. Quinlan, C. D. Malcom, Am. J. Hum. Biol. 25,
4257 (2013).
4. J. Woodburn, Man 17, 431451 (1982).
5. J. Lewis, in Hunter-Gatherers of the Congo Basin: Cultures,
Histories, and Biology of African Pygmies, B. S. Hewlett, Ed.
(Transaction, City, NJ, 2014), pp. 219244.
6. L. Rodseth, R. W. Wrangham, A. M. Harrigan, B. B. Smuts,
Curr. Anthropol. 32, 221254 (1991).
7. B. Chapais, Primeval Kinship: How Pair-Bonding Gave Birth to
Human Society (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 2009).
8. M. L. Wilson, R. W. Wrangham, Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 32,
363392 (2003).
9. J. Mitani, D. Watts, Behaviour 138, 299327 (2001).
10. R. W. Wrangham, D. Peterson, Demonic Males: Apes and the
Origins of Human Violence (Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, City, 1996).
11. P. Sicotte, Am. J. Primatol. 30,2136 (1993).
12. R. Kelly, J. Anthropol. Res. 39, 277306 (1983).
13. F. Marlowe, Curr. Anthropol. 45, 277284 (2004).
14. K. R. Hill et al., Science 331, 12861289 (2011).
15. R. S. Walker, Evol. Hum. Behav. 35, 384388 (2014).
16. C. L. Apicella, F. W. Marlowe, J. H. Fowler, N. A. Christakis,
Nature 481, 497501 (2012).
17. M. Martin, B. Voorhies, Female of the Species (Columbia Univ.
Press, New York, 1975).
18. R. Mace, Evol. Anthropol. 22, 251258 (2013).
19. L. Aiello, P. Wheeler, Curr. Anthropol. 36, 199221 (1995).
20. H. Kaplan, K. Hill, J. Lancaster, A. M. Hurtado, Evol. Anthropol.
9, 156185 (2000).
21. K. Hill, A. M. Hurtado, Proc. Biol. Sci. 276, 38633870
(2009).
22. H. S. Kaplan, J. B. Lancaster, in Offspring: Human Fertility
Behavior in Biodemographic Perspective, K. W. Wachter,
R. A. Bulatao, Eds. (National Academies Press, Washington,
DC, 2003), pp. 170223.
23. L. Vinicius, R. Mace, A. Migliano, PLOS ONE 9, e112236 (2014).
24. K. Hawkes, J. F. OConnell, N. G. Jones, H. Alvarez,
E. L. Charnov, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95,13361339
(1998).
25. J. M. Burkart et al., Nat. Commun. 5, 4747 (2014).
26. D. P. Fry, P. Söderberg, Science 341, 270273 (2013).
27. H. M. Lewis, L. Vinicius, J. Strods, R. Mace, A. B. Migliano,
Nat. Commun. 5, 5789 (2014).
28. M. Kempe, A. Mesoudi, Evol. Hum. Behav. 35, 285290
(2014).
29. K. R. Hill, B. M. Wood, J. Baggio, A. M. Hurtado, R. T. Boyd,
PLOS ONE 9, e102806 (2014).
30. R. Lee, The! Kung San (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge,
1979).
31. R. Tonkinson, The Mardu Aborigines (Holt, Rinehard, and
Winston, City, 1978).
ACKNO WLED GME NTS
This project was funded by the Leverhulme Trust grant
RP2011-R-045 to A.B.M. R.M. also received funding from European
Research Council Advanced Grant AdG 249347. A.B.M.
conceived the project; M.D. created the model; M.D., A.P.,
G.D.S., N.C., A.B.M., D.S., and J.T. collected the data; M.D. and
G.D.S. wrote scripts and analyzed the data. M.D., A.B.M, L.V.,
and R.M. wrote the paper. We thank P. Gerbault, J. Stevenson,
J. Lewis, and R. Schlaepfer for help in the field and the Human
Evolutionary Ecology Group at UCL as well as three anonymous
reviewers for valuable comments on the manuscript. Last, we
thank our assistants in both Congo and the Philippines as well
as the Agta, Paranan, and Mbendjele communities.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS
www.sciencemag.org/content/348/[issue no.]/[page]/suppl/DC1
Materials and Methods
Figs. S1 and S2
Tables S1 to S9
References (3237)
18 December 2014; accepted 16 April 2015
10.1126/science.aaa5139
SCIENCE sciencemag.org 00 MONTH 2015 VOL 000 ISSUE 0000 3
RESEARCH |REPORT
MS no: REaaa5139/CF/ANTHRO
... In particular, population diversity confers a society with large interpersonal heterogeneity in human traits and greater complementarities between individual abilities and the physical environment, thus fostering the returns from economic specialization Galor 2013, 2018;Depetris-Chauvin andÖzak 2020). Previous studies also indicate that the development of patriarchal norms has its deep historical roots in the Neolithic Revolution (Lerner 1986;Diamond 1987;Iversen and Rosenbluth 2010;Dyble et al. 2015;Hansen et al. 2015). For example, Iversen and Rosenbluth (2010, p. 32) provide evidence suggesting egalitarian gender roles in a hunter-gatherer agricultural society, and postulate that the Neolithic transition triggered ''a premium on male brawn in plowing and other heavy farm work'' and therefore gave rise to a patriarchal system emphasizing male dominance. ...
... 4 This argument rhymes well withDiamond (1987) andLerner (1986) documenting that societies with an earlier prehistorical transition to sedentary agriculture are characterized by greater social and gender inequality. In a similar vein,Dyble et al. (2015) demonstrate that societies with longer agricultural histories are endowed with more unequal gender roles because heritable resources became central to reproductive success.© 2024 The Author(s). ...
Article
Full-text available
I study the impact of population diversity, determined predominantly over the prehistorical out‐of‐Africa migration process of anatomically modern humans, on present‐day gender inequality. Leveraging variations across countries and individuals residing in the same country but descending from different prehistorically indigenous ethnic groups, I find that deep‐rooted population diversity negatively affects women's economic and political empowerment. I provide several explanations for this finding. First, preindustrial societies with higher interpersonal diversity tended to experience early technological development, which primarily translated into fertility growth associated with lower women's social status during the Malthusian epoch. Second, population diversity was conducive to economic specialization, which co‐evolved with and amplified the traditional gendered division of labour particularly in preindustrial societies with long agricultural histories. Finally, the early formation and prevalence of unequal gender roles in preindustrial times would gradually increase intergenerationally transmitted cultural beliefs about gender inequality, leading to wide and persistent disparities in gender roles across the contemporary world.
... Frontiers in Pharmacology frontiersin.org 08 excessive inflammatory reactions and autoimmune diseases that may harm healthy tissues (Dai et al., 2014;Dyble et al., 2015). In the endeavor to corral tumor cells, the PD-1/PD-L1 inhibitory axis is a vulnerability. ...
Article
Full-text available
With the use of T cell receptor T cells (TCR-T cells) and chimeric antigen receptor T cells (CAR-T cells), T-cell immunotherapy for cancer has advanced significantly in recent years. CAR-T cell therapy has demonstrated extraordinary success when used to treat hematologic malignancies. Nevertheless, there are several barriers that prevent this achievement from being applied to solid tumors, such as challenges with tumor targeting and inadequate transit and adaption of genetically modified T-cells, especially in unfavorable tumor microenvironments The deficiencies of CAR-T cell therapy in the treatment of solid tumors are compensated for by TCR-T cells, which have a stronger homing ability to initiate intracellular commands, 90% of the proteins can be used as developmental targets, and they can recognize target antigens more broadly. As a result, TCR-T cells may be more effective in treating solid tumors. In this review, we discussed the structure of TCR-T and have outlined the drawbacks of TCR-T in cancer therapy, and suggested potential remedies. This review is crucial in understanding the current state and future potential of TCR-T cell therapy. We emphasize how important it is to use combinatorial approaches, combining new combinations of various emerging strategies with over-the-counter therapies designed for TCR-T, to increase the anti-tumor efficacy of TCR-T inside the TME and maximize treatment safety, especially when it comes to solid tumor immunotherapies.
... 2,45 Traditional egalitarian hunter-gatherer societies practiced equitable resource sharing. 46,47 Some argue the introduction of ploughing agricultural practices or intensive animal husbandry has shifted many societies from matrilineal to patrilineal structures. 48 This change is evident in various regions, with the spread of cattle in sub-Saharan Africa causing a decline of matriliny. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research examines dynamics of kinship systems, emphasizing changes in gender-biased inheritance and social interaction within a formerly matrilineal community. Using demographic data over 70-years of lifespan from 17 Tibetan villages, we observe a significant shift within the predominantly matrilineal inheritance structure: a once-prevalent preference for females in older cohorts has now gone in recent generations. We explore two possible explanations: that this is driven by changes in subsistence system or by changes in sibling configuration. Our investigation reveals that a change from agriculture to non-traditional economy with more market integration marks a pivot from matrilineal to non-unilineal inheritance systems. Moreover, results from economic games conducted in two distinct survey periods (2015 and 2021), indicate that high donations for females in 2015 has become unbiased in 2021. These findings provide concrete evidence of shifts in gender preference both at the level of familial resource allocation and broader societal interactions.
... The Mbendjele are a subgroup of this population who speak Mbendjele 45 . The BaYaka live in multifamily camps, consisting of 10-60 individuals 46 , and move campsites based on the availability of forest products and trading opportunities throughout the year. The mode of subsistence is largely dependent on forest products: with food obtained by hunting, fishing and gathering wild products such as yams, caterpillars and honey depending on the season. ...
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary hunter-gatherers are highly active, but little is known about physical activity levels in hunter-gatherer children. We analysed 150 days of accelerometer data from 51 BaYaka hunter-gatherer children (aged 3–18) in the Republic of Congo, comparing it with British and American children using samples from Millennium Cohort Study (MCS) and National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES). BaYaka children were highly active, engaging in over 3 h of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) daily, surpassing British adolescents by over 70 min. Unlike US children, whose activity diminished with age, BaYaka children’s activity levels increased, irrespective of gender. This trend suggests that formal education may suppress activity among American children, a pattern not seen in the BaYaka community. Reflecting their foraging lifestyle, activity patterns varied within and between days in BaYaka children, a contrast to the more uniform daily activity observed in American children. Furthermore, our data challenges the concept of ‘teenage chronotypes’ prevalent in post-industrial societies, with adolescent BaYaka maintaining shorter sleep phases and later bedtimes, synchronized with sunrise. These findings highlight the impact of a foraging upbringing on children’s activity levels, providing a benchmark for understanding childhood physical activity and wellbeing.
... A study in the USA found that one third of individuals who would have been classified as 'single' in these surveys were in fact in LAT relationships (Strohm et al., 2009) The last two systems (which are similar enough that they are often used interchangeably)ambilocal and bilocal residence -are particularly common in mobile and egalitarian huntergatherers (often referred to in the literature as immediate-return -see chapter xx). Research across multiple such hunter-gatherer populations such as the Hadza, Aché, Hiwi, Agta, BaYaka, Pumé and Aka, alongside computational modelling, has demonstrated that because either the men or the women can co-reside with their family, often brothers and sisters are co-resident in a camp, as well as lots of unrelated people (Dyble et al., 2015;Hill et al., 2011). This makes an interesting situation where individuals co-reside and cooperate on a daily basis with both close kin and non-kin, which has implications for the societal structure (i.e. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
A family is a network of individuals tied to each other by blood, marriage, adoption or fostering and comes in many forms and sizes. There is no singular or universal type of family. Some common family types or structures include a single parent and children, a ‘nuclear’ family comprising two parents and children, or extended families consisting of one or two parents, children and other relatives. However, a range of different family types exist within this spectrum. A household is a commonly used term or unit of analysis to describe a group of people’s living arrangements and relationships to each other. What comprises a household can vary across cultures, social contexts and even seasons. While family and households are often conflated in the literature, in reality they are different. Families refer to specific kinship structures, whereas not all members of a household are necessarily kin but instead are tied to each other through co-residence and roles and responsibilities around the production and consumption of food and labour. One key reason we see so much variation in families and households is because people usually tend to live with other people - although, of course, living alone is also a type of household. Thus, family and household size, structure and composition can vary for multiple reasons across cultures including the number of children a parent/couple have, child and adult survival rates, the types of marriage which form the family and the post-marital residence systems which influence the generational configuration of households. This chapter underlines how flexible and context-specific human family systems are, and how the vast diversity present in humans leads to endless types of families and households.
Article
Charles Darwin’s theory of evolution through natural selection, explaining geographical distributions and the fossil record, is rightly regarded as one of the greatest scientific theories of all time, taking its place alongside Isaac Newton’s theory of gravitational attraction, explaining the Copernican heliocentric world picture. There is, however, a tendency to think that Darwin’s work is finished. It belongs to Victorian history rather than as something that has crucial social relevance today. This essay shows how mistaken it is to make this assumption. Through a series of case histories―foreigners, class, sexual orientation, and women―Darwinism is shown to be as vibrant and important today as it was when Darwin was young. It is an essential tool for analyzing and solving some of the biggest and most pressing social issues facing us in the twenty-first century.
Article
La teoría de la evolución por selección natural de Charles Darwin, que explica las distribuciones geográficas y el registro fósil, se considera, con razón, una de las teorías científicas más importantes de todos los tiempos, y ocupa su lugar junto con la teoría de la atracción gravitacional de Isaac Newton, que explica la imagen heliocéntrica del mundo de Copérnico. Sin embargo, existe una tendencia a pensar que el trabajo de Darwin está acabado. Que pertenece más a la historia victoriana que a algo que tenga una relevancia social crucial en la actualidad. Este ensayo evidencia cuán equivocada es esa suposición. A través de una serie de casos históricos (extranjeros, clase social, orientación sexual y mujeres), se demuestra que el darwinismo es tan vibrante e importante hoy como lo era cuando Darwin era joven. Es una herramienta esencial para analizar y resolver algunos de los problemas sociales más importantes y apremiantes que enfrentamos en el siglo XXI.
Article
Full-text available
Humans evolved to solve adaptive problems with kin and nonkin across fitness-relevant domains, including childcare and resource sharing, among others. Therefore, there is a great diversity in the types of interdependences humans experience across activities, relationships, and ecologies. To identify human psychological adaptations for cooperation, we argue that researchers must accurately characterize human fitness interdependence (FI). We propose a theoretical framework for assessing variation in FI that applies to the social interactions humans would have experienced across situations, relationships, and ecologies in the ancestral past and continue to experience today. According to this model, FI is characterized along four dimensions: (a) corresponding versus conflicting interests (b) mutual dependence versus independence, (c) asymmetrical versus symmetrical dependence (i.e., power), and (d) coordination. Because humans evolved to be highly mutually dependent on others to solve myriad adaptive problems, even compared to our closest living relatives, there is immense variability in the types of interdependences humans experience in daily life. Here, we describe the kinds of variation in interdependence humans experience, paying particular attention to social life in small-scale societies. In demonstrating the diversity of conflicts and coordination problems humans manage, we contend that humans evolved psychological adaptations to infer from signals, cues, and properties of the environment the four dimensions of FI under degrees of uncertainty to reduce the costs of cooperation. We conclude by discussing the theoretical implications of FI theory and emphasize that when individuals understand that others depend on them, it gives way to a new means of leverage to influence how others behave toward them.
Article
Full-text available
The nature of hunter-gatherer mobility strategies--the way in which hunter-gatherers move about a landscape over the course of a year--is discussed, using ethnographic data. Several mobility variables that measure residential and logistical mobility are defined; several environmental variables which measure resource accessibility and resource monitoring costs are also defined. Ethnographic data are used to demonstrate patterning between the nature of mobility strategies and the resource structure of an environment. The data show that the extent to which a group of hunter-gatherers emphasizes residential or logistical mobility is closely related to the structure of resources in their environment.
Article
Full-text available
'Simple' hunter-gatherer populations adopt the social norm of 'demand sharing', an example of human hyper-cooperation whereby food brought into camps is claimed and divided by group members. Explaining how demand sharing evolved without punishment to free riders, who rarely hunt but receive resources from active hunters, has been a long-standing problem. Here we show through a simulation model that demand-sharing families that continuously move between camps in response to their energy income are able to survive in unpredictable environments typical of hunter-gatherers, while non-sharing families and sedentary families perish. Our model also predicts that non-producers (free riders, pre-adults and post-productive adults) can be sustained in relatively high numbers. As most of hominin pre-history evolved in hunter-gatherer settings, demand sharing may be an ancestral manifestation of hyper-cooperation and inequality aversion, allowing exploration of high-quality, hard-to-acquire resources, the evolution of fluid co-residence patterns and egalitarian resource distribution in the absence of punishment or warfare.
Article
Full-text available
Most accounts of human life history propose that women have short reproductive spans relative to their adult lifespans, while men not only remain fertile but carry on reproducing until late life. Here we argue that studies have overlooked evidence for variation in male reproductive ageing across human populations. We apply a Bayesian approach to census data from Agta hunter-gatherers and Gambian farmers to show that long post-reproductive lifespans characterise not only women but also males in some traditional human populations. We calculate three indices of reproductive ageing in men (oldest age at reproduction, male late-life reproduction, and post-reproductive representation) and identify a continuum of male reproductive longevity across eight traditional societies ranging from !Kung, Hadza and Agta hunter-gatherers exhibiting low levels of polygyny, early age at last reproduction and long post-reproductive lifespans, to male Gambian agriculturalists and Turkana pastoralists showing higher levels of polygyny, late-life reproduction and shorter post-reproductive lifespans. We conclude that the uniquely human detachment between rates of somatic senescence and reproductive decline, and the existence of post-reproductive lifespans, are features of both male and female life histories, and therefore not exclusive consequences of menopause.
Article
Full-text available
Proactive, that is, unsolicited, prosociality is a key component of our hyper-cooperation, which in turn has enabled the emergence of various uniquely human traits, including complex cognition, morality and cumulative culture and technology. However, the evolutionary foundation of the human prosocial sentiment remains poorly understood, largely because primate data from numerous, often incommensurable testing paradigms do not provide an adequate basis for formal tests of the various functional hypotheses. We therefore present the results of standardized prosociality experiments in 24 groups of 15 primate species, including humans. Extensive allomaternal care is by far the best predictor of interspecific variation in proactive prosociality. Proactive prosocial motivations therefore systematically arise whenever selection favours the evolution of cooperative breeding. Because the human data fit this general primate pattern, the adoption of cooperative breeding by our hominin ancestors also provides the most parsimonious explanation for the origin of human hyper-cooperation.
Article
Full-text available
Our species exhibits spectacular success due to cumulative culture. While cognitive evolution of social learning mechanisms may be partially responsible for adaptive human culture, features of early human social structure may also play a role by increasing the number potential models from which to learn innovations. We present interview data on interactions between same-sex adult dyads of Ache and Hadza hunter-gatherers living in multiple distinct residential bands (20 Ache bands; 42 Hadza bands; 1201 dyads) throughout a tribal home range. Results show high probabilities (5%–29% per year) of cultural and cooperative interactions between randomly chosen adults. Multiple regression suggests that ritual relationships increase interaction rates more than kinship, and that affinal kin interact more often than dyads with no relationship. These may be important features of human sociality. Finally, yearly interaction rates along with survival data allow us to estimate expected lifetime partners for a variety of social activities, and compare those to chimpanzees. Hadza and Ache men are estimated to observe over 300 men making tools in a lifetime, whereas male chimpanzees interact with only about 20 other males in a lifetime. High intergroup interaction rates in ancestral humans may have promoted the evolution of cumulative culture.
Article
Full-text available
The relatedness of human groups has important ramifications for kin (group) selection to favor more collective action and invites the potential for more exploitation by political leaders. Endogamous marriages among kin create intensive kinship systems with high group relatedness, while exogamous marriages among nonrelatives create extensive kinship with low group relatedness. Here, a sample of 58 societies (7,565 adults living in 353 residential groups) shows that average group relatedness is higher in lowland horticulturalists than in hunter-gatherers. Higher relatedness in horticulturalists is remarkable given that village sizes are larger, harboring over twice the average number of adults than in hunter-gatherer camps. The relatedness differential between subsistence regimes increases for larger group sizes. Large and dense networks of kin may have favored an increased propensity for some forms of in-group cooperation and political inequality that emerged relatively recently in human history, after the advent of farming.
Article
Greater equality of wealth, of power and of prestige has been achieved in certain hunting and gathering societies than in any other human societies. These societies, which have economies based on immediate rather than delayed return, are assertively egalitarian. Equality is achieved through direct, individual access to resources through direct, individual access to means of coercion and means of mobility which limit the imposition of control through procedures which prevent saving and accumulation and impose sharing through mechanisms which allow goods to circulate without making people dependent upon one another. People are systematically disengaged from property and therefore from the potentiality in property for creating dependency. A comparison is made between these societies and certain other egalitarian societies in which there is profound intergenerational inequality and in which the equality between people of senior generation is only a starting point for strenuous competition resulting in inequality. The value systems of non-competitive, egalitarian hunter-gatherers limit the development of agriculture because rules of sharing restrict the investment and savings necessary for agriculture they may limit the care provided for the incapacitated because of the controls on dependency they may in principle, extend equality to all mankind.-Author